and perceptions of mathematics learners - uw-stout anxiety levels and perceptions of mathematics...

61
The Anxiety Levels and Perceptions of Mathematics Learners from a Midwestern Technical College on Selected Classroom Climate Factors in Mitigating the Effects of Math Anxiety Cheryl M. Sutter A Research Paper Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Science Degree in Career and Technical Education Concentration: Teaching Approved: 2 Semester Credits The Graduate School University of Wisconsin-Stout May, 2006

Upload: nguyendung

Post on 21-Mar-2018

218 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

The Anxiety Levels and Perceptions of Mathematics Learners

from a Midwestern Technical College on

Selected Classroom Climate Factors in

Mitigating the Effects of Math Anxiety

Cheryl M. Sutter

A Research Paper Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Master of Science Degree

in

Career and Technical Education

Concentration: Teaching

Approved: 2 Semester Credits

The Graduate School

University of Wisconsin-Stout

May, 2006

The Graduate School University of Wisconsin-Stout

Menomonie, WI

Author: Sutter, Cheryl M.

Title: The Perceptions of Mathematics Learners from a Midwestern

Technical College on Selected Classroom Climate Factors in

Mitigating the Effects of Math Anxiety

Graduate Degree/ Major: MS Career and Technical Education

Research Advisor: Amy Gillett, Ph.D.

MontWY ear: May, 2006

Number of Pages: 54

Style Manual Used: American Psychological Association, 5th edition

Abstract

Adult learners (n=47) from a technical college were surveyed for their

mathematics anxiety level, their perceptions on 10 relational coping strategies, and 17

affective learning environment qualities, related to potential reduction in math anxiety.

The strategies and qualities were of an interpersonal and intrapersonal nature. Strategies

asked for use and helpfulness; qualities requested interest and helpfulness to decreasing

tension or worry. Brain function and resiliency validate promoting emotional health in

math education. Results across low, medium, and high anxiety learners showed use of

peer-related strategies as most used and most helpful while instructor and counselor

strategies were much lower. Medium anxiety learners used strategies the most, believed

to result from the combination of high need and sufficient academic self-efficacy to

pursue strategies.

Qualities most helpful were an instructor who responds to class needs, friendships

in class, appropriate humor, class discussion, and working with peers. Competition and

electronic discussion were wanted by less than 7% of the participants.

Acknowledgments

My thesis admittedly took on a topic that has been written about many times. To

venture into having anything newer to contribute or concrete to offer made my start to

this process full of reservation. But having a gut feeling that there must be something

worth thinking about on math anxiety that offers someone a hint or some language to

meaningfully connect with learners who struggle with math anxiety, gnawed at me. It is

with deep gratitude that the fuzzy beginnings and reformulation periods were supported

by my thesis advisor, Dr. Amy Gillett. I have been buoyed by her sincere interest and

encouragement in the short exchanges (and the not so short) we have had throughout this

process. To feel commensurate support for a challenge is a gift and an educational ideal.

Many thanks to Dr. Gillett for modeling this so well.

I would also like to anonymously thank the midwestem technical college and the

math instructor who coordinated my access to their adult learners as participants for this

study. Finding out what these learners thought was an audience I wanted to tap. The

access and their participation have been appreciated.

Table of Contents

....................................................................................................................................... Page

. . ............................................................................................................................... Abstract 11

. . .................................................................................................................... List of Tables vll

........................................................................................................ Chapter I: Introduction 1

Background .................................................................................................................... 1

.................................................................................................. Statement of the Problem 2

Purpose of the Study ......................................................................................................... 3

Research Questions ......................................................................................................... 4

................................................................................................... Signrficance of the Study 4

Assumptions ..................................................................................................................... 5

Scope and Delimitations ................................................................................................... 5

........................................................................................................... Definition of Terms 6

............................................................................................. Chapter 11: Literature Review 7

Introduction .................................................................................................................... 7

Anxiety Described ............................................................................................................. 7

........................................................................ What is the Brain Doing During Anxiety? 9

................................ Learner Reactions to Math Anxiety .. ........................................... 11

..................................................... Coping Strategies and Classroom Climate Qualities 12

........................................................................................................................ Resiliency 16

Chapter 111: Methodology ................................................................................................. 20

Introduction .................................................................................................................... 20

Selection and Description of Subjects ........................................................................... 20

............................................................................................................... Instrumentation 20

Data Collection ............................................................................................................... 22

........................................................................................ Data Processing and Analysis 23

...................................................................................................................... Limitations 24

Chapter IV: Results ........................................................................................................... 26

.......................................................................................... ................... Introduction .. 26

................................................................................................................. Demographics 26

................................................................................................ Anxiety Composite Score 29

............................................................................................................ Coping Strategies 30

................................................................... Learning Environment/Relational Qualities 36

.......................................... Chapter V: Discussion, Conclusions, and Recommendations 39

................................................................................... Introduction ...................... ... 39

....................................................................................................................... Discussion 39

Conclusions ................................................................................................................. 41

Recommendations ........................................................................................................... 43

......................................................................................................................... References 46

.............................................. Appendix A: Developmental Assets Related to Academia 50

Appendix B: Survey .......................................................................................................... 51

List of Tables

. .............................. Table 1: Coping Strategy Usage Comparison Peskoff Study 13

...................................... Table 2: Math Background Variables by Anxiety Level 28

..................................... Table 3 : Strategy Usage and Helpfulness - Low Anxiety 31

................................ Table 4: Strategy Usage and Helpfulness - Medium Anxiety 32

.................................... Table 5: Strategy Usage and Helpfulness - High Anxiety 33

Table 6: Coping Strategy Usage Comparison - Current Study ............................... 34

................................................ Table 7: Coping Strategy Helpfulness Rankings 35

Table 8: Interest in Classroom Qualities ......................................................... 37

Table 9: Top Ranked Qualities to Reduce Tension ............................................. 38

Chapter I: Introduction

"I just don't get it. Math isn't my thing and I'll never understand it." Learners

facing a mathematics course who believe similar sentiments tend to feel defeated before

they even begin the course. A sense of shutting down often occurs (Holley & Steiner,

2005; Godbey, 1997). Anecdotal evidence of this phenomenon is commonplace and

mathematics anxiety has been documented across the country's spectrum of learner age

ranges and their educational settings (Perry, 2004; Taylor & Fraser, 2003; Safford-

Ramus, 2001).

Having a fear or dread of mathematics, whether found in academia or infiltrated

with daily living, leaves some people cognitively and emotionally "stuck." Distancing

themselves from mathematics may be a preferred reaction, but doing so exacerbates their

lack of mathematical confidence and competency (Ryan, Gheen, & Midgley, 1998). By

fearing math or assuming dismal results for the effort, individuals steer their career and

leisure choices so they can minimize their math anxiety (Bankhead, 2002; Tobias, 1991).

Background

The early 1980's marked a new link between brain research and educational

implications. Brain research has increasingly been considered through an educational lens

in an effort to disclose conditions under which attention, memory, and conceptual

understanding are impacted by the learning environment. Brain research allows

understanding how the brain develops and changes over time, when critical periods are

optimal for particular development (e.g. language, vision), and how the brain

compensates for loss or damage (Sousa, 2006: Sprenger, 1999). Brain research findings

have helped inform best practices within education in many ways and it offers insight as

to why math anxiety tends to be a chronic condition unless countered in meaningful

ways. This will be discussed further in the literature review.

Anxiety, in general, has been challenging to define in terms of which aspects to

include in its construct. The Merriam-Webster Collegiate Dictionary (2000) included the

doubt a person has about a situation and one's capability to adequately respond to it. The

New Oxford American Dictionary (2001) recognized the aspect of an uncertain outcome.

Mathematics anxiety is considered a state-specific form of general anxiety. Further

description of general and math anxieties, along with symptoms of math anxiety are

included the literature review.

A healthy learning climate is commonly understood as integral to the success of

all learners, but may be particularly significant for math anxiety learners. This study

sought literature about classroom climate components in relation to math anxiety. It

focused on those that signal the importance of interpersonal and intrapersonal

relationships.

Statement of the Problem

Safford-Ramus (2001) reported that between 1980 and 2000, only 10% of

abstracts for published, mathematically-oriented dissertations (n=12) were related to

interventions for adults with math anxiety. Furthermore, the dissertations neglected

certain aspects of mathematics education, including communication through writing and

cooperative activities (Safford-Ramus, 2001). These topics relate to the affective and

relational natures of math education which have been the angle of math anxiety

considered in this study.

Math anxiety has several inherent challenges related to its study. One challenge is

working with definitions that emphasize different aspects of the phenomenon, of which

different learners experience various aspects and with different intensities. Another

challenge regards the ongoing assimilation of current brain research findings and how

they develop or hone subsequent attempts to decrease math anxiety. And considering

math anxiety is an "o ld topic, it may be challenging in some circles to keep collegial

conversation fresh and energy directed on potentially helpful strategies. There is great

demand on academic departments to meet a broadening array of institutional and

community needs. In recognition of expanding and competing demands of education, it is

hoped that developing an understanding of current brain research, honing the learning

environment toward affective health, and acquiring frequent learner input can efficiently

contribute toward minimizing math anxiety.

In this study, perceptions by a sample of post-secondary math learners were

collected to offer their perspectives on the prevalence and or effectiveness of selected

coping strategies and classroom climate qualities related to learning math. The learners

were surveyed to ascertain their math anxiety levels for potential relationship to their

perception data.

Purpose Statement

The purpose of this study was to document and compare math anxiety levels with

perceptions of math learners from a midwestern technical college on interpersonal and

intrapersonal coping strategies and affective classroom climate factors potentially

available within math learning environments. Data was collected in April 2006 using a

three part survey.

Research Questions

The following questions helped guide the search for relevant literature and target

the nature of the researcher's interests.

1. Are there any approaches to understanding or conceptualizing math anxiety

that might freshen conversation on this topic?

2. As customers of math education, what do learners of a midwestern

technical college perceive as helpful coping strategies for their math learning

environment that involve personal relationships?

3. Beyond coping strategies, what support or relational learning qualities do

these learners associate with tension reduction in math?

Significance ofthe Study

With math anxiety's presence over decades and across learner age groups, the

psychological and educational professions continue to deal with a phenomenon that

hinders the pursuit and enjoyment by learners of activities in their personal, academic,

and professional lives intertwining with mathematical content.

Math anxiety reduction appears related to a healthy learning climate. One

synonym used for a healthy learning climate is safe space. Little information has been

written to account for a learner's perspective of safe space (Holley & Steiner, 2005).

Determining factors that learners view as creating or enhancing a healthy learning

climate, with an emphasis on factors that reduce excess tension and worry, is a goal with

professional practice implications.

Assumptions

Research reported for this study attempted to focus specifically on post-secondary

math learners. When content was not found to address this population, some research

using other populations (e.g. younger math learners or post-secondary non-math learners)

was reported. While there was no assumption that findings for these populations transfer

in a one-to-one fashion to post-secondary math learners, it was assumed that the research

had something valuable to add to the discourse.

It was also assumed that all survey participants have provided honest and candid

feedback to all survey items.

Scope and Delimitations

The scope of this study involved one midwestern technical college which has

multiple campuses within a state system. The sample of learners who participated in the

survey was currently attending the same campus during April 2006. At the time,

participants were enrolled in one of the following face-to-face math courses:

Trigonometry, Business Math, or Introduction to College Mathematics. These courses

were taken as requirements within their respective programs. The desired sample size for

each course was 50 or more, however 30 or more would allow applying correlation

statistics within and between courses. Fewer than 30 participants per course would limit

the statistics to frequencies and measures of central tendency. The degree of

generalizability to this campus for these courses is dependent on how representative the

sample is of the population attending this campus. Generalizability beyond this campus

can only be speculative, given sample sizes and the amount of demographic information

collected to compare with other populations.

Definition of Terms

Below are several terms defined according to their use in this study. The term

anxiety, shown below, and the term math anxiety, not shown below, will both have

further description in the literature review.

Anxiety -

an abnormal and overwhelming sense of apprehension and fear often marked by

physiological signs (as sweating, tension, and increased pulse), by doubt

concerning the reality and nature of the threat, and by self-doubt about one's

capacity to cope with it. (Merriam-Webster Collegiate Dictionary, 2000, p. 53)

Classroom climate or learning climate - the affective nature of the space and

relationships comprising a learning environment; this includes, but is not limited to, the

aesthetics, comfort, and appropriateness of the learning space and the levels of mutual

respect, personal sharing, resource access, inclusion of alternate viewpoints, support and

encouragement, risk and reward, and the placement of decision making in the

environment.

Safe space -

a classroom climate that allows students to feel secure enough to

take risks, honestly express their views, and share and explore their knowledge,

attitudes, and behaviors. Safety in this sense does not refer to physical safety.

Instead, classroom safe space refers to protection from psychological or emotional

harm. (Holley & Steiner, 2005, p. 50)

Self-efficacy - the thoughts and beliefs about one's power or capacity to produce

a desired effect; the thoughts and beliefs about one's personal ability to achieve results.

Chapter 11: Literature Review

Introduction

This chapter will present interrelated ideas for understanding math anxiety and

selected approaches to helping reduce it. These include brief descriptions of general

anxiety and math anxiety, a section on brain function, a section on learner reactions under

anxiety perceived conditions, and a look at classroom qualities and coping strategies

considered helpful to minimizing anxiety. The classroom qualities and coping strategies

of focus in this study relate to affective and relational characteristics shared among

learners and the instructor when participating in face-to-face classroom settings. An

emphasis on resiliency is considered.

Anxiety Described

Baloglu (1999) has researched definitions of general anxiety, mathematics

anxiety, and statistics anxiety in a comprehensive attempt to consolidate years of

descriptors for these phenomena. Statistics anxiety will not be considered in the current

study. He found variations and occasional discrepancies between the 1950's to the time

of his writing, however three general anxiety constructs surfaced.

General anxiety is composed of trait anxiety and state anxiety, two components

which are widely accepted as descriptive of anxiety (Baloglu, 1999). Trait anxiety is the

individual's natural propensity toward anxiety and is considered a relatively stable,

personal characteristic, while state anxiety refers to a specific situation perceived as

threatening that varies in duration. General anxiety may also be viewed as comprising

cognitive and affective components (Olson, cited in Baloglu, 1999). This construct

attempts to distinguish between worry-type elements of anxiety and its emotional

elements. A third construct considers general anxiety as having cognitive, behavioral, and

emotional elements (Wine, cited in Baloglu, 1999). The constructs are not exclusive of

one another.

Math anxiety is "an inability by an otherwise intelligent person to cope with

quantification, and more generally, mathematics" (Kranz, cited in Perry, 2004, p. 321).

Similarly it has been described as "the feelings of tension and anxiety that interfere with

the manipulation of numbers and solving of mathematical problems in a wide variety of

ordinary life and academic situations" (Richardson & Suinn, cited in Baloglu, 1999, p. 4).

Less definitional sounding, Kitchens (1 995, p. 6) indicated "any feeling that prevents you

from learning math in a natural way as you did as a young child.. .is math anxiety."

Kitchens placed emphasis on the learner's thoughts and fears while downplaying any lack

of capacity to learn math. And while a comprehensive review of math anxiety definitions

over time indicate there has not been full agreement on what it is (Kazelskis, cited in

Baloglu, 1999), it is not to include or to be confused with the construct of test anxiety

(Arem, 2003), a separate phenomenon about test taking skills or fears of being evaluated.

Symptoms of math anxiety experienced by different learners are many. They can

include nausea, perspiration, hot tingling sensations, extreme nervousness, inability to

hear the instructor, upset or distraction from noises like crumpling paper, inability to

concentrate, negative self-talk, headache, stomachache, muscle tension, a blank mind,

sweaty palms, shortness of breath, and others (Arem, 2003; Kitchens, 1995).

Anxiety's symptoms arise from the perceptions and messages a learner processes

(Arem, 2003; Kitchens, 1995). Messages may be personal in the form of thoughts and

self-talk (a continuous inner dialogue) or external in the form of verbal comments and

body language from those around the learner. The messages from people significant to

the learner can be particularly impactful, either positively or negatively, and both

researchers advocated careful selection of the company a math anxious learner keeps.

What Is The Brain Doing During Anxiety?

Prior to the 1 98OYs, learning about the human brain required autopsying cadavers,

a limited discovery mode given the organ was no longer functioning. Recent decades

have seen tremendous growth in neuroscience due to brain-imaging and testing

techniques (Sousa, 2006; Sprenger, 1999), allowing real time acquisition of brain data in

response to varying stimuli.

Some brain anatomy background allows understanding how certain approaches to

minimizing math anxiety have validity grounded in neuroscience. To that end, a brief

orientation to the brain follows.

Scientists have divided the exterior regions of the brain into four lobe areas with

different, and some overlapping, functions (Sousa, 2006). The frontal lobes are behind

the forehead and continue to mature into early adulthood. This region is responsible for

planning, judgment, higher-order thinking, problem solving, regulating excesses of the

emotion system, and personality. This is where most of working memory happens, so it is

an area where focus occurs. The temporal lobes are above the ears and responsible for

sound, music, speech (primarily on the left), face and object recognition, and some long

term memory. The occipital lobes reside at the middle back of the brain and handle visual

processing. The parietal lobes are at the top back of the brain and deal with spatial

orientation, calculation, and certain types of recognition.

The brainstem is evolutionarily the oldest and physically the deepest brain area

(Sousa, 2006; Sprenger, 1999). It is the home base for 11 of 12 body nerves and it

monitors and controls heartbeat, respiration, temperature, and digestion. It also is home to

a screening system that determines which incoming sensory data is more important than

others. Data perceived as survival-related gets a highly important status, while other data

drops out within a few seconds. The brainstem is a survival-oriented structure, and the

physical symptoms experienced by learners with math anxiety indicate their brains have

perceived stress or threat.

The limbic system, minimally a convenient term for a group of four structures

with different functions, resides just above the brainstem in the center of the brain and

generally has functional duplication in both brain hemispheres (Sousa, 2006; Jensen,

1998). The system interacts with many parts of the brain and is responsible for emotions,

sleep, and the production of most of the brain's chemicals. One structure, the thalamus,

receives sensory data first and then monitors it for survival content, using past

experiences to determine importance (Sousa, 2006). Input of a higher priority inhibits

brain processing of lower priority data. Any threat is processed immediately, causing

adrenaline and other chemicals to be released brain-wide, shutting down unnecessary

brain activity. In this case, "unnecessary brain activity" refers to learning or other activity

not related to survival. The use of past experiences in determining importance is partly

due to the arnygdala, a limbic system structure thought to encode an emotional message,

assuming there is one, whenever a memory is marked for long-term memory. As a result,

as a long-term memory is recalled, say from sixth grade math class, cognitive elements

(e.g. definition, formula) and emotional elements (e.g. panic, failure) are part of recall.

Recalling the emotion is enough to re-experience the emotions and physical

manifestations from the memory.

When a learner experiences a threat, real or perceived, the brain reacts to the

situation the same way (Sprenger, 1999; Beck, cited in Baloglu, 1999). Anxiety hijacks

normal cognitive processing. Anything that embarrasses a learner becomes a threat that

inhibits the brain processes used in learning (Jensen, cited by Tileston, 2000). "When

threat exists, the brain operates in survival mode and while we can learn in that mode, we

do so at the expense of higher-order thinking" (p. 47).

Learner Reactions to Math Anxiety

Among learning strategies, help-seeking is a valuable skill. As young learners

mature, they begin to sense their own academic prowess and develop recognition for

when they need assistance (Ryan, Gheen, & Midgley, 1998). Help-seeking combines a

learning strategy with a social interaction, and both are important types of support (Ryan,

Patrick, & Shim, 2005). Math anxious people, however, feel they are alone (Salinas,

2004; Kitchens, 1995). They fear being judged as not having ability when they ask for

help (Arem, 2003; Ryan, Gheen, & Midgley, 1998, Tobias, 1993). Thinking it would be

far better to appear disinterested, not attend class, or not turn in homework than to be

identified as producing work reflecting low performance, some learners will opt to avoid

the very behaviors that could assist their learning. According to Spandel(2005), asking

learners about what grades mean to them indicates, "many students will tell you that the

worst grade to receive is a C because while an F means that you didn't try or didn't

care.. .a C indicates that you did your best but still failed - and that's depressing." Fear of

exposure and embarrassment prevents constructive action (Kitchens, 1995; Tobias,

1993).

Self-efficacy is the notion, one's thoughts and beliefs, about having the personal

power or capacity to produce a desired effect. Academic self-efficacy refers to a learner's

judgment about hisher capability to successfully complete schoolwork (Pintrich &

Schunk, cited in Ryan, Gheen, & Midgley, 1998). Separate fiom the possibility of lacking

prerequisite knowledge, most college undergraduates have the cognitive capacity to

handle advanced algebra, intermediate statistics, and college calculus but they do not

believe they can (Tobias, 1991). Learners with high levels of academic self-efficacy are

more likely to perceive help seeking as a constructive learning strategy (Ryan, Patrick, &

Shim, 2005); while learners with low academic self-efficacy are more likely to think their

help-seeking behaviors are interpreted by others as low ability. These learners are less

likely to seek help. Seeking help is positively related to the affective environment of the

learners' classroom (Stipeck et al., cited in Patrick et al., 2003).

Coping Strategies and Classroom Climate Qualities

A math anxiety study at a community college with 279 learners and 50 faculty

members from math and counseling departments was conducted (Peskoff, 2001). The

study was aimed at determining three things: learners' levels of anxiety with their

perceptions and use of ten coping strategies, math and counseling faculty perceptions of

helpfulness of those coping strategies, and a comparison of strategy helpfulness among

the three groups of participants. Three outcomes resulted.

First, low anxiety learners (as determined by the Composite Math Anxiety Scale)

employed a wider variety of strategies than the high anxiety learners. This was

understood as low anxiety learners were in a better position to pursue coping strategies,

considering that anxiety in general has a disabling effect on learners (Peskoff, 2001).

A second outcome (see Table 1) was that high anxiety learners used only 2 of the

10 coping strategies more than low anxiety learners (tutoring and counselor discussion)

and these 2 were considered by all learners as least helpful among the 10 (Peskoff, 2001).

Finally, there was general agreement among the groups regarding perceived

helpfulness of these strategies (Peskoff, 2001). All groups ranked the best two strategies

as completing one's homework on time and letting the instructor know when course

content was not being understood. They also ranked extra time to study for an exam and

asking questions in class as among the best strategies of the 10 studied. Staff, however,

considered tutoring a more helpful strategy than did learners.

Table 1 Coping Strategy Usage Comparison - Peskoff Study

Coping Strategy Utilized More By Learners With

Low Anxiety High Anxiety

Complete Homework On Time X

Communicate with Instructor if You Do

Not Understand X

Allow Extra Review Time Before Exams X

Ask Questions In Class X

Use Additional Texts or Review Books X

Discuss Class Experiences or Difficulties

with Other Learners X

Remind Yourself You Are A Good Student X

Include Relaxation (exercise, physical

activity, or systematic relaxation) X

Use Tutoring Services X

Discuss Difficulties with Counselor X

Math anxiety researchers, when considering the affective realm of learning math,

believe adult learners need opportunities to express their thoughts and feelings about

math and to become aware that other competent adults also have math uncertainties

(Godbey, 1997; Archambeault, 1993; Tobias, 199 1). Emotion either impedes or

motivates learning (Dirkx, 2001). Negative self-talk and negative feelings will prevent a

learner from concentrating on math (Tobias, 1991), while letting out frustration is

important to being able to focus attention on problem solving (Salinas, 2004).

Tobias (1991) held math clinics or math anxiety reduction programs where

university learners countered myths about learning math and discussed thoughts and

feelings about math. This process occurred en route to cultivating behaviors practiced by

strong math learners (i.e. hard work, persistence, alternate problem solving approaches).

According to Tobias (1993, 1991), influential to the clinics were: using personally written

math autobiographies to discover past rub spots or negative math experiences; meeting

with other math anxious learners to share feelings and thoughts (i.e. group de-tox

sessions), and gaining awareness of one's negative self-talk for learning to replace it with

more constructive messages. Clinic meeting time was separate from any class. It allowed

group bonding and created an atmosphere of growing trust that developed toward

incorporation of math content in small doses. The math instruction continually allowed

expression and processing of how learners perceived new math experiences. Tobias

claimed that over 600 university learners went through the clinic in the 1970's and 1980's

and all went on to take and pass calculus (1 991).

Written exercises within the math anxiety clinics helped learners grasp the

dialogue they continually heard inside themselves and to better express and challenge

those thoughts. This relates to one of the findings of a study among 24 pre-service

elementary teachers taking an undergraduate math course (Salinas, 2004). The use of

reflective notebooks, with entries requested daily after class, contained diary-like entries,

journal writing, and personal understandings. These notebooks were reviewed by the

instructor every one or two weeks, informing the instructor of questions and individual

insights in which to make class adjustments, and offering learners written comments and

encouragement made in response to their leggings. The study collected data via the

learner notebooks, an instructor notebook of content related to class happenings, learner

surveys at semester's end, and interviews of a random sample of learners. As a result, the

study revealed three themes.

Learner writings indicated they acquired new understandings of math, gained

awareness of their understanding relative to the learning community (they generally

worked in groups), and prompted their own self-evaluation (Salinas, 2004). New

understandings represented things like math does not always have one right answer nor a

single approach for problem solving, and math is not "'just know(ing) what steps to do,

but how and why we use the steps"' (Salinas, 2004, p. 320). Learning one's place in the

learning community came from sentiments of "'I'm not the only one"' (p. 322), "'I think

I work better with people"' (p. 323), and "'Being able to turn in my work and questions

and then receiving responses from the teacher helps me understand better"' (p. 323). Self-

evaluation came across in reflecting on one's thoughts and feelings or putting them in

some perspective. One learner wrote, "'Fractions have always scared me. I had a hard

time motivating myself to even attempt this homework. But, somehow reflecting in my

notebook gave me a way to let out what I felt"' (p. 324). Salinas indicated frustration and

attitudes were seen by learners as hampering their progress. The process of writing

allowed the frustration, as recorded by one learner, "(to) 'get it out of my system so I can

concentrate better"' (p. 325).

Resiliency

Promoting resiliency among learners may be an umbrella perspective to apply to

math competency (Malloy & Malloy, 1998) and by association, math anxiety. Resiliency

is the capacity to bounce back from adverse circumstances. Merriam-Webster defined it

as "an ability to recover from or adjust easily to misfortune or change" (2000, p. 993).

For learners with math anxiety, drawing upon their resiliency resources is part of

mounting a coping response. Sources to follow bring resiliency into the math picture,

pointing to its ties with interpersonal and intrapersonal health.

Learner characteristics, believed to contribute to passing a state mandated high

school Algebra I course, were identified by a group of math teachers (n=20) studied in

four North Carolina high schools that had general academic profiles at or above statewide

averages (Malloy & Malloy, 1998). Data collection methods included teacher

questionnaires, observations of teachers and students in algebra classes, pre- and post-

observation teacher interviews, school-based focus groups with teachers, and student

achievement data by school. The learner characteristics were corroborated by the

researchers and included "taking responsibility for learning, persistence, self-confidence,

risk taking, collaborative interactions, transfer of learning, enthusiasm, help seeking, and

sense of humor" (p. 3 15). The researchers considered these characteristics as themed

around resiliency. They proposed resiliency as worth infusion into math education for all

learners.

Practices employed by the teachers, as observed by Malloy and Malloy (1998),

supported resilient learner characteristics, even though the teachers primarily identified

themselves and were observed as users of traditional teaching methods. The practices

were constant monitoring of student learning (e.g. daily homework, expectations of

students explaining solutions to classmates, never accepting self-defeating responses to

challenging problems); encouraging alternate routes to problem solving (e.g. allowed

students who developed nontraditional solutions to share them with the class, prompting

others to explore other routes); encouraging risk taking (e.g. pairing marginal and higher-

performing students together to make contributions collectively, learners seeing that

correct or incorrect work can be learned from); creating enthusiasm for a stimulating

class; and using collaboration and humor (e.g. encouraged conversation that kept learners

on task, allowed working together to buoy spirits when work was difficult).

From the focus group feedback with the teachers, the researchers reported,

a rigid, no-nonsense approach in traditional mathematics does not hinder the

progress of high-performing students because their self-confidence and

persistence thrive in an austere and competitive atmosphere. But conversely, that

same atmosphere tends to create apprehension on the part of the low and marginal

students because it tends to stultify creativity and discourage risk-taking behavior

and collaboration. (Malloy & Malloy, 1998, p. 3 16)

The teachers added that humor helped student willingness to try math problems and

reduced tension.

Search Institute, a non-profit organization located in Minneapolis, MN, has as its

primary goal to determine the factors or assets that help children and young people

develop into healthy, well-adjusted people, for themselves and for their communities.

They have conducted and reviewed studies showing there is a positive correlation

between levels of developmental assets youth have and various measurements of

academic achievement (Scales & Roehlkepartain, 2003).

Developmental assets are defined as "positive factors in young people, families,

communities, schools, and other settings that have been found to be important in

promoting young people's healthy development" (Scales & Roehlkepartain, 2003, p. 2).

Forty developmental assets, 20 considered external and 20 considered internal, have been

articulated by Search Institute. Beyond studies that link higher asset levels to greater

academic competency, analyses of surveys of over 2 15,000 youth across raciallethnic and

socio-economic groups have determined that asset development applies across these

groups both in terms of the reduction of risk behaviors and the increase of thriving

behaviors (Sesma & Roehlkepartain, 2003). Both the reduction of risk behaviors and the

increase of thriving behaviors play roles in academic success.

Educational professionals and the learning environments they promote account

for approximately one-fourth of the external assets and three-fourths of the internal

assets. Post-secondary social work learners in a study by Holley and Steiner (2005)

attributed more responsibility to the instructor for creating safe space than they did to

their peers or themselves. Safe space was defined as "a classroom climate that allows

students to feel secure enough to take risks, honestly express their views, and share and

explore their knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors" (Holley & Steiner, 2005, p. 50). Safe

space is not to be understood as a learning environment without any stress, conflict, or

challenge, but from this researcher's perspective these elements are guided by respect and

coupled with commensurate support to match the challenge at hand. The assets associated

with academia are noted in Appendix A with categories as determined by Search

Institute. The assets have been identified and described in terms of youth ages 12 to 18.

So, where might assets have connection with reducing math anxiety? The

common quality may be in how steeped the assets are in interpersonal and intrapersonal

relationships within the learning environment. With more assets in a learner's personal

arsenal, researchers have theorized that adverse circumstances, including academic

difficulties, are more likely to be countered with resiliency characteristics and appropriate

help-seeking behaviors (Scales & Roehlkepartain, 2003).

Chapter 111: Methodology

Introduction

This chapter outlines subject selection and description, details the nature of the

survey used to acquire data, and provides the data collection procedures, types of data

analysis, and limitations of the methodology.

Selection and Description of Subjects

Adult learners who attend a technical college do so for a variety of reasons and at

different times in their lives. They may be seeking a first career, skill enhancement,

voluntary or involuntary career change, or personal interest. This study's participants

were volunteers from three math courses held at a midwestern technical college. All

learners in attendance at their face-to-face math class were asked to participate in the

survey.

Though not mandatory for the study, course selection considered whether learners

might represent some differences in quantity of prior math preparation at the post-

secondary level. As a result, one course with a prerequisite math course and two courses

without a math prerequisite were surveyed.

Instrumentation

A hardcopy, anonymous survey was used to gather data. Brief demographic

information consisted of six items, mostly fill-in-the-blank. Requested data included the

participant's current math course, program name, gender, age, number of years of high

school math passed, indication of whether high school math was taken through block

scheduling, number of math courses passed since high school graduation or GED

attainment, and how many years it had been since taking a prior math course. These items

were included to allow potential identification of differences or correlations in survey

responses by gender, age, and math background. The block scheduling question was only

included to determine how many participants may have had to interpret how to respond to

the question regarding the number of high school math years. One open-ended question

concluded the demographic background to allow any clarification, if desired.

The remainder of the survey consisted of three sections: one on personal anxiety

with math, the second on interpersonal and intrapersonal coping strategies experienced in

math, and the final section on selected affective qualities wanted in a math learning

environment, with specific interest on those that learners considered helpful to reducing

math tension.

The math anxiety section was modeled after a ten question, Likert math anxiety

subscale taken from the Fennema-Sherman Mathematics Attitudes Scales Test (1 976).

Some subscale ideas were adopted with minor changes for clarity, while others were

modified or replaced to better represent more current literature. One open-ended question

was added to conclude this section, allowing learners to clarify answers or add

information, if desired.

The second section on 10 coping strategies was modeled after a study on math

anxiety and coping strategies among adults from a community college (Peskoff, 2001).

Using Likert-type questions, the model prompted how often a coping strategy was

engaged in and then its effectiveness as perceived by the learner. For strategies not

experienced, the learner could still indicate how effective helshe thought they would be if

they were to be used. The current study employed this format. The six coping strategies

that involved classroom or educational personnel from Peskoff s study were adopted with

minor modifications, and the remaining coping strategies were developed by the

researcher. Questions were designed so that this section focused on math coping

strategies with an interpersonal or intrapersonal nature.

The final section, created by the researcher, allowed bringing in additional

affective and relational qualities that learners may want and find helpful to tension

reduction. The format provided a list of 17 qualities that learners were asked to

checkrnark if they were considered as desirable in their math learning environment. The

final question had learners indicate which of their chosen qualities were also considered

as helpful to tension reduction.

Outside of this study, the survey has not had replicated use; therefore, its

statistical measures of validity and reliability are unknown. The survey was piloted with a

class of 16 graduate students from an area university for intended content validity, clarity,

and approximate time length for completion. Minor revisions were made to wording

based on their feedback. Several changes were made to the demographic background

after the pilot to better capture each learner's self-reported math background.

The survey was presented in booklet form with the consent form on page one,

followed by three survey pages. Participants were offered an identical copy of the consent

form to keep for contact information. A copy of the finalized survey is located in

Appendix A.

Data Collection

The Institutional Review Board of the researcher's university campus approved

this study's proposal and survey. Permission to survey was then sought from a

midwestern technical college and subsequently denied due to a sense of survey burnout

within the institution. Permission to survey was then sought and obtained from an

alternate midwestern technical college prior to survey administration. The surveys were

completed and collected at the start of each class. Variability in environment was limited

to the time of day as the classes met in the same room, on the same day, and were taught

by the same instructor. Survey time ranged from 7 to 15 minutes based on arrival time of

the participant. The researcher administered the surveys and batched them by course

enrollment.

Data Processing and Analysis

The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), Version 12, was utilized

for processing and analyzing the data. The raw data was loaded into the program. After

entry of raw data, a random audit of approximated 25% of the data, as well as data that

looked curious, was conducted. One error was found and corrected.

Anxiety questions had Likert responses of Strongly Agree (SA), Agree (A),

Undecided (U), Disagree (D), and Strongly Disagree (SD). These responses were

assigned values of one to five, respectively, for the positively worded anxiety statements

(e.g. I usually don't worry about my ability to solve math problems). Reverse coding

within SPSS was used on the negatively worded statements (e.g. I get a sinking feeling

when I think of trying hard math problems). Composite anxiety scores could vary from

10 (lowest) to 50 (highest). The low anxiety range was defined as 10 - 24 (the lower 35%

of the scale), the medium range was 25 - 35 (the middle 30%), and the high range was

36 - 50 (the upper 35%).

Frequencies, percents, and cross-tabulations were calculated using SPSS. Measures of

central tendency were calculated manually. Based on sample sizes, no correlation data

were calculated, with the exception of one Spearman rho. The results can be found in

Chapter IV.

Limitations

A variety of literature indicates there is an inverse relationship between math

background and level of anxiety (Ulrich, 1989; Betz, 1978). As a result, the survey

participants were asked to indicate the length of time since their prior math course, how

many years of high school math they passed, and how many courses of math were passed

since high school graduation or GED attainment. The intention was to have some means

for gauging the recentness and quantity of math background. The survey did not request

the specific coursework taken, or how successful learners were in those courses beyond

specifying the courses were passed.

Continuing with math background, the survey did not request the quantity or

depth that life experiences with math may have played for some participants. For

example, a non-traditional learner who was changing careers may have had significant

math experience in some content areas through handling the family finances or through

work responsibilities. This type of data, while meaningful to one's background, did not

seem logistically feasible to collect through a short survey. Data of this nature would be

better collected through interviews or case studies. As a result, no attempt was made to

collect and compare the value of those life experiences, so the depth and recentness of

math background was confined to academic coursework.

As mentioned in the Subjects section of this chapter, adults attend technical

colleges for a variety of reasons. The motivations or rationales for learners pursuing their

particular programs which resulted in their math course requirements were not

considered. The degrees to which these elements play a role in defining participant

anxiety and their perceptions of helpful factors were not within the scope of this study.

Chapter IV: Results

Introduction

This chapter consists of a brief overview of how participants were acquired, the

demographics describing them, the anxiety levels and comments of participants, their use

and perceptions of helpfulness on 10 coping strategies, and their interest and perceptions

of helpfulness on 17 affective learning environment qualities. Research Questions #2, #3,

and #4 will be addressed as the respective data are presented. Due to Research Question

#1 being covered by the literature review, its discussion has been included in Chapter V.

A survey was offered to five sections of three math courses held by a midwestern

technical college. A total of 61 learners were registered for these sections and 48 were in

attendance on the day of surveying. The survey was administered to determine the

learners' math anxiety levels and their perceptions of strategies and qualities that may

have been part of their past learning experiences in math. Of the 48 learners, 47 chose to

participate by taking the survey, a return rate of 97.9%. These learners made up 77.0% of

the registered learners in these face-to-face classes.

Demographics

There were 26 male, 20 female, and 1 unrecorded participants in this study. They

were pursuing a variety of programs, with over half seeking Criminal Justice, Mechanical

Design, or Architectural Commercial Design. As program requirements, they were either

enrolled in Trigonometry, Introduction to College Mathematics, or Business Math

(38.3%, 3 1.9%, and 29.8%, respectively, n=47).

Thirty-four participants were traditional learners (ages 18-25 years, 73.9%) and

1 1 participants were non-traditional learners (ages 26+, 23.9%). One 17 year old learner,

unbeknownst to the researcher, also participated (2.2%). Any subsequent reference to

traditional and non-traditional learners will group this participant with traditional

learners.

Math background was collected through three variables. The amount of high

school math passed ranged from 0 to 5 years. The data showed 20.5% had 0 to 2.5 years

and 79.5% had 3 or more years (n=44). Seventeen participants (37.0%, n=44) indicated

they had some high school math through block scheduling. Eight of these indicated they

passed 4 years and the one learner who indicated 5 years did also. Seven of the remaining

eight learners showed 2 or 3 years, and the remaining learner indicated 0 years passed.

The second background variable indicated the quantity of post-secondary math

coursework passed, ranging from 0 to 4 courses. The distribution was 28.3% with no

course, 30.4% with one course, 21.7% with two courses, 15.2% with three courses, and

4.3% with four courses (n=46).

The final background variable was the time elapsed since a prior math course and

this ranged from 0 to 28 years. The majority of these learners (64.4%) had a one year

break or less (n=45). Seven learners (15.6%) had a 2 to 4 year break, five learners

(1 1.1%) had a 5 to 19 year break, and four learners (8.9%) had a break greater than 20

years. Table 2 collectively shows the three math background variable frequencies and

their measures of central tendency, categorized by anxiety levels.

Table 2 Math Background Variables by Anxiety Level

High School Math Anxiety Level

Passed (yrs) Low Medium High

Mean 3.6 3.2 2.2 Median 4 3 2 Mode 4 3 2,4

n=23 n=15 n=6

Post-Secondary Math

Courses Passed (#)

Mean Median Mode

Time Since Previous

Math Course (yrs)

Mean Median Mode

Anxiety Composite Score

Learner anxiety scores ranged from 10 - 50, matching the theoretical range. The

low, medium, and high anxiety percentages for male participants were 61.5, 30.8, and

7.7, respectively. For female participants, the respective percentages were 35.0,40.0, and

25.0. Since female participants in this sample had higher anxiety by category than their

male counterparts, the non-parametric Spearman rho correlation was calculated between

gender and the three variables of math background to see if any statistical significance

existed between gender and background. Mild statistical significance was found at the

0.029 level for gender as related to time length since a prior math course (rho=0.325), and

not with the other two variables.

Composite scores by course yielded low, medium, and high anxiety percentages

for Trigonometry learners at 61.1, 33.3, and 5.6, Introduction to College Mathematics

learners at 53.3,33.3, and 13.3, and Business Math learners at 5 1.1, 34.0, and 14.9,

respectively.

The open-ended question which concluded this survey section allowed

clarification or comments by participants about their comfort level. Eight learners offered

comments that concurred with their anxiety level. For example, two highly anxious

learners (composite scores of 49 each) wrote, "I don't understand math, it is hard for

me", and "I strongly hate math!!" Two low anxiety learner wrote, "I like m a t h

(composite score of 23), and "I enjoy math! It's the instructors that make people

understand it which makes them (people) feel at ease or ill with math" (composite score

of 14). Additional comments collected indicated some math is easier than others, a person

can enjoy one type of math but not another, math homework can go well while tests do

not, and that Core Math in high school did not adequately prepare that learner for college

level math.

Coping Strategies

The coping strategy data had a response range of 1 (not at all) to 5 (a lot). Of the

middle options, only 3 was labeled (somewhat), leaving 2 and 4 as additional relative

options. The data, organized by anxiety level, have been presented in Tables 3,4, and 5.

Usage and helpfulness per strategy have been combined on one table for each anxiety

level. Usage results will be reviewed first for all anxiety levels and then helpfulness will

be addressed in response to Research Question #2.

For low anxiety participants (n=23, Table 3), five of the strategies were used

somewhat or more by 70 - 80% of the participants. These were working with a group in

class, working with a partner in class, asking the instructor questions in class, discussing

experiences or difficulties related to math with other students from class, and reminding

oneself of being mentally capable when starting to feel incompetent. The sixth item used

most was studying with a partner outside of class (50.0%).

Strategies used least were tutoring (83.3% never), speaking with a counselor

about math experiences or difficulties (79.2% never), phoning or emailing the instructor

to discuss material not understood (70.8% never), and meeting the instructor for help

(62.5% never). These learners, however, had a favorable perception of tutoring, with

75.0% thinking it could be somewhat or more helpful.

The medium anxiety participants (n=16, Table 4), showed higher relative usage

for its top five strategies (81.4 - 93.8% at somewhat or more). These were working in a

group in class, discussing experiences or difficulties related to math with other students

from class, asking the instructor questions in class, having a partner in class, and having a

partner outside of class. The sixth strategy was reminding oneself of being mentally

capable (62.5%).

Table 3 Strategy Usage and Helpfulness - Low Anxiety Participants (n=24)

Usage (%) Helpfulness (%)

I I I I

I I I I I 1 Class

83.3 4.2

79.2 8.3 12.5 0 0

8.3

Counselor 33.3 8.3

Partner Outside 12.5 0

8.3 16.7

25.0 4.2

12.5 8.3 37.5 33.3 8.3 Group InClass 4.2 8.3 25.0 37.5 20.8

62.5 12.5

70.8 12.5

12.5 12.5

The strategies used least (never or less than somewhat) by those with medium

anxiety were phoning or emailing the instructor (8 1.3%), speaking with a counselor about

math experiences or difficulties (68.8%), meeting the instructor (68.8%), and tutoring

(56.3%).

0 4.2 1 Tutor

54.2

47.1

45.8

33.3

4.2 0

33.3 12.5

12.5

16.7

37.5

20.8 4.2

20.8 8.3

25.0 12.5

37.5 1 20.8 16.7

8.3 4.2

0 0

20.8 16.7

Informal

Discussion

Mentally Capable

in Class

MeetingInstructor

Phone~Email

Instructor

Partner In Class

8.3 16.7

20.8 0

25.0 12.7

50.0 20.8

8.3 8.3

50.0

29.2

16.7 8.3

20.8 29.2

37.5

20.8

29.2

12.5 12.5

4.2 4.2

33.3 20.8

Table 4 Strategy Usage and Helpfulness - Medium Anxiety Participants (n=16)

Usage (%) Helpfulness (%)

Tutor 25.0 6.3 18.8 18.8 31.3

Counselor 25.0 25.0 31.3 12.5 6.3

Partner Outside 0 0 50.0 18.8 31.3

Class

Informal

Discussion

Mentally Capable

AskingQuestions

in Class

High anxiety participants (n=7, Table 5) indicated their most used strategies as a

partner outside of class (loo%), discussing experiences or difficulties related to math

with other students from class and asking questions of the instructor in class (both

85.8%), and working with a partner in class, working with a group in class, and

reminding oneself of being mentally capable (each 71.4%).

Meeting Instructor

PhoneIEmail

Instructor

Partner In Class

Group In Class

0 12.5

0 25.0

18.8 31.3

18.8 37.5

43.8 25.0

0 0

0 0

43.8

56.3

18.8

37.5 6.3

6.3 12.5

18.8 12.5

31.2

25.0

31.2

25.0

0 12.5

0 6.3

43.8 25.0

37.5 37.5

Table 5 Strategy Usage and Helpfulness - High Anxiety Participants (n=7)

Usage (%)

0 0 1 57.1 0 42.9 Partner Outside I O 0 I 57.1 14.3 28.6

Helpfulness (%)

d

-a C, cd C, z + c'4

57.1 0

42.9 28.6

C,

2 3

C,

0 d

4 m d-

I I 1 Experiences 1 I I

' f l

Coping Strategy

14.3

28.6

0 14.3

d - cd

C,

C,

2 3 2 0

I I I in Class I I I

c.)

0 4

C1 "?

c'4 l m 0 28.6

0 0

42.9

d- P: 'f

28.6 0 57.1

14.3 O I 14.3

Strategies used least by the high anxiety group (at levels below somewhat)

included phoning or emailing the instructor (81.3%), speaking with a counselor and

meeting with the instructor (both 68.8%), and tutoring (56.3%).

Table 6 provides a comparison among the three anxiety groups to highlight which

group used each strategy the most by percent of participants. The medium anxiety group

used 8 of 10 strategies more often than other learners.

Tutor

Counselor

0 42.9

14.3 42.9

28.6 14.3

14.3 0 ~ Mentallycapable

42.9 28.6 AskingQuestions

42.9 28.6

57.1 28.6

28.6 0

14.3 14.3

14.3 42.9

42.9 28.6

Class

Discussing

28.6

42.9

28.6

14.3

14.3

28.6

14.3 0

14.3 0

14.3

14.3

14.3 14.3

I

0 0

0 0

28.6 28.6

14.3 28.6

0 28.6

14.3 0

42.9

Meeting Instructor

PhoneIEmail

Instructor

Partner In Class

Group In Class

0 28.6

42.9 28.6

57.1 28.6

14.3 0

0 14.3

14.3

14.3

28.6

28.6

0 14.3

0 0

14.3 42.9

28.6 28.6

Table 6 Coping Strategy Usage Comparison - Current Study

Coping Strategy Utilized Most By Anxiety Level

Low Medium High

Use Tutoring Services X

Discuss Difficulties with Counselor X

Work with Partner Outside of Class X

Discuss Experiences or Difficulties with Other X

Learners from Class

Remind Yourself You Are Mentally Capable X

Ask Questions in Class X

Meet Instructor on Material Not Understood X

PhoneIEmail Instructor on Material Not Understood X

Work with Partner in Class X

Work in a Group in Class X

Research Question #2 asked, "As customers of math education, what do learners

of a midwestern technical college perceive as helpful coping strategies for their math

learning environment that involve personal relationships?' The second section of the

survey prompted these results through asking for the relative helpfulness of each strategy

by answering, "how helpful has it been OR how helpful do you think it would be if you

tried it?'Responses of somewhat or more were combined for the percentage results that

follow.

Low math anxiety participants indicated the most helpful strategies were working

with a group in class and a partner outside of class (both 87.5%), having a partner in class

(83.3%), reminding oneself of being mentally capable (79.2%), tutoring and asking

questions in class (both 75.0%).

Medium anxiety participants perceived it most helpful to have a partner in class,

work with a group in class, and have a partner outside of class (each loo%), discuss

experiences or difficulties related to math with other students from class (87.5%), and

work with a tutor (68.9%). Half of these participants indicated asking questions in class

as helpful.

High anxiety participants identified the most helpful strategies as a partner outside

of class (loo%), a partner in class, working with a group in class (both 85.7%), and

discussing experiences or difficulties related to math with other students from class

Table 7 shows the relative rankings for helpfulness of the coping strategies. When

summative percents for responses of somewhat helpful to a lot were the same, the

distribution of responses from somewhat to a lot generally differentiated the rankings.

Table 7 Coping Strategy Helpfulness Rankings

Coping Strategy Anxiety Level

Low Medium High

Use Tutoring Services

Discuss Difficulties with Counselor

Work with Partner Outside of Class

Discuss Experiences or Difficulties with Other

Learners from Class 7 4 4

Remind Yourself You Are Mentally Capable 4 5 7

Ask Questions in Class 6 7 5

Meet Instructor on Material Not Understood 8 9 9

PhoneIEmail Instructor on Material Not Understood 10 10 10

Work with Partner in Class 3 2 2 tie

Work in a Group in Class 1 1 2 tie

Learning Environment/Relational Qualities

Research Question #3 asked, "Beyond coping strategies, what support or

relational learning qualities do these learners associate with tension reduction in math?"

The third section of the survey provided 17 affective possibilities of which they could

select as many as were applicable to those they wanted in their math environment and

then those they considered as helpful to reducing worry about math. Since they could

want certain qualities for reasons not associated with tension reduction, both questions

were asked to improve the clarity of results, presented in Table 8.

Table 8 lists the percents by anxiety level of those who considered the quality

wanted plus helpful to reducing anxiety (Helpful) and the percents that combined

responses of wanted alone with those marked wanted plus helpful (Total). The last

column provides an overall percent of the interest of participants in having that quality

present in their math learning environment (i.e. minimally they want it present).

One low anxiety participant did not respond to the qualities section at all and three

medium anxiety participants indicated the qualities they wanted, while not indicating any

as helpful to reducing worry. By not indicating any as helpful, it is unknown whether this

represented their perceptions or whether they did not see or answer this last survey

question. As a result, some helpful qualities identified by medium anxiety participants

could be underrepresented by 0, 6.2, 12.5 or 18.8 percent, though their overall total

percents, which combine wanted with helpful, would not be impacted. Data in Tables 8

and 9, for medium anxiety participants only, are potentially affected.

ÿ able 8 Interest in Classroom Qualities (%)

Sample

Total

Interest

Classroom

Quality

Work with partner

Work in groups

Work alone

Work with instructor

Instructor responds

to class needs

A sense of hope is in

the classroom 8.7 43.5 18.8 62.6 , 0.0 57.1

Anxiety Level

Low (n=23) Medium (n= 16) High (n=7)

Organized

competition

Internal competition

In class discussion

Electronic

discussion

Someone in class

understands me

8.7 39.1

8.7 34.8

13.0 50.8

0.0 4.3

8.7 39.1

Freedom to approach

problems differently

Friendships in class 1 39.1 60.8 12.5 50.0 14.3 85.7 1 60.9

Helpful Total

57.1 100

42.9 85.8

0.0 14.3

28.6 57.2

57.1 100

Helpful Total

34.8 69.6

34.8 60.9

26.1 69.6

21.7 30.4

39.1 86.9

13.0 52.1

A spirit of "we're in

this together"

Group members are I I I

Helpful Total

31.3 81.3

43.8 62.6

6.3 37.6

18.8 43.8

37.5 81.3

0.0 30.4

about one another 34.7

Appropriate humor 73.9

held accountable

Classmates care

0.0 34.8 6.3 62.6 0.0 28.6 43.5

Eight qualities were identified as the top five rankings for helpfulness (see Table

9). Participants identified an instructor who responds to class need as the number one

ranked factor, regardless of anxiety level. Friendships in class and appropriate humor also

tied for first among low anxiety learners while working with a partner tied for first among

high anxiety learners. Other top qualities across two or three anxiety levels were working

with a partner, working in groups, in class discussion, and appropriate humor.

Two learners (both low anxiety) thought internal competition was helpful to

tension reduction, while 41 learners did not. Three learners (2 low, 1 medium) thought

organized competition was helpful, while 40 did not. Electronic discussion was wanted

by 1 learner but not marked as helpful, while 42 learners did not want it nor mark it as

helpful. Anecdotally, all learners have campus email accounts, and their math instructor

indicated that approximately 70% have internet access at home.

Table 9 Top Ranked Qualities to Reduce Tension

Quality Helpfulness by Anxiety Level

Low Medium High

Instructor responds to class needs 1 tie 1 1 tie

Work with partner 4 tie 3 1 tie

Work in groups 4 tie 2 3

In class discussion - 4 tie 4 tie

A spirit of "we're in this together" - - 4 tie

Classmates care about one another - - 4 tie

Friendships in class 1 tie - - Appropriate humor 1 tie 4 tie -

Note. Items marked with a hyphen can be viewed as percentages in Table 8 with all

qualities.

Chapter V: Discussion, Conclusions, and Recommendations

Introduction

This chapter includes a recap of the rationale for the study, a discussion of

Research Question #1, highlights of survey findings, conclusions drawn, and general

recommendations for instructors and further research.

A comprehensive look at math anxiety could entail evaluating study skills,

identifying gaps in knowledge, gleaning the relevance a learner attributes to math,

identifying the classroom goal structure promoted or perceived, finding triggers of the

past or present that block progress, assessing the influences of learning climate, etc. A

range of approaches seems appropriate to maximize improvements.

Tobias' math clinics and personal self-help book emphasized emotional

processing with content instruction (1991, 1993). Kitchens' work did, too (1 995). The

efforts of those who recognize and work on the affective realm of math anxiety do not do

so instead of cognitive content work, but as an integral part. This study took the angle

that an affective lens has a strong presence across math anxiety issues, and that learner

perceptions are a powerful source for determining the affective elements that impact their

own anxiety levels.

Discussion

This study collected anxiety data from 47 math learners from a midwestern

technical college and used it for comparison to classroom climate factors. The goal was

to determine learner perceptions of what is helpful to mitigating their math anxiety.

While the data came from one instructor's classes, the survey was about the body of their

math experiences and not about their current instructor or course.

Research Question #1 asked, "Are there any approaches to understanding or

conceptualizing math anxiety that might freshen conversation on this topic?" To this end,

the literature review offered brain fhction and resiliency as windows for looking at how

each contribute to the affective resources available as learners cope with academic stress,

of which math anxiety is certainly one.

The brain is a complex organ. It has as its primary function the survival of the

organism. While human learning is a dynamic process involving the brain beyond the

context of this study, the fundamental nature of the brain's use of emotion and priority

assignment to ensure survival, decreasing or shutting down other cognitive processing if

necessary, validates the importance of the affective learning environment. Helpful to all

learners, math anxiety learners especially need positive affective experiences with math

and to have supportive opportunities to rethink some of their negative experiences. The

literature indicated several strategies such as discussing math difficulties with other

learners to know one is not alone, using written reflections to sort what is being learned

and to safely unload emotional math baggage, replacing one's negative self-talk with

more positive and realistic messages, and working together on challenging problems. In

conversation with the instructor of the learners in this study, the instructor shared that

more vocal learners who expressed their frustration and anxiety about math seemed to do

better academically than those suspected of keeping their frustrations inside.

Resiliency-building, as suggested by Search Institute and others, is the platform

from which adolescents develop into healthy and productive adults. The extent to which

developmental assets are present and working well for learners when they arrive at post-

secondary institutions becomes their operational base for layering additional learning

experiences. In this study, 76.1% of technical college participants were traditional

learners. It does not seem like a stretch to this researcher that they are continuing their

asset development. Infusing resiliency-building into math education at the high

school level was advocated by Malloy and Malloy (1 998). Is post-secondary math

education a place to extend resiliency-building to reduce adult math anxiety? What about

learners (ages 17-60+) who do not have asset rich backgrounds? Learners facing serious

deficits may need services outside the math classroom. The idea is whether a resiliency or

asset development perspective has a place within it. This study presented work showing

resiliency efforts overlap with relational and affective learning environment factors. For

some educators or school programs, considering resiliency for assisting those with math

anxiety does not need to compete with what is already being done to foster a good

learning environment, rather it offers another language or slant from which to think about

it.

Conclusions

In this study, learners rated multiple peer-connected coping strategies as the most

used and the most helpful among the relationship strategies studied. When differentiated

by anxiety level, the peer-oriented strategies remained in the top half. These included

working in groups in class, working with a partner in or out of class, and discussing

experiences and problems about math with their classmates. Low anxiety learners were

more apt to think asking questions of the instructor in class yielded favorable results than

their classmates. In the Peskoff study of community college math learners (2001), the

coping strategies were not all relational. Its top three strategies were completing one's

homework on time, letting the instructor know when material was not understood, and

allowing extra time to study for tests. Communication with the instructor was its highest

relational coping strategy.

Medium anxiety learners appeared to use coping strategies more than either the

low or high anxiety learners in this study. In the community college study (Peskoff,

2001), learners were grouped only into low and high anxiety categories. There the low

anxiety group was found to use a wider range and more strategies than the high anxiety

group. The idea of higher academic self-efficacy among the low anxiety group was

thought to explain the difference. In the present study, it may be that the medium anxiety

group used more strategies than the low anxiety group out of having a greater need for

them, and they used more strategies than the high anxiety group out of a greater sense of

self-efficacy. While the Peskoff study rated letting the instructor know if material wasn't

being understood as one of the best coping strategies, the current study showed asking

questions in class and meeting the instructor for help as middle to low ranked items, and

more so for high anxiety learners. The sample size, however, was particularly small for

the high anxiety group. The lowest ranking strategies in Peskoff s group were tutoring

and counselor discussion, and these strategies were also low ranked items for usage and

helpfulness here.

From the perspective of the researcher, an affective approach to countering math

anxiety may have three points of attack: better understanding of what the brain is or is not

doing during anxiety so that the role of emotion during learning is not undervalued with

adults; drawing learners toward realizing that the presence of anxiety is not a sign of

lacking the intellect necessary for math competence; and promoting a learning

environment that supports more learners, keeping particularly attuned to learners with

anxiety. Strategies and classroom qualities promoted would seek ways to positively

impact all learners, yet any differences between those factors with respect to anxiety

levels would be one legitimate way to prioritize efforts.

Recommendations

Study Replication

The relatively low quantity of participants in this study means there is limited

generalizability of findings beyond the institution's face-to-face math classroom

population. No ethniclrace data were collected on the survey, though the sample was

observed by the researcher as considerably European-American. A larger sample is

recommended so statistically valid correlations between anxiety levels and respective

perceptions could be calculated for the population of interest.

The use of three variables to determine a learner's math background was initiated

as a theoretical improvement over the more common solo variable of years of high school

math. It turned out that there was a statistically significant relationship (rho=0.325;

p=0.029) between anxiety by gender and years since a prior math course for this sample.

Replication with more participants at each anxiety level would better confirm whether

multi-variables for math background are valuable. The use of the block scheduling

question toward common interpretation of the number of high school years of math a

learner had is not clear from the data obtained.

Instructors

As practitioners committed to having math learners master competencies for

success in their respective careers, the literature points to the importance of positive

affective qualities within the learning environment. Regarding math anxiety, this includes

making opportunities for learners to express their apprehensions about math and to

experience positive feedback, given emotion's central role in learning and how negative

emotion can shut down cognitive processing. This can look differently for different

practitioners and be guided by the learners. Options could include:

Invite the learners' input prior to the end of class evaluation. Individually

designed surveys, journaling, group discussion, or a learning environment

inventory can offer insights. Educators often have a more positive view of their

classroom environments than do their learners (source unknown). Combine past

experiences with the perspectives of current learners.

Ask a colleague to observe several classroom sessions for feedback specifically

on affective qualities of interest, displayed by learners and the educator.

Initiate data collection on why learners underutilize certain strategies like tutoring

or meeting the instructor outside of class. This can be but does not have to be

formal. Are certain inhibitory logistics or perceptions able to be modified?

Further Research

An affective lens across components of math anxiety could be looked at from the

instructor's viewpoint. What is the prevalence and perception by faculty using

competency-based math instruction on the inclusion of affective learning climate factors?

Do some forms of promoting a healthy learning climate compete with other forms?

Which factors yield strongest results given the amount of time a learner spends in

technical college math courses? To what degree does faculty report getting emotional and

inservice support for including social and support factors for learners?

Resiliency within learners has focused on those 12 to 18 years of age. To what

extent are the assets, or the relationships that foster the assets, relevant to math anxiety

reduction in adults? How different would a list of assets for post-secondary learners look

or does it change significantly over the span of an adult learner's lifetime? Social learning

theory and cooperative learning would be two bodies of work, some dealing with adult

learners, to show overlap with the youth assets. A broader literature search and more

research could help address these questions. As post-secondary institutions serve growing

numbers of non-traditional learners, determining the longevity and importance of

developmental assets for non-traditional learners will increase.

References

Archarnbeault, B. (1993, September-October). Holistic mathematics instruction.

Interactive problem solving and real life situations help learners understand math

concepts. Adult Learning, 5(1), 2 1-23.

Arem, C. S. (2003). Conquering math anxiety. A self-help workbook (Second Edition).

Pacific Grove, CA: BrooksICole Publishing Co.

Baloglu, M. (1 999). A comparison of mathematics anxiety and statistics anxiety in

relation to general anxiety. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 436

703)

Bankhead, M. (2002). Reducing math anxiety, improving standards, and maximizing

student participation and student interaction using special techniques and peer

responsibility. A practical solution for the classroom. (ERIC Document

Reproduction Service No. ED 474 799)

Betz, N. E. (1 978). Prevalence, distribution, and correlates of math anxiety in college

students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 25(5), 44 1-448.

Dirkx, J. M. (2001). The power of feelings: Emotion, imagination, and the construction

of meaning in adult learning. In S. B. Merriam (Ed.), The new update on adult

learning theory (pp. 63-8 1 ) . San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. A. (1 976). Fennema-Sherman mathematics attitudes test.

Tests in Microfiche, Set D, p. 15.

Godbey, C. (1 997). Mathematics anxiety and the underprepared student. (ERIC

Document Reproduction Service No. ED 426 734)

Holley, L. C., & Steiner, S. (2005, Winter). Safe space: Student perspectives on

classroom environment. Journal of Social Work Education, 41(1), 49-64.

Jensen, E. (1 998). Teaching with the brain in mind. Alexandria, VA:

Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Kitchens, A. N. (1 995). Defeating math anxiety. Chicago, IL: Richard D. Irwin, Inc.

Malloy, C. E., & Malloy, W. W. (1998, MayIJune). Resiliency and Algebra I: A

promising non-traditional approach to teaching low-achieving students. Clearing

House, 71(5), 3 14-3 17. Retrieved November 2,2005, from: EbscoHost database.

Merriam- Webster S collegiate dictionary. (loth ed.; 2000). Springfield, MA: Merriam-

Webster, Inc.

The New Oxford American dictionary. (2001). Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.

Patrick, H., Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., & Midgley, C. (2003). How teachers establish

psychological environments during the first days of school: Associations with

avoidance in mathematics. Teachers College Record, 105(8), 152 1 - 1558.

Perry, A. B. (2004, June). Decreasing math anxiety in college students [Electronic

version]. College Student Journal, 38(2), 32 1-324. Retrieved November 1 1,2005,

from: Wilson Web.

Peskoff, F. (2001). Mathematics anxiety and the adult student: An analysis of successful

coping strategies. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 474 042)

Ryan, A. M., Gheen, M. H., & Midgley, C. (1998). Why do some students avoid asking

for help? An examination of the interplay among students' academic efficacy,

teachers' social-emotional role, and the classroom goal structure. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 90(3), 528-53 5.

Ryan, A. M., Patrick, H., & Shim, S. (2005). Differential profiles of students identified

by their teacher as having avoidant, appropriate, or dependent help-seeking

tendencies in the classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(2), 275-285.

Safford-Ramus, K. (2001). A review and summary of research on adult mathematics

education in North America (1980-2000). (ERIC Document Reproduction Service

No. ED 478 899)

Salinas, T. M. (2004, December). Effects of reflective notebooks on perceptions of

learning and mathematics anxiety [Electronic version]. PRIMUS, 14(4), 3 15-327.

Scales, P. C., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2003). Boosting student achievement: New

research on the power of developmental assets. Search Institute Insights &

Evidence, 1(1), 1 - 10. Retrieved December 1,2005, from:

http://www.search-institute.org/research/insights

Sesma, A., Jr., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2003). Unique strengths, shared strengths:

Developmental assets among youth of color. Search Institute Insights &

Evidence, 1(2), 1- 13. Retrieved December 6,2005, from:

http://www.search-institute.org/research/insights

Sousa, D. A. (2006). How the brain learns (Third Edition). Thousand Oaks, CA: Convin

Press.

Spandel, V. (2005). Creating writers through 6-traint writing assessment and instruction

(Fourth Edition). Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc.

Sprenger, M. (1999). Learning & memory. The brain in action. Alexandria, VA:

Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Taylor, B. A., & Fraser, B. J. (2003). The influence of classroom environment on high

school students ' mathematics anxiety. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service

No. ED 476 644)

Tileston, D. W. (2000). 10 Best teachingpractices. How research, learning styles, and

standards define teaching competencies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Convin Press, Inc.

Tobias, S. (1 993). Overcoming math anxiety. Revised and expanded. New York, NY:

W. W. Norton Company, Inc.

Tobias, S. (1991, Summer). Math mental health: Going beyond math anxiety. College

Teaching, 39(3), 91-93.

Ulrich, M. B. (1 989). A study of adult participation in mathematics courses as a function

of mathematics anxiety and other variables. Retrieved November 15, 2005, from:

http://wwwlib.urni.corn/cr/ilstdfi~llcit?p8907679

Appendix A: Developmental Assets Related to Academia

Category Asset and Description

Support Adult relationships - person receives support from three or more non-parent adults

Caring school climate - school provides a caring, encouraging environment

Empowerment Safety - person feels safe at home, school, and in the neighborhood

Boundaries & School boundaries - school provides clear rules and consequences Expectations Adult role models - adults model positive, responsible behavior

Positive peer influence - person's best friends model responsible behavior

High expectations - both parent(s) and teachers encourage the person to do well

Constructive Programs - person spends three or more hours per week in sports, Use of Time clubs, or organizations at school andlor in the community

Commitment to Achievement motivation - person is motivated to do well in school Learning School engagement - person is actively engaged in learning

Homework - person reports doing at least one hour of homework every school day

Bonding to school - person cares about herhis school

Positive Values Caring - person places high value on helping other people Integrity - person acts on convictions and stands up for own beliefs Honesty -person "tells the truth even when it is not easy" Responsibility - person accepts and takes personal responsibility

Social Planning and decision making - person knows how to plan ahead Competencies and make choices

Interpersonal competence - person has empathy, sensitivity, and friendship skills

Cultural competence - person has knowledge of and comfort with people of different cultural/racial/ethnic backgrounds

Resistance skills - person can resist negative peer pressure and dangerous situations

Peaceful conflict resolution - person seeks to resolve conflict nonviolently

Positive Identity Personal power - person feels control over "things that happen to me"

Self-esteem - person reports having a high self-esteem Sense of purpose - person reports that "my life has a purpose" Positive view of personal future - person is optimistic about herhis

personal future

Appendix B 5 1 - --

This research has been approved by the UW-Stout IRB as required by the Code of Federal Regulations Title 45 Part 46.

1

Consent to Participate in UW-Stout Approved Research

Title: Determining the Comfort Levels and Perceptions of Math Learners on Selected Coping Strategies in Mathematics

Investigator: Cheryl M. Sutter 736 Bolles St. Eau Claire, WI 54703

Research Sponsor: Dr. Amy Gillett, UW-Stout [email protected] (7 15) 232-2680

Description: The purpose of the study is to collect data from adult math learners to compare their math comfort levels with the prevalence/effectiveness of some social factors related to math. Participants will be currently enrolled in a math course at a Midwestern technical college. The data is collected through a three-section survey.

Risks and Benefits: There is no perceived risk for the participant or the technical college. As potential benefit, a participant may become aware of another avenue for math support or encouragement from completion of the survey. The data a participant supplies will help inform the investigator and broader audience who have an interest in math anxiety among learners.

Time Commitment: Approximately 10 minutes

Confidentiality: Your name is not collected on this survey document and no participant names will be seen by the investigator at any point in this study. All information collected will remain anonymous.

Right to Withdraw: Your participation in this study is entirely voluntary. You may choose not to participate without any adverse consequences to you. If you choose to participate and later wish to withdraw from the study, there is no way to identify your anonymous document after it has been turned in to the investigator.

IRB Approval: This study has been reviewed and approved by The University of Wisconsin-Stout's Institutional Review Board (IRB). The IRB has determined that this study meets the ethical obligations required by federal law and university policies. If you have questions or concerns regarding this study please contact the Investigator or Advisor. If you have any questions, concerns, or reports regarding your rights as a research subject, please contact the IRB Administrator.

Investigator: Cheryl M. Sutter (7 15) 836-7903 sutterc@,uwstout .edu

Advisor: Dr. Amy Gillett (7 15) 232-2680 pilletta@,uwstout.edu

IRB Administrator Sue Foxwell, Director, Research Services 152 Vocational Rehabilitation Bldg. UW-Stout Menomonie, W 5475 1 (7 15) 232-2477 foxwells~uwstout.edu

Statement of Consent: By completing the following survey found in this booklet, you agree to participate in the study entitled, The Anxiety Levels and Perceptions of Mathematics Learnersfiorn a Midwestern Technical College on Selected Classroom Climate Factors in Mitigating the Eflects of Math Anxiety.

Page 1 of 4

Demographic Information Name of Current Math Course: Your Program Name: Gender (please circle): Male Female Age:

How many years of high school math did you pass? Was any of your high school math taken through block scheduling? (please circle) Yes No How many math courses have you passed since earning your high school or GED diploma? How many years has it been since your last math class? Do you wish to clarify any of your answers?

Survey on Comfort Level with Math

The following ten items are statements that may represent your feelings about math. Please complete all items and clearly circle the answer that best represents your feelings.

SA = Strongly Agree A = Agree U = Undecided D = Disagree SD = Strongly Disagree

1. SA A U D SD I wouldn't mind taking more math courses as long as it didn't require

more time and money.

2. SA A U D SD I get a sinking feeling when I think of trying hard math problems.

3. SA A U D SD My emotions about math generally hurt my performance on a math test.

4. SA A U D SD I usually don't worry about my ability to solve math problems.

5. SA A U D SD I seldom get concerned when something math-related comes up in

real life.

6. SA A U D SD My mind goes blank and I am unable to think clearly when doing math.

7. SA A U D SD I have usually been at ease during math tests.

8. SA A U D SD I have usually been at ease during math courses.

9. SA A U D SD Math makes me feel uncomfortable and nervous.

10. SA A U D SD Math can make me feel physically ill.

Do you wish to clarify any of your answers or share anything else concerning your comfort level

with math?

Please continue on next page. ?,

Page 2 of 4

Survey on Selected Relational Behaviors used as Coping Strategies for Math

The following is a partial list of strategies that students may use in order to learn mathematics effectively and do well in their math courses. Please respond to !&I questions listed below each of the following behaviors. Clearly circle the most appropriate number for you from 1 to 5 where:

1 = not at all 3 = somewhat 5 = a lot

Please respond to all questions in relation to your math experiences.

1. Using the school's tutoring center or a private tutor a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

2. Discussing with the school counselor my math course experiences or difficulties a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

3. Meeting a study partner outside of class to work on homework or prepare for quizzes/tests a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

4. Discussing experiences or difficulties related to your math course with other students from class

a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

5. Reminding yourself that you are mentally capable even when you start to feel incompetent a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

6. Asking your instructor math questions in class a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

Please continue on next page. -+

Page 3 of 4

1 = not at all 3 = somewhat 5 = a lot

7. Meeting your instructor in person for help on material you don't understand a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

8. Conversing by phone or email with your instructor for help on material you don't understand a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

9. Working with a partner during class to review, clarify, solve problems, or encourage one another

a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

10. Working with a group of students during class on activities that review, clarify, solve problems, or encourage one another

a. How often have you tried this? 1 2 3 4 5 b. How helpful has it been OR how helpful

do you think it would be if you tried it? 1 2 3 4 5

Survey on Selected Relational Qualities in Math Education

Of the following possibilities, which ones do you want in your math learning environment? 1. Check all that apply.

working with a partner working in groups working alone working with the instructor an instructor who responds to class needs a sense of hope in the classroom organized competition (e.g. quiz bowl, review games) internal competition (ways to compare my performance to that of my classmates)

in-class discussion electronic discussion someone in class who understands me being free to approach problems differently a spirit of "we're in this together" friendships in class all group members being held accountable for group work classmates caring about one another appropriate humor

2. Of the items you checked, clearly circle those that help you reduce any tension or worry about math.

Thank you for your time andparticipation. Again, all responses will remain anonymous.

Page 4 of 4