a new test for assessing preschool motor development: dial-3 · a new test for assessing preschool...

17
ADAPTED PHYSICALACTIVITY QUARTERLY, 2000,17,78-94 O 2000 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. A New Test for Assessing Preschool Motor Development: DIAL-3 Carol Mardell-Czudnowski Northern Illinois University Dorothea S. Goldenberg University of Illinois, Chicago Recent research and legislation in the United States regarding assessment of preschool children have guided the development of the latest version of the Developmental Indicators for the Assessment of Learning, DIAL-3. This pa- per briefly describes the history of this test's previous two versions (DIAL, 1975 and DIAL-R, 1983, 1990) followed by a description of the research and development of the motor items in DIAL-3. Then DIAL-3 is evaluated, using the key features for selecting an appropriate preschool gross motor assess- ment instrument (Zittel, 1994). DIAL-3 meets all of the common criteria for a technically adequate screening test. Accurate motor assessment of young children with special needs is neces- sary for quality intervention.According to the Individuals with Disabilities Educa- tion Act (IDEA), this assessment should be completed by a multidisciplinary team (Federal Register, 1997). Special educators, including adapted physical educators and related service providers such as physical therapists, need to know about the best instruments available for the screening and diagnosis of motoric problems in young children. Developmental screening should identify children who are at risk for devel- oping various learning or behavior problems (Gredler, 1992; 1997). The term at risk is widely used but ambiguous because risk is not static, standardized tests generally are not effective predictors of risk, and children are not isolated entities but develop within an ecological context (Gredler, 1992;Hrncir & Eisenhart, 1991; Keogh & Speece, 1996).Additionally, Keogh and Speece (1996) found protective factors, either within the child or the environment, that may compensate or out- weigh the risks. Due to these problems, the concept of who is at risk varies. An- other term often used as the end goal of screening is developmental delay. McLean, Smith, McCormick, Schakel, and McEvoy (1991) define this term as a condition Carol Mardell-Czudnowski is Professor Emeritus, Department of Educational Psy- chology, Counseling, and Special Education, Northern Illinois University. Dorothea S. Goldenberg is with the Department of Psychiatry, Institute for Juvenile Research at the University of Illinois, Chicago.

Upload: lebao

Post on 04-Sep-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

ADAPTED PHYSICAL ACTIVITY QUARTERLY, 2000,17,78-94 O 2000 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

A New Test for Assessing Preschool Motor Development: DIAL-3

Carol Mardell-Czudnowski Northern Illinois University

Dorothea S. Goldenberg University of Illinois, Chicago

Recent research and legislation in the United States regarding assessment of preschool children have guided the development of the latest version of the Developmental Indicators for the Assessment of Learning, DIAL-3. This pa- per briefly describes the history of this test's previous two versions (DIAL, 1975 and DIAL-R, 1983, 1990) followed by a description of the research and development of the motor items in DIAL-3. Then DIAL-3 is evaluated, using the key features for selecting an appropriate preschool gross motor assess- ment instrument (Zittel, 1994). DIAL-3 meets all of the common criteria for a technically adequate screening test.

Accurate motor assessment of young children with special needs is neces- sary for quality intervention. According to the Individuals with Disabilities Educa- tion Act (IDEA), this assessment should be completed by a multidisciplinary team (Federal Register, 1997). Special educators, including adapted physical educators and related service providers such as physical therapists, need to know about the best instruments available for the screening and diagnosis of motoric problems in young children.

Developmental screening should identify children who are at risk for devel- oping various learning or behavior problems (Gredler, 1992; 1997). The term at risk is widely used but ambiguous because risk is not static, standardized tests generally are not effective predictors of risk, and children are not isolated entities but develop within an ecological context (Gredler, 1992; Hrncir & Eisenhart, 1991; Keogh & Speece, 1996). Additionally, Keogh and Speece (1996) found protective factors, either within the child or the environment, that may compensate or out- weigh the risks. Due to these problems, the concept of who is at risk varies. An- other term often used as the end goal of screening is developmental delay. McLean, Smith, McCormick, Schakel, and McEvoy (1991) define this term as a condition

Carol Mardell-Czudnowski is Professor Emeritus, Department of Educational Psy- chology, Counseling, and Special Education, Northern Illinois University. Dorothea S. Goldenberg is with the Department of Psychiatry, Institute for Juvenile Research at the University of Illinois, Chicago.

Preschool Motor Development 79

which represents a significant delay in the process of development. The child is not slightly or momentarily lagging in development. Rather, development is sig- nificantly affected and, without special intervention, it is likely that educational performance at school age will be affected. DIAL-3 is designed to identify both of these populations of young children, keeping in mind the importance of the suc- ceeding steps to diagnostically verify thedegree of risk or delay and its impact on future developmental growth as well as academic programming (Mardell- Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Accurate motor assessment requires instruments that meet key criteria. The purpose of this paper is to describe ;specific test and evaluate it according to the six criteria identified by Zittel (1994). Although this test covers the five develop- mental areas (motor, cognition, language, psychosocial, and self-help skills) man- dated in the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA; Federal Register, 1997), this paper will be limited to the motor skills component.

Historical Development of DIAL and DIAL-R

A brief history of the first two versions, DIAL (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1975) and DIAL-R (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1983, 1990) will be given since each new version was built upon the previous one(s). More than a quarter of a century ago, in 1971, the Illinois legislature passed two bills that required the de- velopment of a screening procedure that would identify young children with either current or potential learning problems (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972). To meet this challenge, the Illinois State Board of Education funded, through federal assis- tance for the education of handicapped children (Title VI, ESEA), a project that resulted in the development and standardization of a screening test known as DIAL (Developmental Indicators for the Assessment of Learning).

The initial set of procedures and items (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) was developed by project staff and approved by a 20-member advisory board on the basis of 10 criteria that were found in some tests, but no test at that time met all ten. DIAL was designed to (a) be a screening test rather than a diagnostic test (rela- tively short, of a surface nature, and indicate the possibility of a variance in devel- opment); (b) cover the age range of 2.5 to 5.5 years; (c) be administered individually, since this is the only way that gross motor skills, articulation, and receptive lan- guage can be assessed adequately, but in a group setting that simulates a typical preschool or kindergarten classroom; (d) take about 30 min due to the frequent difficulty of maintaining a young child's attention on activities that are not of his or her choosing and the need to screen many children; (e) be multidimensional (cover many areas of development); (f) be noncategorical and attempt to identify at risk children regardless of the reason for the potential learning problem; (g) be scored on the basis of observable performance rather than the subjective opinion of the tester; (h) be process-oriented as well as product-oriented (i.e., evaluate how a child does something in addition to the end result or score); (i) be applicable to culturally different populations; and (j) be normed on a large stratified sample. Thus, DIAL was normed on 4,356 children across the State of Illinois in the spring of 1972, stratified by sex, race, size of the community, and socioeconomic level. DIAL met all of the 10 criteria according to three independent external evaluators (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972). Materials were designed to meet the screening criteria with some items presented one at a time on a format that resembles a

80 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

telephone dial to reduce distraction. This format has remained as a unique feature of all DIAL tests.

The theoretical basis for DIAL. and hence subseauent versions. was eclec- tic. Past and current theories and research findings were taken into consideration, but the major thrust was examining what was being used in the early 70s to iden- tify young children with special needs. There were many tests for young children that preceded DIAL. Ninety-four instruments used for the identification of prekindergarten high risk children were analyzed (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972, 1973) on the basis of age range (2-6 years); depth (screening or diagnostic); ad- ministrative factors (individual vs. group, test time, timed or untimed items, quali- fications of person giving it); type of response (vocal or motoric); performance factors (auditory discrimination, articulation, language, developmental, visual per- ception, motor, school readiness, social skills, self-concepts, conceptual skills); and whether the tests were measurements requiring subjective judgment (rating scale, interview, observation). From this analysis, six areas of behaviors were iden- tified to be screened: sensory, motor, affective, social, conceptual, and language. Normed data from the items within the tests were plotted to assist in determining appropriate age ranges for each task.

The theoretical antecedents of motoric items for DIAL were based on the early research by Gesell and Thompson (1938) and McGraw (1943), who had established norms for motor skill development. Gesell (1952) postulated that all mental life probably has a motor basis and a motor origin. Kephart (1960) labeled the first interaction of a child and his environment as motor activity. Many re- searchers (Cratty, 1967; Dunsing & Kephart, 1965; Kephart, 1960) believed that specific motor controls are a necessity for learning. By exploring and interacting with the environment, a child receives stimulation and processes these responsive patterns into learned actions. Thus, the importance of motor development became an assessment priority in the development of DIAL. The delay of motor learning and motor skills was viewed as a significant factor in the identification of children with potential learning problems (Barsch & Rudell, 1962; Bryant, 1964; Karlin, 1957).

Motor development items on DIAL were formatted into two areas for ease of assessment, gross motor and fine motor (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1975). Gross motor behaviors were assessed more easily when the child was in an upright posi- tion, whereas fine motor behaviors were measured more easily while the child was sitting at a table. In the gross motor area, tasks of throwing, catching, jumping, hopping, skipping, standing still, and balancing were assessed. In the fine motor area, tasks of visual matching, block building, cutting, copying of shapes and let- ters, finger touching, and hand clapping were assessed. Justification and develop- mental norms for all of these tasks were found in the literature (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972), but there were no established norms on previous tests for the standing still item. In addition, all of the gross and fine motor tasks were statisti- cally analyzed and found to be developmental (i.e., indicative of increased levels of motor coordination as children grew older; Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972,1975). Research conducted during the next 2 years confirmed the validity and reliability of the items and procedures (Hall, Mardell, Wick, & Goldenberg, 1976). Addi- tional validity and reliability studies are described elsewhere (Mardell-Czudnowslu, 1980; Wright & Masters, 1982).

Preschool Motor Development 81

DIAL was revised in 1983 as DIAL-R (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1983). The revision maintained those features that were most valuable for screen- ing young children while improving the predictability of the screening results. The previous criteria were retained but others were added. Three changes in particular affected every aspect of DIAL-R construction: (a) DIAL-R was standardized on a national sample, whereas the DIAL standardization sample had been restricted to the state of Illinois; (b) DIAL-R extended the age range to ages 2.0 to 6.0, whereas the age range of DIAL was from 2.5 to 5.5; and (c) DIAL-R combined the gross motor and fine motor items of DIAL into one area, motor. This change was made for two reasons: (a) it reduced the weight of motoric items in the total score from one half to one third, so it was comparable to the weight of either the concepts or language items; and (b) it reduced the size of the screening team from four to three operators, which was important from a practical point of view (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1983).

The seven items in gross motor and the eight items in fine motor were re- vised in the following manner. Throwing, standing still, balancing, and clapping hands were deleted based on scoring problems and feedback from the field over the decade of its use; catching, matching, and copying were revised to clarify scar- ing procedures; jumping, hopping, and skipping were combined into one item; and building and cutting were revised only to accommodate for the extended age range. Finally, one new item, writing name, was added as a school-related task identified by nursery school, kindergarten, and f~st-grade teachers. This item was adminis- tered only to children 4.0 and older.

The output of all eight motor items in DIAL-R is only motoric, while the input for all motoric items is both visual and auditory. This was done to ensure the children's understanding of how they were to respond independent of whether they had the particular skill being assessed. Thus, even though they might not hear or understand what was said (possibly due to a lack of comprehension in English), they have an opportunity to see a demonstration of what is expected. For children with a visual problem, other than total blindness, having the directions given auditorially allows them to compensate to some degree.

DIAL-R was substantially modified in 1990 (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1990). In particular, the DIAL-R norms were reanalyzed, reliability and validity data were expanded, a wider range of cut-off points was made avail- able, and materials for training examiners were included in the test kit.

Development of DIALS

Since DIAL-R is widely used throughout the United States and in several other countries (Australia, Taiwan, Canada, Hong Kong, Indonesia), the authors and publisher decided to revise the DIAL-R test based on three considerations: (a) norms must be kept current (less than 15 years old) to be valid (Salvia & Ysseldyke, 1998); (b) access to a free, appropriate, public education (FAPE) is now mandated for eligible children of preschool age in all 50 states and several United States territories (NEC*TAS, 1992), therefore children cannot be kept out of school be- cause they did not pass a screening test; and (c) recent research suggests different items may be more discriminating in the early identification of at-risk children during the preschool years. Again, the focus of this section will be on the motor items.

82 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

Item Development

DIAL-3 continues to measure a subset of the most fundamental or basic skills that are acquired by young children. The following is a list of general principles that were adhered to in the development and selection of items: They should (a) be developmentally appropriate, (b) be precursors of school success, (c) have enough floor for the younger children, (d) be good discriminators, (e) be easy to adminis- ter, (0 be unambiguous to score, (g) cover the entire age range, and (h) be limited to 10 min for the administration time of each area. Items were chosen that met the majority of these principles and would be meaningful to children.

Children who may develop learning problems very often have difficulty with their gross and fine motor movements. Johnson and Myklebust (1967) noted that minor incoordination problems with tasks of buttoning and tying laces are ob- served in young children with learning disabilities. According to Smith (1991), over 75 % of all poor readers have motor disturbances, but only 25 % of these children have visual-motor disturbances. Therefore, motor weaknesses often are not the result of underlying visual-perceptual deficits. A recent study by Fawcett and Nicolson (1995) confirms that a number of children with dyslexia have persis- tent deficits in motor skills that continue into adolescence.

Descriptions of the 7 items in the motor area and the rationale for their inclu- sion in the DIAL-3 follow (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998). The rec- ommendations made by the National Association for the Education of Young children (NAEYC) were followed for age-appropriate expectations in specific tasks (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Thus, by using entry and exit points throughout the assessment, tasks that would be too difficult for younger children and cause un- necessary frustration and tasks that would be too simple for older children and cause unnecessary boredom are avoided.

Item I - Catching. Catching is assessed by the child having four opportu- nities to catch a beanbag from a distance of 6 feet. In DIAL-R, the child had three opportunities, but a one-handed catch was added to ensure appropriate difficulty for 6-year-olds. Campbell (1985) suggested that to catch, the child needs the abil- ity to predict the trajectory of an object. Normed data for catching were originally found (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) on three early childhood instruments: Den- ver Developmental Screening Test (Frankenburg & Dodds, 1968); Preschool At- tainment Record (Doll, 1966); and Quick Screening Scale of Mental Development (Banham, 1963). Additional norms developed as landmarks for pediatricians (American Academy of Pediatrics. n.d.) were also utilized.

Item 2 - Jump, Hop, and Skip. Jump, Hop, and Skip consists of three sepa- rate tasks. First, the child is required to jump up to touch a beanbag held slightly above his or her head. Campbell (1985) noted that to jump, the child needs the ability to produce the proper velocity of body movement. Normed data for jump- ing were originally found (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) on five early childhood instruments: Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley, 1968), Denver Devel- opmental Screening Test (Frankenburg & Dodds, l968), Developmental Diagno- sis (Gesell & Amatruda. 1947), Preschool Attainment Record (Doll, 1966), and Quick Screening Scale of Mental Development (Banham, 1963). Additional norms had been developed by Kephart (1960) and as landmarks by the American Acad- emy of Pediatrics (n.d.).

The hopping task requires the child to hop six times on each leg. Normed data for hopping were originally found (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) on four

Preschool Motor Development 83

early childhood instruments: Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley, 1968), Denver Developmental Screening Test (Frankenburg & Dodds, 1968), Develop- mental Diagnosis (Gesell & Amatruda, 1947), and Preschool Attainment Record (Doll, 1966). Additional norms had been developed by Kephart (1960) and as land- marks by the American Academy of Pediatrics (n.d.). A study of normal and at- risk 3- and 5-year-olds by Huttenlocher, Levine, Huttenlocher, and Gates (1990) found only three neurological test items that distinguished between the two groups. One was hopping, which supports its importance as a DIAL3 task. Hopping car- ries 80% of the item score, although it is only one of the three tasks within this item. At ages 5 to 7, this task requires the ability to hop first on one leg and then on the other.

The third task, skipping, requires the child to skip one or two times after watching the operator do so (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972). Normed data for skip- ping were originally found (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) on three early child- hood instruments: Developmental Diagnosis (Gesell & Amatruda, 1947), Preschool Attainment Record (Doll, 1966), and Quick Screening Scale of Mental Develop- ment (Banharn, 1963). Additional norms had been developed by Kephart (1960). Although it is a complicated gross motor activity that requires coordinated bilat- eral use of the body, Hynd and Willis (1988) state that skipping emerges in nor- mally developing children around the age of 4 to 5 years. When this task is administered to 3-year-olds, as well as to older children, points are given for pre- cursor behaviors to skipping (e.g., stephop, gallop).

Item 3 - Building. DIAL-R required the child to model the building of a three-block tower, a three-block bridge, and a six-block pyramid, in addition to building a six-block stairs from memory. DIAL-3 has the same first three struc- tures but substitutes a six-block child figure built from a model, thus gaining a more difficult task for the older child while eliminating memory as a factor. Normed data for building were originally found (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) on 10 early childhood instruments: Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley, 1968), Mea- surement of Intelligence of Infants and Young Children (Cattell, 1960), Denver Developmental Screening Test (Frankenburg & Dodds, 1968), Developmental Diagnosis (Gesell & Amatruda, 1947), Merrill Palmer Scales (Stutsman, 1926- 48), Minnesota Preschool Scale (Goodenough, Maurer, & Van Wagenen, 1940), Preschool Attainment Record (Doll, 1966), Quick Screening Scale of Mental De- velopment (Banham, 1963), Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (Terman & Merrill, 1960), and Tests of Mental Development (Kuhlman, 1939). Additional norms had been developed as landmarks by the American Academy of Pediatrics (n.d.). The natural progression in block assembly from a tower to a bridge to more compli- cated structures was elaborately documented by Case (1985), justifying the con- tinued inclusion of building as a strong developmental item.

Item 4 - Thumbs and Fingers. Thumbs and Fingers has two tasks. First, the child is shown how to twiddle thumbs and then asked to repeat the movement. The second task is one of motor sequencing. It requires touching each of the four fin- gers to the thumb in sequence without skipping a finger or repetitively touching a finger. This task is often given as part of a standard neurological examination. Normed data for finger agility were originally found (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) on one early childhood instrument: Merrill Palmer Scale (Stutsman, 1926-48). Welsh, Pennington, and Groisser (199 1) reported this task to be related neurologi- cally to verbal fluency and to be developmentally sensitive, with differences be- tween 12-year-olds and adults. A similar task was part of both the original DIAL

84 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

and DIAL-R, but twiddling thumbs was added to the DIAL-3 item for the younger child.

Item 5 - Cutting. As on the DIAL-R, children are asked to cut snips, a straight line, and a curved line. The older child (age 5 and up) also is asked to cut out a figure of a dinosaur, a task new to DIAL-3. Normed data for cutting were originally found (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972) on five early childhood instru- ments: Developmental Diagnosis (Gesell & Arnatruda, 1947), Merrill Palmer Scale (Stutsman, 1926-48), Preschool Attainment Record (Doll, 1966), Tests of Mental Development (Kuhlman, 1939), and Vineland Social Maturity Scale (Doll, 1935). Cutting skills hold up well as a developmental item, based on both DIAL-R and DIAL-3 data. Additionally, cutting is highly regarded by early childhood person- nel as a school-related task and is enjoyed by young children as an activity.

Item 6 - Copying. The child is requested to copy four geometric shapes and four letters. The eight figures contain vertical and horizontal strokes (develop- mentally easiest), diagonal strokes, and curves that meet lines (most difficult). All eight figures are presented on the Dial format. Normed data for copying were originally found on all of the early childhood instruments previously listed (Mardell & Goldenberg, 1972). Fletcher and Satz (1982) found the use of the Developmen- tal Test of Visual-Motor Integration (VMI; Beery, 1982), on which children copy 24 geometric figures and three other brief tests correctly predicted 85 % of kinder- garten children who were problem readers 7 years later. Fowler and Cross (1986) reached a similar conclusion about the value of copying as a predictor.

In addition, Simner (1994) has done considerable research on young children's ability to copy letters and shapes. He has concluded that an excessive number of copying errors (addition, omission, andlor misalignment of parts leading to a marked distortion in the overall shape or form) appearing in samples of printing obtained from 4- to 6-year-old children can be an extremely important warning sign of later school failure. A 3-year follow-up study of 171 children by Simner (1989) showed that form errors in printing, even as early as the start of prekindergarten, could be scored reliably, remained stable over time, and were tied to performance in first grade. Simner (1994) also investigated which shapes are most predictive of school success and the manner in which they should be scored. Three of the four DIAL-3 shapes are on his list. He then applied his findings to two different groups of chil- dren and established that copying shapes is closely linked to school success. Smith (1991) states that, on the basis of her research, copying ability is one of the most consistent correlates of early math and reading success.

Item 7 - Writing Name. This item is a unique task in DIAL tests. A survey of DIAL-R users indicated that Writing Name was an item they believed should be kept on the basis that, in their experience, it is a measure of school success. Fur- thermore, in her research of children's development of writing, Sulzby (1990) found the writing of one's name to be one of the landmarks along the path from scrib- bling to conventional writing. In addition, DIAL-R and DIAL3 data analyses show a developmental progression in this skill. It is administered only to children 4-0 or older, the ages at which it is a developmentally appropriate item (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997).

Content Analysis

The content analysis that had been performed for the DIAL and DIAL-R was re- viewed and modified to reflect the abilities the items were intended to assess such

Preschool Motor Development 85

as visual motor integration, short term memory, previous learned association, preacademic skills, and speech and language (Table 1). Administration and scor- ing of some items were simplified to increase interrater reliability and shorten testing time.

Evaluation of DIALS According to Pre-Established Key Features

Zittel (1994) listed six criteria that adapted physical educators should use in the selection of a preschool assessment instrument. Each will be discussed as it relates to DIAL-3.

Purpose

Selection features are "Resource materials state: what the instrument is designed to provide; how the measurements can be used; the type of reference" (Zittel, 1994, p. 247).

What the Instrument is Designed to Provide. The DIAL-3 Manual states that DIAL3 is an individually administered screening test designed to identify young children in need of further diagnostic assessment. DIAL-3 is a 30-min assessment of motoric, conceptual, and language behaviors. The DIAL-3 Parent Questionnaire, completed during screening by a parent or caregiver, provides normed scores for the child's self-help and social skills. The child's psychosocial behaviors also are assessed by means of a rating scale completed by the testers in the three performance areas (Motor, Concepts, and Language; Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

How the Measurements Can be Used. The results of DIAL-R screening may be communicated to the parent(s) during a conference immediately after the testing process or at a later time. These results will allow the coordinator to make

Table 1 DIAL3 Motor Area Illustrating the Abilities Assessed by the Items

DIAL-3 items

Visual- Previous Pre- motor Short-term learning academic Speech &

integration memory association skills language

Motor area Catching * Jump, hop, & skip * Building * * Thumbs & fingers * * Cutting * * Copying * * Writing name * * * * *

Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg (1998, p. 15).

86 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

one of the following statements about the child: (a) The child's development ap- pears to be delayed when compared with those of the same age group, and further assessment is recommended; or (b) The child appears to be developing in a satis- factory manner, and no serious difficulty is foreseen (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

The Type of Reference. Although items were operationalized on the basis of skills that kindergarten and first-grade teachers indicated were essential for suc- cess in school (criterion-referenced), DIAL, DIAL-R, and DIAL-3 have always been standardized tests (norm-referenced). This means that each child is evaluated in terms of other children's performances. For this comparison to be meaningful, the norms must be adequate, which is dependent on three factors: the representa- tiveness of the norm sample (age, gender, ethnicity, etc.), the size of the norm sample, and the relevance of the norms in terms of the purpose of testing (Salvia & Ysseldyke, 1998). In the case of DIAL-3, the purpose is screening. All of this information is available in depth in the DIAL-3 manual (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998) but is described briefly in the following sections.

Technical Adequacy

"Evidence for validity; evidence for reliability; standardized population" (Zittel, 1994, p. 247).

Evidence for Validity. According to the DIAL-3 Manual, "The validity of a test is defined as the degree to which it accomplishes what it is designed to do" (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998, p. 82). Anastasi and Urbina (1997) observe that construct validity has come to be recognized as the fundamental and all-inclusive validity concept. DIAL-3 is based on the accurate measurement of motor, concepts, language, self-help, and psychosocial skill development in young children. Each DIAL-3 task had to demonstrate a consistent developmental growth pattern across the age groupings of the DIAL-3 in order to be included in the final 20 items. Age differentiation is a major criterion used to validate developmental tests, even though the existence of progressive increases in test scores with in- creased age does not guarantee that the test is measuring development.

Content and criterion-related validity provide valuable information in their own right on the definition and understanding of the constructs measured by a test. A number of concurrent and discriminant validity studies were conducted with other screening and diagnostic tests. The results comparing DIAL-3 with two tests (one screening and one diagnostic test) that have motor components are reported here.

A total of 166 children from the DIAL-3 standardization sample were also given the DIAL-R. The children were divided into two groups on the basis of age. One group consisted of 88 children aged 3.5 to 4.5 (mean age 48.4 months, SD 3.2) and an older group of 78 children aged 4.6 to 5.4 (mean age 58.4 months, SD 3.2). Test administration was counterbalanced: Some children took the DIAL-R before taking the DIAL-3, and others took the DIAL-3 first. The interval between tests ranged from 13 to 77 days, with a mean interval of 38 days. Results are presented in Table 2. Because of some sampling fluctuation in the standard devia- tions of the DIAL-3 scores (this affects the values of correlation coefficients), all obtained correlations were corrected assuming a population standard deviation of 15 standard score points (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Preschool Motor Development 87

Table 2 Correlations of Motor and Total Scores on the DIAL-3 With DIAL-R and DAS

DIAL3 Motor DIAL3 Total r Con." r Corr."

Age 3.5 to 4.5 DI AL-R

Motor Total

DIAL3 Mean SD

4.6 to 5.4 DIAL-R

Motor Total

DIAL-3 Mean SD

DAS subtests Copying Nonverbal cluster

GCA DIAL-3

Mean SD

DIAL-R Mean SD

D AS Mean SD 46.4 9.8 97.3 15.2 98.3 13.8

=All coefficients were corrected for the variability of the norm group (SD = IS), based on the standard deviation obtained on DIAL-3, using Guilford's (1954, p. 392) formula.

From Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg (1998, pp. 84, 88).

The DIAL-3 Total score has a moderately high corrected correlation with the DIAL- R Total score (0.91 for the younger sample and 0.84 for the older sample). In addition, the DIAL-3 Motor scores correlate moderately well with the DIAL-R Motor scores for both age groups (0.72 for the younger sample and 0.74 for the older sample). These results provide good support for the convergent and discrimi- nant validity of the DIAL-3 scores.

Fifty children from the DIAL-3 standardization sample also were given the Differential Ability Scales (DAS; Elliott, 1990), which is a diagnostic test. They covered an age range of 3.7 through 5.10 (mean age 4.7). They were given the DAS and the DIAL-3 in counterbalanced order, with the interval between tests varying from 1 to 72 days (mean interval 11 days). Table 2 shows the correlations between the DIAL-3 motor and total scores and the DAS motor measures and DAS General Conceptual Ability (GCA) score, which is the overall composite.

88 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

The DIAL-3 Total correlates substantially (0.79) with the DAS overall composite score (GCA). The DIAL-3 Motor score has a corrected correlation of 0.51 with DAS Copying (a task included in the DIAL3 Motor area). Motor also correlates substantially (0.62) with DAS Matching Letter-Like Forms, which is a nonverbal subtest, and with the Nonverbal Cluster score (0.51). These results show strong relationships between DIAL-3 and the DAS and support the validity of the DIAL-3.

Although it is not one of the categories of technical validity, Anastasi (1988) states that face validity is considered essential to obtain cooperation from partici- pants and to instill acceptance in the minds of test users. Face validity is a first impression of whether a test appears to be measuring the intended content in an appropriate manner. For instance, a screening test for young children should be gamelike and appealing. Young children have no concept of the meaning of "tak- ing a test," so their typical performance has to be elicited in other ways. DIAL-3 includes a variety of appealing tasks, novel materials, colorful pictures, age-ap- propriate manipulatives, and carefully controlled entry and exit requirements, which all contribute to maximizing the child's cooperation and interest. Increased inter- est and cooperation, in turn, contribute to the validity of the obtained scores (Mardell- Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Evidence for Reliability. The concept of reliability concerns the accuracy or consistency of scores. Two types of reliability are internal consistency based on inconsistencies in a person's responses to individual items in the test and test- retest reliability based on the inconsistencies due to fluctuations in performance over time. The median alpha (internal) reliability of the DIAL-3 Total score is 0.87, based on the standardization sample of 1,560 children. Motor area alpha reliabilities range from 0.45 to 0.74. The median of 0.66 is brought down by the older children (ages 6-6 to 6-11) because a number of children of this age obtain near-maximum scores. As expected, area scores demonstrate somewhat lower in- ternal reliability, primarily because area scores are based on fewer items than is the DIAL-3 Total score. The DIAL-3 Total scores have adequate reliability at every 6- month age span (but the above mentioned one) for screening purposes, according to Salvia and Ysseldyke's (1998) standard of 0.80 for a screening test.

To measure test-retest reliability, DIAL-3 was administered twice to 158 children (86 boys and 72 girls) from the standardization sample, who were divided into two groups on the basis of age. One group consisted of 80 children aged 3.6 to 4.5 (M = 47.4 months, SD = 3.5) and an older group of 78 children aged 4.6 to 5.10 (M = 60.4 months, SD = 3.6). The interval between tests ranged from 12 to 65 days (M = 28 days). The DIAL-3 Total scores (0.88 and 0.84 for the two groups) have very satisfactory test-retest reliabilities that are above the Salvia and Ysseldyke (1998) criterion of 0.80 for a screening test. The DIAL-3 Motor area reliabilities (0.69 and 0.67) cluster around 0.70, indicating that the Motor area should not be used as a separate test but only as part of the total screening (Mardell- Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Standardized Population. DIAL-3 was normed on a national sample of 1,560 English-speaking and 650 Spanish- speaking children, stratified by chrono- logical age, gender, geographic area, race or ethnic group, and parent education level. As the sampling plan was completed, its correspondence to the population target figures was tracked at the level of joint frequencies of the stratification vari- ables. Therefore, the sample was matched to the population not only for one de- mographic variable at a time but also for combined variables (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Preschool Motor Development 89

Nondiscriminatory

"Adaptations permitted, multisource information permitted, standardized sample sensitive to culture and disability" (Zittel, 1994, p. 247).

Adaptations Permitted. Any adaptations are permitted as long as they do not interfere with the standardized manner in which the test is to be administered. For example, children would have to be ambulatory to participate in the two gross motor items, but if not, they could still participate in the five fine motor items.

Multisource Znfomzation Permitted. DIAL-3 is designed not only to per- mit multisource information but to require it. Each area is administered by a differ- ent adult so that the child is screened by a minimum of three adults. Along with the abilities and behaviors that are evaluated by the screening team, the DIAL-3 in- cludes the parent questionnaire, which concentrates on the child's self-help and social development. It provides normed scores for the child's self-help and social skills which, combined with the behavioral ratings from the DIAL-3 operators, provide the basis for the need of further assessment in the psychosocial area. The parent questionnaire also requests other information that should be shared between parents and professionals, such as medical history, family background, and gen- eral development. Information received from parents, who see the child in his or her natural environment, adds to the social and ecological validity of the screening in ways that standardized assessments cannot match (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Standardized Sample Sensitive to Culture and Disability. Both the DIAL- 3 and the Speed DIAL, a shortened version administered by one person rather than a team, may be administered in English or Spanish. The Spanish version is not merely a translation of DIAL-3 but a test that has been normed on a national sample of young Spanish-speaking children. Great care was taken to make sure DIAL3 measures accurately in English or Spanish. The goal was to keep the two versions as similar as possible.

Children with special educational needs comprised approximately 10% of the total sample. A total of 161 children in the standardization sample were re- ported as receiving special education services. These 161 children received 292 services. They were categorized into the following groupings: physical, cognitive, communication, social or emotional, and adaptive.

Administrative Ease

"Scoring more than passlfail; interpretation includes a raw score summary, com- ments related to performance, or level of mastery indicated; administration time clearly stated or flexibility in test component administration allowed" (Zittel, 1994, p. 247).

Scoring More Than Pass/Fail. Every item is scored by objective criteria on the record form. The raw score is easily converted to a 5-point scaled score. Then the scaled scores are added within each area and the three areas are com- bined for a DIAL-R total score. A computer software program is available to assist in making this process very accurate and fast. It also contains a summary for par- ents and parent-child activities (both in English or Spanish) that are gamelike yet educationally sound.

Interpretation includes a raw score summary, comments related to peifor- mance, or level of mastery indicated. The record form allows examiners to know

90 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

exactly what the child could do by looking at one page for each area that contains all of the tasks. The scaled score indicates a rough measure of mastery according to age. The final decision is based on a number of factors selected by each screen- ing site (e.g., the percentage of children the site wants to identify).

Administration Time Clearly Stated or Flexibility in Test Component Ad- ministration Allowed. With one exception in the Concepts area, DIAL-3 items and tasks are not timed; children are allowed to take as much time as they need to respond. However, it is the responsibility of the operator to present the materials in such a way that children know what is expected and want to respond. This is ac- complished through a training workshop that introduces operators to the DIAL-3 screening procedures. The DIAL-3 kit contains a reusable training packet that con- tains written tests, answers for the written tests, performance tests, and scripts for roleplaying. The DIAL-3 training videotape demonstrates the administration of all items in each area besides showing the overall screening situation (Mardell- Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Instructional Link

"Curriculum-referenced, test items sequenced to provide low inference for instruc- tional objectives, ability to monitor progress" (Zittel, 1994, p. 247).

Curriculum-Referenced. Guidelines have been developed by a national task force on screening and assessment for the National Center for Clinical Infant Pro- grams. One of the ten guidelines states "Processes, procedures, and instruments intended for screening should only be used for their specified purposes7' (1989, p. 24). The specific purpose of DIAL3 is to identify children with current or poten- tial learning problems. Thus, we do not believe that a screening test should have an instructional link nor be used to determine cumculum. However, it is easy to see, using the record form, exactly which skills are age appropriate and which ones require instructional links. Additionally, parent and child activity forms that are identical in content to the ones in the computer program suggest general activities that parents may use to enhance their child's overall development in each of the five areas. Hence, there are suggestions for motor activities. The activities pre- sented require minimal commercial materials and can be accomplished in a short period of time (Mardell-Czudnowski & Goldenberg, 1998).

Test Items Are Sequenced to Provide Low Inference for Instructional Objectives. Since DIAL-3 was normed on 1,560 English-speaking and 605 Span- ish-speaking children stratified by age, tasks within each test item have been se- quenced according to difficulty level to allow for appropriate age level entrance and exit points. This also assists in the development of instructional objectives based on substantive data of what children are typically capable of doing at given ages.

Ability to monitorprogress. As a screening test, DIAL3 was not designed to monitor progress. However, retesting over a 6-month or 12-month period does show the child's progress (Mardell-Czudnowski, Goldenberg, Suen, &Fries, 1988).

Ecological Validity

"Familiar materials, familiar setting, caregiver present" (Zittel, 1994, p.247). Familiar Materials. While DIAL-3 does employ familiar materials (e.g.,

red and white blocks, scissors, primary pencil) in the motor area, it also uses novel

Preschool Motor Development 91

materials (e.g., red apple beanbag, cutting cards, Copying Dial) to interest the child in a new activity.

Familiar Setting. DIAL-3 screening is often conducted in the child's school or childcare center. However, allowance is made for the occasion when the child is screened in an unfamiliar setting. All children are allowed to sit (and play) at a play table for as long as necessary before going to any of the three areas to be screened. This is another unique feature of DIAL tests.

Caregiver Present. All screening is done in the presence of the caregiver. A part of the screening room is set aside for caregivers and younger siblings. If necessary, the caregiver may even accompany the child to the screening area.

Summary

Developing a nationally standardized screening test is both time consuming and costly. This article briefly describes the previous versions of DIAL and DIAL-R and the development of new and revised items in the motor area for DIAL-3. The same procedures were followed for the development of items in the other four sections of the test: concepts, language, self-help, and psychosocial. Then DIAL- 3 is evaluated using the key features for selecting an appropriate preschool gross motor assessment instrument (Zittel, 1994).

The quest for the most predictive preschool screening test will continue until educators and caregivers, working together, can accurately identify all children whose developmental delay or differences suggest the need of special educational services and then provide such services within and outside of the early childhood setting. It is important that adapted physical educators, as members of the screen- ing team, be aware of the latest screening tests and how they meet the criteria established for evaluating all of the components of the test.

References

American Academy of Pediatrics (n.d.). Developmental landmarks. Washington, DC: Ameri- can Academy of Pediatrics.

Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing (6th ed.). New York: Macmillan. Anastasi, A,, & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing (7th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:

Prentice-Hall. Banham, K. (1963). Quick screening sc& u~mental development. Murfreesboro, TN: Psy-

chometric Affiliates. Barsch, R., & Rudell, R. (1962). A study of reading development among 77 children with

cerebral palsy. Cerebral Palsy Review, 23,3-13. Bayley, N. (1968). Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corpora-

tion. Beery, K. (1982). Revised administration, scoring, and teaching manual for the Develop-

mental Test of Wsual-Motor Integration. Cleveland, OH: Modem Curriculum Press. Bredekamp, S., & Copple (Eds.) (1997). Developmentally appropriate practices in early

childhoodprograms serving children from birth to age 8. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.

Bryant, N. (1964). Some conclusions concerning impaired motor development among reading disability cases. Bulletin of Orton Society, 14, 16-17.

92 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

Campbell, S. (1985). Assessment of the child with CNS dysfunction. In J. Rothstein, (Ed.), Measurement in physical therapy (pp. 207-228). New York: Churchill Livingstone.

Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adult. London: Academic Press. Cattell, P. (1960). Measurement of intelligence of infants and young children. New York:

Johnson Reprint Corp. Cratty, B. (1967). Movement behavior and motor learning (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lea &

Febiger. Current population survey (March, 1994) [Machine-readable data file]. Washington, DC:

Bureau of the Census (Producer and Distributor). Doll, E. (1935). Vineland social maturity scale. Circle Pines, M N : American Guidance

Service. Doll, E. (1966). Preschool attainment record. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Ser-

vice. Dunsing, J., & Kephart, N. (1965). Motor generalization in space and time. Learning disor-

ders (Vol. 1). Seattle, WA: Special Publications. Elliott, C.D. (1990). DAS administration and scoring manual. San Antonio: The Psycho-

logical Corporation. Fawcett, A., & Nicolson, R. (1995). Persistent deficits in motor skill of children with dys-

lexia. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27,235-240. Federal Register. (1997). PL 105-17. Individuals with disabilities education act. Fletcher, J., & Satz, P. (1982). Kindergarten prediction of reading achievement: A seven

year longitudinal follow-up. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 42,681- 685.

Fowler, M., & Cross, A. (1986). Preschool risk factors as predictors of early school perfor- mance. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 7(4), 237-241.

Frankenburg, W., & Dodds, J. (1968). Denver developmental screening test. Denver: Ladoca Publishing Foundation.

Gesell, A. (1952). Infant development: The embryology of early human behavior. New York: Harper.

Gesell, A., & Amatruda, E. (1947). Developmental diagnosis (2nd ed.). New York: Hoeber- Harper.

Gesell, A., & Thompson, H. (1938). The psychology of early growth including norms of infant behavior and a method of genetic analysis. New York: Macmillan.

Goodenough, F., Maurer, K., &Van Wagenen, M. (1940). Minnesota Preschool Scale. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Gredler, G. (1992). School readiness: Assessment and educational issues. Brandon, VT: Clinical Psychology Publishing.

Gredler, G. (1997). Issues in early childhood screening and assessment. Psychology in the Schools, 34(2), 99-105.

Guilford, J. P. (1954). Psychometric methods (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Hall, J., Mardell, C., Wick, J., & Goldenberg, D. (1976). Further development and refine-

ment of DIAL, final report. Resources in Education (ED 117 200). Hrncir, E., & Eisenhart, C. (1991). Use with caution: The "at-risk" label. Young Children,

46, 23-27. Huttenlocher, P., Levine, S., Huttenlocher, J., & Gates, J. (1990). Discrimination of normal

and at-risk preschool children on the basis of neurological tests. Developmental Medi- cine and Child Neurology, 32,394-402.

Hynd, G., & Willis, W.G. (1988). Pediatric neuropsychology. Orlando: Grune & Stratton. Johnson, D., & Myklebust, H. (1967). Learning disabilities, education principles andprac-

tices. New York: Grune & Stratton.

Preschool Motor Development 93

Karlin, R. (1957). Physical growth and success in undertaking beginning reading. Journal of Educational Research, 51, 19 1-201.

Keogh, B., & Speece, D. (1996). Learning disabilities within the context of school. In D. Speece & B. Keough (Eds.), Research on classroom ecologies: Implications for in- clusion of children with learning disabilities (pp. 1-14). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kephart, N. (1960). The slow learner in the classroom. Columbus, OH: Charles Merrill. Kuhlman, F. (1939). Tests of mental development. Minneapolis: Educational Test Bureau. Mardell, C., & Goldenberg, D. (1972). Learning disabilities/early childhood research project.

Springfield, E: Ofice of the Superintendent of Public Instruction, State of Illinois. Mardell, C., & Goldenberg, D. (1973). Instruments for screening of prekindergarten chil-

dren. Research in education, (ERIC No. ED 082 408). Mardell, C., & Goldenberg, D. (1975). Developmental indicators for the assessment of

learning (DIAL). Edison, NJ: Childcraft Educational Corporation. Mardell-Czudnowski, C. (1980). Validity and reliability studies with DIAL. Journal for

Special Educators, 17(1), 32-45. Mardell-Czudnowski, C., & Goldenberg, D. (1983). Developmental indicators for the as-

sessment of leaming-revised (DIAL-R). Edison, NJ: Childcraft Educational Corpo- ration.

Mardell- Czudnowski, C., Goldenberg, D., Suen, H., &Fries, R. (1988). Predictive validity of DIAL-R. Diagnostique, 14(1), 55-62.

Mardell-Czudnowski, C., & Goldenberg, D. (1990). Developmental indicators for the as- sessment of learning-revised, American Guidance Service edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Mardell-Czudnowski, C., & Goldenberg, D. (1998). Developmental indicators for the as- sessment of learning-third edition (DIAL-3). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

McGraw, M. (1943). The neuromuscular maturation of the human irzfant. New York: Co- lumbia University Press.

McLean, M., Smith, B., McConnick, K., Shakel, J., & McEvoy, M. (1991). Developmental delay: Establishing parameters for a preschool category of exceptionality. Position paper for Division for Early Childhood, Council for Exceptional Children.

National Center for Clinical Programs (1989). Screening and assessment: Guidelines for identiJfving young disabled and developmentally vulnerable children and their fami- lies. Washington, DC: Author.

NEC*TAS (1992). Section 619 profile: A profile of Part B section 619 services. Chapel Hill, NC: National Early Childhood Technical Assistance System, Frank Porter Gra- ham Child Development Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

Salvia, J., & Ysseldyke, J. (1998). Assessment (8th ed.) Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Simner, M. (1989). Predictive validity of an abbreviated version of the Printing Perfor-

mance School Readiness Test. Joumal of School Psychology, 27,2, 189-195. Simner, M. (1994). Improving the predictive validity of geometric-design copying tasks on

instruments used to evaluate school readiness. In C. Faure, , P. Keuss, G. Lorsette, & A.Vinter, (Eds.) Advances in handwriting and drawing: A multidisciplinary approach (pp. 489-500). Paris: Europia.

Smith, C. (1991). Learning disabilities: The integration of learnel; task and setting. Bos- ton: Allyn & Bacon.

Stutsman, R. (1926-48). Merrill Palmer scale of mental tests. Wood Dale, L: Stoelting. Sulzby, E. (1990). Assessment of emergent writing and children's language while writing.

In L. Morrow & J. Smith (Eds.), Assessment for Instruction in Early Literacy (pp. 83-109). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

94 Mardell-Czudnowski and Goldenberg

Terman, L., & Menill, M. (1960). Stanford-Binet intelligence scale. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Welsh, M., Pennington, B., & Groisser, D. (1991). Anonnative study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 131-149.

Wright, B. & Masters, G. (1982). Rating Scale Analysis: Rasch Measurement. Chicago: Mesa Press.

Zittel, L. (1994). Gross motor assessment of preschool children with special needs: Instru- ment considerations. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 11,245-260.

Authors' Notes

The authors wish to thank the entire staff at American Guidance Service (AGS), the publishers of DIAL-R and DIAL-3, who were instrumental in the item development of DIAL-3. In particular, we are most grateful to Colin Elliott, Shelly Saunders, Lora Oberle, J.J. Wang and Tsuey- Hwa Chen at AGS. More information about ordering DIAL-3 may be obtained from AGS. Tel: (800) 328- 2560; Fax: (612) 786-9077; E-mail: <[email protected]>.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Carol Mardell-Czudnowski, 1605-B Pacific Rim Court, PMB 7 1-429143905, San Diego, CA 92143-9015 <[email protected]>; Dorothea Goldenberg, Dial Inc., PO Box 911, Highland Park, IL 60035.