26.full

6
http://cdp.sagepub.com/ Science Current Directions in Psychological http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/17/1/26 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00542.x 2008 17: 26 Current Directions in Psychological Science Malcolm D. MacLeod and Jo Saunders Retrieval Inhibition and Memory Distortion : Negative Consequences of an Adaptive Process Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Association for Psychological Science can be found at: Current Directions in Psychological Science Additional services and information for http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: What is This? - Feb 1, 2008 Version of Record >> at Ruppin Institute on January 26, 2013 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Upload: asafbend

Post on 11-Nov-2015

214 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

DESCRIPTION

Retrieval Inhibition and MemoryDistortion

TRANSCRIPT

  • http://cdp.sagepub.com/Science

    Current Directions in Psychological

    http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/17/1/26The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00542.x 2008 17: 26Current Directions in Psychological Science

    Malcolm D. MacLeod and Jo SaundersRetrieval Inhibition and Memory Distortion : Negative Consequences of an Adaptive Process

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    Association for Psychological Science

    can be found at:Current Directions in Psychological ScienceAdditional services and information for

    http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    What is This?

    - Feb 1, 2008Version of Record >>

    at Ruppin Institute on January 26, 2013cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Retrieval Inhibition and MemoryDistortionNegative Consequences of an Adaptive ProcessMalcolm D. MacLeod1 and Jo Saunders2

    1University of St. Andrews, Fife, Scotland, and 2University of Wales Swansea, Swansea, Wales

    ABSTRACTDespite the fact that misinformation effects

    have long been studied by both applied researchers and

    modelers of human memory, there is little consensus as to

    the value of such endeavors. We argue that this may be due

    to a failure to identify the underlying mechanism respon-

    sible for such memory distortions. We consider novel

    evidence for a relationship between retrieval-induced for-

    getting and the reporting of misinformation. We also ex-

    plore the extent towhich retrieval inhibition underpins this

    relationship and the implications this has for the modeling

    of memory and nding potential solutions to real-world

    problems.

    KEYWORDSmisinformation effect; eyewitnessing; retriev-

    al-induced forgetting; retrieval inhibition

    Little in our social world remains constant for long; relationships

    change, interests develop, and people move on. In order to meet

    the myriad challenges that accompany such changes, we rely

    heavily upon our portable store of socially relevant informa-

    tionour memory. For the most part, we encounter few dif-

    culties in our ability to retrieve target information, but the

    apparent ease with which this is accomplished belies its com-

    plexity. Our social world is in a constant state of ux and, as

    a result, memory needs to be continually updated; otherwise its

    functional utility would soon be drawn into question. But there

    lies the problem: How is it possible to access the information we

    need when memory is being continually updated with related

    material?

    On rst inspection, the most obvious solution to this updating

    problem might be to replace or substitute redundant material

    with information that is current. Indeed, such a mechanism

    would probably prove more than adequate if memory consisted

    solely of telephone numbers or addresses. However, the material

    stored in memory is innitely more rich and varied. In addition,

    we have no way of labeling particular memories as being

    potentially useful or obsolete; the material deemed redundant

    for todays task may ultimately prove critical for the completion

    of some future task. Thus, there is a need to retain older, po-

    tentially useful material at the same time as storing current or

    new information.

    RETRIEVAL COMPETITION

    Continually adding material to other related memories, however,

    creates a tricky problem for effective information retrieval. Our

    memories are highly interrelated in terms of their semantic

    and/or episodic content. Yet, the cues used to access particular

    memories tend not to be well specied. Thus, we access not only

    the information we wish to retrieve but also other related ma-

    terial that, in turn, represents an unwanted source of competi-

    tion. If this went unchecked, it could mean the difference

    between retrieval success and failure. So how do we deal with

    unwanted retrieval competition?

    A possible solution has emerged from studies concerned with

    retrieval-induced forgettingthe forgetting of material as a re-

    sult of remembering other related material (Anderson, Bjork, &

    Bjork, 1994; Anderson & Spellman, 1995). The basic argument

    is that, by forgetting related material, it becomes possible to

    promote the retrieval of target information via a reduction in

    retrieval competition. On rst inspection, the notion that

    forgetting promotes remembering seems counterintuitive, but it

    makes perfect sense if forgetting is targeted at specic related

    memories that might otherwise compete for retrieval.

    The mechanism responsible for this kind of forgetting is

    considered to be retrieval inhibition (see Anderson & Spellman,

    1995; Saunders & MacLeod, 2006), and it has been established

    in a variety of contexts including memory for facts (Anderson &

    Bell, 2001), exam performance (Macrae & MacLeod, 1999),

    person perception (MacLeod & Macrae, 2001), and eyewitness

    Address correspondence to Malcolm D. MacLeod, Social and AppliedCognition Lab, School of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, Fife,Scotland KY16 9JP, United Kingdom; e-mail: [email protected].

    CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

    26 Volume 17Number 1Copyrightr 2008 Association for Psychological Science at Ruppin Institute on January 26, 2013cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • memory (MacLeod, 2002; Shaw, Bjork, & Handal, 1995).

    Retrieval inhibition is a form of forgetting whereby unwanted

    memories are actively inhibited or suppressed to the extent that

    they are no longer available to conscious inspection (Anderson

    et al., 1994; Anderson & Spellman, 1995)which is quite

    different from traditional conceptions of forgetting such as trace

    decay (i.e., the gradual deterioration of the memory trace over

    time) or associative interference (i.e., the weakening of asso-

    ciative links between particular retrieval cues and memories via

    the strengthening of other related associative links).

    MEMORY DISTORTION

    Most recently, our studies have sought to explore the possibility

    that retrieval inhibition may also underlie misinformation ef-

    fects. Misinformation effects have traditionally been explored

    using the misinformation paradigm (cf. Loftus, Miller, & Burns,

    1978). After viewing a target event (e.g., a carpedestrian

    accident), subjects are typically presented with a narrative or

    questionnaire that includes some misleading information about

    the event (e.g., suggesting that the car in question was green

    when in fact it had been blue). In this setup, misled subjects

    reliably choose the misinformation item in preference to the

    original in a subsequent test of memory, whereas nonmisled

    subjects tend to choose the original information.

    Explanations for this kind of memory distortion range from

    the operation of a destructive updating process whereby older

    memories are erased by newer material (e.g., Loftus et al., 1978)

    to the inadequate monitoring of memory sources at encoding

    (e.g., Lindsay & Johnson, 1989). Others have argued that the

    absence of effective retrieval cues during the nal test reduces

    retrieval accessibility for the original material relative to more

    recent misinformation (e.g., Bekerian & Bowers, 1983). Some

    have even argued that such studies reveal less about how human

    memory works than about biases inherent within the paradigm

    (e.g., McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1985).

    We considered an alternative: namely, the possibility that the

    apparent lack of clarity on this issue may be symptomatic of a

    failure to identify the basic cognitive mechanism underlying

    the misinformation effect. In doing so, we explored the possi-

    bility that retrieval-induced forgetting may be such a mecha-

    nism. We also explored the possibility that an increased

    susceptibility to suggestion may represent the cost of an other-

    wise adaptive inhibitory mechanism that serves to facilitate the

    retrieval of target memories.

    RETRIEVAL-INDUCED FORGETTING

    Retrieval-induced forgetting has been explored via the retrieval-

    practice paradigm (cf. Anderson et al., 1994), in which subjects

    are typically shown sets of categoryexemplar pairs (e.g., tree

    sycamore, treespruce . . . shsalmon, shhaddock . . . sport

    tennis, sportfootball . . . fruitbanana, fruitpear . . .). After

    initial presentation, subjects are given selective retrieval

    practice (usually three repetitions) on a subset of items from a

    subset of categories using cued stem tests (e.g., treesy____;

    shsa____). Following a distracter task that lasts up to 20

    minutes, subjects are asked to recall all the exemplars originally

    presented.

    Consistent with expectations, a facilitation effect is evident for

    practiced items frompracticed sets (designatedRp1 items); that

    is, Rp1 items (e.g., sycamore, salmon) are better remembered

    than either unpracticed items from the practiced sets (Rpitems; e.g., spruce, haddock) or unpracticed items from the

    unpracticed sets (Nrp items; e.g., sport and fruit exemplars).

    What is much less intuitive, however, is that Rp items tendto be more poorly recalled than Nrp items (see Fig. 1). Thus,

    despite the fact that neither Rp items nor Nrp items receiveany retrieval practice, Rp items are more poorly recalled thanNrp items are.

    This pattern of recall performance is consistent with an in-

    hibitory account whereby Rp items are actively suppressed orinhibited in order to minimise unwanted competition during the

    repeated retrieval of Rp1 items. Nrp items, in contrast, tend to

    be better remembered than are Rp items because they do notrepresent a source of retrieval competition and therefore there is

    no need for inhibitory control (see Anderson et al., 1994).

    RETRIEVAL-INDUCED FORGETTING AND THE

    MISINFORMATION EFFECT

    The selective-retrieval procedure is critical to the emergence of

    retrieval-induced forgetting. Where subsets of exemplars are

    0

    0.1

    0.2

    0.3

    0.4

    0.5

    0.6

    0.7

    0.8

    Rp+ Rp NrpItem type

    *

    * = retrieval-induced forgetting

    Prop

    ortio

    n re

    call

    Fig. 1. Retrieval-induced forgetting of nonpracticed items from prac-ticed categories. Rp15 practiced items from practiced category; Rp5unpracticed items from practiced category; Nrp5 unpracticed items fromunpracticed category. Despite Rp and Nrp items being treated in thesame manner (i.e., neither item type receives retrieval practice), Rpitems tend to be more poorly recalled than Nrp items. FromMacLeod andMacrae (2001, Study 1).

    Volume 17Number 1 27

    Malcolm D. MacLeod and Jo Saunders

    at Ruppin Institute on January 26, 2013cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • simply re-presented, forgetting effects for Rp items fail toemerge (Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 2000). Given this pattern

    of forgetting, our basic premise is that the postevent question-

    naire typically used to introduce misleading information in the

    misinformation paradigm is also likely to give rise to the same

    kinds of retrieval conditions known to elicit retrieval inhibition.

    By giving subjects a set of questions about a target event as a

    means of introducing misleading material, conditions are ripe

    for the selective retrieval of information (Saunders & MacLeod,

    2002). The misinformation paradigm therefore may unwittingly

    incorporate a selective-retrieval-practice procedure not unlike

    that found in the retrieval-practice paradigm. It is important to

    note, however, that we are not arguing that the misinformation

    effect is an artifact of the paradigmfar from it. Rather, we

    are saying that the retrieval conditions created by the postevent

    questionnaire are very similar to those that exist in the real

    world (i.e., the selective retrieval of information) and that such

    conditions, in turn, are likely to elicit retrieval inhibition. Given

    this, are misinformation effects more likely to emerge where the

    original target items have been subject to retrieval-induced

    forgetting?

    To explore this possibility, we adapted the standard retrieval-

    practice paradigm in order to incorporate key phases from the

    misinformation paradigm. Specically, after reading two narra-

    tives containing information about a series of stolen household

    items, subjects engaged in selected retrieval practice for items

    about one of the burglaries. This took the form of questions

    concerning half of the stolen items from one of the houses.

    Subjects were prompted to retrieve each item three times. A

    manipulation check to measure the presence of retrieval-

    induced forgetting followed this retrieval-practice procedure:

    Subjects were rst asked to recall all the stolen items from both

    houses and then were asked additional questions regarding

    scene-setting information contained within the original narra-

    tives. Included in one of these questions was a piece of misin-

    formation about either an Rp1 item (i.e., a practiced item from

    the practiced burglary), an Rp item (i.e., a nonpracticed itemfrom the practiced burglary), or an Nrp item (i.e., a nonpracticed

    item from the nonpracticed burglary). Finally, memory for both

    events, including themisleading item, was tested using a forced-

    choice recognition task that required subjects to choose between

    the original item and two novel items for noncritical items, and

    between the original, a misleading item, or a novel item for the

    misinformation item.

    Using this paradigm, we found that misinformation effects

    occurred only where misleading information had been intro-

    duced about items that were subject to retrieval-induced for-

    getting; that is, memory distortions occurred only for non-

    practiced items that were in the same set as practiced items (i.e.,

    Rp items). When misinformation had been introduced abouteither Rp1 or Nrp items, the level of misinformation reported at

    the nal test was no different from that for controls who had

    received no relevant retrieval practice (see Fig. 2).

    If retrieval-induced forgetting plays such a pivotal role in the

    production of misinformation effects, these memorial errors

    should emerge only where retrieval-induced forgetting remains

    demonstrably active. In a test of the boundary conditions asso-

    ciated with retrieval-induced forgetting, MacLeod and Macrae

    (2001) had established that the timing of the retrieval-practice

    procedure is critical. Specically, they found that a delay of

    24 hours between retrieval practice and nal test resulted in the

    dissipation of retrieval-induced forgetting (i.e., recall perfor-

    mance for Rp items had recovered), whereas Rp perfor-mance remained impaired following a 24-hour delay between

    original presentation and retrieval practice (i.e., retrieval-

    induced forgetting was still evident). Our studies not only con-

    rmed MacLeod and Macraes earlier ndings but also showed

    that the interval between the selective-retrieval-practice pro-

    cedure and the introduction of misleading material was critical

    in producing misinformation effects (Saunders & MacLeod,

    2002).

    IS IT INHIBITION?

    Having established a link between retrieval-induced forgetting

    and the production of misinformation effects, there remains

    the thorny issue of whether the mechanism is truly inhibitory or

    not. In order to explore this, we devised a set of studies that

    incorporated the independent-cue technique rst devised by

    Anderson and Spellman (1995). This technique involves

    prompting subjects with novel cues at test, rather than using the

    0

    0.1

    0.2

    0.3

    0.4

    0.5

    0.6

    0.7

    Prop

    ortio

    n of

    mis

    info

    rmat

    ion

    effe

    cts

    MisRp+ MisNrp MisControl MisRpCondition

    Fig. 2. Effects of introducing postevent misleading information followingselective-retrieval practice. When misleading postevent information isintroduced concerning an item subject to retrieval-induced forgetting(MisRpcondition), the majority of subjects (M 5 .60) choose the mis-leading item on a forced-choice recognition test. When misinformation isintroduced about items not subjected to retrieval-induced forgetting (i.e.,practiced items from practiced categories, MisRp1 condition, and un-practiced items from unpracticed categories, MisNrp condition), the levelof misinformation reported is no different from that of controls where norelevant retrieval practice has taken place (i.e., MisControl condition).From Saunders and MacLeod (2002, Study 1).

    28 Volume 17Number 1

    Retrieval Inhibition and Memory Distortion

    at Ruppin Institute on January 26, 2013cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • cues originally linked to exemplars during the retrieval-practice

    phase of the retrieval-practice paradigm.

    The logic behind this modication is this: If retrieval-induced

    forgetting is due to noninhibitory means (such as associative

    interference or blocking, which affect the utility of the retrieval

    route and cue), the presentation of novel cues at test should

    prove sufcient to circumvent any such interference. Thus, as

    Anderson and colleagues have argued (Anderson et al., 1994;

    Anderson & Spellman, 1995), if Rp items are simply blockedrather than inhibited, the use of novel cues at test should ensure

    that Rp items are as well remembered as are Nrp items. But ifRp items are inhibited, they should remain unavailable toconscious inspection and should be more poorly recalled, de-

    spite the use of novel cues at test (see Fig. 3). Recently, Storm,

    Bjork, Bjork, and Nestojko (2006), using an impossible re-

    trieval practice condition, found that the effort to retrieve Rp1

    items even when no actual Rp1 items could be recalled still

    produced retrieval-induced forgetting of Rp itemsa resultthat strongly argues against a blocking explanation.

    Consistent with Andersons thesis on inhibitory control, we

    found that Rp items remained below Nrp baseline perfor-mance despite the use of such novel cues at test (MacLeod &

    Saunders, 2005; Saunders &MacLeod, 2006). We also surmised

    that, if such effects are due to retrieval inhibition, Nrp items that

    are semantically related to either Rp1 or Rp items should alsobe susceptible to the introduction of misleading information. We

    conrmed that misinformation effects emerged where misinfor-

    mation had been introduced about items that had been subject to

    rst-order inhibition (i.e., Rp items), cross-category inhibition(i.e., Nrp items related to Rp1 items), and even second-order

    inhibition (i.e., Nrp items related to Rp items). In contrast, Nrpitems that were not semantically related to items from the

    practiced category (i.e., unrelated to either Rp1 or Rp items)were susceptible neither to inhibition nor to misinformation.

    CONCLUSIONS

    The current studies suggest that some memory distortions may

    be a function of a normally adaptive inhibitory mechanism that

    is activated in response to unwanted competition from related

    memories. This kind of retrieval-induced forgetting creates

    conditions wherein misleading postevent information can un-

    wittingly be reported instead of original material. By exploring

    the inhibitory mechanisms underlying this relationship, it be-

    comes possible to predict when such memory distortions are

    likely to occur.

    Our research points to two potentially important principles in

    interviewing witnesses. The rst is that the initial search for

    information about a witnessed event should be as exhaustive as

    possibleotherwise conditions that lead to the selective re-

    trieval of information may be created, which, in turn, give rise to

    retrieval inhibition for items that were not probed. The second is

    that sufcient time should elapse between initial information

    retrieval and any nal statement so that, should retrieval-in-

    duced forgetting have occurred during the selective retrieval of

    information, its effects will have had time to dissipate. In this

    way, it may be possible to minimize the risk of witness suscep-

    tibility to postevent suggestion.

    Finally, although our research is consistent with an inhibitory

    account of retrieval-induced forgetting, we are aware that there

    is still much to be done to establish how such inhibitory mech-

    anisms actually work. In particular, we need to understand the

    levels at which inhibitory control operates (see Saunders &

    MacLeod, 2006). For example, does inhibition operate at the

    level of the retrieval cue, the retrieval route (i.e., the link be-

    tween the retrieval cue and memory), or the memorial repre-

    sentation itself? We also need to explore more thoroughly the

    effects of repeated inhibition over longer periods of time and the

    extent to which resultant memory distortions become permanent

    features of memory. Through an improved understanding of such

    mechanisms, wemay nd ourselves better placed not only to nd

    effective solutions to real-life problems but also to gain impor-

    tant insights into fundamental issues concerning memory

    structure and updating.

    Recommended ReadingAnderson, M.C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive

    control and the mechanisms of forgetting. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 49, 415445. A clearly written, user-friendly, andcomprehensive review for readers who wish to expand their

    knowledge on inhibitory processes in memory.

    FRUIT

    baseball orange

    ROUND

    Retrieval practice cue Novel test cue

    Rp+ Rp

    Fig. 3. Inhibitory account of retrieval-induced forgetting. Noninhibitoryaccounts of forgetting assume that the association between the retrievalpractice cue (ROUND) and the practiced (Rp1) item (baseball) isstrengthened (bold arrow) through retrieval practice while the associationbetween the retrieval practice cue and the non-practiced (Rp) item(orange) is weakened (dashed arrow). If retrieval-induced forgetting isdue tononinhibitory factors, the presentation of novel cues at test (FRUIT)should circumvent any interference between the retrieval practice cue andthe Rp item. If, however, retrieval-induced forgetting is due to inhibi-tion, then Rp items should still remain unavailable to conscious inspec-tion despite the use of novel cues at test (i.e., designated by dashed lines).

    Volume 17Number 1 29

    Malcolm D. MacLeod and Jo Saunders

    at Ruppin Institute on January 26, 2013cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Bjork, E.L., Bjork, R.A., & MacLeod, M.D. (2006). Types and conse-

    quences of forgetting: Intended and unintended. In L-G. Nilsson&

    N. Ohta (Eds.), Memory and society: Psychological perspectives(pp.134158). New York: Psychology Press. Discusses the con-

    sequences of forgetting in more detail than the current article.

    MacLeod, M.D., & Saunders, J. (2005). (See References). A represen-

    tative study that illustrates original research about the role of in-

    hibition in misinformation effects.

    REFERENCES

    Anderson, M.C., & Bell, T. (2001). Forgetting our facts: The role of

    inhibitory processes in the loss of propositional knowledge.

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 544570.

    Anderson, M.C., Bjork, E.L., & Bjork, R.A. (2000). Retrieval-induced

    forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specicmechanism.PsychonomicBulletin & Review, 7, 522530.

    Anderson, M.C., Bjork, R.A., & Bjork, E.L. (1994). Remembering can

    cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Jour-nal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition,20, 10631087.

    Anderson, M.C., & Spellman, B.A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory

    mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case.

    Psychological Review, 102, 68100.

    Bekerian, D.A., & Bowers, J.M. (1983). Eyewitness testimony: Were we

    misled? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory &Cognition, 9, 139145.

    Lindsay, D.S., & Johnson, M.K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility

    effect and memory source. Memory and Cognition, 17, 349358.

    Loftus, E.F., Miller, D., & Burns, H. (1978). Semantic integration of

    verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 4, 1931.

    MacLeod, M.D. (2002). Retrieval-induced forgetting in eyewitness

    memory: Forgetting as a consequence of remembering. AppliedCognitive Psychology, 16, 135149.

    MacLeod, M.D., & Macrae, C.N. (2001). Gone but not forgotten: The

    transient nature of retrieval-induced forgetting. PsychologicalScience, 12, 148152.

    MacLeod, M.D., & Saunders, J. (2005). The role of inhibitory control in

    the production of misinformation effects. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 31, 964979.

    Macrae, C.N., & MacLeod, M.D. (1999). On recollections lost: When

    practice makes imperfect. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 77, 163473.

    McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985). Misleading postevent infor-

    mation and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against

    memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psy-chology: General, 114, 116.

    Saunders, J., & MacLeod, M.D. (2002). New evidence on the suggest-

    ibility of memory: The role of retrieval-induced forgetting in

    misinformation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Applied, 8, 127142.

    Saunders, J., & MacLeod, M.D. (2006). Can inhibition resolve retrieval

    competition through the control of spreading activation? Memory& Cognition, 34, 307322.

    Shaw, J.S., Bjork, R.A., & Handal, A. (1995). Retrieval-induced for-

    getting in an eyewitness paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin andReview, 2, 249253.

    Storm, B.C., Bjork, E.L., Bjork, R.A., & Nestojko, J. (2006). Is retrieval

    success necessary for retrieval-induced forgetting? PsychonomicBulletin & Review, 13, 10231027.

    30 Volume 17Number 1

    Retrieval Inhibition and Memory Distortion

    at Ruppin Institute on January 26, 2013cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    /ColorImageDict > /JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict > /JPEG2000ColorImageDict > /AntiAliasGrayImages false /CropGrayImages true /GrayImageMinResolution 300 /GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleGrayImages true /GrayImageDownsampleType /Average /GrayImageResolution 150 /GrayImageDepth -1 /GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2 /GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000 /EncodeGrayImages true /GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterGrayImages true /GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /GrayACSImageDict > /GrayImageDict > /JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict > /JPEG2000GrayImageDict > /AntiAliasMonoImages false /CropMonoImages true /MonoImageMinResolution 1200 /MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleMonoImages true /MonoImageDownsampleType /Average /MonoImageResolution 300 /MonoImageDepth -1 /MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000 /EncodeMonoImages true /MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode /MonoImageDict > /AllowPSXObjects false /CheckCompliance [ /None ] /PDFX1aCheck false /PDFX3Check false /PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false /PDFXNoTrimBoxError true /PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox true /PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfile (None) /PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier () /PDFXOutputCondition () /PDFXRegistryName () /PDFXTrapped /False

    /CreateJDFFile false /Description > /Namespace [ (Adobe) (Common) (1.0) ] /OtherNamespaces [ > /FormElements false /GenerateStructure false /IncludeBookmarks false /IncludeHyperlinks false /IncludeInteractive false /IncludeLayers false /IncludeProfiles false /MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings /Namespace [ (Adobe) (CreativeSuite) (2.0) ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK /PreserveEditing true /UntaggedCMYKHandling /LeaveUntagged /UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UseDocumentBleed false >> ]>> setdistillerparams> setpagedevice