4 - abernethy & brownell 1997 - management control systems

Upload: itzhak-david-simao-kaveski

Post on 07-Jul-2018

243 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    1/16

    Accounti ng, Or ganizations and Society,

    Vol.

    22, NO.

    3/4, pp. 233-248, 1997

    c 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd

    All rights resewed. Printed in Great Britain

    0361.3682/97 17.00+0.00

    PII: SO361-3682(96)00038-4

    MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT

    ORGANIZATIONS: THE ROLE OF ACCOUNTING, BEHAVIOR AND

    PERSONNEL CONTROLS

    MARGARET A. ABERNETHY and PETER BROWNELL

    i e Un i v er si t y o f Me l bou r ne

    bstract

    This paper reports the result of an empirical study examining the role of accounting and non-accounting

    controls in a research and development setting. The paper draws on Pet-row’s model of technology and

    structure to explore the influence of task characteristics on the effectiveness of accounting, behavior and

    personnel forms of control (Perrow, 1970). The main contribution of the study is the finding that non-

    accounting controls, especially personnel forms of control, contribute to organization effectiveness, par-

    ticularly where task characteristics are not well suited to the use of accounting-based controls. In addition,

    the results suggest that of the two task dimensions examined, task analyzability and number of excep

    tions, the latter proves to have the more significant inIIuence on the suitability of the controls.

    “Programmed” types of controls (such as either accounting or behavior controls) appear unsuitable

    where number of exceptions is high. 8 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd

    Much management accounting research has

    focused on the nature of control system design

    in manufacturing production settings where

    tasks are considered to be particularly well sui-

    ted to the use of accounting-based controls.

    Considerable attention is now being directed

    towards understanding the role of accounting

    controls in other settings where the character-

    istics of the tasks undertaken bring into ques-

    tion the suitability of conventional, accountmg-

    based controls and raise the prospect that for

    effective control, organizations will need to

    design their control systems around a variety of

    non-accounting controls (Abernethy 8~ Stoel-

    winder, 1995; Rockness & Shields, 1988; Foster

    & Gupta, 1994). This study contributes to this

    body of literature by examining management

    control systems (MC ) in research and devel-

    opment (R&D) organizations.

    The distinctive feature of R&D tasks which

    potentially influences the effectiveness of

    accounting controls is, at least in some cases,

    their lack of routineness. Perrow’s model of

    technology and structure highlighted two key

    dimensions of routineness: the existence (or

    otherwise) of well-established techniques for

    performing tasks (task analyzability), and the

    degree of variety in the tasks encountered

    (number of exceptions) (Pet-row, 1970). The

    two dimensions are commonly, and collectively,

    The research reported in this paper was supported by an Australian Research Council (Large) Grant to the second-named

    author. The authors appreciate greatly the constructive comments received on this, or earlier drafts, from Rob Chenhall,

    Ken Merchant, Nick Dopuch, Kevin Davis, Alan Dunk, participants in workshops at The University of Melbourne, The

    University of Sydney, The University of Southern California, The University of Queensland, The University of Auckland, the

    1992 AAANZ Conference (Palmerston North, New Zealand), the 1992 Management Accounting Research Conference

    (University of New South Wales), the 1994 Annual Meeting of the American Accounting Association (New York), and two

    anonymous reviewers.

    233

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    2/16

    234

    M. A. ABERNETHY and P. BROWNELL

    referred to as task uncertainty and the results of

    stood, nor extensively researched, and because

    research studies in management accounting

    of the desirability of triangulating the assess-

    have persistently shown that accounting forms ment of the non-accounting controls studied,

    of control are poorly suited to highly uncertain

    an interview phase supplemented the ques-

    tasks (Hirst, 1983; Brownell & Hirst, 1986;

    tionnaire which was used to gather the primary

    Brownell & Dunk, 1991). data.

    Despite the apparent appeal of these argu-

    ments, evidence obtained from empirical stu-

    dies of R&D settings is sparse, and that which

    does exist is mixed. For example, Rockness and

    Shields (1984) while able to show that the

    importance of differing types of controls was

    associated with knowledge of the task transfor-

    mation process, were unable to find evidence

    that measurability of outputs, task complexity

    and task dependence mattered.

    The balance of the paper is structured as fol-

    lows. In the next section, the theoretical fra-

    mework underlying the study is developed.

    This leads to the statement of hypotheses. Fol-

    lowing that, the empirical study is described,

    with sections addressing the method, sample

    and results. The paper concludes with a recog-

    nition of some of the limitations of the study,

    and some avenues for future research.

    This study seeks to extend the research of

    Rockness and Shields (1984,1988) in several

    ways. First, the study introduces a criterion

    variable (managerial performance) which facili-

    tates a more complete test of the theoretical

    propositions of interest. Rockness and Shields

    restricted their tests to an assessment of the

    covariation of task characteristics and control

    system elements and did not attempt to evalu-

    ate whether the “right” calibration of control

    system design and task characteristics was

    associated with important organizational out-

    comes. Second, the study addresses the mea-

    surement issues associated with the manner in

    which task characteristics were operationalized

    by Rockness and Shields (1984,1988). The two

    task dimensions examined in this study are

    empirically assessed using an established

    instrument (Withey

    et a l .

    1983). Rockness and

    Shields conducted their study before the publi-

    cation of the Withey

    et a l .

    (1983) study, and

    acknowledged the limitations associated with

    their measurement of the task dimensions.

    Third, we expand the array of controls available

    for coordinating and controlling tasks by

    examining the use of accounting, behavior and

    personnel controls. It is hypothesized that dif-

    fering combinations of the two task variables

    give rise to differing relations between reliance

    on each of the three types of controls, and R&D

    management performance. Fourth, because the

    R&D setting is neither particularly well under-

    THEORETICAL. FRAMEWORK

    Perrow (1970) argued that organizational struc-

    ture and, in particular, reliance on a bureau-

    cratic organizational form, will be dependent

    upon the degree of “routineness” of task

    technology. “Routineness” is unbundled by

    Perrow into the two dimensions, described

    above, of task analyzability and number of

    exceptions. Where established techniques for

    handling tasks do not exist (low analyzability),

    or where there exists substantial variety or

    novelty in the tasks encountered (high number

    of exceptions), Perrow describes the task set-

    ting as “non-routine”, and conversely when

    tasks are analyzable with few exceptions, the

    task setting is “routine”. Perrow proceeds to

    describe the structural arrangements necessary

    to achieve effective control and co-ordination

    for the different task environments faced by an

    organization. Figure 1 presents a diagrammatic

    representation of Perrow’s framework.

    Tasks in Cell 1 of Fig. 1 are repetitive and

    familiar (few exceptions), with known routines

    for handling them (high analyzability). Long

    production runs of highly standardized pro-

    ducts would be typical here, and this setting is

    readily recognized as one where conventional

    accounting controls, such as standard costing

    and flexible budgeting, are well suited (Bruns &

    Waterhouse, 1975). With little task variety and

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    3/16

    MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN R&D ORGANIZATIONS

    235

    a clear view of input-output relations in task

    In contrast, Cell 3 represents the “non-

    execution, the organization can justify the

    routine” situation and it is here that Perrow

    development and use of these forms of control.

    expects that formal, bureaucratic controls will

    Perrow also argued that organizations facing not be effective for controlling performance.

    such tasks would be able to rely on procedure

    Tasks cannot be “programmed” and thus beha-

    guides, operating manuals, job codification and vior cannot be controlled by implementing

    rigid lines of reporting and accountability for procedures which pre-specify desired actions

    controlling employee behavior. In this paper, or by monitoring individual actions through the

    we refer to these collectively as “behavior” use of supervisors. Perrow argues that “profes-

    controls, the effectiveness of which requires sional or collegial” structural arrangements are

    the existence of tasks which are transparent in required in this setting. The training of profes-

    terms of input-output knowledge, and pre- sionals and their socialization is based on a col-

    dictable or regular in terms of task variety. In legial model of control which regulates

    this setting, the choice between accounting behavior primarily through self and peer group

    controls, and behavior controls, will be dictated control processes. Organizations operationalize

    by their relative cost and issues of timeliness. these self and group controls through the use of

    Generally, where timeliness is at a premium, selection and training policies/procedures

    and behavior controls are feasible, they will be

    which ensure that people who have been

    preferred to accounting controls because they

    exposed to appropriate training and socializa-

    operate in “real-time” during task execution, tion processes are employed. We follow Mer-

    rather than “ex post”, as with accounting con- chant (1985) and refer to this as “personnel”

    trols (Merchant, 1985). control.

    Low

    Number of

    Exceptions

    High

    High

    Task Analyzability

    Low

    Formal

    Administrative

    Controls

    1

    2

    4 3

    Informal Co-ordination

    Mechanisms

    Fig. 1. Perrow’s Model of Structure and Technology (Perrow, 1970)

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    4/16

    236

    M. A. ABERNE’IHY and P. BROWNELI

    The above arguments can be summarized in

    terms of the first two hypotheses of the study:

    Hl: Where tasks are highly analyzable

    and exceptions are few, reliance on both

    accounting and behavior controls will be

    positively related to R&D management

    performance.

    H2: Where task analyzability is low and

    exceptions are many and frequent, reli-

    ance on personnel controls will be posi-

    tively related to R&D management

    performance.

    The remaining two situations depicted in

    Fig. 1 (Cells 2 and 4) involve task settings

    where it is more difficult to predict which

    forms of control will prove most effective.

    Tasks may have little variety, yet exhibit uncer-

    tainty in the transformation process (Cell 2);

    others may have little uncertainty but a great

    deal of variety (Cell 4). They are in the middle

    of the routine/non-routine continuum, but they

    are not the same; they differ from one another

    (Perrow, 1970). We attempt to extend prior lit-

    erature by developing and testing hypothesized

    relations between control system choices, and

    managerial performance, in Cells 2 and 4 of

    Fig. 1.

    Considering Cell 2 first, tasks here are low in

    variety suggesting the potential suitability of

    behavior controls. However, notwithstanding

    the lack of variety of tasks, little has been

    learned about the process by which inputs are

    transformed into outputs; programs for per-

    forming these tasks have not been established

    (i.e., analyzability is low). It is, therefore, not

    possible to develop a set of manuals, guides or

    procedures which can be drawn upon during

    task performance. Thus, behavior controls will

    be unsuitable in this task setting. However,

    accounting controls may offer some potential

    here. Budgets are a planning device used to set

    performance targets and subsequently serve to

    measure the results of individual actions (Mer-

    chant, 1985). Use of accounting controls in this

    manner is not dependent upon identifying

    input-output relations but simply requires the

    ability to pre-determine and quantify desired

    outputs. The means of achieving these targets

    are not specified (unlike standard costing sys-

    tems) but rather left to the judgment and

    experience of those performing the tasks. One

    can think of the sales function as being con-

    trolled in this manner. Little is known regarding

    how the range of marketing inputs (advertising,

    pricing, promotions and so forth) combines

    with sales effort to translate into task perfor-

    mance (Rockness & Shields, 19SS), yet the

    variety of problems faced is relatively limited.

    Budget targets which incorporate expense

    allowances as a percentage of the sales dollar (a

    form of flexible budgeting) could be an effec-

    tive form of control in Cell 2.

    In Cell 4, much variety is encountered in

    tasks, but notwithstanding this variation, input-

    output relations can be readily specified. This

    enables the development and implementation

    of programmed controls such as procedure

    guides and operating manuals e3c an t e and/or

    the use of superior authority to ensure that

    behavior is in accordance with that known to

    achieve desired results. Accounting controls, on

    the other hand, may be less effective when task

    variety is high. Task variety inhibits the devel-

    opment of pre-specified financial targets as it is

    difficult to anticipate in advance the nature of

    the tasks to be performed and thus to prepare

    cost estimates which can serve as benchmarks

    for measuring desired behavior. Further, beha-

    vior controls will, as noted earlier, be more

    timely than accounting controls. The complex

    code of rules and procedures used to control

    the behavior of airline pilots is a clear case of

    where timeliness dictates their use in pref-

    erence to

    expost

    controls.

    However, it becomes difficult to use behavior

    controls in Cell 4 if task variety (number of

    exceptions) is very high. Behavior controls will

    either need to be written at too general a level

    to be useful, or the number of procedures

    required to handle all the exceptions would be

    prohibitively costly to document. Having ruled

    out a role for accounting controls here, the

    organization may need to resort to personnel

    controls to achieve the coordination needed

    for task execution. Indeed, it might be argued

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    5/16

    MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN R D ORGANIZATIONS

    237

    that all “programmed” forms of control, such

    as both accounting and behavior controls,

    might be restricted to settings where the range

    of tasks encountered is quite small (few excep-

    tions). It is predicted that Cell 4 tasks will be

    best controlled with either behavior or person-

    nel controls, the latter perhaps constituting the

    preferred choice.

    In summary:

    a>

    @>

    H3: Where tasks are low in analyzability

    but few exceptions are encountered,

    reliance on accounting controls will be

    positively related to R&D management

    performance.

    H4: Where tasks are highly analyzable,

    but many exceptions are encountered,

    reliance on behavior and personnel con-

    trols will be positively related to R&D

    management performance.

    THE EMPIRICAL STUDY

    Sample

    Survey data were collected by both ques-

    tionnaire and interview, administered to 150

    senior research officers in the research and

    development (R&D) divisions of a large Austra-

    lian industrial company and a major US scien-

    tific organization.

    Initial contact with both organizations was

    through their respective Research Directors,

    with whom the purpose of the study was dis-

    cussed and agreement to participate secured.

    Each Director gave approval to the study via the

    provision of a covering memorandum on cor-

    porate letterhead attached to the front of the

    questionnaires, which were distributed via each

    organization’s internal mail system. Stamped

    envelopes for direct return of completed

    questionnaires to the researchers accompanied

    the questionnaires. Anonymity was assured,

    both to the respective organizations and to all

    respondents.

    A total of 138 questionnaires (92 ) was

    returned, eleven of which were incomplete,

    resulting in a useable sample set of 127. With

    only twelve of the distributed questionnaires

    not being returned, and in view of the fact that

    the assurances of anonymity prevented target-

    ing any follow-up, no attempt was made to

    supplement the sample size. On average, the

    respondents were

    40

    years of age, had worked

    for their present employer for 10 years, and had

    been in their current position for three years.

    The respondents all held first-line supervisory

    positions and were responsible for managing

    separate,

    identifiable work groups. These

    groups constituted the unit of analysis in the

    study.

    The question of whether the focus on R&D

    organizations might cause range restriction in

    the measures of the task variables was an issue

    of concern. Indeed, Perrow (1970) offered

    research organizations as an example of a Cell 3

    organization (refer Fig. 1). The concern was

    whether range restriction would place few

    respondents in the other cells of Fig. 1. This

    issue also arose in the study carried out by

    Rockness and Shields (1988). Rockness and

    Shields characterized the task setting of R&D in

    terms of Ouchi’s (1979) framework which

    focussed on output measurability and knowl-

    edge of transformation process (Ouchi, 1979)

    Rockness and Shields regarded R&D as uni-

    formly low on both of Ouchi’s dimensions.

    However, in the context of the present paper

    we do not expect Cell 3 to be overpopulated

    relative to other cells. We believe that there is

    likely to exist considerable between work-

    group variance in the two task dimensions as a

    function of the type of R&D work undertaken.

    In spite of the position taken by in the Rock-

    ness and Shields, 1988 paper, they argued in

    their earlier (Rockness & Shields, 1984) paper

    that “basic research”, “applied research” and

    “development” (National Science Foundation,

    1976) were types of R&D activity that would

    locate on a continuum in terms of task charac-

    teristics with basic research at the more

    “uncertain” end, and development at the more

    “routine” end.

    This issue was explored in preliminary inter-

    views with senior management of both compa-

    nies. The interview data confirmed that, in both

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    6/16

    238

    M. A. ABERNETHY and P. BROWNELL

    companies,

    research activities ranged from

    basic, or fundamental research, often funded by

    recurrent grants from corporate headquarters,

    to applied and developmental activities nego-

    tiated in arms length transactions, both with

    other operating divisions within their respec-

    tive companies, and with unrelated external

    customers. In the words of one manager:

    it (external work) is more routine in the sense that

    there is a well defined contract set of requirements

    that centres around routine schedules.. .the end goal

    is more clearly specified than in corporate sponsored

    research

    On the basis of this evidence, it was felt that

    cross-sectional variation in the task variables

    would be sufficient to ensure that all four cells

    of Fig. 1 were adequately populated.

    Qu est i onn a i r e measu r es

    S i x

    variables were measured by the ques-

    tionnaire: managerial performance, the two

    task characteristics (number of exceptions and

    task analyzability), accounting control, behavior

    control and personnel control. Each is dis-

    cussed in turn.

    Per f o rmance .

    The unit of analysis in the

    study, as noted earlier, is the work group and

    its manager, and it is this individual’s perfor-

    mance for which measurement was sought. We

    assessed overall performance via a self-rating,

    single-item instrument, and used the taxonomy

    developed by Mahoney

    et a l .

    (1963,1965) to

    assess the proportion of the variance in the

    overall measure accounted for by the eight sub-

    dimensions of managerial performance. The

    purpose of this test was not to validate the

    overall rating as such, but rather to establish

    whether or not variations in the overall rating

    are accounted for by a set of sub-dimensions

    developed originally by Mahoney

    et a l .

    (1963,

    1965) to represent dimensions of managerial

    work. While the Mahoney

    et a l .

    instrument has

    been in the literature for a number of years, a

    recent assessment of its validity suggests that

    the functions identified by Mahoney

    et a l .

    “still

    represent the most useful way of conceptualiz-

    ing the manager’s job” and that the dimensions

    are relevant across a variety of settings (Carroll

    & Gillen, 1987).

    The test was based on a multiple regression

    of the overall rating on the eight sub-dimen-

    sions, which produced a squared multiple cor-

    relation coefficient of 0.52. Of interest is that

    this coefficient is typical of those reported by

    Mahoney

    et aZ.

    suggesting that what con-

    stitutes “good performance” for R&D managers

    is similar to that in other organizational func-

    tions. In the hypothesis tests which follow, the

    single-item overall rating of performance is used

    as the criterion variable and this is consistent

    with much prior research focusing on both

    manufacturing and service industries at differ-

    ing managerial levels (Abemethy Sr Stoelwin-

    der, 1995; Brownell, 1982; Brownell & Dunk,

    1991; Kren, 1992; McInnes & Ramakrishnan,

    1991; Venkatraman & Ramanujam, 1987).

    Ta sk cha r ac t e r i st i c s.

    We used the Withey

    et a t .

    (1983) IO-item instrument, in seven-point

    partially anchored form, to measure both task

    analyzability and number of exceptions. Ortho-

    gonal factor analysis of the 10 items (provided

    in Appendix A) resulted in two factors con-

    sistent with the two theoretical constructs of

    interest, factor I capturing number of excep-

    tions and factor II task analyzability. In the

    analysis we employ summed scores for the set

    of items loading on each factor, as opposed to

    the factor scores because of the conventional

    caution of the instability of factor loading solu-

    tions. While this approach resulted in correla-

    ted dimensions (r=O.59), this posed no serious

    concern to our theoretical arguments. Indeed,

    the framework constructed by Perrow (1979)

    posited that the two dimensions would covary,

    and prior empirical research examining the

    relationship between the two constructs and

    control system design also have found signifi-

    cant and positive correlations of the same order

    or magnitude to that reported here (Abemethy

    & Stoelwinder, 1991; Van de ven & Delbecq,

    1974; Daft & Macintosh, 1981; Withey

    et a l .

    1983). A test for the reliability of each dimen-

    sion resulted in a Cronbach (1951) alpha coeffi-

    cient of 0.91 for number of exceptions and 0.85

    for task analyzability.

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    7/16

    MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN R&D ORGANIZATIONS

    239

    Accounting controls.

    The extent to which

    accounting is used as a form of control was

    captured in this study using a purpose-devel-

    oped measure based on early work of Hop-

    wood (1972) and others. Respondents were

    asked to indicate on a seven-point partially

    anchored Likert-type scale, the extent to which

    each of three items represented the use of

    accounting control in their unit. The items

    included in the instrument are provided in

    Appendix B. The highest inter-item correlation

    obtained from the use of this measure was only

    0.20, suggesting reliability problems and the need

    to exercise caution in interpreting the results

    which follow. These results were compared

    with three further sets of results obtained using

    each of the three items individually. No material

    differences were found between the reported

    results, which use the aggregate measure, and

    any of the results using the individual measures.

    Behavior controls.

    Hage and Aiken’s 21-

    item scale w s used in modified form to mea-

    sure behavior control @Iage & Aiken, 1967).

    The instrument was developed by Hage and

    Aiken to measure the degree of centralization

    and was initially conceived as having five

    dimensions: participation in decision making,

    hierarchy of authority, job codification, rule

    observation and formalization. Behavior con-

    trols have often been described in terms of the

    last four dimensions of this instrument (Ouchi,

    1979; Merchant, 1985) so the items relating to

    these four dimensions were summed to mea-

    sure the extent to which reliance is placed on

    behavior control. The Cronbach alpha of 0.82

    provides support for the reliability of measure.

    While the Hage and Aiken instrument has been

    in the literature for many years, its recent use

    by other researchers (e.g., Banker

    et al.,

    1993),

    who have found excellent reliability coeffi-

    cients, supports its continuing suitability for

    operationalizing this construct.

    Personnel controls.

    Recall that personnel

    controls are the means to facilitate the opera-

    tion of self- and group control processes. In

    contrast to behavior and accounting controls,

    personnel controls operate without imposing

    formal bureaucratic forms of control. Instead,

    they influence the types of individuals who

    interact in the work group, the kind of profes-

    sional activity undertaken by group members,

    and the level of peer-group self-regulation, via

    strict personnel selection, and perhaps place-

    ment policies. By getting the “right” people,

    the organization can rely on the professionalism

    of group members and the sharing of common

    values. The operation of self- and group con-

    trols has been described by others as “clan”

    control (Ouchi, 1980; Govindarajan & Fisher,

    1990)

    as

    “social control” (e.g., Merchant,

    1985; Rockness & Shields, 1988) and as “pro-

    fessional control” (Orlikowsky, 1991; Aber-

    nethy & Stoelwinder, 1995).

    While we are interested in capturing the

    extent to which tasks are controlled through

    self- and group control processes, because of

    the subtlety of these processes, it is difficult to

    measure these directly. Indeed Birnberg (1988)

    in his commentary on Rockness and Shields

    (1988) cautions against using conventional

    questionnaire measures to capture reliance on

    these forms of control. Birnberg suggests that,

    unlike accounting and behavior controls, pcr-

    sonnel or group controls, because of their less

    obvious manifestations, may be measurable

    with conventional survey instruments only with

    considerable error.

    Hage and Aiken (1967) provide support for the

    notion that an individual’s level of professional-

    ism can be represented by the length of profes-

    sional training and socialization processes to

    which they are exposed. It is these processes

    which lead professionals to behave according

    to the collegial model of control noted by Per-

    row (1970). Numerous other researchers also

    support the idea that the operation of these

    processes is directly related to external training

    and socialization processes (Freidson, 1983;

    Orlikowsky,

    1991; Mintzberg, 1979). We,

    therefore, use Hage and Aiken’s measure of

    formal training (Hage & Aiken, 1967) to capture

    personnel control as it would appear to be a

    particularly appropriate proxy for representing

    the presence of self- and group controls in R&D

    settings which are often dominated by profes-

    sionals. It was felt that the level of formal training

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    8/16

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    9/16

    MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN R&D ORGANIZATIONS

    241

    the four cells. The purpose of this set of tests is

    to ascertain the task situations to which each of

    the three control types is best suited. The fol-

    lowing equation was fitted for each control

    type (suppressing the i subscript):

    Y = bo + biXi + bzXa + b3Xb + b4Xc

    + b5XlX, + bGXlXb + b7X1Xc + e

    (2)

    where Y=performance

    Xi=reliance on control type

    types 2,3)

    X,=1 for cell 2, 0 otherwise

    X,=1 for cell 3, 0 otherwise

    X,=1 for cell 4, 0 otherwise

    1 (repeat for

    The coefficients estimated by this regression

    allow a statistical comparison of Xi’s effects

    across the four cells of Fig. 1. To illustrate,

    where X,=reliance on accounting controls, we

    expect these to have more positive effects on

    performance in Cells 1 and 2 than in either

    Cells 3 or 4. Equation (2) reduces to:

    Y b. + blX,

    in Cell 1, so the coefficient b, captures the

    effects of accounting controls on performance

    in CelI 1. In Cell 2, equation (2) becomes:

    Y =

    bo +

    bz) + (bl + bs)Xl

    The coefficient b5 assesses the difference in

    Table 1 also shows negative correlations

    the effects of accounting controls in Cell 1 ver- between reliance on accounting control and

    sus Cell 2. By parallel argument, bb assesses the

    each of the task dimensions. This supports the

    difference in the effects of accounting controls generalization, and the evidence from prior

    in Cell 1 versus Cell 3 and b, allows comparison

    with Cell 4. In terms of hypothesis 1, therefore,

    we expect b5=0 because we hypothesize no

    differential effects of accounting controls on

    performance between Cells 1 and 2. However,

    we expect

    b6

    and

    b,

    both to be negative

    because we hypothesize less positive effects of

    accounting controls on performance in both

    Cells 3 and 4 compared to Cell 1. The expecta-

    tions regarding behavior and personnel controls

    were developed similarly and the upper panel

    of Table 5 summarizes the specific expectations

    for each control type.

    RESULTS

    Table 1 contains descriptive statistics as well

    as the matrix of zero-order correlations among

    all variables used in the study. For purposes of

    all analysis, the items for both task dimensions

    were scored so that high scores reflected high

    task uncertainty. It is interesting to note the

    virtual absence of any correlation among the

    three control types. This suggests that, at the

    organizational level, the reliance on any one

    form of control is independent of any other. It

    is well to recall, however, that this does not

    rule out the prospect that at the level of par-

    ticular sub-groupings of tasks (such as those

    suggested in Fig. 1) considerable co-variation

    might exist between particular forms of control

    in place.

    TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlation matrix (significance) (n= 127)

    VARIABLES MEAN S.D. Theoretical 1 2 3 4 5

    Range

    1. Managerial Performance 5.24 0.82 l-7

    2. Task Analyzability 21.90 5.73 5-35 -0.12(0.09)

    3. Number of Exceptions 26.82 5.49 5-35 0.10(0.14> 0.59(0.00)

    4. Accounting Control

    10.40 3.23 3-21 O.Ol(O.44) -0.26(0.00) -0.12(0.09)

    5. Behavior Control

    50.64 13.06 17-119 -0.09(0.17) -0.14(0.05) 0.15(0.04) 0.13(0.08)

    6.

    Personnel Control

    5.30 0.96 1-6 0.33(0.00) 0.04(0.32) 0.07(0.23)

    0.00(0.48) O.iO(O.13)

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    10/16

     4

    M. A. ABERNETHY and P. BROWNELL

    work, that accounting controls tend to be de-

    emphasized whenever task uncertainty is high.

    Also noteworthy is the restricted range of

    scores for personnel control; for all practical

    purposes, the measure reduces to a binary vari-

    able, respondents scoring either a 5 or a 6 on

    the scale. This is a result which can perhaps, be

    attributed to the choice of sample, and

    although it does not affect interpretation of the

    test results which follow, it raises the

    need for

    care when generalizing those results to other

    occupational groups which may show less

    extreme scores on the measure. Also likely to

    be the result of sampling is the highly sig-

    nificant and positive zero-order correlation

    (0.33) between reliance on personnel forms of

    control and performance.

    To examine the hypotheses of the study, we

    present first the results of fitting equation (1) to

    the data in each cell of Fig. 1, along with selec-

    tions of the interview data. Following this, the

    results of fitting equation (2) are presented. The

    results of fitting equation (1) to the data in Cells

    1 through 4 of Fig. 1 are presented in Table 2.

    Tables 2 and 4 report standardized beta coeffi-

    cients so as to remove the effects of differing

    scaling

    used to measure the independent vari-

    ables. The absolute magnitudes of the resulting

    coefficients are, therefore, meaningfully com-

    parable. Important in testing the hypotheses

    are the results obtained when the coefficients

    for each type of control are compared in each

    cell. This set of results is presented in Table 3.

    The results for Cell 1 show that use of

    accounting control is the only variable which

    significantly affects performance. Table 2

    reports 2-tailed probabilities, but it is worthy of

    note that the effect of using accounting con-

    trols is positive, as predicted. In Cell 1 of

    Table

    3, the results show that the coefficient for

    accounting controls (a,) is significantly larger

    than the coefficient for behavior controls (aa),

    but only at the 10 level of confidence, using a

    l-tailed test. The coefficient for accounting

    control was not significantly larger than the

    coefficient for personnel controls. Hypothesis 1

    predicted the importance of accounting con-

    trols here, but also predicted equivalent impor-

    tance for behavior controls. This hypothesis

    was, therefore, only partially confirmed.

    Hypothesis 2 stated that in Cell 3, personnel

    controls would assume greatest importance.

    The coefficient (a,) is significant and positive

    (Table 2) and, importantly, is significantly

    greater than the coefficients for both behavior

    control (aa) and accounting control (at). It is

    also interesting to note that accounting controls

    have a significantly more positive effect on per-

    formance than behavior controls in this setting.

    Table

    3

    sets out these latter results which show

    a clear preference ordering in Cell 3, the high-

    est task uncertainty cell, with personnel

    TABLE 2. Standardized beta coefficients and &statistics (significance levels’) from regressing managerial performance on

    accounting, behavior and personnel controls. Equation (1)

    Coefficient Cell 1

    Cell 2 Cell 3 Cell 4

    _

    a,, constant 3.97 4.02 3.50 2.80

    5.60 (0.00) 3.80 (0.00)

    3.29 (0.00) 2.26 (0.04)

    a, accounting control 0.30 - 0.10

    0.06 - 0.27

    1.98 (0.05) ~ 0.42 (0.68)

    0.39 (0.70) - 1.41 (0.18)

    az behavior control 0.03 - 0.03

    - 0.21 0.21

    0.19 (0.85) - 0.12 (0.90)

    - 1.39 (0.17) 1.05 (0.31)

    a3 personnel control 0.12 0.33

    0.42 0.50

    0.77 (0.44) 1.45 (0.16)

    2.89 (0.01) 2.56 (0.02)

    RZ 0.11 0.11

    0.25 0.41

    F 1.59 0.72

    4.05 3.75

    P

    0.21 0.55

    0.01 0.03

    n 44 22

    41 20

    *

    2-tailed tests.

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    11/16

    ‘MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN R&D

    ORGANIZATIONS

    243

    Low

    Number of

    Exceptions

    High

    TABLE 3. Summary of expectations and results [t-statistics

    (significance)‘] of comparing the coefficients derived from

    fitting equation (1).

    Task analyzability

    High Low

    Expectations Expectations

    Ll,=ai

    al>aZ

    aI’ a,‘

    >a,

    Results Results

    a,>a2 t =1.30 (pCO.10) a,al t=1.76 @a, t=1.67 (pcO.05)

    as>a, k2.53 @a2 t=2.64 (pa* t=1.41 @

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    12/16

    244

    M. A. ABERNETHY and P. BROWNELL

    Finally, consistent with the statistical results

    which showed that behavior controls are the

    least effective in Cell 3 type task situations, one

    respondent offered this remark:

    SO

    ow the “control guys” over there are putting him

    [the researcher] in a vice. He’s supposed to be doing

    research, supposed to be developing ideas and

    projects and steering people technology-wise. Now,

    all of a sudden, he’s got this [a range of bureaucratic,

    behavior controls] which puts him under constraints.

    In all, the results provide strong support for

    hypothesis 2.

    Hypothesis 3 predicted a key role only for

    accounting controls in Cell 2. The results fail to

    confirm this. The coefficient for accounting

    controls (ai) is not significant (Table 2), nor is

    the overall regression itself. Of interest is the

    result that personnel control, while not sig-

    nificant itself (Table 2) shows significantly

    more positive effects on performance than

    accounting controls, but only at the 10 level

    of confidence (Table 3).

    Hypothesis 4 suggested that behavior con-

    trols and personnel controls would be the most

    effective choices in Cell 4. Again, the coeffi-

    cient for behavior controls is, itself, not sig-

    nificant (a2 in Table 2), but the results in

    Table 3 show that it is significantly larger than

    that for accounting controls (a,), as predicted.

    As in the case of hypothesis 2, personnel con-

    trols again emerge as important, relatively, in

    that its coefficient is significantly larger than

    that for accounting controls (al). These results

    confirm hypothesis 4.

    Apparently, the “exceptions” dimension of

    the task is important to the effective operation

    of personnel controls. Where this dimension is

    high, personnel controls have highly significant,

    positive effects on performance, regardless of

    the level of task analyzability. By contrast, pro-

    grammed controls,

    such as accounting and

    behavior controls do not appear compatible

    with task situations involving many exceptions.

    The results of fitting equation (2) to the data

    for each control type are presented in Table 4

    and a summary of the relevant tests is contained

    in Table 5. With regard to accounting controls,

    the results show that their most positive effects

    are in Cell 1 (bl>O). We predicted similar

    effects in Cell 2 and while bs=O at conventional

    probability levels, it is tending negative, sug-

    gesting a less positive role in Cell 2. The less

    positive effects of accounting controls in Cells

    3

    and 4 were predicted;

    bb and b,, are

    both

    negative, although at conventional leveis only

    b, is significant.

    Behavior controls were predicted to have

    most positive effects in Cells 1 and 4. Neither

    result is confirmed (bI=O and b,, while positive,

    is not significant). Of note is the significantly

    @=O.lO) less positive effects of behavior con-

    trols in Cell 3. Moreover, a comparison of the

    coefficients bb and b, reveals that they are sig-

    nificantly different (t= 1.63,

    pO

    b,>O

    bG>O

    b5=0

    b,=O

    b,=O

    b6, b,0

    [p=O.O4]

    b5=0

    bT=O

    b,=O

    b =0 b5=0

    b5=b6=by

    b,

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    13/16

    MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN R&D ORGANIZATIONS

    245

    2. Reliance on accounting controls has sig-

    nificant positive effects on performance

    only where task uncertainty is lowest

    (Cell 1). The prediction that it would have

    similar effects in Cell 2 was not borne out.

    3. The task dimension, number of excep-

    tions, appears to have the greater bearing

    on the suitability of personnel controls

    and the unsuitability of either accounting

    or behavior controls.

    4. Behavior controls appear to contribute

    positively to performance in no situation,

    and contribute negatively where task

    uncertainty is highest (Cell 3) although

    they are preferred to accounting controls

    in Cell 4. However, it is the importance of

    personnel controls in Cells 3 and 4 and

    the need to avoid reliance on either

    accounting or behavior controls in such

    task situations, which constitutes the main

    result of the paper.

    DISCUSSION, LIMITATIONS AND

    CONCLUSIONS

    The management accounting literature has

    paid some attention to the question of the design

    of control systems in research and development

    organizations and this paper extends that line

    of inquiry. The tasks undertaken in such orga-

    nizations are likely to include some which are

    relatively extreme in terms of task uncertainty.

    Using Perrow’s model of technology and struc-

    ture (Pet-row, 1970) this study sought to

    examine the effectiveness of conventional

    accounting controls, behavior controls, such as

    procedure guides and job codification, and per-

    sonnel controls in settings which varied in

    terms of task analyzability and the number of

    exceptions encountered in task execution.

    The main contribution of the study is the

    finding which shows that where task uncer-

    tainty is highest (that is, where task analyz-

    ability is low and number of exceptions high)

    reliance on personnel forms of control has a

    positive and significant effect on performance.

    Moreover, this effect is significantly more posi-

    tive than that of either accounting or behavior

    controls. This confirms the theoretical expecta-

    tions of the study.

    Also consistent with expectations was the

    result that behavior controls prove significantly

    more effective than accounting controls in Cell

    4 of Fig. 1. Tasks high in analyzability, but also

    high in number of exceptions, are not as amen-

    able to accounting forms of control. This is

    because such tasks, while analyzable when they

    arise, cannot be foreshadowed with sufficient

    notice to enable the ex ante specification of

    input-output relations necessary for the use of

    many forms of accounting control (e.g., stan-

    dard costing).

    The results of the study should be considered

    in light of its limitations. First, it is necessary

    to acknowledge the usual limitation of cross-

    sectional based survey research, namely that

    such a study design does not permit causal

    statements. Inferences of causation implied are

    all based on theoretical positions taken. Second,

    variable measurement remains an area worthy

    of further research attention. While a recog-

    nized measure of task characteristics was used

    in the study, the measurement of reliance on all

    forms of control needs further development,

    especially in the area of personnel controls,

    where an unproven proxy was employed. We

    rely on two separate tests of convergent valid-

    ity, and on the strong, theoretically consistent

    results for the role of personnel controls to

    vindicate the means of its measurement, but

    readily acknowledge that the latter is an

    expost

    rationalization and that there exists much scope

    for further developmental work in this area.

    The use of a self-rating performance measure

    may constitute a limitation of the study. How-

    ever, there are few alternative means available

    to capture managerial performance with a

    cross-sectional sample. We were unable to use

    superior ratings as we guaranteed anonymity to

    the respondents and there were also no appar-

    ent objective and comparative measures which

    could be used reliably to capture performance

    across our sample of respondents.

    Third, the study stopped short of predicting/

    estimating the interactive effects of various

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    14/16

    246 M. A. ABERNETHY and P. BROWNELL

    controls on performance. That is, we did not

    consider the prospect that, for example, the

    effectiveness of personnel controls in a given

    task situation may differ according to the

    degree of reliance placed simultaneously on

    accounting controls. The data gathered are suf-

    ficient to estimate such interactions, but the

    significant prospect of experimenter-wise error

    cautioned against this. When various measure-

    ment issues are better settled, and when a more

    solid theoretical foundation is developed for the

    prediction of such effects, such a study should

    be undertaken.

    Notwithstanding these limitations, the results

    of the study have provided clear evidence, in

    support of prior research, of settings not suited

    to the use of accounting forms of control. The

    more important contribution of the study has

    been to identify the potential of non-accounting

    controls, especially personnel forms of control,

    to contribute to the overall management con-

    trol effort of the organization where task char-

    acteristics are not well suited to the use of

    traditional, accounting based controls.

    Future research could usefully focus on sev-

    eral related avenues of enquiry. First, it could

    provide further evidence confirming the results

    of the present study, while overcoming some of

    its limitations. Second, the external validity of

    the present study’s findings could be enhanced

    by extending the inquiry to other organizational

    functions and settings. And third, with a

    strengthened theoretical basis, research could

    begin to explore the implications of combina-

    tions of controls, studying their interactive

    effects on important organizational outcomes.

    It is clear that organizations rely on combina-

    tions of control mechanisms in any given set-

    ting, yet virtually nothing is known about how

    the effects of any one control are governed by

    the level of simultaneous reliance on other

    forms. Until empirical work begins to examine

    this complex question, our understanding of

    how the full range of management controls

    operates will remain piecemeal. Finally, we are

    inclined to suggest that the treatment of

    accounting controls in our framework deserves

    further thought. In particular, the view that

    accounting information is limited to the exer-

    cise of control based on just outputs ignores its

    potential role (such as in the context of cost

    centres) as a form of behavior control. Further

    research would do well to distinguish these two

    potential roles.

    BIBLIOGRAPHY

    Abernethy, M. A., & Stoelwinder, J. U. (1991). Budget use, task uncertainty, system goal orientation and

    subunit performance: a test of the “fit” hypothesis in not-for-profit hospitals. Accounting, Organizations

    andSociety,

    16, 105-120.

    Abernethy, M. A., & Stoelwinder, J. IJ. (1995). The role of professional control in the management of

    complex organizations.

    Accounti ng, Orguni zutions and Society, 20,

    1- 18.

    Banker, R. D., Potter, G., & Schroeder, R. G. (1993). Reporting manufacturing performance measures to

    workers: an empirical study.

    Journal of Management Accounting Research,

    Fall,

    33-55.

    Bimberg, J. G. (1988). Discussion of an empirical analysis of the expenditure budget in reseach and

    development.

    Contemporar y Accounti ng Research, 582-587

    Brownell, P. (1982). The role .of accounting data in performance evaluation, budgetary participation and

    organizational effectiveness.

    Journal of Accounting Research,

    Spring, 12-27.

    Brownell, P., 8r Dunk, A. S. (1991). Task uncertainty and its interaction with budgetary participation and

    budget emphasis: some methodological issues and empirical investigation.

    Accounting, Organizations

    and Society, 16, 693-703.

    Brownell, P., & Hirst, M. (1986). Reliance on accounting information, budgetary participation and task

    uncertainty: tests of a three-way interaction. Journal of Accounting Research, Autumn, 241-249.

    Bruns, W. J., & Waterhouse, J. H. (1975). Budgetary control and organization structure, Journal of

    Accounting Research, Autumn, 177-203.

    Carroll, S. J., & Gillen, D. J. (1987). Are the classical management functions useful in describing managerial

    work?

    Academy of Management Review, 1, 38-51.

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    15/16

    MANAGEMENT CONTROL SYSTEMS IN R&D ORGANIZATIONS

    247

    Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psycbometrika, 297-334.

    Daft, R. L., & Macintosh, N. 3. (1981). A tentative exploration into the amount and equivocal&y of infor-

    mation processing in organizational work units.

    Admini strative Science Quar terfy,

    June, 207-224.

    Foster, G., & Gupta, M. (1994). Marketing, cost management and management accounting. Journal of

    Management Accounti ng Research,

    Fall,

    43-77.

    Freidson, E. (1983). The theory of professions: state of the art. In R. Kingwall & P. Lewis (Eds) The

    Socfology of

    Profession.

    London: Macmillan Press.

    Govindarajan, V., & Fisher, J. (1990). Strategy, control systems and resource sharing: effects on business

    unit performance. Academy of Management Journal. 259-285.

    Hage, J., & A&en, M. (1967). Relationship of centralization to other structural properties. Administrative

    Science QuarterIy

    June, 72-92.

    Hirst. M. K. (1983). Reliance on accounting performance measures, task uncertainty and dysfunctional

    behavior: some extensions.

    Journal of Accounting Research,

    Autumn, 596-605.

    Hopwood, A. G. (1972). An empirical study of the role of accounting data ln performance evaluation.

    Journal of Accounting Research,

    Supplement, 156-182.

    Kren, L. (1992). Budgetary participation and managerial performance: the impact of information and

    environmental volatility. The

    Accounting Review,

    July, 51 l-526.

    Mclnnes, M.. & Ramakrishnan, R. ‘T. S. (1991). A decision theory model of motivation and its usefulness in

    the diagnosis of management control systems.

    Accounting, Organizations

    and Society, 16 167- 184.

    Mahoney, T. A., Jerdee, T. H., & Carroll, S. J. (1963). Deveiopment of manageri al perf ormance: a

    research approach. Cincinnati: South-Western Publishing Co.

    Mahoney, T. A., Jerdee, T. H., 8t Carroll, S. J. (1965). The jobs of management.

    I ndustri al Relations,

    Feb-

    ruary, 97-l 10.

    Merchant, K. A. (1985).

    Contr ol i n business or ganizations.

    Boston: Pitman.

    Mintzberg, H. (1979). The

    structuri ng of organizations.

    Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    National Science Foundation (1976). Nationalpatterns of R D resources: funds and manpower in the

    United States, 1953-197G. NSF.

    Orlikowsky, W. J. (1991). Integrated information environment or matrix of control? The contradictory

    implications of information technology.

    Accounting, Management and I nformation Technologies, 9-

    42.

    Ouchi, W. G. (1979). A conceptual framework for the design of organizational control mechanisms.

    Management Science, September, 833-848.

    Ouchi, W. G. (1980). Markets, bureaucracies and clans.

    Admini strative Science Quar terly,

    March, l29-

    141.

    Perrow, C. (1970).

    Organ izationat analysis: a sociol ogfcal view. New

    York: Tavistock Publications.

    Perrow, C. (1979) Organ izational analysis: a sociol ogical view. London: Tavistock Publications.

    Rockness. H. O., & Shields, M. D. (1984). Organizational control systems in research and development,

    Accounti ng, Organi zations and Society, 9, 165-177.

    Rockness. H. O., & Shields, M. D. (1988). An empirical analysis of the expenditure budget in research and

    development.

    Contemporary Accounti ng Research, 568-581.

    Van de ven, A. H., & Delbecq, A. L. (1974). A task contingent model of work-unit structure.

    Adminis-

    trative Science Quarterly, June, 183-197.

    Venkatraman, N., & Ramanujam, V. (1987). Measurement of business economic performance: an exami-

    nation of method convergence.

    Journal of

    Management, Spring, 109-l 22

    Withey, M., Daft R. L.. & Cooper, W. H. (1983). Measures of Perrow’s work unit technology: an empirical

    assessment of a new scale.

    Academy ofM anugementJour nal,

    45-63.

  • 8/19/2019 4 - Abernethy & Brownell 1997 - Management Control Systems

    16/16

    248

    M. A. ABERNETHY and P. BROWNELL

    APPENDIX A. FACTOR ANALYSIS OF THE lo-ITEM TASK UNCERTAINTY MEASURE

    ITEMS

    How repetitious are the duties of those in your unit?

    To what extent would you say the work of your unit is routine?

    Basically, unit members perform repetitive activities in doing their jobs.

    How many of the tasks in your unit are the same from day to day?

    People in my unit do about the same job in the same way most of the time.

    To what extent is there an understandable sequence of steps that can be

    followed in doing the work of your unit?

    To what extent is there an understandable sequence of steps that can be

    followed in carrying out the work in your unit?

    To what extent is there a clearly known way to do the major types of work

    normally encountered in your unit?

    To what extent is there a clearly defined body of knowledge of subject

    matter which can guide the work done in your unit?

    To do the work of your unit, to what extent can personnel actually rely on

    established procedures and practices?

    Variance explained

    FACTOR

    I

    II

    0.876

    0.863

    0.825

    0.779

    0.75 1

    0.799

    0.773

    0.727

    0.720

    0.696

    54.2

    14.6

    APPENDIX B. ITEMS USED TO MEASURE ACCOUNTING CONTROL AND

    PERSONNEL CONTROL

    (a) Accounting control measure

    The items in this section have the purpose of assessing the role you feel that accounting infor-

    mation plays in the evaluation of your own performance. That is, to what extent does your

    superior rely on accounting information such as budgets, spending limits, and financial targets,

    in judging your performance. For each item,

    please ci rcle one number.

    (l=Definitely false,

    7=Definitely true).

    1. No accounting numbers of any sort figure prominently when my performance is judged

    (reverse scored).

    2. Control over the activities of research departments is achieved principally with periodic

    reporting of accounting information.

    3. My ability to meet financial targets is a preoccupation of my superior.

    (b) Proxy measure used for personnel control

    (Hage & Aiken, 1967)

    Respondents were asked to check the appropriate response category. Responses were coded

    1-6 with lowest score being l(a) and highest score being 6(f). High scores proxy for extensive

    reliance on personnel control.

    What level of formal education did you complete? (Check One)

    a. High school graduate (or less) with no professional training

    b. High school graduate (or less) with some professional training

    c. College degree, or some college education, but no professional training

    d. College degree, or some college education and some professional training

    e. Post-graduate degree, but no professional training

    f. Post-graduate degree and some professional training