a neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

31
A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing Jeremy R. Manning 1, 2 , Justin C. Hulbert 1 , Jamal Williams 1 , Luis Piloto 1 , Lili Sahakyan 3 , Kenneth A. Norman 1, 4 1 Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540 2 Department of Computer Science, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540 3 Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC 27412 4 Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540 Correspondence should be addressed to J.R.M. ([email protected]). Mailing address: PNI 136C Princeton Neuroscience Institute Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 Abstract The mental context in which we experience an event plays a fundamental role in how we organize our memories of the event (e.g. in relation to other events) and, in turn, how we re- trieve those memories later. Because we use contextual representations to retrieve information pertaining to our past, processes that alter our representations of context can enhance or dimin- ish our capacity to retrieve particular memories. We designed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment to test the hypothesis that people can intentionally forget previously experienced events by “flushing out” the contextual information associated with those events. We had participants study two lists of words, manipulating whether they were told to forget (or remember) the first list prior to studying the second list. We used pattern classifiers to track neural patterns that reflected contextual information associated with the first list and found that, consistent with the notion of contextual flushing, the activation of the first-list contextual 1

Upload: vuhuong

Post on 02-Jan-2017

218 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextualflushing

Jeremy R. Manning1, 2, Justin C. Hulbert1, Jamal Williams1, Luis Piloto1,Lili Sahakyan3, Kenneth A. Norman1, 4

1Princeton Neuroscience Institute,Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540

2Department of Computer Science,Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540

3Department of Psychology,University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC 27412

4Department of Psychology,Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540

Correspondence should be addressed to J.R.M. ([email protected]).Mailing address:

PNI 136C Princeton Neuroscience InstitutePrinceton UniversityPrinceton, NJ 08540

Abstract

The mental context in which we experience an event plays a fundamental role in how we

organize our memories of the event (e.g. in relation to other events) and, in turn, how we re-

trieve those memories later. Because we use contextual representations to retrieve information

pertaining to our past, processes that alter our representations of context can enhance or dimin-

ish our capacity to retrieve particular memories. We designed a functional magnetic resonance

imaging (fMRI) experiment to test the hypothesis that people can intentionally forget previously

experienced events by “flushing out” the contextual information associated with those events.

We had participants study two lists of words, manipulating whether they were told to forget

(or remember) the first list prior to studying the second list. We used pattern classifiers to track

neural patterns that reflected contextual information associated with the first list and found

that, consistent with the notion of contextual flushing, the activation of the first-list contextual

1

Page 2: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

representation was lower following a forget instruction than a remember instruction. Further,

the magnitude of this neural signature of contextual flushing was negatively correlated with

participants’ abilities to later recall items from the first list. Lastly, we identified a network

of regions, primarily in posterior medial and lateral temporal cortex, whose activity correlates

with these contextual flushing e↵ects. Although a large behavioral literature has amassed sup-

port for the notion of forgetting through contextual flushing, our work provides the first neural

support for this phenomenon and gives us a glimpse into its neural underpinnings.

Introduction

Although it is frustrating when we forget information unintentionally, sometimes forgetting can

be beneficial (e.g. we might wish to exclude unwanted memories from our awareness; Anderson

et al., 2004). Here we ask how we can intentionally forget recently experienced events by examining

neuroimaging data collected during a list-method directed forgetting experiment. This paradigm asks

participants to forget list A items prior to studying list B, resulting in impaired list A recall and

enhanced list B recall (Bauml, 2008; Sahakyan et al., 2013).

The most prevalent explanation for degraded list A recall following a forget instruction is

that contextual information associated with list A is “flushed out” of the currently active state

of context (Sahakyan and Kelley, 2002), possibly using active control mechanisms (Anderson and

Hanslmayr, 2014). According to this view, list A items are associated with whatever other thoughts

are present at the time they are studied; these co-active thoughts constitute the participant’s mental

context (Manning et al., 2015). Normally, these thoughts persist until the recall test, at which point

they can facilitate list A recall. However, when participants receive a forget cue, they deliberately

change their mental context, flushing out old contextual elements and possibly adding new ones.

Consequently, the contextual features that were (previously) linked to list A items are no longer

2

Page 3: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

available at test, impairing list A recall. Note that the benefits of directed forgetting (i.e., enhanced

list B recall) are thought to result primarily from mechanisms other than context change (Sahakyan

et al., 2013). Our focus in this paper is on explaining the costs of directed forgetting (i.e., impaired

list A recall).

Despite extensive behavioral support for the contextual flushing hypothesis (Sahakyan and

Kelley, 2002), no study to date has obtained neural evidence for the hypothesis. Here we used

multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) of fMRI data to track context-related neural patterns as

participants studied word lists A and B. We presented images (passively viewed by the participants)

of outdoor scenes interspersed between the list A words, but not the list B words. Our goal was

to inject scene information into participants’ mental contexts during list A study and then to

assess (using MVPA) the extent to which scene information persisted during later points in the

experiment, when scenes were not displayed. We interpreted high levels of persistent scene

activity after list A as reflecting continued activation of the list A context, an approach we have

employed successfully in another context-dependent memory paradigm (Gershman et al., 2013).

We hypothesized that if participants “flush out” their list A mental context after forget cues, we

should see a corresponding sharp decrease in scene activity following the forget cue, and the size of

this decrease should negatively correlate with participants’ list A recall performance. In addition

to testing these core predictions, we also explored the neural correlates of context flushing more

generally, by looking for brain regions whose post-forget-cue activity predicted the size of the

contextual flush (as measured by our scene classifier).

3

Page 4: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Methods

Participants

Our functional neuroimaging study at Princeton University included 25 participants (10 male,

15 female, ages 19 to 34 (mean ± SEM: 21.3 ± 0.6 years). The participants were paid $20/hour

for their participation, and experimental testing sessions lasted approximately 1.5 hours. We

excluded one 19-year-old male participant from all of our analyses because his recall performance

was near perfect (and greater than 3 standard deviations above the mean performance) in all

of the experimental conditions (see Experimental paradigm), which limited our ability to observe

his behavioral and neural signatures of intentional forgetting. Our experimental protocol was

approved by Princeton’s institutional review board.

Experimental paradigm

Our experimental paradigm was organized into 8 study-test blocks and 1 localizer block. Each of

these blocks occurred during a distinct functional run (see Functional neuroimaging). Participants

were encouraged to take short breaks (while lying in the scanner) between each block.

[Figure 1 about here.]

In each study-test block (Figure 1a), participants viewed a central fixation cross for 3 s (not

shown in the figure), followed by a 3 s delay. They then studied a 16-word list, list A, followed

by an on-screen memory cue instruction telling them to either forget or remember the list A items.

Participants then studied a second 16-word list, list B. (See List construction for a detailed description

of how we generated the random word lists.) Finally, participants received an on-screen recall cue

instructing them to verbally recall either list A or list B (they were given 1 minute to recall the

words in any order they wished; see Audio recording for a description of how we recorded and

4

Page 5: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

scored the verbal responses). Prior to the start of the experiment, participants were told that, with

100% certainty, a forget instruction meant that they would be asked to recall list B on that block

(despite this instruction, we tested participants’ memory for list A on the final forget block as

described below). We also told participants (truthfully) that, if they instead received a remember

instruction, then they would be asked to recall either list A or list B, with equal probability.

Each list word appeared onscreen for 3 s, and the word presentations were separated by 3 s.

During the 3 s inter-word intervals between list A words, participants viewed 3 randomly chosen

images of outdoor scenes (presented for 1 s each in immediate succession). Crucially, scenes were

not presented during the inter-word intervals during list B study (instead, the screen was left

blank). Each scene image appeared only once during the entire experiment. Prior to the start of

the experiment we (truthfully) told participants that we would not test their memory for the scene

images, but that they should passively view the scene images when they appeared.

Each participant received a total of 4 remember instructions and 4 forget instructions, across

the 8 study-test blocks they experienced. The order in which participants experienced remember

or forget blocks was randomized independently for each participant, subject to the constraints

that (a) the same cue type could not appear in 3 successive blocks, and (b) the last block was

always a forget block. (These two constraints also meant that the second-to-last block was always

a remember block.) During two (randomly chosen) remember blocks, participants were asked to

recall the list A words, and on the remaining two remember blocks the participants were asked to

recall the list B words. Participants were asked to recall list B words on every forget block except

the last, when they were instead asked to recall the list A words. In other words, participants were

misled into believing they could forget the list A words during the last study-test block, but were

then nonetheless asked to recall list A. This allowed us to study the behavioral and neural e↵ects

of the forget instruction on the to-be-forgotten information.

5

Page 6: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

The localizer block (Figure 1b) occurred after the last study-test block. The localizer block

provided data for training pattern classifiers to track scene-related activity throughout the study-

test blocks. In this block, participants viewed images from three categories: outdoor scenes,

phase-scrambled scenes (described in the next paragraph), and everyday objects. The outdoor

natural scene images used in our experiment were selected from the Scene UNderstanding (SUN)

Database (Xiao et al., 2010) and the object images were selected from the Amsterdam Library of

Object Images (ALOI; Geusebroek et al., 2005). Each image was displayed for 500 ms followed

by a 1500 ms pause. Images were organized into 27 sets of 8 same-category images (9 sets per

category; the assignment of images to sets was done randomly for each participant). Each set

of 8 images was displayed (one at a time), followed by a 12 s pause before the next set of 8.

Participants performed a one-back task as they viewed the images, whereby they were instructed to

press a button on a handheld controller when an image exactly matched the image that preceded

it. (Repetitions occurred on 15% of the image presentations.)

Each of the phase-scrambled scene images was based on a photograph of a natural outdoor

scene (drawn from the SUN database). To create these images, we computed the 2-dimensional

fast Fourier transform of each color channel of the photograph (red, green, and blue intensities,

respectively). Within each color channel, we randomly perturbed the phase at each pixel (by

an amount between 0 and 2⇡ radians, chosen uniformly) and converted the channel back into

native space. The resulting phase-scrambled image retained the same spatial frequencies as the

original image, but was not (subjectively) identifiable as a scene. We included phase-scrambled

scene images in our localizer in order to distinguish patterns that reflected the frequency content

of scene images, independent of the images reflecting scenes per se.

6

Page 7: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

List construction

Each participant studied a total of sixteen 16-word lists (2 per block). All of the participants stud-

ied the same lists, but in a unique randomized order. Each list was assigned (randomly for each

participant) to one of the four experimental conditions. To construct the lists, we first drew 256

words uniformly at random from the Medical Research Council Psycholinguistic Database (Colt-

heart, 1981). We then constructed 16 lists that were matched according to word frequency (mean:

49.3, SD: 16.9), number of letters (mean: 5.4, SD: 0.3), number of syllables (mean: 1.7, SD: 0.1),

concreteness (mean: 540.3, SD: 12.0), and imageability (mean: 559.7, SD: 8.9). (These means and

standard deviations are computed across lists.)

Audio recording

Because the scanning environment is very loud, it can be di�cult to distinguish speech from

background noise. We recorded participants’ verbal recalls using a customized MR-compatible

recording system (FOMRI II, Optoacoustics Ltd.). The recording system employs two optical

microphones that are orthogonally oriented. One microphone captures both the participant’s

speech and the background noise, and the second (reference) microphone is intended to capture

only the background noise. We subtract the reference recording from the speech recording to

obtain a cleaner signal. We used the Penn TotalRecall tool (http://memory.psych.upenn.edu) to

score and annotate the verbal responses.

Functional neuroimaging

Imaging parameters

All participants were scanned using a Siemens Skyra 3 T full-body scanner (Siemens, Erlangen,

Germany) with a volume head coil. We collected, from each participant, 10 functional runs: 8

7

Page 8: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

study-test blocks and 1 localizer block (see Experimental paradigm), plus one additional run (in

which participants studied and recalled a list of 12 words prior to the localizer block) that we did

not examine in this paper. The functional runs comprised T2*-weighted gradient-echo echo-planar

(EPI) sequences (voxel size = 3⇥ 3⇥ 3 mm; repetition time [TR] = 2000 ms, echo time [TE] = 30 ms;

flip angle = 71�; matrix = 64⇥ 64; slices = 36; field of view [FoV] = 192 mm). We also collected, for

each participant, a single high-resolution T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition

gradient echo (MPRAGE) image to facilitate registration and normalization (voxel size = 1 ⇥ 1 ⇥ 1

mm; TE = 3.3 ms; flip angle = 7�; matrix = 256 ⇥ 256; slices = 176; FoV = 256 mm), and a single

fast low-angle shot (FLASH) field map to correct spatial distortions of the EPI images (voxel size

= 0.75⇥ 0.75⇥ 3 mm; TE = 2.6 ms; flip angle = 70�; matrix = 256⇥ 256; slices = 36; FoV = 192 mm).

Image preprocessing

We preprocessed the fMRI data using the FMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) Version 6.00, which

is part of FMRIB’s Software Library (FSL, http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). We removed the first

three brain volumes from each functional run to allow for T1 stabilization. We then applied

the following pre-statistics processing steps to the functional images: motion correction using

MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002); slice-timing correction using Fourier-space time-series phase-

shifting; non-brain removal using BET (Smith, 2002); spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel

of FWHM 5 mm (for the pattern classification analyses we did not spatially smooth the images);

grand-mean intensity normalization of the entire 4D dataset by a single multiplicative factor; and

high-pass temporal filtering (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting, with sigma =

64.0 s). We then used FLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001) to register each

participant’s functional images to standard (MNI) space.

8

Page 9: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA)

After pre-processing the fMRI data (without spatial smoothing), we used the Harvard-Oxford

cortical atlas (Desikan et al., 2006; Frazier et al., 2005; Goldstein et al., 2007; Makris et al., 2006)

to define a mask (using an inclusion threshold of 25%) consisting of the union of the posterior

and anterior parahippocampal gyrus; the posterior cingulate; and the anterior temporal, posterior

temporal, and temporal occipital fusiform (this mask was intended to encompass the parahip-

pocampal place area and retrosplenial cortex, as these regions have been previously implicated

in scene processing; Epstein et al., 1999; Epstein and Ward, 2010). We used the in-mask voxels to

train L2-regularized multinomial logistic regression classifiers using data from each participant’s

localizer block. (We trained the classifiers independently for each participant.) To account for the

6 s delay in the peak of the hemodynamic response function, we shifted the event labels forward in

time by 6 s (3 images), such that each brain volume was matched up with the event that occurred

6 seconds earlier. The multinomial classifiers were trained to discriminate when the participants

were viewing images of scenes versus everyday objects versus phase-scrambled scenes versus

rest (where “rest” was defined as the last 3 volumes collected during the 12 s pause between the

8-image blocks). We evaluated the classifiers’ abilities to estimate scene-related activity (versus

non-scene activity) using 9-fold cross validation applied to data from the localizer block (mean

area under the receiver operating characteristic curves across 24 participants ± SEM: 0.78 ± 0.006).

We used the trained classifiers to predict the degree of scene-related activity (ranging from 0 to 1,

inclusive) reflected in each brain volume collected during the study-test blocks.

The primary goal of our study was to test the hypothesis that participants respond to the

forget cue by flushing contextual information related to list A. This would manifest as a larger

decrease in scene-related activity following a forget cue than following a remember cue. We

predicted that the contextual flushing process would occur directly in response to the forget cue,

9

Page 10: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

even before participants began to study list B. We refer to this time interval (from the time of

the forget/remember instruction until the beginning of list B) as the critical period (Figure 1a).

We defined a measure called scene drop to quantify the degree of contextual flushing following a

memory cue. Scene drop was defined as the decrease in scene-related activity from just before

the critical period to the time after the critical period. Specifically, for each block, we took a pre-

critical-period measurement of scene activity (averaging over the interval beginning after the last

scene had been presented in list A and ending just before the forget/remember memory cue) and

subtracted out a post-critical-period measurement of scene activity (averaging over the interval

beginning when the first word in list B appeared onscreen and ending when the last word in list

B disappeared from the screen). Note that, as described above, we applied a 6 second shift in

matching up scene activity estimates to events (so, for example, the first scene activity estimate

assigned to the post-critical period was acquired 6 seconds after the beginning of list B study).

Importantly, we hypothesized that scene activity could decrease from list A to list B for two reasons:

(1) because scenes are no longer being viewed onscreen (this is true for both the remember and

forget conditions), and (2) because of “flushing out” of context information in the forget condition.

By taking our initial measurement of scene activity after the last of the list A scenes was presented,

we hoped to minimize the influence of the former factor (i.e., whether or not participants were

actually viewing scenes) and, consequently, to increase the sensitivity of our scene drop measure

to the “contextual flushing” process. We note, however, that our measure of scene drop likely

included some lingering traces of scene-related activity from the list A scene presentations, which

would add noise to our scene drop measure. Crucially, this activity should not exert a systematic

bias on our analyses because it should be equally present, on average, in the forget and remember

conditions.

10

Page 11: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Generalized linear modeling framework

We used generalized linear models (GLMs) in a multi-level framework to identify brain regions

whose responses (during either the critical period or list B) di↵ered according to whether partici-

pants had seen a forget or a remember cue (see Forget versus remember contrast analysis), and regions

that might contribute to contextual flushing during the critical period (see Contextual flushing

analysis).

We constructed, for each study-test block, three boxcar functions whose onsets and durations

followed the onsets and durations of list A, list B, and the critical period, respectively. We convolved

these boxcar functions with a canonical hemodynamic response function (a gamma kernel with a

phase shift of 0 s, lag mean of 6 s, and lag standard deviation of 3 s) to obtain GLM regressors. We

also included in our analysis separate regressors for motion parameters (see Image preprocessing),

corrected for local autocorrelations, and pre-whitened the data (using FILM). We used FEAT to fit a

GLM to each participant’s block-specific responses to each regressor, obtaining a map of per-voxel

weights for each regressor, for each study-test block, for each participant.

Forget versus remember contrast analysis

We used the forget versus remember contrast analysis to identify regions that responded di↵er-

entially following a forget cue versus following a remember cue (either during the critical period

or list B, as described above). In the critical period variant of this analysis, we examined the

di↵erences between the critical period regressor weights and the non-critical period (list A and B)

weights. This contrast identifies regions that are di↵erentially active in the critical period versus

studying lists A and B. In the list B variant of this analysis, we examined the di↵erences between

the list B weights and the list A weights. This contrast identifies regions that are di↵erentially

active during list B versus list A study. We obtained one critical period di↵erence map and one list

11

Page 12: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

B di↵erence map for each study-test block, for each participant.

For the critical period and list B di↵erence maps, respectively, we used FEAT to contrast the

maps from forget versus remember blocks. This resulted in a single critical period forget versus

remember contrast and a single list B forget versus remember contrast, for each participant. In

other words, we identified voxels whose critical period activity (versus baseline) or list B activity

(versus list A activity) was sensitive to the type of memory cue participants received. (The analysis

was performed in both directions so that we could identify either increases or decreases in activity

in response to forget versus remember cues.) We used FSL’s analysis of mixed e↵ects (FLAME)

tool to combine the participant-specific forget versus remember contrasts. We created whole-

brain statistical maps according to Gaussian random field theory (Worsley et al., 1992) with a

cluster-forming threshold of z > 2.3 and a (corrected) cluster significance threshold of p < 0.05.

Contextual flushing analysis

We used the contextual flushing analysis to identify regions whose critical period forget versus

remember contrast maps were correlated with the mean di↵erence in scene drop between forget

and remember blocks. As in the forget versus remember contrast analysis, we obtained a forget

versus remember contrast map for each participant’s critical period. We also computed, for each

participant, the di↵erence between the mean scene drop on forget blocks and the mean scene

drop on remember blocks. We called this di↵erence the contextual flushing score, reflecting that

participants with large scores exhibited more contextual flushing (scene drop) in response to forget

cues than remember cues. We then computed Pearson correlations (across participants) between

each voxel’s contrast values and the contextual flushing scores. We used the corresponding p-

values to create a map of z-scores (using the unit Normal-inverse transform). To ensure that our

analysis was sensitive to the directions of the correlations, we set the z-scores of all voxels with r < 0

12

Page 13: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

to 0. Finally, we used FSL’s SMOOTHEST and CLUSTER tools with a cluster-forming threshold

of z < �2.3 (corresponding to p < 0.01, uncorrected) and a corrected cluster significance threshold

of p < 0.05 to identify clusters of voxels that exhibited reliable correlations. (We also repeated the

analysis using only negative correlations, but found no reliable clusters; all results reported below

reflect positive correlations.)

Results

Figure 2a shows behavioral recall performance for list A and list B, as a function of whether

participants were given a forget or remember cue. Specifically, we asked whether participants

were better able to recall list B items following a forget cue (relative to following a remember cue)

and whether they were better able to recall list A items following a remember cue (relative to

following a forget cue). As described in the Introduction, these patterns reflect the benefit and cost

of directed forgetting, respectively. Qualitatively, we observed the standard benefits and costs of

directed forgetting. The benefit was significant, t(23) = 4.6, p = 0.0001, and the cost was trending

towards significance, t(23) = �1.7, p = 0.1 (note that we had a clear directional hypothesis here,

and the corresponding one-tailed p-value is 0.05).

[Figure 2 about here.]

We designed our study to test the hypothesis that participants respond to the forget instruc-

tion by flushing away traces of list A contextual information. By presenting images of scenes

interspersed between the list A words, we hoped that neural patterns that reflected those scenes

would be (passively) incorporated into participants’ list A contextual representations. If so, the

level of scene-related neural activity throughout the remainder of the experiment (as estimated by

pattern classifiers trained on data from a di↵erent functional run; see Multivariate pattern analysis)

13

Page 14: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

should reflect currently active traces of list A context. Supporting the hypothesis that participants

flush out list A contextual information following a forget cue, we found that participants exhibited

substantially lower levels of scene activity during list B following a forget cue than following a

remember cue (t(23) = �3.0, p = 0.006; Figure 2b).

Although the order in which participants experienced the experimental conditions was ran-

domized with respect to memory cue and recall instruction for the first 6 study-test blocks, study-

test blocks 7 and 8 were always a remember and forget block, respectively, and participants always

recalled list A in the final block. To ensure that the observed di↵erence in scene activity during

list B was not due to order confounds, we carried out a control analysis to verify that the forget

versus remember scene activity di↵erence shown in Figure 2 also held for the first 6 study-test

blocks alone; this was indeed the case (scene-related activity during list B following a forget versus

remember cue: t(23) = �3.0, p = 0.006).

If participants use currently active contextual representations to probe their memory, then the

degree to which participants flush out list A context in response to the memory cue should be

negatively correlated with their ability to recall list A items (when asked to do so on list A blocks).

Crucially, our theory predicts that the relationship between scene drop and list A recall should be

most visible on forget blocks (where flushing is hypothesized to occur); on remember blocks, if

participants are not flushing, there may not be enough variance to detect a relationship between

scene drop and list A recall. In keeping with these ideas, we found that scene drop and number

of list A recalls were reliably correlated (across participants) following a forget cue (Figure 3a;

r = �0.5, p = 0.02), but not following a remember cue (Figure 3b; r = �0.1, p = 0.6).

[Figure 3 about here.]

By showing that participants exhibited lower levels of scene-related activity following a forget

cue, and that the magnitude of scene drop was negatively correlated with participants’ list A recall

14

Page 15: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

performance, the above analyses provide neural support for the contextual flushing account of

list-method directed forgetting. The TR-by-TR scene activity estimates also provide a means of

observing the detailed timecourse of the response to the memory cue after list A (Figure 2c). We

observed that the scene activations were relatively high as participants studied list A words (and

passively viewed scene images between the word presentations), and then began to decrease fol-

lowing the last scene presentation. The levels of scene-related activity began to diverge (according

to which memory cue participants had received on that block) just after the cue’s appearance on

the screen, after accounting for the 6 s lag in the peak of the canonical hemodynamic response

function. The scene activation traces leveled o↵ just prior to the start of list B, running roughly

parallel to each other as participants studied list B. The timecourse suggests that participants flush

out traces of list A context during the critical period beginning just after the forget cue appears and

ending just prior to the start of list B.

Critical period GLM analyses

We used our GLM framework to attempt to identify regions whose critical period responses varied

with memory cue type (forget versus remember). However, no regions survived our inclusion

criteria when we performed a simple forget versus remember contrast (see Forget versus remember

contrast analysis). (We looked for regions that showed either reliable increases or reliable decreases

in activity following a forget versus a remember instruction.) One possible explanation for this null

finding is that neural patterns during the critical period result from many (potentially competing)

neural processes that introduce unexplained variability into the signal. If so, teasing out responses

specifically related to contextual flushing might provide a more sensitive means of identifying

brain regions involved in flushing list A context during the critical period. We carried out an

across-participants analysis whereby we correlated the contrast of each participant’s critical period

15

Page 16: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

regressor weights during forget versus remember blocks with the contrast of each participant’s

scene drop scores on forget versus remember blocks (see Contextual flushing analysis). The analysis

revealed a network of regions including bilateral insular cortex, right intraparietal sulcus, bilateral

anterior and right posterior cingulate, right superior temporal sulcus, and right angular gyrus

(Figure 4a). As mentioned in the Discussion, this network includes several regions previously

implicated in processing event boundaries (Ezzyat and Davachi, 2014; Speer et al., 2007; Zacks

et al., 2010). The full set of clusters that we identified as contributing to contextual flushing during

the critical period are summarized in Table 1.

Because these regions were identified based on their correlation with scene drop [i.e., they were

regions whose changes in activity during forget blocks (versus remember blocks) were correlated

with the degree to which scene activity decreased in forget blocks (versus remember blocks)], it

is important to consider the possibility that these regions are simply involved in thinking about

scenes, as opposed to a more general process of contextual flushing. (To be clear, given the

directionality of our results as described above, these would have to be regions that activate more

when people think less about scenes.) While we can not rule this out definitively, we think it is

unlikely that these areas were engaged merely because of their sensitivity to scenes. For example,

none of the areas listed in Table 1 were significantly activated (or deactivated) in a simple GLM

analysis of the localizer data contrasting scenes vs. non-scenes (scrambled scenes, objects).

[Figure 4 about here.]

[Table 1 about here.]

List B GLM analysis

Prior neural studies of list-method directed forgetting have focused on identifying regions that

are di↵erentially engaged during list B study after forget versus remember cues (e.g. Bauml et al.,

16

Page 17: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

2008; Hanslmayr et al., 2012). We carried out an analysis similar to that of Hanslmayr et al.

(2012) whereby we contrasted the list B responses on forget versus remember blocks (using list A

responses as a baseline; see Forget versus remember contrast analysis). As shown in Figure 4b, we

found a robust increase in activity during list B following a forget instruction in the left dlPFC (the

clusters we found overlapped with 5 of the 6 cue-responsive cluster peaks identified by Hanslmayr

et al., 2012, in their Table 1 and Figure 4). Our analysis also revealed increased activations following

forget instructions in bilateral superior parietal lobule and angular gyrus, left middle and inferior

frontal gyrus, left precuneus, and left superior and middle temporal gyrus [Table 2 provides a full

list of the clusters that exhibited reliable cue-specific responses during list B; note that the regions

listed in the table reflect the clusters’ centers of gravity (defined as the z-score–weighted average

of the in-cluster voxel locations), whereas the preceding list also reflects other regions over which

the clusters extend their mass as shown in the figure]. No regions showed reliable decreases in

activity following a forget instruction.

[Table 2 about here.]

Discussion

We sought to test the hypothesis that participants can intentionally forget information pertaining to

previously experienced events by flushing out contextual information associated with those events.

When participants studied a list of words (list A), we “injected” scene information into their mental

context by presenting scene images between the words. We then used pattern classifiers to estimate

the level of scene-related activity as participants studied a second list of words (list B), manipulating

whether participants were asked to forget (or remember) the list A words prior to studying list

B. We found that the levels of scene-related activity measured during list B were reliably lower

17

Page 18: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

following a forget instruction than following a remember instruction. Across participants, the

magnitude of the decrease in scene activity in response to a forget instruction (“scene drop”) was

negatively correlated with participants’ list A recall performance on that block. These results

provide the first neural support for the hypothesis that we can intentionally forget by flushing out

contextual information associated with old events.

Brain regions associated with contextual flushing and their roles in processing event

boundaries

To identify brain regions associated with the contextual flushing process, we looked for regions

where the di↵erence in critical period activity during forget versus remember blocks was correlated

with the size of the contextual flushing e↵ect (i.e., the degree of di↵erential scene drop in response

to forget versus remember instructions). The analysis identified reliable correlations in bilateral

insular cortex, right intraparietal sulcus, bilateral anterior and right posterior cingulate, right

superior temporal sulcus, right angular gyrus, and left precuneus (Figure 4a and Table 1; note that

the regions listed in the table reflect the clusters’ centers of gravity, whereas the preceding list also

reflects other regions over which the clusters extend their mass as shown in the figure).

Previous work has shown that these regions also display an increased response when people

encounter event boundaries, or transitions from one event (e.g. eating dinner) to another (e.g.

washing dishes; Ezzyat and Davachi, 2014; Speer et al., 2007; Zacks et al., 2010, e.g. compare our

Figure 4a with Zacks et al., 2010 Figure 1). Although the list-method directed forgetting paradigm

is, in many ways, very di↵erent from our everyday experiences, there is evidence to suggest that

something similar to the contextual flushing we observed in our study (in response to the forget

instruction) may also occur at event boundaries. For example, a number of studies have found that,

for a fixed time interval, people will forget more information (from before the interval to after it)

18

Page 19: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

if that interval contains an event boundary (Ezzyat and Davachi, 2011; Radvansky and Copeland,

2006; Swallow et al., 2011, 2009). These boundary e↵ects have been explained in terms of the idea

that, during an event, participants activate situation models that store knowledge relevant to that

particular situation (Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012). When a new event begins, Zacks et al. (2007)

and others have argued that participants flush out the old situation model and activate a new

situation model. To the extent that situation models constitute part of the participant’s mental

context, this switching of situation models acts as a context change that can impede participants’

access to information from the previous event. We hypothesize that participants treat the forget

cue in list-method directed forgetting like an event boundary of sorts, triggering a corresponding

flushing away of list A context, as if list A were part of a no-longer-relevant situation. By contrast,

if participants encounter a remember cue, it is as though they treat studying lists A and B as a

single event or situation (for additional discussion of this idea see Sahakyan and Kelley, 2002).

Relation to prior neural studies of directed forgetting

Whereas the focus in our study was on the evoked response to the forget cue, other recent work

has focused on how neural activity during list B study varies as a function of whether participants

are given a forget or remember cue. In particular, Hanslmayr et al. (2012) and Bauml et al.

(2008) found EEG desynchronization e↵ects during list B that were larger following forget cues

than remember cues. These desynchronization e↵ects were correlated (across subjects) with the

behavioral cost of directed forgetting (Bauml et al., 2008) and were accompanied by an increase in

dlPFC activity (Hanslmayr et al., 2012). Further, stimulating the dlPFC with transcranial magnetic

stimulation (TMS) increased the magnitude of these desynchronization e↵ects and led to increased

list A forgetting. Taken together, the authors interpret these findings as showing that active control

processes as participants study list B contribute to list A forgetting.

19

Page 20: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Although our study was not in a position to replicate the above EEG and TMS e↵ects, we

did replicate the finding that the dlPFC increases its activity during list B following a forget cue

(Figure 4b), providing converging support for Hanslmayr et al. and Bauml et al.’s accounts. Our

results extend these findings by also looking at the evoked response to the forget cue prior to list B

and by relating the post-forget cue responses to contextual representations (Figure 2, Figure 4a). We

found that the neural signature of contextual flushing following a forget cue predicts participants’

abilities to later recall list A (Figure 3a) and that contextual flushing was associated with a di↵erent

set of brain regions (that have been previously linked with parsing event boundaries, as discussed

above) than the set of regions identified by Hanslmayr et al. (2012).

Taken together, our study and these previous studies suggest that the process of shifting one’s

mental context has multiple components. After being told to forget list A, event boundary regions

respond to the forget cue by flushing out no-longer relevant contextual information associated

with list A. Speculatively, the desynchronization e↵ect described by Hanslmayr et al. (2012) and

Bauml et al. (2008) may reflect loading new stimulus features (e.g. those associated with list B) into

context.

One last puzzle is how to reconcile our results (showing that scene drop, measured prior to

list B study, is associated with impaired list A recall) with other results showing that study of

list B items is needed to induce directed forgetting (e.g. Bjork, 1989; Pastotter and Bauml, 2007,

but see Unsworth et al., 2012). One possibility is that participants can recover from flushing

list A context (by reinstating list A context) if they are tested immediately after the “flush,” but

this recovery becomes more di�cult once participants have loaded in a new contextual state that

incorporates list B items (Lehman and Malmberg, 2009; Sahakyan et al., 2013).

20

Page 21: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Concluding remarks

We used a simple list-learning paradigm to elucidate the neural mechanisms underlying how

we intentionally forget. Our work highlights the fundamental role that contextual information

plays in our ability to organize and retrieve information pertaining to previous experiences, and

shows that one tool we use to forget information about our recent past is to flush out contextual

information associated with those old experiences.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the John Templeton Foundation and by NIH R01

MH069456. J.R.M. was supported by the NSF Collaborative Research in Computational Neuro-

science Program, grant IIS-1009542. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does

not necessarily represent the o�cial views of our supporting organizations. We also acknowledge

useful discussions with Mariam Aly, Janice Chen, Megan deBettencourt, J. Benjamin Hutchinson,

Talia Manning, and Anna Schapiro.

References

Anderson, M. C. and Hanslmayr, S. (2014). Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 18(6):279 – 292.

Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W., Glover, G. H.,

and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories.

Science, 303(5655):232 – 235.

21

Page 22: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Bauml, K.-H. (2008). Inhibitory processes. In Roediger, H. L., editor, Cognitive psychology of memory.

Oxford: Elsevier.

Bauml, K.-H., Hanslmayr, S., Pastotter, B., and Klimesch, W. (2008). Oscillatory correlates of

intentional updating in episodic memory. NeuroImage, 21:596 – 604.

Bjork, R. A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In Roediger,

H. L. and Craik, F. I. M., editors, Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel

Tulving, pages 309–330. Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 33(4):497 – 505.

Desikan, R., Segonne, B., Fischl, B., Quinn, B., Dickerson, B., Blacker, D., Buckner, R. L., Dale,

A., Maguire, A., Hyman, B., Albert, M., and Killiany, N. (2006). An automated labeling system

for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest.

NeuroImage, 31(3):968–80.

Epstein, R., Harris, A., Stanley, D., and Kanwisher, N. (1999). The Parahippocampal Place Area.

Neuron, 23(1):115–125.

Epstein, R. and Ward, E. (2010). How reliable are visual context e↵ects in the parahippocampal

place area? Cerebral Cortex, 20(2):294.

Ezzyat, Y. and Davachi, L. (2011). What constitutes an episode in episodic memory? Psychological

Science, 22(2):243–252.

Ezzyat, Y. and Davachi, L. (2014). Similarity breeds proximity: Pattern similarity within and across

contexts is related to later mnemonic judgments of temporal proximity. Neuron, 81(5):1179–1189.

22

Page 23: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Frazier, J. A., Chiu, S., Breeze, J. L., Makris, N., Lange, N., Kennedy, D. N., Herbert, M. R., Bent, E. K.,

Koneru, V. K., Dieterich, M. E., Hodge, S. M., Rauch, S. L., Grant, P. E., Cohen, B. M., Seidman,

L. J., Caviness, V. S., and Biederman, J. (2005). Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging

of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,

162(7):1256 – 1265.

Gershman, S. J., Schapiro, A. C., Hupbach, A., and Norman, K. A. (2013). Neural context reinstate-

ment predicts memory misattribution. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(20):8590 – 8595.

Geusebroek, J. M., Burghouts, G. J., and Smeulders, A. W. M. (2005). The Amsterdam Library of

Object Images. International Journal of Computer Vision, 61(1):103 – 112.

Goldstein, J. M., Seidman, L. J., Makris, N., Ahern, T., O’Brien, L. M., Caviness, V. S., Kennedy,

D. N., Faraone, S. V., and Tsuang, M. T. (2007). Hypothalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia:

sex e↵ects and genetic vulnerability. Biological Psychiatry, 61(8):935 – 945.

Hanslmayr, S., Volberg, G., Wimber, M., Oehler, N., Staudigl, T., Hartmann, T., Raabe, M., Greenlee,

M. W., and Bauml, K.-H. T. (2012). Prefrontally driven downregulation of neural synchrony

mediates goal-directed forgetting. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(42):14742 – 14751.

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimisation for the robust

and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage, 17(2):825–

841.

Jenkinson, M. and Smith, S. M. (2001). A global optimisation method for robust a�ne registration

of brain images. Medical Image Analysis, 5(2):143 – 156.

Lehman, M. and Malmberg, K. J. (2009). A global theory of remembering and forgetting from

multilple lists. Journal Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cogntion, 35(4):970–988.

23

Page 24: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Makris, N., Goldstein, J. M., Kennedy, D., Hodge, S. M., Caviness, V. S., Faraone, S. V., Tsuang,

M. T., and Seidman, L. J. (2006). Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in

schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 83(2-3):155 – 171.

Manning, J. R., Norman, K. A., and Kahana, M. J. (2015). The role of context in episodic memory.

In Gazzaniga, M., editor, The Cognitive Neurosciences, Fifth edition. MIT Press.

Pastotter, B. and Bauml, K.-H. (2007). The crucial role of post-cue encoding in directed forget-

ting and context-dependent forgetting. Journal Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and

Cogntion, 33:977 – 982.

Radvansky, G. A. and Copeland, D. E. (2006). Walking through doorways causes forgetting:

situation models and experienced space. Memory & Cognition, 34(5):1150 – 1156.

Ranganath, C. and Ritchey, M. (2012). Two cortical systems for memory-guided behavior. Nature

Reviews Neuroscience, 13:713 – 726.

Sahakyan, L., Delaney, P. F., Foster, N. L., and Abushanab, B. (2013). List-method directed forgetting

in cognitive and clinical research: a theoretical and methodological review. Psychology of Learning

and Motivation, 59:131 – 189.

Sahakyan, L. and Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting

e↵ect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6):1064–1072.

Smith, S. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping, 17(3):143–155.

Speer, N. K., Zacks, J. M., and Reynolds, J. R. (2007). Human brain activity time-locked to narrative

event boundaries. Psychological Science, 8(5):449 – 455.

Swallow, K. M., Barch, D. M., Head, D., Maley, C. J., Holder, D., and Zacks, J. M. (2011). Changes

24

Page 25: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

in events alter how people remember recent information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,

23(5):1052 – 1064.

Swallow, K. M., Zacks, J. M., and Abrams, R. A. (2009). Event boundaries in perception a↵ect

memory encoding and updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(2):236 – 257.

Unsworth, N., Brewer, G. A., and Spillers, G. J. (2012). Variation in cognitive failures: An individual

di↵erences investigation of everyday attention and memory failures. Journal of Memory and

Language, 67(1):1 – 16.

Van Essen, D. C., Drury, H. A., Dickenson, J., Harwell, J., Hanlon, D., and Anderson, C. H. (2001).

An integrated software suite for surface-based analysis of cerebral cortex. Journal of the American

Medical Informatics Association, 8:443 – 459.

Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., Marrett, S., and Neelin, P. (1992). A three-dimensional statistical

analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism,

12:900.

Xiao, J., Hays, J., Ehinger, K., Oliva, A., and Torralba, A. (2010). SUN database: Large-scale scene

recognition from abbey to zoo. In IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.

Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K., Swallow, K. M., Braver, T. S., and Reynolds, J. R. (2007). Event perception:

a mind-brain perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 133:273 – 293.

Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K., Swallow, K. M., and Maley, C. J. (2010). The brain’s cutting-room floor:

segmentation of narrative cinema. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4(168):1 – 15.

25

Page 26: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

1 3 5 7 15 29 31 33 43 309 311 313 323

Localiz

er b

lock

MATCH!

Time (s)

Stu

dy-te

st b

lock

SPRING <LIST B> “spring” “kid” “builder”DIRT KID BUILDER<FORGET>FRAME

91 97 105 111 117 195 201 214 217 231

TEACHER

1 4 5 6

CHINA

7 10 11 12

PASSAGE

85

Critical periodStudy list A Study list B Recall

Study-test block 1 Study-test block 2 Study-test block 8 Localizer block

Sum

mary

Use localizer data

to train pattern

classifiers

b.

a.88 89 90

Figure 1: Methods overview. a. Study-test block. During each study-test block, participants first studied list A while passively viewingimages of outdoor scenes between the word presentations. They then received a memory cue instructing them to either forget orremember the list A words. Next they studied list B (without viewing scene images between the word presentations). Finally they wereinstructed to verbally recall as many words (from either list A or list B) as they could remember in 1 minute. Participants experienced atotal of 8 study-test blocks during the experiment. b. Localizer block. During each localizer block, participants viewed images drawnfrom three categories: outdoor scenes, phase-scrambled scenes, and everyday objects. Participants were instructed to press a button ona handheld control pad when an image exactly matched the image that preceded it. Data from this block were used to train patternclassifiers to estimate scene-related activity during the study-test blocks. A summary of the entire experiment, comprising 8 study-testblocks and the localizer block, is shown at the bottom of the figure; blue indicates list B study following a forget cue, and red indicateslist B study following a remember cue.

26

Page 27: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

Study list A Study list B

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

Scen

e-re

late

d ac

tivity

Recall list A Recall list B

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

Cor

rect

reca

lls (%

)

p = 0.0001

p = 0.1

Remember list AForget list A

a. b. c.

Time (TRs)

Scen

e-re

late

d ac

tivity

20 40 60 80 100

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

p = 0.006

Figure 2: Identifying a neural signature of intentional forgetting. a. Memory performance. The proportion of correctly recalledwords is shown for each of the four experimental conditions. b. Average scene activity. The bars display the mean level of scene-relatedactivity measured by the classifier as participants studied lists A and B (the mean is taken across blocks and participants, and reflectsthe entire study period for each list). The list B blocks are divided according to whether participants received a forget cue (blue) or aremember cue (red) following list A. The p-values in Panels a and b are from two-tailed across-participant paired t-tests. c. Time course

of scene-related activity. The ribbon plots display the mean level of scene-related activity measured by the classifier as each brainvolume was collected (where the mean is taken across participants). The vertical lines denote the times of experimental events (shiftedforward by 6 s to account for hemodynamic lag): the time the last scene disappeared from the screen during list A (cyan), the time thememory cue appeared on the screen (magenta), and the time the first list B word appeared on the screen (brown). The green shadingdenotes the critical period (Figure 1a). Error bars in Panels a and b and line thicknesses in Panel c denote ±1 SEM, taken across the 24participants.

27

Page 28: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

b.a.

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.80

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

Scene drop

Cor

rect

list

A re

calls

Forget list A

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.80

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

Scene drop

Cor

rect

list

A re

calls

Remember list A

r = -0.5p = 0.02

r = -0.1p = 0.6

Figure 3: Flushing list A context leads to fewer list A recalls on forget blocks. a. Forget list A

blocks. The x-coordinate of each dot indicates, for a single participant, the degree of contextualflushing [as measured by the scene drop (see Multivariate pattern analysis), for blocks where theparticipants were told to forget list A and then nonetheless were asked to recall list A]. The dots’y-coordinates reflect the numbers of list A items the participants recalled during those blocks. b.

Recall list A blocks. This panel is in the same format as Panel a, but reflects blocks on whichparticipants were asked to remember list A and then asked to later recall list A. (Each dot reflectsan average across all remember/recall list A blocks for one participant.) The correlations reportedin each panel are computed across participants.

28

Page 29: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

a.

b.

Critical period

List B (F vs R)

2.3 4.0

Figure 4: GLM results. a. Map of regions that contributed to contextual flushing during the

critical period. The coloring highlights regions for which the di↵erence (between forget versusremember blocks) in the critical period contrast (see Generalized linear modeling framework) wasreliably correlated (across participants) with the di↵erence in contextual flushing (measured usingscene drop) between forget versus remember blocks. Table 1 provides a full list of the clusterswe identified using this analysis. b. List B forget versus remember cue contrast. The coloringhighlights regions where the di↵erence in list B versus list A regressor weights was reliably larger(across participants) following a forget cue than following a remember cue. Table 2 provides a fulllist of the clusters we identified using this analysis. The inflated brain maps in both panels werecreated using the Computerized Anatomical Reconstruction and Editing Toolkit (CARET), version5.65 (Van Essen et al., 2001).

29

Page 30: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

x (mm) y (mm) z (mm) Location Volume (voxels) Peak z statistic20 -86 12 R. postcentral gyrus 641 3.46

50 -32 20R. parietal operculum cortexR. planum temporaleR. supramarginal gyrus

435 3.77

6 -2 40 R. cingulate gyrus 418 3.33-44 -4 16 L. central opercular cortex 366 3.8226 0 14 R. putamen 335 4.2

-52 -30 22L. parietal operculum cortexL. planum temporaleL. supramarginal gyrus

136 3.31

18 -18 56 R. precentral gyrus 130 3.38

Table 1: List of regions that contribute to contextual flushing during the critical period. Thecoordinates and location names reflect the centers of mass of each cluster (in MNI 152 space).The location names were determined using the Harvard-Oxford Cortical Structural Atlas and theHarvard-Oxford Subcortical Structural Atlas. A map displaying the identified regions is shown inFigure 4a.

30

Page 31: A neural signature of intentional forgetting via contextual flushing

x (mm) y (mm) z (mm) Location Volume (voxels) Peak z statistic

-38 18 29 L. middle frontal gyrusL. inferior frontal gyrus 3081 5.12

-31 -54 47 L. superior parietal lobuleL. angular gyrus 1952 3.98

21 -66 -25 R. cerebellum 1553 3.97

-32 1 -34 L. anterior parahippocampal gyrusL. anterior fusiform cortex 634 4.14

-16 -5 7 L. pallidumL. thalamus 525 3.69

-57 -43 5 L. middle temporal gyrus 510 3.66

-43 -50 -25 L. fusiform cortexL. inferior temporal gyrus 452 4.17

31 -53 46 R. superior parietal lobuleR. angular gyrus 347 3.56

Table 2: List of regions that responded reliably more following a forget cue during list B. Thecoordinates and location names reflect the centers of mass of each cluster (in MNI 152 space).The location names were determined using the Harvard-Oxford Cortical Structural Atlas and theHarvard-Oxford Subcortical Structural Atlas. A map displaying the identified regions is shown inFigure 4b.

31