1-s2.0-s1526590012009637-main

Upload: vieriu-alexandra

Post on 04-Jun-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    1/11

    Acceptance, Cognitive Restructuring, and Distraction as Coping

    Strategies for Acute Pain

    Annika Kohl, Winfried Rief, and Julia Anna GlombiewskiDepartment of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Philipps-University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany.

    Abstract: Little is known about treatment mechanisms underlying acceptance strategies. Accep-tance is a strategy that is expected to increase pain tolerance more than distraction, while distraction

    should lead to lower pain intensity. The effect of cognitive restructuring on experimental pain has

    not yet been investigated. The present study aimed to explore differential short-term effects of

    acceptance, distraction, and cognitive restructuring on pain tolerance and intensity. Pain was induced

    in a sample of 109 female students using a thermode. We conducted analyses of covariance with in-

    struction as the independent variable and posttest scores on pain variables as dependent variables,

    covarying for pretest scores. In addition, adherence to instructions and credibility of instructions

    were included as covariates. Acceptance led to a higher increase in pain tolerance than did cognitive

    restructuring of pain-related thoughts. No differences were detected between either acceptance and

    distraction or distraction and cognitive restructuring with respect to pain tolerance. Distraction led to

    lower pain intensity compared to acceptance. Cognitive restructuring did not differ from either ac-

    ceptance or distraction with respect to pain intensity. As a short-term strategy, cognitive restructur-

    ing was not as useful as acceptance in increasing pain tolerance. Further studies should evaluate the

    preconditions under which different strategies are most effective.

    Perspective: This study demonstrated that acceptance was superior to cognitive restructuring inincreasing tolerance for experimentally induced pain, but was inferior to distraction with respect

    to decreasing pain intensity. Knowledge about the types of strategies that are useful in targeting di-

    verse pain-related outcome measures is important for efforts to refine the treatment of chronic pain.

    2013 by the American Pain Society

    Key words: Acceptance, cognitive restructuring, distraction, pain tolerance, pain intensity, acute pain,

    short-term strategies.

    Numerous studies have investigated the effects ofacceptance-based interventions for chronic

    pain.37,39,40 Although the theoretical concept ofacceptance has existed for many years and has beenextensively described by the developers of Acceptanceand Commitment Therapy (ACT),16 the mechanismsthrough which acceptance exerts its effects remain un-clear. Furthermore, little is known about the relative ef-ficacy of acceptance strategies compared to otherstrategies used in established treatments for chronicpain, such as distraction and cognitive restructuring.

    Experimental studies are needed to better evaluatethe mechanisms of these strategies. Therefore, the pres-

    ent study used an experimental design to compare vari-ous short-term coping strategies with respect to theireffects on pain tolerance and pain intensity.

    In the context of treatment for pain, acceptance isintended to disrupt the link between thoughts andbehaviors such that participants are willing to toleratepainful stimulation for longer periods of time (withnegative thoughts and feelings) in order to pursuevalues-based activities. The majority of experimentalstudies support this hypothesis: an acceptance strategyhas been shown to be more effective at increasingpain tolerance than were other pain-regulationstrategies, for example distraction or suppres-sion.3,13,15,24,26,30 About half of the studies inducedpain through thermal stimuli, and the remainingstudies applied electrical stimulation. Only 1 studyinvestigated influences on pain tolerance andintensity in a clinical sample.38 A meta-analytic reviewfound a statistical advantage of acceptance strategies

    Received September 19, 2012; Revised November 26, 2012; AcceptedDecember 7, 2012.The study was funded by Philipps-University of Marburg.There is no actual or potential conflict of interest for any of the authors.Address reprint requests to Dr. Julia Anna Glombiewski, Departmentof Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Philipps-University ofMarburg, Gutenbergstrasse 18, Marburg, Germany. E-mail: [email protected]

    1526-5900/$36.00

    2013 by the American Pain Society

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.12.005

    305

    The Journal of Pain, Vol 14, No 3 (March), 2013: pp 305-315

    Available online atwww.jpain.organdwww.sciencedirect.com

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.12.005http://www.jpain.org/http://www.sciencedirect.com/http://www.sciencedirect.com/http://www.jpain.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.12.005mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    2/11

    compared to other pain regulation strategies for paintolerance.20

    Distraction aims to shift the attentional focus awayfrom painful stimulation and thereby to lessen pain in-tensity. Some studies indicate that strategies such as dis-traction or suppression are more effective at reducingpain intensity relative to an acceptance strategy.3,13,30

    On the other hand, a meta-analytic review indicatedthat distraction and suppression were not superior to ac-ceptance strategies at reducing pain intensity.20 Due tothe fact that studies came to contradictory results, dis-traction was included as an adaptive and well-knowncontrol group for acceptance strategies.

    In addition to distraction, cognitive-behavioral treat-ments (CBT) for chronic pain implement cognitive re-structuring.18,28 Patients are trained to alter theappraisal of dysfunctional pain-related thoughts in orderto improve coping with pain. Some studies also suggestinfluences of cognitive variables on pain intensity.4,8

    Based on the theoretical assumptions of the CBT

    model, it appears that restructuring of pain-relatedthoughts may affect disability-related behavior such aswithdrawing from painful stimuli. One study investigat-ing anxiety found that a reappraisal strategy was moreeffective at moderating the subjective feeling of anxietycompared to an acceptance strategy, while for other out-come measures no significant differences were found.17

    A study on anger found a greater reduction in the reap-praisal condition than in the acceptance condition, andparticipants in the reappraisal condition tolerated a frus-tration task longer than those in the acceptance condi-tion.35

    To the best of our knowledge, no experimental study

    to date has compared the effects of acceptance, distrac-tion, and cognitive restructuring on pain tolerance andpain intensity.

    Therefore, we aimed to investigate whether thesestrategies differentially affect pain tolerance and pain in-tensity.

    Methods

    ParticipantsAn a priori power analysis was performed with

    G*Power 3.1.3. The power analysis indicated that givenan effect-size (f) of .3, 3 groups and 2 degrees of free-dom, and 3 covariates, a sample of 111 participants wasexpected to be suitable for detecting main and interac-tion effects with a power of .8 and an alpha criterionof .05. We expected an effect size of .3 because previousstudies showed medium effect sizes.13,20,26

    Participants were German first-year female undergrad-uate psychology students recruited through announce-ments at the Philipps-University of Marburg and via the

    internet. Students received course credit for participa-tion. Exclusion criteria were male gender, chronic andacute pain conditions, Raynauds disease, high bloodpressure, neuropathy, coronary diseases, diabetes,insufficient knowledge of German language, and cur-rent pain-medication use. We excluded male participants

    because several recommendations suggest investigatingfemales (eg12) due to the greater prevalence of pain con-ditions in women and differences in pain sensitivity. Fur-thermore, men and women differ in pain sensations,5,34

    and therefore including both genders could producemore subgroups or confounding variables. Moreover,we excluded men in order to prevent any interaction

    effect of experimenter gender and participant genderon pain tolerance.22 We chose female experimenters toavoid potential influences of a cross-gender interaction.

    One hundred fifteen participants completed onlinequestionnaires. Five participants were excluded due topain medication intake, chronic or acute pain, or Ray-nauds disease. One hundred ten participants were ran-domly allocated to 1 of the 3 conditions: acceptance,distraction, or cognitive restructuring. One participantwas excluded after randomization due to insufficientlanguage skills (total N = 109). Participants ages rangedfrom 19 to 30 years (mean = 22.1, SD = 2.38).

    The research project was approved by the Ethics Com-

    mittee of The German Psychological Society (DeutscheGesellschaft fur Psychologie, DGPs, WR032010). Partici-pants were informed of the procedure and had the op-portunity to withdraw from the study at any time.

    Study DesignA mixed between-within design with 3 factors was

    employed. The between-group factor was instructioncondition (acceptance, distraction, and restructuring).The within-group factor was time point (pretest versusposttest).

    Procedure

    Self-Report Measures

    All participants completed 3 questionnaires assessinghabitual coping strategies corresponding to the 3 differ-ent instructions. This procedure allows testing regardingwhether group differences existed in habitual copingstrategies due to unsuccessful randomization.

    Questionnaires were completed at home prior to theexperimental session. Habitual cognitive restructuringof pain-related thoughts and mental distraction from

    pain-related thoughts were assessed using the CopingStrategies and Pain-Related Distress Questionnaire(FESV; Cronbachs a= .77, test re-test reliability = .79).10

    An example item assessing cognitive restructuring isWhen I am in pain, I say to myself that because ofpain, I learn to appreciate painless conditions. An exam-ple item assessing mental distraction is When I am inpain, I distract myself by listening to good music. Habit-ual acceptance of pain was measured using a question-naire created by the authors with 10 items rated ona 5-point Likert scale. Items were generated by adapting

    items fromthe Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire(CPAQ),25,29 the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire(AAQ II),2 and the Difficulties in Emotion RegulationScale (DERS).11 Items were reformulated to refer topain conditions and to be appropriate for use in healthysamples (I accept when I am in pain or I do a lot of

    306 The Journal of Pain Strategies for Acute Pain

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    3/11

    activities when I am in pain). Furthermore, consultationwith experts in the field of acceptance-based treatmentsas well as pain suggested that our questionnaire has highface validity. Each item was also discussed within ourworking group. Participants also completed the DERSand the AAQ II in order to validate the newly createdquestionnaire. Data for these measures are not reported

    within this article because these questionnaires were notused in analyses. The measure of habitual coping strate-gies (acceptance) showed good internal consistency(Cronbachs a= .76). It was significantly correlated withmeasures of similar constructs such as the AAQ II (r =.39; P< .01) and the Non-Acceptance subscale of theDERS(r = .3; P< .01). The questionnaire assessing habit-ual coping strategies (acceptance) was not significantlycorrelated with measures of theoretically distinct con-structs such as the cognitive restructuring subscale ofthe FESV (r = .06; P= .55) and the mental distraction sub-scale of the FESV (r = .02,P= .8).

    In addition, participants completed the German ver-

    sion of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)14

    and thePain Sensitivity Questionnaire(PSQ; Cronbachs a= .92,test re-test reliability = .83).33 In the PSQ, respondentsrate the degree of pain associated with various scenarios(eg, Imagine you burn your tongue on a very hot drink)on an 11-point Likert scale. We also assessed level of paincatastrophizing using the German version of the PainCatastrophizing Scale (PCS; Cronbachs a= .92).27 An ex-ample item is I worry all the time about whether thepain will end.

    Estimates of internal consistency and reliability ratingsare drawn from prior research, with the exception of the

    data from the questionnaire designed by the authors

    (habitual acceptance of pain).

    Pretest

    Two different female experimenters conducted the ex-perimental sessions. Participants signed an informedconsent form. They were given the opportunity to askquestions and were informed about the procedure andthe application of the thermode. Before the pretest, par-ticipants had the opportunity to familiarize themselveswith the procedure and to practice stopping the heatstimulus.

    Participants underwent the same assessment twice:pain tolerance and pain intensity were measured at pre-

    test and at posttest.

    Stimulus Material and Outcome Measures

    We employed thermal stimuli between 32 and 50C toinduce pain. Stimuli were delivered to the dominantforearm via a 3- 3-cm peltier-based thermode (TSA II:Thermal Sensory Analyzer; Medoc Ltd, Ramat Yishai,Israel). The thermal stimulus started at 32C and rosewith a slope of .5C per second. The thermode remainedat the same place on the skin for pretest and posttest.

    Participants were asked to tolerate stimuli as long as pos-sible and to stop the stimulus by clicking a computermouse. Pain tolerance was determined by the tempera-

    ture at which the participant stopped the thermal stimu-

    lus. When the maximum temperature was reached, thesoftware automatically returned the thermode to thebaseline temperature of 32C.

    Pain intensity was assessed with a 10-cm visual analogscale (VAS) immediately after the termination of eachthermal stimulus. The minimum anchor was no painand the maximum anchor was worst imaginable pain.

    Instructional Set

    Participants listened to 1 of the 3 instructions via head-

    phones. Instructions were based on those used in severalprior studies.13,31,32,38 All instructions were approved byseveral experts on pain treatments, mindfulness-basedtreatments, and CBT. Several pilot tests were conductedand instructions were modified after each trial accordingto experts ratings.

    All instructions were approximately the same length(5.5 minutes) and followed the same structure: partici-pants were asked to write down 3 thoughts that hadled to termination of pretest stimuli in order to work

    on these thoughts using the specific strategies. Next,a description of the strategy was given, followed by anexample of successful application of the strategy (a pain-ful situation at a dentists office was used as an example).Participants were given the opportunity to practice thestrategy before the concrete instructions for the secondtrial (posttest) were provided.

    Acceptance Instruction. It was explained thatthoughts often initiate behavior, but that it is also possi-ble to disengage oneself from these thoughts (defusion)through nonjudgmental awareness (mindfulness) or ac-ceptance. The strategy of regarding thoughts as cloudsin the sky was discussed as an example of defusion. If

    thoughts can be accepted, they no longer control behav-ioral tendencies and do not inhibit personal goals.Within the exercise, participants were asked to imaginethat they were experiencing the thermal stimulus andto regard their thoughts as clouds in the sky passing by.

    Cognitive Restructuring Instruction. It was explainedthat thoughts, feelings, and actions are related. It is pos-sible to alter the appraisal of a situation so that feelingsand behavior tendencies also change according to the re-appraisal. Negative and dysfunctional thoughts can bereplaced by more functional ones, and the new pointof view results in increased freedom of action. Withinthe exercise, participants were asked to imagine the ap-

    plication of the thermode and to develop alternativethoughts such as this painful experience does notmean that my skin will be damaged or toleratingpain is a challenge and I have already overcome worsepain.

    Distraction Instruction. It was explained that distrac-tion can lead to reduced perception of thoughts andfeelings. Attention can work like a spotlight: depending

    on which thoughts and feelings come into focus, otherthoughts and feelings may be blanked out. It is possi-ble to distract oneself internally or externally. Internaldistraction may take place via imagination or recallingpast experiences, while external distraction may involve

    increasing attention to environmental stimuli. Within

    Kohl, Rief, and Glombiewski The Journal of Pain 307

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    4/11

    the exercise, participants were asked to imagine feelingthe heat stimulus and to distract themselves by imaginga pleasant scene. For further details, see the Appendix.

    Posttest

    Following the instructions (approximately 6 minutesafter pretest), the heat stimulus was applied again. Par-

    ticipants stopped the thermal stimulus by a mouse clickwhen no longer willing to tolerate it and were askedto rate pain intensity on the VAS afterward.

    Manipulation Check and Pain Threshold

    After the posttest, participants received a manipula-tion check questionnaire assessing the extent to whichthe instructions were applied (rated as a percentage).Participants also rated the credibility of the instructionson a 4-point scale ranging from very much to notat all.

    Furthermore, pain thresholds were determined by themean value of 3 trials for the heat stimulus, in which

    participants were instructed to stop the stimulus whenit became painful. The interstimulus interval betweeneach of the trials was set to 4 seconds. Pain thresholdwas determined after pre- and posttest assessment ofpain tolerance and intensity in order to avoid habitua-tion to the heat stimulus.

    Results

    Statistical AnalysisThree participants were able to tolerate the maximum

    of heat of 50C during the pretest and were excludedfrom further analyses of pain tolerance. We also

    excluded 1 outlier with pain tolerance levels for bothpretest and posttest that were >2.5 SD below the meanpain tolerance level (seeTable 1).

    All analyses were performed using the Statistical Pack-age for Social Sciences (SPSS, Windows v.19: SPSS Inc,Chicago, IL).

    To assess the differential effects of instructions on paintolerance and pain intensity, we performed separateanalyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) for each outcomemeasure. Instruction condition was entered as theindependent variable, posttest data as the dependent

    variable, and pretest data as a covariate. In these ANCO-VAs, we also controlled for the information gathered viathe manipulation check (the percentage rating of adher-ence to instructions and the rated credibility of the in-structions). We assumed the instructions to be morepowerful when rated as more credible. Post hoc pairwisecomparisons were performed to identify differences be-

    tween the 3 types of instructions.

    Baseline CharacteristicsNo significant differences were found in any of the

    baseline measures (eg, age or self-report measures);thus, we concluded that the randomization was success-ful. For further details seeTable 2. There were no signif-

    icant effects of experimenter on outcome measures.We explored whether adherence to instructions

    differed across groups using an analysis of variance(ANOVA) with instruction group as the independent var-iable and adherence to instructions as the dependentvariable. Significant group differences were found (F[2,

    106] = 3.48,P< .05). Pairwise post hoc analyses indicatedthat participants in the distraction condition showedhigher adherence to instruction compared to those inthe acceptance (P < .05) and cognitive restructuring(P< .05) conditions, while acceptance and cognitive re-

    structuring did not differ (P= .731).Furthermore, we calculated an ANOVA to test whether

    instruction conditions differed with respect to credibility.The effect was nonsignificant (F[2, 106] = 2.16,P= .121).

    Pain ToleranceMeans and standard deviations for pain tolerance

    (temperature in C) at pretest and posttest for the 3 con-

    ditions are shown inTable 3.The ANCOVA identified a significant effect of instruc-

    tion condition on pain tolerance at posttest (F[2,99] = 3.2,P< .05, partial Eta2 = .061), when controllingfor pretest scores. According to Ferguson,7 Eta2 can be

    interpreted as follows: .04 = small; .25 = moderate;and .64 strong. Furthermore, Ferguson notes that thedistinction between Eta2 and partial Eta2 tend to besmall in large samples. Pairwise post hoc analyses indi-cated that acceptance was significantly more effectivethan cognitive restructuring at increasing pain

    Table 1. Outlier Analysis

    CONDITION BDI PSQ PCS FESV-KU * FESV-MAy

    HABITUAL

    ACCEPTANCE

    OFPAINz DERS-NONACC.x AAQ-II{

    Ceiling effects

    No. 1 Distraction 2 4.14 28 5 10 28 16 58

    No. 2 Restructuring 18 3.86 32 7 8 29 8 48

    No. 3 Distraction 8 3.21 20 10 10 34 19 55

    Outlier6 2.5 SD

    No. 1 Acceptance 4 2.14 27 7 16 23 6 52

    *FESV, cognitive restructuring subscale.

    yFESV, mental distraction subscale.

    zPain Acceptance Questionnaire.

    xDifficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale, subscale Nonacceptance.

    {Acceptance and Acton Questionnaire.

    308 The Journal of Pain Strategies for Acute Pain

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    5/11

    tolerance (P< .05). Pain tolerance at posttest did notdiffer between the acceptance and distraction condi-tions (P= .058) or between the distraction and cognitiverestructuring conditions (P= .692) when controlling for

    pretest scores (seeFig 1). In sum, instructions to acceptthoughts and feelings related to painful stimuli pro-longed the tolerance of these stimuli relative to an in-struction to cognitively restructure pain-relatedthoughts and feelings. In addition, there was a trendfor acceptance to be superior to distraction in increas-ing tolerance for thermal stimuli.

    The effect of the covariate pretest was also significant(F[1, 99] = 316.7,P< .01). This finding indicates that pre-test and posttest data were highly correlated.

    In addition to the main effects for instruction condi-tion and for pretest, credibility of instructions was signif-icantly related to posttest scores (F[1, 99] = 10.49, P< .01).

    The interaction between credibility and instruction con-dition was nonsignificant.

    A post hoc analysis revealed a significant partial corre-lation between credibility and pain tolerance at posttest(r = .25,P< .05), when controlling for pain tolerance atpretest.

    Pain IntensityTable 4shows means and standard deviations for pain

    intensity (VAS) at pretest and posttest for the 3 instruc-tion conditions. Examination of means indicated thatpain intensity increased during posttest compared topretest scores.

    Controlling for pretest scores, the ANCOVA showeda significant effect for instruction condition on pain in-

    tensity from pretest to posttest (F[2, 103] = 3.97,P< .05,partial Eta2 = .073). Post hoc pairwise comparisons indi-cated significant differences between acceptance anddistraction (P < .01), favoring distraction. In other

    words, participants who received the distraction in-structions had lower pain ratings compared to partici-pants who were instructed to accept painful thoughtsand feelings. No significant differences in posttestpain intensity were found between the acceptanceand cognitive restructuring conditions (P = .098) orthe cognitive restructuring and distraction conditions(P = .244) when controlling for pretest scores (seeFig 2).

    Moreover, the effect of the covariate pretest was sig-nificant (F[1, 103] = 155.26, P< .01), indicating that pre-test and posttest data were highly correlated.

    Credibility of instructions was not significantly re-

    lated to posttest pain intensity (F[1, 102] = .013,P= .908).

    DiscussionThe present study investigated the effects of differ-

    ent short-term pain regulation strategies (acceptance,distraction, and cognitive restructuring) on experimen-tally induced heat pain tolerance and pain intensity.We found that acceptance led to increased pain toler-ance relative to cognitive restructuring. Distraction ledto lower pain intensity compared to acceptance.

    Table 2. Sample Characteristics

    ACCEPTANCE(N= 38) DISTRACTION(N= 36) C OGNITIVERESTRUCTURING(N= 35) F VALUE*

    Age (mean, SD) 22.53 (2.28) 21.39 (1.83) 22.43 (2.84) 2.624

    BDI (mean, SD) 8.71 (6.79) 11.39 (8.43) 9.31 (8.77) 1.123

    PSQ (mean, SD) 3.40 (1.18) 3.35 (.92) 3.25 (1.05) .190

    PCS (mean, SD) 24.63 (7.87) 24.39 (8.72) 22.4 (10.37) .66

    Room temperature (mean, SD) 24.36 (3.84) 23.72 (3.52) 24 (4.04) .258

    Habitual acceptance of painy(mean, SD) 30.61 (5.94) 30.33 (5.45) 29.57 (5.19) .336

    FESV-cognitive restructuring (mean, SD) 12.05 (3.35) 13.31 (3.99) 12.6 (3.45) 1.12

    FESV-mental distraction (mean, SD) 15.18 (4.81) 13.33 (5.09) 14.09 (3.58) 1.549

    Pain thresholdz(mean, SD) 44.13 (3.24) 43.50 (2.68) 43.87 (2.73) .437

    *All F values are nonsignificant (P> .05).

    yPain Acceptance Questionnaire.

    zPain threshold was assessed after posttest.

    Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations forPretest and Posttest for Pain Tolerance

    PAINTOLERANCE(C)

    ACCEPTANCE

    (N= 37)

    DISTRACTION

    (N= 34)

    COGNITIVE

    RESTRUCTURING

    (N= 34)

    Pretest 44.91 (3.02) 44.79 (2.55) 44.46 (2.54)

    Posttest 47.38 (2.14) 46.97 (1.61) 46.63 (2.22)

    Estimated marginal

    means*

    47.34 (.17)y 46.87 (.18) 46.77 (.17)

    *Standard errors in parentheses.

    yF(2, 99) = 3.2, P< .05, favoring acceptance over cognitive restructuring.Figure 1. Estimated marginal means and standard errors forpain tolerance (assessed in the range between 32 and 50C).

    Kohl, Rief, and Glombiewski The Journal of Pain 309

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    6/11

    Pain ToleranceTolerance time went up for all participants across the

    instructional sets. We found that acceptance was more

    effective at increasing pain tolerance than was cognitiverestructuring, with a small effect size. Previous studieshave also shown that acceptance is especially effectivewhen participants perceive high discomfort during ex-perimental pain procedures.13,30 For instance, a priorexperimental study of food cravings found thatacceptance was only superior to cognitive restructuringamong participants reporting a high susceptibility tofood.9

    We considered the possibility that the ineffectivenessof cognitive restructuring might have been due to an in-

    sufficient level of pain catastrophizing in healthy volun-teers. Interestingly, our results indicate that the meanvalue of pain catastrophizing was higher than in anotherstudy with low back pain patients.27

    Nevertheless,although participants endorsed relatively

    high levels of pain catastrophizing in a questionnaire, theexperimental pain might have not been sufficient to pro-voke catastrophizing in this particular situation.

    Our results contrast with those of previous studies ofcoping strategies for anxiety and anger.17,35 However,comparisons between anxiety or anger and paintolerance are limited, because pain tolerance is

    a behavior and anxiety and anger are experiences.Differences between the present and prior results may

    have occurred due to the use of different outcome mea-

    sures or because the instructions differed in content. Inaddition, some studies have suggested that acceptancestrategies may have the greatest impact when the strat-egy is embedded in a values-based context3,30 or whenacceptance instructions involve metaphors andexercises,26 such as the present studys use of the exampleof thoughts as clouds in the sky.

    No significant differences were detected between dis-traction and acceptance with respect to pain tolerance.Our results are in line with those of a previous studythat also failed to find significant differences betweenacceptance and distraction.19 Our results run counter tothe results of another study that found that acceptancewas superior to distraction at increasing pain toler-ance.13 Our findings are also consistent with the resultsof a recent meta-analysis indicating that acceptancestrategies appear to be more effective than other strate-gies at increasing pain tolerance.20 When acceptancewas compared to control groups that are consideredless powerful, such as suppression,24,30 or to conditions

    that did not involve 1 specific strategy, significantdifferences were more easily detected.3,15,32

    With respect to pain tolerance, we can draw the pre-liminary conclusion that cognitive restructuring is no dif-ferent from distraction in increasing pain tolerance.

    Interestingly, we found that credibility had an effecton the extent of pain tolerance, with higher credibilityof instructions accompanied by greater tolerance forpainful stimuli.

    To summarize, acceptance strategies can change theway of responding to pain-related thoughts and feelingsand offer a broader scope of behavior. Cognitive restruc-

    turing does not appear to change pain-related behavior

    as much as acceptance. Nevertheless, it can be an impor-tant tool for other clinically relevant experiences, ie, anx-iety or anger.

    Pain IntensityIn line with a previous study,13 distraction proved to be

    more effective than acceptance with respect to pain in-tensity ratings, with a small effect size. Because accep-tance strategies do not seek to alter inner experiences,it is reasonable that an acceptance strategy did not im-pact a sensory nociception. Previous studies comparingthe effects of acceptance versus distraction or suppres-sion on pain intensity have yielded mixed find-

    ings.3,19,26,30,31

    Furthermore, a meta-analysis found nosignificant differences in the effects of acceptanceandother pain regulation strategies on pain intensity.20 Di-vergent results may have emerged due to different oper-ationalizations of constructs in the instructions or type of

    comparison condition. Distraction was not more effec-tive than cognitive restructuring at regulating pain in-tensity in the present study.

    In line with the results of a previous study, pain inten-sity increased from pretest to posttest across all groups.32

    It is possible that the skin surface became sensitized toadditional exposure to warm stimuli, such that partici-pants perceived stimulation as more painful at posttest.

    It is not surprising that the acceptance group had thehighest pain ratings, because participants in this group

    Table 4. Means and Standard Deviations forPretest and Posttest for Pain Intensity

    PAININTENSITY(VAS)

    ACCEPTANCE

    (N= 38)

    DISTRACTION

    (N= 36)

    COGNITIVE

    RESTRUCTURING

    (N= 35)

    Pretest 68.63 (15.62) 60.39 (16.18) 59.80 (16.59)

    Posttest 78.39 (14.90) 65.33 (16.82) 67.34 (17.32)

    Estimated marginal

    means*

    74.33 (1.75) 67.16 (1.78)y 69.88 (1.79)

    *Standard errors in parentheses.

    yF(2, 102) = 4.01, P< .05 favoring distraction over acceptance.

    40

    45

    50

    55

    60

    65

    70

    75

    80

    AcceptanceN=38

    DistractionN=36

    CognitiveRestructuring

    N=35

    Pain

    intensity

    (VAS)

    *F(2, 102) = 4.01;partial Eta2=0.073

    Figure 2. Estimated marginal means and standard errors forpain intensity assessed by the VAS ranging from no pain = 0to worst imaginable pain = 100.

    310 The Journal of Pain Strategies for Acute Pain

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    7/11

    tolerated stimuli for a longer period of time than par-ticipants in the other 2 groups. We suggest that pain in-tensity and pain tolerance are based on a commonlatent factor, and therefore we decided not to covaryeither pain tolerance or pain intensity in analyses ofthe other.

    Advantages of This StudyThis is the first study to test the effects of different cop-ing instructions within a highly standardized thermalpain induction procedure. Previous studies have notcompared acceptance strategies with cognitive restruc-turing in the context of experimentally induced pain.The sample size in our study was larger than in most ofthe other studies and was adequately powered to detectsignificant differences. Moreover, we assessed potentialpreexisting differences in habitual coping strategiesand tested the credibility of our manipulation and thedegree of adherence to instructions. Results highlightthe importance of experimental research investigating

    mechanisms of therapeutic interventions. The presentstudy indicated that differences emerge between short-term strategies, and that advantages of simple andunique strategies should be better disentangled.

    LimitationsThe generalizability of our results to males might be

    limited because of gender-specific effects on pain thresh-old andpain tolerance5,19,34;inaddition,resultsmightnotgeneralize to other age cohorts due to the restricted agerange. Generalizability to chronic pain patients may alsobe limited. Pain stimuli might have a different meaningor affective valence for chronic pain patients than for

    a sample of students. It is unclear to what extentexperimentally induced pain is comparable to chronicpain conditions. Nevertheless, conducting a study withhealthy participants to detect patterns in pain behaviorand pain sensations independently of chronic conditionsrepresents an important initial step.

    Because participants level of catastrophizing was rela-

    tively high, the cognitive restructuring instruction mighthave been insufficient to significantly reduce the level ofcatastrophizing. Cognitive restructuring is a very com-plex strategy that requires practice. The instructionswithin the present study lacked some elements of cogni-tive restructuring such as rational rebuttal to automatic

    thoughts. On the other hand, experimental studieswith even shorter reappraisal instructions have shownbeneficial effects.17,35

    Furthermore, distraction was associated with greateradherence to instruction than cognitive restructuring

    or acceptance. Distraction may be the most commonlyknown strategy for coping with pain. Therefore, peoplemight find it easier to apply such a strategy. This pointadds weight to the argument that cognitive restructur-ing and acceptance might require more training to opti-mize effectiveness.

    One might argue that the lack of a control condition isanother limitation. However, several studies have al-

    ready demonstrated the effectiveness of strategies suchas distraction and acceptance in comparison to a control

    group.1,20 In line with the rationale for randomizedcontrolled trials, which allows the comparison of newtreatments to established ones without a controlgroup, we decided not to include a control group.

    Another limitation of our study is that the proceduredid not allow all participants to reach the limit of theirpain tolerance, because the thermode stops at 50C for

    ethical reasons. However, we were able to includemore than 95% of participants in analyses for heat paintolerance.

    Some research suggests that previous knowledgeabout a specific coping strategy might influence the ad-equate application of this strategy.6,9,21,23 It should benoted that our study is not a comparison between ACTand CBT, because we tested only isolated strategiesfrom a larger toolbox for both treatment types.

    Clinical ImplicationsAlthough it is difficult to translate the present results

    into clear clinical recommendations for chronic pain pa-

    tients, we know that the willingness to tolerate painfor longer periods of time is crucial for chronic pain pa-tients. Attempting to avoid pain results in disability, re-duced participation in daily life activities, and lowerfunctioning. Furthermore, chronic pain patients sufferenormously from pain and therefore may benefit fromstrategies that lead to reduced pain intensity. Thus, it isuseful to know that distraction may be more helpfulthan acceptance in decreasing the nociceptive sensationof pain, and that cognitive restructuring seemed to be asuseful as distraction. In sum, we recommend applyinga range of strategies for managing chronic pain in orderto influence both pain tolerance and pain intensity. Thefact that credibility of instructions was related to out-come variables demonstrates the importance of present-ing strategies convincingly. This finding suggests that itmight be beneficial to allocate patients to the treat-ments they rate as the most credible.

    Implications for Future ResearchFuture studies should evaluate the usefulness of the

    various coping strategies at modifying a wider range ofclinically relevant outcome variables. Expectanciesmaybe important moderators of treatment outcomes,36 andourfindings indicate the importance of assessing this var-

    iable within experimental and clinical trials, especially intrials of procedures that aim to alter behavior. We recom-mend assessing credibility immediately after the instruc-tions rather than after posttest, because experiencesduring posttest might influence credibility ratings.

    Future research should address whether different in-structions are associated with different effects withinchronic pain populations. Further studies are needed todraw conclusions about long-term effectiveness of thevarious strategies.

    The majority of research in this field is conducted withhealthy participants, such that replicating results inclinical samples is a priority. Furthermore, it is important

    to investigate the circumstances under which particularstrategies are most effective for differenttarget variables.

    Kohl, Rief, and Glombiewski The Journal of Pain 311

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    8/11

    References

    1. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S: Emotion-regu-lation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analyticreview. Clin Psychol Rev 30:217-237, 2010

    2. Bond FW, Hayes SC, Baer RA, Carpenter KC, Guenole N,Or HK, Waltz T, Zettle RD: Preliminary psychometric proper-

    ties of the Acceptance and Action QuestionnaireII: A re-vised measure of psychological flexibility and acceptance.Behav Ther;1-38, 2011

    3. Branstetter-Rost A, Cushing C, Douleh T: Personal valuesandpain tolerance: Does a values intervention add to accep-tance? J Pain 10:887-892, 2009

    4. Ehde DM, Jensen MP: Feasibility of a cognitive restructur-ing intervention for treatment of chronic pain in personswith disabilities. Rehabil Psychol 49:254, 2004

    5. Ellermeier W, Westphal W:Gender differences in pain rat-ings and pupil reactions to painful pressure stimuli. Pain 61:435-439, 1995

    6. Feldner MT, Zvolensky MJ, Eifert GH, Spira AP: Emotionalavoidance: An experimental test of individual differencesand response suppression using biological challenge. BehavRes Ther 41:403-411, 2003

    7. Ferguson CJ: An effect size primer: A guide for cliniciansand researchers. Prof Psychol Res Pr 40:532-538, 2009

    8. Flor H, Turk DC: Chronic back pain and rheumatoid arthri-tis: Predicting pain and disability from cognitive variables. JBehav Med 11:251-265, 1988

    9. Forman EM, Hoffman KL, McGrath KB, Herbert JD,Brandsma LL, Lowe MR: A comparison of acceptance- andcontrol-based strategies for coping with food cravings: Ananalog study. Behav Res Ther 45:2372-2386, 2007

    10. Geissner E: Fragebogen zur Erfassung der Schmerzverar-beitung: FESV. Gottingen, Hogrefe, 2001

    11. Gratz KL, Roemer L: Multidimensional assessment ofemotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, fac-tor structure, and initial validation of thedifficulties in emo-tion regulation scale. Journal Psychopathol Behav Assess 26:41-54, 2004

    12. Greenspan JD, Craft RM, LeResche L, Arendt-Nielsen L,Berkley KJ, Fillingim RB, Gold MS, Holdcroft A,Lautenbacher S, Mayer EA: Studying sex and gender differ-ences in pain and analgesia: A consensus report. Pain 132:S26-S45, 2007

    13. Gutierrez O, Luciano C, Rodrguez M, Fink BC: Compar-ison between an acceptance-based and a cognitive-control-based protocol for coping with pain. Behav Ther35:767-783, 2004

    14. Hautzinger M, Kuehner C, Keller F: Das Beck Depression-sinventar II. Deutsche Bearbeitung und Handbuch zum BDIII. Frankfurt, 2006

    15. Hayes SC, Bissett RT, Korn Z, Zettle RD, Rosenfarb IS,Cooper LD, Grundt AM: The impact of acceptance versuscontrol rationales on pain tolerance. Psychol Rec 49:33-34,1999

    16. Hayes SC, Strohsal KD, Wilson KG: Acceptance and Com-mitment Therapy: An experiential approach to behaviorchange. New York, NY, Guilford Press, 1999

    17. Hofmann SG, Heering S, Sawyer AT, Asnaani A: How tohandle anxiety: The effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and

    suppression strategies on anxious arousal. Behav Res Ther47:389-394, 2009

    18. Keefe FJ: Cognitive behavioral therapy for managingpain. Clin Psychol 49:4-5, 1996

    19. Keogh E, Bond FW, Hanmer R, Tilston J: Comparingacceptance-and control-based coping instructions on thecold-pressor pain experiences of healthy men and women.

    Eur J Pain 9:591-598, 2005

    20. Kohl A, Rief W, Glombiewski JA: How effective areacceptance strategies? A meta-analytic review of experi-mental results. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 43:988-1001,2012

    21. Kuehner C, Huffziger S, Liebsch K: Rumination, distrac-tion and mindful self-focus: Effects on mood, dysfunctionalattitudes and cortisol stress response. Psychol Med 39:219-228, 2009

    22. Kallai I, Barke A, Voss U: The effects of experimentercharacteristics on pain reports in women and men. Pain112:142-147, 2004

    23. Levitt JT, Brown TA, OrsilloSM, Barlow DH: Theeffects ofacceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjectiveand psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide chal-lenge in patients with panic disorder. Behav Ther 35:747-766, 2004

    24. Masedo AI, Rosa Esteve M: Effects of suppression, ac-ceptance and spontaneous coping on pain tolerance,pain intensity and distress. Behav Res Ther 45:199-209,2007

    25. McCracken LM, Vowles KE, Eccleston C: Acceptance ofchronic pain: Component analysis and a revised assessmentmethod. Pain 107:159-166, 2004

    26. McMullen J, Barnes-Holmes D, Barnes-Holmes Y,Stewart I, Luciano C, Cochrane A: Acceptance versus distrac-tion: Brief instructions, metaphors and exercises in increas-ing tolerance for self-delivered electric shocks. Behav ResTher 46:122-129, 2008

    27. Meyer K, Sprott H, Mannion AF: Cross-cultural adapta-tion, reliability, and validity of the German version of thePain Catastrophizing Scale. J Psychosom Res 64:469-478,2008

    28. Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A: Systematic review andmeta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitivebehaviour therapy and behaviour therapy for chronic painin adults, excluding headache. Pain 80:1-13, 1999

    29. NilgesP, KosterB, Schmidt CO:Pain acceptance - conceptand validation of a German version of the chronic pain ac-ceptance questionnaire. Schmerz (Berlin, Germany) 21:57-58, 60-67, 2007

    30. Paez-Blarrina M, Luciano C, Gutierrez-Martnez O,Valdivia S, Ortega J, Rodrguez-Valverde M: The role ofvalues with personal examples in altering the functions ofpain: Comparison between acceptance-based andcognitive-control-based protocols. Behav Res Ther 46:84-97, 2008

    31. Paez-Blarrina M, Luciano C, Gutierrez-Martnez O,Valdivia S, Rodrguez-Valverde M, Ortega J: Coping withpain in the motivational context of values: Comparison be-tween an acceptance-based and a cognitive controlbasedprotocol. Behav Modif 32:403, 2008

    32. Roche B, Forsyth JP, Maher E: The impact of demandcharacteristics on brief acceptance-and control-based

    312 The Journal of Pain Strategies for Acute Pain

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    9/11

    interventions for pain tolerance. Cogn Behav Pract 14:381-393, 2007

    33. Ruscheweyh R, Marziniak M, Stumpenhorst F,Reinholz J, Knecht S: Pain sensitivity can be assessed byself-rating: Development and validation of the Pain Sensitiv-ity Questionnaire. Pain 146:65-74, 2009

    34. Sullivan MJL, Tripp DA, Santor D: Gender differences in

    pain and pain behavior: The role of catastrophizing. CognitTher Res 24:121-134, 2000

    35. Szasz PL,Szentagotai A, Hofmann SG: Theeffect of emo-tion regulation strategies on anger. Behav Res Ther 49:114-119, 2011

    36. Vlaeyen JWS, Morley S: Cognitive-behavioral treatmentsfor chronic pain: What works for whom? Clin J Pain 21:1,2005

    37. Vowles KE, McCracken LM: Acceptance and values-based action in chronic pain: A study of treatment effec-

    tiveness and process. J Consult Clin Psychol 76:397-407,2008

    38. Vowles KE,McNeil DW, Gross RT, McDaniel ML, Mouse A,Bates M, Gallimore P, McCall C: Effects of pain acceptanceand pain control strategies on physical impairment inindividuals with chronic low back pain. Behav Ther 38:412-425, 2007

    39. Wetherell JL, Afari N, Rutledge T, Sorrell JT,Stoddard JA, Petkus AJ, Solomon BC, Lehman DH, Liu L,Lang AJ, Atkinson JH: A randomized, controlled trial ofacceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy for chronic pain. Pain 152:2098-2107,2011

    40. Wicksell RK, Melin L, Lekander M, Olsson GL: Evaluatingthe effectiveness of exposure and acceptance strategies toimprove functioning and quality of life in longstanding pe-diatric painA randomized controlled trial. Pain 141:248-257, 2009

    Kohl, Rief, and Glombiewski The Journal of Pain 313

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    10/11

    Appendix

    Complete Instructions1. Acceptance: Please list three different thoughts

    which urged you to stop the painful stimulus.(break of 1 minute)

    Now I would like you to put down your pencil.

    (short break)Sometimes our thoughts control our behavior. Howeverthis connection between thoughts and actions can beloosened. Sometimes it is even very important to ignoreour thoughts if we want to achieve our goals. It isa good strategyto merely perceive your thoughts without

    judging or changing them. You may think of thoughtslikeclouds in the sky. If you do not regard your thoughts as tobe of importance but just observe them, they will pass bylike clouds in the sky. The acceptance of unpleasantthoughts mayleadto different actions thanyour thoughtswant you to act. Thus your thoughts do not control yourlife because they are not the cause of your behavior, and

    because it is important to pursue certain goals.Example: Imagine one of your last visits to the dentist.

    Maybe at one of those visits you had to experience pain.However you have accepted the pain because you pur-sued the goal to preserve your teeth as long as possible.Maybe there have been thoughts which urged you tostop the treatment. And maybe you have not concededthem spontaneously but just noticed and acceptedthem, and finally let them go. Thus you just kept sittingthere and endured the pain.

    Exercise: Please read again through your thoughtswhich urged you to stop the stimulus. Close your eyesand imagine the thermode is causing you pain again.

    Try to perceive these thoughts neutrally and let thempass by like clouds in the sky. Please note that you areable to act independently of your thoughts if you merelyperceive your thoughts without any judgments. (breakof 1 minute)

    Specific instruction: Now I would like you to openyour eyes. Please try to increase the duration of thehot and cold stimulus by accepting all your thoughtsand sensations. Let go of the thoughts you had in thebeginning which urged you to stop the stimulus. Beready to experience the pain without any attempt tochange them.

    2. Distraction: Please list three different thoughts

    which urged you to stop the painful stimulus.(break of 1 minute)

    Now I would like you to put down your pencil.(short break)

    If you try to distract yourselves from these thoughtsyou might perceive them less intense than before. Some-times you have to distract yourself from disturbing influ-ences in order to achieve your goals. Our attention workslike a spotlight. Depending on which thoughts and feel-ings come into focus, other thoughts and feelings areblanked out. Thus we are able to shift our attention toother aspects to avoid unpleasant thoughts and feelings.On the one hand you are able to distract yourself inter-

    nally, for example by recalling a pleasant situation. Onthe other hand you are able to distract yourself exter-

    nally, for example by concentrating on your environ-ment.

    Example: Imagine one of your last visits to the dentist.Maybe at one of those visits you had to experience pain.To endurethis pain, youpossibly concentratedon a paint-ing on the wall. Perhaps you even tried to look at thatpainting very closely in order to use your attention to ca-

    pacity. And maybe in your thoughts you planned the restof the day and were thinking about the things you can doafter your visit at the dentist. By means of the distractionyou were able to cope with the treatment.

    Exercise: Please read again through your thoughtswhich urged you to stop the stimulus. Close your eyesand imagine the thermode is causing you pain again.Imagine you are lying on a lawn on a warm summerday. You are watching other people who are lying onthe lawn as well, looking at the clouds und listening tothe singing birds. Try to imagine this scene and distractyourself from the thoughts which have urged you tostop the stimulus.(break of 1 minute)

    Specific Instruction: Now I would likeyou to open youreyes. Please try to increase the duration of the hot andcold stimulus by distracting yourself from all yourthoughts and feelings. Imagine the scene where youare lying on a lawn. Thus you may distract yourselffrom the pain and the thoughts which urge you to stopthe stimulus.

    3. Cognitive restructuring: Please list three differentthoughts which urged you to stop the painful stim-ulus.(break of 1 minute)Now I would like you to put down your pencil.(short break)

    Sometimes it helps to view a situation from a different

    angle and to develop another point of view. Dependingon how we judge a certain situation, we show differentfeelings and behavior tendencies. On the one hand neg-ative thoughts and appraisals may cause dejection anduncertainty, and may influence bodily sensations ina negative way. On the other hand positive thoughtsmay cause pleasant bodily sensations. Thus the same sit-uation can lead to completely different appraisals. Theway you feel and the way your body responds dependson how you judge the situation. Unpleasant thoughts

    can be replaced by helpful thoughts. As a consequenceyou act differently depending on your point of view.

    Example: Imagine one of your last visits to the dentist.

    Maybe at one of those visits you had to experience pain.And maybe you viewed the pain from another point ofview: For example, you were thinking that you have toendure just a little pain in order to avoid much worsepain in the future, and that the dentist knows what heis doing. Or perhaps you took the pain as a challengeand were thinking that you can be proud of yourself ifyou master this challenge.

    Exercise: Please read again through your thoughtswhich urged you to stop the stimulus. Close your eyesand imagine the thermode is causing you pain again.Now you may develop alternative points of views. For ex-ample you may think something like I have already

    overcome worse pain, or this painful experience lastsjust a short period of time, compared to all the things I

    314 The Journal of Pain Strategies for Acute Pain

  • 8/13/2019 1-s2.0-S1526590012009637-main

    11/11

    will be doing afterwards, or this pain is unpleasant butdoes not indicate any serious injury. Maybe you comeup with even more revaluations.(break of 1 minute)

    Specific Instruction: Now I would like you to open youreyes. Please try to increase the duration of the hot and

    cold stimulus by developing another point of view ofthe pain. Please restructure your thoughts to alternativeand helpful thoughts which will make it easier for you toendure the pain. You may use the thoughts you just de-veloped.

    Kohl, Rief, and Glombiewski The Journal of Pain 315