a role for adult born neurons in memory processing by ... · barbieri, maria paula almeida, andre...

276
A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by Maithe Arruda Carvalho A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctorate of Philosophy Institute of Medical Science University of Toronto © Copyright by Maithe Arruda Carvalho 2012

Upload: others

Post on 07-Aug-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing

by

Maithe Arruda Carvalho

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctorate of Philosophy

Institute of Medical Science

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Maithe Arruda Carvalho 2012

Page 2: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

ii

A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing

Maithe Arruda Carvalho

Doctor of Philosophy

Institute of Medical Science

University of Toronto

2012

Abstract

Throughout adulthood, the brain continuously generates new neurons in two neurogenic

regions: the subgranular zone of the hippocampus and the subventricular zone on the lateral wall

of the lateral ventricles. These neurons have been shown to integrate into hippocampal and

olfactory bulb circuitry, respectively. Nevertheless, their specific contribution to hippocampal or

olfactory function remains unclear. Previous studies have tried to assess adult born neuron

contribution to memory function by suppressing neurogenesis and examining the impact on

memory acquisition. Although ablation of neurogenesis has been shown to impair performance

in hippocampus dependent and olfactory tasks, many studies fail to see an effect. Compensation

from residual cells in either system after ablation may underlie these contradictory findings.

Thus, a more direct approach to answer this question would be to ablate adult born neurons after

their incorporation into the memory trace. To do this, we established a double transgenic strategy

to tag and selectively ablate adult born neurons with temporal control. Ablation of a population

of predominantly mature, adult generated dentate granule cells did not prevent acquisition of

contextual fear conditioning or Morris Water Maze memories. Removal of that same population

of cells after training, however, led to memory degradation in three hippocampus dependent

tasks. Similarly, post-training ablation of a population of adult generated olfactory interneurons

Page 3: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

iii

impaired performance in an associative odour memory task, whereas pre-training ablation had no

impact. Together, these data show that adult generated neurons form a crucial component of both

hippocampal and olfactory memory traces.

Page 4: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

iv

Acknowledgments

Paul, it is hard to put into perspective how much I have learned and grown in the five years since

I started in your lab. You have given me the opportunity to do a level of research I never

imagined, accompanied by an enthusiasm for each experiment I never thought I would have.

Thank you for all the opportunities, discussions, for disagreeing (and occasionally agreeing) with

me, for challenging me into becoming a better scientist. I have learned so much about the

elegance, hardship and finesse of scientific life from your example, and for all you have given

me I will forever be indebted.

Sheena, you have always supported and encouraged me throughout these years. I cannot thank

you enough for your generosity and openness in sharing with me your experience, giving me

precious advice, and all our discussions that have meant so much to me.

James Ellis and Freda Miller, thank you so much for your invaluable input and support over

these years, and for patiently heping me with our ‗schedule wars‘.

Cindi Morshead and Amelia Eisch, thank you for generously accepting to be reviewers for this

thesis, and sharing your suggestions to improve it.

Masanori Sakaguchi, who taught me so much in my first few years in the lab, whose lessons I‘ll

always carry with me and always be thankful.

Katherine Akers, who was such a crucial part of this thesis on all its levels, thank you so much

for being such a great partner (and editor!) and having the patience to work with me in both these

projects and put so much effort in revising this text.

Mika Yamamoto, you have been with us from the beginning, through the toughest times in our

hippocampal project. I have such admiration for you and wish to thank you so, so much for the

enormous help (even when I‘m not looking) and support throughout so many years.

Page 5: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

v

Toni DeCristofaro and Russell Braybon, who do such an amazing job managing the lab. Not a

single line in this thesis could have been written without your help and support, for which I

cannot thank or appreciate you enough.

The Josselyn/Frankland lab past and present members: Leigh Botly, Christy Cole, Loren DeVito,

Matthew Florczynski, Anna Gianlorenco, Liz Hsiang, Rachel Kang, Aneta Krakowski,

Alexander Marsolais, Tetyana Pekar, Asim Rashid, Blake Richards, Adam Santoro, Dani Sarkis,

Jason Snyder, Scellig Stone, Matt Tran, Joel Ross, Michel Van den Oever, Gisella Vetere, Afra

Wang, Chen Yan, Kirill Zaslavsky, Jeremy Zung, for all their support, without whom this thesis

would not have been possible.

Special thanks to: Ann Victory, who saved my life in so many burocratic situations and ensured

our collective sanity in our tuesday night movies; Catia Teixeira, Anne Wheeler and Melanie

Sekeres, who always helped and supported me through these years and without whom life is half

as amusing; Adelaide Yiu, who made everything seem so easy and simple with her generosity;

Jonathan Epp and Axel Guskjolen, who help me so much with our CamKII project (and weird

stem cell questions, Jon…); Alonso Martinez, who spent so much time troubleshooting

stereological tangles, Yosuke Niibori, who has patiently and generously assisted us in different

steps of these papers; Derya Sargin, for our conversations over tea or breaks in fear conditioning;

Leonardo Restivo and Valentina Mercaldo (and Sammy), for all their help and kindness inside

and outside the lab; Gemma Higgs, without your friendship and unconditional support these

years would have been so difficult, thank you!

Analena Mileo, Anelise Leite, Manuela Roitman, Marina Verjovsky, Matias Lopez, Nina

Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda

vida, nunca se movendo de mim, nao importa aonde va o trem.

Monika, thank you for being everything. For your never ending patience, unbounded support and

everlasting generosity; I truly hope you know.

Renée e Apo, René e Angela, apesar da distancia e da saudade voces nunca questionaram minhas

escolhas e sempre me encorajaram a viver minha vida como eu a quero. Nao tenho palavras para

expressar a honra, gratidao e orgulho de ter voces comigo, sempre. Voces sao o exemplo e a

motivacao, a base e a escada. Espero com amor muitos outros sonhos para compartilharmos.

Page 6: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

vi

Table of Contents

Contents

Acknowledgments .......................................................................................................................... iv

Table of Contents ........................................................................................................................... vi

List of Abreviations ....................................................................................................................... ix

List of Tables ................................................................................................................................. xi

List of Figures ............................................................................................................................... xii

Chapter 1 Literature Review ........................................................................................................... 1

1.1 Memory ............................................................................................................................... 1

1.1.1 Founding concepts .................................................................................................. 1

1.1.2 Memory gradients and grades of memory .............................................................. 5

1.1.3 Memory consolidation ............................................................................................ 8

1.2 Hippocampus .................................................................................................................... 12

1.2.1 The hippocampal formation: an overview ............................................................ 13

1.2.2 Hippocampal functions: we are more than just our memories ............................. 14

1.2.3 Hippocampal functions: learning and memory ..................................................... 16

1.2.4 Dentate gyrus ........................................................................................................ 25

1.3 Adult neurogenesis ............................................................................................................ 30

1.3.1 History ................................................................................................................... 30

1.3.2 Hippocampal neurogenesis ................................................................................... 32

1.3.3 Olfactory neurogenesis ......................................................................................... 50

Chapter 2 Aims/hypotheses .......................................................................................................... 61

Chapter 3 General methods ........................................................................................................... 63

3.1 Cell culture ......................................................................................................................... 63

Page 7: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

vii

3.2 Mice .................................................................................................................................... 63

3.3 Drugs .................................................................................................................................. 65

3.4 Immunohistochemistry ....................................................................................................... 65

3.5 Imaging and quantification ................................................................................................ 66

3.7 General behavioral apparatus and procedures .................................................................... 67

3.8 Specific experimental protocols ......................................................................................... 70

3.9 Data analysis ...................................................................................................................... 74

Chapter 4 Posttraining Ablation of Adult Generated Neurons Degrades Previously Acquired

Memories.................................................................................................................................. 75

4.1 Abstract .............................................................................................................................. 75

4.2 Introduction ........................................................................................................................ 76

4.3 Results ................................................................................................................................ 77

4.3.1 Murine cells are insensitive to DT ........................................................................... 77

4.3.2 Characterization of ‗tag and ablate‘ mice ................................................................ 78

4.3.3 Post-training ablation of tagged neurons degrades a contextual fear memory ........ 84

4.3.4 Pre-training ablation of tagged neurons does not prevent formation of new

contextual fear memory ........................................................................................ 87

4.3.5 Post-training ablation of tagged neurons degrades spatial memory ........................ 89

4.3.6 Post-training ablation of tagged neurons degrades remote spatial memory ............ 92

4.3.7 Post-training ablation degrades visual discrimination memory ............................... 92

4.4 Discussion .......................................................................................................................... 95

Chapter 5 Post-training Ablation of Adult Generated Olfactory Interneurons Impairs

Associative Odour Memory Expression .................................................................................. 99

5.1Abstract ............................................................................................................................... 99

5.2 Introduction ........................................................................................................................ 99

5.3 Results .............................................................................................................................. 101

5.3.1 CREERT2

expression is restricted to neural stem cells ............................................ 101

Page 8: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

viii

5.3.2 Characterization of the tagging .............................................................................. 101

5.3.3 Post-training ablation of adult born olfactory interneurons impairs associative

olfactory memory expression .............................................................................. 103

5.3.4 Pre-training ablation of adult born olfactory interneurons does not interfere with

associative olfactory memory acquisition ........................................................... 106

5.4 Discussion ........................................................................................................................ 108

Chapter 6 General Discussion ..................................................................................................... 111

6.1 Summary of Results ......................................................................................................... 111

6.2 Pre vs. Posttraining........................................................................................................... 112

6.3 Erasure vs. Degradation ................................................................................................... 114

6.4 Drowning by numbers ...................................................................................................... 116

6.5 Avenues for silencing neurons ......................................................................................... 117

6.6 The unbearable lightness of inconsistencies .................................................................... 120

6.6.1 In the hippocampus ................................................................................................ 120

6.6.2 In the olfactory system ........................................................................................... 122

6.7 What are aDGCs (and DGCs) really good for? ............................................................... 125

6.8 What are aOGCs (and OGCs) really good for? ............................................................... 129

Chapter 7 Future Directions ........................................................................................................ 131

References ................................................................................................................................... 135

Appendix 1 .................................................................................................................................. 217

Page 9: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

ix

List of Abbreviations

CamKII - -Calcium calmodulin kinase II

ACSF - Artificial cerebrospinal fluid

aDGC - Adult born DGC

AMPA - α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid

aOGC - Adult born OGC

AraC - Arabinofuranosyl Cytidine, also known as cytosine arabinoside

BDNF - Brain derived neurotrophic factor

bHLH - Basic helix-loop-helix

BrdU - 5-bromo-2'-deoxyuridine

CA- Cornu ammonis

cAMP - Cyclic adenosine monophosphate

Cdk5 - Cycline dependent kinase 5

CFC- Context fear conditioning

CNTF - Ciliary neurotrophic factor

CR - Calretinin

CREB- cAMP responsive element binding protein

CS- Conditioned stimulus

DCX - Doublecortin

DG - Dentate gyrus

DGCL - Dentate granule cell layer

DMTS – Delayed match to sample

DNMTS - Delayed non match to sample

DT - Diphtheria toxin

EC - Entorhinal cortex

EE - Environmental enrichment

EGF - Epidermal growth factor

E-LTP- Early long-term potentiation

EPL - External plexiform layer

ERK - Extracellular signal-regulated protein kinase

FGF2 - Fibroblast growth factor 2

FMRP - Fragile X mental retardation protein

GABA - γ-Aminobutyric acid

GFAP - Glial fibrillary acidic protein

GL - Glomerular layer

H.M. - Patient Henry Molaison

HB-EGF - Heparin binding EGF

HPA- Hypothalamo pituitary adrenocortical

iDTR - Inducible diphtheria toxin receptor

ISD - Immediate shock deficit

IPL - Internal plexiform layer

KCC2 - K+-coupled Cl

- transporter

LTM- Long term memory

LTP - Long term potentiation

Page 10: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

x

MAM - Methylazoxymethanol acetate

MCL - Mitral cell layer

MEC - Medial entorhinal cortex

MF - Mossy fiber

ML - Molecular layer

MTT - Multiple trace theory

MWM - Morris water maze

NCAM - Neural cell adhesion molecule

NeuN - Neuronal nuclei (maturation marker)

NKCC1 - Na+–K

+–2Cl

– co-transporter

NMDA- N-Methyl-D-aspartate

NMDAR - NMDA receptor

NR2b - N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunit

OB - Olfactory bulb

OGC - Olfactory granule cell

OGCL - Olfactory granule cell layer

ONL - Olfactory nerve layer

PGC - Periglomerular cell

PKA - Protein kinase A

PP1 - Protein phosphatase 1

PRP - Plasticity related proteins

PSA-NCAM - Polysialated neural cell adhesion molecule

RMS - Rostral migratory stream

SGZ - Subgranular zone

STM - Short term memory

SVZ - Subventricular zone

TAM - Tamoxifen

TH - Tyrosine hydroxilase

UR- Unconditioned response

US - Unconditioned stimulus

VEGF - Vascular endothelial growth factor

VTA - Ventral tegmental area

Page 11: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

xi

List of Tables

Table 1: Comparative table of olfactory adult neurogenesis papers……………………………..60

Table 2: Maturation analysis of CREERT2

positive cell population in the DG…………………...94

Table 3: Maturation analysis of LacZ positive cell population in the DG………………………95

Table 4: Maturation analysis of CREERT2

positive cell population in the SVZ………………...107

Table 5: Maturation analysis of LacZ positive cell population in the OB……………………...108

Page 12: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

xii

List of Figures

Figure 1: Scheme of memory classification. ……………………………………………………..8

Figure 2: Trisynaptic circuit……………………………………………………………………..14

Figure 3. MWM scheme………………………………………………………………………....20

Figure 4. Fear conditioning scheme……………………………………………………………..23

Figure 5. Dentate gyrus organization……………………………………………………………26

Figure 6. Scheme of the maturational stages of aDGCs………………………………………...36

Figure 7. Olfactory system………………………………………………………………………49

Figure 8. Structure of the olfactory bulb………………………………………………………...51

Figure 9.DT-based ablation……………………………………………………………………...78

Figure 10. CreERT2

expression is restricted to progenitor cells and limited to adult neurogenic

regions……………………………………………………………………………………………80

Figure 11.Tagging new neurons…………………………………………………………………81

Figure 12. Ablating tagged neurons……………………………………………………………...82

Figure 13.DT-induced ablation produces minimal inflammation………………………………..83

Figure 14. General health and behavior are not altered by DT-induced ablation………………..84

Figure 15. Post-training ablation of adult-generated neurons degrades contextual fear memory.86

Figure 16. Pre-training ablation of adult-generated neurons does not prevent the formation of a

new contextual fear memory……………………………………………………………………89

Figure 17. Post-training (but not pre-training) ablation of adult-generated neurons impairs spatial

memory expression……………………………………………………………………………..91

Page 13: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

xiii

Figure 18. Post-training ablation of adult-generated neurons impairs visual discrimination

memory…………………………………………………………………………………………94

Figure 19: CreERT2

expression is restricted to neural stem cells……………………………….101

Figure 21: Maturation analysis of adult born olfactory granule cells…………………………103

Figure 22: Post-training ablation of adult-generated olfactory interneurons impairs expression of

an associative olfactory memory………………………………………………………………105

Figure 23: Open field performance following DT-induced ablation of adult generated olfactory

interneurons. …………………………………………………………………………………..105

Figure 24: Post-training ablation of adult-generated olfactory interneurons impairs expression of

an associative olfactory memory………………………………………………………………106

Figure 25: Pre-training ablation of adult-generated olfactory interneurons does not impair

acquisition of an associative olfactory memory………………………………………………..107

Page 14: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

1

Chapter 1 Literature Review

This literature review is structured in a funnel, starting from basic concepts of memory

and a review of landmark discoveries pertinent to the field of neuroscience of memory. Within

this framework, the role of the hippocampus in memory processing is discussed, followed by a

description of the physiological functions and anatomy of a structure within the hippocampal

formation, the dentate gyrus. Next, the introduction focuses on a specific subset of dentate

granule cells, those born during adulthood, their genesis, regulation and potential role in memory

processing. Finally, a separate subsection provides an overview of the olfactory system and

possible functional implications of olfactory interneurons born during adulthood.

1.1 Memory

1.1.1 Founding concepts

The turn of the twentieth century is considered by many to be a golden era in the study of

brain and behaviour, around which the emergence of several key concepts and discoveries would

arguably come to found the field of modern neuroscience. The ground for this flourishing was set

by a few prior discoveries, two examples being the initial descriptions of brain anatomy by

Willis in the 17th

century, and Galvani‘s description of electricity as the means of nervous

conduction in the 18th

century (Eichenbaum, 2002).

This period is marked by vigorous debates and the establishment of what are now core

concepts in learning and memory. One such important debate involved the compartmentalization

of brain function, a new concept encountering strong resistance in the research community (Zola-

Morgan, 1995). In the early 1800‘s, Franz Gall performed the first systematic analysis of cortical

localization. He developed a theory, known as organology or phrenology, that stated that

psychological characteristics were mediated by specific regions in the brain (Zola-Morgan,

Page 15: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

2

1995), and went on into mapping them. Even though character and personality traits never

proved to be compartmentalized as predicted by Gall, subsequent evidence showed that specific

brain functions were.

The first demonstration of this compartmentalization came from the work of Paul Broca,

through a case study of a patient, who, due to a localized lesion to the frontal lobe (in what was

later referred to as Broca‘s area) became aphasic but could still understand language (Stone,

1991). In the second half of the 19th

century, studies done by Gustav Fritsch and Eduard Hitzig,

using brain stimulation in dogs, and by David Ferrier, with monkeys, provided strong evidence

for distinct motor areas in the cortex (Gross, 2007), further refuting the view of the cortex as a

unitary unit.

The debate continued with the strengthening of the anti-localization view through the

work of Karl Lashley, who systematically removed cortical surface areas in the rat to study its

effects on memory. Lashley concluded that the extent of memory deficit was proportional to the

size of cortical area removed, regardless of its location, which he referred to as the law of mass

action (Lashley, 1929, 1950). Only many years later and with additional experimental work it

was possible to arrive at a different conclusion from this discovery.

At the cellular level, another ‗unitarian‘ type debate was taking place, between the

reticularists, who believed the brain was formed by a unitary interconnected fiber network in

which all cells were fused to each other, and the antireticularists, who believed the brain was

formed by independent nerve cells. Santiago Ramon y Cajal was a key figure in ending this

debate, developing a ‗neuron doctrine‘ that would merit him a Nobel prize in 1906. Using

Camillo Golgi‘s method for staining brain tissue, Cajal was able to demonstrate that neurons are

single independent units and describe in detail their anatomical components (Ramón y Cajal,

1995).

These observations also allowed Cajal to infer that nerve cells were polarized, with

information flowing from the dendrites to the axon of one cell and through a specialized region –

later coined ‗synapse‘ by Charles Sherrington – into the dendrites of another, so that the

integration of information occurred through the sum of the axons of several neurons converging

into the dendrites of another. Additionally, by comparing brains of different species, Cajal noted

a correlation between number of connections between neurons and place of the animal in the

Page 16: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

3

phylogenetic scale, concluding that connectivity could be a measure of intellectual power. He

also suggested that this level of connectivity could be altered by behavior, suggesting that

plasticity underlies learning, an association that still guides memory research today.

Charles Sherrington‘s study of the reflex arc provided the next step in understanding

nerve cells and their circuitry. By describing the circuit of the knee-jerk reflex (Sherrington,

1892), he observed that even though sensory neurons possessed dedicated inputs into the spinal

cord, the output neurons received input from many such cells, evidence that complex behaviours

could consist of an integration of these inputs. Sherrington also contributed to the understanding

of reciprocal innervations - or how excitatory and inhibitory influences work together to generate

movement - and proposed a hierarchy of brain structures in mediating coordinated actions, with

the cortex as the most complex followed by brain stem and spinal cord (Eichenbaum, 2002;

Molnár and Brown, 2010).

Ivan Pavlov described a somewhat similar reflex arc for the digestive system involving

the brain stem and vagus nerve, elucidating the loop between food stimulation and ingestion (i.e.

activation of the gustatory sensory system in the mouth and gullet) and the release of gastric

fluids in the stomach for which he won the Nobel prize in 1904. His major breakthrough came

through the observation that the same loop could be activated by the mere sight of the food, or of

the person bringing the food.

Based on this observation, Pavlov proposed the existence of two types of reflexes:

unconditioned reflexes (like the ones described by Sherrington), which are innate and

uncontrollable; and conditioned reflexes, which are acquired through experience (Pavlov, 2003).

In an unconditioned reflex, an unconditioned stimulus (US), such as food in the mouth, drives an

unconditioned response (UR), such as salivation and increase of gastric fluids. In a conditioned

reflex, an arbitrary conditioned stimulus (CS) is paired with the US such that the CS alone comes

to elicit a conditioned response similar to the UR. This description was critical to the concept of

classical conditioning and the methodology later used to study memory.

Herman Ebbinghaus, inspired by this research, set on to develop a framework for a

systemic study of memory, creating rigorous methodology for objective and quantitative

assessment of memory. His intention was to develop methodology that would minimize variation

and render reliable and reproducible results, starting a school of thought called behaviourism. In

Page 17: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

4

his words: ―We must try in experimental fashion to keep as constant as possible those

circumstances whose influence on retention and reproduction is known or suspected (...). The

material must be so chosen that decided differences of interest are, at least to all appearances,

excluded; equality of attention may be promoted by preventing external disturbances; sudden

fancies are not subject to control, but, on the whole, their disturbing effect is limited to the

moment, and will be of comparatively little account if the time of the experiment is extended,

etc.‖ (Ebbinghaus, 1913). Behaviourists believed behaviour could be broken down into elements

of conditioned reflexes and associations.

Based on observations of animal learning, Edward Thorndike made his contribution to

the field by proposing his ‗law of effect‘, stating that within a specific situation, a behavior that

generates a reward is likely to be repeated given that same situation, meaning rewards will

reinforce the behavior that ensues them (Thorndike, 1998). Conversely, a behavior that leads to

an unpleasant or discomforting stimulus will be avoided. This observation would guide much of

what would later be called instrumental learning in the years to come.

William James, in his Principles of Psychology, also emphasizes the importance of reflex

mechanisms for the formation of a memory, calling them the building blocks of a ‗habit‘.

Moreover, according to James, complex behaviours such as walking or singing consist of well

rehearsed sequences of simple habits or reflexes, a concept very akin to the behaviourists. With

regard to memory per se, James proposes for the first time the existence of types of memory. The

first type is an initial ‗primary memory‘ in which information is processed and remains in our

‗stream of thought‘. A ‗secondary memory‘ is more enduring, consisting of both the actual

information and the experience of learning that information. In James‘s words: ―it is the

knowledge of an event, or fact, of which meantime we have not been thinking, with the

additional consciousness that we have thought or experienced it before‖ (James, 1905).

James‘ idea of a two-component memory, although attractive, was relatively forgotten for

a few decades - when behaviourists were focused largely on animal studies. Starting in the late

1950‘s it was reintroduced as a distinction between short-term memory (STM) and long-term

memory (LTM) (Broadbent, 1958; Glanzer and Cunitz, 1966; Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1971).

Experimental confirmation of that distinction came from studies with amnesic patients that

showed preserved STM but impaired LTM (Drachman and Arbit, 1966; Baddeley and

Page 18: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

5

Warrington, 1970; Cave and Squire, 1992). The molecular and cellular underpinnings of this

distinction will be reviewed in section 1.1.3.

Importantly, William James‘ description of secondary memory captures the other

prominent school of thought in the memory field at the time, cognitivism. Cognitivists

interpreted memory and cognition as complex and elaborate phenomena, more than mere sums

of reflexes. The first step towards a resolution of the debate between cognitivists and

behaviourists came through the work of French philosopher Theodore Ribot, which started a

significant shift in the way long-term memory was perceived, as reviewed in the next sub-

section.

1.1.2 Memory gradients and grades of memory

Theodore Ribot, reviewing several cases of patients who suffered brain damage caused

by head trauma, noted that not only did they experience loss of memories prior to the trauma,

known as retrograde amnesia, but there was also a gradient in this memory loss: recent memories

were more affected than older memories. This led to the formulation of a law of regression, also

known as Ribot‘s law, that states that the likelihood of memory dissolution is inversely

proportional to the age of the memory, meaning older memories are less likely to be lost than

younger memories, suggesting a temporal reorganization of memory (Ribot, 1882).

In his words: ―We have stated these two facts in the dissolution of memory: the new

perishes before the old, the complex before the simple. The law which we have formulated is

only the psychological expression of a law of life, and pathology shows in its turn that memory is

a biological fact‖. And for a bit of poetry: ―It is a well-known fact in organic life that structures

last formed are the first to degenerate. It is, says a physiologist, analogous to what occurs in a

great commercial crisis. The old houses resist the storm; the new houses, less solid, go down on

every side. Finally, in the biological world, dissolution acts in a contrary direction to evolution: it

proceeds from the complex to the simple. Hughlings Jackson was the first to show that the higher

functions - the complex, special, voluntary functions of the nervous system - were the first to

Page 19: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

6

disappear; that the lower, the simple, general automatic functions were the last to go.‖ (Ribot,

1882).

In the decades to come, much attention was paid to the behavioural characterization of

amnesic patients, a classic case being that of patient Henry Gustave Molaison (H.M). H.M. was

one of a few patients to undergo bilateral resection of the whole medial temporal lobe to treat

severe epilepsy. After performing these surgeries, Dr. William Scoville saw ―no marked

physiologic or behavioral changes with the one exception of a very grave, recent memory

loss”(Scoville, 1954). Given the severity of his deficit, his agreeability and the lack of

confounding factors such as psychosis, H.M. was the most studied case from that series of

surgeries. He displayed both retrograde and anterograde amnesia, retaining memories from his

childhood but losing the ones closest to the time of the surgery, consistent with the law of

regression, and being unable to form new memories (Scoville and Milner, 1957), at least at first

glance.

Two important developments ensued from the study of H.M.‘s amnesia. It was the first

time that a ‗pure‘ memory deficit was identified, with no other cognitive, perceptive or motor

consequences. This really settled the debate on whether there are regions in the brain with

specific functions, and also weakened the line of thought that the cortex played the most

important role in these processes. Secondly, as devastating as H.M.‘s amnesia was, deeper tests

mostly conducted by Brenda Milner showed that H.M. still retained the ability to learn. At least

certain things. She observed that, by training him in a motor learning task, in which subjects

learn to draw a star by looking at the reflection of their hand in a mirror, H.M. showed consistent

improvement in performance across trials, in spite of never remembering doing the task (Milner

et al., 1968, 1998).

This finding led the way to a series of careful investigations of the memory deficits in

amnesic patients, with important methodological developments. The initial findings suggested

that motor learning had a decisive role in determining whether memory for a task would be

preserved. Subsequent research, however, showed that amnesic patients also performed similarly

to controls in tasks involving learning of perceptual and cognitive skills (Corkin, 1968; Brooks

and Baddeley, 1976; Benzing and Squire, 1989), word priming (Graf et al., 1984; Shimamura,

1986; Tulving and Schacter, 1990), and simple pavlovian conditioning (Daum et al., 1989).

Page 20: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

7

The first to suggest that these ‗exceptions‘ to amnesia actually composed a separate type

of memory were Cohen and Squire. After training amnesic patients to perform a pattern

analyzing skill – reading mirrored words – they saw that the patients were able to improve their

mirror reading skills despite claiming not to remember any of the words they had read. They

proposed that this evidence provided physiological basis for a distinction between procedural or

rule-based information (spared in amnesiacs) and declarative, or data-based information;

between knowing how and knowing that (Cohen and Squire, 1980). Daniel Schacter refers to

these two types of memory as implicit, when it does ―not involve any conscious or explicit

recollection of a prior episode‖, or explicit, when it does (Schacter, 1990).

Besides categorizing memory into different types, this mapping soon was extended to

anatomy. A more recent and well accepted division of these multiple memory systems refers to

declarative and nondeclarative memory (Squire, 1992). Declarative memory (or explicit

memory) refers to memory that involves conscious recollection, is fast, flexible, not always

reliable (subject to forgetting) and is dependent on the hippocampus and surrounding medial

temporal lobe areas and diencephalon (Squire et al., 1993). Non-declarative memory (or implicit

memory) encompasses a number of independent abilities, is generally slow, inflexible (meaning

the parameters have to be the same in a given situation for the memory to be accessed), reliable,

and involves areas outside of the medial temporal lobe and diencephalon (Squire et al., 1993).

Declarative memory can be further classified into episodic and semantic memory.

Episodic memory encompasses personal or autobiographical memories (including contextual and

spatial memories), generally regarding questions such as what, where and when (Tulving, 2002).

Semantic memory relates to a general or concept-based knowledge of the world. Semantic

memory refers to facts, whereas episodic memory has to do with events. Both types of memory

are declarative, in the sense that retrieval of information is carried out explicitly and subjects are

aware that stored information is being accessed (Tulving, 1987, 1992; Squire, 1992; Squire et al.,

1993).

Non-declarative (implicit or procedural) memory is perhaps the type of memory that best

relates to what behaviourists were investigating in their studies of reflexes, and what William

James qualified as ‗habits‘: skillful behavior that does not require conscious recollection for

retention. It includes motor, perceptual, perceptuo-motor and cognitive skills, simple classical

conditioning, emotional learning and priming (Squire and Zola-Morgan, 1988; Squire, 1992).

Page 21: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

8

Studies showed that these sub-categories often rely in different brain areas, constituting

independent systems. For instance, skills are mostly dependent on the neostriatum (Packard et

al., 1978; Heindel et al., 1988, 1989; Saint-Cyr et al., 1988), emotional conditioning on the

amygdala (Davis, 1992; Kim and Fanselow, 1992), and some types of priming on the neocortex

(Squire et al., 1992). A scheme of this classification can be seen in Fig. 1.

Figure 1: Scheme of memory classification. Types of memory and the main underlying brain structures are

represented. Adapted from Squire, 1992.

1.1.3 Memory consolidation

Georg Müller and Alfons Pilzecker were the first to introduce the concept of memory

consolidation in 1900, proposing that the progression from learning of new information to

forming a permanent memory is not instantaneous, but that the information needs to be fixated or

consolidated, during which period it remains vulnerable to loss (Lechner et al., 1999). This

concept gained popularity in the late 1940‘s with the observation that electroconvulsive shocks

caused retrograde amnesia in rats (Duncan, 1949), which contributed to a long quest for animal

models of retrograde amnesia (McGaugh, 1966).

The study of memory consolidation can be divided into two levels: synaptic and systems

consolidation. The first refers to cellular and molecular events that take place in a time scale of

minutes to hours to render information fixed in synapses. The second refers to a reorganization

of memory within and across brain regions taking place in the time scale of weeks to years. Both

types of consolidation will be reviewed in separate subsections.

Page 22: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

9

1.1.3.1 Synaptic consolidation

In 1949, Donald Hebb proposed a dual trace mechanism for memory, in which an initial

memory relies on neuronal reverberations, whose coordinated persistence leads to synaptic

connections, the cellular mechanism for consolidation: ―Let us assume that the persistence or

repetition of a reverberatory activity (or "trace") tends to induce lasting cellular changes that add

to its stability. (…) When an axon of cell A is near enough to excite a cell B and repeatedly or

persistently takes part in firing it, some growth process or metabolic change takes place in one or

both cells such that A's efficiency, as one of the cells firing B, is increased‖ (Hebb, 1949).

The discovery that protein synthesis inhibitors selectively disrupted LTM (Barondes and

Jarvik, 1964; Agranoff et al., 1966; Davis and Squire, 1984), whereas stimulant drugs could

enhance it (McGaugh, 1973), helped crystallize the concept of consolidation as a cellular process

that transforms ephemeral traces into persistent stable memories.

Extensive understanding of the molecular and cellular components of short and long-term

implicit memory came from studies with invertebrates such as the marine snail Aplysia

(reviewed in Kandel, 2002). An important insight into the cellular basis of explicit memory came

from the work of Timothy Bliss and Terje Lømo, who observed that persistent stimulation of the

perforant path led to a prolonged enhancement of excitatory post-synaptic potentials (EPSPs) in

dentate granule cells, a phenomenon they called long-term potentiation (LTP) (Bliss and Lømo,

1973).

Certain shared characteristics between LTP and memory made it an appealing cellular

mechanism for memory: it is present in the hippocampus (a crucial memory hub), is fast and

long-lasting, specific (neighboring synapses are unaffected) and associative, meaning integration

of different inputs leads to better potentiation (Morris et al., 1990a). Additionally, inhibition of

N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, which blocked LTP, also impaired spatial memory

(Morris et al., 1986) and place field stability (Kentros et al., 1998) in rats.

Later research showed that LTP also posessed two phases. Early LTP (E-LTP) is

produced by a single train of stimuli, lasts from 1 to 3 hours and can still be induced in the

presence of transcription or translational inhibitors (Krug et al., 1984). Persistent stimulation

Page 23: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

10

gives rise to late phase LTP (L-LTP), dependent on gene expression and protein synthesis

(Nguyen et al., 1994). As with memory formation (Restivo et al., 2009), L-LTP is associated

with neuronal structural changes (Fifková and Van Harreveld, 1977), some of which depend on

protein synthesis (Fifková et al., 1982; Stanton et al., 1984).

The molecular cascade responsible for L-LTP involves the activation of NMDA receptors

and influx of calcium (Nguyen and Kandel, 1996), activation of Calcium-calmodulin kinase II

(CamKII) (Silva et al., 1992a, 1992b), cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) and protein

kinase A (PKA) (Frey et al., 1993; Abel et al., 1997), cAMP responsive element binding protein

(CREB) (Bourtchuladze et al., 1994; Kida et al., 2002) and on the opposite vector of the cascade

calcineurin (Mansuy et al., 1998; Malleret et al., 2001) and protein phosphatase 1 (PP1) (Blitzer

et al., 1998).

To attempt to explain the synapse specificity of L-LTP, Richard Morris and his

colleagues put forward a theory called synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. According to this

theory, stimulation (E-LTP) leads to the formation of a ‗synaptic tag‘, a protein synthesis

independent process that recruits plasticity related proteins (PRPs) to the potentiated synapse,

prepping it for L-LTP (Frey and Morris, 1997; Redondo and Morris, 2011).

The first experimental evidence for this was a dual LTP experiment, in which L-LTP was

formed in the presence of protein synthesis inhibitors if a nearby synapse had been potentiated

(E-LTP) shortly before (Frey and Morris, 1997). Although there remain some limitations of the

theory (such as updating the PRP mechanism to local protein synthesis in dendrites and dendritic

mRNAs) (Redondo and Morris, 2011), it is an interesting challenge to the original consolidation

hypothesis, turning synaptic consolidation into a pliant entity, and the synaptic milieu (or

heterosynapses) as a possible influence on the fate of memory traces.

Another challenge to the consolidation hypothesis gained notoriety in 2000, when Karim

Nader and colleagues showed that retrieval returned previously consolidated memories to a labile

state, vulnerable to protein synthesis inhibitors (Nader et al., 2000a). They stated that retrieval

triggered a process called reconsolidation, which shared the same molecular signatures as

synaptic consolidation (Kida et al., 2002; Nader and Einarsson, 2010). Long-term memories

Page 24: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

11

were not as static as originally thought, but were revisited and updated during retrieval (Nader et

al., 2000b).

1.1.3.2 Systems consolidation

The study of long-term consolidation or systems consolidation was initially based on data

from amnesic patients, in which a localized brain lesion could be associated to a specified degree

of memory loss. A few models were developed to explain how remote memory is formed in the

brain, two of which are reviewed here.

The first to propose a model to explain how remote memories are formed was David

Marr in the 1970‘s. According to him, simple representations of events (simple memories) are

formed in the hippocampus and, through replay during sleep, are transferred to cortical areas to

be reorganized and reclassified (Marr, 1971). These concepts remain in the core of contemporary

systems consolidation theories.

The standard model of consolidation predicts that information is originally processed in

the neocortex but needs the hippocampus to be initially stored. The medial temporal lobe or

hippocampal formation acts as a temporary store or guide reinforcing the neocortical connections

representing the memory (Squire and Alvarez, 1995). With time, the hippocampus becomes

disengaged and remote memory becomes stored and can be retrieved directly from a more

permanent memory system confined to neocortical areas (Squire and Alvarez, 1995). This is

concluded largely from studies with patients with and animal models of hippocampal lesion (for

a review see section 1.1.2), which display temporally graded retrograde amnesia and anterograde

amnesia (Squire and Alvarez, 1995).

Importantly, this model predicts that information is initially processed in parallel in the

hippocampus and cortex, and the replay of the hippocampal network leads to strengthening of

cortico-cortico connections until the memory is incorporated with older cortical memories and

becomes independent of the hippocampus, implying that changes in strength of hippocampal-

Page 25: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

12

cortex connections are fast and temporary, whereas cortico-cortico connections are slow and

permanent (O‘Reilly and McClelland, 1994; Frankland and Bontempi, 2005).

Nadel and Moscovitch proposed an alternative model of systems consolidation, Multiple

Trace Theory (MTT). Through a review of the case studies and animal literature they point out

two inconsistencies with the standard model. First, medial temporal lobe damage can generate

ungraded retrograde amnesia, particularly for episodic (autobiographical and spatial) memories

(Rosenbaum et al., 2000; Cipolotti et al., 2001; Viskontas et al., 2002; Martin et al., 2005).

Second, retrieval of detailed remote autobiographical memories engages the hippocampus (Ryan

et al., 2001; Gilboa et al., 2004).

According to MTT, new information is initially coded by the hippocampus, that later acts

as a pointer to form a memory trace binding hippocampal and cortical networks. Each

reactivation recruits the hippocampus to form another memory trace that is linked to the already

existing cortical one (Nadel and Moscovitch, 1997). This multitude of traces facilitates the

extraction of factual from contextual information, so that the hippocampus persistently stores the

temporal and spatial context of a memory, whereas the cortex stores the semantic information

(Nadel and Moscovitch, 1997). Although reports of preserved spatial and autobiographical

memories in a patient with extensive medial frontal lobe damage exist (Teng and Squire, 1999),

MTT supporters question the degree of detail of these preserved memories (Rosenbaum et al.,

2000).

The next sub-section further explores the anatomy and function of the hippocampal

formation before going into more depth on the animal literature pertaining to hippocampal lesion

and inactivation studies and their consequences for learning and systems consolidation.

1.2 Hippocampus

The hippocampus is an inherent part of memory processing. This section discusses the

anatomy, development and function of the hippocampus. Next follows an examination of the

dentate gyrus; a sub-region which continues to generate neurons throughout adulthood.

Page 26: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

13

1.2.1 The hippocampal formation: an overview

The hippocampal formation is comprised of eight regions: cornu ammonis (CA)3, CA2,

CA1, dentate gyrus (DG), subiculum, presubiculum, parasubiculum and entorhinal cortex. Given

the considerable amount of debate over this nomenclature in the literature, to facilitate the

dialogue with hippocampal lesion studies, these eight regions are collectively refered to as the

hippocampal formation, and to CA1, CA2, CA3 and the DG as hippocampus proper throughout

this thesis.

One feature that makes the hippocampal formation unique is the predominance of

unidirectional connections within its structure. Most neocortical inputs reach the hippocampal

formation through the entorhinal cortex. This structure launches two sets of unidirectional

projections forming one of the major hippocampal input pathways: the perforant path. Neurons

from layer II of the entorhinal cortex project to the dentate gyrus and CA3. Neurons from layer

III project to CA1 and subiculum. In another unidirectional pathway, DG granule cells project to

CA3 pyramidal cells, through the mossy fibers. CA3 pyramidal cells, following this same rule,

project to CA1 forming the Schaffer collaterals. This part of the circuit, comprising the synapses

EC-DG, DG-CA3 and CA3-CA1 is referred as the trisynaptic circuit (Andersen et al., 1971)

(Fig. 2).

CA1, in its turn, projects nonreciprocably to the subiculum and entorhinal cortex. The

subiculum projects to the parasubiculum and presubiculum, as well as to the entorhinal cortex.

Hence, CA1 and subiculum form a loop, receiving input from the superficial layers of the

entorhinal cortex and projecting back to its deep layers.

Page 27: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

14

Figure 2: Trisynaptic circuit. Representation of hippocampal areas CA1, CA3 and dentate gyrus (DG) and the

projections that form the trisynaptic circuit. Projections from the entorhinal cortex reach the DG through the

perforant path (PP). Dentate granule cells project to pyramidal CA3 cells through mossy fibers (MF). CA3

pyramidal cells project to CA1 cells forming the Schaffer collaterals (SC).

1.2.2 Hippocampal functions: we are more than just our memories

The shape and anatomy of the hippocampus raised scientific interest very early in history,

starting as early as ancient Greece. The term hippocampus – named because of its sea horse

shape – first appeared in the 16th

century, being proposed by Giulio Cesare Aranzi (Lewis,

1923). This comparison was not so welcomed amongst his contemporaries, some preferring the

likeness of a silk worm or the horn of a ram (Lewis, 1923).

In the 18th

century, de Garangeot proposed a new term, Ammon‘s horns, after the

Egyptian god: ―Following the ventricles in their downward curve . . . they terminate by a blunt

point which resembles sufficiently a ram‘s horn. It is alleged that this blunt point makes a small

winding at its tip, as do the ends of rams‘ horns and for this reason, then, they are named the

horns of Ammon” (Lewis, 1923). Even though the silk worm resemblance seems to have been

forgotten, the comparisons to a sea horse and the horns of Ammon remain to this day. In the 19th

century hippocampal cytoarchitecture fuelled Camillo Golgi‘s interest, featuring in some of his

most beautiful pictures (Bentivoglio and Swanson, 2001).

Page 28: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

15

Function attributed to the hippocampus changed quite a few times in the last century.

Until the 1940‘s, the hippocampus was considered part of the olfactory system, largely due to the

presence of olfactory projections surrounding the hippocampal formation, coupled with its

particularly large size in macrosmatic animals, and reports of olfactory related behaviors in

response to hippocampal stimulation or seizures (Brodal, 1947). However, the absence of an

olfactory deficit in animals with neocortical or hippocampal lesions and the presence of

hippocampi in microsmatic animals, among other evidence, mostly discredited this view (Brodal,

1947).

Papez proffered that the hippocampus, along with the hypothalamus and the gyrus

cinguli, formed an ―ensemble of structures (…) proposed as representing theoretically the

anatomical basis of emotions‖ (Papez, 1937), an assumption again to be refuted by Brodal for a

lack of biological basis (Brodal, 1981). Around the 1950‘s the discovery of theta oscillations

and their relation to attention (Green and Arduini, 1954) led to a series of studies linking the

hippocampus with attention control (Kaada and Pribram, 1949; Sloan and Jasper, 1950; Kaada et

al., 1953; Grastyan et al., 1959).

In the 1960‘s, studies showing an increase in basal glucocorticoid levels in response to

hippocampal lesions (Knigge, 1961) drew attention to the mechanisms mediating the relationship

between stress and cognition (McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995; de Kloet et al., 1999).

Glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptors are abundant in the hippocampus (Herman et al.,

1989), and particularly the ventral hippocampus has been shown to negatively control the

hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis (Jankord and Herman, 2008), even though the

precise mechanism is still unclear.

Presently, the most prominent line of hippocampal function research involves its role in

learning and memory. Significant early efforts on this line date back to 1950‘s and 60‘s, through

the work of Penfield, Scoville and Milner (see next sub-section). Despite the current prevalence

of a hippocampal view of learning and memory, alternative theories and criticism are present, a

couple of which are mentioned in the next paragraphs.

Vanderwolf and Cain (1994) made an extensive case against limiting the study of

behaviour to pre-existing psychological theories expressing a reductionist view of the mind,

compartmentalized into perception, attention, memory, motivation, etc. According to these

Page 29: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

16

authors, this ―psychological approach (…does) not take adequate account of the importance of

the intrinsic organization of the brain and the instinctive behavior that results from it. Further, if

the behavior of all mammals is discussed in terms of the same processes of sensation, perception,

cognition, memory, etc., actual behavior is trivialized and interspecific differences tend to be

ignored.‖ (Vanderwolf and Cain, 1994). They argue for a conceptual reorientation, through

which the hippocampus is not ‗uniquely important‘ for memory but part of a broader circuit, one

to be unlocked by unbiased assessment of experience driven behavior.

Another theory involves the role of the hippocampus in behavioural inhibition (Kimble,

1968) and anxiety (Gray, 1982). Gray and McNaughton (2003) proposed that the hippocampus

acts as a conflict solver, thus serving a function more basic than memory. Within this framework,

the hippocampus detects and solves conflicts by suppressing conflicting or competing

information, thereby minimizing interference (Gray and Mcnaughton, 2003; Davidson and

Jarrard, 2004). This is an interesting counterpoint to theories of the hippocampus being involved

in one type of memory, as will be reviewed in the next sub-section.

1.2.3 Hippocampal functions: learning and memory

A connection between memory and the hippocampus first arose in the 1880‘s through the

description of what is now known as Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, an illness derived from

high alcohol consumption that leads to amnesia (Victor and Yakovlev, 1955). This syndrome

was later associated with a pathology of the mammillary bodies and the mediodorsal nucleus of

the thalamus, targets of hippocampal efferent input (see Kopelman et al., 2009 for a review).

Evidence for a direct role of the hippocampal formation in memory became available a

few years later, through the emergence of case studies of patients with damage to the

hippocampus who presented memory deficits ( see Orbach et al., 1960 for a review). In the early

1950‘s, Wilder Penfield and Brenda Milner observed that some patients who had undergone

unilateral temporal lobe removal to treat epilepsy presented anterograde amnesia (Penfield and

Milner, 1958). These results interested another surgeon, William Scoville, who contacted

Penfield and Milner on account of some of his own patient findings.

Page 30: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

17

Following a series of bilateral medial temporal lobe resections performed in patients who

suffered from psychosis or severe epilepsy, William Scoville observed an unexpected result:

severe memory loss (Scoville, 1954). Further analysis of the outcome of this type of operation

led Scoville and Milner to establish a relationship between the degree of memory impairment

and the extent of the hippocampal lesion (Scoville and Milner, 1957). In particular, observations

of one such patient, H.M., who incurred anterograde and retrograde amnesia following bilateral

hippocampal resection (Scoville and Milner, 1957; Sagar et al., 1985), was arguably a turning

point for the study of memory, setting new theoretical and experimental groundwork (see section

1.1.2).

This discovery incited numerous but somewhat contradictory attempts at finding an

animal model for amnesia, in monkeys (Orbach et al., 1960; Correll and Scoville, 1967) and rats

(Kaada et al., 1961; Kimble, 1963, 1968; Kveim et al., 1964; Isaacson and Kimble, 1972). Some

clarification was brought to the field in the 1970‘s with theoretical advances in the classification

of types of memory, which were presumably subserved by different circuits (see section 1.1.2).

This allowed for the development of targeted tasks and clarified the role of the hippocampus in

encoding of spatial and contextual memory. The following section explores in detail some of

these tasks and the level of hippocampal involvement therein.

The theory proposing a connection between hippocampus and declarative memory led to

the development of an animal model of amnesia in primates, with a task termed delayed

matching to sample (DMTS). In this recognition memory task, monkeys were shown a particular

object and were subsequently (at varying time intervals) given the option of choosing between a

new object and the one that was previously shown (a measure of STM) (Gaffan, 1974). Choosing

the familiar object was rewarded with a sugar puff. Two variants were added to the task: longer

delays and a choice phase in which several previously rewarded objects were to be selected from

non-rewarded ones (lists) (Gaffan, 1974). Animals subjected to fornix lesions showed delay-

dependent and list-length deficits, but intact short-term memory and intellectual function (since

they could remember the matching-to-sample rule at shorter delays) (Gaffan, 1974), consistent

with the behavior of amnesic patients.

Using a variation of DMTS in which animals were required to select the novel object

(delayed nonmatching to sample or DNMTS), Mishkin showed that combined (but not separate)

Page 31: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

18

lesions of amygdala and hippocampus caused severe impairments in delay-dependent and list

length tests, thus reproducing the lesion type and behavioural profile of patient H.M. (Mishkin,

1978). DNMTS became the most widely used task in a battery of declarative memory animal

models of amnesia (Squire and Zola-Morgan, 1988). In the years to come much of these data has

been revisited and concerns have been raised with regard to the extent of the lesions and

interpretation of the results (Mumby, 2001; Squire, 2004), questioning the role of the

hippocampus in recognition memory and declarative memory theory of hippocampal function as

a whole (Murray and Wise, 2004).

In 1971, O‘Keefe and Dostrovsky recorded a population of hippocampal cells that

displayed preferential activation in specific spatial locations, referred to as place cells (O‘Keefe

and Dostrovsky, 1971). This discovery led O‘Keefe and Nadel to propose a new theory of

hippocampal function, the cognitive map theory, claiming that ―the hippocampus is the core of a

neural memory system providing an objective spatial framework within which the items and

events of an organism's experience are located and interrelated‖ (O‘Keefe and Nadel, 1978).

The cognitive map theory‘s basic proposition is that spatial information (with regard to

an allocentric representation) is stored in a map format - constituting a framework they called

‗locale‘system, which is a cognitive map or schema used by animals in order to navigate. They

claimed that all allocentric spatial information, or locale, is permanently stored in the

hippocampus, and that this memory system emerged neuroethologically in response to

environmental pressures for naturalistic behaviours such as food storing in birds and mating in

small mammals (O‘Keefe and Nadel, 1978; Jacobs and Spencer, 1994; Clayton and Krebs,

1995).

This theory was mostly based from findings that hippocampal lesions impair performance

in spatial tasks in rats (O‘Keefe et al., 1975; Olton et al., 1978; O‘Keefe and Nadel, 1978) and

indications that place cells encode a map of environmental cues (Muller and Kubie, 1987;

O‘Keefe and Burgess, 1996), presumably forming a topological representation of space.

However, much evidence has shown that this spatial representation is not cohesive, with

overlapping or fragmented coding (Shapiro et al., 1997; Tanila et al., 1997; Eichenbaum et al.,

1999). The circuit underlying spatial representation has also been put in question, especially with

regard to which brain areas are involved and how this cognitive map is used for navigation.

Page 32: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

19

Although the most distinct firing patterns are predominant in the CA1 region, all

hippocampal subfields have place-responding cells (Barnes et al., 1990). Moreover, spatial

computation is not exclusive to the hippocampus, since place firing in CA1 persists in the

absence of intrahippocampal inputs from the DG (Mcnaughton et al., 1989) or CA3 (Brun et al.,

2002), suggesting that CA1 receives direct spatial signals from the entorhinal cortex via the

perforant path.

Indeed, the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) has cells that respond to spatial signals (Fyhn

et al., 2004), but unlike hippocampal place cells, these cells have multiple firing fields organized

in an array of tessellated triangles, or grid: a very likely element of a spatial metric system

(Hafting et al., 2005). Nevertheless, having a functional metric system – or a cognitive map - and

using it for navigation are different things. In order to move from one location to another, some

algorithm must be in place to link one map to the next, a path integrator. This integrator is most

likely outside of the hippocampus – which would facilitate discrimination between overlapping

spatial maps - and might involve MEC and grid cells, but is currently the subject of theoretical

models (Moser et al., 2008). Although it is irrefutable that the hippocampus is critical for spatial

memory, how this spatial memory is accessed, integrated and commanded is still unclear.

1.2.3.1 A task for spatial memory: Morris Water Maze

The cognitive map theory‘s premise that the hippocampus is necessary for navigation led

to the development of navigational tasks, such as the radial maze (Olton and Samuelson, 1976)

and the Morris Water Maze (MWM) (Morris, 1981). Richard Morris created a task in which the

goal was invisible (distal localization), inaudible, could not be detected by smell and was not

marked by local cues, so that finding the hidden goal could only be achieved by inferring its

location relative to distal cues (Morris, 1981), a concept akin to place cells.

The task apparatus consists of a large circular pool of opaque water. The pool hides a

submerged escape platform placed at a fixed location and is surrounded by distal cues on the

walls (see Fig. 3 for a scheme). An animal is placed placed in the water at different starting

locations. Over several trials the animal finds the hidden platform with decreasing latencies.

Page 33: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

20

During a probe test, the platform is absent and the time spent close to its former location is used

as a measure of learning. Both rats and mice are natural swimmers, although there is evidence

that rats are better swimmers (Dagg and Windsor, 1972).

Figure 3. MWM scheme. Distal cues are located on the walls and a camera records the animal‘s swim path. The

animal is dropped at random start locations to find a submerged platform.

Morris and colleagues demonstrated that animals with hippocampal (but not cortical)

lesions were severely impaired in finding the hidden platform (place navigation) (Morris et al.,

1982). When they were trained with a visible platform (cue navigation), however, they behaved

similarly to controls (Morris et al., 1982). Since the cue navigation version of the task shares the

same basic elements as the place navigation, i.e. swimming, motivation, vision, this shows that

the deficit exhibited by the hippocampal lesioned animals was in the spatial learning component

of the task.

The simplicity of the task, the absence of odour cues (as opposed to dry mazes), the

possibility of using automated tracking systems, and the flexibility of design (visible vs. hidden

platform tasks) have contributed to the popularization of the MWM as one of the most frequently

used tasks in behavioural neuroscience (D‘Hooge and De Deyn, 2001). The MWM is frequently

used as a general task to assess cognitive ability in a variety of models, including ischemia

(Block, 1999), Alzheimer‘s disease (Quon et al., 1991), pre-natal drug exposure (Cutler et al.,

1996) and developmental disorders such as fragile X syndrome (D‘Hooge et al., 1997).

Critics of the task argue that water is aversive to the animals and could lead to

considerable stress and endocrinological changes - particularly on the first training days - and

Page 34: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

21

impact learning (Wenk, 2004). Indeed, MWM training leads to significant increases in serum

corticosterone in mice (Harrison et al., 2009) and rats (Beiko et al., 2004), which can be reduced

with pre-exposure to the pool (Beiko et al., 2004). Stewart and Morris, however, argue that stress

should be minimal at proper water temperature (25oC) (in Arjun Sahgal, 1983).

Later studies showed that water maze deficits were found in animals with lesions to the

entorhinal cortex (Schenk and Morris, 1985), dentate gyrus (Sutherland et al., 1983), perforant

path (Skelton and McNamara, 1992), septum (Hagan et al., 1988), and subiculum (Taube et al.,

1992), leading to the overall agreement that a complete hippocampal formation is necessary for

MWM performance. Importantly, this hippocampal dependency is not temporally graded, since

hippocampal lesions performed long after MWM training still disrupt performance (Clark et al.,

2005; Teixeira et al., 2006), indicating that the hippocampus is always necessary for MWM

memory.

Interestingly, aspiration (Moser et al., 1993) or ibotenic acid (Moser et al., 1995) partial

lesions of the hippocampus revealed a functional dissociation in the septotemporal axis, with

dorsal lesions being far more disruptive for MWM learning than ventral ones: normal MWM

performance can be supported by lesion of ~60-80% of the hippocampus, as long as the dorsal

part is the one spared. Consistent with this, place cells are more numerous and have more

focused place fields in the dorsal compared to ventral hippocampus (Jung et al., 1994). A step

further, Moser and Moser showed a dissociation between acquisition and retrieval in the MWM:

while lesions of ~80% of the hippocampus do not impair acquisition, a loss of as little as ~30%

leads to a deficit in retrieval (Moser and Moser, 1998). This topic will be explored in more detail

in the discussion.

1.2.3.2 A task for contextual memory: Fear conditioning

Studies with amnesic patients strongly indicated a role for the hippocampus in encoding

contextual components of memories (i.e. episodic memories). Cognitive map theory and place

cells also suggested the importance of the hippocampus for spatial representation. Contextual

Page 35: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

22

fear conditioning (CFC) is a task that explores contextual representation memories using a fear

conditioning framework.

Fear is necessary for survival, is a robust and conserved response across species, and

perhaps for that reason it is one of the most studied and well understood neural circuits (Johansen

et al., 2011). Pavlovian fear conditioning is a form of associative learning designed to take

advantage of these characteristics. The basic premise is to assess contextual memory by making

it salient through an association between an aversive stimulus (a foot shock) and the context in

which it is received (fear chamber). Fear conditioning can be learned in a single session and

produces a robust and long-lasting memory, contributing to its popularity to this day, and

recently raising interest as a possible model for post-traumatic stress disorder (Jovanovic and

Ressler, 2010).

The most popular version of the CFC task was developed by Michael Fanselow

(Fanselow, 1980, 2000a) and consists of two phases. In the training phase, an animal is placed in

a chamber with a metal grid floor (context A). The animal is allowed to explore the chamber for

a few minutes and then receives a mild foot shock. In the probe phase, animals are returned to

the context in which they received the shock (Fig. 4A). During probe, learning of the context-

shock association leads to fear to receive a shock, expressed as periods of immobility, or absence

of all movement except for breathing, referred to as freezing behavior (Fanselow, 1982).

Freezing is believed to be part of a species-specific array of innate defensive reactions (Bolles,

1970) that occur in response to natural stimuli (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1972).

In the CFC task, from a Pavlovian perspective, the foot shock represents the US, and

freezing the CR. Freezing is incited by the context, which constitutes the CS (Fanselow, 2000b).

Freezing does not occur in the absence of shock, is specific to the context and proportional to

shock intensity (Fanselow, 1980), although generalization might occur (see later).

In a different version of the CFC task, an audible tone precedes the shock, and, during the

probe in a different chamber (context B), freezing in response to the tone is used as an index of

tone-shock association. Tone fear conditioning or auditory FC has been shown to depend on the

amygdala, where auditory and nociceptive stimuli converge to form the association that leads to

freezing (Maren et al., 1996; Maren, 2003; Rosen and Donley, 2006; Johansen et al., 2010, 2011)

(see Fig. 4B for a scheme).

Page 36: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

23

The first evidence that contextual and auditory fear conditioning could involve different

brain circuits came in 1991. Selden and colleagues paired a clicker with a shock and saw a

double dissociation: animals with amygdala lesions were selectively impaired in conditioning to

the explicit cue (clicker), whereas animals with hippocampal lesions were selectively impaired in

conditioning to the context (Selden et al., 1991). This finding was later confirmed by different

groups (Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992).

Figure 4. Fear conditioning scheme. Training and probe chambers and the experimental designs are illustrated. A.

Contextual fear conditioning. B. Tone fear conditioning. Adapted from Wang et al., 2008.

According to Fanselow, the initial exploration of the chamber creates a contextual

representation (‗gestalt memory‘) integrating all of its sensory components, i.e. visual

appearance, somatosensory components and odour, that is active at the time of, and comes to be

associated with, the shock (Fanselow, 2000b). Evidence for this comes from experiments

showing that a minimum amount of time spent exploring the chamber prior to the shock is

necessary in order for the contextual representation to become associated with the shock,

otherwise no freezing is observed during probe, a phenomenon called immediate shock deficit

(ISD) (Fanselow, 1990; Frankland et al., 2004). Pre-exposure to the context - but not its isolated

features - alleviates this deficit (Fanselow, 1990; Rudy and O‘Reilly, 1999; Frankland et al.,

2004).

Anterograde amnesia for contextual fear memories following dorsal hippocampal lesions

is found in some (Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Young et al., 1994) but not other (Phillips and

LeDoux, 1994; Maren et al., 1997; Frankland et al., 1998) studies. A possible explanation

Page 37: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

24

pertains to two possible forms of acquiring contextual information: polimodal (or configural) vs.

unimodal (or elementar). The hippocampus is believed to form a polimodal unified

representation of context (Sutherland and Rudy, 1989), although a unimodal component of the

context (e.g. tone, or a particular odour) may be sufficient to generate recognition - and freezing

- in the animal.

According to that view, in the absence of the hippocampus, other brain regions may

support learning about particular elements of the context (unimodal or elementar), hence

avoiding the anterograde amnesia, but are still unable to form a configural representation.

Consistent with this, animals with pre-training HPC lesions acquire CFC but are impaired in

contextual discrimination, a task best solved through polimodal-based contextual encoding

(Frankland et al., 1998).

Kim and Fanselow showed that hippocampal dependency of CFC is temporally graded

(Kim and Fanselow, 1992). Rats were subjected to tone and context conditioning and underwent

electrolytic lesions of the hippocampus 1, 7, 14 or 28 days after training. Memory to the context

was spared in all but the day 1 group, while tone memory was preserved, showing a temporally

graded retrograde amnesia for context memory (Kim and Fanselow, 1992), that was later

confirmed with between (Maren et al., 1997) and within subject designs (Anagnostaras et al.,

1999).

Additionally, animals pre-exposed to the training context 28 days before hippocampal

lesions did not present ISD (Young et al., 1994), reinforcing the notion that the hippocampus is

necessary for consolidating this unified representation of the contextual CS (not the CS-US

association), that is later stored elsewhere, consistent with the standard model of consolidation

(see section 1.1.3.2). Interestingly, the level of generalized freezing in context B also increases

with time, suggesting that memory precision is lost over time (Biedenkapp and Rudy, 2007;

Wiltgen and Silva, 2007; Winocur et al., 2007), fitting with multiple trace theory. Although it is

well accepted that the general contextual representation in fear conditioning is hippocampus

dependent, the level of hippocampal involvement in remote CFC memory is still under debate

(see also Wang et al., 2009).

Page 38: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

25

1.2.4 Dentate Gyrus

Even though extensive work has been done in solidifying the connection between the

hippocampus and learning and memory, the specific contribution of the DG remains a topic of

some debate. Historically, the dentate gyrus has been the locus of crucial breakthroughs in the

field. LTP was first demonstrated in dentate granule cells (Bliss and Lømo, 1973), the very cells

in which local dendritic protein synthesis was first demonstrated (Aakalu et al., 2001; Jiang and

Schuman, 2002).

In terms of function, Rolls and others have proposed that the DG could be critical for

integrating information from different sensory inputs during the encoding of spatial

representations (Rolls, 1996). The link between DG function and spatial processing is supported

by an array of DG lesion studies describing significant deficits in acquisition and retrieval in

tasks such as the MWM (Sutherland et al., 1983; Nanry et al., 1989; Xavier et al., 1999; Jeltsch

et al., 2001), radial arm maze (Walsh et al., 1986; McLamb et al., 1988; Emerich and Walsh,

1989) and contextual fear conditioning (Lee and Kesner, 2004).

Spatial pattern separation has also emerged as a putative function of the DG, with support

from both quantitative modeling (O‘Reilly and McClelland, 1994; Rolls, 1996) and experimental

observations (Gilbert et al., 2001; Leutgeb et al., 2007; McHugh et al., 2007). Recently, adult

generated dentate granule cells were also implicated in this function (Clelland et al., 2009) (see

section 1.3.2.3).

The next subsections review some of the anatomical, biochemical and cellular

characteristics that may underlie the role the DG plays in information processing within the

hippocampal formation.

1.2.4.1 Anatomical organization

The dentate gyrus is composed of three layers: the molecular layer (ML), mostly devoid

of cells, but comprising the dendrites of dentate granule cells and efferent perforant path fibers,

Page 39: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

26

the dentate granule cell layer (DGCL), a ~60m layer of densely packed granule cells, and the

hilus or polymorphic cell layer (Fig. 5A). During adulthood, an abundance of progenitor cells are

located at the bottom of the DGCL in contact with the hilus, a region referred to as the

subgranular zone (SGZ) (Fig. 5A) (Amaral et al., 2007).

The DG is further subdivided into three portions. The overall shape of the DG resembles

that of an arrow head, and CA3 protrudes into its concave surface serving as a reference for this

next anatomical division. The suprapyramidal (or upper) blade of the DG comprises the area

above the CA3 protrusion (located between CA3 and CA1). Opposite the upper blade is the

infrapyramidal (or lower) blade, located below the CA3 line. Connecting the two blades is the

crest (Fig. 5B). Granule cells in the upper blade have greater dendritic length, spine number and

density compared to those in the lower blade (Desmond and Levy, 1985; Claiborne et al., 1990).

Figure 5. Dentate gyrus organization. A. micrograph of the DG showing its organization in layers. B. Anatomical

division of the DG. ML=molecular layer, DGCL=dentate granule cell layer, SGZ=subgranular zone.

1.2.4.2 Genesis of the DG

The dentate gyrus is the last hippocampal structure to appear during development (Bayer,

1980). The genesis of the dentate gyrus can be divided into three phases: embryonic, infantile

and adult. Dentate granule cells originate in the primary dentate neuroepithelium, a germinal

matrix adjacent to the one that gives rise to pyramidal cell precursors (Altman and Bayer,

1990a). The onset of dentate development starts at embryonic day 16 (E16) in the rat (Bayer,

1980), with the proliferation of precursor cells from the primary dentate neuroepithelium. At

Page 40: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

27

E18, the primary dentate matrix is surrounded by a secondary matrix that, at E19, initiates a first

wave of migration, leading to the formation of the outer shell of the DGCL, which is completed

at birth (Altman and Bayer, 1990a, 1990b).

A second wave of dentate migration reaches the dentate by the first few days after birth.

This second wave is the source of the tertiary matrix and the subgranular zone, the substrates for

the infantile and adult phases of DG development, respectively. The infantile (or postnatal) phase

starts at P1 and consists of a volumetric expansion of the DGCL by the tertiary matrix of the

dentate (Altman and Bayer, 1990b).

Even though the majority of granule cells originate from the secondary and tertiary

matrices, these germinal regions drastically decline around P10, and by P20-30 proliferating cells

from the tertiary matrix are no longer observed (Altman and Bayer, 1990b). This marks the onset

of the adult phase of DG development, when proliferative cells accumulate and are restricted to

the subgranular zone, where they continue to proliferate throughout the life of the animal

(Altman and Bayer, 1990b).

In the mouse DG a similar patter arises, with the first neurons detected at around E10,

and a peak at E17-18 (Angevine, 1965). The number of new cells largely declines at around P10

and, starting at P20, cells are fewer and restricted to the SGZ (Angevine, 1965), thus establishing

the onset of the adult phase of mouse DG neurogenesis, which also continues throughout life

(Kempermann et al., 1998).

1.2.4.3 Granule cells

Dentate granule cells have an elliptical soma of ~m width by ~18m height

(Claiborne et al., 1990). Their dendrites extend to the ML and receive anatomically-specific

inputs. The entorhinal projections are limited to the outer two thirds of the molecular layer: the

most superficial third of the molecular layer receives input from the lateral perforant path, and

the middle third is innervated by medial entorhinal fibers (Steward, 1976).

Presubiculum and parasubiculum projections also reach the molecular layer in a region

strewn between the perforant path projections (Köhler, 1985), suggesting a thalamic link to the

Page 41: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

28

DG. Furthermore, other sources of input to the DGCL or the hilus are cholinergic and

GABAergic projections from the septal nuclei (Amaral and Kurz, 1985), glutamatergic

projections from hypothalamic nuclei (Kiss et al., 2000), noradrenergic input from the locus

coeruleus (Pickel et al., 1974), dopaminergic from the VTA (Amaral et al., 2007), and

serotoninergic from the raphe nuclei (Conrad et al., 1974; Vertes et al., 1999).

Granule cells project single axons towards CA3, forming the mossy fibers (MF). These

form predominantly excitatory synapses with hilar and CA3 interneurons and pyramidal CA3

cells. Still, MFs are not the only excitatory source for CA3 pyramidal cells. Stellate cells from

layer II of the entorhinal cortex and CA3 collaterals reach CA3 pyramidal cell apical and basal

dendrites (Steward, 1976; Li et al., 1994), and are important sources of excitation (Urban et al.,

2001).

1.2.4.4 Mossy Fibers: form and function

Mossy fibers have three types of terminals: large mossy boutons, filopodial extensions of

mossy boutons, and small varicosities. Mossy boutons predominantly contact pyramidal CA3

neurons at the most proximal point of their apical dendrites, in a narrow band just above the

pyramidal cell layer called stratum lucidum. A single granule cell contacts approximately 15

pyramidal cells, and a pyramidal cell receives a maximum of 50 mossy inputs (Amaral et al.,

1990).

Large mossy boutons have a high density of vesicles (Blackstad and Kjaerheim, 1961) in

more than one active zone and encompass a complex multi-headed postsynaptic CA3 spine

forming a structure called thorny excrescence (Amaral and Dent, 1981; Chicurel and Harris,

1992). From the mossy boutons emerges a set of filopodia collaterals predominantly innervating

a variety of GABAergic interneurons in the hilus (mossy cells, pyramidal basket cells) and CA3.

Small varicosities protrude from the granule cell axon and also contact mostly hilar and CA3

interneurons (Amaral and Dent, 1981).

The MF bouton‘s close proximity to CA3 pyramidal cells, and the large pool of vesicles

readily available (Hallermann et al., 2003) raise the view that the MF synapse could act as a

Page 42: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

29

‗detonator‘, from which a single action potential from a granule cell could exert powerful

excitatory influence on a CA3 pyramidal cell. That and the sparcity of these inputs led to an idea

of a ‗teacher‘ synapse, instructing pyramidal cells to fire according to the pattern of granule cell

activity during learning (Treves and Rolls, 1992a, 1994). In this manner, the DG could have a

role in coding information from the EC into a sparse, pattern separated representation forwarded

to CA3 for storage (Acsády and Káli, 2007).

Not only could the CA3 store information received from the DG, it could also integrate

signals from the strong MF input with the weaker perforant path as a form of associative learning

(Mcnaughton and Morris, 1987; Lisman, 1999). Alternatively, Treves and Rolls proposed that

the MF, as a detonator, could force encoding into CA3 independent of other input sources, and

thereby be the driving force of acquisition; whereas the CA3, through its collaterals and

perforant path inputs would primarily be involved during retrieval (Treves and Rolls, 1992a).

Certain aspects of MF-CA3 physiology, however, argue against this detonator concept.

Small terminals and filopodia synapses outnumber mossy terminals by approximately 10 fold

(Bragin et al., 1995, Acsády et al., 1998). Activation of inhibitory interneurons by MF is very

reliable (Scharfman et al., 1990) and leads to a feedforward inhibition of CA3 pyramidal

neurons, so that MF release leads to monosynaptic activation and disynaptic inhibition of these

cells.

Additionally, glutamate is the primary excitatory neurotransmitter in granule cells

(Crawford and Connor, 1973; Terrian et al., 1988), but GABA is also produced and is present in

vesicles in the release zone, albeit at a lower concentration (Bergersen et al., 2003). These factors

make an argument against high reliability in DGC-pyramidal CA3 synapse relay. In fact, low

firing frequencies (<0.5Hz) rarely elicit a spike in CA3, preferentially activating interneurons

(Bragin et al., 1995a; Penttonen et al., 1997). Higher frequencies, however, breach the spike

threshold and depolarize pyramidal CA3 cells quite reliably (Henze et al., 2002).

This experimental evidence led Urban et al. to propose that instead of detonators, MF

synapses might function as ‗discriminators‘, such that at high frequencies the high reliability of

the MF-CA3 relay favours the decorrelation of similar inputs, or pattern separation, but at low

frequencies the silencing of DG inputs by interneuron driven inhibition would facilitate CA3‘s

role in collapsing similar inputs into one output, or pattern completion (Urban et al., 2001).

Page 43: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

30

Experimental work has confirmed the importance of the trisynaptic pathway to memory

consolidation and its involvement in so called pattern completion (Nakashiba et al., 2008, 2009),

as well as implicating the DG in what is referred to as pattern separation (Gilbert et al., 2001;

Leutgeb et al., 2007), and has led to an an additional area of research, the potential contribution

of granule cells born during adulthood to this circuit (Clelland et al., 2009; Sahay et al., 2011a).

The integration of adult born granule cells into the hippocampal circuit and their contribution to

memory function is the theme of the next section.

1.3 Adult Neurogenesis

In spite of reports of adult neurogenesis from as early as the 1960‘s (Altman, 1962), it

was only recently fully accepted that new neurons are generated throughout adulthood in specific

regions of the brain. These regions include the subgranular zone (SGZ) of the dentate gyrus of

the hippocampus and the subventricular zone (SVZ) of the lateral ventricles (Zhao et al., 2008;

Deng et al., 2010). Under non-pathological conditions, neurogenesis in other areas of the brain is

not consistently found, remaining controversial (Gould, 2007). Although we know that neurons

born during adulthood mature (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002; Espósito et al., 2005; Zhao et

al., 2006), form functional synapses (Toni et al., 2007) and are incorporated into memory

networks (Kee et al., 2007; Stone et al., 2011b), the physiological significance of this

incorporation remains unclear.

This section first describes the history of adult neurogenesis, from discovery to

acceptance by the scientific community, followed by two main parts: hippocampal neurogenesis

and SVZ neurogenesis, reviewing factors involved in the regulation of neurogenesis, maturation

of adult born cells and their incorporation into existing circuitry, and possible functions.

1.3.1 History

Page 44: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

31

The existence of adult generated neurons was controversial for almost a century. In the

beginning of the 20th

century the predominant view was that neurons divided only during

development, and stopped well before puberty (Ramón y Cajal, 1928, 1995; Gross, 2000).

Although reports of mitotic cells in the adult central nervous system of mammals did appear

(Hamilton, 1901; Allen, 1912; Sugita, 1918), it was technically impossible to confirm that

neurons, and not glia, were being born, and the idea was soon discarded.

In the 1950‘s, tritiated thymidine autoradiography became available, and proliferating

cells could finally be birth-marked and tracked. In the 1960‘s, pioneering work by Joseph

Altman showed for the first time 3H-thymidine labelled cells in the brain of adult mammals

(Altman, 1962), particularly in the hippocampus (Altman and Das, 1965) and olfactory bulb

(Altman, 1969). This discovery was disregarded for almost 2 decades, partly due to technical

concerns over the identification of those cells as neurons, and some believe also due to Altman

still being a young scientist going against a strong current (Gross, 2000).

In the 1970‘s, Michael Kaplan and colleagues provided the missing piece of the puzzle.

Through combination of 3H-thymidine injections and electron microscopy they were able to

identify the labeled cells as neurons in the olfactory bulb and dentate gyrus of adult rats and mice

(Kaplan and Hinds, 1977; Kaplan and Bell, 1984). In the 1980‘s, however, Pasko Rakic‘s report

that new neurons were absent in the adult rhesus monkey brain (Rakic, 1985) deeply influenced

the field, which remained dormant for a few years.

A few developments helped change that perspective. A series of elegant papers by

Nottebohm and colleagues using the canary vocal system extended the so far merely

morphological evidence for adult neurogenesis (Goldman and Nottebohm, 1983) into detailed

accounts of migration (Alvarez-Buylla and Nottebohm, 1988), axon targeting (Alvarez-Buylla et

al., 1988) and electrophysiological recordings (Paton and Nottebohm, 1984).

These studies gave way to a rediscovery of adult neurogenesis in the 1990‘s, further

prompted by technical developments such as markers for proliferation, cell type and maturation

stage (Mullen et al., 1992; Kuhn et al., 1996), and suggestions of a physiological function for

neurogenesis from evidence of regulation by stress (Gould et al., 1992; Gould and Tanapat,

1999), learning (Gould et al., 1999) and enrichment (Kempermann et al., 1997a). Finally, the

Page 45: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

32

report of adult DG neurogenesis in humans (Eriksson et al., 1998) consolidated its existence and

increased the interest in studying adult neurogenesis in animal models.

1.3.2 Hippocampal neurogenesis

The generation of new neurons in the adult DG differs from embryonic neurogenesis in a

few critical ways. From P10-20 the progenitor cell pool is much diminished, is restricted to the

SGZ (Altman and Bayer, 1990b in rats; Angevine 1965 in mice; see section 1.2.4.2), and

generates one neuronal cell type: excitatory granule cells (Markakis and Gage, 1999; van Praag

et al., 2002; Lagace et al., 2007; Imayoshi et al., 2008). Given this qualitative and quantitative

difference, and the fact that adult neurogenesis persists throughout the life of the animal (Kuhn et

al., 1996 in rats; Kempermann et al., 1998 in mice), it is difficult to argue that neurogenesis

during adulthood is merely protracted development.

The issue of whether adult-generated dentate granule cells (aDGCs) replace old cells or

are added to a growing DG is still controversial. While some authors claim that the turnover rate

in the DGCL is low (Crespo et al., 1986), and neurons are continuously added to a growing

DGCL (Imayoshi et al., 2008), other authors fail to see an increase after 30d (Lagace et al., 2007)

or 4 months of age (Ninkovic et al., 2007). It is still technically challenging, even with

stereological techniques, to detect small differences in cell population numbers in the DGCL,

especially due to the high baseline (of around half a million cells) and age-related decline in

neurogenesis levels (Kuhn et al., 1996). Technical advances should help clarify this issue in the

near future.

1.3.2.1 Becoming an aDGC: from birth to senescence

1.3.2.1.1 Life and death in the adult DG

Page 46: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

33

Cameron and McKay estimated that, in the rat brain, the duration of the cell cycle is of

approximately 25 hours, and 9000 new cells are generated each day (Cameron and McKay,

2001). This estimation was done using a mixture of two types of cell cycle markers, 5-bromo-2-

deoxyuridine (BrdU) and tritiated thymidine (Cameron and McKay, 2001), which are

incorporated into the DNA during the S phase of the cell cycle (Cavanagh et al., 2011). In the

C57/Bl6 mouse brain, the cell cycle is 12-14h long and 1600 cells are generated per day (Hayes

and Nowakowski, 2002). Furthermore, there are large differences in neurogenesis rates within

species due to genetic influences, with proliferation being at least 1.5 times higher in C57/Bl6

mice compared to other mouse strains (Kempermann et al., 1997b).

Long-term survival rates for adult born neurons are low, approximately 50% of new

neurons survive in the rat (Brandt et al., 2003; Dayer et al., 2003; Snyder et al., 2009a) and 30%

in the mouse (Kempermann et al., 1997b; Snyder et al., 2009), respectively. In C57/Bl6 mice,

BrdU positive cell numbers drop drastically between 1-3 weeks after labeling, but after 4 weeks

remain stable throughout the life of the animals (Kempermann et al., 1997b; Kempermann,

2003). This curve is similar in rats (Biebl et al., 2000; Snyder et al., 2009a). Since neuronal

markers are detected as early as 1-3 days after BrdU labeling (Brandt et al., 2003), this curve

suggests most of the long-term survival outcome occurs during the early post-mitotic period,

which coincides with synapse formation and integration into hippocampal circuitry (see next

sections).

1.3.2.1.2 Looking at morphology and electrophysiology

Adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus starts through the proliferation of neural

progenitor cells (NPCs) in the SGZ of the DG. As with embryonic development (Noctor et al.,

2002) and the SVZ (Doetsch et al., 1999), the SGZ progenitors present typical radial glia-like

morphology, with long processes reaching through the DGCL to the ML, and astrocytic qualities,

such as electrophysiological properties and expression of astrocytic markers (Filippov et al.,

2003; Fukuda et al., 2003).

Page 47: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

34

The vast majority of the progeny of NPCs in the SGZ differentiates into dentate granule

cells (DGCs), whereas a minority becomes glia (Cameron et al., 1993). During the first week

after birth, young neuroblasts undergo a short radial migration into the inner-middle third of the

DGCL (Kempermann, 2003; Espósito et al., 2005; Zhao et al., 2006) and start projecting

processes. Although they lack synaptic connections at this stage, GABA and glutamate receptors

are already present (Espósito et al., 2005).

As is typical of neurons in the immature brain (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Ben-Ari 2002 for a

review), GABA has a depolarizing action on immature aDGCs (Ge et al., 2006). The polarity of

GABA action is mostly driven by the expression of specific Cl- transporters, and the

developmental switch between GABA-driven excitation and inhibition appears to happen

through the sequential expression of the Na+–K

+–2Cl

– co-transporter NKCC1, a chloride

importer, and the K+-coupled Cl

- transporter KCC2, a chloride exporter (Delpire, 2000; Ben-Ari,

2002; Owens and Kriegstein, 2002). Indeed, knockdown of NKCC1 in aDGCs through shRNA-

expressing retrovirus led to GABA-induced hyperpolarization of 7 day old cells (Ge et al., 2006).

Interestingly, NKCC1-knockdown cells displayed defects in GABA- and glutamate-

mediated synapses and reduced dendritic arbourisation, whereas a GABA agonist induced

dendritic growth (Ge et al., 2006). Overall, this period of GABA activation seems critical to the

development of adult-generated granule cells (see also Tozuka et al., 2005), likely through a

CREB-mediated pathway (Ge et al., 2006; Jagasia et al., 2009).

During the second week neuroblasts start to present typical mature neuronal morphology,

with concomitant axonal and dendritic outgrowth. Dendrites reach the molecular layer at around

day 10, and their arbourization becomes complex (Zhao et al., 2006). Axons reach CA3 at

around day 7, before spinogenesis begins (Faulkner et al., 2008). Afferent synaptogenesis begins

around day 8, and, mirroring development, these early inputs are exclusively GABAergic, with

glutamatergic postsynaptic currents only being detected at day 16 (Espósito et al., 2005;

Overstreet Wadiche et al., 2005; Markwardt et al., 2009). Cholinergic innervation from septal

neurons has been reported, but no functional synapse has yet been demonstrated (Ide et al.,

2008).

The third week marks the onset of spinogenesis, on day 16 (Espósito et al., 2005; Ge et

al., 2006; Zhao et al., 2006; Toni et al., 2007). On day 17, synapses between aDGCs and

Page 48: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

35

pyramidal CA3 cells are already present (Toni et al, 2008). Synaptic integration coincides with

the excitatory/inhibitory GABA switch and the onset of glutamatergic inputs, on day 18

(Espósito et al., 2005; Ge et al., 2006). Both filopodia/spines and axonal boutons tend to form

near pre-existing connections, suggesting a role for ongoing circuit activity in this integration

(Faulkner et al., 2008; Toni et al., 2008). Consistent with this, Tashiro and colleagues reported an

NMDAR-dependent critical window for survival at around 3 weeks, suggesting NMDA-receptor

mediated cell autonomous activity regulates cell death/survival during the third week after birth

(Tashiro et al., 2006).

By the fourth week the glutamatergic excitatory drive from the perforant path is fully

formed, with mature synapses reported as early as day 30 (Toni et al., 2007). Spine density is

still not equivalent to a mature cell, increasing until day 56, when it reaches a plateau (Zhao et

al., 2006). Hence, the glutamatergic input is still weak, but the high input resistance (that leads to

increased excitability) allows these immature neurons to spike in response to perforant path

axons (Espósito et al., 2005; Mongiat et al., 2009).

Around 4 weeks in the mouse (Ge et al., 2007b) and 2-3weeks in the rat (Schmidt-Hieber

et al., 2004), aDGCs are more excitable, displaying a lower threshold for LTP and higher LTP

amplitude, a process mainly driven by a differential expression of NR2B-containing NMDARs

(Ge et al., 2007b). This enhanced synaptic plasticity is transient, and restricted to the 4-6 weeks

window (Ge et al., 2007b). During this window, a weak stimulation paradigm is able to induce

LTP that is insensitive to GABAergic inhibition, which some researchers refer to as artificial

cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF)-LTP (Snyder et al., 2001; Saxe et al., 2006; Ge et al., 2008).

At about 6-8 weeks of age these cells are structurally and functionally mature in the

mouse (van Praag et al., 2002; Laplagne et al., 2006, 2007; Zhao et al., 2006; Faulkner et al.,

2008), even though morphological changes (spines, axonal boutons) continue to occur (Zhao et

al., 2006; Toni et al., 2007, 2008; Faulkner et al., 2008). This process appears to be faster in the

rat (Snyder et al., 2009a).

1.3.2.1.3 Looking at stages and markers

Page 49: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

36

Puzzled by the parallel efflux of papers on regulation of neurogenesis (see next

subsection) and of morphological and electrophysiological description of aDGC maturation, but

lack of intersection between the two, Kempermann and colleagues set out to partition the aDGC

maturation process into a series of sequential steps - classified by morphological differences and

expression of selective markers - in the hopes that this would enable a deeper understanding of

neurogenesis regulation on a cellular level (Kempermann et al., 2004). Seri and colleagues had

proposed a different nomenclature and division of maturation stages for aDGCs (Seri et al.,

2001, 2004), but Kempermann‘s became predominant in the literature.

Their model consists of six stages (or milestones) (Kempermann et al., 2004). In the first

stage, a stem-like cell (type 1 cell) divides, leading to three consecutive stages (2-4) of divisions

of progenitor cells (or transit amplifying cells) that increasingly lose the potential to proliferate

and differentiate. These cells are referred to as type 2a, type 2b and type 3. Stage 5 consists of a

recent post-mitotic phase during which these immature neurons start making connections and

survival is determined. Stage 6 is that of a terminally diferentiated mature neuron (Kempermann

et al., 2004). Fig. 6 shows a scheme.

Type 1 cells are the putative stem cells of the SGZ (Seri et al., 2001). A stem cell is

defined by its capacity to self renew and to differentiate into specialized cell types (Weissman,

2000). Whereas the presence of stem cells in the SVZ is well accepted (Doetsch et al., 1999;

Seaberg and van der Kooy, 2003), the ‗stemness‘ of type 1 cells has been challenged for many

years, especially due to a lack of in vivo evidence for multilineage (Seaberg and van der Kooy,

2002, 2003; and see Morshead, 2004). Very recently, Bonaguidi and colleagues showed for the

first time in vivo evidence of self-renewal and generation of glial and neuronal cells from type 1

progenitors (Bonaguidi et al., 2011).

Page 50: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

37

Figure 6. Scheme of the maturational stages of aDGCs. Each stage of maturation and the gradients of expression of

key markers is represented. Approximate peak expression as measured by days after BrdU administration are

signaled for some of the markers (see text for more extensive discussion). DCX=doublecortin.

SGZ progenitors or type 1 cells (Filippov et al., 2003) display astrocytic morphology (see

previous section) and expression of markers such as glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP)

(Filippov et al., 2003; Fukuda et al., 2003) but not S100 (Steiner et al., 2004). Another common

marker used to identify type 1 cells is the transcription factor Sox-2 (Komitova and Eriksson,

2004), from the sex determining region of Y-chromosome, SRY, related HMG- box (Sox) gene

family. Sox-2 is one of the earliest transcription factors to be expressed in the developing neural

tube (Cai et al., 2002).

Type 1 cells also express nestin, an intermediate filament named after ‗neuroepithelium

stem cells‘ due to its expression at earlier stages of neural differentiation (Lendahl et al., 1990).

Although type 1 cells comprise approximately one third of the nestin+ population, they are

responsible for only 5% of its cell divisions, meaning these cells are slow to divide and likely do

so assymetrically, also giving rise to a nestin-expressing type 2 cell (Filippov et al., 2003;

Kronenberg et al., 2003). In fact, nestin ceases to be expressed at the type 3 stage (Kronenberg et

al., 2003).

Type 2a and 2b cells do not express GFAP and have a very different morphology from

type 1 cells, with an ovoid or round nucleus, soma with scant cytoplasm, and short plump

processes that run tangentially to the SGZ, where they reside (Filippov et al., 2003). They are

Page 51: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

38

also highly proliferative, particularly the type 2a cells (Kronenberg et al., 2003). Two main

factors distinguish type 2a from 2b: electrophysiological properties, mainly distinguished by the

presence of Na+ currents in the more differentiated type 2b cells (Filippov et al., 2003), and the

expression of the immature neuron marker doublecortin (DCX), which is absent in 2a

(Kronenberg et al., 2003).

DCX was first identified as being involved in neuronal migration in the cortex, since it is

mutated in lissencephaly syndrome/band heterotopia (a.k.a. double cortex) cases (Gleeson et al.,

1998; des Portes et al., 1998). It is expressed throughout development in migrating neurons of

the central and peripheral nervous system (Gleeson et al., 1999) and consists of a microtubule-

associated cytoplasmic protein necessary for migration (Tanaka et al., 2004). In the adult DG,

DCX is first expressed by type 2b cells and its expression continues until the immature neuron

stage. In a time course analysis with BrdU, DCX is expressed very early, consistent with its

presence in proliferating cells, which is further confirmed by its double-labeling with Ki67

(Brown et al., 2003). DCX expression in the rat peaks at 3-7 days and rapidly decreases

thereafter, being minimal at 4 weeks and undetectable at 8 (Brown et al., 2003). In the mouse,

DCX expression remains high until approximately day 21 (Snyder et al., 2009).

Another marker that mostly overlaps with DCX is the polysialated form of neural cell

adhesion molecule (PSA-NCAM). PSA-NCAM is a cell surface glycoprotein involved in

processes such as cell adhesion and neural morphogenesis (Seki and Arai, 1993a), and was one

of the first markers of immature neurons to be used in the adult DG (Seki and Arai, 1993b; Seki,

2002). Type 3 cells still express DCX and PSA-NCAM (but not nestin); are proliferative and

have a round nucleus (Kempermann et al., 2004).

After the last cell division these immature neurons undergo a phase marked by the

transient expression of calretinin (CR) around 3 days after the last division (Brandt et al., 2003).

CR is a calcium binding protein expressed in the hippocampus and commonly known as a

marker for specific interneuron populations (Rogers, 1987; Baimbridge et al., 1992; Gulyás et

al., 1992; Freund and Buzsáki, 1996). CR positive cells do not colabel with Ki67, thus CR is a

marker for the postmitotic stage (Brandt et al., 2003). Calretinin expression is detected one day

after BrdU, increases during the first week with a peak at 7 days, and decreases thereafter, being

undetectable at 6 weeks (Brandt et al., 2003).

Page 52: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

39

Early post-mitotic cells exhibit vertical morphology (perpendicular to the SGZ in the

coronal plane) with an almost triangular nucleus and a pronounced apical dendrite (Kempermann

et al., 2004). The onset of CR expression nearly coincides with that of another marker, NeuN.

NeuN is a neuron-specific nuclear protein (Neuronal Nuclei) that was discovered through an

immunological screening with immunizations of brain cell nuclei (Mullen et al., 1992). It is

expressed by most neurons in the central nervous system and has recently been identified as the

RNA splicing regulator Fox-3 (Kim et al., 2009).

NeuN is detected as early as 1 day after BrdU injection by some groups (Brandt et al.,

2003; Kempermann et al., 2004), or at 10 days by others (Brown et al., 2003). NeuN/DCX

populations show the largest overlap between days 10-14, which subsides thereafter (Brown et

al., 2003). According to Brown and colleagues, the proportion of NeuN positive cells rapidly

increases from day 14-30, and then slows down (Brown et al., 2003; but see Snyder et al., 2009

for a discussion on making sense of the Brandt and Brown curves), but expression persists in

mature DGCs (Brandt et al., 2003; Brown et al., 2003), showing that NeuN is a marker of

immature and mature neurons.

The last stage is marked by a switch between CR expression and that of another calcium-

binding protein, calbindin (Brandt et al., 2003). Calbindin was originally isolated as an intestinal

calcium transport protein (Baimbridge and Miller, 1982), and was later found to be expressed in

the adult brain in all mature dentate granule cells (Sloviter, 1989). Also, through its calcium

binding abilities, it has been shown to play a facilitating role in MF synapses (Blatow et al.,

2003).

The CR-calbindin switch happens around 2-3 weeks after exiting the last cell division

(Kempermann et al., 2004), with calbindin expression being low on the first week after BrdU

labeling, but increasing over time and peaking at 3 weeks (Snyder et al., 2009a). Therefore, cells

still undergo maturation while expressing calbindin before they become indistinguishable from

their developmentally generated counterparts at 6-8 weeks of age (van Praag et al., 2002;

Laplagne et al., 2006, 2007; Zhao et al., 2006; Faulkner et al., 2008).

Page 53: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

40

1.3.2.2 Regulating adult hippocampal neurogenesis

Adult neurogenesis has been shown to be bi-directionally regulated by a multitude of

factors. The number and heterogeneity of these factors have led researchers to wonder what does

not regulate adult neurogenesis (Kempermann, 2011). The next two sections review some of

these factors, from recent studies of punctual gene deletion to the most prominent extrinsic

manipulations shown to regulate neurogenesis.

To facilitate this description, neurogenesis regulators have been divided into two

categories, intrinsic or extrinsic, based on the nature of the manipulation. Therefore, studies that

have identified regulators of neurogenesis through gene deletion have been grouped in the

intrinsic section (1.3.2.2.1), and external manipulations (such as infusion of growth factors,

running, or stress hormones) are described in the extrinsic section (1.3.2.2.2). Importantly, it is

often technically challenging to determine whether certain factors exhert their action on aDGCs

in an exclusively intrinsic (cell autonomous) or extrinsic (non-cell autonomous) manner, and the

following separation is only meant to facilitate the review of this literature.

1.3.2.2.1 Intrinsic regulation

The use of transgenic and viral strategies to knock down specific genes has helped

surface several adult neurogenesis regulators. Starting this review from the nucleus onwards,

many transcription factors have been implicated in the regulation of adult neurogenesis. The

transcription co-factor PC3/Tis21 regulates proliferation and terminal differentiation of aDGCs

(Farioli-Vecchioli et al., 2008, 2009). Knockdown of transcription factor NF-B with an

CamKII promoter (around type 2b-3 stage) suggested a role in synaptogenesis, axogenesis and

survival (Imielski et al., 2012). During the critical period for survival of aDGCs, Krüppel like

factor 9, an activity-dependent transcription factor, seems critical for maturation and synaptic

plasticity, since its knockdown impairs ACSF-LTP (Scobie et al., 2009).

Page 54: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

41

The HMG-box transcription factor Sox2 is a progenitor marker, present in type 1 and 2

cells, that precedes the expression of the proneural basic helix- loop-helix (bHLH) transcription

factor NeuroD1, transiently expressed at the type 3-immature stage (Kuwabara et al., 2009).

Work led by the groups of Jenny Hsieh and Fred Gage revealed a pathway in which Wnt/-

catenin signaling alleviates Sox2 repression to turn on NeuroD1 expression (Kuwabara et al.,

2009), which is necessary for aDGC survival and maturation (Gao et al., 2009). Another bHLH

transcription factor, Ascl1, is expressed in transient amplifying progenitors (Kim et al., 2011),

and has been implicated in neuronal commitment, since retrovirus mediated overexpression of

Ascl1 led to a switch in progenitor cells to the oligodentrocytic lineage (Jessberger et al., 2008b).

Sox2 has also been directly implicated in stem cell maintenance and differentiation (Ferri

et al., 2004; Cavallaro et al., 2008). Moreover, SoxC transcription factors Sox4 and Sox11 are

expressed at a time of neuronal commitment - starting at late transient amplifying stage (2b cells)

and stopping before calbindin expression - and seem to be necessary for aDGC differentiation

(Mu et al., 2012). Retrovirus mediated knockdown of the transcription factor cAMP response

element-binding protein (CREB) led to deficits in survival, maturation and morphological

development (Jagasia et al., 2009), with the reduction in dendritic length and arbourisation likely

being driven by CREB-mediated expression of microRNA mir132 (Magill et al., 2010).

Classic cell signalling molecules are also regulators of adult neurogenesis. Erk5 is

expressed in adult neurogenesis regions and regulates aDGC proliferation (Pan et al., 2012). The

transmembrane receptor Notch 1 is expressed in type 1 cells and maturing DCX positive cells

(Breunig et al., 2007). Ables and colleagues showed that Notch1 knockdown starting at the

nestin stage led to a decrease in proliferation, number of type 1-3 cells and dendritic complexity

of immature neurons (Ables et al., 2010). A similar study knocking down Notch1 in GFAP

positive cells in the postnatal brain (P10, P12 and P14) found comparable results (Breunig et al.,

2007).

Additionally, knockdown of Rbpj, an intracellular mediator of all Notch receptors, in the

adult brain led to premature differentiation of stem cells into transient amplifying cells, which

caused a transient increase in proliferation but later cessation of SVZ neurogenesis due to

depletion of the stem cell pool (Imayoshi et al., 2010). Together these studies open a door for a

deeper study of the role of Notch1-mediated lateral inhibition in the adult brain (Kageyama et al.,

Page 55: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

42

2008; Ables et al., 2011). Curiously, a similar premature differentiation and depletion of

progenitor cells was induced by the overexpression of PC3, a gene associated with terminal

differentiation (Farioli-Vecchioli et al., 2008).

The same phenomenon was found with yet another cell signaling molecule, Pten, a tumor

suppressor protein that negatively regulates of the phosphatidylinositol-3-kinase (PI3K)

signalling cascade (Maehama and Dixon, 1998). Pten knockdown in adult progenitor cells leads

to increased proliferation and differentiation of aDGCs, eventually depleting the progenitor pool

(Amiri et al., 2012). Another tumor suppressor protein, Neurofibromin, a negative regulator of

Ras, modulates aDGC proliferation likely through an extracellular signal-regulated kinase

(ERK)-mediated pathway, but had no effect on differentiation (Li et al., 2012). Additionally,

both the orphan nuclear receptor TLX (Zhang et al., 2008) and adhesion molecules such as

NCAM (Amoureux et al., 2000) have been implicated in the control of aDGC proliferation.

Cycline dependent kinase 5 (Cdk5) is expressed in DCX+ and NeuN+ neurons (Lagace et

al., 2008) and plays an important role in neuronal migration, maturation and survival during

development (Jessberger et al., 2010). Two groups have independently shown that Cdk5 is also

important for aDGC maturation and survival (Jessberger et al., 2010). Using a transgenic

strategy, Lagace and colleagues knocked down Cdk5 in progenitor cells and showed that its

expression in immature neurons is necessary for survival (Lagace et al., 2008). Through

retrovirus mediated knockdown of Cdk5, Jessberger and colleagues showed a deficit in

migration and morphological maturation, with aberrant dendrite extension and formation of

ectopic synapses (Jessberger et al., 2008a).

Fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP), a protein associated with a common

inherited form of mental retardation, was also shown to modulate adult neurogenesis, since

knockdown of fmrp in nestin positive progenitor cells led to increased proliferation, decreased

neuronal differentiation and dendritic complexity, as well as increased astrocytic differentiation

(Guo et al., 2011), suggesting a possible role of adult neurogenesis in the cognitive deficits seen

in fragile X syndrome patients.

Finally, on the interface between extrinsic and intrinsic, between environment and genes,

lie a complex array of epigenetic factors whose function in the regulation of adult neurogenesis

is beginning to be elucidated (Ma et al., 2010; Sun et al., 2011).

Page 56: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

43

1.3.2.2.2 Extrinsic regulation

Environmental enrichment (EE) consists of a more complex and socially rich

environment compared to standard animals housing conditions, usually involving a bigger cage

with toys, tunnels, nesting materials and often running wheels (van Praag et al., 2000). EE leads

to increased survival (Kempermann et al., 1997a; Nilsson et al., 1999; van Praag et al., 1999b) of

nestin negative cells (type 3 onwards) (Kronenberg et al., 2003) or cells of about 2 weeks of age

(Tashiro et al., 2007), probably during the critical period for long-term survival of aDGCs.

Voluntary running has been shown to increase proliferation in young (van Praag et al.,

1999a, 1999b; Kronenberg et al., 2003) and old (van Praag et al., 2005) animals. This effect

seems to take place at the level of type 2b cells (Kronenberg et al., 2003), and leads to an

increase in the proportion of NeuN positive cells (van Praag et al., 1999b) and in the number of

arc positive cells in response to seizures, possibly indicative of functional maturation (Snyder et

al., 2009b). With at least 14 days of running, survival of new neurons is also increased (Muotri et

al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2009b).

Network activity also alters rates of adult neurogenesis. Entorhinal cortex stimulation

through deep brain stimulation leads to an increase in cell proliferation in the DG (Stone et al.,

2011a). LTP induction through perforant path tetanus enhances survival of cells at least 1-2

weeks old (Bruel-Jungerman et al., 2006) and proliferation (Bruel-Jungerman et al., 2006; Chun

et al., 2006) in a NMDA-dependent manner (Chun et al., 2006). As with glutamate (through

NMDARs) and GABA (see section 1.3.2.1.2), other neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine

(Kaneko et al., 2006), serotonin (Brezun and Daszuta, 1999) and dopamine (Höglinger et al.,

2004) have been proposed to alter the proliferation, survival or differentiation of aDGCs (Jang et

al., 2007; Balu and Lucki, 2009). Additionally, epileptic seizures increase proliferation and

interfere with the maturation of type 3 cells leading to abnormal migration and functional

integration (Parent et al., 1997; Jessberger et al., 2005, 2007; Overstreet-Wadiche et al., 2006).

Growth factors such as fibroblast growth factor 2 (FGF-2) (Rai et al., 2007; Zhao et al.,

2007) and vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) (Jin et al., 2002), and neurotrophic factors

Page 57: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

44

such as brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Scharfman et al., 2005; Balu and Lucki,

2009) interfere with proliferation and survival and are likely important endogenous regulators of

adult neurogenesis.

The relationship between stress and the hippocampus has been extensively studied, from

glucocorticoids affecting hippocampal structure and function (McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995;

McEwen, 2006; Joëls et al., 2007) to hippocampal regulation of the HPA axis (Jankord and

Herman, 2008; and see section 1.2.2). Stress is a powerful suppressor of neurogenesis (Gould

and Tanapat, 1999). Physical (Malberg and Duman, 2003; Pham et al., 2003; Vollmayr et al.,

2003), psychosocial (Gould et al., 1998; Czéh et al., 2002), acute (Gould et al., 1997; Heine et

al., 2004) or chronic stress (Czéh et al., 2002; Heine et al., 2004) decrease proliferation and

survival.

Interestingly, depression-like behaviours are associated with dysregulation of

glucocorticoids (Holsboer and Ising, 2010), and most antidepressants stimulate neurogenesis

(Malberg et al., 2000; Sahay and Hen, 2007). Linking neurogenesis and depression, Dranovsky

and colleagues recently showed that the actual ratio of stem cells to neurons can be modulated by

experience, with social isolation causing an accumulation of type 1 cells to the detriment of

neurogenesis (Dranovsky et al., 2011). In a recent report, Snyder and colleagues directly

implicated aDGCs in HPA axis regulation, showing they function as a buffer to temper stress

responses, which possibly provides a physiological link to depression (Snyder et al., 2011).

The first indication that learning could regulate neurogenesis came in 1999 with work by

Elizabeth Gould and colleagues. They showed that two hippocampus-dependent tasks (trace

eyeblink conditioning and MWM), but not their hippocampal independent counterparts (delay

eyeblink conditioning and visible MWM), increased the survival of aDGCs (Gould et al., 1999).

The survival effect seems to be specific to cells 6-10 days old (Ambrogini et al., 2000; Hairston

et al., 2005; Epp et al., 2007; Sisti et al., 2007), a time in which new neurons are extending axons

and starting to form connections.

Another report claimed different phases of MWM training had different effects on

neurogenesis. The initial, more challenging, phase of MWM training increased survival of

aDGCs, whereas a later phase of training increased cell death of 5d old neurons, which

correlated with performance and increased proliferation (Döbrössy et al., 2003). The same group

Page 58: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

45

later showed that this increase in cell death of 5d old neurons seems to be necessary for learning,

and that the increase in proliferation and the pro-survival effect on 1 week old cells are

homeostatic responses to this cell death (Dupret et al., 2007).

1.3.2.3 Functional significance of adult hippocampal neurogenesis to learning and memory

The fact that the hippocampus is one of two brain regions with adult neurogenesis

prompted the question of whether these aDGCs could be involved in memory formation. The

function of aDGCs in learning and memory has been investigated within two basic frameworks:

in silico computational modeling studies and animal gain or loss of function studies. Due to the

nature of our experiments and to facilitate the description of the literature, the animal studies will

be further divided into two parts: one involving the behavioural impacts of extrinsic or intrinsic

manipulations through which neurogenesis levels are increased or decreased (referred to as

correlative studies), and ablation studies.

1.3.2.3.1 Function of aDGCs: computational networks

Several computational models of DG function have been critical for testing predictions

and putting forward new potential functions for aDGCs. Even though there is a considerable

degree of variability in the models, some general concepts emerge from the in silico adult

neurogenesis work. The potentially privileged position of the DG to pattern separate, largely

driven by its characteristic sparsification and orthogonalization of signals to CA3, has been

predicted by models (Treves and Rolls, 1992b) and found experimental support (Lee and Kesner,

2004; Leutgeb et al., 2007; McHugh et al., 2007) (see section 1.2.4), but how would

continuously adding neurons to this network contribute to its function?

Page 59: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

46

Aimone and colleagues propose that whereas mature granule cells contribute to pattern

separation, immature aDGCs are ‗pattern integrators‘ (Aimone et al., 2009). Their rationale is

that the hyperexcitability that aDGCs experience during their maturation could lead to

indiscriminate firing and incorporation into an already existing CA3 representation. Thus, while

mature DGCs orthogonalize signals from the EC and pattern separate, immature aDGCs are

forming associations between events, or pattern integrating (Aimone et al., 2009). Consistent

with this view, a recent report has shown, in hippocampal slices, that 4 week old cells are almost

twice more likely to spike in response to two independent perforant path inputs (pattern

integrate) when compared to mature DGCs (Marín-Burgin et al., 2012).

However, this hyperexcitable period is time restricted, and the surviving neurons will

indelibly mature and contribute to sparse encoding. Therefore, within each wave of neurogenesis

exists a unique window of time when less strict associations are formed. It has been speculated

that this window could promote associations between events that happened around the same

period of time (Aimone et al., 2006). Although alluring, this prediction has not yet been

confirmed experimentally.

Becker and Wojtowicz also take this hyperplasticity period of aDGCs into consideration

to propose what they call a ‗functional cluster‘ (Becker and Wojtowicz, 2007). Since adult

neurogenesis occurs in waves and clusters of proliferating cells are commonly seen, they

stipulate that these clusters, maturing at the same rate, could encode different features of an event

or context through their differential perforant path afferent inputs, and, due to their shared

plasticity tempo, remain tuned to promptly respond to re-exposure to the same context (Becker

and Wojtowicz, 2007). Aimone and colleagues find that the neurons that respond the most to a

given environment are the ones that had been exposed to that environment when around 3 weeks

of age (Aimone et al., 2009), consistent with Becker and Wojtowicz‘s idea of a maturation-stage

dependent collective representation of a context.

Another prediction from computational models comes to tackle a riddle in hippocampal

function. It is speculated that within a restricted set of neurons, continuous new learning could

eventually exhaust the number of possible encoding patterns, thus leading to new information

overwriting an already existing pattern, a phenomenon known as catastrophic interference

(McClelland et al., 1995). In that sense, the continuous addition of new neurons throughout life

Page 60: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

47

could increase memory storage capacity and prevent such catastrophic interference (Wiskott et

al., 2006).

On the other hand, this constant addition of new neurons and excess of excitatory drive

could in itself lead to indiscriminate activation of downstream neurons and degradation of pre-

existing memory traces. A model created by Meltzer and colleagues shows that addition of new

neurons in the absence of homeostasic mechanisms (i.e, turnover of cells or scaling back of

overall network activity) can lead to vast memory impairment (Meltzer et al., 2005).

1.3.2.3.2 Function of aDGCs: correlative studies

Several of the intrinsic and extrinsic regulators of neurogenesis reviewed in sections

1.3.2.2.1 and 1.3.2.2.2 also influence learning and memory. Some of the first observations

strengthening the idea that new neurons could be important for memory came from observations

that manipulations that increase neurogenesis (e.g. EE, running) also facilitate learning, whereas

manipulations that decrease neurogenesis (e.g. stress, aging, depressive states) also impair

learning. For instance, EE causes a mild facilitation of MWM acquisition (Kempermann et al.,

1997a; Nilsson et al., 1999) and improves performance in an object recognition task (Bruel-

Jungerman et al., 2005), and running increases DG LTP and facilitates MWM acquisition (van

Praag et al., 1999a, 2005).

However, manipulations such as running or EE have physiological consequences beyond

neurogenesis (e.g. effects on anxiety, motivation, and mood), which has made it difficult to

prove the causal link between enhanced neurogenesis and improved cognition. Running or EE in

the absence of neurogenesis, through pharmacological agents or X-ray irradiation, has yielded

mixed results, eliminating the EE-driven improvement in recognition memory (Bruel-Jungerman

et al., 2005), but not interfering with the spatial memory improvement (Meshi et al., 2006).

Using the inverse approach, Sahay and colleagues devised a transgenic strategy to

increase the survival of aDGCs in a cell autonomous manner (Sahay et al., 2011a). Augmenting

Page 61: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

48

neurogenesis enhanced ACSF-LTP and facilitated acquisition of a contextual discrimination

task, but had no effect on MWM learning (Sahay et al., 2011a).

Additionally, although there are reports of correlations between neurogenesis levels and

cognitive performance during aging (Drapeau et al., 2003; Driscoll et al., 2006) or stress (Shors,

2004; Montaron et al., 2006), they are not always consistent (Bizon et al., 2004) and, as with

running and EE studies, could be a symptom of a more complex phenomenon (Shors, 2004;

Klempin and Kempermann, 2007).

More recently, genetic deletion of some intrinsic regulators of neurogenesis was also

shown to impact behaviour. Decrease of neurogenesis caused by deletion of the orphan receptor

TLX (Zhang et al., 2008) or the fragile X syndrome protein FMRP (Guo et al., 2011) led to a

mild impairment in MWM, and to deficits in contextual and tone fear conditioning, respectively.

Attenuation of neurogenesis caused by Erk5 knockdown in progenitor cells impaired CFC (weak

shock) and reversal learning in the MWM (Pan et al., 2012). Interestingly, lentivirus mediated

knockdown of the Wnt signaling pathway in the DG also decreased neurogenesis and impaired

long-term MWM memory retention (Jessberger et al., 2009).

Manipulations that alter the maturation program of aDGCs can also lead to memory

deficits. Induction of differentiation in progenitor cells through expression of the

prodifferentiative gene PC3 decreased LTP and caused deficits in the MWM, radial maze and

CFC (Farioli-Vecchioli et al., 2008), whereas embryonic deletion of the transcription factor

Krüppel-like factor 9, which decreases differentiation, led to an anxiety phenotype and impaired

contextual fear discrimination (Scobie et al., 2009).

1.3.2.3.3 Function of aDGCs: ablation studies

Several studies have used pharmacological, irradiation, and, more recently, transgenic

approaches to examine the effects of ablating adult born neurons on memory formation. The first

direct implication of aDGCs in memory function came in 2001, when Shors and colleagues

reduced neurogenesis levels by treating rats with the antimitotic drug methylazoxymethanol

Page 62: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

49

acetate (MAM) and saw an impairment in trace eyeblink conditioning, a hippocampus dependent

task, but not in delay eyeblink conditioning, which is hippocampus independent (Shors et al.,

2001). Subsequent work showed, however, that not all hippocampus dependent tasks are

impaired by blockade of neurogenesis (Shors et al., 2002), on perhaps an epilogue for the whole

subfield (see the supplemental table in Deng et al., 2011 for a complete listing).

Ablation of neurogenesis is sometimes found to impair hippocampus dependent

contextual fear conditioning (Saxe et al., 2006; Winocur et al., 2006; Imayoshi et al., 2008;

Warner-Schmidt et al., 2008; Hernández-Rabaza et al., 2009; Ko et al., 2009; Snyder et al.,

2009a; Denny et al., 2011) but other times has no effect (Shors et al., 2002; Dupret et al., 2008;

Zhang et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2009) . Although one study reported a deficit in hippocampus

independent tone fear conditioning after ablation of neurogenesis (Shors et al., 2002), most

studies see no effect (Saxe et al., 2006; Winocur et al., 2006; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Warner-

Schmidt et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2008).

Regarding spatial memory, some studies report that neurogenesis ablation does not affect

MWM performance (Shors et al., 2002; Madsen et al., 2003; Raber et al., 2004; Meshi et al.,

2006; Saxe et al., 2006), whereas other studies report a deficit during acquisition (Dupret et al.,

2008; Zhang et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2009) or reversal learning (Garthe et al., 2009).

Furthermore, some studies show intact acquisition but a deficit in long-term ( > 2 weeks)

retention (Snyder et al., 2005; Jessberger et al., 2009).

Interestingly, consistent with the deficits found when flexibility of spatial information

was required, as in reversal learning (Dupret et al., 2008; Garthe et al., 2009), a recent report

showed a role for adult born neurons in cognitive flexibility, or the ability to selectively use

previously acquired information when there is a change in contingencies (Burghardt et al., 2012),

which had been predicted earlier by computational models (Wiskott et al., 2006)

These ablation studies vary in terms of ablation technique, degree of ablation, age of the

cells affected, animal species (and strain) and behavioural protocol. Some or all of these factors

could contribute to the contradictory results. For a detailed discussion on this topic see chapter 6.

A few papers have raised other interesting functions for adult hippocampal neurogenesis,

including regulation of stress-induced social avoidance (Lagace et al., 2010), vulnerability to

Page 63: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

50

cocaine addiction and relapse (Noonan et al., 2010), systems consolidation, by regulating the

hippocampus dependent period of memory (Kitamura et al., 2009), and pattern separation

(Clelland et al., 2009; Sahay et al., 2011a; Nakashiba et al., 2012; Tronel et al., 2012). This last

topic has emerged as a strong putative function of aDGCs and will be discussed in detail in

chapter 6.

1.3.3 Olfactory neurogenesis

Similar to the DG, most the olfactory bulb (OB) neurogenesis occurs postnatally and

continues throughout adult life (Kaplan and Hinds, 1977; Bayer, 1983). The largest germinal

zone in the adult brain lies in the lateral walls of the lateral ventricles, the SVZ. Neurons born in

the SVZ migrate anteriorly through the rostral migratory stream (RMS) to the olfactory bulb,

where they differentiate mostly into two types of interneurons, olfactory granule cells (OGCs)

and periglomerular cells (PGCs) (Altman, 1969; Lois and Alvarez-Buylla, 1994; Doetsch and

Alvarez-Buylla, 1996) (see Fig. 7A).

Figure 7. Olfactory system. A. Neuroblasts born in the subventricular zone migrate through the rostral migratory

stream toward the olfactory bulb where they differentiate into granule cells and periglomerular cells. B. Scheme of

the olfactory circuit. Sensory information is transduced from olfactory sensory neurons into mitral and tufted cells in

the OB. These relay information to higher order cortical structures, which is modulated by olfactory interneurons,

granule and periglomerular cells. The flow of information from the outside world into the cortex is named bottom up

pathway (black arrows). Cortical areas also regulate olfactory interneurons through centrifugal fibers in what is

referred to as the top-down pathway (purple arrows). LV=lateral ventricle, SVZ= subventricular zone, RMS= rostral

migratory stream, GCL=olfactory granule cell layer, Glut=glutamate

Page 64: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

51

In the following subsections, the olfactory system is briefly described with an emphasis

on anatomical circuitry, followed by a discussion of adult neurogenesis in the olfactory system

and description of the the trajectory of olfactory neuroblasts from birth through their migration

and final differentiation. Behavioural experiments that attempt to elucidate the function of these

adult born interneurons are discussed in a separate sub-section.

1.3.3.1 Olfactory system: an overview

Olfaction is a very important sensory modality, crucial for finding food, social

interactions like mating, conspecific recognition and maternal behaviours, as well as detecting

danger. The chemosensory starting point of this system is the main olfactory epithelium, where

olfactory sensory neurons – lined with olfactory receptors – transduce chemical stimuli into

spike trains. This leads to glutamate release through the olfactory nerve to glomeruli in the main

olfactory bulb, reaching the distal dendrites of mitral and tufted cells (Lledo et al., 2005 but see

Gire et al., 2012). These mitral and tufted cells act as relay neurons to higher olfactory structures

such as piriform cortex, anterior olfactory nucleus, and entorhinal cortex (Korsching, 2002) that

further process olfactory information (Wilson and Sullivan, 2011). Because this flow of

information goes from the external environment to the brain this is called a bottom-up pathway

(see Fig. 7B for a scheme).

Nonetheless, the OB circuit is more than a relay station, it refines olfactory information

through two classes of inhibitory interneurons, OGCs and PGCs, that facilitate mitral cell

syncronization and network oscillations (Bathellier et al., 2006; Lagier et al., 2007). PGCs are

GABAergic and dopaminergic, and connect either directly to the olfactory nerve or the primary

dendrites of mitral/tufted cells (Kosaka and Kosaka, 2005). OGCs are GABAergic and represent

over 90% of bulbar interneurons, connecting and inhibiting mitral cells via reciprocal

dendrodendritic synapses (Wilson, 2008; Mouret et al., 2009b).

Page 65: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

52

Excitation of mitral/tufted cells leads to glutamate release into OGCs that in turn inhibit

neighbouring mitral/tufted cells. This lateral inhibition has been shown to be essential for

olfactory discrimination (Yokoi et al., 1995; Abraham et al., 2010), and enhancement of

olfactory discrimination is accompanied by an increase in OGC recruitment (Mandairon et al.,

2008). Additionally, it is speculated that the oscillatory synchronization generated by

interneurons could be critical for sparcification of an otherwise distributed and redundant

stimulus representation, thus being essential for olfactory learning/encoding (Laurent, 2002;

Lledo and Lagier, 2006; Nusser et al., 2012; but see Wilson and Stevenson, 2003 and Vincis et

al., 2012).

The OB is a laminar structure divided in seven layers (see Fig. 8). The most external is

the olfactory nerve layer (ONL), containing axons from olfactory sensory neurons and glia. The

next layer is the glomerular layer (GL), containing spherical neuropil rich structures surrounded

by neurons and glia called glomeruli. It is through the ONL that external input reaches the

glomeruli, activating mitral/tufted and periglomerular cells (Shipley and Ennis, 1996; Lazarini

and Lledo, 2011).

Figure 8. Structure of the olfactory bulb. OB micrograph showing its layers. From the most superficial to the

deepest: the olfactory nerve layer (ONL), followed by the glomerular layer (GL), external plexiform layer (EPL),

mitral cell layer (MCL), internal plexiform layer (IPL) and finally the olfactory granule cell layer (OGCL). At the

core of the OB, with a higher nuclear density, is the rostral migratory stream.

Under the GL is the external plexiform layer (EPL), containing mostly dendrites of

mitral/tufted cells and granule cells. Subjacent to the EPL lies the mitral cell layer (MCL), a thin

monolayer of mitral cell somata. Immediately deeper is the internal plexiform layer (IPL),

Page 66: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

53

containing the axons of mitral/tufted cells that lead out to the cortex as well as granule cell

dendrites. This layer is followed by the granule cell layer (OGCL to distinguish from the

DGCL), containing the aggregated granule cell bodies (often united through gap junctions).

Finally, at the core of the OB lies the rostral migratory stream (Shipley and Ennis, 1996; Lazarini

and Lledo, 2011).

This circuit is further regulated by a top-down pathway that stems from higher order

structures such as frontal and olfactory cortices through centrifugal fibers back to OGCs and

PGCs at the OGCL and GL, respectively (Fig. 7B). The inputs from these centrifugal fibers are

believed to add contextual information to olfactory processing, such as attention, behavioural

state and learning context (Mouret et al., 2009b). Furthermore, the addition of new neurons and

their early regulation by centrifugal forces could contribute to this process (Whitman and Greer,

2007; Panzanelli et al., 2009).

1.3.3.2 Olfactory neurogenesis: birth, migration and maturation

The subventricular germinal zone is composed of three basic cell types: stem cells,

proliferating progenitors and migrating neuroblasts (Doetsch et al., 1997). The SVZ is separated

from the ventricular cavity by an epithelium monolayer of ependymal cells (E cells) (Doetsch et

al., 1997). The stem cells of the SVZ are periventricular astrocytes, also referred as B cells.

These cells have been shown to have all the characteristics of stem cells (Doetsch et al., 1999;

Seaberg and van der Kooy, 2003; Garcia et al., 2004), astrocytic morphology and expression of

GFAP and nestin (Doetsch et al., 1997). B cells give rise to C cells, transit amplifying cells, that

in turn give birth to migrating neuroblasts, A cells (Doetsch et al., 1999). D cells, or tanycytes,

are less common but reside between E cells, contacting the ventricle with microvilli in their

luminal surface (Doetsch et al., 1997).

C cells are the most proliferative cells in the SVZ. They are large and have a spherical

morphology, are nestin positive and are normally present in clusters or isolated in proximity to a

chain of A cells, which they presumably generate (Doetsch et al., 1997). Type A cells have

elongated cell bodies with one or two processes, are nestin, PSA-NCAM and Tuj1 [a class III

Page 67: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

54

tubulin marker that stains young neurons (Geisert and Frankfurter, 1989; Easter et al., 1993)]

positive, forming elongated chains of 1-4 cells wide and 20-30 cells long ensheathed by B cells

(Doetsch et al., 1997).

These chains of A cells migrate tangentially 3-8 mm through the rostral migratory stream

until they reach the OB, in approximately 5-12 days (Altman, 1969; Lois and Alvarez-Buylla,

1994; Doetsch and Alvarez-Buylla, 1996; Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002). Once there, they

detach from the chains and migrate radially to reach the outer or more superficial layers of the

OB (Lois and Alvarez-Buylla, 1994). The vast majority of neuroblasts (over 90%) differentiate

into granule cells, with a minority (~3%) differentiating into periglomerular cells (Winner et al.,

2002; Lazarini and Lledo, 2011). Neuroblasts can also differentiate into juxtaglomerular cells,

glutamatergic interneurons, but in a very small proportion (Brill et al., 2009). The rate of cell

death for these differentiating neuroblasts is very high, over 50% during the first 2 months

(Winner et al., 2002).

Granule cells form clusters in the OGCL, where, having no axon, they extend a large

dendrite towards the EPL, making dendrodentritic connections with mitral cells present in the

mitral cell layer, and becoming fully mature at around 2 weeks of age (Shipley and Ennis, 1996;

Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002). Using retrovirus labeling of neurons born in the adult SVZ,

Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla divided granule cells into 5 classes based on their maturational

state (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002).

Class 1 is composed of tangentially migrating neuroblasts in the RMS, from days 2 to 7.

These cells have a small elongated cell body with one prominent process and small trailing ones.

Around day 5, class 2 neuroblasts arrive at the OB and start radial migration, during which their

processes become longer and they start expressing Tuj1. At day 9, class 3 cells have completed

their migration, are larger, rounder and have a single process, the apical dendrite, that remains

unbranched and does not extend beyond the MCL (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002).

Class 4 cells, around day 13, have elaborate branched dendrites but no spines, whereas

class 5 cells, around day 15, are mature OGCs with a high density of dendritic spines on their

apical dendrite (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002). Although spines are present around day 15,

pre and postsynaptic markers are not observed until day 21, suggesting a role for system activity

Page 68: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

55

(centrifugal, mitral/tufted cells) in regulating aOGC maturation and incorporation into the

olfactory circuitry (Whitman and Greer, 2007)

Class 1 and 2 cells lack voltage-gated Na+ conductance, and are thus unable to spike, but

already express functional GABAA receptors and some AMPA receptors with GluR2 subunits

(Carleton et al., 2003). Over the course of radial migration (class 2) they start expressing

NMDARs. Migrating neuroblasts (class 1 and 2) express DCX and PSA-NCAM (Bonfanti and

Theodosis, 1994; Belluzzi et al., 2003).

Immature aOGCs are silent or non-spiking for most of their maturation: class 3 cells

already receive synaptic inputs but action potentials only start to be seen at the end of class 4 and

beginning of class 5 (Carleton et al., 2003). Starting at day 10 (class 3) cells start expressing

mature markers such as NeuN, calretinin, GABA and TH (Winner et al., 2002). Class 5 cells

were reported to be indistinguishable from their developmentally-generated counterparts

(Carleton et al., 2003), but were later shown to display a restricted time window of

hyperplasticity, present at 2 weeks of age and mostly lost at 8 weeks (Nissant et al., 2009).

The maturation of aPGCs differs from that of OGCs in a few key aspects. Periglomerular

cells‘ full axonal and dendritic maturation takes longer, around 4 weeks and, in constrast to

OGCs, their maturation of voltage dependent Na+ current and consequent ability to generate

action potentials precedes the presence of synaptic contacts (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002;

Belluzzi et al., 2003; Carleton et al., 2003). Similar to aOGCs, the first input to aPGCs is

GABAergic, which later becomes glutamatergic (Belluzzi et al., 2003).

Unlike the DG, in the olfactory bulb several pieces of evidence point towards a dynamic

replacement or turnover of neurons: the OB does not increase in size (Pomeroy et al., 1990),

there is continual apoptosis in the layers where new neurons are present (Fiske and Brunjes,

2001), and a very small proportion of neurons survive past 21 months (Kaplan et al., 1985).

More recently, definitive evidence confirmed the existence of the turnover, showing that adult

neurogenesis in the OB replenishes the population of interneurons (Imayoshi et al., 2008).

Page 69: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

56

1.3.3.3 Olfactory neurogenesis: intrinsic regulators

The specialized microenvironment responsible for regulating adult stem cell renewal and

neuroblast differentiation is referred to as the adult neurogenic niche (Alvarez-Buylla and Lim,

2004), allowing for a series of intrinsic-extrinsic interactions necessary for regulating

proliferation, cell fate and migration.

Classical development signaling pathways are present in the adult neurogenic niche, such

as BMP-Noggin, which is implicated in adult SVZ cell fate (Lim et al., 2000). Similarly, the

Ephrin family and their Eph tyrosine kinase receptors are crucial for axon guidance and cell

migration in the adult brain (Egea and Klein, 2007). EphB1–3, EphA4, EphA7 and their

transmembrane ligands, ephrins-B2/3 and A2, are expressed in the SVZ and are necessary for

neuroblast migration and proliferation (Conover et al., 2000; Holmberg et al., 2005). Sonic

Hedgehog signaling regulates progenitor cell maintenance and proliferation in the adult SVZ and

SGZ (Lai et al., 2003; Machold et al., 2003), similar to its functions during development

(Fuccillo et al., 2006). In vitro neurosphere assay data also suggests a role for Notch signaling in

the control of SVZ progenitor differentiation (Grandbarbe, 2003).

The transcription factor Pax6 is necessary for initial neuronal specification, being

expressed in an increasing gradient along the RMS towards the OB, with its persistent expression

leading to a dopaminergic periglomerular cell phenotype (Hack et al., 2005; Kohwi et al., 2005).

Transcription factors such as E2F1, traditionally linked to cell cycle regulation (Yoshikawa,

2000), seem to be involved in the control of adult neuroblast proliferation in the SVZ and SGZ

(Cooperkuhn et al., 2002). The glicosilphosphatidil inositol anchored signaling protein CD24 has

also been implicated in the control of SVZ proliferation (Belvindrah et al., 2002).

Lastly, a recent report shines light onto possible molecules necessary for the synaptic

integration of adult born olfactory interneurons into the OB circuit. The proteoglican Agrin, also

important during development and primarily studied as a synapse inducing factor in the

neuromuscular junction (Williams et al., 2008), was found to be necessary for survival and

morphological differentiation of olfactory interneurons (Burk et al., 2012).

Page 70: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

57

1.3.3.4 Olfactory neurogenesis: extrinsic regulators

The migration of A cells through the RMS is regulated by a series of extracellular signals,

some of which are beginning to be described. During tangential migration, chemorepulsive or

permissive signals including Slit proteins (Wu et al., 1999; Nguyen-Ba-Charvet et al., 2004),

integrin/laminin (Emsley, 2003a) and Erb4 receptor/neuregulin (Anton et al., 2004) interactions

help guide the neuroblasts. Arriving in the OB, chain detachment signals are received through

reelin-mediated mitral cell interactions (Hack et al., 2002) and tenascin-R present in the

extracellular matrix, which also marks the start of radial migration (Saghatelyan et al., 2004).

Several neutrotransmitters modulate SVZ proliferation: GABA (Liu et al., 2005) and

nitric oxide (Packer et al., 2003; Moreno-López et al., 2004) decrease proliferation, serotonin

increases proliferation (Brezun and Daszuta, 1999; Banasr et al., 2004), and dopamine can either

decrease or increase proliferation depending on the manipulation (Baker et al., 2004; Höglinger

et al., 2004; Kippin et al., 2005).

Growth factors also modulate SVZ proliferation in the adult brain, some examples being

BDNF (Zigova et al., 1998), which also modulates survival (Bath et al., 2008), FGF2, epidermal

growth factor (EGF) and heparin binding EGF (HB-EGF) (Kuhn et al., 1997; Doetsch et al.,

2002; Jin et al., 2003), ciliary neurotrophic factor (CNTF) (Emsley, 2003b), and vascular

endothelial growth factor (VEGF) (Jin et al., 2002), which is particularly relevant to the interplay

between vasculogenesis and neurogenesis that takes place in the SVZ (Alvarez-Buylla and Lim,

2004).

SVZ neurogenesis is also regulated by sensory input. Long-term odour enrichment

increases the survival of olfactory interneurons (Rochefort et al., 2002; Bovetti et al., 2009),

whereas sensory deprivation leads to OB atrophy, reducing survival (Corotto et al., 1994;

Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002; Mandairon et al., 2003, 2006b) of granule cells particularly

around 14-28 days of age (Yamaguchi and Mori, 2005), which is consistent with the critical

period of survival of OGCs.

Similar to the hippocampus, olfactory learning modulates SVZ neurogenesis. Survival of

olfactory interneurons is increased by olfactory associative learning (water deprived animals

Page 71: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

58

learn to lick for water in reponse to a specific odour, or food deprived animals to dig for a food

reward) (Alonso et al., 2006; Mouret et al., 2008; Sultan et al., 2010), but not by mere exposure

to odours (Alonso et al., 2006). A more detailed analysis uncovered a rather complex

relationship between olfactory learning and neuron survival, with learning inducing both survival

of 18-30 day old neurons and death of 38-45 day old neurons, along with spatial redistribution of

adult born neurons (Mandairon et al., 2006a; Mouret et al., 2008).

1.3.3.5 Olfactory neurogenesis: functions

Olfactory interneurons are thought to be critically involved in olfactory discrimination,

mainly through lateral inhibition and the synchronization of network oscillations (see section

1.3.3.1). In the rabbit, granule cells and their inhibitory influence on mitral/tufted cells were

shown to mediate inhibitory responses to similar odorants (Yokoi et al., 1995), and disruption of

oscillatory synchronization in the honeybee impaired the discrimination of molecularly similar

(but not dissimilar) odorants (Stopfer et al., 1997).

In mice, an increase in network oscillation due to developmental knockdown of GABAA

receptor 3 subunit altered the discrimination between individual and mixtures of odours (Nusser

et al., 2012). Similarly, an increase or decrease in mitral cell inhibition levels, through granule

cell ionotropic glutamate receptor manipulation, led to acceleration and deceleration of olfactory

discrimination, respectively (Abraham et al., 2010). Odour enrichment leads to facilitation of

short-term olfactory memory in an odour recognition task, which was correlated to a transient

increase in OB cell number (Rochefort and Lledo, 2005), suggesting a link between olfactory

memory performance and adult generated olfactory interneurons (see also Moreno et al., 2009).

Several strategies have been used to alter adult neurogenesis levels and examine the

impact on olfactory function, including developmental knockdowns, SVZ irradiation,

pharmachological treatment, aging, apoptotic inhibitors and genetic ablation (Lazarini and Lledo,

2011). The results have been quite mixed, with some manipulations impairing olfactory

discrimination (Gheusi et al., 2000; Enwere et al., 2004; Bath et al., 2008; Moreno et al., 2009;

Page 72: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

59

Mouret et al., 2009a) and others seeing no effect (Kim et al., 2007; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-

Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009).

Importantly, olfactory discrimination has been assessed using different tasks, some being

associative and operant (Enwere et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-

Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009; Mouret et al., 2009a), and

others being non-associative and spontaneous (Gheusi et al., 2000; Bath et al., 2008; Moreno et

al., 2009) (for a full comparison of the studies see table 1).

Most studies do not see impairment in odour detection following reduction of

neurogenesis (Gheusi et al., 2000; Mechawar et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Lazarini et al., 2009),

although one study found that AraC infusion led to a loss of odour detection at lower

concentrations (Breton-Provencher et al., 2009). Similar mixed results have been found with

regard to odour memory, with studies reporting no deficit (Imayoshi et al., 2008; Mouret et al.,

2009a), short-term memory deficits (Gheusi et al., 2000; Mechawar et al., 2004; Breton-

Provencher et al., 2009; Sultan et al., 2010) or long-term memory deficits (Lazarini et al., 2009).

Many factors could contribute to the mosaic of results found in this literature, including

compensation, non-specific effects of the manipulations, the developmental stage of the

manipulation, age of the cells affected, and type of task used. For a detailed discussion of these

factors see section 6.2.2.

Furthermore, odour is an important component in other types of behaviour, such as

detecting danger or social interactions. In fact, ablation of neurogenesis has been found to disrupt

odour fear conditioning (Valley et al., 2009). Interestingly, adult OB neurogenesis is also

involved in several social behaviours (Gheusi et al., 2009), including afilliative and social

behaviours in adolescent mice (30-40 days old) (Wei et al., 2011), female-male interaction

(Feierstein et al., 2010) and female mate preference (Mak et al., 2007), and paternal offspring

recognition (Mak and Weiss, 2010).

Page 73: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

60

Table 1. Comparison between olfactory neurogenesis behavioural papers

Study Method

Impact on immature

adult-generated

neurons

Impact on mature adult-

generated neurons

Odor

detection

Habituation

test

Odour memory

Task used for Odour

discrim

Cross-Hab

test

Odour

discrimination

(other)

Gheusi et

al., 2000 NCAM-/- Proliferation: no change OGCL width: 35% reduction No deficit

No deficit but less

exploration in

NCAM-/-

Impaired short-term

(80-100min)

Cross-habituation

Impaired ---

Enwere et

al., 2004

Aging, Lifr+/,

Tgfawa1/wa1

BrdU/Calretinin+ cells

(4 wpi): 59% reduction

in GL in aged mice, 57% Tgfa

BrdU/GABA+ cells (4 wpi):

41% reduction in GL, 55%

reduction in OGCL in aged mice,61% tgfa

---

---

Associative, Operant

(mix of coconut or

almond in water for fine discrimination)

--- Impaired In fine

discrim (not

100:0)

Bath et al.,

2008

BDNF+/-,

TrkB+/-, BDNFMet/Met

Proliferation: no change

BrdU+ cells in OGCL

(4 wpi): 10% reduction in

BDNF+/- and 30% reduction in TrkB+/- and BDNFMet/Met

mice

---

---

Cross-habituation

---

Impaired BDNF+/- ,

BDNF+/Met and

BDNFMet/Met

Imayoshi et

al., 2008

Nestin-

CRE-ERT2× NSE-DTA

--- NeuN+ cells in OGCL: 10%

reduction ---

No deficit (from

discrim task, 1or 7d)

Associative, Operant

(digging)

No deficit No deficit

Lazarini, et

al., 2009

Focal SVZ

irradiation

DCX+ cells in GL and

GCL: 70% reduction --- No deficit

Impaired long-term

(30d) (Operant 2 odour task)

Associative, Operant (go-no go licking) and

cross-habituation

(as in Gheusi et al)

No deficit

No deficit

Breton-Provencher

et al., 2009

SVZ AraC 28d

infusion

DCX+ cells in GCL:

75% reduction

NeuN+ cells in OGCL:

No change Impaired

No deficit long term

(Imayoshi et al)

Impaired short-term (60-120min)

Associative,

Operant

(digging) (as in Imayoshi et al.)

No deficit No deficit

Kim et al., 2007

Bax-KO mouse

No change in prolif,

SVZ, RMS enlargement

with ectopic NeuN+

No effect in OB size No deficit

---

Associative,

Operant

(as in Enwere et al.)

---

No deficit

Sultan et

al., 2010

SVZ AraC

25d infusion

Vast reduction BrdU+

cells in GL and GCL No effect in OB size ---

Impaired 5d

No effect acquisition

(associative learning digging task one

odour)

---

--- ---

Moreno et

al., 2009

SVZ AraC

21d infusion

Decreased proliferation No effect in OB size,

decrease in GAD67 labeling ---

---

Cross-habituation

Impaired

(after enrichment)

Mouret et al., 2009

zVAD

infusion into

OB

Increased survival

of granule cells ---

No deficit on go-no go

Associative, Operant

(go-no go licking)

No deficit

(overall longer explorations in

zVAD);

No deficit

(lag in discrimination

latency)

Mechawar et al., 2004

Nicotinic

AchR 2-/-

mice

No effect in prolif,

migration or

differentiation

Increased survival granule cells, more cells in OGCL

No deficit

Impaired short term

(240m)

---

--- ---

Page 74: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

61

Chapter 2 Aims/Hypotheses

This thesis is part of a broad scope of efforts trying to elucidate the physiological

significance of adult neurogenesis, particularly with respect to memory function. Current adult

neurogenesis literature encompasses efforts centered on the cellular and molecular levels, in

understanding and trying to control proliferation, survival and cell fate choices in the adult brain,

and on the system level, through models and behavioural studies trying to determine whether

there is a meaning in having a continuous pool of cells after development. The thesis‘ general

aim is to try and address the issue of adult born neuron contribution to memory function with a

more direct approach, hopefully helping to explain some of the apparent contradictions found in

the literature.

For technical reasons, all manipulations of adult neurogenesis thus far have been done

prior to memory acquisition. Although important correlations have been drawn from this

literature, it has many inconsistencies and raises a concern for compensation in the system

influencing memory acquisition/retrieval. We reasoned that a more direct way to establish if

adult born neurons are involved in representing a memory trace would be to ablate them after

encoding. That way, if these neurons indeed support memory, ablation will lead to a deficit in

retrieval. Thus, we developed a ‗tag and ablate‘ strategy to temporally control the ablation of

adult born neurons, gaining the freedom to independently target different stages of memory

processing.

Our first goal was to compare ablations of adult neurogenesis done before and after

training in hippocampus dependent tasks. Hippocampal lesion literature shows that

posttraining lesions are more disruptive than pretraining ones (Moser and Moser, 1998). Given

that some pretraining ablation studies see a deficit in memory tasks, we hypothesized that: (1)

posttraining ablation of aDGCs would impair retrieval of hippocampus dependent memory, and

(2) posttraining ablations would cause bigger deficits than pretraining ablations. If a deficit is

seen, it will provide the first direct evidence of adult born neurons supporting memory retrieval.

Page 75: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

62

Adult neurogenesis in the olfactory system has been implicated in odour memory and

odour discrimination. Since studies pertaining to this literature have also been limited to

pretraining strategies and the findings are quite contradictory, our second goal is to explore the

effect of pre- and posttraining ablation of adult born neurons in an associative odour

discrimination task. To do this we use our tag and ablate strategy in a mouse line with higher

recombination efficiency in the SVZ, in a hippocampus independent task. We hypothesized that

posttraining ablation of adult born olfactory interneurons would impair performance in an

associative memory task, and that these effects would be greater than pretraining ones.

Overall in this thesis we intended to develop a technique to allow the dissection of task

components that might offer insight into questions that have been raised in the field, rendering

some direct answers and hopefully helping understand past results and generate new questions.

Page 76: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

63

Chapter 3

General Methods

3.1 Cell culture

In order to verify that murine-derived cells are insensitive to DT, we conducted cell

viability assays using two cell lines, one from mice (3T3 cells) and one from monkey (2-2 cells).

Cells were seeded in 6-well plates (2 × 105 cells per plate). 24 h later DT (0, 0.01, 0.1, or 1

ng/ml) was applied to fresh medium. Supernatant and trypsinized cells were collected 72 h later.

Cell viability was assessed by exclusion of trypan blue (Sigma, IL). Internal duplicates were run

for each condition.

3.2 Mice

Nestin-CreERT2

mice. Nestin-CreERT2

+ mice express TAM-inducible Cre recombinase

under the control of a nestin promoter, and have been previously described (Imayoshi et al.,

2008). The lines of nestin-CreERT2

mice we used in our experiments corresponds to line 4 (for

chapter 4) and 5-1 (chapter 5) in Imayoshi et al. (Imayoshi et al., 2008), and have the highest

recombination efficiency in the subgranular zone of the hippocampus and subventricular zone,

respectively.

iDTR mice. Inducible diphtheria toxin receptor (iDTR) mice have been previously

described (Buch et al., 2005; Gropp et al., 2005). In iDTR+ mice, the gene encoding DTR

(simian Hbegf, heparin-binding epidermal growth factor-like growth factor) is under the control

of the ubiquitous Rosa26 locus promoter, but expression of the DTR transgene is dependent on

the Cre recombinase-mediated removal of a transcriptional STOP cassette. It is important to note

that neither high doses of DT in wild-type mice (Saito et al., 2001) nor expression of DTR alone

(without DT) (Buch et al., 2005) produces behavioral abnormalities or cell death.

Page 77: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

64

Rosa-LacZ mice. The Rosa-LacZ reporter mice have been previously described

(Zambrowicz et al., 1997). Similar to iDTR mice, the gene encoding LacZ is under the control of

the ubiquitous Rosa26 locus promoter, and expression of the LacZ transgene is dependent on the

Cre recombinase-mediated removal of a transcriptional STOP cassette. We crossed this reporter

line with nestin-CreERT2

mice to initially characterize TAM-induced recombination because,

unlike LacZ, which is expressed in the nucleus, DTRs are expressed as membrane proteins and

therefore difficult to quantify precisely. Importantly, we observed similar patterns of LacZ and

DTR expression following TAM treatment when crossed with nestin-CreERT2

mice.

All lines were maintained on a C57BL/6 background (Taconic Farms, Germantown, NY).

Genotypes were determined by PCR analysis of tail DNA samples as previously described (Buch

et al., 2005; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Zambrowicz et al., 1997). Nestin-CreERT2+

mice were bred

with iDTR+/-

or iDTR+/+

mice, resulting in nestin-CreERT2+

/iDTR+, nestin-Cre

ERT2+/iDTR

-, nestin-

CreERT2-

/iDTR+, and nestin-Cre

ERT2-/iDTR

- offspring. An equivalent breeding strategy was used

for the LacZ reporter line. Except in the case of wild-type mice, all of the transgenes were kept

as heterozygote in the chromosome of each transgenic mice to avoid possible complications by

over-expressing Cre recombinase (Forni et al., 2006) or loss of the Rosa allele (Zambrowicz et

al., 1997). In most ablation experiments we compared double transgenic mice (2xTg, nestin-

CreERT2+

/iDTR+) to control, single transgenic littermate mice (CTR, nestin-Cre

ERT2+/iDTR

- or

nestin-CreERT2-

/iDTR+). Both CTR and 2xTg mice were treated with TAM and subsequently DT

(see below). Importantly, this design ensures that group effects cannot be attributed to non-

specific effects of TAM or DT. In order to characterize TAM-induced recombination we

compared nestin-CreERT2+

/Rosa-LacZ+ vs. Nestin-Cre

ERT2-/Rosa-LacZ

+ mice.

Mice were bred in our colony at The Hospital for Sick Children, and maintained on a 12

h light/dark cycle with free access to food and water. Prior to all behavioral experiments, mice

were handled for 2 min twice a day for 5 days. Male and female offspring were used in all

experiments. All experiments were performed in the light cycle and conducted in accordance

with the Hospital for Sick Children Animal Care and Use Committee.

Page 78: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

65

3.3 Drugs

Tamoxifen (TAM) treatment. TAM (Sigma, IL) was dissolved in minimal ethanol (10%)

and suspended in sunflower seed oil (Lagace et al., 2007). Mice received either one (chapter 5)

or three (chapter 4) rounds of TAM treatment. In each round, mice received daily injections (180

mg/kg; i.p.) for 5 days, and each round was 4 weeks apart. TAM injections started at 4 weeks of

age for the hippocampal experiments (chapter 4), and around 4-5 or 8 weeks for the olfactory

experiments (chapter 5).

Diphtheria toxin (DT) treatment. Preparation and delivery of DT was described

previously (Han et al., 2009). DT (Sigma, IL) was dissolved in phosphate buffered saline (PBS)

and readily crosses the blood-brain barrier (Wrobel et al., 1990). In the majority of hippocampal

(chapter 4) experiments mice received daily injections of DT (16 µg/kg; i.p) for 7 days. In two

experiments (the visual discrimination and remote water maze experiments) mice received

injections for 2 days. We chose 2 days of DT treatment in these experiments because we

observed significant forgetting after a week-long retention delay in preliminary visual

discrimination experiments. For the olfactory experiments (chapter 5) mice received 5 days of

injections.

3.4 Immunohistochemistry

Mice were perfused transcardially with PBS (0.1 M) and 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA).

Brains were removed, fixed overnight in PFA and transferred to 0.1 M PBS. Coronal sections

(40 µm) were cut using a vibratome (Leica VT1200S). The following primary antibodies were

used: rabbit monoclonal anti-calbindin (D28K; 1:600; Cell Signaling Technology, MA), mouse

monoclonal anti-calretinin (1:1500; Swant, Bellinzona, Switzerland), goat polyclonal anti-

doublecortin (1:4500; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, CA), rabbit polyclonal anti-Egr1 (Zif268;

1:10000; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, CA), rabbit polyclonal anti-ERalpha (1:1000; Santa Cruz

Biotechnology, CA), mouse monoclonal anti-glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP; 1:120000;

Cell Signaling Technology, MA), rabbit polyclonal anti-Iba1 (1:1200; Wako Chemicals, Wako,

Japan), mouse monoclonal anti-NeuN (1:1000; Millipore, MA), rabbit polyclonal anti-LacZ

Page 79: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

66

(1:6000; Molecular Probes, OR), mouse monoclonal anti-nestin (1:150; BD Pharmingen, NJ),

rabbit polyclonal anti-Ki67 (1:10000; Abcam, MA) and goat anti-human HB-EGF (DTR; 1:150;

R&D Systems, MN). All sections were treated with 1% hydrogen peroxidase. Sections were then

incubated overnight with the primary antibody and then for 60 min at 20 ºC with HRP-

conjugated or biotinylated secondary antibodies (1:750; Jackson Immuno-research, PA). Signals

were amplified and visualized using Vectastain Elite ABC kit (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame,

CA), tyramide signal amplification, Alexa-Fluor conjugated Streptavidin (Invitrogen, Carlsbad,

CA) or DAB. Sections were mounted on slides with Permafluor anti-fade medium. For Ki67

staining slides were counterstained with methyl green and mounted with Cytoseal 280 mounting

medium (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA).

3.5 Imaging and quantification

All images were acquired using epifluorescent (either a Nikon Eclipse 80i or an Olympus

BX61) or confocal (LSM 710 Zeiss) microscopes. To calculate cell number, cell density, or

proportion of double-positive cells we used 1/5 systematic section sampling fractions covering

the entire anterior-posterior extent of the DG. To calculate the proportion of double-labeled cells,

confocal 1 µm Z-stack images were obtained using ZEN software (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany)

with a minimal interval of 15 µm to prevent duplicate counts of the same cell. We quantified

Ki67+ cells throughout the anterior-posterior extent of the DG using a 10× objective on the

Nikon Eclipse 80i epifluorescence microscope. We estimated the total number of LacZ+ cells

following TAM treatment using the optical fractionator method on the Olympus BX61

epifluorescence microscope using a 60×, 1.45 N.A objective and a motorized XYZ stage

attached to a computer with Stereoinvestigator 9.1 (MBF bioscience) (Chen et al., 2004). A

random systematic sampling was used for these stereological analyses (section interval of 1/5,

grid size of 250 × 250 m, 2D counting frame of 90 × 90 m using fractionators of 30 m in

thickness). Tissue thickness measured in each counting frame was used to estimate the total

number of LacZ+ cells in the entire DG. Conditions were optimized to obtain a Gundersen

coefficient of error below 0.05 (Gundersen et al., 1999).We quantified GFAP+ cells and Iba1

+

cells in the DG and CA1 throughout the entire anterior-posterior extent of the hippocampus.

Using StereoInvestigator software and the Olympus BX61 epifluorescence microscope, we

Page 80: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

67

created separate contours for the DG and the CA1. All GFAP+ and Iba1

+ cells within the

contours were counted using a 40× objective. Cell density was calculated by the total number of

cells divided by the total area of the contours.

3.7 General behavioral apparatus and procedures

Context fear conditioning. In the fear conditioning experiments, three contexts were used.

Context A (the training context) consisted of a stainless steel conditioning chamber (31 cm × 24

cm × 21 cm; Med Associates, St. Albans, VT), containing a stainless steel shock-grid floor.

Shock grid bars (diameter 3.2 mm) were spaced 7.9 mm apart. The grid floorwas positioned over

a stainless-steel drop-pan, which was lightlycleaned with 70% ethyl alcohol to provide a

background odour.The front, top, and back of the chamber were made of clear acrylicand the two

sides made of modular aluminum. For context B, a white, plastic floor covered the shock grid

bars and a plastic, triangular insert was placed inside the same conditioning chamber used for

context A. One of the walls of this insert had a black/white striped pattern. The other two walls

were white. Context B was cleaned with water. As contexts A and B were located in the same

windowless room and used common apparatus, they shared some overlapping features. In

contrast, context C (37 cm × 16 cm × 27 cm) was located in a different room and contained

features that were largely distinct from contexts A or B. It was made of opaque acrylic walls,

with bedding covering the floor. In contexts A and B, mouse freezing behavior was monitored

via overhead cameras. Freezing was assessed using an automated scoring system (Actimetrics,

Wilmette, IL), which digitized the video signal at 4 Hz and comparedmovement frame by frame

to determine the amount of freezing. Freezing in context C was scored manually.

During training, mice were placed in context A. After 2 min, mice were presented with a

30 s tone (2800 Hz, 85 dB) that co-terminated with a 2 s footshock (0.5 mA). Mice remained in

the context for a further 30 s before being returned to their home cage. Responsivity to the shock

during training was estimated by comparing mouse velocity immediately preceding vs. during

shock presentation using the following formula: (velocityshock – velocitypre-shock)/ (velocityshock +

velocitypre-shock).

Page 81: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

68

Eight days after training, freezing was assessed in 5 min tests in contexts A and B (~ 4 h

inter-test interval). In context B, the tone was presented after a 2 min delay. Three hours later,

freezing was assessed in a 2 min test in context C. Discrimination of contexts A and B was

computed by comparing freezing in contexts A and B during the first 2 min of testing (i.e.,

before the tone was presented). As in previous studies, we used the following discrimination

index (Corvelo and Eyras, 2008; Wang et al., 2009): (freezingcxt A - freezingcxt B)/max(freezingcxt

A,freezingcxt B). Discrimination scores ranged from -1 to +1, with positive scores reflecting

greater levels of freezing in context A compared to context B. This measure changes at the same

rate regardless of the relative difference in freezing levels in contexts A and B, and therefore is

more sensitive in detecting small differences in relative freezing levels than other commonly

used discrimination indices (e.g., (freezingcxt A - freezingcxt B)/(freezingcxt A + freezingcxt B))

(McHugh et al., 2007) that change slowly when differences are small and more rapidly when

differences are large (Corvelo and Eyras, 2008). Importantly, we found no difference between

statistical analyses based on our discrimination index compared to other commonly used

discrimination indices.

Water maze (hidden platform version). The apparatus and behavioral procedures have

been previously described (Teixeira et al., 2006; Kee et al., 2007). Behavioral testing was

conducted in a circular water maze tank (120 cm in diameter, 50 cm deep), located in a dimly-lit

room. The pool was filled to a depth of 40 cm with water made opaque by adding white, non-

toxic paint. Water temperature was maintained at 28 ± 1 ºC by a heating pad located beneath the

pool. A circular escape platform (10 cm diameter) was submerged 0.5 cm below the water

surface, in a fixed position in one of the quadrants. The pool was surrounded by curtains, at least

1 m from the perimeter of the pool. The curtains were white and had distinct cues painted on

them.

Water maze training took place over 5 days. On each day, mice received 3 training trials

(inter-trial interval was ~15 s). For each trial, mice were released into the pool, facing the wall, at

one of 4 pseudorandomly-varied start locations. The trial was complete once the mouse found

the platform or 60 seconds had elapsed. If the mouse failed to find the platform on a given trial,

the experimenter guided the mouse onto the platform. After the completion of training, spatial

memory was assessed in a 60 s probe test with the platform removed from the pool. Behavioral

data from training and the probe tests were acquired and analyzed using an automated tracking

Page 82: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

69

system (Actimetrics, Wilmette, IL). Using this software, we recorded a number of parameters

during training, including escape latency and swim speed. In probe tests, we measured the

amount of time mice searched the target zone (23.6 cm in radius, centered on the location of the

platform during training) vs. the average of three other equivalent zones in other areas of the

pool (Moser et al., 1993). Each zone represents 15% of the total pool surface.

Water maze (visual discrimination version). For the visual discrimination task, we

modified the water maze apparatus described above. The pool contained two visual cues. These

cues were cylindrical (4 cm in diameter, 4 cm in height), with either a vertical or horizontal

black/white striped pattern. One of these cues (counter-balanced across mice) was always

positioned above the submerged escape platform (10 cm in diameter, 0.5 cm below the surface of

the water). A transparent plastic cylindrical bar (1 cm in diameter, 13 cm in height) connected

the cue to the platform. The other, non-reinforced cue was also positioned 13 cm above the

surface of the water. For the non-reinforced cue, an identical transparent plastic cylindrical bar

connected the cue to base of the pool. Non-patterned white curtains replaced the curtains

containing distal cues in order to minimize reliance on spatial strategies.

Visual discrimination training took place over 5 days. Across training trials, the locations

of the reinforced and non-reinforced cues were varied pseudorandomly. On each day mice

received 6 training trials (inter-trial interval was ~10 min). On each trial mice were released into

the pool, facing the wall, at one of 4 pseudorandomly-varied start locations. The trial was

complete once the mouse found the escape platform or 60 s had elapsed. If the mouse failed to

find the platform on a given trial, the experimenter guided the mouse onto the platform. Three

days following the completion of training, discrimination memory was assessed in a 30 s probe

test with both cues present, but with the platform removed from the pool. As before, behavioral

data from training and the probe tests were acquired and analyzed using an automated tracking

system. During the probe test, searching was highly focused around the two cues. Accordingly,

we compared time spent in two zones (15 cm in radius) centered on the cue locations.

Additionally, heat maps representing averaged group data were generated using Matlab

(MathWorks, Natick, MA). In the heat map the average time (s) mice spent in a 6 × 6 cm area of

the pool was normalized for n = 10 mice per group. Behavioral procedures were adapted from

previous studies which established that post-training lesions of the hippocampus disrupt visual

discrimination memory (Sutherland et al., 2001; Epp et al., 2007).

Page 83: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

70

Associative olfactory discrimination task. The apparatus and training schedule have been

described previously (Imayoshi et al., 2008). Mildly food deprived (~90% of body weight) mice

are subjected to four days of training in an associative task. Each day animals have two five-

minute training sessions, one with each of the two odours (+ or – carvone). In all trials, odorant

solution is pipetted into a filter paper inside a petri dish containing small perforations on the

cover for odour flow. The odour containing petri dish is covered in bedding. Animals are

exposed to both odours separately each day, one of which is reinforced with six sugar pellets

hidden under the bedding on top of the dish, so that animals learn to dig under the correct odour

to find the sugar. All mice are counterbalanced for reinforced odour and order or odour exposure.

On the probe day a 10 minute test takes place, when mice are placed in a cage with both odours

present but in the absence of sugar. Learning is measured as time spent digging over the correct

odour. All probe tests were recorded and manually coded. A preference index was calculated as

[(diggingreinforced odour - diggingunreinforced odour)/( diggingreinforced odour + diggingunreinforced odour)].

3.8 Specific experimental protocols

Characterization of TAM-induced recombination. Nestin-CreERT2+

/Rosa-LacZ+ or nestin-

CreERT2-

/Rosa-LacZ+ mice were treated with TAM. Seven weeks following the completion of

TAM treatment the number, distribution and cellular phenotype of recombined LacZ+ cells was

quantified using immunohistochemical methods.

Characterization of TAM-induced recombination and ablation in 2xTg mice. 2xTg mice

were treated with TAM. Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment, mice

received daily injections of PBS or DT for 7 days. Twenty-four hours after the final DT injection

mice were perfused and DTR, nestin, calretinin and DCX expression were quantified

immunohistochemically.

Characterization of basal proliferation rates in 2xTg mice. To evaluate whether our

tagging impacted ongoing proliferation in the adult hippocampus, we examined expression of

Ki67 in CTR and 2xTg mice 7 weeks following the completion of TAM treatment. Ki67 is a cell

Page 84: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

71

cycle related nuclear protein, expressed by proliferating cells in all phases of the active cell cycle

(Kee et al., 2002).

Contextual fear: post-training ablation. CTR and 2xTg mice were treated with TAM.

Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment, mice were trained in context A.

Beginning one day later, mice received daily injections of DT for 7 days. One week after

training, freezing was assessed in contexts A and B, and then in context C in a subset of mice.

Contextual fear: pre-training ablation. CTR and 2xTg mice were treated with TAM.

Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment, mice were trained in context A. One

week after training, freezing was assessed in contexts A and B, and, later, in context C. In this

experiment, mice received daily injections of DT for 7 days during the week preceding training.

Conditioned taste aversion: post-training ablation. To evaluate whether post-training

DT-induced ablation impairs subsequent expression of a conditioned taste aversion memory we

trained CTR and 2xTg mice 7 weeks following the completion of TAM treatment, using

previously described methods (Ding et al., 2008). During training, mice had access to a single

bottle containing saccharin (0.2%) for 30 minutes. Forty minutes later mice were treated with

LiCl (0.15 M, 2% body weight, i.p.). Beginning the next day, mice received daily injections of

DT for 7 days. One day later, conditioned taste aversion was assessed in a choice test in which

mice had access to bottles containing either water or saccharin. An aversion index was calculated

as: saccharin consumed/total fluid consumed. An additional group of mice were treated

identically except that they received an injection of saline (instead of LiCl) during training.

Water maze, hidden platform version: post-training ablation, recent group. CTR and

2xTg mice were treated with TAM. Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment,

mice were trained in the hidden platform version of water maze for 5 days. Beginning one day

following the completion of water maze training, mice received daily injections of DT for 7

days. One day later, spatial memory was assessed in a probe test. Additional groups of CTR and

2xTg mice were treated identically, except that they received PBS rather than DT during the 7

days prior to the probe test.

Water maze, hidden platform version: pre-training ablation. CTR and 2xTg mice were

treated with TAM. Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment, mice were trained

Page 85: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

72

in the hidden platform version of the water maze for 5 days, and spatial memory was tested in a

probe test one week later. In this experiment, mice received daily injections of DT for 7 days

during the week preceding training.

Water maze, hidden platform version: post-training ablation, remote group. CTR and

2xTg mice were treated with TAM. Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment,

mice were trained in the hidden platform version of water maze for 5 days. Mice were treated

identically to above except that 35 d following the completion of water maze training, mice

received daily injections of DT for 2 days. One day later, spatial memory was assessed in a probe

test.

Water maze visual discrimination version: post-training lesion. One day after training in

the visual discrimination task, wild-type mice received sham lesions or NMDA lesions of the

entire hippocampus as previously described (Wang et al., 2009). Two days later, discrimination

was assessed in a probe test. Mice were perfused after the probe test. Brains were removed, fixed

overnight in PFA, and transferred to 30% sucrose. Coronal sections (50 µm) were cut using a

cryostat. Sections were mounted on gelatin-coated slides, stained with neutral red, and cover-

slipped with Cytoseal. Using Stereo Investigator software, the entire hippocampus and the area

of the hippocampus sustaining damage were outlined separately for every fourth section. The

proportion of total hippocampal tissue damaged was 66.2 ± 7.3%.

Water maze visual discrimination version: post-training ablation. CTR and 2xTg mice

were treated with TAM. Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment, mice were

trained in the visual discrimination task. Beginning one day following the completion of training,

mice received daily injections of DT for 2 days. One day later, discrimination was assessed in a

probe test.

General behavioral characterization following DT-induced ablation.CTR and 2xTg mice

were treated with TAM. Seven weeks following the completion of TAM treatment, mice

received daily DT injections for 7 days and behavior was evaluated in a battery of tests. All mice

were given the following tests in the same order (open field, visual discrimination, beam

walking, hanging grip, sticky dot/adhesive removal, forced swim).

Page 86: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

73

For open field testing, mice were placed in the center of a square-shaped arena (45 cm ×

45 cm × 20 cm height) and allowed to explore for 20 min. The open field apparatus was

constructed of Plexiglas, and was dimly-lit from above. Mouse location was tracked by a camera

located above the open field, and total distance traveled as well as time spent in 3 different zones

(outer, middle, inner) was measured (Limelight2, Actimetrics, Wilmette, IL). Total distance

traveled was used as a measure of spontaneous motor activity and distribution of activity in

different regions of the arena was used as a measure of anxiety-related behavior(Archer, 1973).

To evaluate vision we trained mice in the visual discrimination water maze task across

three consecutive days (6 trials/day). The latency to reach the platform and swim speed were

recorded.

To assess sensorimotor control and locomotor activity we used the beam walking task

(Chen et al., 2004). Mice were placed on a beam (70 cm long, 2 cm wide, elevated 30 cm above

floor) for 60 s, and paw slips (forelimb and hindlimb) were recorded. Each mouse received three

consecutive trials, and paw slips were calculated per distance traveled.

To evaluate muscle strength the hanging grip test was used (Chen et al., 2004). In this

test, mice were allowed to hang vertically from a metal wire using their forepaws. The latency to

fall was measured. Importantly, mouse weights did not differ between genotypes.

To assess somatosensory function we used the sticky dot or adhesive removal task

(Schallert et al., 2000). Briefly, a small piece of tape was placed on the plantar surface of the

mouse‘s forepaw. Time taken for mice to sense (i.e., shaking its paw or bringing its paw to its

mouth) and remove the adhesive tape were recorded. Each mouse was tested 3 times, and data

averaged. Each test lasted a maximum of 3 min.

To assess depressive-like behaviors we used the forced swim test (Porsolt, 1979). Mice

were placed in a cylinder (radius, 6.5 cm, height 20 cm) for 6 min. The cylinder was filled to a

depth of 12 cm with 25±1 ºC water. The time spent motionless was recorded for each mouse.

Associative olfactory discrimination: post-training ablation of 3 week old cells. CTR and

2xTg mice were treated with TAM. Three weeks following the start of TAM treatment, mice

were trained in the associative olfactory discrimination task for four days. 24h after mice were

Page 87: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

74

probed to confirm they had learned the association (pre-DT). 24h later animals started five days

of DT treatment. 24h later animals were subjected to another probe trial (post-DT). A variation

of this experiment consisted of the same design except mice were injected with TAM at 8 weeks

of age (and not 4-5 weeks as with the rest of the olfactory experiments).

Associative olfactory discrimination: pre-training ablation of 3 week old cells and long-

term memory test. CTR and 2xTg mice were treated with TAM. Three weeks following the start

of TAM treatment, mice were trained in the associative olfactory discrimination task for four

days. 24h after mice were probed to confirm they had learned the association. In this experiment,

mice received daily injections of DT for 5 days immediately preceding training. Animals also

received a long-term probe test 28d after the first probe to assess long-term memory.

Associative olfactory discrimination: post-training ablation of 8 week old cells. CTR and

2xTg mice were treated with TAM. Eight weeks following the start of TAM treatment, mice

were trained in the associative olfactory discrimination task for four days. 24h after mice were

probed to confirm they had learned the association (pre-DT). 24h later animals started five days

of DT treatment. 24h later animals were subjected to another probe trial (post-DT).

3.9 Data analysis

Data were analyzed using ANOVAs followed by t-tests. Because both male and female

mice were used, sex was initially included as a factor in the ANOVAs. Consistent with previous

studies (Jonasson, 2005), we found that males performed better than females in one of the water

maze experiments (hidden platform version, post-training ablation, recent group). However, we

found no further effects of sex, and no significant interactions between sex and genotype,

therefore this factor was dropped from analysis.

Page 88: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

75

Chapter 4

Posttraining Ablation of Adult Generated Neurons Degrades

Previously Acquired Memories

4.1 Abstract

New neurons are continuously generated in the subgranular zone of the adult

hippocampus, and, once sufficiently mature, are thought to integrate into hippocampal memory

circuits. However, whether they play an essential role in subsequent memory expression is not

known. Previous studies have shown that suppression of adult neurogenesis often (but not

always) impairs subsequent hippocampus-dependent learning (i.e., produces anterograde effects).

A major challenge for these studies is that these new neurons represent only a small

subpopulation of all dentate granule cells and so there is large potential for either partial or

complete compensation by granule cells generated earlier on during development. A potentially

more powerful approach to investigate this question would be to ablate adult-generated neurons

after they have already become part of a memory trace (i.e., retrograde effects). Here we

developed a diphtheria toxin-based strategy in mice that allowed us to selectively ablate a

population of predominantly mature, adult-generated neurons either before or after learning,

without affecting ongoing neurogenesis. Removal of these neurons before learning did not

prevent the formation of new contextual fear or water maze memories. In contrast, removal of an

equivalent population after learningdegraded existing contextual fear and water maze memories,

without affecting non-hippocampal memory. Ablation of these adult-generated neurons even one

month after learning produced equivalent memory degradation in the water maze. These

retrograde effects suggest that adult-generated neurons form a critical and enduring component

of hippocampal memory traces.

Page 89: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

76

4.2 Introduction

Division of progenitor cells in the subgranular zone leads to the continuous addition of

new neurons to the adult hippocampus, a brain region that plays a central role in memory

formation (Deng et al., 2010). These newly-generated neurons initially migrate into the granule

cell layer of the dentate gyrus (DG) and, over the course of several weeks, gradually establish

functional afferent and efferent connections (Toni et al., 2008; Toni et al., 2007; Zhao et al.,

2006). Previous studies have shown that,once sufficiently mature, adult-generated neurons are

activated duringmemory formation and/or expression (Kee et al., 2007; Stone et al., 2011b;

Tashiro et al., 2007; Trouche et al., 2009), suggesting that they become integrated into

hippocampal memory traces. However, these correlative studies do not establish whether these

neurons represent an essential component of a hippocampal memory. A direct way to test this

would be to examine the impact of removingonly this population of neurons after memory

formationon subsequent expression of that memory. While transgenic, pharmacological and

irradiation-based approaches have previously been used to manipulate adult neurogenesis prior

to memory formation (Clelland et al., 2009; Deng et al., 2009; Dupret et al., 2008; Garthe et al.,

2009; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Kitamura et al., 2009; Saxe et al., 2006; Shors et al., 2001; Zhang et

al., 2008), it has been technically challenging to manipulate adult-generated neurons after

learning.

To address this question we developed a ‗tag and ablate‘ transgenic strategy which

allowed us to tag adult-generated neurons, allow them to mature, and ablate them either before or

after training (Fig. 9A). To ablate neurons, we used a diphtheria toxin (DT)-based system (Buch

et al., 2005; Han et al., 2009). Apoptotic cell death is reliably induced after DT binds to the DT

receptor (DTR). Since mice do not express functional DTRs,and are therefore normally

insensitive to DT (Middlebrook et al., 1977), we used transgenic mice that express simian DTRs

in a Crerecombinase inducible manner (iDTR mice) (Buch et al., 2005). To restrict DT-induced

apoptosis to adult-generated neurons we crossed iDTRmice with nestin-CreERT2

mice, in which a

tamoxifen (TAM)-inducible Crerecombinase is expressed under the control of a nestin promoter

(Imayoshi et al., 2008). In adult offspring from this cross, TAM administration induces

Page 90: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

77

permanent expression of DTRs in neural progenitor cells and their progeny, and subsequent

administration of DT ablates only this tagged population of adult-generated neurons. We found

that selective ablation of these tagged, adult-generated neurons immediately (or up to one month)

after training impaired memory expression using three distinct hippocampus-dependent tasks.

Therefore, our data indicate that adult-generated neurons—if available at the time of learning—

come to form an essential and enduring component of hippocampal memory traces.

4.3 Results

4.3.1 Murine cells are insensitive to DT

We verified that murine cells are insensitive to DT using two cell lines, one derived from

mice (3T3 cells) and the other from monkey (2-2 cells). As expected, DT dose-dependently

reduced the number and viability of 2-2, but not 3T3, cells (Fig. 9B-C) (Middlebrook et al.,

1977).

Page 91: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

78

Figure 9.DT-based ablation. A. Schematic of the ‗tag and ablate‘ strategy for ablating mature, adult-generated

neurons. In adult nestin-CreERT2+

/iDTR+ mice (2xTg), TAM administration leads to permanent expression of DTRs

in neural progenitor cells and their progeny. Subsequent administration of DT ablates this tagged population of

adult-generated neurons only. B-C. In vitro assay demonstrating insensitivity of wild-type mouse cell lines to

DT.Whereas mouse-derived 3T3 cells were insensitive to increasing concentrations of DT (n = 2)(b), DT dose-

dependently reduced the number and viability of monkey-derived 2-2 cells (n = 3)(c).

4.3.2 Characterization of ‘tag and ablate’ mice

Tagging new neurons.We first characterized TAM-induced recombination

(―tagging‖) in nestin-CreERT2

mice by crossing them with rosa-LacZ reporter mice. In these

reporter mice, Cre-mediated excision of a STOP cassette induces LacZ expression under the

control of the Rosa26 promoter (the same promoter as in the iDTR mice). Consistent with

previous results, at 4 weeks of age, CreERT2

protein expression innestin-CreERT2+

mice was

robust, restricted to progenitor cells (Fig. 10a and Table 2), and limited to adult neurogenic

regions including the subgranular zone (Fig. 10b-g) (Imayoshi et al., 2008). As expected, TAM

induced recombination (―tagging‖ cells, LacZ+) in nestin-Cre

ERT2+/Rosa-LacZ

+reporter mice and

no recombination was observed in nestin-CreERT2-

/Rosa-LacZ+littermate controls (Fig.11a). We

next examined tagged cells 7 weeks after TAM administration was complete. As previously

observed (Imayoshi et al., 2008), TAM-induced recombination in the dividing progenitor

Page 92: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

79

population was highly efficient, with 86.2 ± 2.7% (mean ± s.e.m.) of Ki67+ cells and 77.7 ±7.8%

of nestin+ cells in the DG expressing LacZ. Furthermore, LacZ

+ cells were uniformly distributed

throughout the anterior-posterior extent of the DG (Fig.11b), suggesting no intra-regional

differences in TAM-induced recombination efficiency.Using stereological methods, we

estimated there were 31,709 ± 2,984 LacZ+ cells and 496,266 ± 46,556 NeuN

+ cells in the DG.

Therefore, our tagged population of cells represents approximately 6% of the entire population of

DG neurons (i.e., 31,709/496,266).

To evaluate the phenotype of tagged cells, we next stained for proteins expressed at

different stages of cell differentiation(Kempermann et al., 2004) (Fig. 11c). Ninety-four percent

of tagged (LacZ+) cells were also positive for markers of mature neurons (NeuN, calbindin) with

far fewer (<5%) that were positive for progenitor cell or immature neuronal markers (nestin,

doublecortin, calretinin) (Fig.11d and Table 3). Therefore, this predominantly mature population

of tagged neurons corresponds to the population of adult-generated neurons that are activated

during the formation and/or expression of hippocampus-dependent memories (Kee et al., 2007;

Stone et al., 2011b). Consistent with this, at this time point LacZ+ cells expressed activity-

dependent genes such as zif268 following training (Fig.11d). Importantly, basal levels of

proliferation in the hippocampus were unaltered by tagging [unpaired t-test: P> 0.05] (Fig.11e).

Page 93: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

80

Figure 10. CreERT2

expression is restricted to progenitor cells and limited to adult neurogenic regions.(a) In nestin-

CreERT2+

mice, CreERT2

protein expression (green) was found in nestin+ and DCX

+ cells (red) but not mature neurons

(NeuN; red) in the DG (scale = 10 µm). (b-g) In these mice, CreERT2

protein expression was limited to the

subgranular zoneof the dentate gyrus (DG) and the subventricular zone of the lateral ventricle (LV) (mm relative to

bregma; scale = 250 µm). Mo = molecular layer, GCL = dentate granule cell layer, CPu = caudate putamen, Pir =

piriform cortex, Thl = thalamus, OB = olfactory bulb, Gr = granular layer, Gl = glomerular layer.

Page 94: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

81

Figure 11.Tagging new neurons. (a)TAM-induced recombination occurs only in nestin-CreERT2+

mice (scale = 50

µm). (b) Recombination occurred throughout the anterior-posterior extent of the DG (mm relative to bregma; scale

= 150 µm). (c)Schematic showing markers associated with different developmental stages of adult hippocampal

neurogenesis. (d) Seven weeks after the completion of TAM treatment, most LacZ+ cells (green) co-stained for

mature neuronal markers (NeuN, calbindin; red), with far less staining for progenitor cell orimmature neuronal

markers (nestin, DCX, calretinin; red). LacZ+ cells additionally expressed activity-dependent gene zif268 (red) after

behavioral testing. (e) Seven weeks after the completion of TAM treatment, Ki67 expression levels were similar in

control (CTR; n = 11) and 2xTg (n = 7) mice, indicating that DTR expression has no effect on ongoing proliferative

activity in the adult hippocampus (scale = 250 µm). Mo = molecular layer, GCL = dentate granule cell layer.

Ablating tagged neurons. In order to ablate these tagged neurons we crossed nestin-

CreERT2

micewith iDTR mice, in which Cre-mediated excision of a STOP cassette renders cells

sensitive to DT (Buch et al., 2005). In TAM-treated nestin-CreERT2+

/iDTR+ (2xTg) mice, DTR-

expressing cells were localized to the subgranular zone and inner-most layer of the DG

(consistent with the pattern of LacZ expression in the reporter mice above) (Fig.12a). Subsequent

systemic injection of DT (but not PBS) virtually abolished these DTR-expressing cells [100%

reduction, unpaired t-test: t8 = 1.93, P< 0.05] (Fig.12a), indicating that DT treatment efficiently

ablated the tagged neurons (Buch et al., 2005; Han et al., 2009). In the same mice, we

Page 95: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

82

additionally quantified numbers ofprogenitor cells and immature neurons in the DG following

DT vs. PBS treatment. We found that DT treatment greatly reduced overall numbers of

doublecortin+

(Fig. 12b), nestin+[~94% reduction, unpaired t-test: t7 = 4.16, P< 0.01](Fig.12c),

calretinin+[~86% reduction, unpaired t-test: t6 = 2.03, P<0.05] (Fig.12d) cells in the DG of 2xTg

mice. While this subpopulation represents only a small proportion of all tagged cells (~94% are

NeuN+ or calbindin

+), nonetheless these data are consistent with the reduction in DTR-

expressing cells and suggests that DT-induced ablation is highly efficient.

Figure 12. Ablating tagged neurons. (a) DT (n = 4) but not PBS (n = 6) efficiently ablated DTR-expressing cells in

the DG (scale = 10 µm).Consistent with this, DT treatment reduced overall numbers of (b) doublecortin+

(DCX;

scale = 150 µm), (c) nestin+

(scale = 10 µm), and (d) calretinin+

(scale = 10 µm) cells in the DG of 2xTg mice. Mo =

molecular layer, GCL = dentate granule cell layer. * denotes P< .05.

Following DT treatment, there were similar numbers of astrocytes [unpaired t-tests: Ps

>0.05] but increased microglia in the hippocampus (Fig.13a-c). The increase in microglia was

limited to the DG [unpaired t-test: t7 = 3.46, P< 0.01], and not observed in CA1 [unpaired t-test:

P> 0.05], and therefore likely reflects localized phagocytosis following DT-induced apoptotic

Page 96: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

83

cell death. Furthermore, TAM and DT treatment produced no changes in mouse weight

[unpaired t-tests: Ps >0.05] (Fig. 14a), or in a range of tests assessing emotion, vision, motor or

somatosensory function [ANOVAs and unpaired t-tests: Genotype effects, Ps >0.05] (Fig.14b-j).

In subsequent behavioral experiments, 2xTg and littermate control mice (either nestin-CreERT2+

or iDTR+ but not both) were used. All mice were treated with TAM and subsequently treated

with DT either before or after training. Importantly, this design ensures that group effects cannot

be attributed to non-specific effects of TAM or DT.

Figure 13.DT-induced ablation produces minimal inflammation. (a) Glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP; red) and

ionized calcium binding adaptor molecule 1 (Iba1; green) expression in CTR (n = 5) and 2xTg (n = 4) mice 24 h

after administration of DT. (b) GFAP levels were similar for CTR and 2xTg mice in both the DG and CA1 regions.

(c) Iba1 expression was increased in 2xTg mice only in the DG and not in the CA1 region. Note that Iba1 expression

was mainly limited to the subgranular zone and innermost layer of the DG, a pattern that matches the distribution of

tagged (i.e., DTR+ or LacZ

+) cells after TAM treatment. * denotes P< .05.

Page 97: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

84

Figure 14. General health and behavior are not altered by DT-induced ablation. Seven weeks after the completion of

TAM treatment, CTR (n = 6) and 2xTg (n =7)mice were treated with DT. (a) Body weights were not different after

the completion of TAM or DT treatments. (b-j)The behavior of TAM- and DT-treated CTR and 2xTg mice was

characterized in a battery of tests. We observed no effect on (b) time spent immobile in the forced swim test, (c)

total exploration in the open field, (d) time spent in the outer, middle and innermost regions of the open field, (e)

latency to find platform in the visual discrimination water maze, (f) swim speed during training in the visual

discrimination water maze, (g) paw slips in the beam walk test, (h) latency to fall in the bar hanging test, (i) latency

to detect adhesive tape in the sticky dot test, or (j) latency to remove adhesive tape in the sticky dot test.

4.3.3 Post-training ablation of tagged neurons degrades a contextual fear

memory

We used our tag and ablate strategy to test whether deletion of adult-generated neurons

after training would impair subsequent memory expression. To test memory,we first used a

contextual fear conditioning task in which mice learn an association between a context and an

aversive event (i.e., the delivery of a mild footshock). When returned to the same context,

contextual fear memory is inferred from an increase in freezing behavior (Kim and Fanselow,

1992). The specificity of the memory may then be evaluated by comparing freezing levels in the

trained versus alternate contexts (Wang et al., 2009). This task is hippocampus-dependent (Kim

and Fanselow, 1992) and engages dentate granule cells (including those generated during

adulthood) (Stone et al., 2011b). We trained 2xTg and control mice with a tone-shock pairing 7

Page 98: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

85

weeks following TAM treatment (Fig.15a), a time-point when the vast majority of tagged, adult-

generated neurons have matured (i.e., ~94% express the mature neuronal marker calbindin,

indicating that they are ~4 weeks of age or older (Zhao et al., 2008)). During training, both 2xTg

and control mice responded similarly to the shock [unpaired t-test: P> 0.05] (Fig.15b) and

exhibited equivalent levels of freezing immediately before and after the footshock [Genotype×

Training phase ANOVA, effect of Training phase only, F1,20 = 31.85, P< 0.001; planned

comparison for after-shock freezing indicated CTR vs. 2xTg, P> 0.05] (Fig.15c), indicating that

tagging neurons (DTR expression) alone does not alter responsivity to shock or general activity

levels. Following DT treatment, control mice exhibited robust freezing in the trained context

(context A) and less freezing in an alternate context (context B) that shared a number of

overlapping features with the training context. In contrast, 2xTg exhibited robust but equivalent

levels of freezing in both [Genotype×Context ANOVA, Genotype×Context interaction, F1,21=

17.66, P< 0.005; Newman-Keuls post-hoc tests indicatedACTR>BCTRonly, P< 0.05] (Fig. 15d),

indicating that this post-training ablation abolished the ability to discriminate two similar

contexts [unpaired t-test,t21 = 4.27, P< 0.001] (Fig.15e). To test whether this post-training

ablation abolished the ability to discriminate dissimilar contexts, we next placed mice in a third

context (context C), which had no overlapping features with the original training context. In

context C, both 2xTg and control mice exhibited similarly low levels of freezing [unpaired t-test:

P>0.05] (Fig.15f). Therefore, these results indicate that post-training ablation of adult-generated

neurons led to the degradation (but not the erasure) of a contextual fear memory: While

recognition of the training context was unaffected, the ability to discriminate between similar

(but not dissimilar) contexts was impaired.

Page 99: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

86

Figure 15. Post-training ablation of adult-generated neurons degrades contextual fear memory. (a) Mice were

treated with TAM and then trained in context A. Following DT-induced ablation of adult-generated neurons,

contextual memory was assessed in contexts A, B and C. During training, CTR (n = 12) and 2xTg (n = 11) mice

exhibited similar (b) response to shock and (c) freezing levels before and after shock delivery. (d)Following DT

treatment, CTR mice froze more in the trained context (A) vs. a similar context (B). In contrast, 2xTg mice froze

equally in both. (e) The DT-induced ablation abolished context discrimination. (f) Freezing in a dissimilar context

(C) and (g) tone fear were similar in CTR and 2xTg mice. (h) Mice were treated with TAM and then trained in a

conditioned taste aversion (CTA) task. During training, saccharin was paired with 0.15 M LiCl. Following DT-

induced ablation of adult-generated neurons, preference for saccharin vs. water was evaluated. CTR/LiCl (n = 12)

and 2xTg/LiCl (n = 11) mice exhibited equivalent preference forsaccharin. Importantly, this preference was lower

compared to mice (n = 11) for which saccharin was paired with saline (rather than LiCl) during training (CTR/Sal).

* denotes P< .05.

The present experimental design rules out several alternative interpretations of the data.

First, the absence of freezing in context C indicates that the discrimination deficit in2xTg mice

was not due to an overall increase in propensity to freeze. Second, because DT was administered

Page 100: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

87

in the home cage, memory degradation effects cannot be due to blockade of a reactivation-

induced phenomenon, such as reconsolidation (Nader et al., 2000). Third, memory for the tone-

shock association was unaltered following the post-training ablation [unpaired t-test: P>0.05]

(Fig.15g). As this type of memory is supported by the amygdala (Han et al., 2009), this suggests

that deleting adult-generated neurons affects hippocampus-dependent memory only. Consistent

with this, in a separate group of mice similar post-training ablation did not affect expression of a

previously-acquired conditioned taste aversion memory [Group ANOVA, Group effect, F4,23 =

5.53, P< 0.01; Newman-Keuls post-hoc tests indicated stronger saccharin preference in the

CTR/SAL group compared tothe CTR/LiCL and 2xTg/LiCl, groups, Ps < .05] (Fig.15h).

4.3.4 Pre-training ablation of tagged neurons does not prevent formation of

new contextual fear memory

We next tested whether ablation of a similar population of adult-generated neurons

immediately before training would impair acquisition of a new contextual fear memory. As in

the previous experiment, 2xTg and control mice were treated with TAM, trained 7 weeks later,

and tested after a one week delay. In this case, however, DT was administered during the week

prior to training rather than during the week following training (Fig.16a). This experimental

design ensures that DT targets an equivalent population of neurons (in terms of number and

maturity), and that the retention delay is identical to the first experiment. During training,

both2xTg and control mice responded similarly to the shock [unpaired t-test: P>0.05] (Fig.16b),

and exhibited equivalent levels of freezing immediately before and after shock delivery

[Genotype ×Training phaseANOVA, effect of Training phase only,F1,25 = 40.95, P< 0.0001;

planned comparison for after-shock freezing indicated CTR vs. 2xTg, P> 0.05] (Fig.16c),

indicating that the pre-training DT-induced ablation did not affect shock reactivity or general

activity levels. One week following training, both 2xTg and control mice froze more in context

A compared to context B [Genotype × Context ANOVA, effect of Context only, F1,25 = 9.41, P<

0.005; planned comparisons indicated ACTR>BCTR and A2xTg>B2xTg, Ps< 0.05] (Fig.16d), and,

furthermore, the degree of discrimination did not differ between groups [unpaired t-test: P>0.05]

(Fig.16e). As expected, both 2xTg and control mice exhibited similarly low levels of freezing in

Page 101: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

88

context C [unpaired t-test: P>0.05] (Fig.16f), and tone freezing was also unaffected by pre-

training ablation [unpaired t-test: P>0.05] (Fig.16g).

In the first experiment, ablation of a population of predominantly mature, adult-generated

neurons immediately after training impaired subsequent memory expression, presumably

because these neurons had become an integral component of the memory trace. In contrast,

deletion of an equivalent population of neurons immediately before training did not prevent the

formation of a new contextual fear memory, indicating that memory formation may be supported

by existing dentate granule cells when this population ofadult-generated neurons isabsent at the

time of training. Moreover, the preserved ability to discriminate between both similar and

dissimilar contexts suggests that pre-training removal of adult-generated neurons did not

significantly impact memory quality.

Page 102: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

89

Figure 16. Pre-training ablation of adult-generated neurons does not prevent the formation of a new contextual fear

memory. (a) Mice were trained in context A following DT-induced ablation of adult-generated neurons. Contextual

memory was assessed in contexts A, B and C one week later. During training, CTR (n = 13) and 2xTg (n = 14) mice

exhibited similar (b) response to shock and (c) freezing levels before and after shock delivery. (d) Following DT

treatment, both CTR and 2xTg mice discriminated between contexts A and B, and (e) the degree of discrimination

did not differ. (f) Freezing in a dissimilar context and (g) tone fear were equivalent in CTR and 2xTg mice. *denotes

P< .05.

4.3.5 Post-training ablation of tagged neurons degrades spatial memory

The hippocampus is engaged by multiple forms of learning. To evaluate the

generality of our findings, we next asked whether similar post-training ablations would impact

spatial memory. To address this, we used the hidden platform version of the water maze task, in

which both acquisition and expression depend on the hippocampus (Riedel et al., 1999; Teixeira

et al., 2006). As before, we trained 2xTg and control mice in the water maze 7 weeks following

TAM treatment (Fig.17a). During training, latency to find the platform declined similarly in both

groups [Genotype × Training day ANOVA, effect of Training day only, F4,22 = 19.21, P<

0.0001], indicating that tagging (DTR expression) does not interfere with swimming, motivation

and vision required for acquisition of a spatial memory (Fig.17b). Following the completion of

training, 2xTg and control mice were administered DT and their spatial memory assessed in a

Page 103: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

90

probe test 7 days later. In this test, 2xTg mice searched less selectively than control mice,

spending less time in the target zone [Genotype × Zone ANOVA, Genotype × Zone interaction

F1,22 = 5.67, P< 0.05; planned comparisons indicated TCTR > T2xTg, TCTR > OCTR, and T2xTg >

O2xTg, Ps < 0.05] (Fig. 17c). We additionally conducted a similar experiment in which TAM-

treated 2xTg and control mice were trained in the water maze but were administered PBS rather

than DT prior to the probe test (Fig.17d). During training, latency to find the platform declined

similarly in both groups [Training day × Genotype ANOVA, effect of Training day only, F4,23 =

22.93, P< 0.0001] (Fig.17e), and in the probe test both 2xTg and control mice exhibited robust

spatial memory [Genotype × Zone ANOVA, effect of Zone only, F1,22 = 38.50, P< 0.0001;

planned comparisons indicated TCTR > OCTRand T2xTg > O2xTg, Ps < 0.05, TCTR vs. T2xTg, P> 0.05]

(Fig.17f). Together, these findings indicate that neither DTR tagging (in the absence of DT) nor

DT administration alone (in the absence of DTR tagging) impairs memory. Instead a

combination of DTR tagging and DT is necessary to induce memory loss.

Consistent with our fear conditioning data, these results indicate that selective

removal of adult-generated neurons following training impairs subsequent memory expression.

In contrast, DT-induced ablation of an equivalent population of adult-generated neurons

immediately before training had no effect on the formation of a new spatial memory (Fig.17g).

Following DT treatment, both 2xTg and CTR mice learned to find the platform with

progressively shorter latencies [Genotype × Training day ANOVA, effect of Training day only,

F4,23 = 9.48, P < 0.01] (Fig.17h). Likewise, in the probe test one week later, both 2xTg and CTR

mice searched selectively, spending equivalent amounts of time in the target zone [Genotype ×

Zone ANOVA, effect of Zone only, F1,23 = 8.14, P< 0.01; planned comparisons indicated TCTR >

OCTR, T2xTg > O2xTg, Ps < 0.05, TCTR vs. T2xTg, P > 0.05] (Fig.17i). This suggests that new spatial

learning may be supported by existing dentate granule cells when adult-generated neurons are

not present at the time of training. Importantly, this pre-training ablation did not affect

swimming, navigation, vision, or motivation required for acquisition and expression of spatial

memory. Therefore, as pre- and post-training ablations targeted equivalent populations of adult-

generated neurons, these results exclude the possibility that such performance factors could

account for spatial memory loss following post-training ablations.

Page 104: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

91

Figure 17. Post-training (but not pre-training) ablation of adult-generated neurons impairs spatial memory

expression. (a) Mice were treated with TAM and then trained in the water maze. Following DT-induced ablation of

adult-generated neurons, spatial memory was assessed in a probe test. (b) During training, latency to find platform

declined equivalently in CTR (n = 12) and 2xTg (n = 12) mice. (c) Following DT-induced ablation, 2xTg mice

searched less selectively compared to CTR mice, spending less time in the target zone (T). (d) Additional groups of

TAM-treated mice were trained in the water maze. However, mice were treated with PBS (rather than DT) during

the week preceding memory testing. (e) During training, latency to find platform declined equivalently in CTR (n =

11) and 2xTg (n = 14) mice. (f) In the probe test, both CTR and 2xTg mice searched selectively, spending more time

in the target zone compared to other (O) non-target zones in the pool. (g) Mice were trained in the hidden version of

the water maze after the completion of TAM treatment. During the week before training mice were treated with DT.

(h) During training, latency to find the platform declined equivalently in CTR (n = 14) and 2xTg mice (n = 11). (i)

In the probe test, both CTR and 2xTg mice searched selectively at the target zone. (j) Mice were treated with TAM

then trained in the water maze. One month later mice were treated with DT, and then spatial memory was assessed

in a probe test. (k) During training, latency to find platform declined equivalently in CTR (n = 14) and 2xTg (n =

12) mice. (l) Following DT-induced ablation at the remote time point, 2xTg searched less selectively compared to

CTR mice, spending less time in the target zone.* denotes P< .05.

Page 105: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

92

4.3.6 Post-training ablation of tagged neurons degrades remote spatial

memory

Expression of water maze memory depends on the hippocampus for at least one

month after training (Clark et al., 2005; Teixeira et al., 2006). To address whether adult-

generated neurons play a persistent role in memory expression, we next treated additional groups

of control and 2xTg mice with DT one month (rather than one day) after training (Fig.17j). As

before, both groups learned to locate the platform during training [Genotype × Training day

ANOVA, effect of Training day only, F4,24 = 26.83, P< 0.0001] (Fig.17k) and control mice

exhibited robust spatial memory even when tested more than onemonth later. In contrast, at this

remote time point, 2xTg mice searched less selectively compared to control mice, spending less

time in the target zone [Genotype × Zone ANOVA, Genotype × Zone interaction, F1,24 = 3.53, P

= 0.07; planned comparisons indicated that TCTR > T2xTg, TCTR > OCTR,and T2xTg > O2xTg, Ps <

0.05] (Fig.17l). These findings indicate that adult-generated neurons play an integral and

enduring role in the expression of a spatial memory.

4.3.7 Post-training ablation degrades visual discrimination memory

Post-training ablation of adult-generated neurons impaired the ability to discriminate

between two similar contexts in the fear conditioning experiment. To further explore the nature

of this deficit, we next developed a water maze visual discrimination task. In this task, two

similar cues (one vertically-striped, the other horizontally-striped) were positioned above the

surface of the water. Across training trials, the locations of these two cues varied

pseudorandomly. However, one of the cues was always located above a hidden platform (e.g.,

horizontal stripes). In order to evaluate whether this form of visual discrimination memory

depends on the hippocampus, we first examined the impact of post-training cytotoxic

hippocampal lesions (Fig.18a). During training, both groups of mice learned to discriminate

between the reinforced and non-reinforced cues [Lesion × Training day ANOVA, effect of

Training day only, F4,68 = 52.69, P< 0.0001] (Fig.18b). Mice received lesion or sham surgery one

day following training and then discrimination memory was subsequently assessed in a probe

test in which both cues were present, but neither reinforced. In this probe test, control mice spent

Page 106: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

93

significantly more time searching close to the previously reinforced cue than the non-reinforced

cue[planned paired t-test, t8 = 4.27, P< 0.05]. In contrast, mice with cytotoxic hippocampal

lesions spent similarly little time close to either cue[planned paired t-test, P> 0.05] (Fig.10c).

Our lesions affected ~66±7% of hippocampal tissue (Fig.18d). Similar hippocampal lesions in

rats produce equivalent deficits (Clark et al., 2007), and indicate that an intact hippocampus is

necessary for the expression of this form of visual discrimination memory.

We next evaluated whether post-training ablation of adult-generated neurons would

similarly impairthe expression of a visual discrimination memory (Fig.10e). During training,

2xTg and control mice learned to discriminate between the reinforced and non-reinforced cues

[Genotype × Training day ANOVA, effect of Training day only, F4,21 = 24.44, P< 0.0001]

(Fig.18f). After training, both groups of mice were administered DT and discrimination memory

was assessed in a probe test. As before, control mice spent more time searching close to the

previously reinforced cue compared to the non-reinforced cue in the probe test [planned paired t-

test, t11 = 3.02, P< 0.01]. Interestingly, DT-induced ablation produced a more subtle deficit than

cytotoxic hippocampal lesions. While discrimination was abolished in the 2xTg mice [planned

paired t-test, P> 0.05], their propensity to search close to either of the cues remained intact

(Fig.18g-h).Therefore, these results indicate that post-training ablation of a population of

predominantly mature, adult-generated neurons led to the degradation (rather than complete

erasure) of a visual discrimination memory: Some general features of the memory were retained

(e.g., cue-platform association), but the ability to discriminate between similar cues was

impaired.

Page 107: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

94

Figure 18. Post-training ablation of adult-generated neurons impairs visual discrimination memory. (a) During

training, a submerged platform was located beneath one of two visual cues (e.g., horizontal stripes). After training,

the hippocampus was lesioned, and discrimination between the reinforced and non-reinforced cues was evaluated in

a probe test. (b) Before surgery, latency to locate the platform declined at similar rates in Lesion (n = 10) and Sham

(n = 9) mice. (c) After surgery, whereas Sham mice searched selectively at the reinforced cue, HPC lesion mice did

not search at either cue. (d) Representative images of brains from Sham and Lesion mice. (e) CTR (n = 12) and

2xTg (n = 11) mice were treated with TAM and then trained in the visual discrimination task. Following DT-

induced ablation of adult-generated neurons, visual discrimination memory was assessed in a probe test. (f) Across

training days, latency to locate the platform declined at similar rates in CTR and 2xTg mice. (g) In the probe test,

whereas CTR mice searched selectively at the reinforced cue, 2xTg mice spent equivalent amounts of time close to

the reinforced and non-reinforced cues. (h) Heat maps reflect preferential searching close to previously reinforced

(left peak) vs. non-reinforced (right peak) cue in CTR but not 2xTg mice. Note that the peak at top of pool

corresponds to the release point at the start of the probe test. * denotes P< .05.

Table 2: CreERT2

expression analysis in 4 week old Nestin+ mice

Cell maturity marker Nestin DCX NeuN

Percent of CreERT2+

cells

expressing marker (±

s.e.m.)

97 ± 0.94 85 ± 2.3 0.72 ± 0.26

Cells analyzed 490 460 474

Page 108: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

95

Table 3: Maturation profile characterization of tagged population at the time of training

Cell Type Type 1, 2a,

2b

Type 2b, 3,

immature neuron Immature neuron

Immature to

mature neuron

Postmitotic mature

neuron

Cell maturity marker Nestin DCX Calretinin NeuN Calbindin

Percent of LacZ+ cells

expressing marker (±

s.e.m.)

3.1 ± 1.1 5.1 ± 1.8 0.72 ± 0.46 94 ± 1.4 94 ± 2.1

Cells analyzed 455 513 457 374 461

4.4 Discussion

In these experiments we used a ‗tag and ablate‘ transgenic strategy to examine the role of

adult-generated neurons in hippocampal memory. This approach offered two key advantages.

First, unlike previous approaches that targeted neural stem or progenitor cells to produce a global

disruption of neurogenesis (Clelland et al., 2009; Deng et al., 2009; Dupret et al., 2008; Garthe et

al., 2009; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Kitamura et al., 2009; Saxe et al., 2006; Shors et al., 2001;

Zhang et al., 2008), our ‗tag and ablate‘ system allowed us to specifically target a population of

adult-generated neurons that were predominantly born several weeks prior to training, without

impacting ongoing proliferative activity in the adult hippocampus. Second, whereas previous

approaches largely manipulated neurogenesis before learning, our strategy allowed us control

over the timing of the ablation. Therefore, using this system we were able to ablate adult-

generated neurons before or after memory formation. We found that selective ablation of adult-

generated neurons immediately (or up to one month) after training impaired subsequent memory

expression in three distinct hippocampus-dependent tasks. Previous studies provided correlative

evidence that adult-generated neurons are activated during the formation and expression of

hippocampal memories. The present findings—that their post-training ablation disrupts

expression of a previously acquired memory—provides direct experimental evidence that these

neurons, if available at the time of learning, come to form an essential and enduring component

of hippocampal memory traces.

Page 109: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

96

To ablate populations of adult-generated granule cells in a temporally-specific manner,

we took advantage of the well-characterized DT system (Buch et al., 2005). DT reliably induces

apoptosis following receptor-mediated endocytosis (Dorland and Middlebrook, 1979), but wild-

type mouse cells are 10,000 fold less sensitive to DT than human or monkey cells (Middlebrook

et al., 1977; Eidels et al., 1983; Stenmark et al., 1988). We exploited the insensitivity of mouse

cells to DT by using a transgenic line of mice in which a functional simian DTR receptor is

expressed in a Cre-recombinase inducible fashion (iDTR mice). Crossing iDTR mice with mice

that express an inducible Cre-recombinase in nestin+ cells allowed us to permanently express

DTRs in neural progenitor cells and their progeny. Importantly, we found that neither DT

administration nor DTR expression alone affected memory, consistent with previous findings

(Han et al., 2009). Rather, only the combination of DTR tagging and DT administration induced

retrograde memory loss, showing the specificity of the system.

The cells targeted for ablation included progenitor cells, immature neurons and mature

neurons. Therefore, our post-training ablation effects on memory might be due to loss of any (or

all) of these different cell populations. Indeed, genetic deletion of predominantly immature adult-

generated neurons is associated with long-term retention deficits in a water maze task (Deng et

al., 2009), suggesting that this population of cells contributes to memory robustness. However,

cellular imaging approaches suggest that adult-generated neurons are not maximally activated

during memory formation and/or expression until they reach a more mature stage (> 4 weeks of

age; (Kee et al., 2007; Stone et al., 2011b)). In our experiments the majority of tagged cells were

mature (e.g., 94% were calbindin+ and therefore >4 weeks in age (Zhao et al., 2008)), and

therefore it is very likely that loss of this population of mature, adult-generated neurons

contributed significantly to memory loss.

In contrast to the impairment produced by post-training ablation of adult-generated

neurons, similar pre-training ablation did not prevent the formation of new memories. These

effects parallel those of partial hippocampal lesions in water maze (both hidden (Moser and

Moser, 1998) and visual discrimination (Epp et al., 2008) versions) and contextual fear tasks

(Maren et al., 1997; Frankland et al., 1998; Wiltgen et al., 2006). For example, whereas chemical

lesions ablating ~30% of hippocampal tissue disrupt the expression of a previously-acquired

water maze memory, similar-sized lesions do not prevent the acquisition of a new water maze

memory (Moser and Moser, 1998). This suggests that, while spatial memories may normally be

Page 110: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

97

distributed throughout the hippocampus, new learning may be supported by residual tissue when

the lesion precedes training. Analogously, here we show that in the absence of a large population

of adult-generated neurons, memory formation may be supported by existing dentate neurons

without an obvious impact on memory quality. This potential for compensation by existing,

developmentally-generated granule cells may account for variable impact of pre-training

disruptions of adult neurogenesis on memory formation. While memory formation may be

impaired by pre-training suppression of adult neurogenesis (Clelland et al., 2009; Deng et al.,

2009; Drew et al., 2010; Dupret et al., 2008; Garthe et al., 2009; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Saxe et

al., 2006; Shors et al., 2001; Tronel et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2008), sometimes the effects of

these types of manipulations are mild or even nonexistent (Hernández-Rabaza et al., 2009;

Jaholkowski et al., 2009; Deng et al., 2010). Many factors differ across studies, and likely

contribute to the discrepant results. The advantage of the current approach is that we were able to

directly contrast pre- and post-training ablations under identical conditions (i.e., method of

ablation, number and type of cells targeted, training paradigm, apparatus, etc.). Our finding that

post-training lesions had greater impact than pre-training lesions is consistent with the idea that

targeting neurons after memory formation is likely more disruptive because these neurons have

already become committed to the memory trace.

Because cell death is induced by apoptosis, rather than necrosis, using our DT system,

impact on surrounding cells is minimized. Indeed, following DT-induced ablation there was no

hippocampus-wide increase in astrocyte number, and the increase in microglia number was

restricted to the DG. Nonetheless, our ablation affected a large number of cells (>30,000), and

therefore might non-specifically impact hippocampal function. However, the dissociable effects

of pre- and post-training ablation on memory suggest that such off-targeteffects cannot account

for the results. For example, the absence of pre-training effects in our water maze experiment

indicates that DT-induced ablation did not simply affect swimming, navigation, vision or

motivation necessary for the expression of a spatial memory. Likewise, in our contextual fear

experiment, the absence of pre-training effects indicates that DT-induced ablation did not simply

affect the ability to perceive differences between similar contexts. Finally, it is worth noting that

our DT-induced ablation should additionally lead to loss of subventricular zone-generated cells

in the olfactory bulb. However, it is unlikely that the loss of these neurons can account for our

retrograde memory effects since we observed consistent memory loss in three different tasks,

Page 111: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

98

each with different stimulus properties and performance demands. In particular, the two water

maze-based tasks do not depend on olfactory information (Morris et al., 1982).

Neurogenesis in the DG can be classified into three broad periods: embryonic, postnatal

and adult. Embryonic and postnatal neurogenesis occurs as a result of proliferation in the

primary, secondary and tertiary dentate matrices. However, these germinal regions decline by

postnatal day 10. By postnatal day 20-30, and continuing throughout adulthood, neurogenesis

becomes restricted to the subgranular zone (Altman and Bayer, 1990). Therefore, in our

experiments we initiated TAM treatment in mice at 4 weeks of age (for similar experimental

strategies see: (Ables et al., 2010; Sierra et al., 2010)). As proliferation rates are higher in this

post-juvenile period, this approach enabled us to ‗tag‘ large numbers of granule cells. Even so

this tagged population (including both cells generated during a post-juvenile period as well as

during adulthood) represents no more than 6% of the entire population of granule cells at the

time of ablation. That deletion of this relatively small proportion of dentate neurons was

sufficient to produce robust retrograde memory deficits across three different tasks is perhaps

surprising, and suggests that this population of subgranular zone-generated neurons plays an

especially important role within a broader network of dentate neurons supporting contextual fear,

water maze and visual discrimination memories. Indeed, it is necessary to lesion >15% of the

entire hippocampus to produce similar retrograde memory deficits in the water maze (Moser and

Moser, 1998). Since cells generated at different stages of development and adulthood appear to

be integrated into hippocampal memory circuits at similar rates (Stone et al., 2011b), this

suggests that this population of subgranular zone-derived neurons may disproportionately

influence hippocampal memory function, and raises the possibility that granule cells derived

from the subgranular zone may make distinct contributions to hippocampal memory relative to

those generated in the primary, secondary and tertiary matrices earlier on during development.

What is the nature of this role? In the fear conditioning and visual discrimination tasks, post-

training ablation led to memory degradation rather than erasure. In both tasks, while some

general features of the memory were retained (e.g., context-shock, cue-platform associations),

the ability to discriminate between similar contexts and patterns was impaired. Consistent with

recent reports (Clelland et al., 2009; Sahay et al., 2011), these deficits may suggest a specialized

role for adult-generated neurons in disambiguating similar (but nonetheless discrete)

representations (or pattern separation).

Page 112: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

99

Chapter 5 Post-training Ablation of Adult Generated Olfactory Interneurons

Impairs Associative Odour Memory Expression

5.1Abstract

In the adult brain, new neurons are continuously generated in the subventricular zone

(SVZ) and migrate into the olfactory bulbs (OB), where they mature and integrate into OB

circuitry. Previous studies have implicated adult-generated OB interneurons in olfactory

function, although their precise contribution to olfactory processing remains unclear. Here, we

examined whether adult-generated OB neurons play a role in the expression of an associative

odor memory. We used a tag and ablate strategy in which tamoxifen treatment of double

transgenic mice (nestin-CreERT2

-iDTR) leads to permanent expression of the diphtheria toxin

receptor exclusively in nestin-positive neural progenitor cells and their progeny (tagging).

Subsequent treatment with diphtheria toxin (DT) leads to highly efficient ablation of adult-

generated OB interneurons with precise temporal control. Three weeks after tagging, adult mice

were trained to associate a particular odor with a reward by consistently pairing one odor (+

odor) but not another odor (- odor) with sugar pellets located under the bedding. Post, but not

pre-training ablation of tagged neurons impaired olfactory associative memory expression. Our

finding that ablation of OB interneurons leads to a retrograde impairment in an olfactory

associative memory task indicates that adult-generated OB interneurons form an essential

component of olfactory memory traces.

5.2 Introduction

Olfaction is a critical sensory modality necessary for danger detection, food retrieval,

maternal and social behaviours. The subventricular zone (SVZ) is a site of continuous

neurogenesis throughout adulthood (Altman, 1969; Kaplan and Hinds, 1977; Bayer, 1983).

Neuroblasts originated from the SVZ migrate through the rostral migratory stream into the

olfactory bulb, where they differentiate into olfactory interneurons and integrate into the circuitry

Page 113: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

100

(Altman, 1969; Lois and Alvarez-Buylla, 1994; Doetsch and Alvarez-Buylla, 1996; Petreanu and

Alvarez-Buylla, 2002; Winner at al., 2002; Belluzzi et al., 2003; Carleton et al., 2003; Belnoue et

al., 2011). Activation of olfactory interneurons through the excitation of mitral/tufted cells leads

to lateral inhibition of neighbouring mitral/tufted cells and synchronization of network

oscillations (Laurent, 2002; Lledo and Lagier, 2006). Both these functions have been shown to

mediate olfactory perception (Kay and Laurent, 1999; Beshel et al., 2007; Cleland et al., 2007;

Mandairon and Linster, 2009) and discrimination (Yokoi et al., 1995; Stopfer et al., 1997; Nusser

et al., 2012). Investigation of the role adult generated olfactory interneurons play in these

processes has yielded mixed results, with some (Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al.,

2009; Moreno et al., 2009; Sultan et al., 2010) but not others (Kim et al., 2007; Imayoshi et al.,

2008; Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009) finding a deficit in olfactory function

due to ablation of neurogenesis. These studies vary in terms of ablation method, age of the cells

affected and behavioural protocol, but have one commonality: they all consist of pre-training

manipulations, exclusively examining effects on acquisition. Presently, it is unclear whether one

or all of these factors are responsible for the contradictory findings, and decisive evidence

concerning the role of adult-generated olfactory interneurons in olfactory function is lacking.

To address this issue, we have devised a transgenic strategy to achieve temporal control

over adult neurogenesis ablation, so that it is possible to compare the ablation of a same cell

population done before or after training (Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011). Using this ‗tag and

ablate‘ strategy, tamoxifen (TAM) treatment of double transgenic nestin-CREERT2

-iDTR mice

leads to exclusive expression of diphtheria toxin receptor (DTR) in nestin expressing progenitor

cells and their progeny (tagging) (Lagace et al., 2007; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Arruda-Carvalho et

al., 2011). Subsequent treatment with diphtheria toxin (DT) leads to highly efficient ablation of

these cells (Buch et al., 2005; Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011). We found that post- (but not pre-)

training ablation led to a deficit in an associative odour memory task, showing that adult-

generated olfactory interneurons are necessary for the expression of olfactory associative

memory.

Page 114: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

101

5.3 Results

5.3.1 CREERT2 expression is restricted to neural stem cells

We used a nestin-CREERT2

mouse line with higher recombination in the SVZ (line 5-1 in

Imayoshi et al., 2008). Consistent with previous reports (Imayoshi et al., 2008), we confirmed

that CREERT2

expression at the time of tamoxifen treatment colocalized exclusively with neural

progenitor cell markers such as nestin and not with the neuroblast (type A cells) marker DCX

(Fig. 19 and table 4).

Figure 19: CreERT2

expression is restricted to neural stem cells. (a) In nestin-CreERT2+

mice, CreERT2

protein

expression (green) was found in nestin+ (red) cells, but not in (b) DCX

+ cells (red) in the SVZ (scale = 10 µm).

5.3.2 Characterization of the tagging

To facilitate the visualization of tagged cells we crossed nestin-CREERT2

mice with a

Rosa26-lacZ reporter line (which uses the same promoter as the iDTR mouse line). It has been

previously described that TAM-induced recombination in this system is specific, only occurring

when both nestin-CREERT2

and Rosa26-LacZ (or Rosa26-iDTR) transgenes are present (Arruda-

Carvalho et al., 2011). Recombined cells were distributed throughout the OGCL and in the GL

(Fig. 20).

Page 115: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

102

Figure 20: Distribution of tagged neurons in the OB. Recombination (LacZ+ cells, green) was distributed in the

OGCL and GL.

At around 2-3 weeks of age, adult olfactory granule cells display a window of

hyperplasticity in which synaptic strength (Nissant et al., 2009) and response to novel odours

(Magavi et al., 2005) are enhanced. Given this critical period of plasticity, we wanted to

investigate whether 3 week old adult generated olfactory interneurons were necessary for

olfactory memory expression. First, we phenotyped these cells 3 weeks after tamoxifen

treatment, at the time of behavioural training. Our tagged granule cell population still retained a

considerable amount of immature cells, with approximately 33% of cells expressing the

immature cell marker doublecortin (DCX), and 63% the cell marker NeuN (Fig. 21, table 5), as

expected (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002; Winner at al., 2002). LacZ-positive cells located

close to the glomerular layer - consistent with periglomerular cells - were also observed but were

a minority (roughly around 10% of total tagged population) (Fig. 20).

Page 116: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

103

Figure 21: Maturation analysis of adult born olfactory granule cells. Three weeks after the start of TAM treatment,

roughly a third of LacZ+ cells (green) co-stained for immature neuronal marker DCX (a), whereas most tagged cells

colabeled with NeuN (red) (b).

5.3.3 Post-training ablation of adult born olfactory interneurons impairs associative olfactory memory expression

Manipulations of adult neurogenesis in olfactory tasks have so far been done before

training (Gheusi et al., 2000; Enwere et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Bath et al., 2008; Imayoshi et

al., 2008; Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Mouret et al., 2009; Moreno et

al., 2009). Evidence from lesion (Moser and Moser, 1998) and hippocampal neurogenesis

(Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011) studies suggests that compensation may mask the effects of pre-

training manipulations. Hence, we were interested in exploring whether a post-training

manipulation would be more efficient at disrupting an olfactory associative memory.

To do this, we treated control (either nestin-CREERT2

or iDTR single positive mice, CTR)

and 2xTg animals (nestin-CREERT2

-iDTR double transgenic animals, 2xTg) with tamoxifen and

waited three weeks to train them in a hippocampus independent (Akers et al., 2011) associative

olfactory task (Imayoshi et al., 2008). In this associative olfactory discrimination task, food

deprived mice were trained for four consecutive days to discriminate between two similar odours

[the enantiomers (+) and (–) carvone], one of which was reinforced with sugar pellets. Mice

learned to dig over the correct odour to find the pellets, which was assessed in a probe trial when

both odours were present in the absence of sugar. Both control and 2xTg learned the association

Page 117: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

104

between the correct odour and the reward, as seen by spending more time digging on the

previously reinforced odour [planned comparisons: Pre-DTCTR t10 = 5.80, P < 0.0001; Pre-

DT2xTg t15 = 4.77, P < 0.001] (Fig. 22c-d, Pre-DT), thus excluding the possibility that tagging had

a detrimental effect on the motivation or motor skills necessary to perform this task.

Animals were then treated with DT for five consecutive days and probed 24h after (Fig.

22a). DT treatment has been previously shown to promote highly efficient and specific ablation

with minimal inflammation (Buch et al., 2005; Han et al., 2009; Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011).

Nevertheless, to account for non-specific effects of the drug treatments, we used a genotype

control to ensure all animals are treated with both TAM and DT.

During a probe test following DT treatment, control animals were still able to remember

the odour-reward association, but 2xTg mice lost that association, as they spent an equivalent

amount of time digging over either odour (Fig.22b, c; Post-DT) [CTR: Odour × Probe ANOVA,

effect of Odour, F1,30 = 17.20, P< 0.001 and Probe F1,30 = 12.14, P< 0.01; planned comparisons:

Post-DTCTR t10 = 2.56, P < 0.05; 2xTg: Odour × Probe ANOVA, Interaction F1,30 = 13.55, P<

0.001; planned comparisons: Post-DT2xTg P > 0.05]. This post-training ablation deficit was

confirmed through a preference index, in which 2xTg, but not control animals, experience a drop

in their preference for the previously reinforced odour after DT treatment (Fig.22d) [Genotype ×

Probe ANOVA, no significant effect, planned comparison 2xTg t15 = 2.23, P < 0.05].

Importantly, this deficit cannot be attributed to impairment in anxiety or motor function, since

ablation did not interfere with performance in the open field (Fig. 23) [Genotype × Zone

ANOVA, effect of Zone only, F2,32 = 525.5, P< 0.0001; unpaired t-test: t16 = 2.16, P <0.05].

Page 118: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

105

Figure 22: Post-training ablation of adult-generated olfactory interneurons impairs expression of an associative

olfactory memory. (a) 8 week old animals were treated with TAM and then trained in an associative olfactory

memory task. After 4 days of training animals performed a probe test (Pre-DT) in the presence of both the

previously reinforced odour (+ odour, black bars) and its enantiomer (-odour, white bars). Following 5 days of DT-

induced ablation of adult-generated interneurons, associative memory was assessed in a probe test (Post-DT).

Control (n = 11) (b) and 2xTg (n = 16) (c) mice learned to associate an odour with the reward equivalently (Pre-DT).

Following DT-induced ablation, 2xTg mice lost the preference for the previously reinforced odour (Post-DT), as can

be visualized through their preference index (d). * denotes P< .05.

Figure 23: Open field performance following DT-induced ablation of adult generated olfactory interneurons. Three

weeks after the start of TAM treatment, CTR (n = 14) and 2xTg (n =4) mice were treated with DT for five days. (a)

We observed no effect on time spent in the outer, middle and innermost regions of the open field, but there was a

significance difference in (b) total exploration in the open field. * denotes P< .05.

Interestingly, TAM treatment started at 4-5 weeks of age (Fig.24a), a strategy used in

order to maximize the number of tagged neurons (see Ables et al., 2010; Sierra et al., 2010;

Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011 for a similar strategy), yields a similar deficit (Fig.24b,c) [CTR:

Odour × Probe ANOVA, effect of Odour, F1,26 = 28.18, P< 0.001 and Probe F1,26 = 5.18, P<

0.05; 2xTg: Odour × Probe ANOVA, effect of Odour only F1,22 = 12.70, P< 0.01; planned

comparisons: Post-DTCTR t13 = 4.88, P < 0.001; Post-DT2xTg P > 0.05], which can also be

visualized in terms of a preference index (Fig 22d) [Genotype × Probe ANOVA, effect of Probe

only F1,24 = 5.72, P< 0.05; planned comparisons: 2xTg t11 = 2.61, P < 0.05 ]. Together, these data

suggest that adult born olfactory interneurons play an important role in olfactory associative

memory expression.

Page 119: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

106

Figure 24: Post-training ablation of adult-generated olfactory interneurons impairs expression of an associative

olfactory memory. (a) 4-5 week old animals were treated with TAM and then trained in an associative olfactory

memory task. After 4 days of training animals performed a probe test (Pre-DT) in the presence of both the

previously reinforced odour (+ odour, black bars) and its enantiomer (-odour, white bars). Following 5 days of DT-

induced ablation of adult-generated interneurons, associative memory was assessed in a probe test (Post-DT).

Control (n = 14) (b) and 2xTg (n = 12) (c) mice learned to associate an odour with the reward equivalently (Pre-DT).

Following DT-induced ablation, 2xTg mice lost the preference for the previously reinforced odour (Post-DT), as can

be visualized through their preference index (d). * denotes P< .05.

5.3.4 Pre-training ablation of adult born olfactory interneurons does not interfere with associative olfactory memory acquisition

The majority of studies fail to see a deficit in olfactory associative memory acquisition

following reduction of adult neurogenesis (Kim et al., 2007; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-

Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009), with one exception (Enwere et al., 2004).

However, since the Enwere study uses global knockouts it is possible that its effects on memory

acquisition are consequence of embryonic events and thus do not pertain to adult neurogenesis.

Next, we wanted to perform a pretraining ablation of adult olfactory interneurons in order to

compare it with our post-training ablation and previous published results.

When the ablation was performed before training, both control and 2xTg animals

acquired the olfactory association similarly (Fig. 25b-c, 1 day; Fig. 25d), and were able to retain

that association for at least 28 days (Fig. 25b-c, 28 days; Fig. 25d) [CTR: Odour × Probe

ANOVA, effect of Odour only, F1,28 = 22.57, P< 0.0001; planned comparisons: 1DayCTR t14 =

5.32, P < 0.001; 28DaysCTR t14 = 4.31, P < 0.001; 2xTg: Odour × Probe ANOVA, Interaction

Page 120: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

107

F1,28 = 4.62, P< 0.05; planned comparisons: 1Day 2xTg t14 = 5.14, P < 0.001; 28Days 2xTg t14 =

2.71, P < 0.05; Preference index Genotype × Zone ANOVA, no significant effect; planned

comparisons P > 0.05]. Our pre and post-training ablations target the same population of cells,

ensuring that the only difference between them is the timing of the ablation. Hence, this data

excludes the possibility that the ablation interferes with motivation, odour detection or motor

skills necessary to express this associative memory, and reinforces the idea that posttraining

elimination of this cohort of cells, and not tagging or DT treatment alone, causes the olfactory

memory expression deficit. These data suggest that, if present at the time of training, adult

generated olfactory interneurons are necessary for associative olfactory memory expression.

Figure 25: Pre-training ablation of adult-generated olfactory interneurons does not impair acquisition of an

associative olfactory memory. (a) 4-5 week old animals were treated with TAM. Five days before training animals

were given daily DT injections. 24h. after the last DT injection animals were trained in an associative olfactory

memory task. After 4 days of training animals performed a probe test (1 Day) in the presence of both the previously

reinforced odour (+ odour, black bars) and its enantiomer (-odour, white bars). Another probe test was conducted 28

days later to assess long-term memory (28 Days). Control (n = 15) (b) and 2xTg (n = 15) (c) mice showed no deficit

in acquiring the olfactory association (1 Day), nor in the long-term retentionof that memory (28 Days), as can be

visualized through their preference index (d). * denotes P< .05.

Table 4: SVZ

Cell maturity marker Nestin DCX

Percent of CreERT2+

cells

expressing marker (± s.e.m.) 96 ± 1.2 2.6 ± 0.3

Cells analyzed 537 536

Page 121: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

108

Table 5:OB

Cell maturity marker

DCX

NeuN

Percent of LacZ+ cells

expressing marker (± s.e.m.)

33 ± 2.7

63 ± 2.3

Cells analyzed

836

658

5.4 Discussion

Using a tag and ablate double transgenic strategy we assessed the contribution of adult

generated olfactory interneurons to the acquisition and retrieval of an associative olfactory

memory task. We confirmed that CREERT2

expression was restricted to neural progenitor cells in

our system and that recombination occurred uniformly throughout the OB. At three weeks of

age, our tagged population was composed of cells expressing a mixture of DCX and NeuN

positive cells. Post-training ablation of three week-old cells led to deficits in associative olfactory

memory expression, while pre-training ablation of the same cell population did not impair

acquisition of the olfactory association or its long term retention. The fact that the ablation of this

population of young OB interneurons after associative learning produces a retrograde

impairment in task performance suggests that adult-generated OB interneurons form an essential

component of olfactory memory traces.

Our tag and ablate strategy allows for high recombination efficiency, targeting a

considerable population of cells, and their specific and highly efficient elimination (Buch et al.,

2005; Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011). This DTR approach offers a few advantages: its specificity

and lack of side effects reduces the opportunity for compensation, and the separation between the

tagging and ablation steps gives temporal flexibility to target different stages of memory

processing. Additionally, the dissociation between pre- and post-training ablation further

excludes the possibility that the deficit in olfactory associative memory found after post-training

ablation is due to non-specific effects in vision, odour detection, motivation or sensorimotor

function. Although this strategy targets cells in both the SVZ-OB and hippocampus, our task has

Page 122: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

109

been shown to be hippocampus independent (Akers et al., 2011), restricting the functional effects

of our ablation to the olfactory system. Our tagged population consisted predominantly of

granule cells, but periglomerular cells were also tagged. Presently, we are unable to assess the

different contributions of these cell types to our findings, and further studies will be needed to

examine the individual role of periglomerular cells to odour memory and discrimination tasks.

Our DT-induced ablation allowed us to select a population of cells and manipulate the

ablation timing to eliminate them before or after memory acquisition. We found that pre-training

ablation did not disrupt acquisition of an associative olfactory memory, suggesting that plasticity

of the remaining olfactory interneurons was sufficient to encode the association. Post-training

ablation of the same cohort of cells disrupted task performance. This dissociation mirrors that of

hippocampal lesions (Moser and Moser, 1998) and adult hippocampal neurogenesis (Arruda-

Carvalho et al., 2011) in which posttraining lesions seem to be more disruptive than pre-training

ones, likely due to commitment of the tagged neurons to that representation during learning.

Given that previous manipulations of adult olfactory neurogenesis were done prior to training, it

is possible that compensation from the remaining olfactory interneurons contributed to the lack

of effect in these studies (Kim et al., 2007; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-Provencher et al., 2009;

Lazarini et al., 2009).

The interpretation of adult olfactory neurogenesis loss of function studies could be

divided into behavioural tasks assessing odour memory and odour discrimination. Often an

associative task similar to ours is used as an assessment of both olfactory discrimination and

long-term memory (Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009).

Most ablation studies fail to see an effect in olfactory discrimination (Imayoshi et al., 2008;

Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009), but some see a deficit in long-term odour

memory (Lazarini et al., 2009; Sultan et al., 2010). We interpreted our retrograde deficit in

associative memory performance as an odour memory deficit for the following reasons: (1) we

did not test the animals using different concentrations or mixtures of our odours i.e. in a protocol

in which fine discrimination could be directly assessed, and (2) pre-training ablation did not

impact the animal‘s ability to discriminate between the two odours and form an association,

suggesting that what is lost in the post-training ablation group is the association between the

odour and the reward, and not the mere ability to distinguish between both odours.

Page 123: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

110

The literature concerning functional implications of adult olfactory neurogenesis

comprises different tasks for assessing associative olfactory memory, that range from a go/no-go

task (Lazarini et al., 2009; Mouret et al., 2009), mixtures in drinking water (Enwere et al., 2004;

Kim et al., 2007), digging tasks as the one used here (Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-Provencher

et al., 2009) and its variation (Sultan et al., 2010). The difference in nature and degree of

difficulty of those tasks could account for differential involvement of higher order structures and,

as shown in the hippocampus (Drew et al., 2010), a varying degree of adult neurogenesis

dependency. Therefore, exploiting variables within a same task, as done here, could prove

especially insightful in overcoming this challenge.

In this study, we chose to target adult olfactory interneurons at 3 weeks of age, because

this stage corresponds to the critical period of granule cells, during which they are more

responsive to novel odours (Magavi et al., 2005) and are more plastic (Nissant et al., 2009). Post-

training ablation of 3 week old cells impaired performance in our associative task. Belnoue and

colleagues described preferential recruitment of 5-9 weeks old granule cells in an associative

go/no-go task (Belnoue et al., 2011). Our data suggests this recruitment might start around the 3

week old stage, a time point unexplored by those authors. Using the flexibility of design in our

tag and ablate strategy, it would be very interesting to explore different stages of cell maturation,

e.g. younger cells, at a stage in which they have reached the OB but lack spines and are mostly

non-spiking (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002; Carleton et al., 2003), thus likely not involved

in odour processing; and older cells past the hyperplastic period, when they are described to

behave similarly to developmentally generated OGCs (Carleton et al., 2003; Nissant et al., 2009).

With this approach we would be able to determine, within the same task, whether the

contribution of olfactory interneurons to associative memory expression is age-dependent.

Page 124: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

111

Chapter 6 General Discussion

6.1 Summary of Results

In this thesis we have applied a transgenic tag and ablate strategy to specifically target

adult born neurons in the hippocampus and olfactory bulb and look at their contribution to

memory function. This enabled us to characterize our tagged population in terms of number,

distribution and maturation stage. Importantly, the temporal versatility of our approach allowed

us to compare adult born neuron ablations done before and after training.

In the hippocampus, we targeted a population of predominantly mature cells evenly

distributed throughout the anterior-posterior axis of the DG comprising approximately 6% of

total dentate granule cell population. Ablation of this cell population before training did not

impair acquisition of two hippocampal dependent tasks, CFC and MWM, suggesting that the

remaining DGCs may support memory formation when in the absence of aDGCs. Immediate (or

up to four weeks) postttraining ablation of this cell population led to memory degradation in

three hippocampal dependent tasks (CFC, MWM and visual discrimination WM), showing that

aDGCs, if present at the time of training, come to form an integral and enduring part of the

memory trace.

In the olfactory system, we investigated whether 3 week old olfactory interneurons, a

time point in which cells are hyperplastic, were involved in a hippocampus independent

associative olfactory task. Ablation of these cells before training did not prevent acquisition of an

olfactory associative memory. Posttrainign ablation of that same cohort of cells, however,

impaired expression of the olfactory association, suggesting that, as with the hippocampus, adult

olfactory interneurons form an essential component of olfactory memory traces.

Page 125: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

112

6.2 Pre vs. Posttraining

Due to technical limitations, prior to the studies reported in this thesis, manipulations of

adult neurogenesis happened before training. The basic experimental design in those papers

consisted of ablation of neurogenesis, followed by a recovery period (typically around 4-8

weeks), then training in a memory task, and a probe test (Snyder et al., 2005; Meshi et al., 2006;

Saxe et al., 2006; Winocur et al., 2006; Hernández-Rabaza et al., 2009).

Although important in establishing a connection between hippocampal neurogenesis and

memory, these results are quite mixed (Deng et al., 2011), and fail in directly establishing

whether adult born neurons functionally support memory encoding for two main reasons. First,

homeostatic response to the ablation likely leads to a change in proliferation and survival levels,

thus DG circuitry 4-8 weeks after ablation is not necessarily comparable to its normal

(physiological) state. Second, plasticity in the system might mask the involvement of these cells,

i.e. if the remaining cells are able to encode a memory in the absence of the aDGCs it does not

necessarily mean aDGCs are not normally involved in this process. Particularly, the size of the

ablation (number of cells affected) compared to the existing DGC population could be crucial in

predicting the behavioural outcome of the manipulation (see discussion below).

Therefore, our group speculated that a direct way of determining whether aDGCs

functionally support memories would be to ablate them after learning: if aDGCs form a critical

part of that memory trace, posttraining ablation will lead to a memory deficit during retrieval. To

do this, we developed a transgenic tag and ablate strategy, in which tamoxifen injection led to

permanent expression of DTR in neural progenitors and their progeny. Subsequent DT treatment

before or after learning led to the specific ablation of the tagged population. We showed that post

(but not pre) training ablations led to memory degradation in three hippocampus dependent

tasks: fear conditioning, MWM and visual discrimination WM. Since pretraining ablations did

not prevent acquisition of these tasks, we could rule out non-specific effects on vision,

motivation, sensorimotor function, inflamation or overall circuit disturbance as alternative

explanations for our findings.

Page 126: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

113

A similar dissociation between pre- and posttraining is found in the hippocampal lesion

literature. Moser and Moser investigated the proportion of hippocampus necessary for

acquisition vs. retrieval of MWM memory. They found that complete hippocampal lesions

impaired both encoding and retrieval of MWM memory (Morris et al., 1982, 1990b). However,

whereas ~20% of the hippocampus (from the dorsal end) is sufficient for MWM learning, 70%

of dorsal hippocampus was necessary for retrieval (Moser and Moser, 1998). Their main

conclusions were that (1) posttraining ablations were more disruptive that pre-training ones, i.e.

hippocampal function is more sensitive to posttraining manipulations, and (2) memories seem to

be distributed in the hippocampus, since lesions located in different hippocampal subregions

mostly did not impair acquisition (Moser and Moser, 1998).

We believe the same is happening in our system, since our posttraining ablations were

consistently more disruptive than equivalent pretraining ones. A likely explanation for this

phenomenon relates to the aforementioned issue of plasticity of the system. In an extremely

simplistic model, memory encoding consists of the recruitment of X number of cells into a

memory trace, and proper retrieval requires reactivation of a proportion of that same population

of cells (see Reijmers et al., 2007 and discussion below). Accepting this model, encoding is

possible as long as the total number of available cells (Y) exceeds or is the same as X. Once the

trace is formed, however, it can only be retrieved if a sufficient proportion of those specific X

cells remains. Therefore, encoding has more cells to spare (Y-X) than retrieval (X), hence the

potential for compensation and consequent increased resilience of the system to pretraining

lesions, explaining why the establishment of a network (during encoding) makes a memory more

sensitive to disruption by posttraining manipulations.

Interestingly, we saw a trend towards impairment in acquisition in some of our

experiments, particularly in the MWM (see Fig. 17). Two points are relevant to that observation.

First, drawing from the Moser and Moser study, an effective pretraining lesion not only needs to

be larger than a posttraining lesion but also needs to be large enough. This might be a key point

in understanding some of the inconsistencies in the pretraining ablation literature. The fact that

the size of the ablation in a pretraining design needs to be so large could explain the absence of

deficits found in many studies (Shors et al., 2002; Madsen et al., 2003; Raber et al., 2004; Meshi

et al., 2006; Saxe et al., 2006; Dupret et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2009),

including our own. It is possible that, had we tagged a larger population of cells, a pretraining

Page 127: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

114

deficit would have appeared. Second, it has been shown that different behavioural protocols have

different sensitivities to aDGC ablation (Drew et al., 2010), which could account, in our data, for

the impairment trend being present in the MWM but not in fear conditioning.

6.3 Erasure vs. Degradation

The quest for the memory engram has permeated the cognitive neuroscience field for

decades. Karl Lashley‘s search for a cortical area responsible for memory was unsuccessful,

leading him to deny the existence of a memory engram (Lashley, 1950), an assumption that

remained unrefuted for many years. Later research showed that areas outside of the cortex are

involved in memory and that the choice of behavioural task is critical in this quest (Squire, 2004;

see introduction), which may underlie Lashley‘s failure in his search.

The last two decades have brought immense advancement to this pursuit. Technical

developments allowed for the identification of neurons involved in encoding and retrieval,

mainly through expression of immediate early genes (reviewed in Guzowski et al., 2005). Mark

Mayford‘s group developed a model to genetically tag c-fos active neurons (Reijmers et al.,

2007), which allowed quantification of the overlap between the neuronal populations activated

during encoding versus retrieval, strengthening the claim that memories might be restricted to a

specific subset of neurons. Another strong indication came from an auditory fear conditioning

task, in which blockade of AMPA receptor incorporation in as little as 10-20% of lateral

amygdala (LA) neurons was sufficient to reduce memory (Rumpel et al., 2005).

The premise of our studies followed this reasoning: if aDGCs encode memory traces,

retrieval can be disrupted if enough cells are ablated. Confirmation of that premise (and of the

existence of memory engrams) came from the work of Sheena Josselyn‘s group. First, they

showed that CREB overexpressing LA neurons were preferentially recruited to an auditory fear

memory trace (Han et al., 2007). Next, the specific ablation of that subset of neurons led to

complete erasure of the fear memory (Han et al., 2009). This phenomenon has been confirmed

through inactivation of CREB overexpressing neurons (Zhou et al., 2009), and, in a recent study,

Page 128: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

115

sheer activation of neurons encoding a previous memory trace (tagged through their c-fos

expression) interfered with new memory acquisition, generating a new (hybrid) memory that

differed from the initial encoded association, which the authors referred to as a synthetic memory

(Garner et al., 2012).

In our study, we tagged a subset of DGCs (80-90% of the aDGC population born after 4

weeks of age), allowed them to be incorporated into a memory trace (by training animals in a

memory task), and subsequently ablated those tagged cells, hoping to see a similar memory

erasure effect found by Han and colleagues (Han et al., 2009). In our experiments, we saw a

consistent effect in posttraining ablations amongst all three hippocampus dependent tasks:

although the bulk of the memory remained, degradation occurred. Collectively, this data

reinforces the claim that a memory trace can be sustained by a restricted set of cells.

The structure of memory is multidimensional: it encompasses different categories of

information. For instance, to learn the visual discrimination water maze task used in our study,

animals need to recognize the existence of a submerged platform, their need to swim to it, the

fact that it is signaled by a cue, its distinction from another cue, etc. Hence, it is conceivable that

memory loss may also follow a gradient in its degradation, when some but not all of these

memory elements are lost. Regarding our observations, unlike the Han study, in which freezing

was greatly reduced by neuronal elimination (Han et al., 2009), our 2xTg animals preserved: (1)

the association between context and shock in the fear conditioning (2xTg mice froze similar to

controls in context A), and (2) the association between cue and platform in the visual

discrimination task (2xTg swam mostly around the two cues). In both these tasks 2xTg

performance was clearly impaired, the memory degraded. The partial nature of this degradation

was particularly obvious in the visual discrimination water maze, since in clear contrast with our

2xTg animals, hippocampal lesioned animals swim aimlessly near the border of the pool, the

embodiment of complete memory erasure.

Ablation of a subset of DGCs in our study consistently led to memory degradation, which

to our knowledge is the first indication that aDGCs support the expression of memory traces, and

that engrams may be found in the DG. Consistent with our observations, optogenetic activation

of DGCs previously tagged to a fear memory trace led to freezing behaviour (Liu et al., 2012),

providing further evidence that memory engrams can also be formed by DGCs.

Page 129: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

116

6.4 Drowning by numbers1

An interesting common point raised by these ‗memory engram‘ type studies (Han et al.,

2009; Zhou et al., 2009; Garner et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2012) is how ablation of only a few cells

(less than 20% of all LA cells in most studies) has such a significant impact on memory. In our

manipulation, we calculated that approximately 6% of total granule cell population was ablated.

The notion that a small subset of cells can have large impact on a system is not

unprecedented. In fact, it is a common feature in several networks. Graph theory predicts that

highly connected self-organizing complex networks, such as the World Wide Web or neuronal

networks, may behave as small world networks (in reference to the ‗small world phenomenon‘ or

6 degrees of separation), which are characterized by a preserved and highly connected topology,

so that any two units in the network can be connected through just a few links (Watts and

Strogatz, 1998).

Some small world networks are scale-free, which means that in a growing network newly

added units connect preferentially to the more highly connected units in the network (Barabási,

1999; Amaral et al., 2000), referred to as nodes or hubs (see Boccaletti et al., 2006 for a review).

A common analogy for this phenomenon comes from airplane routes, when comparing the

impact of closing down an airport such as Chicago O‘Hare versus the airport in Buffalo, NY.

Human brain functional networks also have small world architecture and hub regions

(Achard et al., 2006). Accordingly, targeted attack of the hub regions is more effective at

disrupting the system than an attack on random units (Achard et al., 2006). In the cellular level,

these hubs could represent neurons that are more connected than others and whose impact on the

network as a whole is larger. Indeed, functional neuronal hubs have been demonstrated in the

hippocampus (Bonifazi et al., 2009), and exemplify how a small group of cells can have a large

impact on a system.

1 Reference to the1988 Peter Greenaway movie of the same name

Page 130: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

117

In our study, we see a consistent impact on memory expression after the ablation of a

small subset of cells. In the hippocampal lesion literature, a minimum of 30% of hippocampal

tissue had to be removed in order to cause retrograde amnesia (Moser and Moser, 1998). The

disparity between our 6% and the 30% in the Moser and Moser study raises a few interesting

possibilities regarding the function of aDGCs. A tempting hypothesis is that these aDGCs are

over-represented in the memory trace. A few studies hint at that possibility, through immediate

early gene analysis (Ramirez-Amaya et al., 2006; Kee et al., 2007). However, more recent

evidence has shown that aDGCs are equally as likely to be incorporated into a memory trace as

their developmentally generated counterparts (Stone et al., 2011b).

Another possibility is that, even though aDGCs are recruited at the same rate as other

DGCs, they are functionally over-represented in memory encoding/retrieval, i.e. they behave as

neuronal hubs. If so, the hyper excitable period (Espósito et al., 2005; Ge et al., 2007a; Marín-

Burgin et al., 2012) displayed by aDGCs during maturation could be key in contributing to

higher connectivity and hub formation. Consistent with this, it has been suggested that immature

DGCs have a larger number of MF boutons when compared to mature DGCs, but this analysis

was performed in P15 mice (Yasuda et al., 2011). A thorough assessment of aDGC connectivity

(e.g. spine and bouton numbers) compared to developmentally generated DGCs would prove

insightful in answering this question, as would a functional network analysis of the circuit.

We have shown that aDGCs are capable of sustaining expression of a memory trace, but

our data cannot presently determine whether aDGCs are functionally distinct from any other

DGC. It is plausible that the only reason we saw memory impairment with our ablation is

because we targeted enough DGCs, regardless of when they were generated. An interesting

experiment to test this hypothesis is to perform a posttraining ablation of similar size and

distribution as ours but targeting exclusively developmentally generated DGCs. Unfortunately,

achieving a method to target a similar number and distribution of cells in the DG is still

technically challenging.

6.5 Avenues for silencing neurons

Page 131: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

118

Elucidating the function of a specific neuronal population is a frequent challenge in

various fields of neuroscience. A critical step to achieve this goal involves ensuring the isolation

and targeting of a given population with minimal interference to the surrounding cells. In the

neurogenesis field, a trait that discerns developmentally born from adult born neurons is that, in

the adult brain, only the latter are proliferating. Hence, a common ‗loss of function‘ approach in

the field has been to kill proliferating cells, either through pharmacological treatment with

methylazoxymethanol acetate (MAM), Arabinofuranosyl Cytidine (AraC) or temozolomide

(Shors et al., 2002; Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Garthe et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009;

Sultan et al., 2010), or X-ray irradiation (Madsen et al., 2003; Raber et al., 2004; Meshi et al.,

2006; Saxe et al., 2006; Winocur et al., 2006; Warner-Schmidt et al., 2008; Hernández-Rabaza et

al., 2009; Ko et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2009a; Valley et al., 2009).

Arguably the greatest advantage of both pharmachological and irradiation strategies is

their large impact on neurogenesis [up to 90% in some cases (Valley et al., 2009)], which can be

permanent (Wojtowicz, 2006). Additionally, these manipulations allow for focal intervention, i.e.

independent targeting of SVZ versus SGZ neurogenesis (Wojtowicz, 2006). Their main

disavantage is the possible side effects, described for MAM (Dupret et al., 2005), AraC (Odaimi

and Ajani, 1987) and irradiation (Monje and Palmer, 2003). Although the degree of side effects

caused by irradiation can be minimized with lower doses (Wojtowicz, 2006), it still incurs in

inflamation (See Fig. S21 of Kitamura et al., 2009) and overall health impacts that require a

recovery period. As discussed previously (see section 6.1), it is arguable whether losing the

homeostatic turnover of proliferation and cell death compromises these manipulations as

physiologically-relevant models.

Another distinction between developmentally and adult generated cells in the adult brain

is the expression of immature markers, such as nestin. Recent technical developments allow the

genetic ablation of adult born neurons, a more targeted approach when compared to irradiation

and pharmachological agents. Ablation studies using transgenic approaches kill cells at the

immature nestin expressing stage (Saxe et al., 2006; Dupret et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2009;

Snyder et al., 2011; Wei et al., 2011) or at the start of neuronal differentiation, such as at the

neuron-specific enolase 2 expressing stage (Imayoshi et al., 2008). These studies have two main

strengths: (1) they are more specific than pharmacological and irradiation manipulations, and (2)

they are inducible, therefore excluding embryogenesis-related effects previously unavoidable in

Page 132: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

119

transgenic methods of neurogenesis decrease. Interestingly, the nestin-TK transgenic approach

also has the advantage of being reversible (Deng et al., 2009).

Genetic manipulations of adult neurogenesis represented a large technical advance in the

field, but also have limitations. These include side effects of the drugs administered to induce

trangene expression, in particular ganciclovir (Biron, 2006) and tamoxifen (Vogt et al., 2008)

(although these can be overcome by properly designed controls and/or longer recovery delays),

and lack of temporal control over the age of the cells/ablation, thus restricting the manipulations

to pretraining experimental designs (and the associated issues previously discussed).

Our double transgenic strategy enabled the dissociation between the ‗tag‘ and ‗ablate‘

stages of the manipulation, making it distinct from all previous adult neurogenesis loss of

function strategies in two ways: (1) it conferred temporal control over the ablation, so that a

posttraining design could take place, and (2) this permitted the ablation of mature adult generated

cells, which had never previously been done. This distinction allowed us to provide for the first

time direct evidence of the participation of aDGCs in memory retrieval, and, through the pre vs.

post comparison, offer some insight into some of the contradictory results found in the

pretraining literature. Additionally, the choice of a DTR approach for inducing apoptotic cell

death of aDGCs offered both specificity and low side effects. Our three levels of controls (pre vs.

posttraining dissociation, genotype control and DT vs. PBS control) further excluded any non-

specific effects of drugs, ablation or genotype in our behavioural results.

One limitation of our approach consisted of the simultaneous ablation of SVZ/OB

neurons. Our choice of tasks that were hippocampal dependent and don‘t rely on olfactory cues

(MWM, visual discrimination water maze) reduced concerns with the issue of SVZ

neurogenesis. Another constraint was the heterogeneity of cell ages of the affected population. In

spite of our efforts to target predominantly mature cells (which represented around 94% of our

tagged population), given the nature of our transgenic approach and our high recombination

efficiency, a high proportion of immature cells was also tagged (around 80-90% of the total

nestin or calretinin positive population). Although these cells corresponded to a minority within

the total tagged population (around 6%), we were still targeting most immature cells. Therefore,

our system is not able to determine whether immature cells play a role in supporting memories.

Page 133: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

120

Recently, two new techniques have emerged with promising applications to the adult

neurogenesis field: optogenetics and designer receptor exclusively activated by designer drug

(DREADD) receptors. These allow for selective silencing of a specific neuronal population,

representing a step forward from the killing-type approaches previously available. Chapter 7

includes an in depth discussion of these new techniques and their application to adult generated

neurons.

6.6 The unbearable lightness of inconsistencies2

6.6.1 In the hippocampus

Loss of function studies of adult hippocampal neurogenesis have yielded mixed results,

with some finding no effect (Shors et al., 2002; Madsen et al., 2003; Raber et al., 2004; Meshi et

al., 2006; Saxe et al., 2006; Dupret et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2009; Drew et al.,

2010), and others finding impairment in memory task performance (Saxe et al., 2006; Snyder et

al., 2005; Winocur et al., 2006; Dupret et al., 2008; Imayoshi et al., 2008; Warner-Schmidt et al.,

2008; Zhang et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2009; Garthe et al., 2009; Hernández-Rabaza et al., 2009;

Ko et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2009a; Drew et al., 2010; Denny et al., 2011).

Reconciling these findings is a hard task, but we can start by enumerating the elements

that vary among them, which include ablation technique, degree of ablation, age of the cells

affected, animal species (and strain) and behavioural protocol; and what they have in common:

they are all pre-training interventions. The last topic has already been discussed in length (see

section 6.1). The following paragraphs review other factors possibly underlying the

contradictions found in this literature.

2 Reference to Milan Kundera‘s ―The Unbearable Lightness of Being‖, 1984

Page 134: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

121

The mixture of strategies used to ablate neurogenesis in these studies include

pharmacological treatment (Shors et al., 2002; Garthe et al., 2009), X-ray irradiation (Madsen et

al., 2003; Raber et al., 2004; Meshi et al., 2006; Saxe et al., 2006; Winocur et al., 2006; Warner-

Schmidt et al., 2008; Hernández-Rabaza et al., 2009; Ko et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2009a), and

transgenic mice (Saxe et al., 2006; Zhang et al., 2008; Dupret et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2009).

Lack of specificity and side effects in the pharmacological and irradiation approaches and effects

of compensation in the transgenic studies could contribute to the contradictory findings (see

previous section for a detailed discussion).

Irrespective of the ablation method, the efficiency of that ablation is likely to influence its

behavioural outcome. In fact, dose-related effects of neurogenesis ablation on behavior have

been reported, with only larger ablations leading to behavioural deficits (Shors et al., 2002; Ko et

al., 2009). Notably, the age of the animal at the time of ablation is a determining factor: given

the well known decay of neurogenesis levels with age, an effect is more likely to be seen in

younger animals due to their larger basal neurogenesis levels (Kuhn et al., 1996).

Furthermore, evidence suggests that the difference in the age of the cells affected/spared

in these manipulations could account for some of the mixed results, with the possibility that

immature cells exert unique contributions to memory processing (Shors et al., 2002; Snyder et

al., 2005; Deng et al., 2009; Marín-Burgin et al., 2012; Nakashiba et al., 2012). Interestingly,

abrupt differentiation induced by PC3 expression led to a deficit in CFC and MWM, even when

induced after training (Farioli-Vecchioli et al., 2008, 2009). Together, these reports accentuate a

possible role for 3-4 week old cells in memory processing, which deserves deeper and careful

exploration. Particularly, it is necessary to establish that it is the immature cells themselves that

are supporting the memories, as opposed to a memory impairment caused by disruption in the

homeostatic balance of neurogenesis.

Animal species is another variable in neurogenesis ablation studies, with some studies

done with rats and other studies done with mice. Snyder and colleagues performed a systematic

comparison between rats and mice on a series of common neurogenesis measurements, from

cellular to behavioral (Snyder et al., 2009a). Overall, they found a higher tendency to see a

behavioural effect of neurogenesis reduction in rats compared to mice, possibly due to a greater

functional representation of aDGCs in that species (Snyder et al., 2009a). Also, when dealing

Page 135: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

122

with mice, large variations in neurogenesis levels have been shown among mouse strains

(Kempermann et al., 1997b), which could also contribute to differences found in behaviour

following ablation.

Behavioural protocol is also likely to influence whether performance impairments are

observed after neurogenesis ablation. As has been demonstrated by Drew and colleagues, who

examined the impact of X-ray mediated adult neurogenesis ablation in a series of design

variations commonly used for fear conditioning, certain experimental designs seem to be more

sensitive in revealing disruption following ablation of neurogenesis (Drew et al., 2010). Since the

number of papers reporting a deficit in fear conditioning is higher than in the MWM, it would be

interesting to know, from a similar systematic comparison between memory tasks commonly

used in these studies, which behavioural tasks are more sensitive to neurogenesis ablation.

The last few years have brought on a different trend in the loss of function studies of

adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Instead of piling on conflicting reports, a couple of research

groups have tried to tackle factors that could help make sense of that literature (Snyder et al.,

2009a; Drew et al., 2010). We believe our study also contributes to that effort and provides a

technical step forward in directly assessing the role of aDGCs in memory retention. Hopefully,

combining critical analysis of the literature with new technologies (see previous section) will

ease the journey of elucidation of the physiological function of aDGCs into fruitful and

comprehensive research.

6.6.2 In the olfactory system

Loss of function studies in the adult olfactory neurogenesis literature are fewer than in the

hippocampus, but also comprise contradictory findings. A prominent (and confusing) feature of

the olfactory neurogenesis loss of function literature is that, in addition to the factors that vary

among studies of hippocampal neurogenesis (pre-training design, ablation method, size of the

ablation, age of the cells affected, behavioural protocol), the myriad of behavioural tasks is larger

than that used in hippocampal studies, and often the cognitive interpretation of the task varies

between research groups.

Page 136: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

123

Variables such as ablation method, size and pretraining designs have already been

discussed at length in the hippocampal context, and can be directly translated to the olfactory

literature. Thus, this discussion focuses primarily on the issues of behavioural protocol

(comprising categories of olfactory function, behavioural task design and interpretation) and age

of the cells affected by the manipulation, given their particular resonance in the olfactory

context.

Overall, in spite of one study not finding an effect (Imayoshi et al., 2008), most studies

find deficits in olfactory function following neurogenesis ablation (Breton-Provencher et al.,

2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009; Sultan et al., 2010). A first step when

interpreting these studies is to make a distinction between odour memory and odour

discrimination. Odour memory relates to the animal‘s ability to remember a given scent and/or to

associate it with reward or punishment (after a short or long delay), and odour discrimination

pertains to the ability to perceptually distinguish between odours. While most of these studies see

a deficit in a form of odour memory (Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Sultan

et al., 2010), only one sees impairment in odour discrimination (Moreno et al., 2009).

These adult olfactory neurogenesis studies use different behavioural tasks that vary in

nature (associative vs non-associative), degree of training and level of difficulty. For instance,

odour discrimination tasks vary from a simple cross-habituation task (Gheusi et al., 2000; Bath et

al., 2008; Lazarini et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009) to associative tasks involving digging

(Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Sultan et al., 2010),

licking go/no-go tasks (Lazarini et al., 2009; Mouret et al., 2009), or mixtures in drinking water

(Enwere et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2007). Importantly, the degree of dependency on higher order

structures such as the piriform and orbitofrontal cortices in these tasks is not known, which could

be crucial in influencing behavioural outcome after neurogenesis reduction.

An additional level of complexity is the fact that the same behavioural task is often

interpreted differently depending on the research group. For instance, a habituation-cross

habituation task is used as a measure of olfactory discrimination by the Lledo group (Gheusi et

al., 2000; Lazarini et al., 2009) and of olfactory perceptual learning by the Didier/Mandairon

group (Moreno et al., 2009). Additionally, within a same category, i.e. odour memory,

completely different tasks are used to measure short and long-term memory. Whereas short term

Page 137: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

124

memory is measured through a simple odour habituation task, long-term odour memory is often

assessed by an associative olfactory discrimination task, rendering the interpretation of these

results difficult. One such example is the study by Breton-Provencher and colleagues, that

reports impaired short term memory but no deficit in long-term memory (Breton-Provencher et

al., 2009). This seems like an oxymoron, otherwise how could a memory be present seven days

after training if it is lost after an hour?

Mandairon and colleagues recently proposed an explanation of these discrepancies by

classifying associative digging tasks into either operant (Lazarini et al., 2009; Sultan et al., 2010)

or non-operant (Imayoshi et al., 2008; Breton-Provencher et al., 2009, present study) versions,

and finding that only the first version of the task led to an increase in survival of adult generated

olfactory interneurons (Mandairon et al., 2011). This argument, however, has theoretical

weaknesses. First, both tasks involve digging over an odour to retrieve a reward, the only

difference being that the first version requires more digging than the second version. According

to Thorndike‘s law of effect, instrumental learning implies a change in behaviour (digging on the

correct odour) due to a reinforcement (sugar pellet) (see section 1.1.1), making it hard to justify

how the second version could be non-operant. Second, the amount of training is not comparable

between the two tasks: although the number of trials is the same between tasks, there is a

difference in the number of exposures to the reinforced odour. The first version has 4 reinforced

trials per day, whereas the second version only has 2 reinforced trials per day (Mandairon et al.,

2011). This surplus of reinforced trials may have a greater effect on cell survival.

Given all this diversity in tasks and their interpretation, it is very challenging to translate

conclusions from one study to another, especially between different research groups. This was an

incentive when designing our study, in which we sought to manipulate factors within the same

task, so that our conclusions could cease to be contingent on the task in hand and become about

the cellular mechanisms behind it.

As with the hippocampal neurogenesis studies, the cell population targeted in the

manipulation should be taken into account when interpreting results across studies. Adult

generated olfactory interneurons display a critical period of heightened plasticity at around 2

weeks of age (Nissant et al., 2009). Olfactory granule cells 2-3 weeks of age also seem to be

preferentially activated in response to a novel odour when compared to pre-existing cells

Page 138: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

125

(Magavi et al., 2005). Hence, ablation strategies differentially targeting progenitors/transit

amplifying cells (Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009;

Valley et al., 2009; Sultan et al., 2010) versus maturing cells (Imayoshi et al., 2008) could lead to

different behavioural phenotypes.

Belnoue and colleagues reported a task-dependent recruitment of cells at particular

maturation stages: whereas simple odour exposure led to preferential recruitment (measured by

Fos expression) of 2-week old cells, olfactory discrimination training led to recruitment of 5-9

week old cells (Belnoue et al., 2011). This dissociation might underlie some of the conflicting

findings between odour memory (many of which use simple odour exposure tasks) and

discrimination.

Our results have shown that postraining ablation of 3 week old olfactory interneurons

impairs performance in an associative odour discrimination task. It is possible that at 3 weeks

granule cells are already preferentially recruited to the trace, since Belnoue and colleagues did

not examine recruitment at any time point between 2 and 5-9 weeks (Belnoue et al., 2011).

Given the flexibility of our experimental design we intend to assess other time points to establish

whether we see a similar critical period for functional integration of these interneurons.

Particularly, we want to explore the age of 10 days, when neuroblasts have reached the OB and

finished tangential migration, but lack spines, and 8 weeks, when cells are mostly similar to their

developmentally generated counterparts.

6.7 What are aDGCs (and DGCs) really good for?

While several papers point towards a role for the DG in pattern separation, and

computational models predict a contribution of aDGCs to the process (see section 1.3.2.3.1),

Clelland and colleagues were the first to apply pattern separation-type methodology within

neurogenesis ablation studies. Using two spatial tasks, one using the radial arm maze and the

other using the touchscreen, they showed that animals with reduced neurogenesis (through

irradiation or lentivirus) displayed specific deficits in distinguishing between proximal stimuli,

Page 139: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

126

which did not occur when stimuli had a higher degree of separation (Clelland et al., 2009). The

authors interpreted this as a deficit in spatial pattern separation.

Sahay and colleagues devised a transgenic model with genetic deletion of the pro-

apoptotic gene Bax exclusively in adult progenitors (iBax mice), which leads to a decrease in cell

death and an overall increase in neurogenesis (Sahay et al., 2011a). This was a method to

augment neurogenesis without the non-specific effects on mood found with extrinsic regulators

such as running (Sahay et al., 2011a). Increased neurogenesis in the iBax mice led to improved

performance in a contextual discrimination task, to which the authors also conferred pattern

separation-type properties (Sahay et al., 2011a).

Similarly, Tronel and colleagues showed that inducible ablation of neurogenesis (through

overexpression of Bax in neural precursors, as in Dupret et al., 2008) led to a deficit in

contextual discrimination in the same task (Tronel et al., 2012), which the authors argued

reinforced the role aDGCs play in pattern separation and in decreasing interference (Tronel et al.,

2012) as has been predicted by some computational models (Wiskott et al., 2006). Interestingly,

it has been reported that ablation of neurogenesis leads to an increase in the amplitude of

spontaneous gamma frequency bursts and in the synchronization of firing to these bursts

(Lacefield et al., 2012), thus implicating aDGCs in modulation of network inhibition, which

could underlie these pattern separation-type functions.

Recently, Nakashiba and colleagues found intriguing results. Using a transgenic strategy

in which MFs of all DGCs older than 3 weeks of age were silenced, they saw a facilitation of the

contextual discrimination task used by Sahay and colleagues and a deficit in a version of the WM

in which some distal cues were missing during the probe (referred to as a pattern completion

task) (Nakashiba et al., 2012). The authors concluded that cells younger than 3 weeks have a

primary role in pattern separation, whereas mature neurons have a greater role in pattern

completion (Nakashiba et al., 2012). They argued that because old DGCs have been involved in

encoding memories for longer they have accumulated more overlapping representations, and thus

are more efficient at pattern completion, whereas young DGCs with no memory encoding

experience are more effective at pattern separating (Nakashiba et al., 2012).

Our findings are also in general agreement with those described in these studies.

Posttraining ablation of aDGCs led to a contextual discrimination deficit that shares many

Page 140: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

127

characteristics of the methodology used by the groups of Sahay and Tronel (Sahay et al., 2011a;

Tronel et al., 2012), and our visual discrimination water maze relies on the discrimination

between two very similar cues (or patterns), which may be interpreted as a pattern-separation-

like task.

This recent surge of studies has turned the role of aDGCs in pattern separation into quite

a consensus in the field. However, some caution is due when drawing big conclusions from a

handful of studies that use quite different behavioural tasks. The idea that the DG has pattern

separation potential was derived largely from modeling work and DG anatomical and functional

characteristics (such as MF anatomy and sparse coding) (see sections 1.2.2.4 and 1.3.2.3.1).

However, the behavioural translation of pattern separation is not that straightforward.

Behaviourally, pattern separation is broadly interpreted as the ability to distinguish

between two similar inputs. Two main avenues of behavioural tasks have currently become the

standard: a fear conditioning task in which animals learn to discriminate between two similar

contexts through several sessions of daily shocks received exclusively in one of the contexts

(McHugh et al., 2007; Sahay et al., 2011), and a battery of ‗spatial pattern separation tasks‘,

ranging from an arena in which objects are moved (Gilbert et al., 2001), to radial arm maze and

touchscreen based tasks (Clelland et al., 2009). The basic premise for the latter experiments is

that animals with deficits in pattern separation will be impaired in instances in which objects are

close together, but have no difficulty in discerning between objects far apart.

A major issue arises regarding the interpretation of these tasks. Although rationally akin

to the concept of pattern separation, there is currently no evidence that pattern separation-like

cellular mechanisms actually underlie the resolution of any of these behavioural tasks. In fact,

their behavioural phenotypes can be explained through different mechanisms. In a recent critique

of this literature, Aimone and colleagues point out that, although consistent with a pattern

separation phenotype, the fear conditioning and spatial pattern separation tasks also involve

elements of inhibitory learning and working memory, not to mention that it is impossible to rule

out that different neural circuits are recruited depending on the degree of dissimilarity between

the input patterns (Aimone et al., 2011). Two additional confounds should be taken into

consideration: the stress generated by 9-17 days of daily shocks in the fear conditioning task, and

Page 141: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

128

the fact that varying the length of object separation in the spatial tasks could be simply an issue

of task difficulty.

Aimone and colleagues also emphasize that pattern separation is not an exclusive

function of the DG. In fact, the ability to encode similar inputs as distinct representations is a

basic feature of neural networks, one present in most brain circuits (Aimone et al., 2011).

Instead, they propose that a ‗memory resolution‘ type interpretation is more suited for these

studies, since most of these tasks depend on the animals comparing the present situation they

need to solve with their memory of a previous experience, which resonates with Gray and

McNaughton‘s theory of the hippocampus as a conflict solver (Gray and McNaughton, 2003).

Interestingly, a few papers have also proposed a function for aDGCs in memory

clearance and systems consolidation. In 2001, Feng and colleagues proposed that aDGCs are

involved in forgetting or memory clearance, from an experiment with post-learning EE. They

saw that in a fear conditioning probe after EE, wild type mice froze less compared to transgenic

mice with a deficit in EE-driven neurogenesis (Feng et al., 2001), and postulated that increasing

neurogenesis through EE after learning accelerates memory clearance, making the memory

weaker. This finding is perhaps echoed in Meltzer and colleagues‘ computational model, which

shows that excess neurogenesis could be detrimental to pre-existing memories (Meltzer et al.,

2005).

Consistent with this idea, Kitamura and colleagues implicated aDGCs in systems

consolidation, in modulating the hippocampal dependent period of fear memories (Kitamura et

al., 2009). In their paper, reduction of neurogenesis, through irradiation or a transgenic mouse

with suppressed neurogenesis, extended the period in which hippocampal inactivation would

lead to amnesia. In contrast, running turned retrieval hippocampus-independent sooner,

accelerating the process of systems consolidation (Kitamura et al., 2009). Together, these papers

suggest that the addition of new neurons in the hippocampus may interfere with recently

acquired memories and facilitate their exodus from the hippocampus to cortical structures.

Furthermore, an interesting way of looking at these pattern separation and systems

consolidation/memory clearance studies is to consider the possibility that they represent

complementary roles of aDGCs within hippocampal circuits. It is possible that aDGC addition

initially disturbs previously acquired memories but, once these neurons survive and incorporate

Page 142: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

129

into the circuit, they serve as decorrelators and contribute to solving discrimination and pattern

separation-type situations. Presently it seems one thing is clear, that studies of aDGC function in

a circuit level would prove extremely insightful to both pattern separation and systems

consolidation debates.

6.8 What are aOGCs (and OGCs) really good for?

Single unit recordings of OB (mitral/tufted cells) and piriform cortex cells have shown

that while OB neurons optimally decorrelate mixtures of odorants with very small variations,

piriform cortex ensembles collapse these similar mixtures into the same representation, in a

pattern separation/pattern completion function similar to that of the DG/CA3 areas (Barnes et al.,

2008; Wilson, 2009). Olfactory interneuron inhibition of mitral/tufted cells contributes to this

input separation (Yokoi et al., 1995; Stopfer et al., 1997; Luo et al., 2001; Schoppa and Urban,

2003; Abraham et al., 2010).

Interestingly, olfactory discrimination interferes with aOGC cell survival (Alonso et al.,

2006; Mandairon et al., 2006; Mouret et al., 2008), and characteristics of young aOGCs [their

hyperplasticity (Nissant et al., 2009) and increased response to novel odours (Magavi et al.,

2005)] are consistent with a role in facilitating pattern separation. Altogether, this evidence

points towards a function of OGCs (and aOGCs) in olfactory discrimination, and has led

researchers to speculate that pattern separation might be a general function of adult neurogenesis,

given the similarities between the two main circuits in which it exists (i.e. OB and DG)(Sahay et

al., 2011b).

However, despite the strong indications of the contribution of olfactory interneurons to

olfactory discrimination, very few studies that manipulate adult neurogenesis see a

discrimination deficit. Lazarini and colleagues suggested that olfactory discrimination is

impaired only by manipulations that disrupt neurogenesis from embryogenesis onward (e.g.

Gheusi et al., 2000; Enwere et al., 2004; Bath et al., 2008), and thus may not be mediated by

adult-generated neurons (Lazarini and Lledo, 2011). Still, although developmental perturbations

Page 143: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

130

could definitely play a role in these studies, this cannot explain the impairment in fine

discrimination observed during normal aging (Enwere et al., 2004) or in the facilitation of

olfactory discrimination following increased neurogenesis during adulthood (through odour

enrichment) (Moreno et al., 2009).

Most deficits following neurogenesis ablation are observed in some category of odour

memory (Breton-Provencher et al., 2009; Lazarini et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009; Sultan et al.,

2010). Although there is evidence correlating bulbar network activity (and olfactory

interneurons) with odour representation/olfactory perception (Mandairon and Linster, 2009), it is

less clear how olfactory interneuron function could affect odour memory per se. Importantly, it is

unclear which brain regions (and their hierarchy) are critically involved in the olfactory

perception and olfactory conciousness functions necessary for establishing short and long-term

odour memories (Li et al., 2010; Wilson and Sullivan, 2011). Without a good understanding of

the circuit underlying odour memory it is challenging to interpret the impact of neurogenesis

ablation in certain behavioural tasks (see section 6.4.2).

In the case of our study, it is not clear how posttraining ablation of adult olfactory

interneurons impairs an associative odour memory. Our prediction is that the loss of these

interneurons alters the representation of that odour, losing the link between odour and reward

likely stored at higher order structures. Presently, however, we have no concrete evidence that

this is the case. Hopefully, further elucidation of the olfactory network as a whole will soon help

lay the groundwork for future adult neurogenesis queries.

It is possible that aOGCs are necessary for olfactory discrimination, and adult born

neurons for pattern separation across systems. Nevertheless, as tempting as general theories of

adult neurogenesis function might be, caution must always be taken not to overstate conclusions,

and not to fall too in love with a hypothesis. As with the hippocampus, multiple lines of

definitive evidence must emerge to establish the importance of aOGCs in olfactory pattern

separation, and especially to prove that this is not just a general function of OGCs. Importantly,

even if aOGCs (and aDGCs) turn out to be just the same as any developmentally generated cell,

their renewal in the adult brain is in itself unique, and their presence can still open different

therapeutic avenues.

Page 144: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

131

Chapter 7 Future Directions

In the studies described in this thesis we used a new transgenic approach to explore the

role of adult generated neurons in hippocampal and olfactory memory expression. This tag and

ablate strategy allowed, for the first time, to selectively ablate adult generated neurons after

memory encoding, and represents a more direct way of looking at adult born neuron contribution

to memory expression.

Nevertheless, although our current transgenic method represents a step forward in terms

of specificity and reduction of side effects, killing neurons, even through apoptosis, recruits and

activates phagocytic cells, whose response is still quite disruptive to the system. Ideally, neurons

could be reversibly silenced without inflammation. Two recent technological developments offer

promising new options for loss of function studies: optogenetics and designer receptor

exclusively activated by designer drug (DREADD) receptors.

Optogenetic technology combines genetic engeneering and light to manipulate neural

function in behaving organisms with exquisite spatial and temporal control. The first

demonstration of this was done in hippocampal neurons with the light-responsive channel

channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) (Boyden et al., 2005). ChR2 is a sensory photoreceptor derived

from the green algae Chlamydomonas reinhardtii (Nagel et al., 2003). ChR2 contains a

chromophore which, upon absorption of blue light, undergoes a conformational change that

causes the transmembrane channel to open, an influx of sodium and consequent neuronal

depolarization and generation of action potentials (Nagel et al., 2003; Boyden et al., 2005).

Since this demonstration of light-induced control of neuronal spiking with millisecond

precision, several optogenes were discovered and optimized, now comprising a vast toolbox

varying in wavelength, ion conductance and speed (Gradinaru et al., 2010; Fenno et al., 2011).

One such example is Archaerhodopsin-3 (Arch), an outward proton pump derived from

Halorubrum sodomense. Yellow light shone onto Arch expressing neurons activates proton

pumps, rapidly hyperpolarizing, or silencing, neurons with very high efficiency and a very short

recovery time (Chow et al., 2010). Importantly, these are completely reversible manipulations.

Page 145: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

132

Optogenetic manipulations, therefore, allow for gain and loss of function approaches, even

within the same neuronal population.

Currently there are two paths to targeting adult born neurons with optogenes: retroviral

vectors expressing the optogene of choice, or transgenic mice expressing inducible optogenes

that can be activated by CRE recombinase activity (driven by progenitor specific promoters).

Once the optogene is expressed, specific neuronal activation or inactivation is achieved through

light pulses delivered by surgically implanted optrodes (Aravanis et al., 2007). The strongest

advantages of an optogenetic approach are its temporal control, reversibility and the ability to

assess the function of a subset of neurons online, as a behavior is occurring, which renders it

potentially more disruptive. Still, this strategy is restricted by the spread of the light beam and

incurs in significan tissue damage from optrode implantation.

Unfortunately, these delivery methods still incur some of the same limitations as earlier

transgenic methods: retroviral delivery is restricted in terms of total number of cells infected, and

a nestin-CREERT2

-type transgenic approach, although targeting more cells, lacks the control over

the age of cells affected. Still, the application of optogenetic technology to the adult neurogenesis

field should yield very interesting results. In effect, we have recently had the opportunity to

collaborate with Dr. Shaoyu Ge‘s group in Stony Brook, NY, and do just that (the paper resulting

from that collaboration is in appendix 1).

Dr. Ge‘s group developed a protocol for high titer retrovirus production to induce Arch

expression specifically in aDGCs. After an initial eletrophysiological characterization of the

optogenetic manipulation of aDGCs we were able to silence these cells online as animals were

performing a behavioural probe. Similar to our results using the DTR system, we saw that

silencing of a small subset of aDGCs (around a thousand cells) after training led to memory

degradation in the MWM and CFC (Gu et al., unpublished data; appendix 1). Interestingly, this

effect was more pronounced when cells were 4 weeks of age (Gu et al., unpublished data;

appendix 1), consistent with the view that these immature cells are functionally distinct from

both developmentally generated and mature adult generated DGCs. Very recently, a paper by the

Lledo group applied this technology to the SVZ/OB system. They showed that optogenetic

activation of aOGCs (using channelrhodopsin-2) facilitated learning of a difficult (but not an

easy) olfactory associative memory task (Alonso et al., 2012). Interestingly, light-induced

Page 146: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

133

activation of these cells did not influence olfactory discrimination acuity (Alonso et al., 2012).

The conclusions in that study bear close resemblance to ours, and it will be exciting to see the

effects of a loss of function study using the same technology.

Another interesting new tool that could help in that effort came from Bryan Roth‘s group,

which generated a family of altered G-protein coupled receptors (GPCRs) designed to lose

responsivity to their original agonist and be activated exclusively by a pharmachologically inert

synthetic compound, clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) (Armbruster et al., 2007). This line of altered

muscarinic receptors is referred to as designer receptors activated by designer drug (DREADD),

their main advantages being the ability to silence or depolarize neurons in response to CNO in a

remote and non-invasive manner (Armbruster et al., 2007; Alexander et al., 2009), and, in double

transgenic mice with a tetracycline transactivator system (Mayford et al., 1996), reversibly

(Alexander et al., 2009).

Although lacking the extreme temporal precision of optogenetic approaches, this

technique allows for targeting brain areas inaccessible to optrodes (deeper areas of the brain),

with no tissue damage and not limited by light diffusion. Recently, this technology was coupled

to genetic tagging of c-fos expressing neurons (Reijimers et al., 2007) to specifically re-activate

tagged neurons that were engaged during encoding of a memory trace (Garner et al., 2012, see

discussion above).

Both these techniques could be applied to investigate key remaining questions in the

field. For instance, the question of whether aDGCs are special, or different from developmentally

generated DGCs. Differential silencing of adult vs. developmental populations could be achieved

through retroviral infection of optogenes activated by different wavelengths (one injected at P10

and another during adulthood). Alternatively, application of the technique used by Garner and

colleagues to adult born neurons (using nestin or DCX instead of CamKII as a promoter) could

be insightful in exploring functional integration of aDGCs.

Another important question is that of whether immature aOGCs (and aDGCs) are

involved in memory processing. Tagging and silencing through a retro-optogenetic approach

offers more control over the age of the targeted cells, and our experiments suggest that the small

Page 147: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

134

number of cells infected is nevertheless enough to impact memory. Still, before we have access

to that technology in house, we intent to complement our olfactory data with a few experiments.

The data shown in chapter 5 indicates that 3 week old olfactory interneurons play a role

in olfactory memory expression. With our two-step tag and ablate strategy we can also

manipulate the timing of the ablation, i.e. vary the age of the cells at the time of the ablation. The

morphological, eletrophysiological and chemical sequence of steps taken by adult born cells as

they mature have been extensively studied, and differences in plasticity levels during this process

have been hypothesized to underlie function. We intend to examine the impact of ablating cells

at different maturation stages in our associative memory task. Particularly, we are interested in

looking at 8 week old cells, a time point in which most of the heightened plasticity has been lost

(Nissant et al., 2009), and 10 days, an age in which the cells have arrived at the bulb but still lack

spines (Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla, 2002).

Page 148: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

135

References

Aakalu G, Smith WB, Nguyen N, Jiang C, Schuman EM (2001) Dynamic visualization of local

protein synthesis in hippocampal neurons. Neuron 30:489–502 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11395009.

Abel T, Nguyen PV, Barad M, Deuel T a, Kandel ER, Bourtchouladze R (1997) Genetic

demonstration of a role for PKA in the late phase of LTP and in hippocampus-based long-

term memory. Cell 88:615–626 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9054501.

Ables JL, Breunig JJ, Eisch AJ, Rakic P (2011) Not(ch) just development: Notch signalling in

the adult brain. Nature reviews Neuroscience 12:269–283 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3159580&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract

Ables JL, Decarolis N a, Johnson M a, Rivera PD, Gao Z, Cooper DC, Radtke F, Hsieh J, Eisch

AJ (2010) Notch1 is required for maintenance of the reservoir of adult hippocampal stem

cells. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience

30:10484–10492 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2935844&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract

Abraham NM, Egger V, Shimshek DR, Renden R, Fukunaga I, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH,

Klugmann M, Margrie TW, Schaefer AT, Kuner T (2010) Synaptic inhibition in the

olfactory bulb accelerates odor discrimination in mice. Neuron 65:399–411 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20159452

Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E (2006) A resilient, low-frequency,

small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical

hubs. The Journal of neuroscience 26:63–72 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16399673

Page 149: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

136

Acsády L, Kamondi A, Sík A, Freund T, Buzsáki G (1998) GABAergic cells are the major

postsynaptic targets of mossy fibers in the rat hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience :

the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 18:3386–3403 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9547246.

Acsády L, Káli S (2007) Models, structure, function: the transformation of cortical signals in the

dentate gyrus. Progress in brain research 163:577–599 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17765739

Agranoff BW, Davis RE, Brink JJ (1966) Chemical studies on memory fixation in goldfish Brain

Research 1:303–309.

Aimone J, Deng W, Gage F (2011) Resolving new memories: a critical look at the dentate gyrus,

adult neurogenesis, and pattern separation. Neuron 70:589–596 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21609818

Aimone J, Wiles J, Gage F (2006) Potential role for adult neurogenesis in the encoding of time in

new memories. Nature neuroscience 9:723–727 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16732202

Aimone J, Wiles J, Gage F (2009) Computational influence of adult neurogenesis on memory

encoding. Neuron 61:187–202 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2670434&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 7, 2012].

Akers KG, Kushner S a, Leslie AT, Clarke L, van der Kooy D, Lerch JP, Frankland PW (2011)

Fetal alcohol exposure leads to abnormal olfactory bulb development and impaired odor

discrimination in adult mice. Molecular brain 4:29 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3148973&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Alexander GM, Rogan SC, Abbas AI, Armbruster BN, Pei Y, Allen J a, Nonneman RJ,

Hartmann J, Moy SS, Nicolelis M a, McNamara JO, Roth BL (2009) Remote control of

neuronal activity in transgenic mice expressing evolved G protein-coupled receptors.

Neuron 63:27–39 Available at:

Page 150: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

137

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2751885&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 25, 2012].

Allen E (1912) The cessation of mitosis in the central nervous system of the albino rat. J Comp

Neurol 22:547–568.

Alonso M, Lepousez G, Wagner S, Bardy C, Gabellec M-M, Torquet N, Lledo P-M (2012)

Activation of adult-born neurons facilitates learning and memory. Nature Neuroscience 15

Available at: http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nn.3108 [Accessed May 14, 2012].

Alonso M, Viollet C, Gabellec M-M, Meas-Yedid V, Olivo-Marin J-C, Lledo P-M (2006)

Olfactory discrimination learning increases the survival of adult-born neurons in the

olfactory bulb. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 26:10508–10513 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17035535 [Accessed March 29, 2012].

Altman J (1962) Are New Neurons Formed in the Brains of AduIt Mammals? Science

135:1127–1128.

Altman J (1969) Autoradiographic and Histological Studies o f Postnatal Neurogenesis. J Comp

Neurol 137:433–458.

Altman J, Bayer S (1990a) Mosaic organization of the hippocampal neuroepithelium and the

multiple germinal sources of dentate granule cells. The Journal of comparative neurology

301:325–342 Available at:

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cne.903010302/abstract

Altman J, Bayer S (1990b) Migration and distribution of two populations of hippocampal

granule cell precursors during the perinatal and postnatal periods. The Journal of

comparative neurology 301:365–381 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2262596.

Altman J, Das GD (1965) Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippocampal

neurogenesis in rats. The Journal of comparative neurology 124:319–335 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5861717.

Page 151: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

138

Alvarez-Buylla a, Theelen M, Nottebohm F (1988) Birth of projection neurons in the higher

vocal center of the canary forebrain before, during, and after song learning. Proceedings of

the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 85:8722–8726 Available

at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=282533&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract.

Alvarez-Buylla A, Lim D a (2004) For the long run: maintaining germinal niches in the adult

brain. Neuron 41:683–686 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15003168.

Alvarez-buylla A, Nottebohm F (1988) Migration of young neurons in adult avian brain. Nature

335:353–354.

Amaral D, Dent J (1981) Development of the mossy fibers of the dentate gyrus: I. A light and

electron microscopic study of the mossy fibers and their expansions. Journal of comparative

neurology 195:51–86.

Amaral D, Ishizuka N, Claiborne B (1990) Neurons, numbers and the hippocampal network.

Progress in brain research 83:1–11 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2203093

Amaral D, Kurz J (1985) An analysis of the origins of the cholinergic and noncholinergic septal

projections to the hippocampal formation of the rat. The Journal of comparative neurology

240:37–59.

Amaral D, Scharfman H, Lavenex P (2007) The dentate gyrus: fundamental neuroanatomical

organization (dentate gyrus for dummies). Progress in brain research 163:3–22 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2492885&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract

Amaral L a, Scala a, Barthelemy M, Stanley HE (2000) Classes of small-world networks.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

97:11149–11152 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=17168&tool=pmcentrez&render

type=abstract.

Page 152: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

139

Ambrogini P, Cuppini R, Cuppini C, Ciaroni S, Cecchini T, Ferri P, Sartini S, Del Grande P

(2000) Spatial learning affects immature granule cell survival in adult rat dentate gyrus.

Neuroscience letters 286:21–24 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10822143.

Amiri A, Cho W, Zhou J, Birnbaum SG, Sinton CM, McKay RM, Parada LF (2012) Pten

Deletion in Adult Hippocampal Neural Stem/Progenitor Cells Causes Cellular

Abnormalities and Alters Neurogenesis. Journal of Neuroscience 32:5880–5890 Available

at: http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5462-11.2012

Amoureux MC, Cunningham B a, Edelman GM, Crossin KL (2000) N-CAM binding inhibits the

proliferation of hippocampal progenitor cells and promotes their differentiation to a

neuronal phenotype. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 20:3631–3640 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10804205.

Anagnostaras SG, Maren S, Fanselow MS (1999) Temporally graded retrograde amnesia of

contextual fear after hippocampal damage in rats: within-subjects examination. The Journal

of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 19:1106–1114

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9920672.

Andersen P, Bliss T, Lømo T, Olsen LI, Skrede KK (1971) Lamellar organization of

hippocampal excitatory pathways. Acta physiologica Scandinavica 76:4A–5A Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5823402.

Anton ES, Ghashghaei HT, Weber JL, McCann C, Fischer TM, Cheung ID, Gassmann M,

Messing A, Klein R, Schwab MH, Lloyd KCK, Lai C (2004) Receptor tyrosine kinase

ErbB4 modulates neuroblast migration and placement in the adult forebrain. Nature

neuroscience 7:1319–1328 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15543145

Aravanis AM, Wang L-P, Zhang F, Meltzer L a, Mogri MZ, Schneider MB, Deisseroth K (2007)

An optical neural interface: in vivo control of rodent motor cortex with integrated fiberoptic

and optogenetic technology. Journal of neural engineering 4:S143–56 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17873414

Arjun Sahgal ed. (1983) Behavioural neuroscience : a practical approach. Oxford.

Page 153: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

140

Armbruster BN, Li X, Pausch MH, Herlitze S, Roth BL (2007) Evolving the lock to fit the key to

create a family of G protein-coupled receptors potently activated by an inert ligand.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

104:5163–5168 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1829280&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Arruda-Carvalho M, Sakaguchi M, Akers KG, Josselyn S a, Frankland PW (2011) Posttraining

ablation of adult-generated neurons degrades previously acquired memories. The Journal of

neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 31:15113–15127

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22016545 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Atkinson R, Shiffrin M (1971) The Control of Short-Term Memory. Scientific American:82–91.

Baddeley A, Warrington E (1970) Amnesia and the distinction between long-and short-term

memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 9:176–189.

Baimbridge K, Celio M, Rogers J (1992) Calcium-binding proteins in the nervous system.

Trends in Neurosciences 15:303–308.

Baimbridge KG, Miller JJ (1982) Immunohistochemical localization of calcium-binding protein

in the cerebellum, hippocampal formation and olfactory bulb of the rat. Brain research

245:223–229 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6751467.

Baker S a, Baker KA, Hagg T (2004) Dopaminergic nigrostriatal projections regulate neural

precursor proliferation in the adult mouse subventricular zone. The European journal of

neuroscience 20:575–579 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15233767

Balu DT, Lucki I (2009) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis: regulation, functional implications,

and contribution to disease pathology. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 33:232–252

Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2671071&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract

Page 154: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

141

Banasr M, Hery M, Printemps R, Daszuta A (2004) Serotonin-induced increases in adult cell

proliferation and neurogenesis are mediated through different and common 5-HT receptor

subtypes in the dentate gyrus and the subventricular zone. Neuropsychopharmacology :

official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 29:450–460

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14872203

Barabási A, Albert R (1999) Emergence of Scaling in Random Networks. Science 286:509–512

Available at: http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/doi/10.1126/science.286.5439.509

Barnes C, McNaughton B, Mizumori S, Leonard B, Lin L (1990) Comparison of spatial and

temporal characteristics of neuronal activity in sequential stages of hippocampal processing.

Prog Brain Res 83:287–300 Available at:

http://www.sciencedirect.com.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/science/article/pii/S0079612308

612571

Barnes DC, Hofacer RD, Zaman AR, Rennaker RL, Wilson D a (2008) Olfactory perceptual

stability and discrimination. Nature neuroscience 11:1378–1380 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2682180&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract

Barondes SH, Jarvik ME (1964) the Influence of Actinomycin-D on Brain Rna Synthesis and on

Memory. Journal of neurochemistry 11:187–195 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14166283.

Bath K, Mandairon N, Jing D, Rajagopal R, Kapoor R, Chen Z, Khan T, Proenca C, Kraemer R,

Cleland T, Hempstead B, Chao M, Lee F (2008) Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor

(Val66Met) alters adult olfactory bulb neurogenesis and spontaneous olfactory

discrimination. The Journal of neuroscience 28:2383–2393 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2679965&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract

Bathellier B, Lagier S, Faure P, Lledo P-M (2006) Circuit properties generating gamma

oscillations in a network model of the olfactory bulb. Journal of neurophysiology 95:2678–

2691 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16381804

Page 155: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

142

Bayer S (1980) Development of the hippocampal region in the rat. II. Morphogenesis during

embryonic and early postnatal life. The Journal of comparative neurology 190:115–134

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7381049.

Bayer S (1983) 3H-Thymidine-radiographic Studies of Neurogenesis in the Rat Olfactory Bulb.

Experimental Brain Research 50:329–340.

Becker S, Wojtowicz JM (2007) A model of hippocampal neurogenesis in memory and mood

disorders. Trends in cognitive sciences 11:70–76 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17174137 [Accessed March 6, 2012].

Belluzzi O, Benedusi M, Ackman J, LoTurco JJ (2003) Electrophysiological differentiation of

new neurons in the olfactory bulb. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the

Society for Neuroscience 23:10411–10418 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14614100.

Belnoue L, Grosjean N, Abrous D, Koehl M (2011) A critical time window for the recruitment

of bulbar newborn neurons by olfactory discrimination learning. The Journal of

neuroscience 31:1010–1016 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21248125

Belvindrah R, Rougon G, Chazal G (2002) Increased neurogenesis in adult mCD24-deficient

mice. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience

22:3594–3607 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11978835.

Ben Ari Y, Cherubini E, Corradetti R, Gaiarsa JL (1989) Giant synaptic potentials in immature

rat CA3 hippocampal neurones. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 416, 303–325.

Ben-Ari Y (2002) Excitatory actions of gaba during development: the nature of the nurture.

Nature reviews Neuroscience 3:728–739 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12209121 [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Bentivoglio M, Swanson LW (2001) On the fine structure of the pes Hippocampi major ( with

plates XIII – XXIII ) ( From : On the Fine Structure of the Central Organs of the Nervous

System ) by Camillo Golgi. Brain Research 54:461–483.

Page 156: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

143

Benzing W, Squire L (1989) Preserved learning and memory in amnesia: intact adaptation-level

effects and learning of stereoscopic depth. Behavioral neuroscience 103:538–547 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2736068 [Accessed March 11, 2012].

Bergersen L, Ruiz A, Bjaalie JG, Kullmann DM, Gundersen V (2003) GABA and GABAA

receptors at hippocampal mossy fibre synapses. European Journal of Neuroscience 18:931–

941 Available at: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02828.x [Accessed July 6,

2011].

Beshel J, Kopell N, Kay LM (2007) Olfactory bulb gamma oscillations are enhanced with task

demands. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience

27:8358–8365 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17670982 [Accessed

March 14, 2012].

Biebl M, Cooper C, Winkler J, Kuhn H (2000) Analysis of neurogenesis and programmed cell

death reveals a self-renewing capacity in the adult rat brain. Neuroscience letters 291:17–20

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10962143.

Biedenkapp JC, Rudy JW (2007) Context preexposure prevents forgetting of a contextual fear

memory: implication for regional changes in brain activation patterns associated with recent

and remote memory tests. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, NY) 14:200–203

Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2519802&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 18, 2012].

Biron KK (2006) Antiviral drugs for cytomegalovirus diseases. Antiviral research 71:154–163

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16765457 [Accessed March 22, 2012].

Bizon JL, Lee HJ, Gallagher M (2004) Neurogenesis in a rat model of age-related cognitive

decline. Aging cell 3:227–234 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15268756 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Blackstad T, Kjaerheim A (1961) Special axo-dendritic synapses in the hippocampal cortex:

Electron and light microscopic studies on the layer of mossy fibers. The Journal of

comparative neurology 117:133–159 Available at:

Page 157: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

144

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cne.901170202/abstract [Accessed January 24,

2012].

Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC (1972) Effects of hippocampal lesions on the rat‘s reaction to a cat.

Journal of comparative and physiological psychology 78:77–82 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5027344.

Blatow M, Caputi A, Burnashev N, Monyer H, Rozov A (2003) Ca2+ buffer saturation underlies

paired pulse facilitation in calbindin-D28k-containing terminals. Neuron 38:79–88

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12691666.

Bliss T, Lømo T (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of

the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. The Journal of

physiology 232:331 Available at: http://jp.physoc.org/content/232/2/331.short [Accessed

January 26, 2012].

Blitzer RD, Connor JH, Brown GP, Wong T, Shenolikar S, Iyengar R, Landau EM (1998)

Gating of CaMKII by cAMP-regulated protein phosphatase activity during LTP. Science

(New York, NY) 280:1940–1942 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9632393.

Block F (1999) Global ischemia and behavioural deficits. Progress in neurobiology 58:279–295

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10341364.

Boccaletti S, Latora V, Moreno Y, Chavez M, Hwang D (2006) Complex networks: Structure

and dynamics. Physics Reports 424:175–308 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S037015730500462X [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Bolles R (1970) Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological

Review 77:32–48.

Bonaguidi M a, Wheeler M a, Shapiro JS, Stadel RP, Sun GJ, Ming G, Song H (2011) In vivo

clonal analysis reveals self-renewing and multipotent adult neural stem cell characteristics.

Cell 145:1142–1155 Available at:

Page 158: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

145

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3124562&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Bonfanti L, Theodosis DT (1994) Expression of polysialylated neural cell adhesion molecule by

proliferating cells in the subependymal layer of the adult rat, in its rostral extension and in

the olfactory bulb. Neuroscience 62:291–305 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7816207.

Bonifazi P, Goldin M, Picardo M a, Jorquera I, Cattani a, Bianconi G, Represa a, Ben-Ari Y,

Cossart R (2009) GABAergic hub neurons orchestrate synchrony in developing

hippocampal networks. Science (New York, NY) 326:1419–1424 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19965761 [Accessed June 12, 2011].

Bourtchuladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva A (1994) Deficient long-term

memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein.

Cell 79:59–68 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7923378.

Bovetti S, Veyrac A, Peretto P, Fasolo A, De Marchis S (2009) Olfactory enrichment influences

adult neurogenesis modulating GAD67 and plasticity-related molecules expression in

newborn cells of the olfactory bulb. PloS one 4:e6359 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2709916&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 28, 2012].

Boyden ES, Zhang F, Bamberg E, Nagel G, Deisseroth K (2005) Millisecond-timescale,

genetically targeted optical control of neural activity. Nature neuroscience 8:1263–1268

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16116447 [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Bragin a, Jandó G, Nádasdy Z, van Landeghem M, Buzsáki G (1995a) Dentate EEG spikes and

associated interneuronal population bursts in the hippocampal hilar region of the rat. Journal

of neurophysiology 73:1691–1705 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7643175.

Bragin A, Jandó G, Nádasdy Z, Hetke J, Wise K, Buzsáki G (1995b) Gamma (40-100 Hz)

oscillation in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. Journal of Neuroscience 15.

Page 159: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

146

Brandt M, Jessberger S, Steiner B, Kronenberg G, Reuter K, Bick-Sander A, Behrens W,

Kempermann G (2003) Transient calretinin expression defines early postmitotic step of

neuronal differentiation in adult hippocampal neurogenesis of mice. Molecular and Cellular

Neuroscience 24:603–613 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1044743103002070 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Breton-Provencher V, Lemasson M, Peralta MR, Saghatelyan A (2009) Interneurons produced in

adulthood are required for the normal functioning of the olfactory bulb network and for the

execution of selected olfactory behaviors. The Journal of neuroscience 29:15245–15257

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19955377 [Accessed March 26, 2012].

Breunig JJ, Silbereis J, Vaccarino FM, Sestan N, Rakic P (2007) Notch regulates cell fate and

dendrite morphology of newborn neurons in the postnatal dentate gyrus. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104:20558–20563 Available

at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2154470&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Brezun J, Daszuta A (1999) DEPLETION IN SEROTONIN DECREASES NEUROGENESIS

IN THE DENTATE GYRUS AND THE SUBVENTRICULAR ZONE OF ADULT RATS.

Neuroscience 89:999–1002.

Brill MS, Ninkovic J, Winpenny E, Hodge RD, Ozen I, Yang R, Lepier A, Gascón S, Erdelyi F,

Szabo G, Parras C, Guillemot F, Frotscher M, Berninger B, Hevner RF, Raineteau O, Götz

M (2009) Adult generation of glutamatergic olfactory bulb interneurons. Nature

neuroscience 12:1524–1533 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2787799&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Broadbent D (1958) Perception and communication . Pergamon Press. Available at:

http://books.google.ca/books/about/Perception_and_communication.html?id=GwtKAAAA

MAAJ&redir_esc=y [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Page 160: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

147

Brodal A (1947) The hippocampus and the sense of smell; a review. Brain : a journal of

neurology 70:179–222 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20261820.

Brodal A (1981) Neurological Anatomy in Relation to Clinical Medicine (Oxford Medicine

Publications). Oxford University Press, USA. Available at:

http://www.amazon.com/Neurological-Relation-Clinical-Medicine-

Publications/dp/0195026942 [Accessed January 28, 2012].

Brooks D, Baddeley AD (1976) What can amnesic patients learn? Neuropsychologia 14:111–122

Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0028393276900129

[Accessed March 11, 2012].

Brown J, Couillard-Després S, Cooper-Kuhn C, Winkler J, Aigner L, Kuhn H (2003) Transient

expression of doublecortin during adult neurogenesis. The Journal of comparative

neurology 467:1–10 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14574675

[Accessed March 8, 2012].

Bruel-Jungerman E, Davis S, Rampon C, Laroche S (2006) Long-term potentiation enhances

neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of

the Society for Neuroscience 26:5888–5893 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16738230 [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Bruel-Jungerman E, Laroche S, Rampon C (2005) New neurons in the dentate gyrus are involved

in the expression of enhanced long-term memory following environmental enrichment. The

European journal of neuroscience 21:513–521 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15673450 [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Brun V, Otnæss M, Molden S, Steffenach H, Menno P, Moser M, Moser E (2002) Place Cells

and Place Recognition Maintained by Direct Entorhinal-Hippocampal Circuitry.

Sciencecience 296:2243–2246.

Buch T, Heppner FL, Tertilt C, Heinen TJAJ, Kremer M, Wunderlich FT, Jung S, Waisman A

(2005) A Cre-inducible diphtheria toxin receptor mediates cell lineage ablation after toxin

administration. Nature Methods 2:419–426.

Page 161: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

148

Burghardt N, Park E, Hen R, Fenton A (2012) Adult-born hippocampal neurons promote

cognitive flexibility in mice. Hippocampus 000 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22431384 [Accessed March 21, 2012].

Burk K, Desoeuvre a., Boutin C, Smith M a., Kroger S, Bosio a., Tiveron M-C, Cremer H (2012)

Agrin-Signaling Is Necessary for the Integration of Newly Generated Neurons in the Adult

Olfactory Bulb. Journal of Neuroscience 32:3759–3764 Available at:

http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4906-11.2012 [Accessed March 14,

2012].

Cai J, Wu Y, Mirua T, Pierce JL, Lucero MT, Albertine KH, Spangrude GJ, Rao MS (2002)

Properties of a Fetal Multipotent Neural Stem Cell ( NEP Cell ). Developmental Biology

240:221–240.

Cameron H, McKay R (2001) Adult neurogenesis produces a large pool of new granule cells in

the dentate gyrus. The Journal of comparative neurology 435:406–417 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11406822.

Cameron H, Woolley C, McEwen B, Gould E (1993) DIFFERENTIATION OF NEWLY BORN

NEURONS AND GLIA IN THE DEntATE GYRUS OF THE ADULT RAT. Neuroscience

56:337–344.

Carleton A, Petreanu LT, Lansford R, Alvarez-Buylla A, Lledo P-M (2003) Becoming a new

neuron in the adult olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience 6:507–518 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12704391 [Accessed July 16, 2011].

Cavallaro M, Mariani J, Lancini C, Latorre E, Caccia R, Gullo F, Valotta M, DeBiasi S, Spinardi

L, Ronchi A, Wanke E, Brunelli S, Favaro R, Ottolenghi S, Nicolis SK (2008) Impaired

generation of mature neurons by neural stem cells from hypomorphic Sox2 mutants.

Development (Cambridge, England) 135:541–557 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18171687 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Cavanagh BL, Walker T, Norazit A, Meedeniya ACB (2011) Thymidine analogues for tracking

DNA synthesis. Molecules 16:7980–7993 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21921870 [Accessed May 21, 2012].

Page 162: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

149

Cave CB, Squire LR (1992) Intact verbal and nonverbal short-term memory following damage to

the human hippocampus. Hippocampus 2:151–163 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1308180.

Chen G-H, Wang Y-J, Zhang L-Q, Zhou J-N (2004) Age- and sex-related disturbance in a

battery of sensorimotor and cognitive tasks in Kunming mice. Physiology & behavior

83:531–541 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15581676 [Accessed April

24, 2012].

Chicurel ME, Harris KM (1992) Three-dimensional analysis of the structure and composition of

CA3 branched dendritic spines and their synaptic relationships with mossy fiber boutons in

the rat hippocampus. The Journal of comparative neurology 325:169–182 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1460112 [Accessed November 26, 2011].

Chow BY, Han X, Dobry AS, Qian X, Chuong AS, Li M, Henninger M a, Belfort GM, Lin Y,

Monahan PE, Boyden ES (2010) High-performance genetically targetable optical neural

silencing by light-driven proton pumps. Nature 463:98–102 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2939492&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed June 22, 2011].

Chun SK, Sun W, Park J-J, Jung MW (2006) Enhanced proliferation of progenitor cells

following long-term potentiation induction in the rat dentate gyrus. Neurobiology of

learning and memory 86:322–329 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16824772 [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Cipolotti L, Shallice T, Chan D, Fox N, Scahill R, Harrison G, Stevens J, Rudge P (2001) Long-

term retrograde amnesia...the crucial role of the hippocampus. Neuropsychologia 39:151–

172 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11163373.

Claiborne B, Amaral D, Cowan W (1990) Quantitative, three-dimensional analysis of granule

cell dendrites in the rat dentate gyrus. The Journal of comparative neurology 302:206–219

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2289972.

Clark RE, Broadbent NJ, Squire LR (2005) Hippocampus and remote spatial memory in rats.

Hippocampus 15:260–272 Available at:

Page 163: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

150

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2754168&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed August 1, 2011].

Clark RE, Broadbent NJ, Squire LR (2007) The hippocampus and spatial memory: findings with

a novel modification of the water maze. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of

the Society for Neuroscience 27:6647–6654 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2553679&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Clayton NS, Krebs JR (1995) Memory in food-storing birds: from behaviour to brain. Current

opinion in neurobiology 5:149–154 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7620301.

Cleland TA, Johnson BA, Leon M, Linster C (2007) Relational representation in the olfactory

system. Sciences-New York 104.

Clelland C, Choi M, Romberg C, Clemenson G, Fragniere A, Tyers P, Jessberger S, Saksida L,

Barker R, Gage F, Bussey T (2009) A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in

spatial pattern separation. Science 325:210–213 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2997634&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 6, 2012].

Cohen N, Squire L (1980) Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia:

Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210:207 Available at:

http://www.sciencemag.org/content/210/4466/207.short [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Conover JC, Doetsch F, Garcia-Verdugo JM, Gale NW, Yancopoulos GD, Alvarez-Buylla a

(2000) Disruption of Eph/ephrin signaling affects migration and proliferation in the adult

subventricular zone. Nature neuroscience 3:1091–1097 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11036265.

Conrad LC, Leonard CM, Pfaff DW (1974) Connections of the median and dorsal raphe nuclei

in the rat: an autoradiographic and degeneration study. The Journal of comparative

neurology 156:179–205 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4419253.

Page 164: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

151

Cooperkuhn C, Vroemen M, Brown J, Ye H, Thompson M, Winkler J, Kuhn H (2002) Impaired

Adult Neurogenesis in Mice Lacking the Transcription Factor E2F1. Molecular and Cellular

Neuroscience 21:312–323 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1044743102911760 [Accessed April 5, 2012].

Corkin S (1968) ACQUISITION OF MOTOR SKILL AFTER BILATERAL MEDIAL

TEMPORAL-LOBE EXCISION. Neuropsychologia 6:255–265.

Corotto FS, Henegar JR, Maruniak J a (1994) Odor deprivation leads to reduced neurogenesis

and reduced neuronal survival in the olfactory bulb of the adult mouse. Neuroscience

61:739–744 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7838373.

Correll R, Scoville W (1967) Significance of delay in the performance of monkeys with medial

temporal lobe resections. Experimental brain research Experimentelle Hirnforschung

Expérimentation cérébrale 4:85–96 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4970750.

Corvelo A, Eyras E (2008) Exon creation and establishment in human genes. Genome biology

9:R141 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2592719&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Crawford I, Connor J (1973) Localization and release of glutamic acid in relation to the

hippocampal mossy fibre pathway. Nature 244:442–443 Available at:

http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v244/n5416/abs/244442a0.html [Accessed January

23, 2012].

Crespo D, Stanfield B, Cowan W (1986) Evidence that late-generated granule cells do not simply

replace earlier formed neurons in the rat dentate gyrus. Experimental Brain Research

62:541–548.

Cutler a R, Wilkerson a E, Gingras JL, Levin ED (1996) Prenatal cocaine and/or nicotine

exposure in rats: preliminary findings on long-term cognitive outcome and genital

development at birth. Neurotoxicology and teratology 18:635–643 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8947940.

Page 165: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

152

Czéh B, Welt T, Fischer AK, Erhardt A, Schmitt W, Müller MB, Toschi N, Fuchs E, Keck ME

(2002) Chronic psychosocial stress and concomitant repetitive transcranial magnetic

stimulation: effects on stress hormone levels and adult hippocampal neurogenesis.

Biological psychiatry 52:1057–1065 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12460689.

Daum I, Channon S, Canavan AG (1989) Classical conditioning in patients with severe memory

problems. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 52:47–51 Available at:

http://jnnp.bmj.com/content/52/1/47.short [Accessed March 11, 2012].

Davidson TL, Jarrard LE (2004) The hippocampus and inhibitory learning: a ―Gray‖ area?

Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 28:261–271 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15225970 [Accessed August 18, 2011].

Davis HP, Squire LR (1984) Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychological bulletin

96:518–559 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6096908.

Davis M (1992) The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual review of neuroscience

15:353–375 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22100273.

Dayer AG, Ford A a, Cleaver KM, Yassaee M, Cameron H a (2003) Short-term and long-term

survival of new neurons in the rat dentate gyrus. The Journal of comparative neurology

460:563–572 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12717714 [Accessed

March 8, 2012].

Delpire E (2000) Cation-Chloride Cotransporters in Neuronal Communication. News in

physiological sciences : an international journal of physiology produced jointly by the

International Union of Physiological Sciences and the American Physiological Society

15:309–312 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11390932.

Deng W, Aimone J, Gage F (2010) New neurons and new memories: how does adult

hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nature reviews Neuroscience

11:339–350 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2886712&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Page 166: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

153

Deng W, Saxe M, Gallina I, Gage F (2009) Adult-born hippocampal dentate granule cells

undergoing maturation modulate learning and memory in the brain. The Journal of

neuroscience 29:13532–13542 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2787190&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Denny C, Burghardt N, Schachter D, Hen R, Drew M (2011) 4- To 6-Week-Old Adult-Born

Hippocampal Neurons Influence Novelty-Evoked Exploration and Contextual Fear

Conditioning. Hippocampus 000 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3193906&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 15, 2012].

Desmond NL, Levy WB (1985) Granule cell dendritic spine density in the rat hippocampus

varies with spine shape and location. Neuroscience letters 54:219–224 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3991060 [Accessed January 25, 2012].

Ding H, Teixeira C, Frankland P (2008) Inactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex blocks

expression of remote, but not recent, conditioned taste aversion memory. Learning &

memory (Cold Spring Harbor, NY) 15:290–293 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18441286 [Accessed March 5, 2012].

Doetsch F, Alvarez-Buylla a (1996) Network of tangential pathways for neuronal migration in

adult mammalian brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United

States of America 93:14895–14900 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=26233&tool=pmcentrez&render

type=abstract.

Doetsch F, Caillé I, Lim D a, García-Verdugo JM, Alvarez-Buylla a (1999) Subventricular zone

astrocytes are neural stem cells in the adult mammalian brain. Cell 97:703–716 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10380923.

Doetsch F, García-Verdugo JM, Alvarez-Buylla A (1997) Cellular composition and three-

dimensional organization of the subventricular germinal zone in the adult mammalian brain.

Page 167: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

154

The Journal of neuroscience 17:5046–5061 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9185542.

Doetsch F, Petreanu L, Caille I, Garcia-Verdugo JM, Alvarez-Buylla A (2002) EGF converts

transit-amplifying neurogenic precursors in the adult brain into multipotent stem cells.

Neuron 36:1021–1034 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12495619.

Dorland B, Middlebrook L (1979) Receptor-mediated Internalization and Degradation of

Diphtheria Toxin by Monkey Kidney Cells. 254.

Drachman D, Arbit J (1966) Memory and the hippocampal complex: II. Is memory a multiple

process? Archives of Neurology 15:52 Available at: http://archneur.ama-

assn.org/cgi/content/summary/10/4/411 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Dranovsky A, Picchini A, Moadel T, Sisti A, Yamada A, Kimura S, Leonardo E, Hen R (2011)

Experience dictates stem cell fate in the adult hippocampus. Neuron 70:908–923 Available

at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3124009&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 15, 2012].

Drapeau E, Mayo W, Aurousseau C, Le Moal M, Piazza P, Abrous D (2003) Spatial memory

performances of aged rats in the water maze predict levels of hippocampal neurogenesis.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

100:14385–14390 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=283601&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract.

Drew M, Denny C, Hen R (2010) Arrest of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice impairs

single- but not multiple-trial contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 124:446–454.

Driscoll I, Howard SR, Stone JC, Monfils MH, Tomanek B, Brooks WM, Sutherland RJ (2006)

The aging hippocampus: a multi-level analysis in the rat. Neuroscience 139:1173–1185

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16564634 [Accessed March 27, 2012].

Page 168: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

155

Duncan C (1949) The retroactive effect of electroshock on learning. Journal of Comparative and

Physiological Psychology 42:32–44 Available at:

http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/com/42/1/32/ [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Dupret D, Fabre A, Döbrössy M, Panatier A, Rodríguez J, Lamarque S, Lemaire V, Oliet S,

Piazza P, Abrous D (2007) Spatial learning depends on both the addition and removal of

new hippocampal neurons. PLoS biology 5:e214 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1939885&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Dupret D, Montaron M-F, Drapeau E, Aurousseau C, Le Moal M, Piazza P-V, Abrous DN

(2005) Methylazoxymethanol acetate does not fully block cell genesis in the young and

aged dentate gyrus. The European journal of neuroscience 22:778–783 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16101760 [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Dupret D, Revest J, Koehl M, Ichas F, De Giorgi F, Costet P, Abrous D, Piazza P (2008) Spatial

relational memory requires hippocampal adult neurogenesis. PloS one 3:e1959 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2396793&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 18, 2012].

Döbrössy M, Drapeau E, Aurousseau C, Le Moal M, Piazza P, Abrous D (2003) Differential

effects of learning on neurogenesis: learning increases or decreases the number of newly

born cells depending on their birth date. Molecular psychiatry 8:974–982 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14647395 [Accessed March 22, 2012].

D‘Hooge R, De Deyn PP (2001) Applications of the Morris water maze in the study of learning

and memory. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11516773.

D‘Hooge R, Nagels G, Franck F, Bakker C, Reyniers E, Storm K, Kooy R, Oostra B, Willems P,

De Deyn P (1997) Mildly Impaired Water Maze Performance in Male Fmr1 Knockout

Mice. Neuroscience 76:367–376.

Easter SS, Ross LS, Frankfurter A (1993) Initial tract formation in the mouse brain. The Journal

of neuroscience 13:285–299 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8423474.

Page 169: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

156

Ebbinghaus H (1913) Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. Teachers college,

Columbia university. Available at:

http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&btnG=Search&q=intitle:MEMORY+A+CONTRI

BUTION+TO+EXPERIMENTAL+PSYCHOLOGY#0 [Accessed March 4, 2012].

Egea J, Klein R (2007) Bidirectional Eph-ephrin signaling during axon guidance. Trends in cell

biology 17:230–238 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17420126

[Accessed March 1, 2012].

Eichenbaum H (2002) The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory: An Introduction. New York:

Oxford University Press.

Eichenbaum H, Dudchenko P, Wood E, Shapiro M, Tanila H (1999) The hippocampus, memory,

and place cells: is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron 23:209–226 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10399928.

Eidels L, Proia RL, Hart D a (1983) Membrane receptors for bacterial toxins. Microbiological

reviews 47:596–620 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=283710&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract.

Emerich DF, Walsh TJ (1989) Selective working memory impairments following intradentate

injection of colchicine: attenuation of the behavioral but not the neuropathological effects

by gangliosides GM1 and AGF2. Physiology & behavior 45:93–101 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2727146.

Emsley J (2003a) Α6Β1 Integrin Directs Migration of Neuronal Precursors in Adult Mouse

Forebrain. Experimental Neurology 183:273–285 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0014488603002097 [Accessed March 29,

2012].

Emsley J (2003b) Endogenous and exogenous ciliary neurotrophic factor enhances forebrain

neurogenesis in adult mice. Experimental Neurology 183:298–310 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0014488603001298 [Accessed April 8, 2012].

Page 170: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

157

Enwere E, Shingo T, Gregg C, Fujikawa H, Ohta S, Weiss S (2004) Aging results in reduced

epidermal growth factor receptor signaling, diminished olfactory neurogenesis, and deficits

in fine olfactory discrimination. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the

Society for Neuroscience 24:8354–8365 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15385618 [Accessed March 18, 2012].

Epp JR, Spritzer MD, Galea L a M (2007) Hippocampus-dependent learning promotes survival

of new neurons in the dentate gyrus at a specific time during cell maturation. Neuroscience

149:273–285 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17900815 [Accessed

March 12, 2012].

Eriksson PS, Perfilieva E, Björk-Eriksson T, Alborn a M, Nordborg C, Peterson D a, Gage FH

(1998) Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nature medicine 4:1313–1317

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19714567.

Espósito MS, Piatti VC, Laplagne D a, Morgenstern N a, Ferrari CC, Pitossi FJ, Schinder AF

(2005) Neuronal differentiation in the adult hippocampus recapitulates embryonic

development. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 25:10074–10086 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16267214 [Accessed March 20, 2012].

Fanselow M (1980) Conditioned and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. The

Pavlovian journal of biological science 15:177–182 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7208128.

Fanselow M (1982) The postshock activity burst. Animal Learning &Behavior 10:448–454.

Fanselow M (1990) Factors governing one-trial contextual conditioning. Animal Learning and

Behavior 18:264–270.

Fanselow M (2000a) Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus. Behavioural brain

research 110:73–81.

Fanselow M (2000b) Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus. Behavioural Brain

Research 110:73–81 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10802305.

Page 171: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

158

Farioli-Vecchioli S, Saraulli D, Costanzi M, Leonardi L, Cinà I, Micheli L, Nutini M, Longone

P, Oh SP, Cestari V, Tirone F (2009) Impaired terminal differentiation of hippocampal

granule neurons and defective contextual memory in PC3/Tis21 knockout mice. PloS one

4:e8339 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2791842&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed July 19, 2011].

Farioli-Vecchioli S, Saraulli D, Costanzi M, Pacioni S, Cinà I, Aceti M, Micheli L, Bacci A,

Cestari V, Tirone F (2008) The timing of differentiation of adult hippocampal neurons is

crucial for spatial memory. PLoS biology 6:e246 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2561078&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Feierstein CE, Lazarini F, Wagner S, Gabellec M-M, de Chaumont F, Olivo-Marin J-C, Boussin

FD, Lledo P-M, Gheusi G (2010) Disruption of Adult Neurogenesis in the Olfactory Bulb

Affects Social Interaction but not Maternal Behavior. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience

4:176 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3001759&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed May 9, 2012].

Feng R, Rampon C, Tang YP, Shrom D, Jin J, Kyin M, Sopher B, Miller MW, Ware CB, Martin

GM, Kim SH, Langdon RB, Sisodia SS, Tsien JZ (2001) Deficient neurogenesis in

forebrain-specific presenilin-1 knockout mice is associated with reduced clearance of

hippocampal memory traces. Neuron 32:911–926 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11738035.

Fenno L, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K (2011) The development and application of optogenetics.

Annual review of neuroscience 34:389–412 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21692661 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Ferri ALM, Cavallaro M, Braida D, Di Cristofano A, Canta A, Vezzani A, Ottolenghi S,

Pandolfi PP, Sala M, DeBiasi S, Nicolis SK (2004) Sox2 deficiency causes

neurodegeneration and impaired neurogenesis in the adult mouse brain. Development

Page 172: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

159

(Cambridge, England) 131:3805–3819 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15240551 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Fifková E, Anderson CL, Young SJ, Van Harreveld a (1982) Effect of anisomycin on

stimulation-induced changes in dendritic spines of the dentate granule cells. Journal of

neurocytology 11:183–210 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6279784.

Fifková E, Van Harreveld a (1977) Long-lasting morphological changes in dendritic spines of

dentate granular cells following stimulation of the entorhinal area. Journal of neurocytology

6:211–230 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/856951.

Filippov V, Kronenberg G, Pivneva T, Reuter K, Steiner B, Wang L, Yamaguchi M, Kettenmann

H, Kempermann G (2003) Subpopulation of nestin-expressing progenitor cells in the adult

murine hippocampus shows electrophysiological and morphological characteristics of

astrocytes. Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience 23:373–382 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1044743103000605 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Fiske BK, Brunjes PC (2001) Cell death in the developing and sensory-deprived rat olfactory

bulb. The Journal of comparative neurology 431:311–319 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11170007.

Forni PE, Scuoppo C, Imayoshi I, Taulli R, Dastrù W, Sala V, Betz U a K, Muzzi P, Martinuzzi

D, Vercelli AE, Kageyama R, Ponzetto C (2006) High levels of Cre expression in neuronal

progenitors cause defects in brain development leading to microencephaly and

hydrocephaly. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 26:9593–9602 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16971543

[Accessed April 4, 2012].

Frankland P, Bontempi B (2005) The organization of recent and remote memories. Nature

reviews Neuroscience 6:119–130 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15685217 [Accessed July 19, 2011].

Frankland P, Cestari V, Filipkowski R, McDonald R, Silva A (1998) The dorsal hippocampus is

essential for context discrimination but not for contextual conditioning. Behavioral

neuroscience 112:863–874 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9733192.

Page 173: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

160

Frankland P, Josselyn S, Anagnostaras S, Kogan J, Takahashi E, Silva A (2004) Consolidation of

CS and US representations in associative fear conditioning. Hippocampus 14:557–569

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15301434 [Accessed March 13, 2012].

Freund T, Buzsáki G (1996) Interneurons of the Hippocampus. Hippocampus 6:347–470.

Frey U, Huang YY, Kandel ER (1993) Effects of cAMP simulate a late stage of LTP in

hippocampal CA1 neurons. Science (New York, NY) 260:1661–1664 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8389057.

Frey U, Morris R (1997) synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature 385:533–536.

Fuccillo M, Joyner AL, Fishell G (2006) Morphogen to mitogen: the multiple roles of hedgehog

signalling in vertebrate neural development. Nature reviews Neuroscience 7:772–783

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16988653 [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Fukuda S, Kato F, Tozuka Y, Yamaguchi M, Miyamoto Y, Hisatsune T (2003) Two distinct

subpopulations of nestin-positive cells in adult mouse dentate gyrus. The Journal of

neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 23:9357–9366 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14561863.

Fyhn M, Molden S, Witter M, Moser E, Moser M (2004) Spatial Representation in the

Entorhinal Cortex. Science 305:1258–1264.

Gaffan D (1974) Recognition impaired and association intact in the memory of monkeys after

transection of the fornix. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology 86:1100–

1109 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4209603.

Gao Z, Ure K, Ables JL, Lagace DC, Nave K-A, Goebbels S, Eisch AJ, Hsieh J (2009) Neurod1

is essential for the survival and maturation of adult-born neurons. Nature neuroscience

12:1090–1092 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19701197 [Accessed

July 6, 2011].

Garcia a DR, Doan NB, Imura T, Bush TG, Sofroniew MV (2004) GFAP-expressing progenitors

are the principal source of constitutive neurogenesis in adult mouse forebrain. Nature

Page 174: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

161

neuroscience 7:1233–1241 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15494728

[Accessed March 7, 2012].

Garner AR, Rowland DC, Hwang SY, Baumgaertel K, Roth BL, Kentros C, Mayford M (2012)

Generation of a Synthetic Memory Trace. Science (New York, NY) 335:1513–1516

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22442487 [Accessed March 24, 2012].

Garthe A, Behr J, Kempermann G (2009) Adult-generated hippocampal neurons allow the

flexible use of spatially precise learning strategies. PloS one 4:e5464 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2674212&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 22, 2012].

Ge S, Goh ELK, Sailor K a, Kitabatake Y, Ming G, Song H (2006) GABA regulates synaptic

integration of newly generated neurons in the adult brain. Nature 439:589–593 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1420640&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Ge S, Pradhan D a, Ming G-L, Song H (2007a) GABA sets the tempo for activity-dependent

adult neurogenesis. Trends in neurosciences 30:1–8 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17116335 [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Ge S, Sailor K a, Ming G, Song H (2008) Synaptic integration and plasticity of new neurons in

the adult hippocampus. The Journal of physiology 586:3759–3765 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2538931&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Ge S, Yang C-H, Hsu K-S, Ming G-L, Song H (2007b) A critical period for enhanced synaptic

plasticity in newly generated neurons of the adult brain. Neuron 54:559–566 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2040308&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Geisert EE, Frankfurter a (1989) The neuronal response to injury as visualized by

immunostaining of class III beta-tubulin in the rat. Neuroscience letters 102:137–141

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2682386.

Page 175: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

162

Gheusi G, Cremer H, McLean H, Chazal G, Vincent J, Lledo P (2000) Importance of newly

generated neurons in the adult olfactory bulb for odor discrimination. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences 97:1823–1828 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=26520&tool=pmcentrez&render

type=abstract.

Gheusi G, Ortega-Perez I, Murray K, Lledo P-M (2009) A niche for adult neurogenesis in social

behavior. Behavioural Brain Research 200:315–322 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0166432809000989 [Accessed May 6, 2012].

Gilbert PE, Kesner RP, Lee I (2001) Dissociating hippocampal subregions: double dissociation

between dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus 11:626–636 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11811656 [Accessed September 5, 2011].

Gilboa A, Winocur G, Grady C, Hevenor S, Moscovitch M (2004) Remembering our past:

functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote personal events. Cerebral

cortex (New York, NY : 1991) 14:1214–1225 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15166099 [Accessed March 13, 2012].

Gire D, Franks K, Zak J, Tanaka K, Whitesell J, Mulligan A, Hen R, Schoppa N (2012) Mitral

Cells in the Olfactory Bulb Are Mainly Excited through a Multistep Signaling Path. Journal

of Neuroscience 32:2964–2975 Available at:

http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5580-11.2012 [Accessed March 1,

2012].

Glanzer M, Cunitz A (1966) Two Storage Mechanisms in Free Recall. Journal of Verbal

Learning and Verbal Behavior 5:351–360 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5250164.

Gleeson JG, Allen KM, Fox JW, Lamperti ED, Berkovic S, Scheffer I, Cooper EC, Dobyns WB,

Minnerath SR, Ross ME, Walsh C a (1998) Doublecortin, a brain-specific gene mutated in

human X-linked lissencephaly and double cortex syndrome, encodes a putative signaling

protein. Cell 92:63–72 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9489700.

Page 176: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

163

Gleeson JG, Lin PT, Flanagan LA, Walsh CA (1999) Doublecortin Is a Microtubule-Associated

Protein and Is Expressed Widely by Migrating Neurons Brigham and Women ‘ s Hospital.

23:257–271.

Goldman S, Nottebohm F (1983) Neuronal production, migration, and differentiation in a vocal

control nucleus of the adult female canary brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences of the United States of America 80:2390–2394 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=393826&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract.

Gould E (2007) How widespread is adult neurogenesis in mammals? Nature reviews

Neuroscience 8:481–488 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17514200.

Gould E, Beylin A, Tanapat P, Reeves A, Shors T (1999) Learning enhances adult neurogenesis

in the hippocampal formation. Nature neuroscience 2:260–265 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10195219.

Gould E, Cameron H, Daniels D, Woolley C, McEwen B (1992) Adrenal hormones suppress cell

division in the adult rat dentate gyrus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of

the Society for Neuroscience 12:3642–3650 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1527603.

Gould E, McEwen BS, Tanapat P, Galea L a, Fuchs E (1997) Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus

of the adult tree shrew is regulated by psychosocial stress and NMDA receptor activation.

The Journal of neuroscience 17:2492–2498 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9065509.

Gould E, Tanapat P (1999) Stress and hippocampal neurogenesis. Biological psychiatry

46:1472–1479 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10599477.

Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen B, Flugge G, Fuchs E (1998) Proliferation of granule cell

precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences 95:3168–3171.

Page 177: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

164

Gradinaru V, Zhang F, Ramakrishnan C, Mattis J, Prakash R, Diester I, Goshen I, Thompson

KR, Deisseroth K (2010) Molecular and cellular approaches for diversifying and extending

optogenetics. Cell 141:154–165 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20303157 [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Graf P, Squire L, Mandler G (1984) The information that amnesic patients do not forget. Journal

of experimental psychology Learning, memory, and cognition 10:164–178 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6242734.

Grandbarbe L (2003) Delta-Notch signaling controls the generation of neurons/glia from neural

stem cells in a stepwise process. Development 130:1391–1402 Available at:

http://dev.biologists.org/cgi/doi/10.1242/dev.00374 [Accessed March 29, 2012].

Grastyan E, Lissak K, Madarasz I (1959) Hippocampal electrical activity during the development

of conditioned reflexes. Electroencephalography 11 Available at:

http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0013469459900409 [Accessed January 28,

2012].

Gray J (1982) Precis of The neuropsychology of anxiety : An enquiry into the functions of the

septo-hippocampal system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5:469–534.

Gray J, Mcnaughton N (2003) The Neuropsychology of Anxiety : An enquiry into the function of

the septo- hippocampal system Abstract and Keywords.

Green J, Arduini A (1954) Hippocampal electrical activity in arousal. Journal of

Neurophysiology 17:533–557 Available at: http://doi.apa.org/?uid=1955-05036-001

[Accessed January 28, 2012].

Gropp E, Shanabrough M, Borok E, Xu AW, Janoschek R, Buch T, Plum L, Balthasar N,

Hampel B, Waisman A, Barsh GS, Horvath TL, Brüning JC (2005) Agouti-related peptide-

expressing neurons are mandatory for feeding. Nature neuroscience 8:1289–1291 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16158063 [Accessed April 18, 2012].

Gross CG (2000) Neurogenesis in the adult brain: death of a dogma. Nature reviews

Neuroscience 1:67–73 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11252770.

Page 178: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

165

Gross CG (2007) The discovery of motor cortex and its background. Journal of the history of the

neurosciences 16:320–331 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17620195

[Accessed March 1, 2012].

Gulyás A, Miettinen R, Jacobowitz D, Freund T (1992) Calretinin is present in non-pyramidal

cells of the rat hippocampus--I. A new type of neuron specifically associated with the

mossy fibre system. Neuroscience 48:1–27 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1584417.

Guo W, Allan AM, Zong R, Zhang L, Johnson EB, Schaller EG, Murthy AC, Goggin SL, Eisch

AJ, Oostra B a, Nelson DL, Jin P, Zhao X (2011) Ablation of Fmrp in adult neural stem

cells disrupts hippocampus-dependent learning. Nature medicine 17:559–565 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3140952&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Guzowski JF, Timlin J a, Roysam B, McNaughton BL, Worley PF, Barnes C a (2005) Mapping

behaviorally relevant neural circuits with immediate-early gene expression. Current opinion

in neurobiology 15:599–606 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16150584

[Accessed March 5, 2012].

Hack I, Bancila M, Loulier K, Carroll P, Cremer H (2002) Reelin is a detachment signal in

tangential chain-migration during postnatal neurogenesis. Nature neuroscience 5:939–945

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12244323 [Accessed April 8, 2012].

Hack M a, Saghatelyan A, de Chevigny A, Pfeifer A, Ashery-Padan R, Lledo P-M, Götz M

(2005) Neuronal fate determinants of adult olfactory bulb neurogenesis. Nature

neuroscience 8:865–872 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15951811

[Accessed March 22, 2012].

Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser M, Moser E (2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the

entorhinal cortex. Nature 436:801–806 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15965463 [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Hagan J, Salamone J, Simpson J, Iversen S, Morris R (1988) Place navigation in rats is impaired

by lesions of medial septum and diagonal band but not nucleus basalis magnocellularis.

Page 179: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

166

Behavioural brain research 27:9–20 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3342117.

Hairston IS, Little MTM, Scanlon MD, Barakat MT, Palmer TD, Sapolsky RM, Heller HC

(2005) Sleep restriction suppresses neurogenesis induced by hippocampus-dependent

learning. Journal of neurophysiology 94:4224–4233 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16014798 [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Hallermann S, Pawlu C, Jonas P, Heckmann M (2003) A large pool of releasable vesicles in a

cortical glutamatergic synapse. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100:8975.

Hamilton A (1901) The division of differentiated cells in the central nervous system of the white

rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 11:297–320 Available at:

http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/cne.910110403.

Han J, Kushner S, Yiu A, Hsiang H, Buch T, Waisman A, Bontempi B, Neve R, Frankland P,

Josselyn S (2009) Selective erasure of a fear memory. Science 323:1492–1496 Available at:

http://www.sciencemag.org/content/323/5920/1492.short [Accessed April 24, 2012].

Han J-H, Kushner S a, Yiu AP, Cole CJ, Matynia A, Brown R a, Neve RL, Guzowski JF, Silva

AJ, Josselyn S a (2007) Neuronal competition and selection during memory formation.

Science (New York, NY) 316:457–460 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17446403 [Accessed March 6, 2012].

Hayes NL, Nowakowski RS (2002) Dynamics of cell proliferation in the adult dentate gyrus of

two inbred strains of mice. Brain research Developmental brain research 134:77–85

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11947938.

Hebb D (1949) The organization of behavior, a neuropsychological theory (Wiley, ed). New

York: John Wiley & Sons. Available at:

http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=gUtwMochAI8C&pgis=1 [Accessed March

12, 2012].

Page 180: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

167

Heindel C, Salmon DP, Shults W, Walicke A (1989) Neuropsychological Evidence A

Comparison of Alzheimer ‘ s , Patients for Multiple Implicit Memory Systems : Huntington

‘ s , and Parkinson ‘ s Disease. Dementia:582–587.

Heindel WC, Butters N, Salmon DP (1988) Impaired learning of a motor skill in patients with

Huntington‘s disease. Behavioral neuroscience 102:141–147 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2965592 [Accessed March 11, 2012].

Heine VM, Maslam S, Zareno J, Joels M, Lucassen PJ (2004) Suppressed proliferation and

apoptotic changes in the rat dentate gyrus after acute and chronic stress are reversible.

European Journal of Neuroscience 19:131–144 Available at:

http://doi.wiley.com/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.03100.x [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Henze D, Wittner L, Buzsáki G (2002) Single granule cells reliably discharge targets in the

hippocampal CA3 network in vivo. Nature neuroscience 5:790–795 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12118256 [Accessed June 17, 2011].

Herman JP, Patel PD, Akil H, Watson SJ (1989) Localization and Regulation of Glucocorticoid

and Mineralocorticoid Receptor Messenger RNAs in the Hippocampal Formation of the

Rat. Molecular Endocrinology 3:1886–1894 Available at:

http://mend.endojournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/3/11/1886 [Accessed February 3, 2012].

Hernández-Rabaza V, Llorens-Martín M, Velázquez-Sánchez C, Ferragud a, Arcusa a, Gumus

HG, Gómez-Pinedo U, Pérez-Villalba a, Roselló J, Trejo JL, Barcia J a, Canales JJ (2009)

Inhibition of adult hippocampal neurogenesis disrupts contextual learning but spares spatial

working memory, long-term conditional rule retention and spatial reversal. Neuroscience

159:59–68 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19138728 [Accessed March

7, 2012].

Holmberg J, Armulik A, Senti K-A, Edoff K, Spalding K, Momma S, Cassidy R, Flanagan JG,

Frisén J (2005) Ephrin-A2 reverse signaling negatively regulates neural progenitor

proliferation and neurogenesis. Genes & development 19:462–471 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=548947&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract [Accessed April 5, 2012].

Page 181: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

168

Holsboer F, Ising M (2010) Stress hormone regulation: biological role and translation into

therapy. Annual review of psychology 61:81–109, C1–11 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19575614 [Accessed March 6, 2012].

Höglinger GU, Rizk P, Muriel MP, Duyckaerts C, Oertel WH, Caille I, Hirsch EC (2004)

Dopamine depletion impairs precursor cell proliferation in Parkinson disease. Nature

neuroscience 7:726–735 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15195095

[Accessed March 23, 2012].

Ide Y, Fujiyama F, Okamoto-Furuta K, Tamamaki N, Kaneko T, Hisatsune T (2008) Rapid

integration of young newborn dentate gyrus granule cells in the adult hippocampal circuitry.

The European journal of neuroscience 28:2381–2392 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19087169 [Accessed March 20, 2012].

Imayoshi I, Sakamoto M, Ohtsuka T, Takao K, Miyakawa T, Yamaguchi M, Mori K, Ikeda T,

Itohara S, Kageyama R (2008) Roles of continuous neurogenesis in the structural and

functional integrity of the adult forebrain. Nature neuroscience 11:1153–1161 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18758458 [Accessed July 19, 2011].

Imayoshi I, Sakamoto M, Yamaguchi M, Mori K, Kageyama R (2010) Essential roles of Notch

signaling in maintenance of neural stem cells in developing and adult brains. The Journal of

neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 30:3489–3498 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20203209 [Accessed July 17, 2011].

Imielski Y, Schwamborn JC, Lüningschrör P, Heimann P, Holzberg M, Werner H, Leske O,

Püschel AW, Memet S, Heumann R, Israel A, Kaltschmidt C, Kaltschmidt B (2012)

Regrowing the Adult Brain: NF-κB Controls Functional Circuit Formation and Tissue

Homeostasis in the Dentate Gyrus. PloS one 7:e30838 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3270021&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Isaacson RL, Kimble D (1972) Lesions of the limbic system: Their effects upon hypotheses and

frustration1. Behavioral Biology 7:767–793 Available at:

Page 182: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

169

http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0091677372801706 [Accessed January

30, 2012].

Jacobs L, Spencer W (1994) Natural Space-use Patterns and Hippocampal Size in Kangaroo

Rats. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 44:125–132.

Jaffe DB, Gutiérrez R (2007) Mossy fiber synaptic transmission: communication from the

dentate gyrus to area CA3. Progress in brain research 163:109–132 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17765714 [Accessed August 3, 2011].

Jagasia R, Steib K, Englberger E, Herold S, Faus-Kessler T, Saxe M, Gage F, Song H, Lie D

(2009) GABA-cAMP response element-binding protein signaling regulates maturation and

survival of newly generated neurons in the adult hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience

29:7966–7977 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2776747&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Jaholkowski P, Kiryk A, Jedynak P, Ben Abdallah NM, Knapska E, Kowalczyk A, Piechal A,

Blecharz-Klin K, Figiel I, Lioudyno V, Widy-Tyszkiewicz E, Wilczynski GM, Lipp H-P,

Kaczmarek L, Filipkowski RK (2009) New hippocampal neurons are not obligatory for

memory formation; cyclin D2 knockout mice with no adult brain neurogenesis show

learning. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, NY) 16:439–451 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19553382 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

James W (1905) The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company. Available

at: http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2005-05009-000 [Accessed March 4, 2012].

Jang M, Song H, Ming G (2007) Regulation of Adult Neurogenesis by Neurotransmitters. In:

Adult Neurogenesis (Gage F, Kempermann G, eds), pp.397–423. Cold Spring Harbor, NY:

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.

Jankord R, Herman JP (2008) Limbic regulation of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical

function during acute and chronic stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

1148:64–73 Available at:

Page 183: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

170

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2637449&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed July 18, 2011].

Jeltsch H, Bertrand F, Lazarus C, Cassel JC (2001) Cognitive performances and locomotor

activity following dentate granule cell damage in rats: role of lesion extent and type of

memory tested. Neurobiology of learning and memory 76:81–105 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11525255 [Accessed December 3, 2011].

Jessberger S, Aigner S, Clemenson G, Toni N, Lie D, Karalay O, Overall R, Kempermann G,

Gage F (2008a) Cdk5 regulates accurate maturation of newborn granule cells in the adult

hippocampus. PLoS biology 6:e272 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2581629&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Jessberger S, Clark R, Broadbent N, Clemenson G, Consiglio A, Lie D, Squire L, Gage F (2009)

Dentate gyrus-specific knockdown of adult neurogenesis impairs spatial and object

recognition memory in adult rats. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, NY) 16:147–

154 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2661246&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Jessberger S, Gage F, Eisch A, Lagace D (2010) Making a Neuron: Cdk5 in Embryonic and

Adult Neurogenesis. Trends Neurosci 32:575–582.

Jessberger S, Römer B, Babu H, Kempermann G (2005) Seizures induce proliferation and

dispersion of doublecortin-positive hippocampal progenitor cells. Experimental neurology

196:342–351 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16168988 [Accessed

March 9, 2012].

Jessberger S, Toni N, Clemenson G, Ray J, Gage F (2008b) Directed differentiation of

hippocampal stem/progenitor cells in the adult brain. Nature neuroscience 11:888–893

Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2795354&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 18, 2012].

Page 184: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

171

Jessberger S, Zhao C, Toni N, Clemenson G, Li Y, Gage F (2007) Seizure-associated, aberrant

neurogenesis in adult rats characterized with retrovirus-mediated cell labeling. The Journal

of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 27:9400–9407

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17728453 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Jiang C, Schuman EM (2002) Regulation and function of local protein synthesis in neuronal

dendrites. Trends in biochemical sciences 27:506–513 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12368086.

Jin K, Sun Y, Xie L, Batteur S, Mao XO, Smelick C, Logvinova A, Greenberg D a (2003)

Neurogenesis and aging: FGF-2 and HB-EGF restore neurogenesis in hippocampus and

subventricular zone of aged mice. Aging cell 2:175–183 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12882410.

Jin K, Zhu Y, Sun Y, Mao XO, Xie L, Greenberg D a (2002) Vascular endothelial growth factor

(VEGF) stimulates neurogenesis in vitro and in vivo. Proceedings of the National Academy

of Sciences of the United States of America 99:11946–11950 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=129374&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract.

Johansen J, Cain C, Ostroff L, LeDoux J (2011) Molecular mechanisms of fear learning and

memory. Cell 147:509–524 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3215943&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Johansen J, Hamanaka H, Monfils M, Behnia R, Deisseroth K, Blair H, LeDoux J (2010) Optical

activation of lateral amygdala pyramidal cells instructs associative fear learning.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

107:12692–12697 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2906568&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed July 18, 2011].

Jonasson Z (2005) Meta-analysis of sex differences in rodent models of learning and memory: a

review of behavioral and biological data. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 28:811–

Page 185: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

172

825 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15642623 [Accessed March 20,

2012].

Jovanovic T, Ressler KJ (2010) How the neurocircuitry and genetics of fear inhibition may

inform our understanding of PTSD. The American journal of psychiatry 167:648–662

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20231322.

Joëls M, Karst H, Krugers HJ, Lucassen PJ (2007) Chronic stress: implications for neuronal

morphology, function and neurogenesis. Frontiers in neuroendocrinology 28:72–96

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17544065 [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Jung M, Wiener I, Mcnaughton B (1994) Comparison of Spatial Firing Characteristics of Units

in Dorsal and Ventral Hippocampus of the Rat. Journal of Neuroscience 14:7347–7356.

Kaada B, Jansen J, Andersen P (1953) Stimulation of the hippocampus and medial cortical areas

in unanesthetized cats. Neurology 3:844–857 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/13111345 [Accessed January 28, 2012].

Kaada B, Pribram K (1949) Respiratory and vascular responses in monkeys from temporal pole,

insula, orbital surface and cingulate gyrus: a preliminary report. Fed Proc 8:83–84 Available

at: http://jn.physiology.org/content/12/5/347.full.pdf [Accessed January 28, 2012].

Kaada B, Rasmussen EW, Kveim O (1961) Effects of hippocampal lesions on maze learning and

retention in rats. Experimental neurology 3:333–355 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/13791166.

Kageyama R, Ohtsuka T, Shimojo H, Imayoshi I (2008) Dynamic Notch signaling in neural

progenitor cells and a revised view of lateral inhibition. Nature neuroscience 11:1247–1251

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18956012 [Accessed June 11, 2011].

Kandel ER (2002) The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialog between genes and

synapses. Bioscience reports 24:475–522 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16134023.

Kaneko N, Okano H, Sawamoto K (2006) Role of the cholinergic system in regulating survival

of newborn neurons in the adult mouse dentate gyrus and olfactory bulb. Genes to cells :

Page 186: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

173

devoted to molecular & cellular mechanisms 11:1145–1159 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16999735 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Kaplan M, Bell D (1984) Mitotic neuroblasts in the 9-day-old and 11-month-old rodent

hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience 4:1429–1441.

Kaplan MS, Hinds JW (1977) Neurogenesis in the adult rat: electron microscopic analysis of

light radioautographs. Science (New York, NY) 197:1092–1094 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/887941.

Kaplan MS, McNelly N a, Hinds JW (1985) Population dynamics of adult-formed granule

neurons of the rat olfactory bulb. The Journal of comparative neurology 239:117–125

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4044929.

Kay LM, Laurent G (1999) modulation of mitral cell activity in behaving rats. America:1003–

1009.

Kee N, Sivalingam S, Boonstra R, Wojtowicz JM (2002) The utility of Ki-67 and BrdU as

proliferative markers of adult neurogenesis. Journal of neuroscience methods 115:97–105

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11897369.

Kee N, Teixeira C, Wang A, Frankland P (2007) Preferential incorporation of adult-generated

granule cells into spatial memory networks in the dentate gyrus. Nature neuroscience

10:355–362 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17277773 [Accessed July

6, 2011].

Kempermann G (2003) Early determination and long-term persistence of adult-generated new

neurons in the hippocampus of mice. Development 130:391–399 Available at:

http://dev.biologists.org/cgi/doi/10.1242/dev.00203 [Accessed March 6, 2012].

Kempermann G (2011) Seven principles in the regulation of adult neurogenesis. The European

journal of neuroscience 33:1018–1024 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21395844 [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Page 187: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

174

Kempermann G, Jessberger S, Steiner B, Kronenberg G (2004) Milestones of neuronal

development in the adult hippocampus. Trends in neurosciences 27:447–452 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15271491 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Kempermann G, Kuhn H, Gage F (1997a) More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an

enriched environment. Nature 386:493–495.

Kempermann G, Kuhn H, Gage F (1997b) Genetic influence on neurogenesis in the dentate

gyrus of adult mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States

of America 94:10409–10414 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16999941.

Kempermann G, Kuhn H, Gage F (1998) Experience-induced neurogenesis in the senescent

dentate gyrus. The Journal of neuroscience 18:3206–3212 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9547229.

Kentros C, Hargreaves E, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Shapiro M, Muller RV (1998) Abolition of

long-term stability of new hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade.

Science (New York, NY) 280:2121–2126.

Kida S, Josselyn S, Peña de Ortiz S, Kogan J, Chevere I, Masushige S, Silva A (2002) CREB

required for the stability of new and reactivated fear memories. Nature neuroscience 5:348–

355 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11889468 [Accessed March 13,

2012].

Kim EJ, Ables JL, Dickel LK, Eisch AJ, Johnson JE (2011) Ascl1 (Mash1) defines cells with

long-term neurogenic potential in subgranular and subventricular zones in adult mouse

brain. PloS one 6:e18472 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3069117&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Kim J, Fanselow M (1992) Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science (New York,

NY) 256:675–677 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11981164.

Kim KK, Adelstein RS, Kawamoto S (2009) Identification of neuronal nuclei (NeuN) as Fox-3,

a new member of the Fox-1 gene family of splicing factors. The Journal of biological

Page 188: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

175

chemistry 284:31052–31061 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2781505&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 5, 2012].

Kim WR, Kim Y, Eun B, Park O, Kim H, Kim K, Park C-H, Vinsant S, Oppenheim RW, Sun W

(2007) Impaired migration in the rostral migratory stream but spared olfactory function after

the elimination of programmed cell death in Bax knock-out mice. The Journal of

neuroscience 27:14392–14403 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18160647 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Kimble D (1963) THE EFFECTS OF BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS IN RATS 1

Behavioral effects of chronic lesions in- volving the hippocampus in various animals and

man have been reported by several investigators . Such seemingly diverse find- ings as ―

short term ‖ memory de. Physiological Psychology 56:273–283.

Kimble D (1968) HIPPOCAMPUS AND INTERNAL INHIBITION. Psychological bulletin

70:285–295 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19586159.

Kippin TE, Kapur S, van der Kooy D (2005) Dopamine specifically inhibits forebrain neural

stem cell proliferation, suggesting a novel effect of antipsychotic drugs. The Journal of

neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 25:5815–5823 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15958748 [Accessed March 16, 2012].

Kiss J, Csáki A, Bokor H, Shanabrough M, Leranth C (2000) The supramammillo-hippocampal

and supramammillo-septal glutamatergic/aspartatergic projections in the rat: a combined

[3H]D-aspartate autoradiographic and immunohistochemical study. Neuroscience 97:657–

669 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10842010 [Accessed January 26,

2012].

Kitamura T, Saitoh Y, Takashima N, Murayama A, Niibori Y, Ageta H, Sekiguchi M, Sugiyama

H, Inokuchi K (2009) Adult neurogenesis modulates the hippocampus-dependent period of

associative fear memory. Cell 139:814–827 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19914173 [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Page 189: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

176

Klempin F, Kempermann G (2007) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and aging. European

archives of psychiatry and clinical neuroscience 257:271–280 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17401726 [Accessed February 29, 2012].

de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joëls M (1999) Stress and cognition: are corticosteroids good or bad

guys? Trends in Neurosciences 22:422–426 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0166223699014381 [Accessed June 23, 2011].

Knigge KM (1961) Adrenocortical Response to Stress in Rats with Lesions in Hippocampus and

Amygdala. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine Society for

Experimental Biology and Medicine (New York, NY) 108:18–21 Available at:

http://ebm.rsmjournals.com.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/content/108/1/18.abstract

[Accessed February 3, 2012].

Ko H-G, Jang D-J, Son J, Kwak C, Choi J-H, Ji Y-H, Lee Y-S, Son H, Kaang B-K (2009) Effect

of ablated hippocampal neurogenesis on the formation and extinction of contextual fear

memory. Molecular brain 2:1 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2629467&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 25, 2012].

Kohwi M, Osumi N, Rubenstein JLR, Alvarez-Buylla A (2005) Pax6 is required for making

specific subpopulations of granule and periglomerular neurons in the olfactory bulb. The

Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 25:6997–7003

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16049175 [Accessed March 29, 2012].

Komitova M, Eriksson PS (2004) Sox-2 is expressed by neural progenitors and astroglia in the

adult rat brain. Neuroscience letters 369:24–27 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15380301 [Accessed March 21, 2012].

Kopelman MD, Thomson AD, Guerrini I, Marshall EJ (2009) The Korsakoff syndrome: clinical

aspects, psychology and treatment. Alcohol and alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire) 44:148–

154 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19151162 [Accessed July 20,

2011].

Korsching S (2002) Olfactory maps and odor images. Curr Opin Neurobioll 12:387–392.

Page 190: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

177

Kosaka K, Kosaka T (2005) Synaptic Organization of the Glomerulus in the Main Olfactory

Bulb: Compartments of the Glomerulus and Heterogeneity of the Periglomerular Cells.

Anatomical science international 80:80–90 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15960313.

Kronenberg G, Reuter K, Steiner B, Brandt M, Jessberger S, Yamaguchi M, Kempermann G

(2003) Subpopulations of proliferating cells of the adult hippocampus respond differently to

physiologic neurogenic stimuli. The Journal of comparative neurology 467:455–463

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14624480 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Krug M, Lössner B, Ott T (1984) Anisomycin blocks the late phase of long-term potentiation in

the dentate gyrus of freely moving rats. Brain research bulletin 13:39–42 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6089972.

Kuhn H, Dickinson-Anson H, Gage F (1996) Neurogenesis in the Dentate Gyrus of the Adult

Rat: Age-Related Decrease of Neuronal Progenitor Proliferation. Journal of Neuroscience

16:2027–2033.

Kuhn HG, Winkler J, Kempermann G, Thal LJ, Gage FH (1997) Epidermal growth factor and

fibroblast growth factor-2 have different effects on neural progenitors in the adult rat brain.

The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 17:5820–

5829 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9221780.

Kuwabara T, Hsieh J, Muotri A, Yeo G, Warashina M, Lie D, Moore L, Nakashima K, Asashima

M, Gage F (2009) Wnt-mediated activation of NeuroD1 and retro-elements during adult

neurogenesis. Nature neuroscience 12:1097–1105 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2764260&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed August 2, 2011].

Kveim O, Setekleiv J, Kaada B (1964) Differential effects of hippocampal lesions on maze and

passive avoidance learning in rats. Experimental Neurology 9:59–72 Available at:

http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0014488664900433 [Accessed January 30,

2012].

Page 191: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

178

Köhler C (1985) Intrinsic projections of the retrohippocampal region in the rat brain. I. The

subicular complex. The Journal of comparative neurology 236:504–522 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3902916.

Lacefield C, Itskov V, Reardon T, Hen R, Gordon J (2012) Effects of adult-generated granule

cells on coordinated network activity in the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 22:106–116

Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3282563&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 15, 2012].

Lagace DC, Benavides DR, Kansy JW, Mapelli M, Greengard P, Bibb J a, Eisch AJ (2008) Cdk5

is essential for adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences of the United States of America 105:18567–18571 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2587597&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Lagace DC, Donovan MH, DeCarolis N a, Farnbauch L a, Malhotra S, Berton O, Nestler EJ,

Krishnan V, Eisch AJ (2010) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is functionally important for

stress-induced social avoidance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

United States of America 107:4436–4441 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2840117&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 7, 2012].

Lagace DC, Whitman MC, Noonan M a, Ables JL, DeCarolis N a, Arguello A a, Donovan MH,

Fischer SJ, Farnbauch L a, Beech RD, DiLeone RJ, Greer C a, Mandyam CD, Eisch AJ

(2007) Dynamic contribution of nestin-expressing stem cells to adult neurogenesis. The

Journal of neuroscience 27:12623–12629 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18003841 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Lagier S, Panzanelli P, Russo RE, Nissant A, Bathellier B, Sassoè-Pognetto M, Fritschy J-M,

Lledo P-M (2007) GABAergic inhibition at dendrodendritic synapses tunes gamma

oscillations in the olfactory bulb. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

United States of America 104:7259–7264 Available at:

Page 192: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

179

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1855399&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Lai K, Kaspar BK, Gage FH, Schaffer DV (2003) Sonic hedgehog regulates adult neural

progenitor proliferation in vitro and in vivo. Nature neuroscience 6:21–27 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12469128 [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Laplagne D, Espósito M, Piatti V, Morgenstern N, Zhao C, van Praag H, Gage F, Schinder A

(2006) Functional convergence of neurons generated in the developing and adult

hippocampus. PLoS biology 4:e409 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1637132&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Laplagne D, Kamienkowski J, Espósito M, Piatti V, Zhao C, Gage F, Schinder A (2007) Similar

GABAergic inputs in dentate granule cells born during embryonic and adult neurogenesis.

The European journal of neuroscience 25:2973–2981 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17509085 [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Lashley K (1929) Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence. Chicago: The University Press. Available

at: http://brain.oxfordjournals.org [Accessed March 14, 2012].

Lashley K (1950) In search of the engram. In: Symposia of the society for experimental biology,

pp.30 Available at: http://homepage.mac.com/sanagnos/lashley1950.pdf [Accessed

February 3, 2012].

Laurent G (2002) Olfactory network dynamics and the coding of multidimensional signals.

Nature reviews Neuroscience 3:884–895 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12415296 [Accessed March 3, 2012].

Lazarini F, Lledo P (2011) Is adult neurogenesis essential for olfaction? Trends in neurosciences

34:20–30 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20980064 [Accessed July 20,

2011].

Lazarini F, Mouthon M, Gheusi G, de Chaumont F, Olivo-Marin J, Lamarque S, Abrous D,

Boussin F, Lledo P (2009) Cellular and behavioral effects of cranial irradiation of the

Page 193: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

180

subventricular zone in adult mice. PloS one 4:e7017 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2737283&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed July 20, 2011].

Lechner HA, Squire LR, Byrne JH (1999) 100 Years of Consolidation −− Remembering Müller

and Pilzecker. Learning & Memory 6:77–87.

Lee I, Kesner RP (2004) Differential contributions of dorsal hippocampal subregions to memory

acquisition and retrieval in contextual fear-conditioning. Hippocampus 14:301–310

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15132429 [Accessed August 22, 2011].

Lendahl U, Zimmerman LB, McKay RD (1990) CNS stem cells express a new class of

intermediate filament protein. Cell 60:585–595 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1689217.

Leutgeb J, Leutgeb S, Moser M, Moser E (2007) Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3

of the hippocampus. Science (New York, NY) 315:961–966 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17303747 [Accessed July 15, 2011].

Lewis F (1923) The significance of the term hippocampus. The Journal of Comparative

Neurology 35:213–230 Available at:

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cne.900350303/abstract [Accessed January 31,

2012].

Li W, Lopez L, Osher J, Howard JD, Parrish TB, Gottfried J a (2010) Right orbitofrontal cortex

mediates conscious olfactory perception. Psychological science 21:1454–1463 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20817780 [Accessed March 19, 2012].

Li X, Somogyi P, Ylinen A, Buzsáki G (1994) The hippocampal CA3 network: an in vivo

intracellular labeling study. The Journal of comparative neurology 339:181–208 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8300905.

Li Y, Li Y, Mckay M, Riethmacher D, Parada LF (2012) and Behavioral Effects of

Antidepressants. Human Genetics 32:3529–3539.

Page 194: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

181

Lim D a, Tramontin a D, Trevejo JM, Herrera DG, García-Verdugo JM, Alvarez-Buylla a (2000)

Noggin antagonizes BMP signaling to create a niche for adult neurogenesis. Neuron

28:713–726 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11163261.

Lisman JE (1999) Relating hippocampal circuitry to function: recall of memory sequences by

reciprocal dentate-CA3 interactions. Neuron 22:233–242 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10069330.

Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajan A, Deisseroth K, Tonegawa S (2012)

Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature

484:381–385 Available at: http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nature11028

[Accessed March 23, 2012].

Liu X, Wang Q, Haydar TF, Bordey A (2005) Nonsynaptic GABA signaling in postnatal

subventricular zone controls proliferation of GFAP-expressing progenitors. Nature

neuroscience 8:1179–1187 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1380263&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 29, 2012].

Lledo P, Gheusi G, Vincent J (2005) Information Processing in the Mammalian Olfactory

System. Physiological Reviews:281–317.

Lledo P-M, Lagier S (2006) Adjusting neurophysiological computations in the adult olfactory

bulb. Seminars in cell & developmental biology 17:443–453 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16757194 [Accessed March 31, 2012].

Lois C, Alvarez-Buylla A (1994) Long-distance neuronal migration in the adult mammalian

brain. Science 264:1145–1148.

Luo M, Katz LC, Carolina N (2001) Response Correlation Maps of Neurons in the Mammalian

Olfactory Bulb. 32:1165–1179.

Ma DK, Marchetto MC, Guo JU, Ming G, Gage FH, Song H (2010) Epigenetic choreographers

of neurogenesis in the adult mammalian brain. Nature neuroscience 13:1338–1344

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20975758 [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Page 195: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

182

Machold R, Hayashi S, Rutlin M, Muzumdar MD, Nery S, Corbin JG, Gritli-Linde A, Dellovade

T, Porter J a, Rubin LL, Dudek H, McMahon AP, Fishell G (2003) Sonic hedgehog is

required for progenitor cell maintenance in telencephalic stem cell niches. Neuron 39:937–

950 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12971894.

Madsen T, Kristjansen P, Bolwig T, G W (2003) Arrested neuronal proliferation and impaired

hippocampal function following fractionated brain irradiation in the adult rat. Neuroscience

119:635–642 Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0306452203001994

[Accessed March 27, 2012].

Maehama T, Dixon JE (1998) The Tumor Suppressor, PTEN/ MMAC1, Dephosphorylates the

Lipid Second Messenger, Phosphatidylinositol 3,4,5-Trisphosphate. The Journal of

biological chemistry 273:13375–13378.

Magavi S, Mitchell B, Szentirmai O, Carter B, Macklis J (2005) Adult-born and preexisting

olfactory granule neurons undergo distinct experience-dependent modifications of their

olfactory responses in vivo. The Journal of neuroscience 25:10729–10739 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16291946 [Accessed April 1, 2012].

Magill ST, Cambronne X a, Luikart BW, Lioy DT, Leighton BH, Westbrook GL, Mandel G,

Goodman RH (2010) microRNA-132 regulates dendritic growth and arborization of

newborn neurons in the adult hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences of the United States of America 107:20382–20387 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2996687&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed July 20, 2011].

Mak GK, Enwere EK, Gregg C, Pakarainen T, Poutanen M, Huhtaniemi I, Weiss S (2007) Male

pheromone-stimulated neurogenesis in the adult female brain: possible role in mating

behavior. Nature neuroscience 10:1003–1011 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17603480 [Accessed March 6, 2012].

Mak GK, Weiss S (2010) Paternal recognition of adult offspring mediated by newly generated

CNS neurons. Nature neuroscience 13:753–758 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20453850 [Accessed March 18, 2012].

Page 196: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

183

Malberg JE, Duman RS (2003) Cell proliferation in adult hippocampus is decreased by

inescapable stress: reversal by fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology : official

publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 28:1562–1571

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12838272 [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Malberg JE, Eisch a J, Nestler EJ, Duman RS (2000) Chronic antidepressant treatment increases

neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of

the Society for Neuroscience 20:9104–9110 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11124987.

Malleret G, Haditsch U, Genoux D, Jones MW, Bliss TV, Vanhoose a M, Weitlauf C, Kandel

ER, Winder DG, Mansuy IM (2001) Inducible and reversible enhancement of learning,

memory, and long-term potentiation by genetic inhibition of calcineurin. Cell 104:675–686

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11257222.

Mandairon N, Didier A, Linster C (2008) Odor enrichment increases interneurons

responsiveness in spatially defined regions of the olfactory bulb correlated with perception.

Neurobiology of learning and memory 90:178–184 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18406178 [Accessed March 22, 2012].

Mandairon N, Jourdan F, Didier A (2003) Deprivation of sensory inputs to the olfactory bulb up-

regulates cell death and proliferation in the subventricular zone of adult mice. Neuroscience

119:507–516 Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0306452203001726

[Accessed March 15, 2012].

Mandairon N, Linster C (2009) Odor perception and olfactory bulb plasticity in adult mammals.

Journal of neurophysiology 101:2204–2209 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19261715 [Accessed April 9, 2012].

Mandairon N, Sacquet J, Garcia S, Ravel N, Jourdan F, Didier A (2006a) Neurogenic correlates

of an olfactory discrimination task in the adult olfactory bulb. The European journal of

neuroscience 24:3578–3588 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17229106

[Accessed March 31, 2012].

Page 197: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

184

Mandairon N, Sacquet J, Jourdan F, Didier a (2006b) Long-term fate and distribution of newborn

cells in the adult mouse olfactory bulb: Influences of olfactory deprivation. Neuroscience

141:443–451 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16713121 [Accessed

March 29, 2012].

Mandairon N, Sultan S, Nouvian M, Sacquet J, Didier A (2011) Involvement of Newborn

Neurons in Olfactory Associative Learning? The Operant or Non-operant Component of the

Task Makes All the Difference. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the

Society for Neuroscience 31:12455–12460 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21880907 [Accessed September 2, 2011].

Mansuy IM, Mayford M, Jacob B, Kandel ER, Bach ME (1998) Restricted and regulated

overexpression reveals calcineurin as a key component in the transition from short-term to

long-term memory. Cell 92:39–49 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9489698.

Maren S (2003) The amygdala, Synaptic Plasticity, and Fear memory. Ann NY Acad Sci

985:106–113.

Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow M (1996) Retrograde abolition of conditional fear after

excitotoxic lesions in the basolateral amygdala of rats: absence of a temporal gradient.

Behavioral neuroscience 110:718–726.

Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow MS (1997) Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and

Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behavioural brain research 88:261–274 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9404635.

Maren S, Anagnostaras SG, Fanselow MS (1998) The startled seahorse: is the hippocampus

necessary for contextual fear conditioning? Trends in cognitive sciences 2:39–42 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21227063.

Markakis E a, Gage FH (1999) Adult-generated neurons in the dentate gyrus send axonal

projections to field CA3 and are surrounded by synaptic vesicles. The Journal of

comparative neurology 406:449–460 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10205022.

Page 198: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

185

Markwardt SJ, Wadiche JI, Overstreet-Wadiche LS (2009) Input-specific GABAergic signaling

to newborn neurons in adult dentate gyrus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal

of the Society for Neuroscience 29:15063–15072 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2846629&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 7, 2012].

Marr D (1971) Simple Memory: A Theory for Archicortex. Philosophical Transactions of the

Royal Society of 262:23–81 Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2417171

[Accessed March 13, 2012].

Martin S, de Hoz L, Morris R (2005) Retrograde amnesia: neither partial nor complete

hippocampal lesions in rats result in preferential sparing of remote spatial memory, even

after reminding. Neuropsychologia 43:609–624 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15716151 [Accessed March 13, 2012].

Marín-Burgin A, Mongiat L a, Pardi MB, Schinder AF (2012) Unique Processing During a

Period of High Excitation/Inhibition Balance in Adult-Born Neurons. Science (New York,

NY) 1238 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22282476 [Accessed March

1, 2012].

Mayford M, Bach ME, Huang YY, Wang L, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER (1996) Control of

memory formation through regulated expression of a CaMKII transgene. Science (New

York, NY) 274:1678–1683 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8939850.

McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O‘Reilly RC (1995) Why there are complementary learning

systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of

connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological review 102:419–457

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7624455.

McEwen B (2006) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators: central role of the brain.

Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 8:367–381.

McEwen BS, Sapolsky RM (1995) Stress and cognitive function. Current opinion in

neurobiology 5:205–216 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7620309.

Page 199: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

186

McGaugh J (1966) Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science (New York, NY)

153:1351–1358 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/172964.

McGaugh JL (1973) Drug facilitation of learning and memory. Annual review of pharmacology

13:229–241 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/609580.

McHugh T, Jones M, Quinn J, Balthasar N, Coppari R, Elmquist J, Lowell B, Fanselow M,

Wilson M, Tonegawa S (2007) Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern

separation in the hippocampal network. Science 317:94–99 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17556551 [Accessed March 5, 2012].

McLamb RL, Mundy WR, Tilson H a (1988) Intradentate colchicine impairs acquisition of a

two-way active avoidance response in a Y-maze. Neuroscience letters 94:338–342

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3205409.

Mcnaughton B, Barnes C, Meltzer J, Sutherland R (1989) Hippocampal granule cells are

necessary for normal spatial learning but not for spatially-selective pyramidal cell

discharge. Experimental Brain Research 76:485–496.

Mcnaughton B, Morris R (1987) Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage

within a disMbuted memory system. TINS 10.

Mechawar N, Saghatelyan A, Grailhe R, Scoriels L, Gheusi G, Gabellec M-M, Lledo P-M,

Changeux J-P (2004) Nicotinic receptors regulate the survival of newborn neurons in the

adult olfactory bulb. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States

of America 101:9822–9826 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=470758&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract.

Meltzer L a, Yabaluri R, Deisseroth K (2005) A role for circuit homeostasis in adult

neurogenesis. Trends in neurosciences 28:653–660 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16271403 [Accessed March 28, 2012].

Meshi D, Drew M, Saxe M, Ansorge M, David D, Santarelli L, Malapani C, Moore H, Hen R

(2006) Hippocampal neurogenesis is not required for behavioral effects of environmental

Page 200: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

187

enrichment. Nature neuroscience 9:729–731 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16648847 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Middlebrook JL, Dorland B, M USA (1977) Response of cultured mammalian cells to the

exotoxins of Pseudomonas aeruginosa and Corynebacterium diphtheriae : differential

cytotoxicity AND REBECCA. Can J Microbiol 23.

Milner B, Corkin S, Teuber HL (1968) Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome:

14-year follow-up study of HM. Neuropsychologia 6:215–234 Available at:

http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0028393268900213 [Accessed March 11,

2012].

Milner B, Squire LR, Kandel ER (1998) Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory.

Neuron 20:445–468.

Mishkin M (1978) Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not separate removal

of amygdala and hippocampus. Nature 273:297–298.

Molnár Z, Brown RE (2010) Insights into the life and work of Sir Charles Sherrington. Nature

reviews Neuroscience 11:429–436 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20445541 [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Mongiat L a, Espósito MS, Lombardi G, Schinder AF (2009) Reliable activation of immature

neurons in the adult hippocampus. PloS one 4:e5320 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2670498&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Monje ML, Palmer T (2003) Radiation injury and neurogenesis. Current opinion in neurology

16:129–134 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12644738 [Accessed April

29, 2012].

Montaron M, Drapeau E, Dupret D, Kitchener P, Aurousseau C, Le Moal M, Piazza P, Abrous D

(2006) Lifelong corticosterone level determines age-related decline in neurogenesis and

memory. Neurobiology of aging 27:645–654 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15953661 [Accessed March 20, 2012].

Page 201: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

188

Moreno M, Linster C, Escanilla O, Sacquet J, Didier A, Mandairon N (2009) Olfactory

perceptual learning requires adult neurogenesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences 106:17980–17985 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2764902&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Moreno-López B, Romero-Grimaldi C, Noval JA, Murillo-Carretero M, Matarredona ER,

Estrada C (2004) Nitric oxide is a physiological inhibitor of neurogenesis in the adult mouse

subventricular zone and olfactory bulb. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of

the Society for Neuroscience 24:85–95 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14715941 [Accessed March 29, 2012].

Morris R (1981) Spatial Localization Does Not Require the Presence of Local Cues. Learning

and Motivation 12:239–260.

Morris R, Anderson E, Lynch G, Baudry M (1986) Selective impairment of learning and

blockade of long term potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP-5.

Nature 319:774–776 Available at: http://en.scientificcommons.org/53359398 [Accessed

March 13, 2012].

Morris R, Davis S, Butcher SP (1990a) Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptors: a

role in information storage? Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London

Series B, Biological sciences 329:187–204 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1978364 [Accessed March 13, 2012].

Morris R, Garrud P, Rawlins J, O‘Keefe J (1982) Place navigation impaired in rats with

hippocampal lesions. Nature 297:681–683 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7088155.

Morris R, Schenk F, Tweedie F, Jarrard L (1990b) Ibotenate Lesions of Hippocampus and/or

Subiculum: Dissociating Components of Allocentric Spatial Learning. The European

journal of neuroscience 2:1016–1028 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12106063.

Page 202: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

189

Morshead CM (2004) Adult neural stem cells: attempting to solve the identity crisis.

Developmental neuroscience 26:93–100 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15711053 [Accessed March 21, 2012].

Moser E, Kropff E, Moser M (2008) Place cells, grid cells, and the brain‘s spatial representation

system. Annual review of neuroscience 31:69–89 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18284371 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Moser E, Moser M, Andersen P (1993) Spatial learning impairment parallels the magnitude of

dorsal hippocampal lesions, but is hardly present following ventral lesions. The Journal of

neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 13:3916–3925 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8366351.

Moser M, Moser E (1998) Distributed encoding and retrieval of spatial memory in the

hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 18:7535–7542 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9736671.

Moser M, Moser E, Forrest E, Andersen P, Morris R (1995) Spatial learning with a minislab in

the dorsal hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 92:9697–9701.

Mouret A, Gheusi G, Gabellec M-M, de Chaumont F, Olivo-Marin J-C, Lledo P-M (2008)

Learning and survival of newly generated neurons: when time matters. The Journal of

neuroscience 28:11511–11516 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18987187 [Accessed March 27, 2012].

Mouret A, Lepousez G, Gras J, Gabellec M-M, Lledo P-M (2009a) Turnover of newborn

olfactory bulb neurons optimizes olfaction. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal

of the Society for Neuroscience 29:12302–12314 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19793989 [Accessed July 18, 2011].

Mouret A, Murray K, Lledo P-M (2009b) Centrifugal drive onto local inhibitory interneurons of

the olfactory bulb. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1170:239–254 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19686142 [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Page 203: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

190

Mu L, Berti L, Masserdotti G, Covic M, Michaelidis TM, Doberauer K, Merz K, Rehfeld F,

Haslinger a., Wegner M, Sock E, Lefebvre V, Couillard-Despres S, Aigner L, Berninger B,

Lie DC (2012) SoxC Transcription Factors Are Required for Neuronal Differentiation in

Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis. Journal of Neuroscience 32:3067–3080 Available at:

http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4679-11.2012 [Accessed March 2,

2012].

Mullen RJ, Buck CR, Smith a M (1992) NeuN, a neuronal specific nuclear protein in vertebrates.

Development (Cambridge, England) 116:201–211 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1483388.

Muller R, Kubie J (1987) The Effects of Changes in the Environment on the Spatial Firing of

Hippocampal Cells. Journal of Neuroscience 7:1951–1968.

Mumby DG (2001) Perspectives on object-recognition memory following hippocampal damage:

lessons from studies in rats. Behavioural brain research 127:159–181 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11718890.

Muotri A, Zhao C, Marchetto M, Gage F (2009) Environmental influence on L1 retrotransposons

in the adult hippocampus. Hippocampus 19:1002–1007 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2758700&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Murray E a, Wise SP (2004) What, if anything, is the medial temporal lobe, and how can the

amygdala be part of it if there is no such thing? Neurobiology of learning and memory

82:178–198 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15464403 [Accessed

March 14, 2012].

Nadel L, Moscovitch M (1997) Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal

complex. Current opinion in neurobiology 7:217–227 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9142752.

Nader K, Einarsson EO (2010) Memory reconsolidation: an update. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences 1191:27–41 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20392274 [Accessed March 3, 2012].

Page 204: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

191

Nader K, Schafe G, LeDoux J (2000a) Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala

for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406:722–726 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10963596.

Nader K, Schafe G, LeDoux J (2000b) The labile nature of consolidation theory. Nature reviews

Neuroscience 1:216–219 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11257912.

Nagel G, Szellas T, Huhn W, Kateriya S, Adeishvili N, Berthold P, Ollig D, Hegemann P,

Bamberg E (2003) cation-selective membrane channel.

Nakashiba T, Buhl DL, McHugh TJ, Tonegawa S (2009) Hippocampal CA3 output is crucial for

ripple-associated reactivation and consolidation of memory. Neuron 62:781–787 Available

at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2728553&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 26, 2012].

Nakashiba T, Cushman JD, Pelkey K a, Renaudineau S, Buhl DL, McHugh TJ, Barrera VR,

Chittajallu R, Iwamoto KS, McBain CJ, Fanselow MS, Tonegawa S (2012) Young Dentate

Granule Cells Mediate Pattern Separation, whereas Old Granule Cells Facilitate Pattern

Completion. Cell:1–14 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22365813

[Accessed March 10, 2012].

Nakashiba T, Young JZ, McHugh TJ, Buhl DL, Tonegawa S (2008) Transgenic inhibition of

synaptic transmission reveals role of CA3 output in hippocampal learning. Science (New

York, NY) 319:1260–1264 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18218862

[Accessed March 12, 2012].

Nanry KP, Mundy WR, Tilson H a (1989) Colchicine-induced alterations of reference memory

in rats: role of spatial versus non-spatial task components. Behavioural brain research

35:45–53 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2803543.

Nguyen PV, Abel T, Kandel ER (1994) Requirement of a critical period of transcription for

induction of a late phase of LTP. Science (New York, NY) 265:1104–1107 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8066450.

Page 205: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

192

Nguyen PV, Kandel ER (1996) A macromolecular synthesis-dependent late phase of long-term

potentiation requiring cAMP in the medial perforant pathway of rat hippocampal slices. The

Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 16:3189–3198.

Nguyen-Ba-Charvet KT, Picard-Riera N, Tessier-Lavigne M, Baron-Van Evercooren A, Sotelo

C, Chédotal A (2004) Multiple roles for slits in the control of cell migration in the rostral

migratory stream. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 24:1497–1506 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14960623

[Accessed March 22, 2012].

Nilsson M, Perfilieva E, Johansson U, Orwar O, Eriksson PS (1999) Enriched environment

increases neurogenesis in the adult rat dentate gyrus and improves spatial memory. Journal

of neurobiology 39:569–578 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10380078.

Ninkovic J, Mori T, Götz M (2007) Distinct modes of neuron addition in adult mouse

neurogenesis. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 27:10906–10911 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17913924 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Nissant A, Bardy C, Katagiri H, Murray K, Lledo P-M (2009) Adult neurogenesis promotes

synaptic plasticity in the olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience 12:728–730 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19412168 [Accessed March 14, 2012].

Noctor SC, Flint AC, Weissman T a, Wong WS, Clinton BK, Kriegstein AR (2002) Dividing

precursor cells of the embryonic cortical ventricular zone have morphological and

molecular characteristics of radial glia. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of

the Society for Neuroscience 22:3161–3173 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11943818.

Noonan M a, Bulin SE, Fuller DC, Eisch AJ (2010) Reduction of adult hippocampal

neurogenesis confers vulnerability in an animal model of cocaine addiction. The Journal of

neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 30:304–315 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2844797&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Page 206: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

193

Nusser Z, Kay LM, Laurent G, Homanics GE, Wesson DW, Borkowski AH, Landreth GE,

Nixon RA, Levy E, Wilson DA, Giridhar S, Doiron B, Urban NN, Beshel J, Mody I (2012)

Disruption of GABA A Receptors on GABAergic Interneurons Leads to Increased

Oscillatory Power in the Olfactory Bulb Network Disruption of GABA A Receptors on

GABAergic Interneurons Leads to Increased Oscillatory Power in the Olfactory Bulb

Network. Journal of Neurophysiology.

Odaimi M, Ajani J (1987) High dose chemotherapy. American Journal of Clinical Oncology

10:123–132.

Olton D, Samuelson R (1976) Remembrance of places passed: Spatial memory in rats. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2:97–116.

Olton D, Walker J, Gage F (1978) Hippocampal Conections and Spatial Discrimination. Brain

Research 139:295–308.

Orbach J, Milner B, Rasmussen T (1960) Learning and retention in monkeys after amygdala-

hippocampus resection. Archives of Neurology 25:230–251 Available at:

http://archneur.ama-assn.org/cgi/content/summary/3/3/230 [Accessed January 30, 2012].

Overstreet Wadiche L, Bromberg D a, Bensen AL, Westbrook GL (2005) GABAergic signaling

to newborn neurons in dentate gyrus. Journal of neurophysiology 94:4528–4532 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16033936 [Accessed March 20, 2012].

Overstreet-Wadiche L, Bromberg D, Bensen A, Westbrook G (2006) Seizures accelerate

functional integration of adult-generated granule cells. The Journal of neuroscience

26:4095–4103 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16611826 [Accessed

March 18, 2012].

Owens DF, Kriegstein AR (2002) Is there more to GABA than synaptic inhibition? Nature

reviews Neuroscience 3:715–727 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12209120 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

O‘Keefe J, Burgess N (1996) Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal

neurons. Nature 381:425–428.

Page 207: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

194

O‘Keefe J, Dostrovsky J (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map . Preliminary evidence from

unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research 34:171–175.

O‘Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map (Press C, ed). Oxford.

O‘Keefe J, Nadel L, Keightley S, Kill D (1975) Fornix Lesions Selectively Abolish Place

Learning in the Rat. Experimental Neurology 48:152–166.

O‘Reilly RC, McClelland JL (1994) Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall:

avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus 4:661–682 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7704110.

Packard MG, White M, Ha Q (1978) Differential Effects of Fornix and Caudate Radial Maze

Tasks : Evidence for Multiple Nucleus Lesions on Two Memory Systems. Science:1465–

1472.

Packer M a, Stasiv Y, Benraiss A, Chmielnicki E, Grinberg A, Westphal H, Goldman S a,

Enikolopov G (2003) Nitric oxide negatively regulates mammalian adult neurogenesis.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

100:9566–9571 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=170958&tool=pmcentrez&rend

ertype=abstract.

Pan Y-W, Chan GCK, Kuo CT, Storm DR, Xia Z (2012) Inhibition of Adult Neurogenesis by

Inducible and Targeted Deletion of ERK5 Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinase Specifically in

Adult Neurogenic Regions Impairs Contextual Fear Extinction and Remote Fear Memory.

Journal of Neuroscience 32:6444–6455 Available at:

http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6076-11.2012 [Accessed May 9,

2012].

Panzanelli P, Bardy C, Nissant A, Pallotto M, Sassoè-Pognetto M, Lledo P-M, Fritschy J-M

(2009) Early synapse formation in developing interneurons of the adult olfactory bulb. The

Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 29:15039–

15052 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19955355 [Accessed March 7,

2012].

Page 208: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

195

Papez JW (1937) A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of neurology and psychiatry

38:725.

Parent JM, Yu TW, Leibowitz RT, Geschwind DH, Sloviter RS, Lowenstein DH (1997) Dentate

granule cell neurogenesis is increased by seizures and contributes to aberrant network

reorganization in the adult rat hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official

journal of the Society for Neuroscience 17:3727–3738 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9133393.

Paton J, Nottebohm F (1984) Neurons generated in the adult brain are recruited into functional

circuits. Science (New York, NY) 225:1046–1048 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6474166.

Pavlov I (2003) Conditioned Reflexes. Mineola, NY: Courier Dover Publications. Available at:

http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=cknrYDqAClkC&pgis=1 [Accessed

February 3, 2012].

Penfield W, Milner B (1958) Memory Deficit Produced by Bilateral Lesions in the Hippocampal

Zone. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry 79:475–497.

Penttonen M, Kamondi A, Sik A, Acsády L, Buzsáki G (1997) Feed-forward and feed-back

activation of the dentate gyrus in vivo during dentate spikes and sharp wave bursts.

Hippocampus 7:437–450 Available at:

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:4%3C437::AID-

HIPO9%3E3.0.CO;2-F/abstract [Accessed January 24, 2012].

Petreanu L, Alvarez-Buylla A (2002) Maturation and death of adult-born olfactory bulb granule

neurons: role of olfaction. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society

for Neuroscience 22:6106–6113 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12122071.

Pham K, Nacher J, Hof PR, McEwen BS (2003) Repeated restraint stress suppresses

neurogenesis and induces biphasic PSA-NCAM expression in the adult rat dentate gyrus.

European Journal of Neuroscience 17:879–886 Available at:

http://doi.wiley.com/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02513.x [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Page 209: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

196

Phillips R, LeDoux J (1992) Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and

contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 106:274–285.

Phillips R, LeDoux J (1994) Lesions of the Dorsal Hippocampal Formation Interfere with

Background but Not Foreground Contextual Fear Conditioning. Learning & Memory 1:34–

44.

Pickel VM, Segal M, Bloom FE (1974) A radioautographic study of the efferent pathways of the

nucleus locus coeruleus. The Journal of comparative neurology 155:15–42.

Pomeroy SL, LaMantia a S, Purves D (1990) Postnatal construction of neural circuitry in the

mouse olfactory bulb. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 10:1952–1966 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2355260.

Porsolt R (1979) Animal model of depression. Biomedicine 30:139–140.

des Portes V, Pinard JM, Billuart P, Vinet MC, Koulakoff a, Carrié a, Gelot a, Dupuis E, Motte

J, Berwald-Netter Y, Catala M, Kahn a, Beldjord C, Chelly J (1998) A novel CNS gene

required for neuronal migration and involved in X-linked subcortical laminar heterotopia

and lissencephaly syndrome. Cell 92:51–61 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9489699.

van Praag H, Christie BR, Sejnowski TJ, Gage FH (1999a) Running enhances neurogenesis,

learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences of the United States of America 96:13427–13431 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=23964&tool=pmcentrez&render

type=abstract.

van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH (1999b) Running increases cell proliferation and

neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nature neuroscience 2:266–270 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10195220.

van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH (2000) Neural consequences of environmental

enrichment. Nature reviews Neuroscience 1:191–198 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11257907.

Page 210: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

197

van Praag H, Schinder A, Christie B, Toni N, Palmer T, Gage F (2002) Functional neurogenesis

in the adult hippocampus. Nature 415:1030–1034 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11875571.

van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage F (2005) Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal

neurogenesis in aged mice. The Journal of neuroscience 25:8680–8685 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1360197&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed February 29, 2012].

Quon D, Wang Y, Catalano R, Scardina J, Murakami K, Cordell B (1991) Formation of b-

amyloid protein deposits in brain of transgenic mice. Nature 352:239–241.

Raber J, Rola R, LeFevour A, Morhardt D, Curley J, Mizumatsu S, VandenBerg S, Fike J (2004)

Radiation-induced cognitive impairments are associated with changes in indicators of

hippocampal neurogenesis. Radiat Res 162:39–47.

Rai KS, Hattiangady B, Shetty AK (2007) Enhanced production and dendritic growth of new

dentate granule cells in the middle-aged hippocampus following intracerebroventricular

FGF-2 infusions. The European journal of neuroscience 26:1765–1779 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17883411 [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Rakic P (1985) Limits of Neurogenesis in Primates. Science 227:3–5.

Ramón y Cajal S (1928) Degeneration and Regeneration of the Nervous System, Trans. Day.

New York: Oxford University Press, USA.

Ramón y Cajal S (1995) Histology of the nervous system of man and vertebrates. New York:

Oxford University Press. Available at: http://www.getcited.org/pub/103101205 [Accessed

March 4, 2012].

Redondo R, Morris R (2011) Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture

hypothesis. Nature reviews Neuroscience 12:17–30 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21170072 [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Page 211: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

198

Reijmers LG, Perkins BL, Matsuo N, Mayford M (2007) Localization of a stable neural correlate

of associative memory. Science (New York, NY) 317:1230–1233 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17761885 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Restivo L, Vetere G, Bontempi B, Ammassari-Teule M (2009) The formation of recent and

remote memory is associated with time-dependent formation of dendritic spines in the

hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex. The Journal of neuroscience : the official

journal of the Society for Neuroscience 29:8206–8214 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19553460 [Accessed March 14, 2012].

Ribot T (1882) Diseases of memory: An essay in the positive psychology. D. Appleton and

company. Available at:

http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&amp;lr=&amp;id=CiUAAAAAQAAJ&amp;oi=fnd

&amp;pg=PA9&amp;dq=DISEASES+OF+MEMORY:+AN+ESSAY+in+THE+POSITIVE

+PSYCHOLOGY.&amp;ots=5lGtq1nu10&amp;sig=SaTsv0eHYvmGje3GvsrwaUGZFzk

[Accessed March 4, 2012].

Riedel G, Micheau J, Lam AGM, Roloff EL, Martin SJ, Bridge H, Hoz LD (1999) Reversible

neural inactivation reveals hippocampal participation in several memory processes. Nature

Neuroscience 2.

Rochefort C, Gheusi G, Vincent J-D, Lledo P-M (2002) Enriched odor exposure increases the

number of newborn neurons in the adult olfactory bulb and improves odor memory. The

Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 22:2679–2689

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11923433.

Rochefort C, Lledo P-M (2005) Short-term survival of newborn neurons in the adult olfactory

bulb after exposure to a complex odor environment. The European journal of neuroscience

22:2863–2870 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16324121 [Accessed

March 30, 2012].

Rogers JH (1987) Calretinin: a gene for a novel calcium-binding protein expressed principally in

neurons. The Journal of cell biology 105:1343–1353 Available at:

Page 212: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

199

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2114790&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Rolls ET (1996) A theory of hippocampal function in memory. Hippocampus 6:601–620.

Rosen JB, Donley MP (2006) Animal studies of amygdala function in fear and uncertainty:

relevance to human research. Biological psychology 73:49–60 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16500019 [Accessed March 16, 2012].

Rosenbaum RS, Priselac S, Köhler S, Black SE, Gao F, Nadel L, Moscovitch M (2000) Remote

spatial memory in an amnesic person with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions. Nature

neuroscience 3:1044–1048 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11017178.

Rudy J, O‘Reilly R (1999) Contextual fear conditioning, conjunctive representations, pattern

completion, and the hippocampus. Behav Neurosci 113:867–880.

Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R (2005) Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a

form of associative learning. Science (New York, NY) 308:83–88 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15746389 [Accessed March 14, 2012].

Ryan L, Nadel L, Keil K, Putnam K, Schnyer D, Trouard T, Moscovitch M (2001) Hippocampal

complex and retrieval of recent and very remote autobiographical memories: evidence from

functional magnetic resonance imaging in neurologically intact people. Hippocampus

11:707–714 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11811665 [Accessed

March 13, 2012].

Sagar H, Cohen N, Corkin S, Growdon J (1985) Dissociations among processes in remote

memory. Ann NY Acad Sci 444:533–535 Available at:

http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1986-17660-001 [Accessed January 30, 2012].

Saghatelyan A, de Chevigny A, Schachner M, Lledo P-M (2004) Tenascin-R mediates activity-

dependent recruitment of neuroblasts in the adult mouse forebrain. Nature neuroscience

7:347–356 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15034584 [Accessed April

5, 2012].

Page 213: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

200

Sahay A, Hen R (2007) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nature neuroscience

10:1110–1115 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17726477 [Accessed

March 4, 2012].

Sahay A, Scobie K, Hill A, O‘Carroll C, Kheirbek M, Burghardt N, Fenton A, Dranovsky A,

Hen R (2011a) Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern

separation. Nature 472:466–470 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3084370&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed June 13, 2011].

Sahay A, Wilson D, Hen R (2011b) Pattern separation: a common function for new neurons in

hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron 70:582–588 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3109085&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed July 4, 2011].

Saint-Cyr J, Taylor A, Lang A (1988) Procedural learning and neostyrial dysfunction in man.

Brain 111:941 Available at: http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/111/4/941.short

[Accessed March 12, 2012].

Saito M, Iwawaki T, Taya C, Yonekawa H, Noda M, Inui Y, Mekada E, Kimata Y, Tsuru a,

Kohno K (2001) Diphtheria toxin receptor-mediated conditional and targeted cell ablation

in transgenic mice. Nature biotechnology 19:746–750 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11479567.

Saxe M, Battaglia F, Wang J, Malleret G, David D, Monckton J, Garcia A, Sofroniew M, Kandel

E, Santarelli L, Hen R, Drew M (2006) Ablation of hippocampal neurogenesis impairs

contextual fear conditioning and synaptic plasticity in the dentate gyrus. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103:17501–17506 Available

at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1859958&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Schacter D (1990) Perceptual Representation Systems and Implicit Memory: Toward a

Resolution of the Multiple Memory Systems Debate. Ann NY Acad Sci 608:543–567.

Page 214: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

201

Schallert T, Fleming SM, Leasure JL, Tillerson JL, Bland ST (2000) CNS plasticity and

assessment of forelimb sensorimotor outcome in unilateral rat models of stroke, cortical

ablation, parkinsonism and spinal cord injury. Neuropharmacology 39:777–787 Available

at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10699444.

Scharfman H, Goodman J, Macleod A, Phani S, Antonelli C, Croll S (2005) Increased

neurogenesis and the ectopic granule cells after intrahippocampal BDNF infusion in adult

rats. Experimental neurology 192:348–356 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15755552 [Accessed March 23, 2012].

Scharfman H, Kunkel DD, Schwartzkroin P a (1990) Synaptic connections of dentate granule

cells and hilar neurons: results of paired intracellular recordings and intracellular

horseradish peroxidase injections. Neuroscience 37:693–707 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2247219.

Schenk F, Morris R (1985) Dissociation between components of spatial memory in rats after

recovery from the effects of retrohippocampal lesions. Experimental brain research

Experimentelle Hirnforschung Expérimentation cérébrale 58:11–28 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3987843.

Schmidt-Hieber C, Jonas P, Bischofberger J (2004) Enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly

generated granule cells of the adult hippocampus. Nature 429:184–187 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15107864.

Schoppa NE, Urban NN (2003) Dendritic processing within olfactory bulb circuits. Trends in

Neurosciences 26:501–506 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0166223603002285 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Scobie K, Hall B, Wilke S, Klemenhagen K, Fujii-Kuriyama Y, Ghosh A, Hen R, Sahay A

(2009) Krüppel-like factor 9 is necessary for late-phase neuronal maturation in the

developing dentate gyrus and during adult hippocampal neurogenesis. The Journal of

neuroscience 29:9875–9887 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2753873&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed September 2, 2011].

Page 215: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

202

Scoville W (1954) The limbic lobe in man. Journal of Neurosurgery 11:64 Available at:

http://ukpmc.ac.uk/abstract/MED/13131095 [Accessed January 30, 2012].

Scoville W, Milner B (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal

of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 20:11 Available at:

http://jnnp.bmj.com/content/20/1/11.short [Accessed January 30, 2012].

Seaberg RM, van der Kooy D (2002) Adult rodent neurogenic regions: the ventricular

subependyma contains neural stem cells, but the dentate gyrus contains restricted

progenitors. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for

Neuroscience 22:1784–1793 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11880507.

Seaberg RM, van der Kooy D (2003) Stem and progenitor cells: the premature desertion of

rigorous definitions. Trends in Neurosciences 26:125–131 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0166223603000316 [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Seki T (2002) Expression patterns of immature neuronal markers PSA-NCAM, CRMP-4 and

NeuroD in the hippocampus of young adult and aged rodents. Journal of neuroscience

research 70:327–334 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12391592

[Accessed March 9, 2012].

Seki T, Arai Y (1993a) Distribution and possible roles of the highly polysialylated neural cell

adhesion molecule ( NCAM-H ) in the developing and adult central nervous system.

Neuroscience 17:265–290.

Seki T, Arai Y (1993b) Highly polysialylated neural cell adhesion molecule (NCAM-H) is

expressed by newly generated granule cells in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat. The Journal

of neuroscience 13:2351–2358 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7684771.

Selden NR, Everitt BJ, Jarrard LE, Robbins TW (1991) Complementary roles for the amygdala

and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience

42:335–350 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1832750.

Page 216: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

203

Seri B, García-Verdugo JM, Collado-Morente L, McEwen BS, Alvarez-Buylla A (2004) Cell

types, lineage, and architecture of the germinal zone in the adult dentate gyrus. The Journal

of comparative neurology 478:359–378 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15384070 [Accessed March 22, 2012].

Seri B, García-Verdugo JM, McEwen BS, Alvarez-Buylla a (2001) Astrocytes give rise to new

neurons in the adult mammalian hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official

journal of the Society for Neuroscience 21:7153–7160 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11549726.

Shapiro ML, Tanila H, Eichenbaum H (1997) Cues that hippocampal place cells encode:

dynamic and hierarchical representation of local and distal stimuli. Hippocampus 7:624–

642 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9443059.

Sherrington C (1892) LOCALISATION OF THE KNEE-JERK. The Brittish Medical Journal.

Shimamura AP (1986) Priming effects in amnesia: Evidence for a dissociable memory function.

The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38:619–644 Available at:

http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/14640748608401617 [Accessed March 11,

2012].

Shipley MT, Ennis M (1996) Functional organization of olfactory system. Journal of

neurobiology 30:123–176 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8727988.

Shors T (2004) Learning during stressful times. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, NY)

11:137–144 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15054128 [Accessed

March 27, 2012].

Shors T, Miesegaes G, Beylin A, Zhao M, Rydel T, Gould E (2001) Neurogenesis in the adult is

involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature 410:372–376 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11268214.

Shors TJ, Townsend D a, Zhao M, Kozorovitskiy Y, Gould E (2002) Neurogenesis may relate to

some but not all types of hippocampal-dependent learning. Hippocampus 12:578–584

Available at:

Page 217: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

204

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3289536&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 2, 2012].

Sierra A, Encinas J, Deudero J, Chancey J, Enikolopov G, Overstreet-Wadiche L, Tsirka S,

Maletic-Savatic M (2010) Microglia shape adult hippocampal neurogenesis through

apoptosis-coupled phagocytosis. Cell stem cell 7:483–495 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20887954 [Accessed July 19, 2011].

Silva A, Paylor R, Wehner J, Tonegawa S (1992a) Impaired spatial learning in alpha-calcium-

calmodulin kinase II mutant mice. Science (New York, NY) 257:206–211 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1321493.

Silva A, Stevens C, Tonegawa S (1992b) Deficient Hippocampal Long-Term Potentiation in a-

calcium-calmodulin kinase II mutant mice. Science 257:201–206 Available at:

http://www.sciencemag.org/content/257/5067/201.short [Accessed March 13, 2012].

Sisti H, Glass A, Shors T (2007) Neurogenesis and the spacing effect: learning over time

enhances memory and the survival of new neurons. Learning & memory (Cold Spring

Harbor, NY) 14:368–375 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1876761&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 15, 2012].

Skelton RW, McNamara RK (1992) Bilateral knife cuts to the perforant path disrupt spatial

learning in the Morris water maze. Hippocampus 2:73–80 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1308173.

Sloan N, Jasper H (1950) Studies of the regulatory functions of the limbic cortex.

Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophys 2:317–327 Available at:

http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0013469450900630 [Accessed January 28,

2012].

Sloviter RS (1989) Calcium-binding protein (calbindin-D28k) and parvalbumin

immunocytochemistry: localization in the rat hippocampus with specific reference to the

selective vulnerability of hippocampal neurons to seizure activity. The Journal of

Page 218: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

205

comparative neurology 280:183–196 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2925892.

Snyder J, Choe J, Clifford M, Jeurling S, Hurley P, Brown A, Kamhi J, Cameron H (2009a)

Adult-born hippocampal neurons are more numerous, faster maturing, and more involved in

behavior in rats than in mice. The Journal of neuroscience 29:14484–14495 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2830901&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 15, 2012].

Snyder J, Glover L, Sanzone K, Kamhi J, Cameron H (2009b) The effects of exercise and stress

on the survival and maturation of adult-generated granule cells. Hippocampus 19:898–906

Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2755652&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 6, 2012].

Snyder J, Hong N, McDonald R, Wojtowicz J (2005) A role for adult neurogenesis in spatial

long-term memory. Neuroscience 130:843–852 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15652983 [Accessed March 7, 2012].

Snyder J, Kee N, Wojtowicz J (2001) Effects of adult neurogenesis on synaptic plasticity in the

rat dentate gyrus. Journal of neurophysiology 85:2423–2431 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11387388.

Snyder J, Soumier A, Brewer M, Pickel J, Cameron H (2011) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis

buffers stress responses and depressive behaviour. Nature:3–7.

Squire L (1992) Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys,

and humans. Psychological review 99:195 Available at:

http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/99/2/195/ [Accessed March 11, 2012].

Squire L, Knowlton B, Musen G (1993) The structure and organization of memory. Annual

review of psychology 44:453–495 Available at:

http://www.annualreviews.org/doi/pdf/10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.002321 [Accessed

March 8, 2012].

Page 219: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

206

Squire LR (2004) Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective.

Neurobiology of learning and memory 82:171–177 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15464402 [Accessed March 3, 2012].

Squire LR, Alvarez P (1995) Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological

perspective. Current opinion in neurobiology 5:169–177 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7620304.

Squire LR, Ojemann JG, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Videen TO, Raichle ME (1992) Activation of

the hippocampus in normal humans: a functional anatomical study of memory. Proceedings

of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 89:1837–1841.

Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S (1988) Memory: brain systems and behavior. Trends in neurosciences

11:170–175 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2469186.

Stanton PK, Sarvey JM, Harmon M (1984) BLOCKADE OF LONG-TERM potentiation in IN

RAT HIPPOCAMPAL CA1 REGION BY INHIBITORS OF PROTEIN SYNTHESIS.

Journal of Neuroscience 4:3080–3088.

Steiner B, Kronenberg G, Jessberger S, Brandt M, Reuter K, Kempermann G (2004) Differential

regulation of gliogenesis in the context of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Glia

46:41–52 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14999812 [Accessed March

9, 2012].

Stenmark H, Olsnes S, Sandvig K (1988) Requirement of specific receptors for efficient

translocation of diphtheria toxin A fragment across the plasma membrane. The Journal of

biological chemistry 263:13449–13455 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3417666.

Steward O (1976) Topographic organization of the projections from the entorhinal area to the

hippocampal formation of the rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 167:285–314

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1270625.

Stone J (1991) Paul Broca and the first craniotomy based on cerebral localization. J Neurosurg

75:154–159.

Page 220: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

207

Stone S, Teixeira C, DeVito L, Zaslavsky K, Josselyn S, Lozano A, Frankland P (2011a)

Stimulation of Entorhinal Cortex Promotes Adult Neurogenesis and Facilitates Spatial

Memory. Journal of Neuroscience 31:13469–13484 Available at:

http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3100-11.2011 [Accessed

September 22, 2011].

Stone S, Teixeira C, Zaslavsky K, Wheeler A, Martinez-Canabal A, Wang A, Sakaguchi M,

Lozano A, Frankland P (2011b) Functional convergence of developmentally and adult-

generated granule cells in dentate gyrus circuits supporting hippocampus-dependent

memory. Hippocampus 21:1348–1362 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20824726 [Accessed March 19, 2012].

Stopfer M, Bhagavan S, Smith BH, Laurent G (1997) Impaired odour discrimination on

desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature 390:70–74 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9363891.

Sugita N (1918) COMPARATIVE STUDIES ON THE GROWTH OF THE CEREBRAL

CORTEX. Journal of Comparative Neurology 29:61–117.

Sultan S, Mandairon N, Kermen F, Garcia S, Sacquet J, Didier A (2010) Learning-dependent

neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb determines long-term olfactory memory. FASEB journal

24:2355–2363 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20215526 [Accessed

March 10, 2012].

Sun J, Sun J, Ming G, Song H (2011) Epigenetic regulation of neurogenesis in the adult

mammalian brain. The European journal of neuroscience 33:1087–1093 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3076719&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 19, 2012].

Sutherland R, Rudy J (1989) Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal

formation in learning, memory, and amnesia. Psychobiology 17:129–144 Available at:

http://www.psychonomic.org/search/view.cgi?id=7738.

Page 221: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

208

Sutherland R, Whishaw I, Kolb B (1983) A behavioural analysis of spatial localization following

electrolytic, kainate-or colchicine-induced damage to the hippocampal formation in the rat.

Behavioural Brain Research 7:133–153.

Sutherland RJ, Weisend MP, Mumby D, Astur RS, Hanlon FM, Koerner a, Thomas MJ, Wu Y,

Moses SN, Cole C, Hamilton D a, Hoesing JM (2001) Retrograde amnesia after

hippocampal damage: recent vs. remote memories in two tasks. Hippocampus 11:27–42

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11261770.

Tanaka T, Serneo FF, Tseng HC, Kulkarni AB, Tsai LH, Gleeson JG (2004) Cdk5

phosphorylation of doublecortin ser297 regulates its effect on neuronal migration. Neuron

41:215–227 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14741103.

Tanila H, Shapiro ML, Eichenbaum H (1997) Discordance of spatial representation in ensembles

of hippocampal place cells. Hippocampus 7:613–623 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9443058.

Tashiro A, Makino H, Gage F (2007) Experience-specific functional modification of the dentate

gyrus through adult neurogenesis: a critical period during an immature stage. The Journal of

neuroscience 27:3252–3259 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17376985

[Accessed March 18, 2012].

Tashiro A, Sandler V, Toni N, Zhao C, Gage F (2006) NMDA-receptor-mediated, cell-specific

integration of new neurons in adult dentate gyrus. Nature 442:929–933 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16906136 [Accessed March 1, 2012].

Taube JS, Kesslak JP, Cotman CW (1992) Lesions of the rat postsubiculum impair performance

on spatial tasks. Behavioral and neural biology 57:131–143 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1586352.

Teixeira C, Pomedli S, Maei H, Kee N, Frankland P (2006) Involvement of the anterior cingulate

cortex in the expression of remote spatial memory. The Journal of neuroscience 26:7555–

7564 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16855083 [Accessed March 7,

2012].

Page 222: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

209

Teng E, Squire LR (1999) Memory for places learned long ago is intact after hippocampal

damage. Nature 400:675–677 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10458163.

Terrian D, Johnston D, Claiborne B, Ansah-Yiadom R, Strittmatter W, Rea M (1988) Glutamate

and dynorphin release from a subcellular fraction enriched in hippocampal mossy fiber

synaptosomes. Brain research bulletin 21:343–351 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2905627.

Thorndike E (1998) Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associate processes in

animals. American Psychologist Available at:

http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/amp/53/10/1125/ [Accessed February 3, 2012].

Toni N, Laplagne D, Zhao C, Lombardi G, Ribak C, Gage F, Schinder A (2008) Neurons born in

the adult dentate gyrus form functional synapses with target cells. Nature neuroscience

11:901–907 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2572641&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed February 29, 2012].

Toni N, Teng E, Bushong E, Aimone J, Zhao C, Consiglio A, van Praag H, Martone M, Ellisman

M, Gage F (2007) Synapse formation on neurons born in the adult hippocampus. Nature

neuroscience 10:727–734 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17486101

[Accessed March 8, 2012].

Tozuka Y, Fukuda S, Namba T, Seki T, Hisatsune T (2005) GABAergic excitation promotes

neuronal differentiation in adult hippocampal progenitor cells. Neuron 47:803–815

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16157276 [Accessed March 8, 2012].

Treves A, Rolls ET (1992a) Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input

systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus 2:189–199 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1308182.

Treves A, Rolls ET (1992b) Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input

systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus 2:189–199 Available at:

Page 223: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

210

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.450020209/abstract [Accessed January 25,

2012].

Treves A, Rolls ET (1994) Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory.

Hippocampus 4:374–391 Available at:

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.450040319/abstract [Accessed January 25,

2012].

Tronel S, Belnoue L, Grosjean N, Revest J-M, Piazza P-V, Koehl M, Abrous DN (2012) Adult-

born neurons are necessary for extended contextual discrimination. Hippocampus 22:292–

298 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21049483 [Accessed March 12,

2012].

Tulving E (1987) Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Human neurobiology 6

Available at: http://alicekim.ca/HumanNeurobiol87.pdf [Accessed March 11, 2012].

Tulving E (1992) Memory systems and the brain. Clin Neuropharmacol 15 Suppl 1:327A–328A

Available at: http://www.annualreviews.org/doi/pdf/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.599

[Accessed March 11, 2012].

Tulving E (2002) EPISODIC MEMORY: From Mind to Brain. Annual review of psychology

Available at:

http://www.annualreviews.org/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114

[Accessed March 11, 2012].

Tulving E, Schacter D (1990) Priming and human memory systems. Science 247:301–306

Available at: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/247/4940/301.short [Accessed March 11,

2012].

Urban N, Henze D, Barrionuevo G (2001) Revisiting the role of the hippocampal mossy fiber

synapse. Hippocampus 11:408–417 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11530845.

Valley MT, Mullen TR, Schultz LC, Sagdullaev BT, Firestein S (2009) Ablation of mouse adult

neurogenesis alters olfactory bulb structure and olfactory fear conditioning. Frontiers in

Page 224: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

211

neuroscience 3:51 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2858604&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 31, 2012].

Vanderwolf CH, Cain DP (1994) The behavioral neurobiology of learning and memory: a

conceptual reorientation. Brain research Brain research reviews 19:264–297 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7820133.

Vertes RP, Fortin WJ, Crane AM (1999) Projections of the Median Raphe Nucleus. Journal of

Comparative Neurology 582:555–582.

Victor M, Yakovlev P (1955) SS Korsakoff‘s psychic disorder in conjunction with peripheral

neuritis A translation of Korsakoff's original article with brief comments on the author and

his contribution to clinical medicine. Neurology 5:394–406 Available at:

http://web.sfn.org/skins/main/pdf/HistoryofNeuroscience/korsakoff.pdf [Accessed January

28, 2012].

Vincis R, Gschwend O, Bhaukaurally K, Beroud J, Carleton A (2012) Dense representation of

natural odorants in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature Neuroscience 15:537–539 Available

at: http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nn.3057 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Viskontas I, McAndrews M, Moscovitch M (2002) Memory for famous people in patients with

unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. Neuropsychology 16:472–480 Available at:

http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0894-4105.16.4.472 [Accessed March 13, 2012].

Vogt M a, Chourbaji S, Brandwein C, Dormann C, Sprengel R, Gass P (2008) Suitability of

tamoxifen-induced mutagenesis for behavioral phenotyping. Experimental neurology

211:25–33 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18329640 [Accessed March

18, 2012].

Vollmayr B, Simonis C, Weber S, Gass P, Henn F (2003) Reduced cell proliferation in the

dentate gyrusis not correlated with the development of learned helplessness. Biological

Psychiatry 54:1035–1040 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0006322303005274 [Accessed March 7, 2012].

Page 225: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

212

Walsh TJ, Schulz DW, Tilson H a, Schmechel DE (1986) Colchicine-induced granule cell loss in

rat hippocampus: selective behavioral and histological alterations. Brain research 398:23–36

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3801898.

Wang S, Teixeira C, Wheeler A, Frankland P (2009) The precision of remote context memories

does not require the hippocampus. Nature neuroscience 12:253–255 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19182794 [Accessed July 19, 2011].

Warner-Schmidt JL, Madsen TM, Duman RS (2008) Electroconvulsive seizure restores

neurogenesis and hippocampus-dependent fear memory after disruption by irradiation. The

European journal of neuroscience 27:1485–1493 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18336568 [Accessed March 27, 2012].

Watts DJ, Strogatz SH (1998) Collective dynamics of ―small-world‖ networks. Nature 393:440–

442 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9623998.

Wei L, Meaney MJ, Duman RS, Kaffman A (2011) Affiliative behavior requires juvenile, but

not adult neurogenesis. The Journal of neuroscience 31:14335–14345 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3204413&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 18, 2012].

Weissman IL (2000) Stem cells: units of development, units of regeneration, and units in

evolution. Cell 100:157–168 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10647940.

Wenk GL (2004) Assessment of spatial memory using the radial arm maze and Morris water

maze. Current protocols in neuroscience / editorial board, Jacqueline N Crawley . [et al]

Chapter 8:Unit 8.5A Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18428607.

Whitman MC, Greer C a (2007) Synaptic integration of adult-generated olfactory bulb granule

cells: basal axodendritic centrifugal input precedes apical dendrodendritic local circuits. The

Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 27:9951–9961

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17855609 [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Page 226: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

213

Williams S, Ryan C, Jacobson C (2008) Agrin and neuregulin, expanding roles and implications

for therapeutics. Biotechnology advances 26:187–201 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18222620 [Accessed April 10, 2012].

Wilson D a, Sullivan RM (2011) Cortical processing of odor objects. Neuron 72:506–519

Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3223720&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Wilson D a. (2009) Pattern Separation and Completion in Olfaction. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences 1170:306–312 Available at: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1749-

6632.2009.04017.x [Accessed May 6, 2012].

Wilson D a., Stevenson RJ (2003) The fundamental role of memory in olfactory perception.

Trends in Neurosciences 26:243–247 Available at:

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0166223603000766 [Accessed March 27,

2012].

Wilson RI (2008) Neural and behavioral mechanisms of olfactory perception. Current opinion in

neurobiology 18:408–412 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2596880&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 9, 2012].

Wiltgen BJ, Sanders MJ, Anagnostaras SG, Sage JR, Fanselow MS (2006) Context fear learning

in the absence of the hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the

Society for Neuroscience 26:5484–5491 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16707800 [Accessed April 5, 2012].

Wiltgen BJ, Silva AJ (2007) Memory for context becomes less specific with time. Learning &

memory (Cold Spring Harbor, NY) 14:313–317 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17522020 [Accessed March 12, 2012].

Winner B, Cooper-Kuhn CM, Aigner R, Winkler J, Kuhn HG (2002) Long-term survival and

cell death of newly generated neurons in the adult rat olfactory bulb. European Journal of

Page 227: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

214

Neuroscience 16:1681–1689 Available at: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1046/j.1460-

9568.2002.02238.x [Accessed March 29, 2012].

Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Sekeres M (2007) Memory consolidation or transformation: context

manipulation and hippocampal representations of memory. Nature neuroscience 10:555–

557 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17396121 [Accessed March 16,

2012].

Winocur G, Wojtowicz J, Sekeres M, Snyder J, Wang S (2006) Inhibition of neurogenesis

interferes with hippocampus-dependent memory function. Hippocampus 16:296–304

Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16411241 [Accessed March 10, 2012].

Wiskott L, Rasch MJ, Kempermann G (2006) A functional hypothesis for adult hippocampal

neurogenesis: avoidance of catastrophic interference in the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus

16:329–343 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16435309 [Accessed

March 11, 2012].

Wojtowicz JM (2006) Irradiation as an experimental tool in studies of adult neurogenesis.

Hippocampus 16:261–266 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16435311

[Accessed April 5, 2012].

Wrobel CJ, Wright DC, Dedrick RL, Youle RJ (1990) Diphtheria toxin effects on brain-tumor

xenografts. Implications for protein-based brain-tumor chemotherapy. Journal of

neurosurgery 72:946–950 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2159988.

Wu W, Wong K, Chen J, Jiang Z, Dupuis S, Wu JY, Rao Y (1999) Directional guidance of

neuronal migration in the olfactory system by the protein Slit. Nature 400:331–336

Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2041931&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Xavier GF, Oliveira-Filho FJ, Santos AM (1999) Dentate gyrus-selective colchicine lesion and

disruption of performance in spatial tasks: difficulties in ―place strategy‖ because of a lack

of flexibility in the use of environmental cues? Hippocampus 9:668–681 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10641760.

Page 228: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

215

Yamaguchi M, Mori K (2005) Critical period for sensory experience-dependent survival of

newly generated granule cells in the adult mouse olfactory bulb. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102:9697–9702 Available

at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1157102&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract.

Yasuda M, Johnson-Venkatesh EM, Zhang H, Parent JM, Sutton M a, Umemori H (2011)

Multiple forms of activity-dependent competition refine hippocampal circuits in vivo.

Neuron 70:1128–1142 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3124368&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 7, 2012].

Yokoi M, Mori K, Nakanishi S (1995) Refinement of odor molecule tuning by dendrodendritic

synaptic inhibition in the olfactory bulb. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

of the United States of America 92:3371–3375 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=42168&tool=pmcentrez&render

type=abstract.

Yoshikawa K (2000) Cell cycle regulators in neural stem cells and postmitotic neurons.

Neuroscience research 37:1–14 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10802339.

Young S, Bohenek D, Fanselow M (1994) NMDA processes mediate anterograde amnesia of

contextual fear conditioning induced by hippocampal damage: immunization against

amnesia by context preexposure. Behav Neurosci 108:19–29.

Zambrowicz BP, Imamoto a, Fiering S, Herzenberg L a, Kerr WG, Soriano P (1997) Disruption

of overlapping transcripts in the ROSA beta geo 26 gene trap strain leads to widespread

expression of beta-galactosidase in mouse embryos and hematopoietic cells. Proceedings of

the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 94:3789–3794 Available

at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=20519&tool=pmcentrez&render

type=abstract.

Page 229: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

216

Zhang C, Zou Y, He W, Gage F, Evans R (2008) A role for adult TLX-positive neural stem cells

in learning and behaviour. Nature 451:1004–1007 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18235445 [Accessed March 17, 2012].

Zhao C, Deng W, Gage F (2008) Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis.

Cell 132:645–660 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18295581 [Accessed

February 29, 2012].

Zhao C, Teng E, Summers R, Ming G, Gage F (2006) Distinct morphological stages of dentate

granule neuron maturation in the adult mouse hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience

26:3–11 Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16399667 [Accessed March 1,

2012].

Zhao M, Li D, Shimazu K, Zhou Y-X, Lu B, Deng C-X (2007) Fibroblast growth factor

receptor-1 is required for long-term potentiation, memory consolidation, and neurogenesis.

Biological psychiatry 62:381–390 Available at:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17239352 [Accessed March 18, 2012].

Zhou Y, Won J, Karlsson MG, Zhou M, Rogerson T, Balaji J, Neve R, Poirazi P, Silva AJ

(2009) CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in

the amygdala. Nature neuroscience 12:1438–1443 Available at:

http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2783698&tool=pmcentrez&ren

dertype=abstract [Accessed March 7, 2012].

Zigova T, Pencea V, Wiegand SJ, Luskin MB (1998) Intraventricular Administration of BDNF

increases the number of newly generated neurons in the Adult Olfactory Bulb. Molecular

and Cellular Neuroscience 11:234–245.

Zola-Morgan S (1995) LOCALIZATION OF BRAIN FUNCTION: The Legacy of Franz Joseph

Gall (1758-1828). Annu Rev Neurosci 18:359–383.

Page 230: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

217

Appendix 1

A critical time window for adult-born dentate granule cells in hippocampal function

revealed by studies of optically-controlled newborn neurons

Yan Gu1, Maithe Arruda-Carvalho

3,4, Jia Wang

1, Stephen Janoschka

1,2, Sheena Josselyn

3-5, Paul

Frankland3-5,*

and Shaoyu Ge1,2,*

1Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, SUNY at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794

2Program in Neuroscience, SUNY at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794

3Program in Neurosciences and Mental Health, Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Ave,

Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M5G 1X8

4Institute of Medical Science,

University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M5S 1A8

5Department of Physiology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M5S 1A8

*Correspondence can be addressed to:

Shaoyu Ge Ph.D.

Department of Neurobiology and Behavior

SUNY at Stony Brook

Stony Brook, NY 11794, USA

E-mail: [email protected]

Tel: 631-632-8799

Fax: 631-632-6661

Or

Paul Frankland Ph.D.

Hospital for Sick Children

Neurosciences & Mental Health

Hospital for Sick Children

Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M5G 1X8

E-mail: [email protected]

Tel: 416-813-7654 x1823

Fax: 416-813-7717

Page 231: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

218

Abstract

Neurogenesis persists into adulthood in the hippocampus. Accumulating evidence

suggests that global depletion of adult neurogenesis influences hippocampal function and

the timing of the depletion impacts the severity of the observed deficits. However,

behavioral roles of adult-born neurons during their circuit integration, specifically during

the establishment of projections to CA3 pyramidal neurons, remain largely unknown. Here

we combined retroviral and optogenetic approaches to birth-date and reversibly excite or

inhibit a group of adult-born neurons. We show that young adult-born neurons form

functional synapses on target CA3 pyramidal neurons as early as 2 weeks after birth, and

that this projection to the CA3 area becomes stable by 4 weeks in age. Newborn neurons at

this age exhibit enhanced plasticity compared to other stages. Strikingly, we found that

reversibly silencing this cohort of ~4 week-old cells after training, but not cells of other

ages, substantially disrupted retrieval of hippocampus-dependent memory. Our results

identify a restricted time window for adult-born neurons during which time they exhibit an

essential role in hippocampal memory retrieval.

Introduction

The adult hippocampus continues to give rise to several thousand new dentate granule

cells each day and less than half of them survive 1-3

. Since the discovery of neurogenesis in the

adult brain, the question of how newborn neurons contribute to hippocampal function has been

raised. The hippocampus is important for many forms of memory 4-7

, and accumulating evidence

from studies using global perturbation or ablation of adult hippocampal neurogenesis has

revealed deficits in some forms of hippocampal memory in rodents 8-12

. As the morphological

and physiological phenotypes of adult-born cells change dramatically as they mature, they may

play distinct roles at different stages following integration into hippocampal circuits.

Accordingly, while manipulations of adult neurogenesis may disrupt hippocampal memory

function, it is not clear whether the observed memory deficits are due to loss of immature vs.

mature adult-born neurons or, more simply, due to a global disruption of neurogenesis. Some

recent work from our and several other groups shows that surviving adult-born dentate granule

(DG) cells extend dendrites and receive functional input from the existing neural circuits as early

as 2 weeks after birth 13-17

. In contrast to many studies on input synapses, little is known about

the establishment of functional projections of adult-born DG cells to pyramidal neurons in CA3.

Page 232: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

219

Anatomical studies have established that young newborn DG cells gradually extend axonal fibers

into the CA3 area. Two to three weeks after birth, terminals of these axons form bouton-like

structures similar to mature granule cells 18-21

, suggesting that they may form functional

connections 3. A recent study demonstrated that mature (two months old) newborn neurons

exhibit synaptic responses in the CA3 area 19

. However, the precise timing for functional output

synapse formation and maturation remains unknown.

We previously found that input (dendritic) synapses of adult-born neurons show

enhanced plasticity between 4-6 weeks after birth compared to other stages 22

, during which time

they presumably also exhibit heightened intrinsic excitability 16,23

. This coincides with the timing

when newborn neurons are recruited into adult neural circuits mediating behavior 24-27

. These

findings suggest that a cohort of young adult-born neurons of similar age may form a

hypersensitive unit that preferentially responds to stimuli during hippocampal memory

formation. A related interesting question is whether output synapses, if formed and functional,

also exhibit heightened plasticity.

Combining retroviral birth-dating and gene delivery 13-14

with optogenetic stimulation28

,

here we examine the behavioral roles of adult-born neurons during their output circuit

development. We found that adult-born DG cells establish functional synapses with CA3

pyramidal neurons as early as 2 weeks after birth and synaptic responses remain stable by 4

weeks in age. Fully established output synapses of adult-born neurons exhibit enhanced plasticity

at ~4 weeks after birth. Importantly, optogenetic silencing of a cohort of 4 but not 2 or 8 weeks

old newborn neurons substantially impacted the retrieval of hippocampal dependent memory

following the completion of training. These data characterize the development of output circuit

function for adult-born DG cells, revealing a precise time window where newborn neurons

exhibit enhanced plasticity at CA3 synapses and play a critical role in processing hippocampal

memories.

Results

The development of functional output synapses of adult-born neurons

To examine a role of adult-born neurons in hippocampal function during their circuit

integration, we first determined the timing for newborn neurons to establish functional

projections to CA3 pyramidal neurons. We previously found that adult-born DG cells start to

Page 233: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

220

receive functional glutamatergic inputs as early as 2 weeks after birth 14

. Anatomical studies

show that adult-born DG cells gradually form synapses on CA3 neurons 18-21

. However, because

of the sparse targeting of DG cells and the difficulty in electrically stimulating a population of

newborn neurons simultaneously, little is known about the development of functional synaptic

projections of adult-born neurons within the hippocampal trisynaptic circuit 3. Here, we elected

to use an optogenetic method to excite a group of adult-born DG cells simultaneously to

determine the development of functional output circuit in the CA3 area 19,28

. We constructed a

retroviral vector expressing EGFP-tagged Channelrhodopsin 2 (ChR2-EGFP), a gene encoding a

light-sensitive channel 28-29

, to birth-date and specifically express ChR2-EGFP in adult-born

hippocampal neurons 13-14,19

. ChR2-EGFP retroviruses were microinjected into the hilus of the

DG in adult mice and acute brain sections were prepared 1-8 weeks post infection (wpi) using

previously described methods (Supplememtary Fig. 1; Fig. 1a; Methods; 14

). As expected, ChR2-

EGFP was specifically expressed in newborn neurons (Supplementary Fig. 2), and brief, 5 ms

pulses of blue light reliably induced action potentials in infected DG cells but not in non-infected

neighbors (Fig. 1b), indicating that this population of cells could be optically controlled. Because

infected cells were not illuminated during development, and ChR2 remained silent, we did not

observe changes in intrinsic properties in newborn neurons expressing ChR2 at different ages

compared to those expressing EGFP only (Supplementary Table 1), consistent with previous

findings where optogenes have been expressed in other cell types 28

.

Next, we used whole-cell recording to examine postsynaptic currents in CA3 pyramidal

neurons while optically stimulating ChR2-EGFP positive mossy fibers of newborn DG cells

(Fig. 1c). Postsynaptic activity increased with the age of infected adult born DG cells (Fig. 1d).

At 1 wpi, a time-point when mossy fibers have not yet reached the CA3 region, no postsynaptic

responses were observed following optical stimulation. However, at 2 wpi and older (a stage

where mossy fibers have reached the CA3 region), stimulation produced excitatory postsynaptic

responses (EPSCs). Maximal responses were observed at 4 wpi (Fig. 1d, e). These optically

evoked EPSCs, with a latency of ~4ms, were blocked by an AMPA receptor antagonist (CNQX)

and metabotropic glutamate receptor agonists (L-AP4 and LY354740, specifically blocking

mossy fiber-CA3 synapses 30

), indicating a glutamatergic mono-synaptic response (Fig. 1f, g).

These results show young adult-born DG cells form functional synapses with CA3 target cells

which become stable around 4 weeks after birth. These current results, together with previous

Page 234: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

221

studies showing newborn neurons to be functionally innervated by entorhinal cortical projections

13-15, indicate that adult-born DG cells fully integrate into the hippocampal trisynaptic circuits by

~4 weeks of age.

Heightened plasticity of output synapses of young adult-born neurons

We next characterized functional properties of these output synapses. Plasticity, such as

long-term potentiation (LTP), has been considered as an essential functional property of a

synapse 31

. Accordingly we next examined synaptic plasticity of young adult-born DG cells in

the CA3 using a more physiological relevant, but milder induction paradigm, as previously

described 16,22

. To stimulate CA3 synapses of a group of adult-born neurons optically at high

frequency, we replaced the ChR2 construct with ChR2 variants ChETA-EYFP 32

and ChIEF-

dTomato 33

, which respond more reliably to high frequency optical stimulation.Because we

reserved dTomato for labeling adult-born neurons, we selected ChIEF-dTomato for the following

study. As expected, 3, 4 and 8 week-old adult-born DG cells expressing ChIEF responded

reliably to high frequency optical stimulation to their soma (Supplementary Fig. 3a, b).

Recordings from axonal boutons indicated that following optical stimulation action potentials

propagated reliably to the axonal terminals (Supplementary Fig. 3c, d). To assess global circuit

output of newborn neurons, we then implanted an optic fiber in the DG to deliver optical

stimulation and recorded field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) in the CA3 region in

vivo (Fig. 2a; Supplementary Fig. 4a-c). fEPSPs were successfully induced by short pulses of

optical stimulation (Fig. 2b). Similar to postsynaptic activity in vitro (Fig. 1c), fEPSPs recorded

in vivo were blocked by local application of CNQX (Fig. 2b). As expected, high frequency

optical stimulation (~5 mW/mm2 intensity, 50Hz, 2s) evoked consistent fEPSPs (Fig. 2b).

Together with the reliable responses to theta-burst optical stimulation recorded from the soma or

axonal terminals of newborn neurons ( Supplementary Fig. 3) this result suggests that the

experimental system is sufficient for characterizing output synaptic plasticity of newborn dentate

granule cells using a theta burst optical induction paradigm (Supplementary Fig. 4d; 16,22,32-33

).

Therefore, we next delivered theta burst optical stimulation (TBS), and measured fEPSPs slope

before and after the TBS using methods previously described (Methods; 22

). We found that

optical TBS stimulation at 3 and 4 wpi induced LTP of fEPSPs in the CA3 area in nearly all

tested animals (Fig. 2c,d). When the same induction paradigm was used to induce LTP at 8 wpi,

only half of tested animals exhibited potentiation (Fig. 2c-d). Therefore, output synapses of

Page 235: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

222

adult-born DG cells at 4 wpi exhibit a lower induction threshold for LTP, sharing a similar

property to their input synapses as previously reported 16,22

. We then analyzed LTP amplitude at

3, 4, and 8 wpi, and found that robust LTP amplitude was maximal at4 weeks. Relatively

smaller LTP was observed in mice that were stimulated at 3 and 8 wpi (Fig. 2c, e). To determine

whether the reduced LTP at 8 wpi resulted from lack of plasticity in the axonal synapses of

newborn neurons around this age, we induced LTP in this circuit with stronger induction

paradigm, tetanic optical stimulation (50Hz, 2s). We performed whole cell patch clamp

recording from their target neurons (Supplementary Fig. 5a). After establishing stable whole cell

recordings in the CA3 area by optically stimulating mature newborn DG cells, we applied a

tetanic optical stimulation. As shown in Supplementary Fig. 5b, c, we recorded a sustained

potentiation in response after induction, suggesting the output synapses remain plastic but with

higher induction threshold. We further examined if stimulated young newborn with the tetanic

paradigm whether the heightened axonal plasticity could be still observed. We then did whole

cell recording on CA3 pyramidal cells while stimulating young newborn neurons with the tetanic

stimulation. We found a significantly higher level of LTP expression compared to that induced

from stimulating mature newborn neurons (Supplementary Fig. 5b, c, d), consistent with our

observation using theta-burst induction in Fig. 2. Our results here indicate that young newborn

neurons exhibit heightened plasticity that peaks ~4 wpi (Fig. 2e).

We next asked what might contribute to the heightened plasticity in young newborn

neurons. A recent study found that T-type Ca2+

channels generate isolated Ca2+

spikes and boost

fast Na+ action potential firings and facilitate dendritic synaptic plasticity in young, newborn

(but not mature) DG cells16

. According to previous studies 34,35

, the heightened activation by T-

type Ca2+ channel of young adult-born neurons should be able to efficiently propagate to axonal

terminals to regulate their output synaptic activity. We then examined whether the activity of T-

type Ca2+

channels contributed to the heightened plasticity. After establishing reliable recordings

from the animals at 3, 4 and 8 wpi, we blocked T-type Ca2+

channels with mibefradil, a specific

T-type Ca2+

channel blocker (Methods; 34

). Mibefradil showed no effect on the basal synaptic

transmission (Supplementary Fig. 6). Interestingly, theta burst optical stimulation failed to

induce an enhancement of synaptic transmission from mibefradil-treated animals 3 and 4 wpi

(Fig. 2e), at time-point at which we observed robust potentiation in control animals using the

same induction paradigm (Fig. 2e). In the animals at 8 wpi, the weaker LTP was unaffected by

Page 236: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

223

mibefradil application(Fig. 2e), suggesting the activity of T-type Ca2+

channels in young adult-

born DG cells likely plays an important role in the heightened axonal synaptic LTP. Together

with previous studies on intrinsic excitability and input synapses 16,22,35

, these results reveal that a

cohort of fully-integrated (~4 weeks old) young newborn DG cells have more excitable

membranes and enhanced input/output synaptic plasticity.

Reversible silencing a cohort of 4 weeks old adult-born neurons influences hippocampal

memory retrieval

Accumulating evidence suggests that the effectiveness of ablations of adult neurogenesis

depend on their timing3,9,11,12,27

. This suggests that as adult-born DG cells mature they may

assume distinct behavioral roles, and these distinct behavioral roles may coincide with changes

in their synaptic integration and plasticity (Fig. 2e). To test this hypothesis, we used optogenetic

stimulation to temporarily and reversibly silence groups of different aged adult-born neurons

during memory retrieval. We generated a retrovirus to express inhibitory optogenes,

Halorhodopsin (NpHR-EYFP; 28

) or archaerhodopsin-3 (Arch-EGFP; 36

). We found that both

NpHR and Arch could suppress the activity of newborn neurons when optically stimulated as

previously reported 28,36

. To maximize infection of adult-born neurons, we performed two

retroviral injections (spaced 10 h apart) per animal using a standard protocol as we previously

reported (Methods, 14

). We successfully labeled ~1700 newborn DG cells per animal at 4 weeks

after viral injection (Supplementary Table 2). The Arch expression in labeled adult-born DG

cells showed no observable effect on the development of newborn neurons as shown in

Supplementary Table 1. In acutely prepared brain sections, pulses of optical stimulation

specifically silenced Arch expressing adult-born DG cells, indicating that these neurons can be

reliably and reversibly inhibited by light (Fig. 3a)36

.

To examine the role of a group of newborn neurons in hippocampal memory we used a

hidden platform version of the water maze task, a well-established hippocampal-dependent task

37. We microinjected Arch retroviruses into the hilus of the hippocampus in adult mice, and

implanted customized optrodes to ensure sufficient light delivery into the dorsal hippocampus

bilaterally (Methods; Supplementary Fig. 7). At 4 wpi, mice were trained in the water maze with

optic fibers connected to an orange light source via an optic-rotatory joint half with light off (―no

light‖ group) and half with light on (―light‖ group) during training (Fig. 3b, Supplementary Fig

8a). Across the training trials, latencies to locate the hidden platform declined. There was no

Page 237: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

224

difference in escape latencies in control (no light) vs. inactivated (light) groups, indicating that

silencing this cohort of ~4 week-old adult-born DG cells does not interfere with task acquisition.

We then assessed the effect of silencing newborn neurons on memory retrieval. As

before, after microinjection of Arch or EGFP control retroviruses we implanted optrodes into the

dorsal hippocampus bilaterally. At ~4 wpi, mice were trained in the water maze with optic fibers

connected to an inactive light source (―no light ―condition) via an optic-rotatory joint, and the

mice learnt to locate the hidden platform (Fig. 3b-d). Following the completion of training,

spatial memory was assessed in two probe tests (with the platform absent from the pool). During

the first probe, half the animals received light stimulation for the duration of the test and half

were tested without the light. On the following day, the animals were re-tested in a second probe

trial with the light conditions reversed (Fig. 3c, d). Using this within-subject design, we found

that reversible inactivation of this cohort of 4 weeks old adult-born neurons significantly

decreased the percent time in the target quadrant (Fig. 3e). Importantly, light illumination of 4

week-old EGFP-labeled newborn neurons did not affect the percent time in the target zone

(Supplementary Fig. 9). Furthermore, light-induced inactivation did not impact motor

coordination, as swim speed (Fig. 3f) and distance traveled (Supplementary Fig. 10a) did not

significantly differ from the control condition. Memory impairments depended upon both

retroviral expression of Arch and light illumination since neither retroviral expression of Arch

alone (without illumination) nor illumination alone (in the absence of Arch) impaired memory

(Supplementary Figure 2; 13

) (Fig. 4d).

These results suggest that a cohort of ~4 week-old newborn cells plays a key role in

memory expression. By examining expression of the activity-regulated gene, c-fos, we next

asked whether this population of cells was normally activated by memory recall, and whether

this activation was absent following light-induced inactivation. Mice received injections of Arch

or EGFP retrovirus into the dentate, and 4 weeks later were trained in the water maze. At the

completion of training, half the mice were given a probe test with the light on and the other half

with no light (Supplementary Fig. 11a). Using standard procedures24

, 90 minutes after the probe

tests, we sacrificed the mice and performed c-fos staining. We imaged c-fos+ and Arch+ DG

cells (Supplementary Fig. 11b). In the Arch/no light and EGFP/light groups, about 4% of labeled

adult-born DG cells were c-fos positive (Supplementary Fig. 11c), consistent with our previous

findings24

. This result suggests labeled adult-born DG cells at ~4wpi have been recruited and are

activated by retrieval of a spatial memory. Mice in the Arch/light group searched non-selectively

Page 238: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

225

in the probe test (similar to deficits observed in Fig. 3d). Furthermore, very few Arch-labeled

adult-born DG cells expressed c-fos (Supplementary Fig. 11b & c), confirming the efficiency of

optical inhibition, and additionally, providing support for the conclusion that labeled ~ 4 week-

old adult-born DG cells regulate spatial memory retrieval.

We next evaluated whether silencing these neurons interfered with the expression of a

non-hippocampus-dependent water maze memory. The same mice were trained in a visible

version of the water maze in which the platform was located in the opposite quadrant of the pool

and marked by a visible cue. Across two days of training, mice learned to navigate to the cue

from different start positions (Fig. 3c, d). During two subsequent probe tests (light on and light

off conditions, counterbalanced), we found that light inactivation did not interfere with the

ability to find the visible platform (Fig. 3g), swimming (Fig. 3h) or distance traveled

(Supplementary Fig. 10b).

To ask whether our results would generalize to another form of hippocampus-dependent

memory, we next trained a new group of animals in a fear conditioning paradigm (Fig. 4a), in

which a tone is paired with a mild footshock in a training context. When replaced in the training

context or presented with the tone in an alternate context, mice typically exhibit a range of

species-typical fear behaviors, including freezing (the cessation of all but respiratory-related

movement) 38

. Mice were trained with a single tone-shock pairing and placed back in the training

context 1 day later. In this test, optogenetic silencing significantly reduced levels of conditioned

freezing (Fig. 4b, c), indicating that inactivating 4 week-old adult-born neurons impaired the

retrieval of contextual fear memory. The following day, we placed mice in an alternate context

and presented the tone. In contrast, optogenetically silencing these neurons did not affect

conditioned freezing to the tone (Fig. 4d). As the retrieval of contextual, but not tone, fear

memories depend on hippocampal function 38

, these results suggest that silencing a cohort of 4

week-old adult-born DG cells impaired retrieval of a hippocampus-dependent contextual fear

memory.

Reversible silencing of 2 or 8 weeks old newborn neurons show no significant effect on

hippocampal memory retrieval

We next asked whether silencing different aged cohorts of newborn neurons would have

similar impact on hippocampus-dependent memory retrieval. To address this, we trained mice 2

Page 239: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

226

or 8 weeks following retroviral injections. Silencing cells that were ~2 weeks-old at the time of

training did not disrupt subsequent expression of a spatial or fear memory (Fig. 5a;

Supplementary Fig. 12). Similarly, silencing cells that were ~8 weeks old at the time of training,

an age at which they are considered mature (Fig. 1, 14-15,20

), did not significantly affect

expression of a spatial memory (Fig. 5b) or contextual fear memory (Supplementary Fig. 12).

Not all adult-generated granule cells survive and therefore the absence of effects of silencing at 8

weeks might be because there are fewer Arch+ cells. Accordingly, we counted Arch positive

newborn DG cells after behavioral tests. As shown in Supplementary Table 2, we found that

there was no significant decrease in number from 4 to 8 weeks after birth, consistent with

previous studies 1,8,39

, and, furthermore, infected cells have similar dendritic complexity at ~4

and 8 wpi (Supplementary Fig. 13).

These results indicate ~4 week-old newborn neurons are recruited, and play a key role in

subsequent memory retrieval. To further explore the specificity of these effects we designed a

within subject experiment where mice were tested water maze at 4 wpi and contextual fear

conditioning at 8 wpi, or vice versa (Supplementary Fig. 14). We found that silencing newborn

neurons at 4 wpi, but not 8 wpi, disrupted memory retrieval in both tasks, consistent with our

above observations (Fig. 3-5).

Finally, we next asked whether the coincidentally heightened plasticity (Fig. 2)

contributes to the role of young newborn neurons in retrieval of hippocampal-dependent

memories (Fig. 5d). Since we found that the activity of T-type Ca2+

channels plays an essential

role in the heightened plasticity, we tested hippocampal memory of the Arch-injected animals at

4 wpi in the presence of mibefradil. Interestingly, the application of mibefradil substantially

decreased the searching time in the target quadrant compared to the saline-light off groups (Fig.

5d). In another group of animals, when we applied mibefradil and optical inhibition, we observed

a similar deficit in memory retrieval in the mibefradil treated animals (Supplementary Fig. 15).

Because T-type Ca2+

channels are also required for the heightened plasticity in dendritic

synapses, these data suggest that heightened plasticity in young newborn neurons may play a key

role in memory retrieval. Together, our data suggest that adult-born neurons may transiently play

a critical role in memory retrieval, but the impact, at least in spatial and contextual memory

retrieval, may decline as these adult-born neurons continue to mature.

Discussion

Page 240: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

227

We used optogentic methods to evaluate how adult-born DG cells functionally integrate

into hippocampal circuits. We found that newborn DG cells form functional glutamatergic

synapses in the CA3 area as early as 2 weeks after birth and these synapses become functionally

stable by 4 weeks in age. Our in vivo recording from CA3 area following stimulation of a group

of newborn DG cells revealed that output synapses of 4-week old newborn neurons exhibit

enhanced plasticity. Interestingly, reversibly silencing a population of 4 but not 2 or 8 weeks old

significantly impacted hippocampal memory retrieval. These data indicate that adult-born

neurons influence hippocampal function and hippocampal-mediated behaviors in a maturation-

dependent manner. Together with the finding that young newborn cells have more excitable

membranes 16

and enhanced dendritic synaptic plasticity 22

, our results suggest that cohorts of

newborn neurons transiently represent a unique population of granule cells and may

disproportionately influence hippocampal-mediated behaviors.

Functional output circuit development

The retroviral method has been widely used to birth-date and deliver genes to adult-born

neurons by many groups including ours 13-14,19

. In the current study, we successfully employed

this method to deliver optogenes to cohorts of adult-born DG cells. As we previously reported 13-

14, infection with these retroviruses did not appear to affect the development of labeled newborn

neurons or the physiology of surrounding mature neurons (Supplementary Fig. 2 and Table 1).

By using optogenetic stimulation, in vitro and in vivo physiology and imaging methods, we

established that young adult-born neurons functionally project to the CA3 neural circuitry.

Although we did not fully characterize the pharmacological properties of these synapses (Fig.

1e), blockade of post synaptic responses by applying AMPA receptor or metabotropic glutamate

receptor agonists suggests that they are typical mossy fiber synapses as previously reported 30

.

The functional projection of young newborn neurons to interneurons remains to be examined,

although it is well-established that mature DG cells form such synapses 4,19

. Together with

several previous studies 13-15,20

, it now appears to be clear that adult-born DG cells fully

incorporate into the hippocampal trisynaptic neural circuit around 4 weeks after birth.

Using optogenetic and retroviral methods, we revealed a restricted time window for

newborn neurons during which time they exhibit enhanced plasticity at CA3 synapses. We found

output synapses of adult-born DG cells exhibit enhanced plasticity at ~4 weeks of age (Fig. 2),

Page 241: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

228

similar to the time during which we observed hyper-sensitive input synapses, and when these

cells have more excitable membranes 16,22

. Input synapses of 4-8 week-old newborn neurons

exhibit a high level of anatomical plasticity, with dendritic spine motility peaking at ~4 weeks

and declining thereafter 11,20

. However, the motility of axonal synapses remains unknown.

Mechanistically, we found that mibefradil, a T-type Ca2+

channel blocker, could abolish the

heightened axonal plasticity of young newborn neurons, similar to what has been previously

reported in their dendritic synapses 16

. We previously found that the switch of NMDA receptor

components from 2A to 2B may account for the enhancement of dendritic synaptic plasticity

during 4 and 6 weeks after birth 22

. The essential roles for Ca2+

Channels in axonal synaptic

plasticity of dentate granule cells have been extensively studied 28,41

. However, the roles of

NMDA receptors in synaptic plasticity in the CA3 area remains controversial although some

recent studies suggest their involvement 30,42

. Furthermore, we recently found that continuous

increase in the strength of inhibitory circuits may decrease the power of plasticity in dendritic

synapses 43

, which raises another possibility for the observed enhanced plasticity of output

synapses. A very recent finding showed that the activity may differentiate continuously-

generated newborn neurons into groups to serve hippocampus-related pattern separation 23

. The

heightened axonal plasticity together with the enhanced dendritic plasticity 16,22

of young

newborn may be involved in facilitating this process.

Time-dependent role of young adult-born neurons

Chemical, genetic and irradiation-based methods have been widely used to ablate

neurogenesis and explore the role of adult neurogenesis in hippocampal function 9,44

. While these

studies suggest that adult neurogenesis plays an important role in hippocampal memory, a

limitation is that these manipulations typically affect a broad range of adult-born cells and so it is

unclear whether depletion of immature or mature newborn neurons is responsible for observed

memory deficits. By retrovirally expressing optogenes we were able to address this issue by

silencing distinct of adult-generated neurons. This optogenetic silencing of adult-born neurons by

high frequency light stimulation is likely to result from ―effective but not excessive‖

hyperpolarization, rather than potentially harmful changes in internal chloride levels 36

. Our

experiments indicated that silencing of ~4 week-old newborn neurons led to memory deficits. In

contrast silencing this same population before training did not prevent the acquisition of a

hippocampal memory. The dissociation is consistent with our recent findings using a diphtheria

Page 242: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

229

toxin-based ablation system to study the role of adult-generated neurons in hippocampal memory

(Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011), and suggests that learning may occur in the absence of newborn

neurons. However, if newborn neurons are present at the time of training, they are recruited into

hippocampal memory circuits and silencing (or ablating) these ‗memory-committed‘ cells

reveals that they play an essential role in memory retrieval. While memory retrieval deficits were

only observed after silencing of 4 week-old newborn neurons, we cannot exclude the possibility

that silencing 8 week-old newborn neurons also impacts hippocampal function, albeit to a lesser

degree. It is noteworthy that the retroviral approach only labels a relatively small group of

newborn neurons (Supplementary Table 2), and so even the activity of this small population of

cells strongly influences hippocampal function. Mechanistically, young newborn neurons are

more excitable and exhibit heightened plasticity at around 4 weeks16,22

, and these properties

likely increase their likelihood of being recruited into hippocampal memory circuits23-24,27

. This

also has been suggested by our finding that mibefradil, which blocks the heightened plasticity,

affects the behavioral role of newborn neurons.

Methods

Retroviral production and stereotaxic injection

Engineered self-inactivating murine oncoretroviruses were used to deliver genes of

interest specifically to proliferating cells and their progeny 13-14

. The optimized ChR2 and

ChETA constructs were obtained from Karl Deisseroth and ChIEF construct from Roger Tsien.

The Arch construct from Edward Boyden was purchased from Addgene.

Purified engineered retroviruses were stereotaxically injected into ~5 weeks old adult

female C57BL/6 mice (Charles River). All animal procedures were conducted in accordance

with institutional guidelines.

Optrode implantation and behavioral procedures

All mice were housed under standard conditions. Optrodes (Doric Lenses Inc., Canada,

modified to increase light spread) were implanted bilaterally into the dorsal dentate gyrus

(coordinates: 3.0 mm rostral from bregma, 2.6 mm lateral from the midline and 2.5 mm ventral)

14 days after two retroviral injections (~10 hours interval) unless we specifically addressed in the

main text. After implantation, animals received at least 2 weeks recovery before any behavioral

experiment.

Page 243: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

230

After behavioral experiments, mice were perfused transcardially with PBS followed by

4% PFA. Brains were sectioned and the optrode implantation sites were verified and numbers of

retroviral-labeled adult-born neurons were counted. Mice were excluded if the implantation site

was incorrectly positioned. Mice with missed viral injections were discarded. Mice with correct

injections all had 1500 ~2000 newborn DG cells and all of them have been selected for

behavioral analysis.

Water maze (hidden platform version). The apparatus and behavioral procedures have

been previously described 24,45

. Behavioral testing was conducted in a circular water maze tank

(120cm in diameter, 50cm deep), located in a dimly-lit room. The pool was filled to a depth of

40cm with water made opaque by adding white, non-toxic paint. Water temperature was

maintained at approximately 26oC. A circular escape platform (10 cm diameter) was submerged

0.5 cm below the water surface, in a fixed position in one of the quadrants. The pool was

surrounded by curtains, at least 1 m from the perimeter of the pool. The curtains were white with

distinct cues painted on them.

Water maze training took place across 6 days. Each training session consisted of 3

training trials (inter-trial interval was ~15 s). On each trial, mice were placed into the pool,

facing the wall, in one of 4 pseudorandomly-varied start locations. The trial was complete once

the mouse found the platform or 40 seconds had elapsed. If the mouse failed to find the platform

on a given trial, it was guided onto the platform by the experimenter. Following training, spatial

memory was assessed in two probe tests with the platform removed from the pool. The probe

tests were 40 s in duration and conducted 24 h and 48 h after the last training session. Animals

performed training and probes attached to the optic fibers and rotatory joint. Each animal

experienced one probe with light stimulation and one without, the order of which was

counterbalanced between animals. Behavioral data from training and the probe tests were

acquired and analyzed using an automated tracking system (Ethovision XT, Noldus,

Wageningen, Netherlands). Using this software, we recorded a number of parameters during

training, including escape latency and swim speed. In probe tests, we measured the amount of

time mice searched in the target quadrant vs. the three other quadrants.

Water maze (visible platform version). For the visible platform task, the platform was

moved to the opposite quadrant and marked by a visual cue. The cue consisted of a plastic

cylinder (4 cm in diameter, 4 cm in height) with a horizontal black/white vertical striped pattern

and was placed on top of the platform. Visible platform training started 24h after the last hidden

Page 244: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

231

platform probe and consisted of one training session of 3 trials per day (inter-trial interval was

~15 s) across two days. On each trial mice were placed into the pool, facing the wall, in one of 4

start locations (pseudorandomly-varied). The trial was complete once the mouse found the

escape platform or 40 s had elapsed. Similar to the hidden version of the water maze, animals

were divided into two groups to perform two probe tests, counterbalanced for order of light

stimulation. The probes started 24h after training and were conducted on consecutive days. As

before, behavioral data from training and the probe tests were acquired and analyzed using an

automated tracking system.

Context fear conditioning. The fear conditioning chamber consisted of a stainless steel

conditioning chamber (18 cm × 18 cm × 30 cm; Coulbourn, Whitehall, PA), containing a

stainless steel shock-grid floor. Shock grid bars (diameter 3.2 mm) were spaced 7.9 mm apart.

The grid floor was positioned over a plastic drop-pan, which was lightly cleaned with 70% ethyl

alcohol to provide a background odor. The front of the chamber was made of clear acrylic and

the top, back and two sides made of modular aluminum. Animals were subjected to two probes, a

context test and a tone test. For the context testing animals were placed in the fear chamber,

where they were originally shocked. For tone testing mice were put in a modified version of the

fear chamber that consisted of a white, plastic floor covering the shock grid bars and a plastic,

triangular insert placed inside the same conditioning chamber used for training. One of the walls

of this insert had a black/white striped pattern. The other two walls were white. After each test

the plastic floor was cleaned with water. Mouse freezing behavior was monitored via overhead

cameras and scored manually.

During training, mice were placed in the fear conditioning chamber for a total of 3 min.

After 2 min of free exploration mice were presented with a 30 s tone (2800 Hz, 85 dB) that co-

terminated with a 2 s footshock (0.5 mA). Mice remained in the chamber for a further 30 s before

being returned to their home cage.

Twenty four hours after training, freezing was assessed in two 5 min tests, in the fear

chamber and its modified version, respectively. In the second probe, the tone was presented after

a 2 min delay. Animals were divided into two groups, so that half received light stimulation

during the context test and half during the tone test. Data is presented as function of time for the

context test. For the tone test we measured freezing to the tone for 60 seconds.

Slice and in vivo physiology

Page 245: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

232

Mice were processed at 1, 2, 3 and 4 wpi and electrophysiological recordings performed

at 32°C - 34°C, as previously described 14

. For efferent CA3 synapse slice experiments, short

pulses of blue light were generated by a 50mW 473 nm laser under the control of a standard TTL

board and launched into a Zeiss upright microscope through the epifluorescence light path. The

ending power on brain slices was ~5 mW/mm2 and synaptic transmission was recorded at -

65mV.

For in vivo recording, mice received injections of ChIEF-dTomato retrovirus 3, 4 and 8

weeks prior to in vivo recordings. Briefly, animals were anesthetized and mounted on a

stereotaxic frame. An optrode was inserted into dorsal dentate gyrus (coordinates: 3.0mm rostral

from bregma, 2.6mm lateral from the midline and 2.5mm ventral). Pulses of blue light controlled

by recording software (pClamp10.0) was generated from a 50mW 473nm laser and delivered

thought an optic fiber. A recording electrode was inserted into CA3 molecular layer (coordinates:

2.0mm rostral from bregma, 2.2mm lateral from the midline and 2.2mm ventral), and field

excitatory postsynaptic potentials were recorded upon optical stimulation of the dentate gyrus.

After recording, mice were perfused transcardially with PBS followed by 4% PFA. Brains were

sectioned to check for the presence of retroviral-labeled adult-born neurons, and the sites of

stimulation in dentate gyrus and recording in CA3 (Supplementary Fig. 4). Mice were excluded

if the recording site was out of CA3.

Fos immunohistochemistry and analysis

Neuronal activity during memory retrieval was analyzed by imaging immediate-early

gene c-fos expression as previously described 24

, and illustrated in Supplementary Figure 11a.

Briefly, ninety minutes following the completion of behavioral testing, mice were deeply

anesthetized and perfused transcardially with PBS and then 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains

were removed, fixed overnight in PFA and then transferred to 30% sucrose solution and stored at

4 °C. Brains were sectioned into 50 µm coronal sections of covering the full anterior-posterior

extent on the dentate gyrus. Immunohistochemistry was performed using primary antibodies to

Fos (rabbit anti-Fos polyclonal antibody; 1:1,000, Calbiochem), and Alexa-568 goat anti-rabbit

(1:500, Molecular Probes) as secondary antibodies. Sections were incubated in primary

antibodies overnight, and then with secondary antibodies and anti-NeuN primary antibody for 2

hours at room temperature, in the presence of 2% goat serum, 1% bovine serum albumin and

0.2% Triton X-100. Sections were mounted on slides with Permafluor anti-fade medium

Page 246: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

233

(Lipshaw Immunon). Images of the dentate gyrus were taken on an Olympus FLV1000 confocal

microscope, and we quantified Fos+ and EGFP+/Arch-EGFP+ cells throughout the anterior-

posterior extent of the granule cell layer. Number of Fos+ cells and EGFP+/Arch-EGFP+ cells

quantify from two-dimensional images of the entire dentate gyrus, and the ratio of Fos+EGFP+

in EGFP+ cells was calculated for each animal.

Staining and and reconstruction of biocytin-filled neurons

Brain sections with biocytin-filled neurons were fixed in 4% PFA overnight and stained

using a alexa 647-conjugated streptavidin from Invitrogen. Images were acquired on an

Olympus FLV1000 confocal system and biocytin-filled neurons were reconstructed afterwards

using Olympus FluoView10 software.

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using ANOVAs followed by t-tests. Significance was considered when p <

0.05. ANOVAs and most t tests are described in the figure captions. For all other planned t tests

see supplementary table 3. In the water maze, animals were considered to be searching

selectively if percent search in NE was significantly greater than each of the other quadrants (i.e.,

NE>NW, NE>SE, NE>SW). If NE was not greater than all other quadrants the search was

considered not selective.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank Gary Matthews, Lorna Role, Hongjun Song and Josef Bischofberger for their

critical comments, Qiaojie Xiong and Jason Tucciarone for technical support. We thank Fred

Gage‘s laboratory for sharing their retroviral packaging system and behavioral protocols. This

work was supported by NIH (NS065915) and AHA (0930067N) grants to S.G. and Canadian

Institutes of Health Research grants to P.W.F. (MOP86762) and S.A.J. (MOP74650).

Figure legends

Figure 1. Adult-born neurons stably form functional synapses on CA3 pyramidal neurons

by 4 weeks of age. (a) Upper: Experimental time-line. Adult mice were injected with high-titer

ChR2-EGFP retrovirus and recordings were made on acute brain slices at 1, 2, 3, 4, 6 and 8 wpi.

Page 247: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

234

Lower: Image showing adult-born DG cells (4 wpi) and their axonal fibers (EGFP+). Scale bar:

50 m. (b) Pulses of light (5 ms, 473 nm) elicit action potentials in ChR2-EGFP-infected, but not

neighboring (EGFP-) DG cells. Left: Image showing recorded DG neurons (white, biocytin filled

from the recording pipette) on an acute brain slice (4 wpi). i, ChR2-EGFP+ newborn neurons

(green and white) and ii, non-infected neighbor (white). Right: Light induced action potentials in

EGFP+ (i) but not in EGFP- (ii) DG cell. Scale bar: 100 m. (c) Optically-evoked EPSCs from

adult-born DG cells. Left: Image showing ChR2-EGFP+ axonal terminals (green) of adult-born

DG cells (4 wpi) and a recorded CA3 pyramidal neuron (white). Right: a sample opticallyevoked

EPSCs recorded from this cell, subsequently blocked by 50 M CNQX. Scale bar:

10m. (d) Time-course of axon integration (left, EGFP) and formation of functional synapses on

CA3 neurons (right) by newborn neurons at 1, 2, 3 and 4 wpi. Scale bar: 10 m. (e) Amplitude of

EPSCs. Shown is a summary of mean amplitude of EPSCs at 1, 2, 3, 4, 6 and 8 wpi. (f) Latency

of EPSCs at 2, 3, 4, 6 and 8 wpi. (g) Shown is a summary of EPSCs which were inhibited by 50

M L-AP4 (n=5, p=0.012) or 1 M LY354740 (n=6, p=0.008), and blocked by 50 M CNQX

(n=15, p<0.001). All values represent mean±SEM (*, p<0.05, t-test).

Figure 2. Adult-born neurons at 4 weeks of age show enhanced plasticity at output

synapses. (a) Experimental time-line. Adult mice were injected with retrovirus expressing a

ChR2 variant optimized for reliability under high frequency stimulation (ChIEF-dTomato, Suppl.

Fig.3), and post-synaptic responses in CA3 were recorded in vivo at 3, 4 and 8 wpi by optically

stimulating adult-born neurons in dentate gyrus (Suppl. Fig.4). (b) Optically stimulating

adultborn neurons produces fEPSPs in the CA3 area. Left, fEPSPs blocked by infusion of 50M

CNQX (but not saline). Right, optical stimulation (50 Hz pulses of 5 ms) reliably induced

fEPSPs. Scale bars: 5 ms and 0.1 mV. (c) Theta-burst optical stimulation of adult-generated

neurons produces long-term potentiation at CA3 synapses in an age-dependent manner. Top row,

examples of fEPSPs LTP from a single animal at 3, 4 or 8 wpi (newborn neurons). Insets:

Averaged traces of fEPSPs from 5 consecutive recordings before (1), immediately following (2),

and after (3) LTP induction using physiologically relevant theta burst stimulation (TBS, blue

arrow, Suppl. Fig.4). Shown at the bottom is a summary of LTP from groups of animals,

respectively. (d) Percentage of mice (3, 4 and 8 wpi) exhibiting reliable LTP. (e) Summary of the

mean potentiation of fEPSPs amplitude 45-60 min following TBS from mice under control

condition (3 wpi, n=6; 4 wpi, n=8; 8 wpi, n=8; t-test between groups: 3 wpi vs. 4 wpi, p=0.024; 8

wpi vs. 4 wpi, p=0.007) or after application of mibefradil (3 wpi, p=0.007, n=4; 4 wpi, p<0.001,

n=5; 8 wpi, p=0.41, n=4; t-test between control and mibefradil conditions). In each group, all

animals tested were included. All values represent mean±SEM (*, p<0.05 comparing between

groups under control condition; #, p<0.05 comparing between control and mibefradil conditions;

n.s., p>0.05).

Figure 3. Specifically and reversibly silencing 4 week-old adult-born neurons impairs

hippocampal memory retrieval. (a) Optical stimulation (589 nm light) silences Arch-EGFP

expressing adult-born neurons. Optical stimulation (20 Hz pulses of 5 ms) prevented action

potentials induced by current injection (50 pA) (black bars) in Arch-EGFP+ DG cells (4 wpi) in

acutely-prepared hippocampal sections. Scale bars: 100 ms and 30 mV. (b) Schematic drawing

showing a mouse with implanted optrodes connected to an orange light source via optic fibers

and an optic rotatory joint. (c) Time-line of watermaze experiment. Adult mice were infused with

Arch-EGFP retroviruses, implanted with optrodes and trained in the hidden version of the water

maze at 4 wpi. Spatial memory was assessed in two probe tests with (orange) and without (black)

Page 248: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

235

optical stimulation (counterbalanced, within-subject design). Mice were then trained in a visible

version of the water maze with a visible cue, followed by probes with and without optical

stimulation. (d) During training (no light), latency to find the platform declined. (e) Optically

inactivating a cohort of 4 week-old adult-born neurons impaired hippocampal memory retrieval.

During the probe, mice in the ―no light‖ condition searched selectively – spending more time in

the target quadrant (NE) compared to the other quadrants: (One-way repeated measures ANOVA

F3,39=7.139, p<0.001; NE>NW, SW, SE by paired t-test planned comparison, n=14), showing

robust spatial memory. In contrast, mice in the light on condition (inactivation) spent equivalent

time in NE compared to other quadrants (One-way repeated measures ANOVA F3,39=0.9655,

p=0.4187; p>0.05 NE vs. NW, SW, SE by paired t-test planned comparison; n=14), showing a

disruption of spatial memory (NEno light>NElight t13=2.153, p=0.0253 by planned comparison) . (f-

h) Optical inactivation did not alter swim speed in hidden probe tests (P1 and P2) (f, t13=1.046,

p=0.3145; n=14). In the visible version of the water maze, optical stimulation did not alter the

latency to find the platform (g, t13=0.1215, p=0.4526; n=14) or swim speed (h, t13=0.6464,

p=0.2646; n=14) in the probe tests (P1 and P2). All values represent mean±SEM (*: p<0.05).

Figure 4. Temporary silencing of 4 week-old newborn neurons impairs expression of a fear

conditioning memory. (a) Time-line of fear conditioning test. Adult mice were infused with a

retroviral vector (Arch-EGFP), implanted with optrodes and trained in fear conditioning (single

tone-shock pairing). Twenty-four hours after training, contextual fear memory was assessed. An

additional tone test was performed in which mice were placed in a novel context and the tone

replayed. Animals were divided into two groups and optical silencing was counterbalanced in

contextual and tone freezing tests. (b) Silencing of adult-born DG cells at 4 wpi reduced freezing

to the context compared to control (n=7, 7). (c) Averaged freezing time in the first 2 minutes of

the context test (t12=2.239, p=0.0224; n=7, 7). (d) Optically inactivating these neurons had no

effect on tone fear memory (t12=1.675, p=0.0599; n=7, 7). To avoid potential interference from

with-in session extinction, we measured freezing time in the first 2 minutes. All values represent

mean±SEM (*: p<0.05).

Figure 5. Silencing 2 or 8 week-old adult-born neurons did not interfere with hippocampal

memory retrieval. (a-b) Optically inactivating a cohort of 2 or 8 week-old adult-born neurons

showed no significant effect on hippocampal memory retrieval. During the probe similar to that

described in Fig. 3d, e, mice at 2 (a) or 8 (b) wpi spent more time searching in the target

quadrant (NE) compared to the other quadrants in both ―no light‖ (One-way repeated measures

ANOVA 2 wpiNo light: F3,15=15.25, p<0.0001; 8 wpiNo light: F3,15=8.081, p=0.0019; NE>NW, SW,

SE by paired t-test planned comparison in both groups; n=6,6 in both) and ―light‖ conditions

(One-way repeated measures ANOVA 2 wpiLight: F3,15=9.125, p=0.0011; 8 wpiLight: F3,15=14.20,

p=0.0001; NE>NW, SW, SE by paired t-test planned comparison in both groups; n=6 in both),

showing no disruption of spatial memory expression. (c) Reversible silencing of adult-born DG

cells altered hippocampal memory retrieval in a restricted time window. Shown is a summary of

percent of time the animals spent in the target quadrant (NE) under ―light‖ condition. (d)

Another two groups of mice were tested at ~4 wpi, with application of saline/mibefradil 1 hour

before testing. Mibefradil mimicked the deficit in spatial memory retrieval induced by optical

silencing of 4 week old neurons. Shown is a summary of percent of time the animals spent in

Target (NE) vs. Off Target (average of SE, NW, and SW). (Refer to Supplementary Figure 15

for detailed statistic information.) All values represent mean±SEM (*: p<0.05).

Page 249: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

236

REFERENCES

1 Kempermann, G., Gast, D. & Gage, F. H. Neuroplasticity in old age: sustained fivefold

induction of hippocampal neurogenesis by long-term environmental enrichment. Ann

Neurol 52, 135-143, doi:10.1002/ana.10262 (2002).

2 Zhao, C., Deng, W. & Gage, F. H. Mechanisms and functional implications of adult

neurogenesis. Cell 132, 645-660, doi:S0092-8674(08)00134-7 [pii]

10.1016/j.cell.2008.01.033 (2008).

3 Ming, G. L. & Song, H. Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian brain: significant answers

and significant questions. Neuron 70, 687-702, doi:S0896-6273(11)00348-5 [pii]

10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.001 (2011).

4 Amaral, D. G., Scharfman, H. E. & Lavenex, P. The dentate gyrus: fundamental

neuroanatomical organization (dentate gyrus for dummies). Prog Brain Res 163, 3-22,

doi:S0079-6123(07)63001-5 [pii]

10.1016/S0079-6123(07)63001-5 (2007).

5 Nakashiba, T., Young, J. Z., McHugh, T. J., Buhl, D. L. & Tonegawa, S. Transgenic

inhibition of synaptic transmission reveals role of CA3 output in hippocampal learning.

Science 319, 1260-1264, doi:1151120 [pii]

10.1126/science.1151120 (2008).

6 McNaughton, B. L. & Morris, R. Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information

storage within a distributed memory system Trends in Neurosciences 10, 8 (1987).

7 Moser, E. I., Kropff, E. & Moser, M. B. Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial

representation system. Annu Rev Neurosci 31, 69-89,

doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723 (2008).

8 Kim, W. R., Christian, K., Ming, G. L. & Song, H. Time-dependent involvement of

adult-born dentate granule cells in behavior. Behav Brain Res, doi:S0166-

4328(11)00528-6 [pii]

10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.012 (2011).

9 Aimone, J. B., Deng, W. & Gage, F. H. Resolving new memories: a critical look at the

dentate gyrus, adult neurogenesis, and pattern separation. Neuron 70, 589-596,

doi:S0896-6273(11)00391-6 [pii]

10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.010 (2011).

10 Sahay, A. et al. Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve

pattern separation. Nature 472, 466-470, doi:nature09817 [pii]

10.1038/nature09817 (2011).

11 Arruda-Carvalho, M., Sakaguchi, M., Akers, K. G., Josselyn, S. A. & Frankland, P. W.

Posttraining ablation of adult-generated neurons degrades previously acquired memories.

J Neurosci 31, 15113-15127, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3432-11.2011 (2011).

12 Drew, M. R., Denny, C. A. & Hen, R. Arrest of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice

impairs single- but not multiple-trial contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 124,

446-454, doi:2010-16138-002 [pii]

10.1037/a0020081 (2010).

13 van Praag, H. et al. Functional neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. Nature 415, 1030-

1034, doi:10.1038/4151030a

4151030a [pii] (2002).

Page 250: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

237

14 Ge, S. et al. GABA regulates synaptic integration of newly generated neurons in the adult

brain. Nature 439, 589-593, doi:nature04404 [pii]

10.1038/nature04404 (2006).

15 Esposito, M. S. et al. Neuronal differentiation in the adult hippocampus recapitulates

embryonic development. J Neurosci 25, 10074-10086, doi:25/44/10074 [pii]

10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3114-05.2005 (2005).

16 Schmidt-Hieber, C., Jonas, P. & Bischofberger, J. Enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly

generated granule cells of the adult hippocampus. Nature 429, 184-187,

doi:10.1038/nature02553

nature02553 [pii] (2004).

17 Overstreet Wadiche, L., Bromberg, D. A., Bensen, A. L. & Westbrook, G. L. GABAergic

signaling to newborn neurons in dentate gyrus. J Neurophysiol 94, 4528-4532,

doi:00633.2005 [pii]

10.1152/jn.00633.2005 (2005).

18 Faulkner, R. L. et al. Development of hippocampal mossy fiber synaptic outputs by new

neurons in the adult brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105, 14157-14162, doi:0806658105

[pii]

10.1073/pnas.0806658105 (2008).

19 Toni, N. et al. Neurons born in the adult dentate gyrus form functional synapses with

target cells. Nat Neurosci 11, 901-907, doi:nn.2156 [pii]

10.1038/nn.2156 (2008).

20 Zhao, C., Teng, E. M., Summers, R. G., Jr., Ming, G. L. & Gage, F. H. Distinct

morphological stages of dentate granule neuron maturation in the adult mouse

hippocampus. J Neurosci 26, 3-11, doi:26/1/3 [pii]

10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3648-05.2006 (2006).

21 Hastings, N. B., Seth, M. I., Tanapat, P., Rydel, T. A. & Gould, E. Granule neurons

generated during development extend divergent axon collaterals to hippocampal area

CA3. J Comp Neurol 452, 324-333, doi:10.1002/cne.10386 (2002).

22 Ge, S., Yang, C. H., Hsu, K. S., Ming, G. L. & Song, H. A critical period for enhanced

synaptic plasticity in newly generated neurons of the adult brain. Neuron 54, 559-566,

doi:S0896-6273(07)00334-0 [pii]

10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.002 (2007).

23 Marin-Burgin, A., Mongiat, L. A., Pardi, M. B. & Schinder, A. F. Unique processing

during a period of high excitation/inhibition balance in adult-born neurons. Science 335,

1238-1242, doi:10.1126/science.1214956 (2012).

24 Kee, N., Teixeira, C. M., Wang, A. H. & Frankland, P. W. Preferential incorporation of

adult-generated granule cells into spatial memory networks in the dentate gyrus. Nat

Neurosci 10, 355-362, doi:nn1847 [pii]

10.1038/nn1847 (2007).

25 Ramirez-Amaya, V., Marrone, D. F., Gage, F. H., Worley, P. F. & Barnes, C. A.

Integration of new neurons into functional neural networks. J Neurosci 26, 12237-12241,

doi:26/47/12237 [pii]

10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2195-06.2006 (2006).

26 Tashiro, A., Makino, H. & Gage, F. H. Experience-specific functional modification of the

dentate gyrus through adult neurogenesis: a critical period during an immature stage. J

Neurosci 27, 3252-3259, doi:27/12/3252 [pii]

10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4941-06.2007 (2007).

Page 251: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

238

27 Nakashiba, T. et al. Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old

granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell 149, 188-201,

doi:10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046 (2012).

28 Zhang, F. et al. Multimodal fast optical interrogation of neural circuitry. Nature 446, 633-

639, doi:nature05744 [pii]

10.1038/nature05744 (2007).

29 Nagel, G. et al. Channelrhodopsin-2, a directly light-gated cation-selective membrane

channel. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100, 13940-13945, doi:10.1073/pnas.1936192100

1936192100 [pii] (2003).

30 Nicoll, R. A. & Schmitz, D. Synaptic plasticity at hippocampal mossy fibre synapses. Nat

Rev Neurosci 6, 863-876, doi:nrn1786 [pii]

10.1038/nrn1786 (2005).

31 Sudhof, T. C. & Malenka, R. C. Understanding synapses: past, present, and future.

Neuron 60, 469-476, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.011 (2008).

32 Gunaydin, L. A. et al. Ultrafast optogenetic control. Nat Neurosci 13, 387-392,

doi:10.1038/nn.2495 (2010).

33 Lin, J. Y., Lin, M. Z., Steinbach, P. & Tsien, R. Y. Characterization of engineered

channelrhodopsin variants with improved properties and kinetics. Biophys J 96, 1803-

1814, doi:S0006-3495(09)00016-2 [pii]

10.1016/j.bpj.2008.11.034 (2009).

34 Yoshimura, Y. et al. Involvement of T-type Ca2+ channels in the potentiation of synaptic

and visual responses during the critical period in rat visual cortex. Eur J Neurosci 28,

730-743, doi:EJN6384 [pii]

10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06384.x (2008).

35 Snyder, J. S., Kee, N. & Wojtowicz, J. M. Effects of adult neurogenesis on synaptic

plasticity in the rat dentate gyrus. J Neurophysiol 85, 2423-2431 (2001).

36 Chow, B. Y. et al. High-performance genetically targetable optical neural silencing by

light-driven proton pumps. Nature 463, 98-102, doi:nature08652 [pii]

10.1038/nature08652 (2010).

37 Morris, R. G., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N. & O'Keefe, J. Place navigation impaired in rats

with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297, 681-683 (1982).

38 Kim, J. J. & Fanselow, M. S. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science 256,

675-677 (1992).

39 Deng, W., Aimone, J. B. & Gage, F. H. New neurons and new memories: how does adult

hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 339-350,

doi:nrn2822 [pii]

10.1038/nrn2822 (2010).

40 Hensch, T. K. Critical period regulation. Annu Rev Neurosci 27, 549-579,

doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144327 (2004).

41 Li, L., Bischofberger, J. & Jonas, P. Differential gating and recruitment of P/Q-, N-, and

R-type Ca2+ channels in hippocampal mossy fiber boutons. J Neurosci 27, 13420-13429,

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1709-07.2007 (2007).

42 Kerr, A. M. & Jonas, P. The two sides of hippocampal mossy fiber plasticity. Neuron 57,

5-7, doi:S0896-6273(07)01025-2 [pii]

10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.015 (2008).

Page 252: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

239

43 Ge, S., Sailor, K. A., Ming, G. L. & Song, H. Synaptic integration and plasticity of new

neurons in the adult hippocampus. J Physiol 586, 3759-3765,

doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2008.155655 (2008).

44 Sahay, A., Wilson, D. A. & Hen, R. Pattern separation: a common function for new

neurons in hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron 70, 582-588, doi:S0896-

6273(11)00393-X [pii]

10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.012 (2011).

45 Teixeira, C. M., Pomedli, S. R., Maei, H. R., Kee, N. & Frankland, P. W. Involvement of

the anterior cingulate cortex in the expression of remote spatial memory. J Neurosci 26,

7555-7564, doi:26/29/7555 [pii]

10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1068-06.2006 (2006).

Page 253: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

240

Page 254: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

241

Page 255: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

242

Page 256: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

243

Page 257: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

244

Page 258: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

245

Supplementary Information

A critical time window for adult-born dentate granule cells in hippocampal

function revealed by studies of optically-controlled newborn neurons

Yan Gu1, Maithe Arruda-Carvalho3,4, Jia Wang1, Stephen Janoschka1,2, Sheena

Josselyn3-5, Paul Frankland3-5,* and Shaoyu Ge1,2,*

1Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, SUNY at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794

2Program in Neuroscience, SUNY at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794

3Program in Neurosciences and Mental Health, Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Ave,

Toronto,

Ontario, Canada, M5G 1X8

4Institute of Medical Science, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M5S 1A8

5Department of Physiology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M5S 1A8

Contents:

Supplementary Figures 1-15

Supplementary Tables 1-3

Page 259: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

246

Supplementary Figure 1. Sites and tracks of viral infection in the adult hippocampus. a,

Schematic drawing in the left panel shows the viral injection sites in the dentate gyrus. Right

panel shows the injection coordinates in mm. b and c, Images showing the injection tracks 1week

(b) and 2 weeks (c) after viral injection. Note that injection tracks (arrow) were hardly seen at 2

weeks after injection, as shown in c. A: anterior; P: posterior; M: medial; L: lateral;D: dorsal; V:

ventral. Scale bar: 200μm.

Page 260: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

247

Supplementary Figure 2. Specific labeling of newborn DG cells with high-titer retrovirus.

Shown on the left in a1, b1 and c1 are sample images of newborn neurons at 7, 14 and

28 days after high-titer retroviral infection. Shown on the right in a2, b2 and c2 are enlarged

view of these newborn neurons, respectively. Note that, morphologically, there are no mature

DG cell-like cells at 7 and 14 dpi. Scale bars: 100um.

Page 261: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

248

Supplementary Figure 3. Responses recorded from ChIEF-expressing newborn neurons

following theta-burst optical stimulation. We performed whole cell patch clamping recording

from ChIEF-dTomato expressing newborn neurons at 3, 4, and 8 wpi. Responses were recorded

following theta-burst optical stimulation. a, Schematic diagram depicting theta burst stimulation

(TBS). TBS was used to induce LTP in output synapses of adult-born DG cells in vivo. TBS is

composed of four stimulation episodes at 0.1 Hz. Each episode is a burst of ten 100 Hz, 5ms

pulses delivered at the theta frequency (5 Hz). b, A sample image showing a recorded newborn

DGC filled with biocytin through the recording pipette. Scale bar: 50_m. c, Optical-induced

currents (upper traces) and action potentials (lower traces) recorded from newborn neurons at

3, 4 and 8 wpi in response to theta-burst optical stimulation (blue bars). Scale bars: vertical

50 pA or 20 mV; horizontal 100 ms. In boxes are enlarged traces of one burst (100 Hz, 10

pulses). Scale bars: vertical 50pA or 20 mV; horizontal 50ms. d, A sample image showing a

mossy fiber terminal of newborn neurons filled with biocytin through the recording pipette. Scale

bar: 20um. e, Optical-induced currents (upper traces) and action potentials (lower traces)

recorded from newborn neurons at 3 wpi. Scale bars: vertical 50 pA or 20 mV; horizontal 100

ms. In boxes are enlarged traces of one burst. Scale bars: vertical 50pA or 20mV; horizontal

50ms.

Page 262: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

249

Supplementary Figure 4. Sites of stimulation and recording in the DG-CA3 fEPSPs

examination in vivo. a, Schematic drawing shows the optical stimulation site in the DG and

recording site in the CA3. A: anterior; P: posterior; M: medial; L: lateral; D: dorsal; V: ventral. b,

Sample DIC images showing the tracks of optical stimulation site in the DG (left panel) and

recording site in the CA3 (right panel). Scale bar: 50 μm. c, Two-photon images showing ChIEF-

dTomato expressing adult-born neurons in the DG (left panel) and CA3 (axonal terminals, right

panel). Scale bar: 50 μm.

Page 263: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

250

Supplementary Figure 5. Enhanced plasticity in output synapses of young adult-born neurons

using a titanic induction paradigm. a, Schematic drawing shows whole-cell recording from a

CA3 pyramidal neuron while blue light stimulation was delivered on ChIEF expressing adult-

born DGCs. b-d, Excitatory postsynaptic responses were recorded from CA3 pyramidal neurons

by optically stimulating young (~4 wpi) or old (~ 8 wpi) ChIEF expressing adult-born DGCs.

LTP was induced by a tetanic optical stimulation (50 Hz, 2 s). b, Shown are traces before (black

trace) and after (red trace) LTP induction. Scale bars: vertical: 20 pA; horizontal: 5 ms. c, LTP

expression. d, Histogram showing the amplitudes of LTP measured between 45 and 60 minutes.

All values represent mean±SEM (*: p<0.05, t-test).

Page 264: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

251

Supplementary Figure 6. Mibefradil does not alter basal synaptic transmission. a, Traces of

fEPSPs before and after mibefradil application. Scale bar: vertical 0.05 mV; horizontal 10 ms. b,

Histogram showing normalized fEPSPs slopes before and after application of mibefradil (n=6,

n.s.:p>0.05, t-test). All values represent mean±SEM.

Page 265: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

252

Supplementary Figure 7. Site for optrode implantation in the dorsal hippocampus. a,

Sample image of a sagittal section from mouse brain showing the location of optrode

implantation. Arrowhead: track of optrode implantation. Scale bar: 2mm. b, A sample

two-photon image showing Arch-EGFP expressing adult-born neurons after behavioral

tests. Scale bar: 20μm. c, Shown is a global view of infected neurons in the hippocampus

after viral injection. Diagrammatic drawing roughly illustrates illumination area with the

implanted optrode. Scale bar: 2mm.

Page 266: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

253

Supplementary Figure 8. Optical silencing of newborn DGCs does not influence acquisition

of spatial memory. a, Schematic drawing shows 2, 4,or 8 weeks after injection of Arch-EGFP

retrovirus. Mice were trained in the hidden platform version of the water maze. Half of the

animals were trained with light on in the dentate gyrus during training (―light‖ group), while the

other half were trained with no light (―no light‖ group). b, During training, latency to find the

platform declined normally both in the ―no light‖ and ―light‖ groups at 2 (Two-way ANOVA,

significant effect of Training session only, F5,50=22.08, p<0.0001; n=6,6), 4 (Two-way

ANOVA, significant effect of Training session only, F5,50=44.61, p<0.0001; n=6,6) and 8wpi

(Two-way ANOVA, significant effect of Training session only, F5,50=34.54, p<0.0001; n=6,6).

All values represent mean±SEM (n=6 in each group at each time point).

Page 267: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

254

Supplementary Figure 9. Light illumination of EGFP-labeled newborn DG cells does not

interfere with hippocampal memory retrieval. a-b, Light illumination of a cohort of 4 week-old

adult generated neurons labeled with EGFP showed no effect on hippocampal memory retrieval.

a, During training (no light), latency to find the platform declined normally in both groups. b,

During probe tests, both control (―no light‖) and ―light‖ groups searched selectively (One-way

repeated measures ANOVA No light: F3,21=8.438, p=0.0007; Light: F3,21=14.83, p<0.0001;

NE>NW, SW, SE by paired t-test planned comparison in both groups; n=8) – spending more

time in the target quadrant (NE) compared to the other quadrants, displaying robust spatial

memory. Visible platform tests were also performed, showing no difference of latency in finding

the platform (data not shown). All values represent mean±SEM (*: p<0.05).

Page 268: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

255

Supplementary Figure 10. Swimming distance in hidden and visible platform water maze probe

tests. a, Inactivating adult-born DG cells at 4 wpi showed no effect on swimming distance in

hidden platform water maze probe tests (t13=1.008, p=0.1660; n=14). b, Inactivating adult-born

DG cells at 4 wpi showed no effect on swimming distance in visible platform water maze probe

tests (t13=0.0173, p=0.09867; n=14). All values represent mean±SEM.

Page 269: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

256

Supplementary Figure 11. c-fos staining showing the involvement of newborn neurons in the

spatial memory retrieval during the water maze test. a, Schematic drawing: 4 weeks after

Arch- or EGFP-retrovirus injection and optrode implantation, mice were trained in the water

maze. After training, half of the mice were tested with light illumination, while the other half

with no light. 90 minutes after the probe tests, mice were transcardially perfused with PFA and

brains were fixed. c-fos expression was then stained, imaged and analyzed. b, Shown is c-fos

expression in the EGFP+ or Arch+ 4-week-old newborn neurons. Arrows: c-fos+EGFP+/c-

fos+Arch+ neurons. Scale bar: 50_m. c, Histogram showing activity-induced expression of c-fos

in EGFP+/Arch+ newborn neurons during water maze test. Optical illumination significantly

reduced the number of c-fos+Arch+ newborn neurons. All values represent mean±SEM (*:

p<0.05 and n.s.: p>0.05, t-test).

Page 270: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

257

Supplementary Figure 12. Silencing of adult-born DG cells at 2 or 8 wpi did not interfere with

contextual fear memory expression. Percent freezing of animals in the training context while

light illuminating adult-born neurons expressing Arch at 2 wpi (t4=0.2029, p=0.4246; n=5), 8wpi

(t7=0.3824, p=0.3568; n=8) or EGFP at 4wpi (t5=0.4593, p=0.3326; n=6). All values represent

as mean±SEM of the first two minutes of the context test, comparison was made between ―No

light‖ and ―Light‖conditions.

Page 271: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

258

Supplementary Figure 13. Similar dendritic complexity of labeled adult-born neurons

at 4 and 8 weeks after high-titer viral infusion. a, Example images showing dendritic

density of Arch-EGFP labeled neurons at 4 and 8 wpi. A 50•~50μm2 square area was randomly

selected around the middle molecular layer (dotted line square). The number of dendritic

branches in this area was counted. Scale bar: 50μm. b, Shown is the averaged number of

dendrtic branches at 4 and 8 wpi (p=0.7766, n=10, 8, t-test). All values represent mean±SEM

(n.s.: p>0.05).

Page 272: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

259

Supplementary Figure 14. Task-switching experiments showing adult-born DG cells at 4 weeks

of age are important for memory retrieval. a, Animals were injected with Arch retrovirus and

implanted with optrodes. One group of animals was trained at 4 wpi in the water maze task and

at 8 wpi in contextual fear conditioning (upper panel). Lower panel: At 4wpi, WM trained

animals in the no light condition searched selectively (One-way repeated measures ANOVA

F3,12=9.487, p=0.0017; NE>NW, SW, SE by paired t-test planned comparison; n=5), whereas

when in the light condition they did not (One-way repeated measures ANOVA F3,12=4.004,

p=0.0345 but NE vs. NW t4=0.0702, p=0.43; NE vs. SW t4=1.792, p=0.077). When trained in

the contextual fear conditioning at 8wpi, animals failed to display a contextual fear deficit

(t4=0.3600, p=0.3685). b, After Arch retrovirus injection and optrode implantation, a different

group of mice was trained and tested at 4 wpi for contextual fear conditioning and at 8 wpi in the

water maze (upper panel). Lower panel: At 4wpi animals showed a deficit in contextual fear

conditioning (t4=3.046, p=0.0191). At 8wpi, both No light and Light groups showed intact WM

memory, searching selectively in the target quadrant (One-way repeated measures ANOVA No

light: F3,12=14.48, p=0.0003; Light: F3,12=9.644, p=0.0016; NE>NW, SW, SE by paired t-test

planned comparison in both groups; n=5). All values represent mean±SEM (*: p<0.05).

Page 273: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

260

Supplementary Figure 15. Mibefradil produced similar defect in spatial memory retrieval as

optical silencing of the young neurons. Two groups of mice were tested at ~4 wpi, with

application of saline/mibefradil 1 hour before testing. Mibefradil mimicked the deficit in spatial

memory retrieval induced by optical silencing of 4 week old neurons. (One-way repeated

measures ANOVA SalineNo light: F3,9=10.43, p=0.0027; NE>NW, SW, SE by paired t-test

planned comparison; SalineLight: F3,9=2.849, p=0.0975; MibefradilNo light: F3,9=1.173,

p=0.3730; MibefradilLight: F3,9=1.061, p=0.4128; n=4,4 for all). All values represent

mean±SEM (*: p<0.05).

Page 274: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

261

Supplementary Table 1. Expression of optogenes does not change intrinsic properties of

newborn neurons along time. Mice were injected with retrovirus expressing EGFP,

Channelrhodopsin 2 (ChR2), Channelrhodopsin 2 variant ChIEF, or Archeorhodopsin (Arch),

and acute brain slices were cut at 2, 3, 4 and 8 week post viral injection (wpi). Fluorescent

adult-born neurons were whole-cell patched and intrinsic properties were recorded. Shown are

the Membrane Capacitance, Membrance Resistance, Resting Membrane Potential, Action

Potential (AP) Theshold, AP Peak, AP Amplitude and AP Halfwidth of adult born DG cells at

2wpi (a), 3wpi (b), 4wpi (c) and 8wpi (d). All values represent mean±SEM, and there is no

statistical difference between groups at each time stage (p>0.05 by t-test).

Page 275: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

262

Supplementary Table 2. High titer of Arch-EGFP retrovirus infected a cohort of newborn

DG cells per animal. Mice were perfused after behavior experiments and Arch-EGFP+ adultborn

DGCs were counted in each animal. Shown are the average numbers per mouse of Arch- EGFP+

newborn DG cells in 2wpi, 4wpi, 8wpi and EGFP groups. All values represent mean±SEM, and

there is no statistical difference between groups (p>0.05 by t-test).

Page 276: A Role for Adult Born Neurons in Memory Processing by ... · Barbieri, Maria Paula Almeida, Andre Ferracini e Gisele Neves, Silvia Rebello, amigos de toda vida, nunca se movendo de

263

Supplementary Table 3. Statistic analysis for Water Maze probe tests. All water maze

probe tests were analyzed using ANOVAs followed by t-test planned comparison between

target quadrant (NE) and all other quandrants (NW, SW, SE). Animals were considered to be

searching selectively if percent search in NE was significantly greater than each of the other

quadrants (i.e., NE>NW, NE>SE, NE>SW). If NE was not greater than all other quadrants the

search was considered not selective. This table shows the t, p and df values for every t-test

comparison in the water maze probe tests shown in Figures 3E, 5A, B, C and Supplementary

Figures 9B, 14A, B and 15.