animal behaviours, post-human lives: everyday negotiations of the animal–human divide in...

14
This article was downloaded by: [University of Sydney] On: 14 March 2013, At: 01:33 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Social & Cultural Geography Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rscg20 Animal behaviours, post-human lives: everyday negotiations of the animal–human divide in pet-keeping Rebekah Fox a a Royal Holloway, University of London, Department of Geography, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX, UK E-mail: Version of record first published: 20 Nov 2006. To cite this article: Rebekah Fox (2006): Animal behaviours, post-human lives: everyday negotiations of the animal–human divide in pet-keeping, Social & Cultural Geography, 7:4, 525-537 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14649360600825679 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Upload: rebekah

Post on 03-Dec-2016

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

This article was downloaded by: [University of Sydney]On: 14 March 2013, At: 01:33Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Social & Cultural GeographyPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rscg20

Animal behaviours, post-human lives: everydaynegotiations of the animal–human divide in pet-keepingRebekah Fox aa Royal Holloway, University of London, Department of Geography, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX,UK E-mail:Version of record first published: 20 Nov 2006.

To cite this article: Rebekah Fox (2006): Animal behaviours, post-human lives: everyday negotiations of the animal–humandivide in pet-keeping, Social & Cultural Geography, 7:4, 525-537

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14649360600825679

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematicreproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form toanyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contentswill be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses shouldbe independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims,proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly inconnection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Animal behaviours, post-human lives: everydaynegotiations of the animal–human divide

in pet-keeping

Rebekah FoxDepartment of Geography, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX,

UK, [email protected]

This paper explores the ways in which pet-owners in contemporary Britain mobilize thecategories of ‘animal’ and ‘human’ in their attempts to understand their pets. Pet-keepingforms one of the closest forms of human–animal interaction in modern western societyand as such provides an ideal opportunity to examine the ways in which peopleunderstand the similarities and differences between humans and non-humans in thecourse of their daily lives. Through the close-lived nature of pet–human relationships,people come to understand these animals in a variety of ways which go beyond ideas ofinstinctual behaviour to recognize their individual subjectivity and ‘personhood’, but alsorespect their ‘animalness’ and difference, rather than valuing them simply on the basis oftheir similarity to humans. These disruptions of binary categories provide a model forunderstanding notions of ‘post-humanism’ in the lived reality of everyday life.

Key words: animals, animal–human divide, anthropomorphism, nature, pets, post-humanism.

Introduction

Recent developments in social theory have

increasingly challenged Cartesian notions of

the binary divisions between ‘nature’ and

‘culture’, ‘humans’ and ‘animals’, and ‘wild-

ness’ and ‘civilization’, and raised new

theoretical and ethical questions concerning

human relationships with the non-human

world. Some theorists have argued that we

are living in a ‘post-human’ world, in which

the absolute boundaries between humans and

non-humans, nature and society have been

broken down and all beings are connected

together in a series of overlapping ‘webs’ or

‘networks’ of activity (Instone 1998). Recent

work within environmentalism, science and

technology studies, sociology and animal

geography (Arluke and Sanders 1996; Har-

away 2003; Philo and Wilbert 2000; What-

more 2002; Wolch and Emel 1998) has

recognized the importance of non-human

actors in the fabric of everyday social life

and challenged taken-for-granted notions of

Social & Cultural Geography, Vol. 7, No. 4, August 2006

ISSN 1464-9365 print/ISSN 1470-1197 online/06/040525-13 q 2006 Taylor & Francis

DOI: 10.1080/14649360600825679

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

human uniqueness on the grounds of agency

and intentionality, opening up new and

exciting opportunities for the study of non-

human lives.

However, despite such theoretical advance-

ments residual humanist values continue to

shape much popular and academic thinking

and practice and it is still unclear how ideas of

the ‘post-human’ and ‘post-natural’ are to be

played out in the practicality of everyday

living (Wolfe 2003).

The uncertainty of the human exists alongside

continued claims of certainty. The binary

oppositions of the past tremble but continue to

inform everyday decisions, assumptions and

activities. It is precisely this contradictory

condition . . . with which a ‘critical post

humanism’ (Didur 2003) must now endlessly

engage. (Badmington 2003)

Much of the work on the subject remains

largely theoretical and there have been few

attempts (Haraway 2003; Sanders 1995) to

engage with actual material embodied

relationships between humans and animals.

As Castree (2003: 209) puts it we now need to

‘move beyond suggestive metaphors in order

to properly flesh out what an amodern politics

of socionatural hybridity is all about’. In this

paper I consider the ways in which people

engage with ideas of nature, culture and

animal subjectivity in their everyday inter-

actions with, what is perhaps for most people

in western society, their closest form of contact

with the animal world: the domestic pet.

There has been relatively little previous

work in geography on companion animals

(Howell 2000, 2002; Laurier 2002; Tuan

1984) despite their presence and significance

in many people’s everyday lives. Pets occupy a

liminal position on the boundaries between

‘human’ and ‘animal’, appreciated by their

owners as ‘minded individuals’ (Sanders 1995)

or friends, capable of rational thought and

emotion, yet also treated as objects or

possessions to be discarded if they do not

conform to human expectations and values

(Belk 1996). They are both valued for their

‘animalness’ and subject to practices, such as

selective breeding, training and neutering,

which attempt to ‘civilize’ them and make

them more like ‘little humans’. Thus compa-

nion animal ownership provides a useful

means by which to re-examine recent theor-

etical attempts to deconstruct the nature/cul-

ture boundaries and understand the ways in

which distinctions between the animal and the

human are mobilized, disrupted, crossed and

re-crossed in the practical everyday dilemmas

of ownership, responsibility and affection

for pets.

Various models exist that aim to deal with

issues of human–animal relationships and

what these negotiations might look like if we

begin to take animals seriously as sentient

subjects. The most influential arguments are

those of traditional animal rights theory

(Regan 1983; Singer 1975) which call for the

extension of moral consideration to animals

on the basis of their similarities to humans,

such as their abilities to act on intelligence

rather than instinct, the extent to which they

can demonstrate cognitive skills, curiosity,

problem solving and social awareness, and feel

or express abstract emotions such as loss and

deprivation.

According to Sanders (1993) pet-owners

have long recognized the individuality and

subjectivity of their animals, contesting tra-

ditional ideas of language as the basis of

identity and Cartesian notions of the human–

animal divide. He argues that pet owners are

routinely engaged in a process of identity

construction, using their day-to-day experi-

ence with their dogs to construct them as

Rebekah Fox526

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

‘minded’ social actors. Owners attribute

‘personhood’ to their pets through several

mechanisms, including the recognition of

subjective thought, the possession of individ-

ual personalities, likes and dislikes, seeing the

animal as emotional and capable of engaging

in reciprocal social relationships and affording

the animal a social place as a member of the

family or close friend.

However, more recent work within the fields

of critical and feminist environmentalism

(Plumwood 2002; Seager 2003) has argued

that such theories go about addressing these

problems in the wrong way and allow for

erasure of difference and elevation of ‘higher’

animals (such as pets and primates) to the

status of humanity, whilst maintaining unsus-

tainable attitudes towards the environment

and ‘lower’ animals which are reduced to the

category of resources or ‘things’ for human

exploitation. They argue that many animal

rights philosophers are guilty of ‘neo-carte-

sianism’—simply trying to extend the privi-

leged category of humans in the human/nature

dualism rather than trying to break down the

dualism (Plumwood 2002: 143). Instead of

focusing upon notions of ‘personhood’ we

need to recognize the continuity of all life

forms and value beings for their own intrinsic

worth, autonomy and difference rather than

on the basis of their similarity to humans.

One recent publication that has aimed to

work with such notions of difference is Donna

Haraway’s Companion Species Manifesto

(2003) in which she asks the question ‘How

might an ethics and politics of significant

otherness be learned from taking dog–human

relationships seriously?’ (Haraway 2003: 3).

In this manifesto Haraway considers the

history and significance of dog training and

breeding and the importance of dog–human

relationships for understanding specific and

individual relationships of love and interaction

across the species barrier. Haraway argues that

such relationships cannot simply be seen as

about domination and control, nor a straight-

forward idealization of animals ‘uncondi-

tional love’ for their owners, but about a

messy, difficult love which

seeks to inhabit an inter-subjective world that is

about meeting the other in all the fleshy detail of a

mortal relationship with all its inevitable comic and

tragic mistakes in the permanent search for

knowledge of the intimate other. (Haraway 2003: 34)

In this paper I draw on my own ethnographic

research with pet owners to consider the

various ways in which people practically

engage with understanding the similarities

and differences between themselves and their

animal companions on a day-to-day basis. Pet

owners must try their best to comprehend their

animals behaviour and intentions, balancing

various interpretations of popular animal

psychology that naturalize ideas of ‘instinct’

or animal behaviour with informal notions of

anthropomorphism that deny animals absol-

ute difference from humans and attribute them

with ‘human-like’ intentions or emotions. The

idea that pets have thoughts, feelings, inten-

tions and personalities could be described as

deeply anthropomorphic. These two contra-

dictory models exist side-by-side, with pets

seen as both ‘human’ and ‘animal’, and the

ways in which pet owners move between these

different interpretations provides a useful

means for thinking through more academic

notions of post-humanism, which themselves

aim to disrupt absolute human–animal

binaries.

In this paper I firstly consider my method-

ologies used in this research, before moving on

to discuss two different ways in which people

understand their pets through ideas of both

‘animal instinct’ and ‘anthropomorphism’ and

Animal behaviours, post-human lives 527

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

the ways in which such ‘mixed’ understand-

ings disrupt binary categories of the natural

and social worlds. I then briefly consider the

case of unusual pets which challenge such

understandings and demonstrate the limits of

some pet-owners’ ‘post-humanist’ conceptions

of the animal kingdom.

Methodology

This article is based upon two years of ‘multi-

sited’ ethnographic research (Marcus 1995) in

Britain, including in-depth interviews and

observations with sixteen selected pet-owning

households (involving between one and six

visits) and participant observation in animal

hospitals, veterinary surgeries, pet shows, and

other pet-related sites and locations. In

addition, the selected households were asked

to keep ‘pet-diaries’ over a period of two

weeks, recording their everyday intimate

interactions with their companion animals

that they may not have deemed worthy of

mention in an interview situation. The

research also involved three months of ‘online

ethnography’ (Hine 2000) of internet pet

forums and chatrooms, particularly relating

to the ownership of unusual pets such as

reptiles, whose owners were difficult to

contact through traditional channels due to

recent controversy surrounding the import of

exotic species.

The sampling strategy for this research was

devised to reflect a diversity of pet-owners,

including breed enthusiasts, new pet owners,

those with multiple animals and owners of

exotic pets, as well as social differences

between owners, including a variety of house-

hold types (couples, families, single people and

the elderly). Pet-owners were recruited

through various avenues (pet organizations,

shows, snowballing techniques) and were

largely self-selected, as they had to agree to

devote large periods of time and energy to the

research process. Therefore these owners were

inevitably those with a particular interest in

pet-keeping, possibly excluding more ‘aver-

age’ owners who may be less attached, or even

view their animals with resentment.

The informants were predominantly white

and middle-class (again possibly reflecting

those who have time to devote to such

activities, particularly in the case of those

involved in showing) and included many more

women than men as the main interviewee (the

person who invited me into the house and did

most of the talking: many husbands or partners

were notorious by their absence or hovered in

the background). This may reflect a greater

interest amongst women in pet-keeping, or in

their willingness to talk about their emotions in

relation to it, the fact that some men worked

longer hours so were not in when I visited, or

perhaps my own positionality as a young

female researcher, feeling more confident

approaching another woman in a social

context such as a pet show and accepting

invitations to their home (England 1994; Silvey

2003). Therefore the research cannot be seen as

representative of British pet-keeping as a

whole, but reflects a specific view of pet–

human relations taken from a particular cross-

section of society. However, whilst the owners

represented here may reflect an overly positive

view of the pet-keeping relationship, certain

aspects of these relations are relevant to other

forms of human–animal interactions and can

usefully say something about the everyday

negotiation of post-human ideas.

Natural behaviours?

One of the major dilemmas posed by the pet–

human relationship is that of the pet’s dual

Rebekah Fox528

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

status as both a ‘person’ and possession.

People must negotiate this in their everyday

decisions about their companion’s welfare and

treatment. Relationships with pets are not

necessarily easy and involve two beings

‘getting on together’ (Haraway 2003) in a

relationship that inevitably involves some

forms of restriction, power and control and a

certain degree of guilt, worry or uncertainty

about what the animal is thinking or feeling,

particularly where the relationship is seen to

be unsuccessful.

In his 1984 book, Dominance and Affec-

tion—The Making of Pets, Tuan argues that

pet-keeping is little more than an exercise in

patronizing and playful domination, reflect-

ing ‘man’s inherent insecurity and need to

display his power to subdue the unruly forces

of nature’ (Tuan 1984: 5). However, I would

argue that whilst humans are certainly the

dominant partner in such relationships (and

may see their animals as property, status

symbols or even toys), the relationship is

much more complex than this, involving a

range of reciprocal exchanges and emotions

of both human and animal agency. Thinking

about such relationships and human and

animal understandings of one another can

provide a useful resource for thinking

through the practicalities of post-human

perspectives in the reality of everyday lived

relationships.

One way in which humans try to under-

stand their pets behaviour is through popular

notions of animal psychology, based upon

studies of ‘natural’ behaviours in their wild

ancestors. Such ideas have gained immense

popularity in recent years, promoted through

numerous animal behaviour books (Bessant

2001; Morris 1986) and television pro-

grammes. Such guidebooks provide one

means of challenging anthropocentric under-

standings, revealing a marked change in

human attitudes to accept ‘animals as

animals’ (Philo and Wilbert 2000), rather

than furry ‘little humans’. However, such

readings tend to essentialize animal actions

into supposed ‘biological’ behaviour, such as

the ‘pack instinct’ in dogs, and ignore animal

agency and individual interaction in the

relationship.

In popular guides to pet behaviour notions

of the ‘natural cat’ revolve around ideas of

them as independent, solitary hunters, who

lead a ‘double life’ between the overprotected

child that they are with their human owners

and their ‘wild side’ as miniature versions of

large predatory cats such as lions and tigers

(Bessant 2001). This independent image

means that owners are often uneasy about

restricting their cat’s behaviour and allow

them a degree of freedom and independence in

the relationship not usually given to dogs.

However, such freedom is often difficult to

balance with emotional feelings towards the

cat as a ‘human-like’ being who they may wish

to protect and nurture and can cause a great

deal of worry for owners when cats disappear

for extended periods of time or they cannot

locate them when they are required for a meal

or emotional attention.

Such dilemmas are revealed in the case of

Fiona and her six-month-old kitten Oliver.

The issues here are those of appropriate care

and control, balancing Fiona’s affection and

protective nurturing feelings for this small

creature, with necessary feelings of guilt and

worry about his happiness and freedom.

I’m scared to let Oliver out especially when he’s

full of food and has no reason to come back. I feel

differently about him than my other cat Charlie

because he’s mine, I’ve had him since he was a

baby and brought him up so I feel responsible for

him. Charlie was here with James before I moved

in so there’s not much I can do about him going

Animal behaviours, post-human lives 529

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

off on his own but I want to protect Oliver.

There’s a busy road at the top of the street and I

just don’t trust other people with him, the people

in the house opposite are always trying to feed

Charlie and steal him away from us. The first time

we let him out he disappeared for 20 hours and

got locked in someone’s garage and I sort of

blamed James for it because I just didn’t think he

was looking after him as carefully as I would.

Sometimes I think I’m being unfair though,

because he’s a cat and he should be allowed to

go outside and enjoy himself and chase birds and

things and I’m just being selfish because I worry

so much about him. (Fiona, interview, 2003)

This raises issues about ideas of ‘the natural’

and what humans consider natural cat

behaviour and instincts and their rights to

engage in activities such as hunting or

roaming. This must be balanced with their

expectations of an animal as a possession to

conform to their idealizations of what a pet

should be: always present, loving and grate-

fully reciprocal of human affection.

Many of my interviewees showed a wide

knowledge of popular notions of animal

psychology taken from books or television

programmes and used elements of these ideas

in explaining their pet’s behaviour. Owners

said that they had read up on pet psychology in

an attempt to better understand their relation-

ships and were keen to explain the ‘biological’

‘reasons’ behind their animals behaviour,

seemingly accepting these explanations as

‘scientific’ fact. For dog owners the overriding

argument was that of the ‘pack mentality’,

with owners adjusting their own behaviour to

prevent the dogs from becoming dominant

amongst their human ‘pack’. One owner,

Andrew, particularly adhered to such notions,

rejecting his wife’s ideas that their sheepdog

Molly is very intelligent and can tell what you

are thinking and feeling and arguing that:

She is just a dog and does things because she is a dog.

She thinks of us as her pack and likes to protect the

pack by barking if any visitors arrive and protecting

the weaker members such as the cat if any other cats

or dogs try to harass her. She is always fed after

Poppy (the cat) both to prevent her from stealing the

cat’s food and stop her from thinking she is

dominant, as dominant animals always get first

pick of the prey in the wild. (Andrew, interview,

January 2003)

Cat owners too were keen to use ideas of

‘instinct’ and ‘natural’ behaviour such as

Jane’s explanation of her cat Woody’s beha-

viour below. Such explanations can be useful

in mediating the more problematic aspects of

human–animal relationships, where animals

may behave in ways that go against human

norms and exhibit ‘non-human-like’ beha-

viour that challenges more anthropomorphic

understandings of them as miniature humans

or substitute children.

Woody scratches the door when I shut him out of

the living room when we’re out. I don’t think he

likes being shut out of part of his territory. But if I

let them in they will make a mess, scratching the

carpet and knocking the plants over. I know it’s only

natural for them to mark their territory and sharpen

their claws, but I’d rather they didn’t do it on my

new carpet! (Jane, pet diary, 2003)

However, it was clear that none of those

interviewed fully accepted such reductive

accounts of instinctual behaviour and both used

andrejected these ideas ina self-consciousway. In

most cases these notions were combined with

more ‘anthropomorphic’ understandings on the

basis of individual intelligence and emotion,

seeing their pets as real individuals or characters,

who can challenge taken-for-granted assump-

tions of their psychology through unexpected

agency, reactions or behaviour.

Rebekah Fox530

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

‘Pets as people’: agency and anthro-pomorphism

Living intimately with animals on a day-to-

day basis means that pets and owners come to

know each other’s individual personality

quirks and traits, viewing their animals as

subjective beings and attributing them with

human-like characteristics. Whilst some own-

ers I spoke to were a little wary of highly

anthropomorphized accounts of animal beha-

viour, perhaps viewing me as something of

an ‘expert’ who would look down on such

unreflective understandings, many people do

interpret their animal’s everyday actions in

terms of their own ways of viewing the world.

Animals are regularly attributed with human

emotions such as jealously, anger, disappoint-

ment and love, which is usually a human

means of acknowledging the animal’s role in

an active reciprocal relationship.

Whilst Haraway (2003) is highly suspicious

of any attempt to understand animals through

human ways of seeing the world, she does

recognize that people do not have separate

thought processes for understanding animal

behaviour and to guess at their emotions using

human values may be better than simply

ignoring animal communications as ‘mere’

instinct.

All that philosophically suspect language is

necessary to keep humans alert to the fact that

somebody is at home in the animals they work with.

(Haraway 2003: 49)

Often this anthropomorphism can be very

knowing and self-critical, with people

enjoying creating fanciful notions of their

animals’ thoughts and behaviours, perhaps as

much for the benefit of other humans as their

understandings of the animals themselves. One

interviewee, Caroline, provided very anthro-

pomorphic explanations of her animals’

behaviour in her ‘tongue in cheek’ pet diary,

written from the perspective of her cat Eeyore,

sat at the keyboard, ‘making his paw ache!’

Whilst the diary was obviously intended to be

taken with a pinch of salt, it provided an

interesting insight into human anthropo-

morphic explanations of a ‘cat’s mind’ that

revealed Caroline’s interpretations of the ways

in which her animals see the world.

‘Eeyore’ pet diary—24 April 2003

This morning my mother and aunt and I sat on our

ledge and watched funny white stuff falling out of

the sky, I tried to catch it but it kept vanishing.

Human Mother turned the heating back on in our

house, she is quite thoughtful really, but we do have

very thick fur coats and quite like sitting outside. I

suppose she means well. I was a bit bored being kept

inside so I decided to do some big game hunting,

found a big blue bottle and chased it about for a bit,

then it stopped playing so I left it on the bedroom

floor. Human Mother can hoover it up; she’s always

running around with that noisy thing! Apparently

one of us is always leaving great lumps of hair on

her precious carpets and of course it couldn’t

possibly be Spider (the dog)! The lazy git was meant

to be giving me a paw with this diary but he’s just

sleeping in front of the wood burner as usual.

Others used ideas of anthropomorphism more

subtly, using it to attribute their animals with

an active role in the relationship and a

capacity for intelligent thought or emotion.

Charmaine described the ways in which her

dog Toby had adapted to her increasing

disability, showing intimate knowledge and

levels of thought and cognition well beyond

those of inherited genetic instincts.

Animal behaviours, post-human lives 531

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

Toby is very good and follows me everywhere. He

even used to come with me on my hairdressing runs

when I worked. It’s as if he knows about my

disability and how hard it is for me sometimes. If

my sister Nicole takes him for a walk he will pull on

the lead but he never does with me and he waits for

me at steps where he knows I will be slow or if I’m

in my wheelchair he trots along beside. When we’re

playing with a ball in the house he knows I can’t

keep bending down to get it so he drops it on my

lap. (Charmaine, interview, March 2003)

Elizabeth discussed her cat’s emotional feel-

ings, particularly in relation to the recent

death of her husband Ian. Attributing animals

with feelings such as love and grief recognizes

their subjectivity and reciprocal role in the

pet–human relationship, rather than basing

their actions purely on instinctual behaviours

or abstract attachment to the human who

feeds them or whom they regard as a superior

member of the ‘pack’.

My husband died recently. He and the cat were very

attached. We adopted him when we moved into the

house a few years ago. Ian adored him and used to

talk about him all the time. Because he wasn’t very

well and couldn’t get about much he spent a lot of

time sitting in his chair, usually with Kim on his

knee. I found him lying in the bedroom when I woke

up in the morning. Kim was with me, he jumped up

on the bed and sniffed him, prodding him with his

paw to try and wake him up. He seemed to realize

that something was wrong and curled up asleep

next to him, refusing to leave his side until the

undertakers came to take the body away. After that

he slept on Ian’s clothes all day and night, where I

had left them after he had taken them off the night

before he died. He seemed disturbed and uneasy,

hanging around the house and following me as if he

thought I might go away too. He often went to Ian’s

chair, as if he was checking that he might have

come back. I don’t know how much animals can

understand, but he definitely knew something was

wrong and certainly missed Ian like crazy. I would

say animals can feel grief too. (Elizabeth, interview,

2004)

In the course of my research I was also

constantly aware of my own anthropomorph-

ism. In seeking a less anthropocentric view of

human–animal interactions, which acknowl-

edges the animal role in the relationship, I

subconsciously reflected my interviewees’

language in attributing thought and intention

to my animal subjects. Therefore when

describing a situation, I repeatedly found

myself describing an animal as bored, excited

or inquisitive, without really realizing that I

was doing so. However this in itself is relevant.

If the aim of post-humanism is to destabilize the

categories of ‘human’ and ‘animal’ themselves,

seeing these as interconnected and made up of

a complex network of associations, then the

problematic nature of ‘anthropomorphic’

interpretations is also questioned, because

particular qualities can no longer be seen as

distinctly human.

In addition to the attribution of supposedly

‘human-like’ characteristics and emotions,

pet–human relationships cross understand-

ings of the human–animal divide through the

embodied intimacy of their everyday relations,

revealing the importance of animals in every-

day human social interactions and conceptions

of family, kinship and domesticity. Many of

those I spoke to viewed their animals as part of

the immediate family, sharing in their intimate

daily routines and interactions. The relation-

ship with pets is a very tactile one, providing

both human and animal with physical contact

and affection. Much of the relationship and

interaction is based upon the owner’s sense of

reciprocal exchanges of love and affection,

with animals admitted to the most intimate

areas of people’s lives that would not be shared

Rebekah Fox532

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

even with other humans. For many this

pleasure seems to be mutual, with both

animals and humans enjoying the simple

satisfaction of being together and engaging in

play and close physical contact.

After James has gone, Oliver joins me for cuddles as

it’s my day off. He’s wide awake and nudges me

whenever I relax my arm from tickling him. He rolls

around showing his tummy for difficult bits to be

tickled and looks at me and puts his paws on my

face a lot. I pretend to sleep and he nudges my face

with his and purrs. He makes me feel so happy.

(Fiona, pet diary, 2003)

This intimacy is something that some people

may be embarrassed to admit for fear of

stigmatization or disgust at the close-lived

nature of the relationship with another

species, which is seen as crossing moral or

social boundaries. Whilst it is accepted that

humans and animals will engage in close

physical contact, boundaries for this vary

greatly even within the same society or family

and ideas of hygiene and appropriate beha-

viour are very much a matter of individual

taste. Christine, another interviewee,

described her personal relationship with her

Pyrenean mountain dogs, a secret that even

her husband is not allowed to share.

The dogs sleep in the bedroom at night except the

puppy who isn’t allowed upstairs yet because it’s

bad for her hips. They go straight up there when

we go to bed and lie down still otherwise they

know they won’t be allowed to stay. My husband

doesn’t let Jenna sleep on the bed, but as soon as

she hears the door go when he leaves at five she

jumps straight up and by ten past five her head is

fast asleep on the pillow. I like this time snuggling

up to the warmth of a furry body beside me.

(Christine, interview, 2003)

Such non-verbal, non-cognitive interaction

again challenges human social and conceptual

boundaries, but perhaps in a less human-

centred way, valuing animals for their own

characteristics rather than trying to define

their ‘personhood’ through the recognition of

human-like attributes. This challenges con-

cerns over the ‘dangers’ of anthropomorph-

ism, which themselves stem from a particular

conception of the human rational subject and

consider animals in terms of their similarities

to humans.

However, these issues are not simple and

boundaries and assumptions that people draw

are variable and shifting, and may differ

considerably according to the type of pet

involved. The relationship between humans

and certain mammals such as cats and dogs

has become naturalized in our society and it is

almost assumed that humans will enjoy special

loving relationships with such creatures that

honour their subjectivity and place them in a

special category on the boundaries between

humans and animals. Yet as Seager (2003) and

Plumwood (2002) point out, there are limits to

these considerations and all animals are not

seen as equal in the ‘post-human’ world. In the

case of smaller or more unusual pets such as

reptiles, close inter-personal relationships are

not so widely accepted and may still be seen as

odd in today’s society, particularly in the case

of creatures such as snakes and rats that are

feared for their ‘difference’ (hard scaly skin, no

legs, long rubbery tails) or subversive image.

These animals challenge traditional ideas

of what a pet should be (furry, cuddly,

emotionally responsive) and call for a different

type of relationship to that enjoyed with larger

mammals. They complicate some of the

debates surrounding animal ‘personhood’ and

similarity to humans and how far people are

prepared to extend this into the animal

kingdom (Bride 1998; Griffin 1992). There

Animal behaviours, post-human lives 533

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

was disagreement amongst my interviewees

about the degrees to which such animals are

capable of thought or emotional response, with

even many devoted owners claiming that they

are not capable of love (seen as a defining

feature in most pet-keeping relationships) and

in some cases treating them almost more like

a zoo or botanical collection than individual

social actors.

I think snakes are more a ‘visual’ pet. They are there

to be enjoyed cause they look pretty and also I find it

is rewarding to keep them and breed them as they

need more specialized care than just chucking some

food in a dish, or throwing a rug on a horse kind of

thing. As for what the snakes/geckos get out of the

relationship: they get all their needs delivered.

Food, warmth, a comfy, clean home and its fun

making their vivariums look interesting visually and

for their benefit too! I think I am much closer with

George and Meghan (a horse and dog), simply due

to them being able to understand emotion better

than reptiles can. I have heard that some

lizards/geckos can be ‘affectionate’ though and do

think my geckos are much more ‘friendly’ than the

snakes. (Rachel, livefood.co.uk, private message,

November 2003)

Thus relationships with reptiles reveal another

side to companion animal ownership, whereby

certain pets are placed further down, or even

outside the field of ‘personhood’, putting these

animals in a liminal category of their own on

the borders between pets and other types of

animals displayed in zoos or personal mena-

geries. These animals may be valued more for

their ‘difference’ and biological interest as a

‘foreign’ species, rather than for their person-

alities as human companions and treated more

as possessions or objects than individuals.

Although some owners do attempt to engage

in subjective relationships with such creatures,

they are very aware of and respect their

differences from humans, demonstrating the

limits of traditional animal rights thinking

which values other species simply on the basis

of their similarity to humans.

Conclusion

This paper has demonstrated some of the

sophisticated ways that people think through

their relationships with companion animals in

the course of their everyday lives, balancing

ideas of animal psychology and ‘instinct’ with

their own understandings of their companion

as an individual or friend. Living together with

another species on a daily basis necessitates a

certain intimacy and recognition of indivi-

duality and personality in non-humans,

challenging Cartesian notions of the animal–

human divide. According to Irvine:

The intimate nature of pet-keeping relationships

and everyday practices of interaction, physical

contact and play, by honoring animal’s subjectivity

and communication skills, can gradually undermine

hegemonic views of them as ‘other’ and become an

‘individualized act of political resistance to society’s

disregard for non-human life’. (Irvine 2001: 152)

For me the importance of pet–human

relationships lies in their very individuality

and personal emotion, that allows for the

recognition of not only the similarities

between humans and animals but also their

differences, allowing for an ‘ethics of care’

(Seager 2003) that has been largely rejected

in traditional animal rights philosophy and

recognizing that, as Shiva (2000: 74) puts

it, ‘even the tiniest life form (must be)

recognized as having intrinsic worth, integ-

rity and autonomy’ and should not be

valued simply on the basis of their similarity

to humans.

Rebekah Fox534

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

The ways in which pet-owners readily mix

and match different interpretations of their

animals in the everyday practices can be seen

as a resource to complement more academic

notions of post-humanism and challenge

intellectual debates over blurring of bound-

aries and animal similarity and difference. If

people can happily move between the

categories of ‘animal’ and ‘human’ in their

everyday language, this places questions over

the problematic nature of the use of

‘anthropomorphism’ in academic debates.

This concern over the use of anthropomorph-

ism is itself is based upon a particular

conception of the rational human subject,

which sees certain qualities as particularly

human, which can only be self-consciously

attributed to animals in an attempt to define

their ‘personhood’. The intimate embodied

nature of the human–pet relationship can

challenge such ways of defining subjectivity,

through other types of relationality and

connection, which are not purely based

upon notions of language or intelligence.

This ‘lived intersubjectivity’ of two beings

sharing a messy, awkward, loving relation-

ship provides an ideal opportunity for

thinking practically about some of the real-

life dilemmas presented in recent theoretical

challenges to the animal–human divide and

helps us go beyond theories of destabilized

categories to the complex theorizations and

practices of everyday life.

However pet–human relationships are not

always successful and differences between

human and animal behaviour can lead to

lack of understanding and problems where

animals do not to live up to human

expectations of what a pet should be. It is

also important to note here the very

selectivity of this care and the distinct lack

of connection found in my research between

the love and care of individual animals and

what Jones (2000) refers to as the ‘ambig-

uous and space-specific’ human treatment of

animals, whereby we lavish affection on

certain creatures whilst exploiting, torturing

and eating others for human gain. This

includes the numerous animals killed to

provide ‘luxury’ foods for our valued pets

and their owners and the lab rats upon

which their medicines are tested. However

other types of human–animal relations are

also contested, involving a variety of

individual, emotional and practical under-

standings of animal subjectivity, for example

in recent work on farm animals (Holloway

2001; Tsovel 2005).

Furthermore, human understandings of

subjectivity have their limitations and do not

even extend to all forms of companion

animals, with people drawing variable and

shifting boundaries of ‘personhood’ that do

not necessarily include less similar creatures

such as rodents and reptiles, revealing the

continued valuation of other species on the

basis of their similarity to humans. Whilst pet

owners do challenge ‘post-human’ boundaries

with their understandings, humanity still

remains the key reference point and embedded

distinctions are difficult to undo. It is such

issues that now must be addressed in order to

come to terms with what kinds of ‘care, regard

and responsiveness’ (Cheney 1994: 158) might

be possible for us in a relationship with all

members of the non-human world.

References

Arluke, A. and Sanders, C. (1996) Regarding Animals.

Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.

Badmington, N. (2003) Mapping post humanism, paper,

Institute of British Geographers Conference. IBG

Conference (September), London.

Belk, R.W. (1996) Metaphoric relationships with pets,

Society and Animals 4: 121–146.

Animal behaviours, post-human lives 535

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

Bessant, C. (2001) The Cat Whisperer—The Secret of

How to Talk to Your Cat. London: John Blake

Publishing.

Bride, I. (1998) Herpetofauna pet-keeping by secondary

school students, Society and Animals 6: 31–46.

Castree, N. (2003) Environmental issues: relational

ontologies and hybrid politics, Progress in Human

Geography 27: 203–211.

Cheney, J. (1994) Nature/theory/difference: ecofeminist

reconstruction of environmental ethics, in Warren, J.

(ed.) Ecological Feminism. London: Routledge, pp.

158–177.

Didur, J. (2003) Re-embodying technoscientific fantasies:

post humanism, genetically modified foods and the

colonization of life, Cultural Critique 53: 98–115.

England, K. (1994) Getting personal: reflexivity, position-

ality, and feminist research, Professional Geographer

46: 80–89.

Griffin, D.R. (1992) Animal Minds. Chicago: Chicago

University Press.

Haraway, D. (2003) The Companion Species Manifesto—

Dogs, People and Significant Otherness. Chicago:

Prickly Paradigm Press.

Hine, C. (2000) Virtual Ethnography. Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage.

Holloway, L. (2001) Pets and protein. Placing domestic

livestock on hobby farms in England and Wales, Journal

of Rural Studies 17: 293–307.

Howell, P. (2000) Flush and the banditti: dog-stealing in

VictorianLondon, inPhilo,C. andWilbert,C. (eds)Animal

Spaces, Beastly Places—New Geographies of Animal–

Human Relations. London: Routledge, pp. 35–55.

Howell, P. (2002) A place for the animal dead: pets, pet

cemeteries and animal ethics in late Victorian Britain,

Ethics, Place and Environment 5(1): 5–22.

Instone, L. (1998) The coyote’s at the door: re-visioning

human-environment relations in the Australian context,

Ecumene 5: 452–467.

Irvine, L. (2001) The power of play, Anthrozoos 14:

151–160.

Jones, O. (2000) (Un)ethical geographies, in Philo, C. and

Wilbert, C. (eds) Animal Spaces, Beastly Places: New

Geographies of Animal–Human Relations. London:

Routledge, pp. 268–291.

Laurier, E. (2002) The cat in the shed: pets as moral and

spatial features of a community, unpublished paper,

University of Glasgow.

Marcus, G.E. (1995) Ethnography in/of the world system:

the emergence of multi-sited ethnography, Annual

Review of Anthropology 24: 95–117.

Morris, D. (1986) Dogwatching. London: Jonathan Cape.

Philo, C. and Wilbert, C. (eds) (2000) Animal Spaces,

Beastly Places: New Geographies of Human–Animal

Relations. London: Routledge.

Plumwood, V. (2002) Environmental Culture: The

Ecological Crisis of Reason. London: Routledge.

Regan, T. (1983) The Case for Animal Rights. Berkeley:

University of California Press.

Sanders, C. (1993) Understanding dogs: caretakers’

attributions of mindedness in canine-human relation-

ships, Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 22:

205–226.

Sanders, C. (1995) Killing with kindness: veterinary

euthanasia and the social construction of personhood,

Sociological Forum 10: 195–214.

Seager, J. (2003) Pepperoni or broccoli? On the cutting

wedge of feminist environmentalism, Gender, Place and

Culture 10: 167–174.

Shiva, V. (2000) Stolen Harvest: The Hijacking of the

Global Food Supply. Cambridge, MA: South End Press.

Silvey, R. (2003) Gender and mobility. Critical ethno-

graphies of migration in Indonesia, in Blunt, A.,

Gruffudd, P., May, J., Ogborn, M. and Pinder, D. (eds)

Cultural Geography in Practice. London: Arnold,

pp. 91–105.

Singer, P. (1975) Animal Liberation. New York: Avon.

Tsovel, A. (2005) What can a farm animal

biography accomplish?, Society and Animals 13:

245–262.

Tuan, Y.-F. (1984) Dominance and Affection—The

Making of Pets. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Whatmore, S. (2002) Hybrid Geographies—Natures,

Cultures, Spaces. London: Sage.

Wolch, J. and Emel, J. (1998) Animal Geographies—

Place, Politics and Identity in the Nature–Culture

Borderlands. London: Verso.

Wolfe, C. (2003) Animal Rites: American Culture,

the Discourse of Species and Posthumanist Theory.

Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Abstract translations

Les comportements animaux, les vies posthumaines:les negociations quotidiennes du fosse animal–humain a l’egard des animaux de compagnie

Cet article explore les moyens utilises par lesproprietaires d’animaux de compagnie pour faire

Rebekah Fox536

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3

valoir les categories «animal» et «humain» dansleurs efforts pour comprendre leurs animaux. Lesanimaux de compagnie eleves a la maison represen-tent une des formes d’interaction entre humains etanimaux des plus etroites dans la societe occidentalemoderne et donne ainsi la possibilite revee depouvoir examiner les moyens que les gens utilisentpour comprendre les similitudes et les differencesentre les humains et les non humains tout au long deleur vie. Les rapports etroits qui lient les animauxde compagnie et les humains permettent aux gensde comprendre ces animaux par toute une gamme demoyens qui, au-dela des idees associees au compor-tement instinctif, distinguent leur subjectiviteindividuelle et leur «etat d’etre humain», en plus depouvoir respecter leur «animalitude» et leurdifference au lieu de les valoriser simplement envertu de leur ressemblance aux humains. Cesbouleversements des categories binaires fournissentun modele pour comprendre les notions de «post-humanisme» dans la realite vecue de tous les jours.

Mots-clefs: animaux, fosse animal–humain,anthropomorphisme, nature, animaux de compag-nie, posthumanisme.Comportamientos animales, vidas poshumanas:negociaciones cotidianas de la division animal–humano en la tenencia de mascotas

Este papel explora los modos en que lospropietarios de mascotas en la Gran Bretanacontemporanea utilizan las categorıas de ‘animal’y ‘ser humano’ con el fin de entender susmascotas. La tenencia de mascotas constituyeuna de las formas de interaccion humano–animalmas allegadas en la sociedad occidental contem-poranea y, por lo tanto, ofrece una oportunidadideal para examinar los modos en que laspersonas ven las similitudes y diferencias entreseres humanos y no humanos en la vida cotidiana.Por el hecho de ser una relacion estrecha la demascota–humano, las personas llegan a entenderestos animales en una variedad de maneras quevan mas alla de ideas de comportamientoinstintivo. Reconocen la subjetividad individualy los ‘personajes’ de los animales y, al mismotiempo, respetan su naturaleza animal y susdiferencias en lugar de valorarlos simplementepor su parecido con humanos. Estos trastornos delas categorıas binarias ofrecen un modelo paramejor entender ideas de ‘poshumanismo’ en larealidad vivida de la vida cotidiana.

Palabras claves: animales, division animal–humano, antropomorfismo, naturaleza, mascotas,poshumanismo.

Animal behaviours, post-human lives 537

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Sy

dney

] at

01:

33 1

4 M

arch

201

3