aesthetic preferences and the attribution of meaning

Upload: josepipogarc203

Post on 10-Apr-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    1/8

    Aesthetic preferences and the attribution of meaning:Environmental categorization processes in the

    evaluation of urban scenes

    Ma- Paz Galindo

    Universidad de Sevilla, Spain

    Ma- Carmen Hidalgo

    Universidad de Malaga, Spain

    In the context of research into scenic quality, the purpose of the present paper is to establish categories ofurban landscapes on the basis of users aesthetic judgements. It also explores the role that the restorativecapacity of a placein terms of the attentional restoration theory (ART)together with a set of aesthetic

    attributes, may play in more or less valued places in a city. A total of 132 residents from Malaga (Spain) were

    chosen, with the city providing the physical framework for environmental reference. A questionnaire designed for

    easy self-administration by subjects was used to collect information. Subjects were asked to identify three placesin Malaga that they considered to be most attractive and three that they considered least attractive. Participants

    were asked to evaluate both the restorative propertiesin terms of the ARTand the extent to which their first

    choice displayed certain environmental characteristics. Participants expressed a clear aesthetic preference for

    recreational sites for leisure/walking as well as those closely linked to the citys historical-cultural identity. The

    research also identified other categories of visual settings that could be used as a focal point around which to

    centre future samples of scenes in a city context. Finally, the results obtained from the characterization of more

    and less attractive places, from the variables used in this study, shed light on the dimensions of underlying

    meaning that individuals use to categorize their environment and reinforce the idea that environmental aesthetics

    seem to play an important role in individuals general well-being.

    D ans le cadre de la recherche sur la qualite scenique, le but du present article est detablir des categories depaysages urbains sur la base des jugements esthetiques des utilisateurs. Il explore aussi le role jouepar la capacite de restauration dun lieuselon la Theorie de restauration attentionnelle (ART)conjointementa un ensemble dattributs esthetiques, sur les endroits plus ou moins apprecies dans une ville. Au total, 132

    residents de Malaga (Espagne) furent selectionnes, en considerant que cette ville offre un cadre physique

    de reference environnemental. Un questionnaire auto-administre fut complete par les participants. Ces derniers

    devaient identifier trois lieux a Malaga quils consideraient comme plus attrayants et trois autres quils trouvaient

    moins attrayants. Ils devaient egalement evaluer les proprietes de restaurationselon lARTet lampleur avec

    laquelle leur premier choix concordait avec certaines caracteristiques environnementales. Les participants ont

    exprime une preference claire pour les aires recreatives de loisirs et de marche ainsi que pour les lieux associes a

    lidentite historique et culturelle de la ville. La recherche a aussi permis didentifier dautres categories daspects

    visuels qui pourraient etre utilisees comme elements a partir desquels seront creees les scenes futures en contexte

    urbain. Finalement, les resultats obtenus a partir de la categorisation des endroits plus ou moins attrayants, a

    partir des variables utilisees dans cette etude, nous eclairent sur les dimensions des significations sous-jacentes

    utilisees par les individus pour categoriser leur environnement. Ceci renforce lidee que les aspects esthetiques

    environnementaux joueraient un role important dans le bien-etre general des individus.

    En el contexto de los estudios sobre calidad escenica, el presente trabajo se ha disenado con el objetivo dedeterminar categoras de paisajes urbanos a partir de los juicios esteticos de sus usuarios. Adicionalmente,se ha explorado el papel que la capacidad restauradora de un lugaren terminos de la Teor a de la Restauracion

    Atencionalas como un conjunto de atributos esteticos pueden desempenar en los lugares mas y menos

    valorados de una ciudad. Para ello se selecciono un total de 132 residentes en Malaga (Espana) as como el

    contexto fsico de dicha ciudad como marco de referencia ambiental del estudio. Como instrumento de recogida

    # 2005 International Union of Psychological Science

    http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/00207594.html DOI: 10.1080/00207590444000104

    Correspondence should be addressed to Ma- Paz Galindo, Departamento de Psicologa Experimental, Facultad de Psicologa,

    Universidad de Sevilla, C/Camilo Jose Cela s/n, 41018, Sevilla, , Spain (E-mail: [email protected]).).

    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 40 (1), 1926

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    2/8

    de informacion se utilizo un cuestionario disenado para ser autoadministrado facilmente por los participantes. La

    tarea que se les solicito a estos fue la de identificar los tres lugares de Malaga que les resultaran visualmente mas

    atractivos, as como los tres que consideraran mas feos. Asimismo se les pidio, en relacion al lugar identificado en

    la primera posicion, que evaluaran tanto sus propiedades restauradorasen terminos de la ARTcomo el grado

    en que exhiba determinadas caractersticas ambientales. Los participantes expresaron una clara preferencia

    estetica por los lugares recreativos de ocio/paseo, as como aquellos estrechamente vinculados con el desarrollo

    historicocultural de la ciudad. Asimismo, se han identificado otras categor as de entornos visuales que pueden

    servir como eje en torno al que articular futuros muestreos de escenas en el contexto de la ciudad. Finalmente, losresultados obtenidos a partir de la caracterizacion de los lugares mas y menos atractivosen las variables objeto

    de este estudioofrecen luz sobre las dimensiones de significado subyacentes a traves de la que los individuos

    categorizan su ambiente y apoyan la consideracion de la importante funcion que la estetica ambiental parece

    desempenar en el bienestar general de los individuos.

    In the field of research into environmental

    aesthetics, preference studies have attempted to

    understand the scenic quality of a certain place by

    analysing the responses to it given by different

    groups of nonexpert participants. These

    responses have generally been used as a unit ofanalysis for generating predictive models of visual

    quality (psychophysical models) or frameworks for

    explaining aesthetic experience (cognitive models).

    In research into cognitive models, some studies

    have used preference judgements as a unit of

    judgement to delimit landscape categories that are

    important for users. The results of such studies

    show that one of the underlying dimensions of

    perceptual differentiation is related to the extent

    of human intervention; in other words, the extent

    to which the evaluated scene contains buildings

    and what the perceived equilibrium is betweenhuman and natural elements. On this point, for

    example, a clear distinction in the aesthetic

    judgements of several groups of subjects has been

    established between natural and urban contexts,

    especially when the latter lack natural elements

    such as water and vegetation (Herzog, 1985;

    Kaplan, 1987; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Peron,

    Purcell, Staats, Falchero, & Lamb, 1998; Purcell,

    Lamb, Peron, & Falchero, 1994). Similarly, when

    the study has included a large variety of scenes,

    certain attributes of spatial configuration (such as

    the extent of openness and spatial definition, thenormal function and/or use of the place, and the

    age of the site and its upkeep) form additional

    bases for the perceptive-evaluative categorization

    made by nonexpert individuals (for a review of

    these studies see Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989).

    Most studies attempting to delimit landscape

    categories have used photographs of natural

    scenes that are unfamiliar to participants, having

    been selected by researchers using different cri-

    teria. This procedure of stimuli selection and

    presentation requires the fragmentation of the

    molar environment (as represented in subjects

    memory systems) and depends on the criteria of

    individual experts. Furthermore, very little of the

    research mentioned above evaluated urban stimuli,

    or the role that familiarity or recognition of

    visualized scenes may play in processes of aesthetic

    evaluation. The development of evaluation proce-dures aimed at identifying categories of known

    and/or familiar places for participants, that meet

    their own selection criteria, is an area that has

    received little attention and could also provide

    important information in terms of optimizing

    processes of environmental sampling.

    To overcome some of the above-mentioned

    limitations, an evaluation procedure was designed

    in a previous study to identify urban scene

    categories that were known and/or familiar to

    nonexpert participants, using selection criteria

    established by the subjects themselves (Galindo,1994). A random sample of residents from the city

    of Seville was asked to identify three public

    landscapes in the city that they found most

    attractive in visual terms. The analysis of their

    responses allowed us to establish four main

    perceptive-evaluative categories for urban scenes

    that were important for users: (1) historical-

    cultural landscapes, (2) leisure areas, (3) recrea-

    tional landscapes, and (4) housing areas. These

    categories were configured, however, from the

    analysis of a single range of aesthetic preferences

    (attractive places) and were not characterizedaccording to any other variable by the subjects

    interviewed. Exploring the generality of such a

    categorization in other geographical scenes, incor-

    porating the analysis of places considered unat-

    tractive, is one of the aims of the present study. It

    is based on the assumption that identifying more

    or less attractive landscapes of one specific

    molar setting provides an opportunity not only for

    registering its main scenic resources (descriptive

    objective), but also for clarifying which implicit

    criteria of visual quality are used by nonexpert

    individuals (explanatory objective).

    20 GALINDO AND HIDALGO

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    3/8

    Regarding the (implicit) standards of scenic

    beauty, Nasar proposed (1994, 1997) two types of

    variable in the context of urban design: Attributes

    of formal aesthetics and attributes of symbolic

    aesthetics. Among the former, the authors highlight

    openness (and/or spaciousness), mystery,

    complexity (both related to visual diversity),

    and order (or congruence between the elements

    that make up the scene). In relation to the symbolic

    aesthetic attributes, Nasar mentions variables of

    content such as vegetation, upkeep, style,

    and perceived use. Results from a substantial

    number of empirical studies have demonstrated the

    role that these characteristics play in aesthetic

    evaluation responses. Some of the variables men-

    tioned by Nasar have also been adopted in various

    evolutionist theoretical proposals that have linked

    aesthetic evaluation responses to certain scenes with

    the important psychological benefits likely to arise

    from contact with these scenes (Appleton, 1987;

    Kaplan 1987; Ulrich, 1983). In this context, the

    Attentional Restoration Theory (ART; Kaplan,

    1995; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989) was developed

    around the idea that certain settings may reduce

    tiredness caused by directed attention, thus

    enabling the re-establishment of certain cognitive

    abilities. Other research has focused on the

    construction of an instrument to measure the

    restorative capacity of a specific placein terms

    of the ART(Hartig, Korpela, Evans, & Garling,

    1997) as well as analysing the role that this capacitymay play in the selection of favourite places

    (Korpela & Hartig, 1996; Korpela, Hartig, Kaiser,

    & Fuhrer, 2001) or in aesthetic judgements (Peron,

    Berto, & Purcell, 2002; Purcell, Peron, & Berto,

    2001). Although, in this context, the existence of

    relationships between the restorative capacity of

    natural surroundings and their aesthetic evaluation

    has been documented in some studies, there is very

    little actual evidence of the existence of this

    association in urban settings.

    Within this framework, the overview guiding

    the design of this study is establishing categories ofurban places from the perspective of their daily

    observers, as well as attempting to identify some of

    the important dimensions of meaning that underlie

    aesthetic preferences. Based on this overview, we

    have drawn up the following specific objectives:

    1. Establish categories of more and less valued

    urban places and analyse any similarities/differ-

    ences existing on the aesthetic value assigned (as

    well as their frequency of use).

    2. Determine the role that the restorative capacity

    of a place (in terms of the ART) may exercise in its

    aesthetic evaluation.

    3. Characterize the more and less valued places

    within the set of aesthetic attributes (sensorial and

    semiotic) delimited in Nasars proposal.

    4. Explore the relationships between the restora-

    tive capacity of a place and the aesthetic attributes

    selected in this study.

    METHOD

    Participants

    The sample consisted of 132 university students

    (72 women and 59 men, average age 26 years), all

    residents of Malaga city. They were selected from

    a larger sample of 202 subjects. All participants

    complied with the following criteria: (1) they had

    been born in Malaga or had been living there for a

    minimum of 9 years; (2) they expressed a strong

    sense of belonging to the city. This information

    was collected from two questions in the ques-

    tionnaire described in the following section.

    Procedure

    A questionnaire was designed to be easily self-

    administered by the participants themselves. They

    were asked to do the following. First: Think, for

    a while, about the city of Malaga itself; its streets,

    neighbourhoods, squares and open spaces of any

    type (large and small, attractive or unattractive);think about all the places you have been to or

    seen. After this, using the procedure followed in

    Galindos (1994) study, participants were asked to

    identify the three places in the city that were of

    greatest visual interest to them (open question),

    and state how often (in a question with closed

    categories) they went to or passed by those places.

    The rest of the questions referred exclusively to the

    place that was their first choice; participants were

    asked to complete the Spanish version (Hidalgo &

    Hernandez, 2001) of the Perceived Restorativeness

    Scale (Korpela & Hartig, 1996), and a battery of11 questions related to the (sensorial and semiotic)

    aesthetic attributes identified by Nasar (1994,

    1997) and those featured in the research by

    Galindo (1994). Specifically, participants were

    asked for their opinion about the extent to which

    their first choice had the following characteristics:

    (1) vegetation; (2) visual diversity/richness; (3)

    harmony/congruence between its different ele-

    ments; (4) openness and/or spaciousness; (5)

    luminosity; (6) a historic place or representative

    of the city; (7) cleanliness; (8) maintenance/upkeep;

    (9) place for leisure activities; (10) meeting place;

    AESTHETIC JUDGEMENT OF URBAN SCENES 21

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    4/8

    (11) a novel place. Participants had to assess each

    characteristic on a 5-point scale with values

    ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (a lot). Participants

    were also asked to state how often they visited or

    walked by their first choice. Once they had

    completed the attractive places section, partici-

    pants were asked to place themselves on the other

    side of the aesthetic spectrum and name, in theiropinion, the three most unattractive places in

    the city. These places were assessed with the same

    variables as the most attractive places. Finally,

    the questionnaire included a battery of questions

    related to sample identification data considered

    to be variables of interest in the study (gender,

    age, length of residence in the city, level of

    familiarity with the city, and the extent to which

    they identify with the city).

    RESULTS

    Most attractive and most unattractive

    places in the city: General categorization

    and frequency of use

    First, all the first choices were recorded and

    classified depending on the type or category of

    place to which they belonged. As a provisional

    instrument, the general system of categories

    obtained in Galindos (1994) study was used.

    The analysis of the characteristics of the spatial

    and functional configuration of the places men-

    tioned by the participants yielded a total of five

    categories, three of which were the same as in

    Galindos (1994) study: cultural-historical

    places, recreational places for leisure and/or

    walking, and housing areas. However, our

    findings suggested that two new categories not

    considered in the aforementioned study should be

    introduced: panoramic places and industrial

    places, while the category places for having fun

    was excluded. Table 1 shows the representativeness

    of each of the categories established in the list of

    the most attractive and unattractive places in the

    city (those in the first place), from the percentage

    of responses in each category.

    The data shown in Table 1 reflect a notable

    difference in the percentages of responses asso-

    ciated with each of the categories identified

    depending on the aesthetic label (attractive-

    unattractive). The attractive places weremainly grouped around the following categories:

    recreational places (mentioned by 38.75% of

    participants), historical-cultural places (men-

    tioned by 35.65%), and panoramic places

    (mentioned by 21.7%). On the other hand, these

    three categories are scarce or even nonexistent in

    the unattractive places, with the most numerous

    being housing areas (73.17%), mostly working-

    class housing estates, followed by industrial

    areas (8.13%).

    If we consider the specific places that form the

    categories themselves, rather than the categories assuch, we find that 28 different places were

    mentioned as the most attractive places, while

    there were 45 most unattractive places. In aesthetic

    terms, the most highly appreciated place in the city

    was the Gibralfaro Castle (n526, 19.7%) and,

    more specifically, the panoramic view of the city

    from the castle, which is why it was placed in the

    panoramic places category. In second place (n517,

    12.9%) came the Paseo Martimo (the Promenade),

    a long esplanade running along the seafront which

    we placed in recreational places for leisure/walking.

    In third and fourth position came two places

    closely related to the citys history (placed in the

    category historical-cultural places): the Alcazaba,

    an Arab fortress dating from the 11th century

    (n515, 11.4%), and the Cathedral, built between

    the 16th and 18th centuries (n514, 10.6%). The

    rest of the places were mentioned by less than 10%

    of the sample, with 13 places being mentioned by

    just one participant (0.8% of the sample). As for

    the most unattractive places, the most mentioned

    was the neighbourhood of La Palmilla (n541,

    30.8%), a working-class district with a high crime

    TABLE 1

    Frequency of most attractive and most unattractive places associated with each established category

    Most

    attractive places

    Most

    unattractive places

    Frequency Percentage Frequency Percentage

    Recreational places (for leisure or walking) 50 38.75 7 5.69

    Cultural-historical places 46 35.65 9 7.21

    Panoramic places 28 21.70 0 0

    Housing areas 4 3.10 90 73.17

    Industrial places 1 0.70 10 8.13

    22 GALINDO AND HIDALGO

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    5/8

    rate. The second, mentioned by 10.5% of subjects,

    was another working-class area (housing area) with

    a high crime rate. The remaining places were

    mentioned by less than 10% of participants, with

    31 places being cited on just one occasion. Figure 1

    shows the frequency with which people visit these

    places: 11.5% go to the most attractive place daily

    (against 7.8% who go to the most unattractive

    place); 18.3% go there once a week (11.6% go to

    the most unattractive place); 14.5% go several

    times a month (10.9% to the most unattractive);

    24.4% go once a month (10.9% to the most

    unattractive); 21.4% have not been recently

    (17.8% to the most unattractive place); 8.4%

    hardly ever go (31% to the most unattractive

    place); and 1.5 have never gone to the most

    attractive place (10.1% to the most unattractive

    place). Therefore, if we look at the percentages of

    accumulated answers, 68.4% of the sample go atleast once a month to the most attractive place,

    while the majority (58.8%) never, or hardly ever,

    go to the places considered least attractive.

    Characterization of the most attractive

    and most unattractive places on the

    Perceived Restorativeness Scale

    The citys most attractive places obtain high

    average scores on the Perceived Restorativeness

    Scale (PRS), always over 6, in contrast to the

    unattractive places whose values range from 1 to 3on the scale (see Table 2). To check whether such

    differences were statistically significant, the aver-

    age scores obtained were contrasted using the T-

    test analytical procedure for related samples. All

    the differences were statistically significant. The

    two types of scene differed both in general

    restorativeness (129)526.42, and in fascination

    (128)524.59, compatibility (128)519.79, being

    away (128)518.05, coherence (129)517.87, and

    extent (128)517.81 (p, .0001 in all cases).

    Characterization of most attractive

    and most unattractive places

    according to the sensorial and

    semiotic aesthetic attributes

    In order to meet the third objective, we calculated

    the average scores obtained by the attractive and

    unattractive places (the first choices) for each of

    the aesthetic attributes analysed. In addition, to

    check that the resulting differences were statisti-cally significant, the T-test for related samples was

    used. As with the Restorativeness Scale, places in

    the city considered the most attractive obtained

    the highest scores in all the variables considered

    (see Figure 2). The analyses carried out showed

    that the differences were statistically significant in

    all cases (p, .0001): novel place (128)512.42,

    facilities for leisure activities (125)510.57,

    Figure 1. Frequency of use of most attractive and most unattractive places.

    TABLE 2

    Characterization of most attractive and most unattractive

    places on the Perceived Restorativeness Scale

    Subscales (PRS)

    Most attractive

    place

    Most unattractive

    place

    M SD M SD

    Being away 6.53 1.99 2.31 1.76Fascination 6.73 1.68 1.67 1.74

    Coherence 6.80 1.67 2.89 2.05

    Compatibility 6.32 2.02 2.00 1.68

    Extent 7.00 1.73 3.56 1.81

    General restorat. 6.66 1.45 2.35 1.16

    PRS: Perceived Restorativeness Scale. Values fall on a scale

    from 0 to 10 where lower values indicate, for example, lower

    being away. Difference scores for the two types of places

    were statistically significant in all the subscales (p, .0001).

    AESTHETIC JUDGEMENT OF URBAN SCENES 23

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    6/8

    presence of vegetation (129)511.03, meeting place

    (128)59.97, cleanliness (129)517.22, upkeep/

    maintenance (128)517.18, harmony or congruence

    of scenic elements (128)519.34, visual diversity or

    richness (129)525.86, luminosity (128)59.52, his-

    toric or emblematic place (128)519.12, openness

    and spaciousness (128)512.71.

    Relationship between the Perceived

    Restorativeness Scale and the aesthetic

    attributes

    A correlation analysis was performed between the

    aesthetic attributes and the scores on the Perceived

    Restorativeness Scale. The results are shown in

    Table 3. Of the different attributes evaluated, five

    correlate significantly (p, .01) with restorative

    capacity: harmony, openness, luminosity, suitabil-

    ity for leisure, and meeting place. Of these, the last

    two, along with openness, also present significant

    correlations with the five subscales, although in

    some cases at a lower level (p, .05) of significance

    (with the exception of the correlation between

    harmony and fascination, which is not significant).

    On the other hand, it is worth pointing out that in

    our study the presence of vegetation, a character-

    istic closely related to the restorative capacity of a

    place, does not present a significant correlation

    with the total scale or with any of the factors.

    Neither does the level of upkeep/maintenance, a

    variable closely associated with a preference for

    urban spaces, present a significant correlation with

    the PRS. The three remaining attributesdiversity,

    cleanliness, and historical characteristicspresent

    correlation values that, although low, are in some

    cases significant at a reliability level of .05.

    DISCUSSION

    The analysis of the characteristics of the most

    attractive and most unattractive places in the city

    Figure 2. Average scores in the aesthetic attributes of the most attractive and unattractive places.

    TABLE 3Correlations between the aesthetic attributes and the Perceived Restorativeness Scale

    Being away Fascination Coherence Compatib. Extent General restorat.

    Vegetation .08 .14 .21 .07 .11 .14

    Visual richness .10 .15 .14 .00 .17* .13

    Congruence .26** .15 .26** .18* .26** .27**

    Openness .30** .23** .27** .28** .38** .36**

    Luminosity .19* .15 .16 .30** .17* .26**

    Historical place 2.12 .02 2.07 2.20* .06 2.09

    Cleanliness .05 .11 .20* .05 .19* .13

    Maintenance 2.13 .07 2.01 2.04 .00 2.04

    Facilities for leisure .20* .30** .18* .46** .19* .35**

    Meeting place .30** .21* .28** .43** .22** .37**

    *p,.05 (bilateral); **p,.01 (bilateral).

    24 GALINDO AND HIDALGO

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    7/8

    identified in the study has allowed us to establish

    the existence of five broad aesthetic categories of

    urban scenes: (1) recreational places for leisure/

    walking, (2) historical/cultural places, (3) places

    with panoramic views, (4) housing areas, and (5)

    industrial areas. The first three categories men-

    tioned form 96% of the visually most attractive

    places; the last two groupings form the majority ofthe settings considered most unattractive. Three

    categories (historical-cultural places, recreational

    places, and housing areas) agree with the char-

    acterization made in Galindos (1994) study, which

    was used as a provisional instrument in this study.

    In our opinion, this suggests that the function and

    historical value (cultural representativity) of a

    particular scenein other words, its social leg-

    ibility, to use Stokols and Shumakers (1981)

    termseem to constitute two important dimen-

    sions providing the basis for the establishment of

    aesthetic categories that are important in urbancontexts. Although the function of a scene has

    been an important selection criterion in urban

    landscape preference studies that have incorpo-

    rated photographic stimuli, the historical-cultural

    dimension has been rather neglected. The impor-

    tance that this dimension acquires when evalua-

    tion procedures are developed that facilitate its

    appearance seems to confirm the need for its

    inclusion in contextualized analyses in scenes that

    are familiar to and/or known by subjects.

    The groupings labelled places with panoramic

    views and industrial areas provide two cate-

    gories not included in Galindos (1994) previous

    study. The first category groups together 21.7% of

    the scenes identified as the most attractive and

    includes the place that was named in first position

    by the greatest number of subjects, the Gibralfaro

    Castle. This place presents a set of physical

    characteristics that help explain its high scenic

    evaluation. It is an Arab fortress dating from the

    14th century, which was built into the side of a

    mountain that was originally beyond the city

    walls. The site was chosen for its strategic position,

    overlooking the city and the sea beyond. Due to

    growth and expansion it has now become part of

    the city, although certain planning laws have

    limited urban development in its immediate

    surroundings. It has therefore managed to pre-

    serve a significant amount of the surrounding

    vegetation and is an important transit point

    between the city and nature. The visual config-

    uration characteristics of this place tie in directly

    with Appletons (1975) prospect-refuge theory,

    according to which the qualities of the setting that

    make it aesthetically preferred allow one to see

    without being seen (refuge) and provide a wide

    viewing area (prospect). The settings that possess

    these characteristicsin other words, that allow

    the individual to observe the scene from a safe

    viewing point and with a wide perspective (open

    places)will also be the aesthetically preferred

    settings. The last of the categories, the industrial

    areas, represents the second most frequently

    selected unattractive places, which, together withthe role this variable has in studies of environ-

    mental preference, confirms it as a category to be

    taken into account in future urban landscape

    samples.

    The differences found between this study and

    Galindos (1994) may be due to the different

    characteristics of the geographical settings evalu-

    ated. The city of Seville lacks public places that

    could be clearly identified as panoramic places,

    and in Malaga the places for having fun, a

    category in the Seville study that does not appear

    in this one, are spread throughout the city and notconcentrated in one geographical area. The

    industrial areas category is one that appeared

    in the responses to the most unattractive areas, a

    quality that was not evaluated in the first study. In

    any case, the categories that have emerged in the

    present study should be confirmed in other

    geographical settings and using other nonexplora-

    tory procedures; an aim that we consider to be of

    great importance for research in this field.

    Another of the goals of the present study was to

    explore the role that the restorative capacity of a

    place, in terms of the Attentional Restoration

    Theory, may play in its aesthetic evaluation. To do

    this, subjects evaluated two places (the ones

    considered most attractive and most unattractive)

    using the Spanish version (Hidalgo & Hernandez,

    2001) of the Perceived Restorativeness Scale

    (Korpela & Hartig, 1996). The results obtained

    show that the restorative capacity of a place seems

    to be a characteristic dimension of those places

    considered to be attractive. These places obtained

    a high score both on the general scale (average

    6.66) and on the five subscales, with values ranging

    from 6.32 in the compatibility factor and 7.00 in

    the extent factor (see Table 2). These results

    support those obtained by Korpela and Hartig

    (1996) and Korpela et al. (2001), in which they

    analysed the differences in restorative capacity

    between favourite places and hostile/disagreeable

    places. In the latter study, the greatest differences

    between the two types of place were recorded in

    the subscales of evasion and compatibility.

    However, the results of our researchgiven the

    objective of characterizing the main scenic

    resources of a cityshow that the main differences

    between attractive and unattractive places seem to

    AESTHETIC JUDGEMENT OF URBAN SCENES 25

  • 8/8/2019 Aesthetic Preferences and the Attribution of Meaning

    8/8

    lie in the characteristics grouped in the fascination

    subscale. This subscale includes aspects related to

    the quantity of information available in the

    evaluated place and is closely related to the

    psychological exploration process considered to

    be one of the basic cognitive requirements linked

    to environmental aesthetics in Attentional Resto-

    ration Theory (see, for example, Kaplan & Kaplan,1989). In short, the results mentioned support the

    suggestions of recent studies (Peron et al., 2002;

    Purcell et al., 2001) that the restorative capacity

    of a specific place may constitute one of the

    implicit scenic quality criteria used by subjects in

    their aesthetic judgements.

    Furthermore, the scores that the attractive and

    unattractive places obtained regarding the aes-

    thetic attributes included in this study demonstrate

    their suitability in the study of aesthetic prefer-

    ences, and therefore suggest that these attributes,

    together with restorative capacity, constituteimportant criteria for determining scenic quality.

    The relation that could be established between

    both types of criteria raises a theoretical and

    empirical issue that could be dealt with in future

    studies. The results of the correlation analyses

    between the score obtained in the PRS and these

    attributesdeveloped in an exploratory manner in

    this worksuggest that restorative places tend to

    be open, luminous, and coherent, and are places

    for leisure and for meeting people. It is, however,

    surprising that the presence of vegetation, a

    characteristic closely related to the restorative

    capacity of a place in other research, did not have

    a significant relationship with the factors on the

    Perceived Restorativeness Scale. Given the

    exploratory nature of these analyses, it may be

    premature to venture an explanation. The possi-

    bility does exist, however, that other aesthetic

    attributes, such as those mentioned here, might

    have a greater influence on the evaluation of a

    place as a restorative setting. Future research

    along these lines will clarify this hypothesis.

    REFERENCES

    Appleton, J. (1975). The experience of landscape.London: John Wiley.

    Appleton, J. (1987). Landscape as prospect and refuge.In J. A. Jackle (Ed.), The visual elements of landscape(pp.3974). Amherst, MA: The University ofMassachusetts Press.

    Galindo, M. P. (1994). Evaluacion de la preferenciaambiental de paisajes urbanos. Hacia un modelo

    psicosocial de caracter integrador (in Spanish).Unpublished doctoral thesis, Universidad deSevilla.

    Hartig, T., Korpela, K., Evans, G. W., & Garling, T.(1997). A measure of restorative quality in environ-ments. Scandinavian Housing and Planning Research,14, 175194.

    Herzog, T. R. (1985). A cognitive analysis of preference

    for waterscapes. Journal of EnvironmentalPsychology, 5, 225241.

    Hidalgo, M. C., & Hernandez, B. (2001). Si te sientesbien te recuperaras mejor: La evaluacion de loslugares restauradores In C. San Juan, B. Rodrguez,& A. Vergara (Eds.), Monografas Socioambientales:VII Congreso de Psicologa Ambiental. HumanHabitatsXXI (in Spanish). Barcelona: PublicationsUniversitat de Barcelona.

    Kaplan, S. (1987). Aesthetics, affect and cognition:Environmental preferences from an evolutionaryperspective. Environment and Behavior, 19, 332.

    Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature:Toward an integrative framework. Journal of

    Environmental Psychology, 15, 169182.Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1989). The experience ofnature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

    Korpela, M. K., & Hartig, T. (1996). Restorativequalities of favourite places. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, 221233.

    Korpela, M. K., Hartig, T., Kaiser, F. G., & Fuhrer, U.(2001). Restorative experience and self-regulation infavorite places. Environment and Behavior, 3,572589.

    Nasar, J. L. (1994). Urban design aesthetics. Theevaluative qualities of building exteriors.Environment and Behavior, 26, 377401.

    Nasar, J. L. (1997). New developments in aesthetics forurban design. In G. T. Moore & R. W. Marans(Eds.), Advances in environment, behavior, and design,Vol. 4 (pp. 149193). New York: Plenum Press.

    Peron, E., Berto, R., & Purcell, A. T. (2002).Restorativeness, preference and the perceived natur-alness of places. Medio Ambiente y ComportamientoHumano, 3(1), 1934.

    Peron, E., Purcell, A. T., Staats, H. J., Falchero, S., &Lamb, R. J. (1998). Models for outdoor scenes: Someexperimental evidence. Environment and Behavior, 33,261305.

    Purcell, A. T., Lamb, R. J., Peron, E., & Falchero, S.(1994). Preference or preferences for landscape?Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14, 195209.

    Purcell, A. T., Peron, E., & Berto, R. (2001). Why dopreferences differ between scene types? Environmentand Behavior, 33, 93106.

    Stokols, D., & Shumaker, S. A. (1981). People in places:A transactional view of settings. In J. H. Harvey(Ed.), Cognition, social behavior, and the environment.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response tonatural environment. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill(Eds.), Behavior and the natural environment(pp. 85125). New York: Plenum Press.

    26 GALINDO AND HIDALGO