ao a35 single trial brain electrical patterns of … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical...

94
AO A35 545 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF AN AUDITORY / AND VISUAL PERCEPTU..(U} EEG SYSTEMS LAB SAN FRANCISCO (INCASSI CA A S GEVINS ET AL. JUN 83 AFOSR-TR-83-1014 NLASSIF ED F49620-82-K-0006 F/G 6/16 N im0 MEI mhmhiimni IIMEII

Upload: others

Post on 30-Apr-2020

7 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

AO A35 545 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF AN AUDITORY /AND VISUAL PERCEPTU..(U} EEG SYSTEMS LAB SAN FRANCISCO

(INCASSI CA A S GEVINS ET AL. JUN 83 AFOSR-TR-83-1014

NLASSIF ED F49620-82-K-0006 F/G 6/16 N

im0MEI mhmhiimni

IIMEII

Page 2: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

1.8

MICROCOPY RESOLUTION TEST CHART

WA&TOMAL WURUU or STAMOAts- los3

A

Page 3: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

AFOSR-T. 83-1014

EEG SYSTEMS LABORATORY1855 FOLSOM ST.SAN FRANCISCO CA 94103(415) 621-8343

INTERIM PROGRESS REPORT: 22 FEB 82 to I JUN 83

AFOSR contract: F49620-82-K-0006

A. Fregly, Scientific Officer

DTICI A. Gevins, Director DEC 2

I' H

Approved for publto eleaseI

513 FILE COPY -"t "t-=m='".,83 12 06 109

A mm, l i ,m lammyoe aiklmm w umi~~im_, t " # --1-?: ": _ -U Im lup ~ ~em l

A 1/ P a

/4

Page 4: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

UnclassifiedSECURITY CLASSITICAT1ON Of ThIS PAGE (ISo Dw.EMwtse

AUDIT R EPO DRTA DOUENTTON TACE MUD m**uc** * ~1. AUTN ums.) .. INONT CATAO GAT NUM@9*a

4.'Iil~z(ad vAlan S. Gevins Sore REOR a ressieren

Brian A. Cutillo, Joseph C. Doyle -F49620-82-K-0006Robert S. Tannehill, & Gerald M. Zeitlin

9. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION NAME9 AND ADDRESS 1O0RGA LEE RJC. TASKAREA WOKNTNUER

EEG Systems Laboratory 2313 A*1855 Folsom 61102 Fqgyi Ivsnrwrn CA W&IMl

11. CON4TROLLINGGIVVICE NAME AND ADDRESS 12. REPORT DATEDirectorate of Life Sciences JUNE 1983AFOSR/ML - 13 HUNIIEm OF PAGESBuilding 410, Bolling AFB DC 20332 86

14. MONITORING AGECYC NAMIE AI AORESJ(I different Snow Cuu.J*1ul offie) IS. SECURITY CLASS. (of this two)

unclassified54L FICATION/ DOWNGRADING

IS. DISTRIEUT10ON STATEMENT (of his. Report) AnqtnFr

Approved for "public release; distribution unlimited DTIC TAB

17. D*STRIOUTION STATEMENT (o1 h. astact entrd toDe StoI i Eg..e hasp

7Availab' ity Codes

, ii and/orIII. SUPPLEMENTARY NOTES

It. KEY WORDS (Cedftve res, sig de itne a...O GW lPfdp yIp bb" Mmbw)

perceptuomotor, auditory, visual, brain potentials, event related potentials,

evoked correlations, single trial, digital signal processing, mathematicalI

misri pstl and 2)e tpproesssedo auditory-vsa andetuo pisuadig numri

stimuli. A blooda aigm sufficiently controlled for the applicationof a 49 channel Neurocognitive Pattern (NCP) Analysis has been finalized

DD, 'J0Aw 143 ESTIO as' aV ISOUsIef Unclassified84eUmI'v GLAMM"CATON Op TNIS PAE (Men am a

40

Page 5: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

IMCU74Ty CLAMAlrTIOW Of TIS PA(lbm D. aEnose

(cont. Block #20)

.and participant screening sessions have begun. Sections III and IV of thisInterim Progress Report are comprised of published (Science, 220:97-99,1983) and in preparation papers describing our recent visuospatialmove/no-move study. The results of further signal processing studies onthat data are described in Sections V and VT. The research described insections (III-VI) was also sponsored by the 0 ice of Naval Research andthe Air Force School of Aerospace Medicine.

SCRV LSACy ,Y Ar~.S.W.

A

44

sa ASWS w ot@

Page 6: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

AIR FORCE FI CE r!? SCIETTFIC RESRC 1 ("SC)

IOTICE 0-F I' Dic'This tcndi

app-10V -12.Distr~itl.:'." " ...d

MATTHEW J. X_1RChief, Technical Information Division

Io* OVERVIEW......o*..o*......, ..... *.*o.**.... *.....o...*.oo*..o..o...!e

A. Senior Scientific Personnel of the EEG SystemsLaboratory..0... 1

B. 1983 Papers..... 1

C. Major Presentations, 1982-1993, A. Gevins...... 1

0. Experiments.0....2

E. Analytic Methods......4

1. Similarities and Differences Between NeurocognitivePattern Analysis and Conventional ERP Analysis...';..

2. Evoked Correlations Between Scalp Electrodes......5

II. PILOTING OF AN AUDITORY-VISUAL PERCEPTUOMOTOR TASK...........5

A. Design Considerations... 5

1. Cognitive Considerations.•. .

2. Experimental Ccntrol......6

3. Montage ..... . 6

B. Task development and pilot recordings......6

1. Phase one (P's #1-8)t (Respond to miscues).....•6

a Rationale•....6

b. Task......6

c. Recordings..,.. 7

d. Results and Discussion......8

2. Phase two (P's #9-12): (No response to miscues).,....9

a. Rationale...... 9

b. Task*..,. 9

c. Recordings,..,. 9

d. Results and Discussion.,... 10

/ i

. . .. .. ,_ ... . " r , .. ... ,i~ 'F ' •-

Page 7: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

C. Final Paradigm-Screening of Participants..,... 11

D. Bibliography.o.... 12

III. Shadows of thoughts: Rapidly changing, asymmetric, brainpotential patterns of a brief visuomotor task. Alan S.Gevins, Robert E Schaffer Joeseph C. Doyle Brian A.'Cutillo, Robert L Tannehill and Steven Lo BresslerScienc 1983P 220e 97-99...25

SIV. Neurocognitive pattern analysis of a visuomotor task:

Low-frequency evoked correlations* Alan S Gevins,I Joseph Co Doyley Brian At Cutillo, Robert E. Schaffer,~~Robert So Tarnrehill and Steven L. Bressler. To be submitted ..... 29

V. SIGNAL FPROCESSING STUDIES (Also Sponsored b?-I the Office of Naval Research and the USAF~~~School of Aerospace Medicine)#* #*.. s****. .. ** .. ,. ...

SA. Introduction.,..too62

B-. Methods#.#.. 62

1. Measures,..0,. 62

2. Channels...... 63

3. Time intervals...... 64

C. Results.....,64

1. Crosscovariance Measures......64

2. Single-Channel Power Measures .... . 65

D. Conclusions..... 65

VI. DISTINCT BRAIN-POTENTIAL PATTERNS ACCOMPANYINGBEHAVIORALLY IDENTICAL TRIALS (Also sponsoredby the Office of Naval Research).8.*.. ............... 84

The research described in Sections IoD.4 through I.E.2 and SectionsIII to VI is also sponsored by the Office of Naval Research and theAir Force School of Aerospace Medicine.

4f11 _ _ _ _ __- - ._ _ _ _ _ _

Page 8: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

I. OVERVIEW

A. Senior Scientific Personnel g9f the EU Systems Laboratory

Steven L. Bressler, NeurophysiologistBrian A. Cutillo, Cognitive ScientistJoseph C. Doyle, NeurophysicistAlan S. Gevins, DirectorRonald K. Stone, NeuropsychiatristRobert S. Tannehill, ProgrammerGerald M. Zeitlin, Systems Engineer

B. 1983 Paez

1. Gevins, A.S., Schaffer, R.E., Doyle, J.C., Cutillo, B.A.,Tannehill, R.L. and Bressler, S.L. Shadows of thoughts: Rapidlychanging , asymmetric, brain potential patterns of a brief visuomotortask. Science, 1983, 220, 97-99.

2. Gevins, A.S. Brain potentials and mental functions:methodological requirements. In I* Alter (Ed.), Thi LimitsFunctional LgscalizaigLDY Raven Press, 1983, In press.

3. Gevins, A.S. Brain potential evidence for lateralization ofhigher cognitive functions. In J.B. Hellige (Ed.), CerebralHemisphere Asymmetry' Method, Theory and Application. Praeger Press,1983, In press.

Fuc4n Gevins, A.S. Brain Potentials and Human Higher CognitiveFunctions: Methods, research and future directions. In J. H. Hannay(Ed.), Handbook 2f Neuropsvcholo-v. Oxford Press, 1983, In press.

C. M Presentations, 1, -. Gvin

1. Symposium Chairman, American Association for the Advancementof Science,Washington, D.C., 1982.

2. Symposium Chairman, Winter Conference on Brain Research,Steamboat Springs, 1982.

3. Special Invited Lecturer, Int. Conf. Neuropsychology,Pittsburgt 1982.

4. Invited Speaker, Society for Biological Psychiatry, New York,1983.

5. Symposium Chairman, IEEE Computer Des., New York, 1983.

6# Invited Lecturer, Third European EEG Conference, Basle, 1983.

1NE

° -- ef I-

__________

Page 9: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

1

D. Experiments

The EEGSL is in the process of developing the method of NeurocognitivePattern (NCP) Analysis for measuring aspects of mass neural processesrelated to perceptuomotor and cognitive activities. Severalgenerations of NCP Analysis have been used to study both complex andsimple tasks, and a number of findings have emerged. Taken together,these results suggest that neither strictly localizationist norequipotentialist views of neurocognitive functioning are realistic.Since even simple tasks are associated with a rapidly shifting mosaicof focal scalp-recorded patterns, neurocognitive functioning might bebetter modeled as a network in which the activity of many specializedlocal processing elements is periodically integrated. Our research isdirected toward developing methods for measuring these processes moreprecisely and modeling them more explicitly.

N.B. It must be understood that scalp-recorded potentials, evenunaveraged timeseries, are not necessarily cortical in origin. Untilthis issue is settled, it is essential not to interpret scalpdesignations, which conventionally refer to underlying cortical areas,as implying measurement of the activity of cortical sources. Forconvenience, we use the convential scalp designations subject to thiscaveat.

Specific findings include:

1. Complex perceptuomotor and cognitive activities such asreading and writing have unique, spatially differentiated scalp EEGspectral patterns. These patterns had sufficient specificity toidentify the type of task from the EEG ( US Caino Neurophvsiol.47:693-703, 1979). The results were in accord with previous reportsof hemispheric lateralization of 'spatial' and "linguistic'processing.

2. When tasks are controlled for stimulus, response andperformance-related factors, complex cognitive activities such asarithmetic, letter substitution and mental block rotation haveidentical, spatially diffuse EEG spectral scalp distributions.Compared with staring at a dot, such tasks had approximately 10%reductions in alpha and beta band spectral intensities (EjU CainoNeurophvsiol. 47' 704-7109 1979; Science 203:665-668, 1979)o Thisreduction may be an index of their task workload. Since no patternsof hemispheric lateralization were found, this study suggested thatprevious reports of EEG hemispheric lateralization may have confoundedEEG patterns related to limb and eye movements and arousal with thoseof mental activity w Ue (Scienc 207:1005-10089 1980).

3. Split-second visuomotor tasks, controlled so that only thetype of judgment varied, are associated with complex, rapidly shiftingpatterns of single-trial, evoked inter-electrode correlation of brainpotential timeseries. Differences between spatial and numeric

2

/K

Page 10: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

judgments were evident in the task-cued prestimulus interval. Complexand often lateralized patterns of difference shifted with split-secondrapidity from stimulus onset to just prior to response, at which timethere was no difference between spatial and numeric tasks (Science213:918-922, 1981). This suggested that once task-specific

differential perceptual and cognitive processing was completed, amotor program common to both tasks was executed, regardless ofdifferences in the stimuli or type of judgment.

4. Rapidly shifting, focal brain potential patterns,representing the maximal difference between similar split-secondtasks, can be extracted with NCP analysis. The move and no-movevariants of a split-second visuospatial judgment task, which differedslightly in expectation, differed in type of judgient, and differedgreatly in response, were associated with distinct differences in thepatterns of single-trial evoked correlation between scalp-recordedchannels (Science 220:97-99, 1983; see Sections III and IV). Thesepatterns of difference increased in magnitude in each successiveanalysis interval. In the prestimulus interval, correlation of themidline frontal electrode distinguished the tasks (p<.01). In theinterval spanning the N1, P2 and N2 event-related potential (ERP)peaks, the between-task evoked correlation contrast was focused at themidline parietal electrode (p<.001). In the interval centered on theP3a ERP peak, the focus of correlation difference was at the rightparietal electrode and involved higher correlation of the rightparietal with occipital and midline precentral electrodes in theno-move ta2, and with the right central electrode in the move task(p<5 x 10 -). In an interval centered 135 asec after the P3a ERPpeak, which included right-handed response preparation and initiationtthe focus of contrast shifted to the left central electrode, involvinghigher correlation with midline frontal and occipital electrodes inthe move task a:0 with the midline parietal electrode in the no-movetask p<5 x 10 ). These results concur with neyWopsychologicalmodels of these tasks derived from clinical observations. Theysuggest that although simple perceptuomotor tasks are associated witha complex, dynamic mosaic of brain electrical patterns, it is possibleto isolate foci of maximal differences between tasks. It is clearthat without a split-second temporal resolution it is not possible toisolate the rapid shift in lateralization which presumably isassociated with perceptual-cognitive and efferent processing stages.

5. The focal patterns of evoked correlation derived by NCPanalysis significantly distinguished the single-trial date of 7 of the9 people in the above study. This suggested that similar

Sneurocognitive mechanisms ware being measured across the majority ofparticipants (see Section IV)#

6. Behaviorally identical trials of the move and no-movevisuospatial tasks in the above study were found to be associated withdistinctly different brain potential patterns (Section VI). Thissuggests that appropriate brain potential measures may provide a toolfor sore detailed examination of previously unmeasured neurocognitiveprocesses.

3

_______________________

L f-/

Page 11: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

E. analytic Methods

Neurocognitive Pattern (NCP) analysis currently consists of theapplication of an adaptive-network, nonlinear mathematical patternclassification algorithm to extract task-related signals from sets of

data. The analysis is applied to single-trial timeseries in brieftime windows (100 to 175 msec) for up to 49 scalp electrodes (usingpower measures) and up to 1176 electrode pairings (usingcrosscorrelation or crosscovariance measures). The data windows aredetermined for each person from the peaks of their averaged ERPs aswell as from stimulus and response times, but measures are made onsingle trials.

1. Similarities gnd Differences Between NCP . nalsii maUConventional ERP Analysis

NCP analysis is grounded on the vast body of information gained fromERP methods and has the same underlying goal, namely to resolvespatially and temporally overlapping, task-related mass neuralprocesses. However, it departs in several ways from the currentlypopular approach of extracting independent features from averaged ERPsby principal components analysis (PCA) followed by hypothesis testingwith ANOVA. First, NCP analysis is concerned with spatiotemporaltask-related activity recorded by many electrodes in a number of timeintervals from before the stimulus through the response. Itquantifies neurocognitive activity in terms of a variety ofparameters, rather than amplitude and latency of ERP components.Thus, it is possible that the increased dimensionality ofparametrization may facilitate the measurement of subtler aspects ofneurocognitive processes. Second, the questionable assumption of amultivariate normal distribution of brain potentials is not made inNCP analysis. Third, brain-potential feature extraction andhypothesis testing are performed as a single process which determines Ifeatures which are maximally different between the conditions of anexperiment, rather than those which meet possibly irrelevant criteriasuch as statistical independence. Fourth, task-related patterns ofconsistency are extracted from sets of single-trial data. Significantresults say be obtained as long as there is a pattern of consistentdifference between tasks? even though the means of the two data setsdo not differ significantly.

Taken together, these aspects of NCP analysis may enable it to resolvesmall task-related signals from the obscuring background 'noise' ofthe brain, revealing useful spatiotemporal information about massneural processes. However, this is not without its costs. NCPanalysis requires several orders of magnitude more computing than PCAand ANOVA, and larger data sets than conventional ERP studies. Also,because of its sensitivity, highly controlled experimental paradigmsare required to assure that the results are truly related to thehypothesis and not to spurious or idiosyncratic factors. (The processof developing one such task is described in Section 11 of this

report.) This requires a greater allocation of effort and resourcesto experimental design? recording and analysis than is needed for most

4

...........L

Page 12: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

ERP experiments.

Although we hove obtained several promising results with NCP analysis,the latest of which is described in Sections III and IV, we mustcaution that 'the jury is still out'. Additional basic studies areneeded to determine whether NCP analysis is really worthwhile. If so,it should be possible to optimizer standardize and simplify it for usein other laboratories.

2, Eyoke Correlations Between Scalp Electrodes

For the past few years we have concentrated on a measure of the degreeof waveshape similarity (crosscorrelation) between timeseries frompairs of electrodes. Measures of single channel power are also beingused and will be reported later this year. The crosscorrelationapproach is based on the (unproven) hypothesis that when areas of thebrain are functionally related there is a consistent pattern ofwaveshape similarity between them. There are a number ofconsiderations in interpreting the correlation patterns of scalp

recordings, such as volume conduction from subcortical sources anddriving by distant sources. Some of the ambiguities may be mitigatedby careful experimental design, but the neurophysiologicalinterpretation of correlation patterns is an unsettled issue*

Besides the scientific value of studying the neural activity

associated with preparation to receive and the subsequent processing

of numeric information in two sense modalitiesp the auditory-visualexperiment described in this Interim Progress Report is designed toprovide a data base for refining the NCP analysis and investigatingsome aspects of the neurophysiological interpretation of correlationpatterns. In addition to inter-channel, zero-log correlation, MCPanalysis can employ other measures such as multi-lagged correlation

bands# Preliminary studies described in this report (Section V) haverevealed significant information with such measures. A major goalduring the coming year is to explore and resolve some of these issues.

1I. PITIN QE M AUDITORY-VISUAL PERCEPTUOIOTOR T

A. Din Considerations

A number of issues had to be addressed in designing a bimodal paradigmsufficiently controlled to reveal NCPs which might distinguishauditory end visual perceptuomotor tasks during the modality-cuedprestimulus and post-stimulus intervals4

1. QglJL Considyrations

Two hypotheses are being tested. First, NCPs should showneuroanatosically interpretable differences between the processing ofauditory and visual numeric stimuli in post-stimulus intervals whenfeature extraction is thought to occur in sensory and related corticalareas. There should be minimel diffterences after sense-specific

.5

I , I = I I m---

,;*'y.

Page 13: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

processing is completed. Second, NCPs should differ in themodality-cued prestimulus interval as a function of the preparation toreceive either visual or auditory stimuli. The second hypothesisrequires complete equivalence of cue properties andperformance-related factors between auditory and visual conditions,and also a strong inference that a modality-specific expectancy setexists in the cued prestimulus interval.

2. Experimental Control

The first hypothesis (ie. post-stimulus processing) requires controlof stimulus, response and performance-related factors acrossconditions so that the major difference between conditions is stimulusmodality, Visual and auditory modalities have fundamentaldifferences. Input for the former is parallel (for brief foveallypresented stimuli), while for the latter it is serial. Inherentdifferences in auditory and visual processing latencies can becompensated for by centering several analysis windows on the averageERP peak latencies in each person for auditory and visual conditionsseparately. Differences in ERP amplitudes can be explicitly measuredby NCP analysis of single-channel signal power. With regard to thesecond hypothesis (ie. prestimulus attentional set), the modality-cuedparadigm elicits a contingent negative variation (CNV) between cue andstimulus, with consequent resolution after stimulus presentation. NCPanalysis will be applied to measure pre- and post-stimulus modalitydifferences. The results may shed some light on the interface betweenpreparatory activity and post-stimulus processes.

3. Montage

A 21 electrode scalp montage suitable for recording activity overauditorl visual, motor, parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal corticeswas used during these pilot recordings. Four additional channelsrecorded vertical and horizontal EOG, EMG and the response. The finaldesign will expand this to 49 brain potential channels to allow aresolution of approximately 6 square centimeters (see section II.C.).The investigation and conclusions concerning these issues during thedevelopmental program of 12 pilot recordings is reported in thefollowing section.

B. Task development and pilot recordings

1. Phas one (Ps #1-8): (Reson f2 miscues)

- . Rationale, The existence of a modality-specificprestimulus attentional set was investigated with the "miscueing'technique (Posner, 1978). In this method a randomly ordered 20% ofthe modality cues (a visually presented letter in both conditions) areincorrect. The lengthening of mean response time in these miscuedtrials is considered the 'cost' incurred by the expectation of astimulus in a specific modality, and is used to infer the existence ofa modality-specific preparatory set.

b. II.. Stimulus presentation and response measurement6

/ L

Page 14: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

were performed by the real time subsystem of the ADIEEG system, whichalso digitized the 25 channels of physiological signals at 256samples/sec. The participant (P) was instructed to fixate a point atthe center of the CRT screen of an AED II graphics terminal and awaita visually presented modality cue (V for visual; A for auditory;duration 375 msec) 1.5 sec later the stimulus was presented*Auditory stimuli consisted of the numbers 1 to 9 generated by a Votraxspeech synthesizer and presented through two speakers about 2 ft.above the participant's head. Duration varied from 245 to 430 msec(the number 7 was generated as "SEVN'). Visual stimuli were singledigit numbers presented on the CRT, subtending a visual angle of underone degree. Their duration was equated to that of their correspondingauditory numeric stimuli.

The participant was instructed to attend the modality cue and focushis attention" on the speakers or screen as indicated by the cue,while maintaining his gaze on the screen. He was to respond to thestimulus without hesitation with a ballistic contraction of his righthand index finger on a modified Grass force transducer with a pressurecorresponding to the stimulus number on a linear scale of 1 to 9.Feedback indicating the exact pressure applied was presented as a 2digit number on the CRT screen (duration 375 msec) I second aftercompletion of response as determined by the program.

If the response was sufficiently accurate, the feedback number wasunderlined, signifying a 'win'. The error tolerance for a "win' wascontinually adjusted throughout the session as a moving average of theaccuracy of the preceeding five correctly cued trials in visual andauditory modalities separately.

A random 18Z of the trials were miscued tie. the stimulus .arrived inthe wrong modality). The participant was to respond to these justlike the correctly cued trials. Correctly cued and miscued auditoryand visual trials were presented in randomly ordered blocks of 17trials, self-initiated by the participant.

c# Recordings. Eight normal, right-handed adult maleparticipants were recorded in pilot sessions consisting of about 350to 700 trials each. Three were personal of the EEGSL and 5 were naivepaid participants. Details were settled during the first 6recordings, and technically acceptable recordings were obtained fromthe last 2 people. The electrode montage was Fz, F7, Fg, aF1, aF2,Cz, C3, C4, C, C6, Pz, P3, P4, T3, T4, T5, T6, OzP aOl and a02,referenced to linked mastoids (modified expanded 10-20 systemnomenclature; Picton, et alp 1978). For PO8 several placements werechanged (see section B.2). Vertical and horizontal eye movements,response muscle activity, and force transducer output were also

recorded. All signals were amplified by a 64-channel BioelectricSystems Model AS-64P amplifier with .10 to 100 Hz possband,continuously digitized to 11 bits at 256 samples/see and stored ondigital tape. Signals were also recorded on three 8 channelpolygraphs to monitor the session and for off-line artifact editing.Average response times (RT) and error rate (proportion of 'lose"trials) were computed for behavioral evidence of a 'cost' due to a

7

IV* 1 - ~ r - ~ - __ ____ ____ ___

Page 15: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

prestimulus attentional set in a total data set of 3735 trials.

d. Results and Dscussion. For correctly cued trials, themean response times were quicker for visual stimuli for all but twopeople. Average RT across P's was 675 %sec for visual stimuli and 711msec for auditory. The longer RT for auditory stimuli, which isopposite to the findings in simpler bimodal paradigms, may be due tothe nature of the verbally presented number stimuli. All informationneeded for visual stimulus decoding appeared on the screen within 33msec, while auditory information was not completed for up to severalhundred msec. The longer RT for the auditory stim,,li may also be dueto the use of synthesized speech stimuli.

In the miscued trials a lengthening of RT and increase in error ratewas observed in almost every case (Tables 1 and 2). For miscuedauditory stimuli the average increase in RT was 47 msec and theaverage increase in error rate (proportion of 'lose' trials) was 9%.For misuced visual stimuli the 'costs' of miscueing were slightlygreater (increase in RT - 65 msec; increase in error rate a 10%).There was a small (18 msec) asymmetry in RT effect. That is, miscueinga visual stimulus caused a greater increase in RT than miscueing anauditory stimulus. This asymmetry and the magnitude of the RTlergthening in miscued trials is in agreement with previous studiesusing simpler tasks (Posner, 1979). Further, the standard deviationsfor all cued and miscued conditions were similar within persons,indicating that the RT 'costs' due to miscueing were based on aconsistent effect rather than greater variability in a smaller sample(about 4 to 1 ratio of correctly vs. incorrectly cued trials.) Thusit was verified that there was a 'cost' in the miscued trialsindicative of an attentional committment to a particular modality inthe prestimulus interval.

The ERPs for one person (P#7) are illustrated in Figure 1. In theauditory condition (Figures la and b) t.he Ni peak occurred at 120 msecin correctly cued trials and 136 msec in miscued trials. Itsamplitude was maximal at midline fronto-central sites. The P2 peak at208 msec was maximal at midline fronto-central sites, and was wellrepresented at midline parietal sites. In miscued auditory trials(Figure ib) a P3 peak was apparent at 314 msec, maximal at the midline.parietal derivation, and extending to fronto-central, lateral centraland lateral parietal electrodes.

The visual stimuli elicited an N1 peak at 150 msec in both correctlycued and miscued conditions (Figures Ic and d). Although in thisperson the Ni peak was clearly seen at the midline occipitalelectrode, it was larger over left anterior occipital and posteriortemporal sites (aOl and T5). The P2 peak was maximal at Pz, where itslatency was 215 msec, but at midline fronto-central sites the firstpositive peak occurred at 179 msec. This earlier positive peak atanterior sites (which was also observed in PO8) was actually theresolution of the prestimulus CNV, which in the auditory condition wasmasked by the fronto-central N1 peak. In the miscued visual trials

(Figure 1d) a P3 peak was present at 300 assc in P47 and 330 masc inPOS. It was slightly larger at Cz than Pz, and was also prominent at

8ti*

Page 16: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

nidline frontal and lateral central and pavietal electrodes, Ihe P3peak was of larger amplitude, broader, and more anteriorly distributed:r the visual miscued trials than in, the auditory miscued trials.

rhe pre-stimulus CMV is evident as" a fronto-central negatiedisplacement of the prestimulus baseline in both conditions and itsresolution could occur at different latencies after visual and verbalstimuli. Also, the information content of the verbal stimuli occurredat various latencies (as in "six" and 'seven', or 'four* and 'five',as compared to the other numbers). What effect this may have upon thelatencies of endogenous ERP components is not known, but it is likelyt.;at greater variability of the P3 latency in the auditory conditionmay account for the smaller, lower amplitude peak. rhese issues willbe addressed in analyses of later, more controlled data sets.

A negative going slow potential shift was observed to commence at theP2 latency in all but the miscued visual trials, where it may havebeen obscured by the robust P3 peak. It was maximal at Cz, ano largerover the left central (C3) than the right central (C4) electrode.This lateralization began well before the average response times inPach condition.

2, Phase two (P's #9-12): (No response to miscues)

a, Rationale.

1he behavioral results of phase one confirmed the existence of aprestimulus attentional set, and the respond-to-miscues design wasmodified for the formal recordings to a move/no-move design in whichthe 'response' to a miscued stimulus was to make no movement. Thiswas done so that there would be a behavioral confirmation of attentionto the cue in each trial? and so that post-stimulus processing could.e examined in each modality separately by a within-modality, move vsno-move NCP analysis. The within-modality analyses will serve ast.Fandards to aid in interpreting the results of the between-modalitlyanalyses,

b lask. The task was the same as before: except thatthe par.ticipant was instructed to make no response on miscued trials(random 20%). A monetary incentive was added by rewarding accurate(win) move trials as a function of the accuracy attained (about 5cents for each win). The accrued monetary bonus was displayed at theord of each block of 17 trials, as well as the average error tolerancefor that block (as a performance index) for auditory and visuali.onditions separately, The cue-to-stimulus intervals were 2.5 sec forP's #9-11, and 1 sec for P#12. For P#12 a separate run was recorded.t the 2.5 sec interval for the visual modality only.

Cs Recordings. Four normal, right-handed adult malesparticipated in sessions consisting of about 100 piractice ard 400 tes:ttrials. The 21 channel montage conisted of Fz, F', FS? aF1, aF'.6Cz, Cz- C3: C4, CS? C6, Pz, P3, P4, eP5, aP6. T5? 16f Oz? aOl ernda02, referenced to linked mastoids, rin scalp electrodes wer-at ached v.s ing a stretchable nylon ca, 'Electvocap Inte%-netionpl).i9

.=________,__, .,___ __,_,.____-___-___=_ _______ ___-

Page 17: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

I

Vertical and horizontal eye movements (electrode at outer canthi) andresponse muscle potentials (EMG from flexor digitorum) were recordedwith Ag-ASC1 cup electrodes. Other aspects of th recording procedurewere the same as in Phase one.

Trials were visually inspected off-line to eliminate artifacted trialsand no-move trials where muscle activity was present on the EMGchannel. Average stimulus-registered ERPs were computed for all scalpchannels, as well as averages of vertical and horizontal EOG and EMG.Cue-registered averages were also computed for P#12 for thecue-to-stimulus interval (1. sec for this P); the averaging epochextended from 250 msec before the cue to 250 msec after the stimulus.

d. Result and Discussion, Response timer error rate(proportion of "losem trials), and average error tolerance in the movetrials for the 4 participants are shown in Table 3. Average responsetimes and error rates were similar across move conditions, but theadaptive error tolerance (inversely related to skill level) tended tobe larger for the auditory condition, indicating that performance wasslightly better for visual stimuli. We will attempt to eliminate thisdifference in the formal recordings by providing more practice at theauditory task.

At the 2,5 sec cue-to-stimulus interval (P's #9-11), there was a highrate of data attrition (about 25Z) in the no-move (miscued) trials dueto mistaken overt responses. This may have been due to a decay of theattentiornal set. At the 1 sec interval (P#12) there was no attritiondue to response movements. Since the no-move trials are infrequent,the attrition ,ate must be very low to collect sufficient data forthe within-modality move vs no-move NCP analysis. Thus the I secondinterval seems more desirable.

The stimulus-registered ERPs for the move trials (Fig. 2a and c) weresimilar to the correctly cued trials of Phase one. In the visualcondition the N1 peak was poorly represented at the midline occipitalelectrode, and in P#12 at the lateral occipital placements also (Fig.2c). In all P's (including those in Phase one) the N1 peak to visualstimuli was largest at the lateral posterior temporal sites (T5 andT6). This may have been due to the small visual angle (under 1degree) subtended by the foval stimuli.

In P's #9, 10 and 12 the no-move (miscued) trials elicited an* augmentation of the P3 peak. The P3 peak amplitude in both move and

no-move averages was maximal at midline fronto-central sites.Examination of vertical and horizontal EOG channels revealed no eyemovement which would account for this anterior distribution; thus itwas most likely due to the resolution of a prestimulus CNV, an issueto be addressed in the formal study.

A frontally-dominant negative displacement of the pro-stimulusbaseline (CNV) was seen in all participants, In a cued paradigm suchas this a CNV may well be concomitant to the expectancy set we wish tostudy (reviewed by Tecce, 1972). The cue-registered averages for the1 sec interval (P#12) revealed the time-course of the prestimulus CNV

10/

Page 18: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

to correctly cued auditory and visual stimuli. The negativity waslargest at the midline fronto-central electrodes, but did not seem todiffer between modalities. NCP analysis of the cue-to-stimulusinterval in the formal study may shed more light on themodality-specificity of the anatomical distribution of CNV-relatedactivity, which has not been thoroughly examined (Ritter, et alp

1990).

C, Final Paradigm Screening qf Participants

As of 1 JUN 83, one fell recording has been made. 1000 trials wererecorded from a well-practiced participant using the paradigm?electrode montage, and recording procedure described above. Thecue-to-stimulus interval was 1 sec, the visual stimuli were thickenedand increased in size to just under a 2 degree visual angler and theproportion of miscued (no-go) trials was increased to 22%. ERPs fromthis recording were quite satisfactory in most respects.

This paradigm, and a 49 channel scalp montage (Figure 3) will be usedin the formal experiment# A common average reference will becomputed, and the passband will be .1 to 50 Hz with digitization at128 Hz. A recent study of trimodal attentional set using the regionalcerebral blood flow technique (Roland, 1982) has revealed that thespatial patterns of focal neural processes related to attention andmodality processing are complex even when viewed with a 30 sectemporal resolution. In order to adequately sample brain potentialswhich may correspond to these regions of focal activation, densercoverage of dorsolateral prefrontal areas, superior and inferiorposterior parietal and superior and posterior temporal areas isrequired.

A screening program is being conducted to select and train candidateparticipants. During screening sessions EEGs are recorded from Fz,Cz, Pz, aOl and a02 (to examine the ERP waveform), and Ti and T2 (toassess potentials from temporalis muscles). Vertical and horizontaleye-movements are recorded by a single pair of diagonally placedelectrodes. Behavioral records are examined to assess the quality oftask performance, polygraphs are inspected to determine the amount ofdata attrition due to artifact, and average ERPs are computed toverify the presence of expected peaks. Participants for the formalstudy will be drawn from these candidates. To date (1 JUN 83) 17candidates have been screened in this manner, 6-8 of whom will be

4recalled for the formal experiment.p

11

Page 19: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

D. Bibliographv

Picton, T., Woods, 0., Stuss, 0, and Campbell, K. Methodology andmeaning of human evoked potential scalp distributed studies. Proc.4th Intl. Cong. gn Event Related Potentials of the BraHendersonville, N.C., EPA-60019-77-043, 1978, 515-522.

Posner, M. Chronometric Explorations p Mind. Lawrence ErlbaumAssoc., N.J., 1978.

Ritter, 1., Rotkin, L. and Vaughan, H. The modality specificity ofthe slow negative wave. Psychophys., 1980, 17:3;222-227.

Roland, P. Cortical regulation of selective attention in man. J.Neurophvs,, 1982, 48:5, 1059-1078.

Tecce. J. Contingent negative variation (CNV) and psychologicalprocesses in man. Psych ull., 1972, 77:2;73-108.

*12

.4

12'

J ..: a

)z

Page 20: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Tabl1 - Average response time (in msec) and standard deviation forcorrectly cued and miscued auditory and visual trials for the8 participants recorded in phase one of task development.(Miscues total 18Z of trials; 9Z of each type).

Auditory Visual Increase Increasecorrectly mis- Correctly mis- in R.T. in R.Tcued cued cued cued miscued miscued(S.D.) (S#.D) (SoD) -(S.D.) auditory visual

#1 318 830 803 710 757 -27 msec. +47 *sec(198) (171). (213) (221)

#2 368 702 803 648 796. +101 +148(112) (124) (120) (116)

#3 406 574 632 473 539 +58 +66(81) (89) (93) (93)

*4 609 687 718 578 652 +31 +74(140) (198) (140) (120)

*5 606 850 865 722 737 +15 +15(136) (124) (140) (116)

06 668 528 625 603 664 +97 +61(39) (54) (294) (39) (54) (54)

#7 372 892 978 1052 1106 +86 +54(43) (58) (58) (19)

#8 388 626 636 609 667 +10 +58(95) (81) (110) (80)

x(S.Dacross 711 758 675 740 +47 *sec +65persons) (106) (111) (116) (103) msec

Totaltrials

3735 1560 302 1565 308

13

i ,_

• • m mm m m mmmm m mmm mmm~ l lIN& T TT I

Page 21: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Table - Error rate (Z of glosem trials) in correctly cued andmiscued auditory and visual conditions in phase one of taskdevelopment. (Miscued trials = 18%).

j Trial AuditorLo Visual Loe Auditorycorrect mis- correct miscued increase increasecued cued cued in lose % in lose %

*1 318 57 53 45 80 -4 +35

*2 368 53 57 48 69 +4 +21

03 406 50 63 55 53 +13 -2

#4 609 51 69 59 65 +18 +6

05 606 52 61 54 58 +9 +4

,6 668 49 58 54 53 +9 -1

#7 372 47 66 49 59 +19 +10

#8 388 51 53 53 59 +2 +6

X 3735 51% 60% 52% 62% +9 +10(Total)

1

.

°I 14

_L _ - -. . -_-- _ . -.. .... . _

S f/.... ... ... : .. .... . ... ... .. .. . . . .. ....... ... ... ..... ..... .... .... a = 5 . . . . . --

Page 22: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Table ] Response times, error rate (Z lose trials) and error tolerance(inversely related to skill level) for phase two (Ps #9-12),move/no-move design,

P_# T Average RT in Error LW veraqe errorasec (S.D.) tolerance (S.D.)

audi- visual auditory visual audi- visualtory tory

#9 374 844 846 52% 50% 10.6 6.4(218) (219) (4,0) (2.1)

#10 370 795 797 53% 47% 6.7 5.8(190) (182) (2.4) (2.5)

#11 340 1008 957 55% 59% 8.2 7.5(225) (279) (3.1) (3.1)

#12 410 636 651 54% 51% 6.2 5.6(85) (62) (2.4) (3.8)

------------------------------------------------------------------------------------(Total)1494

X 821 813 53.5% 51.8% 7.9 6.3(S..) (180) (186) (3.0) (2*9)

across

persons

15

15t

/-, . r I II 4 " • "

Page 23: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

'FZ .. •;:;T3 •;;;* o A S I

011 ."t..$.:. a. . .: .... 'A 00 0 o ....*., ao

, -, ,-T 4 ,m e- "*zb a . .. .a *so a ...

O a a a 0 o -l. 0 a

a a -A-

*a 40S

*' . , , a- - " "

.. ..6... .. ..foC4 c n fr - .1 to o H,16 , ,

-.. a. ,. ..... ....

- .. . ** .** .b.. eab..

* a 6 0 a

foCs6ete*cane: 0oz#7 9 ;ras 1t 0 i as~d

16 -a-- . n a , l l i a i 11 a _ _ - - d m ' -

Page 24: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

;FZ :T4,l;L ; " ; ;

3 , T3* . ^, . , .. 9

;q. " ,/i-%q -I--*. -/-- - ..... , .....

• a

a s e e 0 0 4 0 0 0 0 0M:C : :- '''

0 a * I 0

* I t

• I _" I t

Fiur I Audtor mice codiio wit response (p77ras

1 7

* I

* -0 0.4.-* r,- *' . . , . 2

* a Il ,," =*V I Sql

* I

* I I •

a I S•, -

J* •I 9,AiWIIll~l

* 0 I a * a

• * I a • I

SStimulus 500 usec Stimulus 500 amec

i Figure lb - Aadtory aiscued condition with response (p* 7 , 27 triLal8)

J j 17

_ _ t 7 -

Page 25: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

1AO10 0o a a 0 0O

* a.aiu

a .A a a

a0 - 0 0 0

0 0

:P oa a lb 0

.A 10 0 0 es

stiauaus we*pgr c-Vsa corcl cud anRti saetd00= p7 9 ras

Page 26: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

:FZ :- -

* a s 0 0 S

* a

;CZ Omao . . . ... . . . oH U I oe

:=u : : :mean R

S0

, :o a -,l,,o v ,

o % e a o OA 0 40 ak

4C4S 5

a I•

A I deia. .1 9

* a ao * 0 a

O O a

lr-*.- .a'" a'1 a- - - -.. a,* 6 V t

opZ .o .

* -- a e a S a ~ ~ gW 0 =

O -- o• e • e | oe

o -- a a o o BJ i

* al

IO * a

' ' kli. lr.-'- . S~~u.500 mc

a• a l l SI

* 6. 6

* S 0 0 0

Stiuulus 500 n35ec

715ure id - Visual miscued coidit ion with rp se(p#?, 31 trials)

'C -T ---- - ______---- /-

-J l

'a I, l- Il i i m i

Page 27: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

w 0 a .0 wo .. 0 . .b .

1*0u, .

0 o * 0C.. ;,,:,,-.... '. - . a * a

1 - " . -. . . go .: . .... . .

is a It O

P4

C6 O @,

* * a,

10, . a.

Stmls 50 scStluu 5 asec

10 *z ** baa * a

20a

-- -.. . ... . . .: .

C4a a aAP6 : .

a" ",1, da a

. .l) #

i*el o a

oS

I

-C. .. 7 -a "r awr. a * "T. aa , , . : a

..... .G"- .0 -*e,-= i 0 . *

SStiulus " 00 use Stimulus " 500 msec

~Figure 2a - Auditor'y correctly cued (move) condition. Averatle event-related

t potentials from selected channels from p112 (103 trials, .1 to

. 100 lHz pasabend).

*

20 Ii

Page 28: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

oeP0 aa a a a

0.a00 . 0 00 0DO 6 0 a. $.a .

- 's 41

a 40000 0 a we~u 0r

a a *. 00 a f

4 StSl s 0 s c tm l s5 0 m t

I L

Page 29: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

S S C3:

* a.. .. . . . a'. * * ... * . a a* 'e *'°° 't"°"e 'r"°ooo a e ••o

a a man i

O- O 0 0

Sta~us50 necSt0.4u 50 se

0 a 0 da 00a 0D O M s

a£ f a 0 a

- -7,u cnit 1 a

• i 2 2

| r r * . . . . -

Page 30: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

.C3

a . . s .0 s:

.o .T .. . Seaso0 0 a

T5 , H# ' a-5 , lr ,..0.aas

WPa a a

a 0 a a &a a

a a

..- ..... o¥ ...........

* a

* a al••Stm u 50 u 'e Stiulu 500.. sec

Fiue2 iulmscen-oe odton. (02 3til)

23. _ _ _ _ I.. . '.'..: .

Page 31: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

0)7 *F5 0 F3 OFi *Fz SF2 *F4 *F6 *PS

2C 5 *aC3 qaCi .aCz *aC2 *aC4 41aC6

*T3 * C5 * C3 em1 *Cz *C2 *C4 6 OT

sap 5 *aP3 OaPi eafz * aP2 * aP4 *aP6

*T5 * p5 e*P3 e P1 *Pz * P2 0 P4 Oft * T6

0 a03 *aOl * aOz 0a2qO

Figure 3 -49 channel recording montage

24

Page 32: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

SECTION III

SerieSCIENCEI April 1983, Volume 220, pp. 97-99

Shadows of Thought: Shifting Lateralization of HumanBrain Electrical Patterns During Brief Visuomotor Task

Alan S. Gevins, Robert E. Schafer, Joseph C. Doyle, Brian A. Cutillo, Robert S. Tannehill,and Steven L. Bressler

Copyright 0 1963 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science

25

Page 33: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

togs have uadopbtedly contributed toconfictn reports of lateralizatioa ofbrain activty.

To observe the spatial patterns andsequencin of meurocognitive activity.we have developed a new method calledmetuwcognitive pattern (NCP) analysis.Is NC? analysis the average ERP's ofeach persom ae used to determine thetime intervals of task-related neuralprocesses. Within these intervals thesimilarity o(brain-potential waveshapes

trial basis by computing tecross-corre-latint coegcient between paired corabi-atiom; ofelectrodes. Although the neur-anatomic ouigin said neuroiphysioiogicalsignillansce of these correlations is notknown, it has been suggested that cogni-tive activity may be associated withcharacteristic scalp correlation patterns(9). However. task-related electrical sigemis from the brain are spatially smeareda trassesasion to the scalp and are em-bedded in background activity. Sincehear statistical methods were not elfec-tive in dealin with these obstacles, weused a more powerful analysis calledtrainaible classification-network mathe-minhca pater recognition (2.3J. 10-13).For this method. Wartfcia iteiligence 1algorin are used to extract patterns ofcorrelation that dMa lOetween two con-ditions with no assumptions about thedistribution of correlation values. Thealgorithm is first applied to labeled

Simam o flo~gt S~ngLateailadoa . H... . subset of the experimental data calledSbadws f Tbugb: Shlltng O H the training set, and the invariant pat-Brain Electrlc Patterns During Brief Vlsumomotor-Task terns (classification functions) found are'

then verified on a separate unlabeleAbstract. Dynamic spatial patterns of correlation of electrical potentials recorded subset of data called the test set. If the

from the human brain were shown in diagrams generated by mathematical pattern classification functions can significantlyrecognition. The patterns!., "Osove" and "no-move" variants of a br isuospa- separat the test set into the two conditad task were compared. lon the Interval spanning the P300 peak of the evoked tos, the extracted patterns have intrn-potential, higher correlations of the right parietal electrode with occipial and central sic validity.electrodes distinguished the no-move task from the move task. In the next interval. Previously we reported the existencespanning the readiness potential in the move task, higher correlations of the left of complex, rapidly changing patterncentral electrode with occipital and fronital electrodes characterized the move task. of brain-potential correlation involvingThese results conform to nearopsychological expectations of localized processing many areas of both hemispheres; thatand their temporal sequence. The rapid change in the side and site of localized distinguished numeri and spatial judg-processes may account for conflicting reports of lateralization in studies which meats in a visuomotor task (13). Sincelacked adequate spatial and temporal resolution, the sequencing of neurocognitive difer-

ences between numeri and spatial proc.Many investigators have reported that components of averaged event-related essing is not definitely known, the comn-

brin aciiylaterslized duin og potal F:P') may indicatehe se- plx*atternts were duickto intepet

ods reveal relative localization an ae-cse.te aentrvae oss-clarify this situation by highlighting pre-aization. u antrsletmoa eat, robust sign of biteralization. even sumably localized neural processes. Insequencing because of the long time re- for language. (7). Conclusions derived comparing two types of spatial judg-quired for observation. Studies of on- from patients with focal brain lesions or meat, the common activity of brain areasgoing. background electrical activity do with "split-brains." cannot be directly should cancel, revealing dillaernce innot reveal split-second changes in neuro- extended to normal subjects. Lateralized the right parietal area presumed to medi-cognitive patterns. and those that have processes inoerred hao reaction time ate spatial Judgmsents. The right-handed-reported lateralization of neurocognitive difierences to hemifleld or dichotic stim- finger response in one task was designedactivity have been questioned on meth- ulation have also been questioned on to elicit lateralized activity of th leModololeal pounds (1-6). Although the methodofogial pounds (8). These foc- central moto area.

1A93J155326

* 1 Z

Page 34: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

In this study a person estimated the atins within persons, 0.24). Brain po- vas spamed the NI00-P200 and P300distance a "target" should be moved to tentials were recorded from 15 scalp ERP peaks, and the third (RP) intervalintersect a displayed arrow's trajectory. electrodes and referenced to linked mas- spamed most of the readiness patentialThe "move" task required pressure of toids (Fig. IA) (15). Vertical and hori- (in the move task). The centerpoint ofthe right index finger on a transducer zontal eye movements, muscle poten- each interval was determined for eachwith a force proportional to that distance tials from the responding fngr, and the person (18). The correlations were stan-(14). In the "no-move" task the arrow output of the force transducer were also dardized within persons, within elec-pointed directly at the target, and no recorded. The dat were edited to re- trade pain (mean, 0; standard deviation,pressing was required (pseudorandom 20 move trials with artifacts, and a set of 1), and then grouped across people. Thepercent of trials). Thus, the spatial judg- 1612 correct, representative trials (839 tasts and ANOVA's of single-trial cor-meat and response differed between move, 773 no-move) was formed. Aver- relations did not distinguih meaningfultasks, while gross stimulus characteris- aged ERP's were computed for all eec- differences in between-task spatiotempo-tics were the same. trodes (Fig. IB), and mests and analyses nI patterns.

Nine right-handed, healthy adults of variance (ANOVA's) were performed Mathematical pattern classification(eight males, one female) participated in (16,17). was then applied to the single-trial corre-the study. The average response initia- Cross-correlations were computed be- lations of all nine people to search fortion (muscle potential onset) time for the tween 91 paired combinations of the MS subtle between-task differences in eachmove trials was 0.59 second (standard electrodes for each trial in each of three interval. To make the results anatomical-deviation, 0.19; mean of standard devi- 17-msec intervals (Fig. ID). Two inter- ly interpretable, we performed the

search separately on each of 15 sets ofelectrode pairs. Each set consisted of the

FW g. l(A) Montage of A C correlations of a particular electrode15 electrodes. Non- with ten ohu electrodes (Fig. IC). Forsandard placents each interval, the electrode set that dis-

are intended to over- F. tet e wtlie cortical areas of Pa tig hed conditions on the test set withpicu r intrest a- Co the highest significance level (19), andteo occipital (Oy), . :c the most prominent correlations for thataterior Paiep (PS). T3 CS C4 U electrode set (20). were diagramed.(leor e e-C), s . In the NIO(-P200 interval. correla-andmidgei prnoto 0 Was of the mxdmn parietal electrode 4(Ca) ares. (B) Coo- distinguished the tasks (P < .001) (Fig.posite IIs, e.m- 2A). In the P300 interval, correlations ofrelated potentials the right parie electrode with the mid-(ERPs) from fourpseons (5 percent of line occipital and precentral electrodest.e total data from hove (01 tltf.) Hen oe04 "MM) were greater in the no-move task, whileam persn) for the PsP correlations of the right parietal with thePz electrode, showing right central electrode were greater indte m UorP peaks -Spy V . the move task (P < 5 x I0- 1) (Fig. 28).

i corrdation In the RP interval, corTelions of the left

Analysis intevals. central electrode with the midline frontalMwe P30 ERP peak is If Ps and occipital electrodes were greater intherminhe- : ~the move task, while correlations of thequint no-move trials. tler~ o a" a " e f(C) One of the 1 sets often electrode pars inowhhthe91 pairedi cnuis sweseft centrl electrode with themidlineThe anterior occipital (Oy) set is shown. In Fm. 2 the pincipal electrodes of differin sets we parietal electrode were greater in the no-cirded mad the most prominent coelations ae indicated as solid ad dotted as, move task (P < 5 x 10') (Fig. 2C).

The right parietal locus of between-task difference in the P300 interval mayreflect, a lateralization of activity distin-A B C s the two tens of patia dg-

*F7 ,Fz ment (21) or the difference between•emovement sotinon in the move task

J4 C4 e C3 no-move task. The left central focus ofT3 %,% U ", dilerence in the RP interval 135 msec

Zl tero may rellect the preparation andinitiation of the movement of the fight

0Y Oy index &uer. In contrast, the pattern of9 3 ) dierence in the NIO0-PM interval was

P4..001 P-aX1 1 P< -x 10 " notlateralid.NIOO-P200 Interval: P300 Interval: RP Interval: Thsa results may help explain con-

140 to 324 mee 302 to 477 meoc 436 to O11 meec lUcti repots of brain-pottial laterali-zation. In many studies, various "ver-Fig. 2. Digrams of between-task differences in the (A) N100-P2. (3) P3W, and (C) RP al-anlytic" and "spatial' tasks I nun

intervals enerat by neuroconitive patters (NCP) analysis. The m sigaiicantly dififing or in hveelectrode sts. their signifiance level, and the most prominent correlations within the set usshown. A solid ine between two electrodes indicates that the correlations were higher in the ared with relatve lef and rut hemi-move task, while a dotted line indicates higher no-move task correlations. sphere EEG activity (1-61. However. it

27 oSCIENCE. VOL. no

27 - -I- - II - -in

Page 35: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

is m ler hetethis actvity am.so-d in semGavin aat~42)V2 M26 311);. V3 tiM. 4821. V4 (210.

oo topogepmlbic difirences in EG c -I ~ .LS TaIUX J. ff.....m . & ulu it w nve A inn m o, r-otra between IS-second antirc, block CAkrun. L Yage. Scim 23. gig e dmen wo om, s as the 'm .me

(1961). to the keyfor lawse 3. ofn thepe @Mst .i ow ~ etie arottin nd etersubtiuton ubaf P - dsuod gve heeq mo So anilmek stAWO omu-M %eio mcurmy win

terriroisl cntolingotertha-cg. CidS. wil P <. IV'9 Aid hie boon onpue. A m-an ..~a usimsuoneoiv fatask cann. otwev~ler.1. nt . ~the viled s. SSt adsm Corepod d h .W Pikm .1 deuil11

shv.fatos 2-).Hoevrsuch bet- 110 She u1S Stmi a ' hin of in" n~ am S3 jimilt 168n ao

serial cvOnents reflecting difiereflt ISete. fo ISlin did. ftpe. warosi L.bdi.3 m puan Gon ah amndy

anurcognitive processes. We therefore an s Gene benmwi &M Womoter mi 1 2m Of -I Purn. aM acmY Ihil coudrefined our approach by uising short (less SO=~i e Dmi aee 0. 1d WU& Am IMWS 'byem cte wsP.3 acodi.

WWtni cndo ofh au w.oodIk.A ie 10e WAOM sim wl- 32 aik-than I second) tasks, using tim refer- 00 'NUS e 11 I SW 2 Mrom&, by 6;_09 su0o d. Cm emli beueR in

ePMceasueonts coptn Corn met -m~um -il aoso at mmc t

does between channels on a singletria "Boo wM 41 pembe of mama L to SO H&. Woosl mum"n as eind * vm was at thebos. aif sin nvwmaxW attrn bNoodigm ad mcs peseuds we sinc bvd fr niy we of on em spk.

au~e~ Giese -od 6ut Winm w .10. vh~ ,d opm f dstoadwag "ae Sur Theire deam andi.3 Was Per-3oM11sltscn usp- dpu Sm dMi toH I-pi ine We. atn ato am p%,is dV)

quendial processing. This yiede a se- p~nm er popun adw a r 1 i. as6d of hiam 1M is thqijence of char-cu between-task differ. 16 ~l~e)'2.T ds .M nsat teeas saP4r fence pattern involving split-second .a tekefetP. 6) -0. Pc 0021 sat! ftMmimm vtrn.3 heachan in the localization end lateraliza. M wci 2R3.06) - .9. wcSUyie =a"dsCeuidy-6i11 Of nMRss neweal activity. Appropriate IVe umwineaisn. tM WHOM -toske feo-r am

but thr imge heINa IM adoes orstudies of neurocognitive functions t he am IU~U S P < .001 orshould take Into account this rapidly < dewed.. Ws at he mea ris cab phki nbe.Sm ov

shifin network of localized an -a inn by the 6aeter in .meuza op y Me a eam ofalze procsses U~ omlv ak). J. omne@* [hoc. Nedl.

"ie- (P. C 30 R peak dap- iemo ine the ERIP owee triowALAN S. Gzvfl5 lueIersm .6 v . ee kulosoe win boonyet hedima - M-----i-__t. but

Roa E.S.H.. (R. Sinus H. iieeswm d wet un proa@" inthROSEUT ~ ~ ~ ~ a E. awU.~Iii gcoespheg. can. pFMaLllote'ii

JoaUM C. DOYLE 'NemwVPAk7e 4L US 0V7)1 Md wf h e M 22. We MIe oidB Link Ho Me. "hdBRIAN A. Cuinuo Cedo o ano at~sti uoi oi n of wshangIN sons atwe. 0oo 0or

Roaa S. TANNEHILL Mmu NrnrSPMW i I r*to oi am fo "nmm TA6mI-Sizvure L. flaLa 17. tu"-dwnds n-ero mlis a vvi- amiWy-Rka. P . AE. M. mws.larP.

EEG Systems Labortory, of t dlo" of a *81 Nun flle~t 1. 9. aClet"l. W. ner-c. E. NW-Ey.

1855 Pouso treet, 'w- e' dif eetu. *A? inuevre. John. 3. Uin. J. Moniasm M. MNmMV

Sea Frwaw~co. CaWwia 941W. <:- d EleICI (I1. 112) -1.9. J. Spin. H. Voau.. .0. Waiter. C. Woods.teakyeloufod inteamtion adt J. Vitel h1r veindit couen J. mow(FI.12) - 2., P < .001). Comolei mini (Air Fer SodW of Aetaspame Mention). D.

Mik we bl IMM ih end: d~sfo i oo Wowud (Cffis of bave Seunsi). A.010 A~t Snik duffmvere Pmed (Air PCis ONIs otScieftific IMeset),

1. 1. Donehm. M. Knlew. 0. NsCutiy. is Later. P c .00)a wes a3 tM he m deiWe. J~p. Poo d Beco miHfins fo .afted" e hi th Nervw s yinen. 3 umda Wi Mnlsole.So slc"eev -Wnui sePPOutSt.. LB. (Asmieses Proas Now York. IVM. Meie stmlams at P <.05 ml. 9Nvehr 12

2.*r 394..3It The MIWM.P00 en Pne iseSemi. 19 Nwob reaut I Fdeery 1632.A.ovies. 0. Zolch. C. Yk~Aft 3. Dople. N.Doks. I. Winter. 3. Somon. C. Yeps

Wes1ro .ph Caln. Neoepysi. 47.0m

3. A. 5. Gievioe. 0. N. ZONEh. J. C. Doyle. C. D. 11M Ila. mao svwan"IM wvaer (Viewts 15) contea several lae positive peeks.

Y.A. c$wdmc 215. L1M3 R Cie. P42 Is larger I the freuen aI~m i als. mhe P3o0 aalysis Interval was estered4. A. OelUt 13. UI9M.C ' it aev P31 tie earlIest pmose peek to abos a sigificat betvmma-task dIllfere

S. A. S.oevims 3. C. Domis.. X P. Ir. g. mi at tie anterior midlinm peuietal siecizuta (VO.0005). glams P391 eat 1530Cfiil.C. Yager. Schiue 3W. 1006 (190S. onager In1 tia Infreqent so Sav trials. ent P391 folloed a meessIve tifferesce

6. A. oin.n CeveOrgW e Waimy asek WU) at 20 ansc. MI9 end P330 could be teelgite 1Piaen 73 b acordingNaed. llneeY Mod ItJ. d to tie convntion of squires. at &I (DS fba .%I ~ I637-0

1973). IM hi M 120 aterwslot theme fin as r 76Interva.. as nou7. D. Fo L W.u 3it~* 1. Asis pv ostdy of Similar teaics (ovinm. at al. $@U=. 213:916-22. 1961). Thes" ik 13 respornse pmepemmlon (ff) Iterwal. Centered 133 use s te 1w300 interval, aleo().1) C. ..t. hum. per- opens the P3uo push of tie so-sove trials.

COP. 06(No. 1). 3 Wm Ihetd W.

1110 -PAS.CMd sryiF. 3 0f9Mh;

L ving,. ft oupdttl Cere.b1ue9M

N.J oig.BL C 2973): 3. CelmaAsy md20Aedom. Any TePai. Apmwe. Masgo.lite. U-.036 (OSS:S gau. aAaLe.'uini si P Ytr eor 1,,. 3.nedin ea. as . Lb. Amrme s epa. cn.

Yo.iR.29710 . . S -lO) 1. (14)10.. ivs IBM rust. Pas. dMarklow

Ims.e 33(1410 WebS. V1011odaswin Aabe-im@* ad K. F.3 ft U deidc Pbet( A.w

Gevils. 3. 05gb. R. SIM&. S. Codes. ft.

Too". 5. Nieele. seirearep0elog. C11e.

Inn API IM

S 21

Page 36: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Section IV

NEUROCOGNITIVE PATTERN ANALYSIS OF A VISUOMOTOR TASK: LOW-FREQUENCYEVOKED CORRELATIONS

Alan S. Gevins, Joseph C. Doyle, Brian A. Cutillo, Robert E. Schaffer,Robert S. Tannehill, Steven L. Bressler,

EEG SYSTEMS LABORATORY1855 Folsom

San Francisco, CA 94103415-621-8343

Note to colleagues: This manuscript will shortly be submitted forpublication. We would appreciate receiving your cricit*l commentsand suggestions. Thanks.

I

i 29II

Page 37: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

ABSTRACT

Spatial patterns of single-trial evoked correlations of humanscalp-recorded brain potentials were determined by applyingNeurocognitive Pattern (NCP) analysis to data from nine adultsperforming a visuospatial task. Mathematical pattern recognition wasused to determine the differences in the spatial patterns ofcorrelation of *move' and 'no-sove* trials in successive 175-msecintervals. The magnitude of the patterns of difference between tasksincreased in each successive interval. In the prestioulus interval,correlation of the midline frontal electrode with lateral central andleft temporal electrodes was greater for the no-move task, while itscorrelation with the left parietal electrode was greater for the sovetask (p<.01). In the interval spanning the NI, P2 and N2event-related potential (ERP) peaks, the between-task contrast wasfocused at the midline parietal electrode and involved. highercorrelation of that electrode with lateral temporal and midlineprecentral electrodes in the move task, and with the left frontal(F7) electrode in the no-move task (p<.001). In the intervalcentered on the P3a peak, the focus of correlation difference was atthe right parietal electrode and involved higher correlation of theright parietal with occipital and midline precentral electrodes inthe no-moveiask, and with the right central electrode in the movetask (p<5x1O ). In the interval centered 135 msec after the P3a ERPpeak, and which included the right-handed response preparation andinitiation, the major focus of contrast shifted to the left centralelectrode, involving higher correlation of that electrode withmidline frontal and occipital electrodes in the move task,_gnd withthe midline parietal electrode in the no-move task (p<SxlO ). Inseven of the nine participants, the group equations significantlydistinguished the tasks. Move and no-move trials which werebehaviorally correct, but which were misclassified by the algorithmshowed high prestiiulus alpha activity in the averages, and hadpost-stimulus waveform morphologies intermediate between correctlyclassified move and no-move types. Although the neurophysiologicalsignificance of these patterns of evoked correlation is unknown, theresults are consistent with the observation in humans and primatesthat simple visuospatial tasks involve the integration ofspatially-distributed activity in many neural areas.

1

/

Page 38: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

INTRODUCTION

Neurocognitive Pattern'*(NCP) analysis is a method of measuring thefunctional topography of human scalp-recorded brain potentials duringgoal directed activity. It involves application of mathematicalpattern recognition to measures of inter-electrode correlations ofsingle-trial evoked brain potentials. Here we report the measurementof rapidly shifting, focal patterns of correlation which distinguishtwo variants of a brief 'move/no-movea visuospatial task.

It has been proposed that task-specific neural processes manifestpatterns of waveshape similarity (crosscorrelation) of low-frequencymacropotentials (Dumenko, 1970; Livanov, 1977). A number of studieshave approached this issue with scalp-recorded EEGs (Wolter andShipton, 1951; Brazier and Casby, 1952; Callaway and Harris, 1974;Busk and Galbraith, 1975; Livanov, 1977), but this hypothesis remainsunproven due to problems of experimental design and lack ofmethodology for precise measurement of task-related correlationpatterns at the scalp.

Any test of the hypothesis that waveshape similarity amongscalp-recorded brain potentials reflects task-related processing inunderlying neural populations must meet several methodologicalcriteria. First, the functional relationships of specific areas mustbe explicitly manipulated. Well established "landmarks' such assensory, @association' and motor areas must be used as anatomicreference points in the experimental design, and the scalpprojections of the presumed generators must be considered. Second,the experiment must be rigorously controlled for stimulus, cognitive,performance and response-related factors to allow unambiguousassociation of experimental manipulations with spatiotemporalelectrical patterns. Third, a high degree of temporal resolution isrequired, since the neural processes involved in brief cognitivetasks last only a fraction of a second. Fourth, measures must bemade on single-trial EEG timeseries rather then averages since theexact timing of neurocognitive processes may vary from trial totrial. Fifth, the analytic method must be able to extract smalltask-related signals from the obscuring effects of backgroundactivity and volume conduction.

Our first study employing NCP analysis (Gevins, et al, 1981) revealedcomplex, rapidly changing patterns of evoked correlation whichinvolved many areas of both hemispheres which differed betweennumeric and spatial judgments performed an equivalent stimuli.Howeverp the complex patterns were difficult to interpret since thesequencing of neurocognitive activity in numeric and spatialjudgments is not definitively known. The present study was designedto clarify this situation by highlighting presumably localized neuralprocesses. In comparing the move and no-move variants of a spatialjudgment task. the common activity of brain areas should cancel,revealing focal differences in visual and parietal areas presumed tomediate visual discrimination and spatial judgments. The

31

i . . _

...... . i i ~ I I iIJ

Page 39: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

right-handed response in the 'move' task should elicit lateralizedactivity of the left central motor area.

METHODS

Task &U rotocol

The participant (P) was seated in an acoustically dampened recordingchamber with right-hand index finger resting on a force transducer.Stimuli were presented on a Tektronix graphics terminal and subtendeda visual angle of less than 2 degrees horizontally and vertically.They consisted of an arrow originating at center screen and avertical line segment (the 'target') to one side (Fig. 1). Thetarget's vertical position and side of screen changed randomly acrossboth move and no-move trials, as did the angle and direction of thearrow. The arrow's angle varied from 0 to 30 degrees from thehorizontal, and target size ranged from 2 to 36 am (see below).Stimuli remained on the screen until feedback was presented. On movetrials the participant was to estimate the distance the target mustbe moved so that the arrow's trajectory would intersect its center,and apply a pressure proportional to that distance with a ballisticcontraction of the right index finger. Responses were made on aGrass isometric force transducer with maximum 1mm travel at a forcerate of 1 kg/mms The required force varied randomly from .1 to 1 kg.On 'no-move' trials the arrow and target were oriented so that thearrow's trajectory would intersect the center of the target, and nomovement was to be made (Fig. 1).

Trials occurred in blocks of 13 or 17. The blocks wereself-initiated by the participant and lasted about 1.5 min. Theno-move trials constituted 20Z of the total number of trials and werepresented in semi-random order such that the first two trials of ablock were always move trials, and a no-move trial was alwaysfollowed by a move trial. Each trial consisted of a warning symbolfollowed after .2 sac by the stimulus. One second after completion ofresponse in the move task, feedback indicating the response pressurewas presented for I sec. Feedback for no-move trials was presented3.5 sec. post-stimulus. The inter-trial interval was 1.8 sec.

Two factors were included to reduce the automatization of taskperformance. First, at the start of each block of trials the gain ofthe response transducer was switched between 2 levels of sensitivity,requiring the participant to adjust his responses between 2pressure/distance scales. Second, the target automatically shrank orlengthened (from 2 to 36 mm) for both move and no-move trials as anon-line function of accuracy in the previous 5 move trials, Thus taskdifficulty was continually adjusted to match each person's currentperformance level.

Recordings

Nine right-handed adults (M, IF) were recorded. The first five werehealthy students and professionals, ages 20 to 35, who received about

32

Page 40: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

50 practice trials before performing the tasks during 2.5 hour

recording sessions. The last four were highly skilled aircraftpilots who had several hundred practice trials and who performed alarge number of trials in 6 hour recording sessions.

Brain potentials were recorded from 15 scalp electrodes andreferenced to linked mastoids (Fig. 2a). The montage includedseveral non-standard midline placements intended to overlie corticalareas of particular interest: 'a0z' (anterior occipital), "aPz'(anterior parietal), 'aCz' (precentral), and "pFz' (anterior motor).The first five Ps' brain potentials were amplified by two BeckmanAccutraces with .16 to 50 Hz passband; for the other four aBioelectric Systems Model AS-64P amplifier with .10 to 50 Hz passbandwas use . Vertical and horizontal eye-movement potentials(electrodes at outer canthi and above and below one orbit),response-muscle potentials (flexor digitorum), and responsetransducer output were amplified by a Grass Model 6 with .30 to 70 Hzpassband. All signals were low-pass filtered at 50 Hz (40 dB/octaverolloff) and digitized to 11 bits at 128 samples/sec.

Software System

The ADIEEG, integrated software system, was used for all aspects ofthe experiment (Gevins and Yeager, 1972; Gevins, et alp 1975, 1979a,1981, f983b). This system performs real-time control of experimentsand behavioral and physiological data collection; allows automaticon-line modification of experimental parameters as a function of taskperformance; has a flexible database structure and integrated datapath for the recording and analysis of up to 56 physiologicalchannels; allows selection and control of the stimulus, response andperformance-related variables used to aggregate trials into datasets; performs digital filtering and timeseries analysis of EEGs andERPs; and tests hypotheses with linear univariate and multivariateanalyses and mathematical pattern recognition.

Formation of Data Set

Polygraph records were edited off-line to eliminate trials withevidence of eye movement in the EOG channels, or muscle orinstrumental artifacts in the EEG channels, from 0.5 sec before thestimulus to 0.5 sec after response initiation. The total set of 1612trials (839 move, 773 no-move) submitted to analysis consisted of 69to 350 behaviorally correct trials from each of the 9 participants(Table 1). Correct move trials were those in which the participant'sresponse was ballistic, was completed by 1.5 sec after stimulusonset, and was not greatly goff target'. Correct no-move trialswere those in which no EMG was evident in response to the 'no-move'stimulus configurations. There was no difference between the two datasets in the stimulus parameters of arrow angle and side of screen ofthe arrow and target, since these parameters wore randomized by theprogram. Target size was balanced between move and no-move trials.The set of move trials had representative distributions of responsevariables including response initiation time, accuracy, pressure,duration, and velocity. Response initiation was determined by the

33

A. .. ....... .. .... _ I I ;I

Page 41: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

beginning of the average EHG burst of the right index finger's flexordigitorus. Thus move and no-move tasks differed slightly inexpectancy and stimulus configuration, differed in the decision based

on spatial judgment, and differed greatly in type and difficulty ofresponse.

Averag ERPs

Average ERPs for all channels were computed for each person in orderto determine centerpoints of time intervals for NCP analysis.Amplitudes of the major ERP peaks were measured from a 500-msecprestimulus baseline. N1 was the first major negative deflection,maximal posteriorly. P2 was the immediately succeeding positivedeflection, maximal at the anterior parietal electrode. P3a and P3bwere the first and second positive peaks enhanced in the infrequentno-move task and maximal at parietal electrodes. The immediatelysucceeding negative potential shift (in the move trials) was measuredas the slope of a straight line fitted to the ERP in the 175-msecinterval centered 135 mseo after the P3a peak.

iI Evoked Trial Correlations

After applying a phase-preserving, nonrecursive digital lowpassfilter (3 dB amplitude point at 12 Hz) to the single-trialtimeseries, crosscorrelations between pairs of electrodes werecomputed according to the formula:

N N N

N ~XY- X

N2 sx Sy

where X and Y are the sampled voltages of channels x and y at N timepoints, and sx, s their standard deviations. A Fisher's z'transformation xwas Ythen applied to each correlation value.Correlations were computed for each of the 1612 trials in each of 4analysis intervals for 91 of the 105 possible pairwise combinationsof electrodes (Fig. 2b). 14 pairs which were non-homologous orclosely spaced were excluded due to computational limitations.

Since the major ERP peaks indicate the average latencies of distincttask-related processes, the centerpoint locations of three of thefour 175 msec analysis intervals were determined from the peaklatencies of the average ERP (Fig. 3). This was done separately for

each person to account for individual variations. The first intervalwas the 175 msec epoch preceding the stimulus. The second intervalstradled each person's N1-P2 peak complex, and the third was centeredon the Pgo peaky which was the first positive peak to show a betweentask difference. The fourth interval was centered 135 msec after theP3a peak and spanned a portion of the response preparation (RP) inthe move trials and the P3b peak in the no-move trials. (An NCPanalysis synchronized to the movement onset will be reported

elsewhere). 334

Page 42: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

To equalize the scale of correlation values across people, the Fisherz'-transformed correlations were converted to standard scores withineach person's data in each interval (x=O, s=1) and then groupedacross people. ANOVAs and t-tests were performed on the single-trialcorrelations to determine task-related differences observable bylinear statistical methods.

Use of Mathematical Pattern Recognition for Spatiotemporal Analysis

The analysis of between-task differences in spatial patterns ofevoked correlation was performed with nonlinear,distribution-independent, trainable classification-networkmathematical pattern recognition (Viglione, 1970; Gevins, 1980;Gevins, et al, 1979a, 1981, 1983ab). This method is similar inpurpose to stepwise discriminant analysis, but uses a moresophisticated algorithm to search for combinations of variables whichdistinguish the data of two conditions of an experiment. The searchis conducted on a task-labeled portion of the data, called thetraining set? and then the extracted patterns of difference(classification equations) are verified on the remaining unlabeleddata, called the test set. If these classification equations cansignificantly divide the test set into the two conditions, theextracted patterns cart be said to have intrinsic validity.

To avoid spurious results, the sensitivity of this method requiresthat the experimental conditions be highly balanced for all factorsnot related to the intended manipulations (Gevins and Schaffer, 1980;Gevins, et al, 1980, 1983b; Gevins 1980; 1983ab), and that the ratioof observations to variables be on the order of 20 to 1 or more. Thevariables submitted to analysis should be grouped (constrained)according to neuroanatomical and neurophysiological criteria so thatinterpretable results may be obtained (Gevins, et alp 1979ac, 1981,1983ab; Gevins 1980). In this study temporal constraints consistedof locating the analysis intervals according to the major peaks ofeach person's average ERP. Anatomical constraints were applied byforming sets consisting of the correlations of each of the 15 scalpelectrodes (called a principal electrode) with 10 other electrodes(Fig. 2c). (To reduce the amount of computation, 4 of the 14possible pairings were excluded from each set. These involvedelectrodes adjacent to the principal electrode, or pairings nearlyredundant with others.) Midline sets were symmetrical, and lateralsets were mirror images of each other.

Classification equations. A separate classification equation wascomputed for each of the 15 electrode sets in each analysis intervalfor each task-labeled training set. Each classification equationconsisted of a linear combination of the binary decisions of 1 to 6discriminant functions. Each discriminant function consisted of alinear combination of 6 correlations selected by the algorithm fromthe 10 electrode-pair correlations of an electrode set.

35

Page 43: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

A recursive procedure was used to develop each classificationequation. First, 15 discriminant functions were computed (thisnumber was set by computer limitations), and the best was retained asa binary output (move or no-move) times a coefficient weighted foroptimum classification performance by minimization of an exponentialloss function. This process was repeated 6 times; the bestdiscriminant function from each new set of 15 was added to theevolving classification equation, and the weights assigned to eachwere updated. After each pass, the training data were re-weighted

inversely to the classification effectiveness of the classificationequation? so that the next pass would concentrate on the incorrectlyclassified data. In this way a classification equation whichoptimally partitioned the training data set into move and no-movetasks was formed.

Training ia testing (validation) 4j"t sets. The data set of 1612trials was partitioned into 3 non-overlapping test (validation) sets.For each test set, the remaining two-thirds of the data served as itstraining set. This rotation of training and testing sets reducedsampling error due to test-set selection.

A separate classification equation was formed using each of the 3training sets. Then the classification accuracy of each of the 3equations for each interval was measured on its corresponding testset, and the average test-set classification accuracy was determined.

S i gnificance leyel 2t classification. Since our aim was todetermine task-related spatiotemporal patterns, rather than topredict behavior, the analysis was constrained to facilitate aneuroanatomically and neurophysiologically meaningful interpretation.Thus classification accuracies were not as high as they would havebeen without constraints. To determine the significance levels of theclassification accuracies it was necessary to determine a baselinesignificance level and safeguard against a Type 1 error. To do this,equations were formed from sets of randomly task-labeled data foreach analysis interval. The average classification accuracy of 48such random-labeled studies was 50.6%, with a standard deviation of1.1%. This could have occurred by chance with pm.32, according to thenormal-curve approximation to the binomial distribution. Actualtest-set classification accuracies of552.9%v 53.9,_-4.9% and 55.5%correspond to p<.01, p<.OO1, p<5 x 10 and p<5 x 10 respectively.These significance levels were used as an index of the relativeconsistency of differences between move and no-move tasks.

Diagras 2f classification equations. In order to illustrate thestrongest between-task differences, diagrams were drawn showing theprincipal electrode and the electrode pairings which contributed mostto the classification function for the most significant electrode setin each interval. These 'prominent' evoked correlations weredetermined by applying the pattern recognition procedure recursivelyto the most significant electrode set. Each discriminant function(combination of correlations) whose weight was more than 0.1 timesthat of the maximum weighted function was retained on each pass.

36 i1

Page 44: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Within the selected discriminant functions, those correlations whoseweight was more than .25 times the highest weighted correlation wereretained. The selected correlations were weighted by the number ofdiscriminant functions remaining in the classification equation, andsummed over the 3 test sets. The 5 highest weighted correlationswere then input to the pattern classifier. If 'test-set'classification for a Siven interval was still significant at p<.O1,the entire procedure was repeated with the least significantcorrelation removed until a classification function incorporating aminimum set of 3 or 4 'prominent correlations' was produced.

RESULTS

Average ERP Description

The average ER? waveforms from Ps 06-9 (Fig. 4) consisted of aposteriorly maximum negative peak (N163) and a centro-parietallymaximum positive peak (P230) in both tasks. In the move task therewere parietally maximum positive peaks at 425 and 500 msec, followedby a centrally maximum, left-lateralized neSative-going slowpotential shift. In the no-move task a positive peak was observed at391 msec, maximal at the anterior parietal electrode (aPz), anotherat 425 msec and a third at 530 msec, both maximal at the midlineparietal electrode (Pz). Subtraction ERPs (Fig. 5) showed that theP391 peak in the infrequent no-move task immediately follows anegative peak (N2) at 240 msec, and thus may be the probabilitysensitive P3a peak (Squires, et al, 1977). The larger amplitude ofP425 in the move task may be due to the atypical experimentalparadiSm, in which a difficult response is required to the frequenttask-related stimuli. P530 in the infrequent no-move task maycorrespond to the P3b peak observed in So/no-go paradigms and toinfrequent task-related stimuli. Peak latencies, the correspondingNCP analysis intervals, and response initiation times for each personare given in Table 1.

ANOVAs and t-tests were performed for the P391 (P3a) peak amplitudeand the slope of the immediately succeeding slow negative potentialshift. For the P391 peak, a task x electrode x person ANOVA revealeda siSnificant task effect (F(1,B) = 29.0, p<<.001) and task xelectrode interaction (F(13,104) a 2.9, p<.005), but no electrodeeffect (F(13,104) = 1.2, N.S.). Correlated t-tests revealed

gsignificant voltage enhancements in the no-move task for all but thelateral temporal electrodes, the most significant effect being at themidline anterior parietal electrode (aPz) (p<.O005) (Table 2). WhenBonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons, only the aPz, Pz and

tC4 electrodes remained significant (I a 4.35 for p<#05). Meanamplitudes across persons at aPz were .1 uV and 2.3 uV for move andno-move tasks, respectively#

A task x electrode x person ANOVA of the slope of a straight linefitted to the slow potential shift in the response preparation (RP)interval revealed a significant task effect (F(1,8) z 5.6, p<.05),electrode effect (F(14,112) - 1.9, p<.05), and task x electrode

37

T.

Page 45: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

interaction (F(14, 112) = 2.7, p<.OO3). Correlated t-tests showedsignificantly larger move-task slopes for 9 electrodes (Table 3).The most significant difference (p<.005) was at the C3 electrode,where the mean slope values were .24 and -.50 for move and no-movetasks, respectively. When Bonferroni-corrected for multiplecomparisonsi no electrode remained significant (t = 4.35 for p<.05).

Liar Analysii of Evoked Correlations

Mean evoked correlation values over persons and electrode pairs were,for the move trials: prestimulus interval a .64, N1-P2 interval -.65, P3a interval a .65, RP interval - .65; and for the no-movetrials: prestimulus = .65, N1-P2 = .65, P3a a .65, and RP - .64.t-tests of differences in single-trial correlations between taskswere performed for the 91 electrode-pair correlations (Table 4). WhenSonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons only the F7-T3 andFe-Pz pairs in the RP interval reached significance (t- 3.58 forp<.05). Without Bonferroni correction, correlations significant atp<.05 or better were found in every interval. In the prestimulusinterval 5 of the 9 significant electrode pairs included the Fzelectrode. In the N1-P2 interval the 4 significant pairs all includedparietal sites. In the P3a interval the 6 significant pairs werefronto-central, with the exception of the P4-C4 pair. In the RPinterval the 25 significant pairs were widely distributed, but 8included Fz, 9 included F8, and 5 included C3.

Pattern ReconitioD Analysis If Si3-Trial Evoked Correlations

Pattern recognition analysis revealed patterns of difference inevoked correlation which increased in magnitude in each successiveinterval. The principal electrode and prominent correlations of themost significant electrode set in each interval are shown in Figure6. In the prestimulus interval there was a weak between-taskdifference of the Fz electrode-set (p<.01), involving higherprominent correlations of Fz with P3 in the move task and highercorrelations of Fz with T3, C3 and C4 in the no-move task.

In the N1-P2 interval the distinguishing significant difference wasin the Pz electrode set (p<.O1), with higher correlations of Pz withaCz, T3 and T4 in the move task, and higher correlations of Pz withF7 in the no-move task.

In the P3a interval th! most significant difference was in the P4electrode set (p<5 x 10 ), with higher correlations of P4 with C4 inthe move task, and higher correlations of P4 with aCz and aOz in theno-move task. At the p<.001 level the aOz electrode set alsodistinguished the tasks.

In the RP interval theost significant difference was in the C3electrode set (p<'5 x 10 )v with higher correlations of C3 with Fzand aOz in the move task, and higher correlations of C3 with Pz inthe no-move task. Four other electrode sets distinquished the tasksat- 4lower significance levels: C4 (p<l x 10 ), F7 and T3 (p<5 x10 ), and Pz (p<.001).

38

/ *

Page 46: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

For the prestimulus and N1-P2 intervals the reduced classificationfunctions required 4 "prominent correlations' to achieve significantclassification, while in the P3a and RP intervals only 3 were needed.Further, significant classification (p<.05) could be achieved withjust the first term (discriminant function) of the reducedclassification equation (Table 5).

To test the interperson validity of the results, the classificationaccuracies of the classification equations for the P4 electrode setin the P3a interval and the C3 set in the RP interval were assessedon the data of each person individually, and compared with theoverall classification accuracy (Table 6). The group equations werevalid for 7 of the 9 people. As a further test, the entire analysiswas performed on the data of one person (255 trials from P 7) forthe P3a interval. The P4 electrode set _gain achieved the highestclassification accuracy (59.4%; p<5 x 10-).

DISCUSSION

Neurophvsiological Significance of Task-Related Evoked Correlations

In theory, a task-related difference in evoked correlation betweentwo scalp electrodes could be due to one or more possible causes: 1)functional coordination of two distinct cortical populations? 2)driving by a third cortical or subcortical neural area, and 3)volume conducted activity from a distant generator. While it isif the task-related patterns of evoked correlation determined byNeurocognitive Pattern (NCP) analysis reflect functional coordinationbetween cortical (and possibly subcortical) areas, their anatomicaland temporal specificity suggests that significant aspects oftask-related neural processes are being measured. (A preliminary NCPAnalysis of single channel signal power determined significant, butweaker, between-task patterns of difference. Some of the significantelectrodes corresponded to those found with correlation measures.These results will be reported elsewhere.) However, the significanceof waveshape similarity in scalp-recorded brain potentials will notbe understood until further studies are completed.

NCP Anaisis, ERPs and Neuropsvcholog

In this section the main NCP results will be discussed in light ofprevious neuropsychological and electrophysiological (ERP) findings,showing how they concur with and elaborate the information obtainable

4 by those methods. Psychological interpretation of these results mustbe considered speculative, since the processing stages involved inthe task are not definitively known.

The magnitude of between-task difference increased from interval tointerval. The presence of a small significant effect in theprestimulus interval might be the result of a weak task-specificpreparatory set generated in the course of the session by the4 ordering of move and no-move trials. The locus of this difference inthe Fz electrode set is consistent with neuropsychological and

L 39

IT|

Page 47: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

electrophysiological (CNV) findings suggesting involvement ofprefrontal cortex in preparatory activity (Teuber, 1964; Walter,1967; Fuster, 1980). A previous NCP study (Gevins, et al, 1981) alsorevealed evidence of a task-specific preparatory set in the task-cuedprestimulus interval preceding numeric and spatial judgments. Theprominent correlations of Fz with T3P C3, C4 and P3 in the presentstudy suggest that this preparatory activity extends beyondprefrontal areas.

In the N1-P2 interval, correlations of the Pz electrode setdistinguished move and no-move tasks at p<.O01. Subtraction ERPsrevealed an enhancement of the N2 peak no-move trials in 6 of the 9participants (79% of the total data set) (Fig. 5). Its mean latencyof 240 asec. placed it near the center of the NI-P2 analysisinterval, and its amplitude was maximal (1.7 uv) at Pz. Thus thebetween-task correlation differences in this interval may be relatedto N2. Although an amplitude increase in the N2 peak in no-go trialsof a 3o/no-go paradigm with equiprobable conditions has been reported(Simson, et alp 1977), N2 has usually been reported to be sensitiveto infrequent changes in gross stimulus properties or patterns(Naatanen, et alp 1980). However, in the present study, stimuli wereequivalent between conditions in all respects, save that in no-movetrials the arrow pointed directly at the target in variousrandomly-ordered configurations. The N2 effect at 240 esec suggeststhat a no-move configuration has been identified by that timer andthat M2 may reflect a more subtle process than the detection of agross "mismatch' in stimulus characteristics, as indicated by otherrecent studies (Ritter, et al, 19S2). The prominent correlations ofPz with T3, F7, aCz, and T4 suggest that these processes are notconfined to the parietal area.

In the P3a interval (which was centered on the P3a peak andoverlapped a portion of the P3b peak) the right parietal (P4) locusof correlation differences (p<5 x 10 ) provides novel evidence forthe lateralization of neural processes related to these late positiveERP peaks. Although on the basis of lesion evidence, the rightparietal cortex is known to be necessary for such spatial judgments,the late positive ERP peaks have not been found to very inlateralization according to type of cognitive task (Donchin, et al,1977). J. Desmedt (1977) reported a relative right-sidedlateralization in the ERP in a spatial somatosensory-motor task, butthe effect was general and was not present in the P3 peak, nor wasits scalp distribution determined. A previous NCP study (Gevins, etal, 1981) demonstrated lateralized temporo-parietal evokedcorrelation differences between numeric and spatial judgments in theinterval centered on the P3a peak at 340 esec., but the intervalcentered on the P3b peak at 450 msec, exhibited bilateralbetween-task differences from frontal, central, and parietalelectrodes. In the present study, the between-task differences incorrelations of the right parietal electrode with central andoccipital electrodes is in accord with neuropsychologicalexpectations? as is the somewhat weaker effect in the aOz electrodeset. The lateralized NCP finding is in contrast with the anteriormidline parietal (aPz) locus of maximal amplitude difference of the

40

_7 -----------

Page 48: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

P3a ERP peak.

In the response preparation (RP) interval, centered 135 esec after

the P3a interval centerpoint, the focus of between-task difflrenceshifted to the left central (C3) electrode set (p<5 x 10 ),involving higher correlations of C3 with Fz and a0z in the move taskand with Pz in the no-move task. Since the RP interval overlappedEMG onset in a portion of the set of move trials (average responsetime - 590 msec, mean S.D. within persons = 240 msec.), the RPinterval results may also include a contribution from the outputactivity of motor cortex. The C4, F7, and T3 electrode sets, whichdiffered at lower significance levels, may also reflect movementpreparation and initiation, since the presumed generators ofvoluntary finger movements are buried in the lateral bank of thecentral sulcus and their scalp projection may be diffuse. The lesssignificant difference in the Pz electrode set may reflect concurrentprocesses related to P3b.

Rapidl Shifting Lateralization

The rapid (135 msec) shift in side and site of lateralization fromthe P3a to the RP interval may help clarify the controversysurrounding the existence of lateralization of brain potentials indifferent types of cognitive activity. Although various'verbal-analytic' and 'spatial' tasks lasting one minute or more havebeen associated with relative left and right hemisphere activity, itis not clear whether this is due to cognitive activity, or tostimulus, motor, or arousal-related aspects of the tasks (Donchin, etal, 1977; Gevins and Schaffer, 1980; Gevins, et alp 1980; Gevins,1983ab). In an earlier study (Gevins, at alp 1979abc), we firstfound prominent spatial differences, including lateralized patterningof EEG spectra, between one minute linguistic and spatial tasks(reading and writinS, Koh's Block Design and mental cubereconstruction). However, no spatial differences in EEG spectra werefound between similar 15 second tasks which were more controlled forother-than-cognitive factors. Since heterogeneous tasks composed ofmany component operations cannot be clearly resolved into serialprocesses, our subsequent study (Gevins, et al, 1981) refined theapproach. It used short (less than 1 second) visuomotor tasksdiffering only in type of judgment (numeric and spatial), employed175-msec analysis intervals based on person-specific ERPmeasurements, and used measures of between-channel correlations insingle trials as features for NCP Analysis. That study revealed thateven split-second judgments involve a complex, rapidly shiftingmosaic of task-related evoked correlation patterns involving manyelectrodes over both hemispheres. Thus, simplistic views ofneurocognitive processing may be the result of inadequate temporalresolution of rapidly changing neural activity.

The present study confirmed this by comparing move and no-movevariants of the same spatial task. Thv results suggest that the tasksinvolve split-second changes in the relative localization and

41

V.

Page 49: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

lateralization of neural activity. A dramatic switching of the fociof patterns of evoked correlations is seen as the stimulus isanticipated, perceived, judged, and a response executed. Theserapidly shifting patterns are consistent with network models ofhigher cognitive functions (Luriar 1977; Arbib and Caplan, 1979;Zurif 1980; Mesulam, 19810 and Gevins, 1981, 1983b). It should beunderstood that the simplicity of the patterns reported (Figure 6) isdue to the fact that only the most significant results werediagrammed. The inclusion of results at lower significance levelswould create more complex patterns, particularly in the RP interval.Further, in a separate within-task analysis, where each post-stimulusinterval was compared with its prestimulus interval, it was evidentthat within-task differences were complex and increased in magnitudeand anatomic distribution from interval to interval. This isconsistent with a within-task interlatency analysis reportedpreviously (Gevins, et al, 1981).

Individual Differences

Although the classification accuracies of the overall (multiperson)classification equations assessed on the data of the individualparticipants varied appreciably (Table 6), the existence of someinvariant task-related patterns in 7 of the 9 persons was confirmed.The fact that the significant difference between tasks was also found

at the P4 electrode set in the P3a interval when the data ofone-person was subjected to NCP analysis also supports the inferenceof patterns which are invariant across people. Moreover, anonparametric randomization test performed on the individual

classification accuracies of the two groups of P's (#1-5 and #6-9) f

confirmed that the classification equations did not significantlydiffer between the two groups.

t NCP Analysis Useful?

Analytic methodology is a critical factor in determining theprecision and relevance of results in brain potential studies. MCP

analysis uses modern signal processing and pattern recognitiontechnologies to distinguish spatially and temporally overlappingtask-related brain potential patterns. It builds on the vast body of

ERP research by using the average ERP to determine person-specific

time intervals during which successive stages of task-relatedprocessing may be assumed ito occur. It then searches the

single-trial, multichannel brain potential data with a mathematicalpattern classification algorithm to extract spatial patterns whichdistinguish the two conditions of an experiment. As with otheradvanced approaches (reviewed in McGillem, et al, 1981 and Gevins1980), it has the potential to reveal information not obtainable from

averaged waveforms. Further studies will determine whether NCP

analysis produces results meaningful enough to justify the large

amount of computation required.

42.. . . ... L .t/

7 .

Page 50: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

A full comparison of NCP analysis with linear multivariate methods is

beyond the scope of this paper. Two linear tests were performed togive some indication of the differences between methods: Post-ho

task x electrode-pair ANOVAs on selected variables, and the

Sonferroni-corrected t-tests on the full set of single-trial

correlations. The ANOVAs were limited to the 10 correlations of themost significant electrode sets determined NCP analysis: the P4 setin the P3a interval and the C3 set in the RP interval. Only theelectrode-pair effect reached significance (F(14,72) = 57.9p p<<.O01

and F(14,72) a 48.6, p<<.O01 respectively). There was no

significant task main effect or task x electrode-pair interaction.This result and the results of the t-tests (Table 4) suggest that the

variable subset selection and the nonlinear, distribution-independent

properties of the NCP Analysis were both important. This is

consistent with two previous studies where this type of mathematicalpattern recognition proved more effective than ANOVA and stepwise

linear discriminant analysis (Gevins, et al, 1979a; Lieb, et alp

1961). Although the Sonferroni-corrected t-tests were significantfor only two electrode pairs in one interval, at uncorrected

significance levels (p<.05 or better), the significant electrode

pairs did show a slight similarity to the NCP results. Of the

significant Fz pairs in the prestimulus interval, 3 are identical to

the prominent correlations determined by NCP Analysis (Fz-C3, Fz-C4

and Fz-T3), and the frontal distribution of significant pairs accords

with the distinguishing Fz electrode set in the NCP results. For the

N1-P2 and P3a intervals, however, only the T4-Pz electrode pair in

the former interval and the P4-C4 pair in the latter correspond to

prominent evoked correlations of the NCP analysis. In the RP

interval the t-tests were focused on the frontal areas and included

only two significant pairs from the NCP results (C3-Fz and C3-Pz).

In its present form, NCP Analysis seems able to extract patterns of

task-related evoked differences from the obscuring effects of volume

conduction and background EEG. Further research is being conducted

using measures of interchannel timing and single channel power in

paradigms involving manipulation of modality and responding hand.

These studies may help elucidate the significance of inter-electrode

evoked correlations accompanying neurocognitive processes.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

N. Currens for manuscript preparation and artwork, H. Vaughan, D.O.

Walter and C. Woods for critical reviews. E. Callaway of the Langley

Porter Institute' Do Woodward of the Office of Naval Research, A.

Fregly of the Air Force Office of Scientific Research' Jo Miller of

the Air Force School of Aerospace Medicine, and M. Brachman-Hoffman

for research support.

43

t -

Ai i

I _. ... . . . ... ..... ... .. ..

Page 51: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Arbib, M. and Caplan, D. Neurolinguistics must be computational.Behavioral la grain Science r 1979, Zr 449-483.

Brazier, M. and Casby, J. Crosscorrelation and autocorrelationstudies of electroencephalographic potentials. ElectroencephalorphvClinical NourophvsioloQv, 1952, 4, 201-211.

Busk, J. and Galbraith, G. EEG correlates of visual-motor practicein man. ElectroencephalogrPh. 4 Ci Neurophvsiology, 1975, 38,415-425.

Callaway, E. and Harris, P. Coupling between cortical potentialsfrom different areas. Science, 1974, 183, 873-875.

Desmedt, J. Active touch exploration of extrapersonal space. PNational Academ e Science 1977P 74(9), 4037-4040.

Donchin, E., Kutas, M., and McCarthy, G. Electrocortical indices of

hemispheric utilization. In S. Harnad et al. (Eds.), Lateralizationijn &hs Huan ervous Syste. New York, NY1 Academic Press, 1977. Pp.339-384.

Dumenko, V. Electrooncephalographic investigation of cortical

relationships in dogs during formation of a conditioned reflexstereotype. In Rusinoy, V. (Ed.), [lectrophysiology Sf th CentralNervous Syste , New York:Plenum Press, 1970.

Friedman, Dot Vaughan, H. and £rlenmeyer-Kimling , L, Stimulus andresponse related components of the late positive complex in visualdiscrimination tasks# Electroencephalography I Clinical

Neurophvsiolo3 , 1978, 45, 319-330.

Fuster, J. Th Prefrontal Cortex. New York:Raven Press, 1980.

Gevins, A. and Yeager, C. EEG spectral analysis in real time. QECUProceeding, Maynard, Mass;Spring, 1972. Pp. 71-80.

Gevins, A., Yeager, C., Diamond, g., Spire, J., Zeitlin, G., andGevins, G. Automated analysis of the electrical activity of thehuman brain (EEG)t a progress report. Il MProceedings, 1975,63(10), 1382-1399.

Gevins, A., Zeitlin, G., Yingling, C., Doyle, J., Dedon, M.,Henderson, J., Schaffer, R., Roumasset, J. and Yeager, C. EEGpatterns during cognitive tasks: I. Methodology and analysis ofcomplex behaviors. Electroencehalo-rphy I Clinical* Neurophvsiolog,1979(a), 47, 693-703.

4

Page 52: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

r eviros, A., Zeitlin, G., Doyle, J., Schaffer, R. and Callaway, E.

EEGpatterns during cognitive tasks, 1I# Analysis of controlled tasks.Electroencephalography g Clinical Neurophvsiologv, 1979(b), 47,704-710.

Gevins, Ai. Zeitlin, C., Doyle, J., Schaffer, R.t Yingling, C.,Yeager, C. and Callaway E. EEC Correlates of higher corticalfunctions. Science, 1979(c), 203, 665-668.

Gevins, A. and Schaffer, R. Critical review of research on EECcorrelates of higher cortical functions. CRC Reviews irnBioengineerin, CRC Press, October 1980. Pp. 113-164.

Gevins, A,, Doyle, J., Schaffer, R., Callaway, E. and Yeager, C.Lateralized cognitive processes and the electroencephalogram.Science, 1980, 207, 1005-1008.

Gevins, A. Pattern recognition of human brain electrical potentials.ILE Transactions 9n Pattern Analysis U Mahine Intelliaence, 1980,PAMI-2(5), 383-404.

Gevins, A., Doyle, J., Cutillo, B., Schaffer, R., lannehill, R.,Ghannam, J., Gilcrease, V. A Yeager, C. New method reveals dynamicpatterns of correlation of human brain electrical potentials during 0

cognition. Science, 1981, 213, 918-922.

Gevins, A. Dynamic brain electrical patterns of cognition, in ThirdIEEE Conf. Enq. Med.- Bio1, New YorktUEEE Press, 1981.

Gevins, A. Brain potentials and mental function: methodologicalrequirements. In 1. Alter (Ed.), The Limt of FunctionclLocalization, New York;Raven Press, 1983a, £n press.

Gevins, A. Brain potential evidence for lateralization of highercognitive functions. In Cerebral Hemisphere Asymmetry: Method,Theor and Application. J. B. Hellige (Ed.), Praeger Press, 1983b,In press.

Gevins, A., Schaffer, R., Doyle, J., Cutillo, B., Tannehill, R. andBressler, S. Shadows of thought: Shifting lateralization of humanebrain electrical patterns during a brief visuomoto- task. Science,

4 1983(a), 220, 97-99,

*" Ievins, A., Doyle, o., Zeitlin, G., Bressler, S. A trainableclassification network algorithm for studying brain potentialpatterns of higher cognitive functions. Transactions n PatterA11nalysis R£r Machine Intelliqence, 1983(b), In Prep.

Lieb, J., Engel, J., Gevins, A., and Crandall, P. Surface and depthFEG correlates of epileptic foci and surgical outcome in temporallobe epilepsy. Epilepsi, 1981, 22, 515-538.

Livanovp Me Siala O.rgaiat±iAt 9_f CerebaI Poes.ajie New

45

/ a_

Page 53: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

York!Halsted (Wiley), 1977.

Luria, A. Higher Cortical Functions in dart. New York:Basic Books,1977.

McGillem, C., Aunon, Jt and Childers, D. Signal processing in evokedpotential research. CQ Review ji Bioengineerin , 1981, 6(3),225-265.

Mesulam, M. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateralneglect. Ann. Neurol. 1981, 10(4), 309-325.

Naatanen, R., Hukkanen, S. and Jarvilehto. Magnitude of stimulusdeviance and brain potentials. In H. Kornhuber and L. Deeke, (Eds.)Motivation, Motor and Sensory Processes f the Brain: ElectricPotentials, Behavior and Clinical Use, AmsterdamaElsevier, 1980.

Renault, 8., Ragot, R,, Lesev-e, N. and Remond, A. Onset and offsetof brain events as indices of mental chronometry. Science, 1982,215, 1413-1415.

Ritter, W., Vaughan, H. and Simson, R. On relating event-relatedpotential components to stages of information processing. InCaillard, A. and Ritter, W. (Eds.), lutorials in Event-Related OraResearch, Amsterdam:Elsevier, 1993.

Simson, R., Vaughan, H.9 and Ritter, W. Scalp topography ofpotentials in auditory and visual go/no-go tasks.Flectroencenhalographv Clinicsl Naurophvsiologv, 1977, 43, 864-875.

Squires, K., Donchin, E., Herning, R.v and McCarthy, G. On theinfluence of task relevance and stimulus probability on event-relatedpotential components. Electroencenhaloqraphv I ClinicaNeurophvsiologv, 1977, 42, 1-14.

leuber, H. The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In H. Wari-enand K. Abert (Eds.), Frontal Granular Corte #.d Behavior, NewYork:McGraw-Hill, 1964. Pp. 410-477,

Viglione, S. Applications of pattern recognition technology. In J.Mendel and k. Fu (Eds.), Adaptive LUal n RU Pattern Recognition

Systems. New YorkAcademic Press, 1970. Pp. 115-161.

Halter, W. Slow potential changes in the human brain associated withexpectancy, decision and intention, Electroencephalograhv I

Clinica Neurophysiology (Suppl.), 1967, 26, 123-130.

Walter, W. and Shipton, H. A new toposcopic display system.

Electroencephaloqrphv I Clinical Nurophvsioloqv, 1951, 3, 281-292,

Zurif, E# Language mechanisms: A neuropsychological perspective.

American Scientist, 1980, 68, 305-310.

46

Page 54: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

.LEGENDS

Figure 1 - Examples of stimuli for move and no-move trials. Arroworiginated at center screen; its direction and the location of thetarget changed randomly across trials. The labels "Move' and"No-Move' did not appear in the actual stimuli.

Figure A - Electrode montage.

Figure 2B - 91 pairwise correlations were computed between the 15electrodes.

Figure 2C - Anatomical constraints. The correlations of a principalelectrode was measured with 10 other electrodes. The aOz electrodeset is shown.

Figure3 - The major peaks of the average event-related potential(ERP) and Neurocognitive Pattern (NCP) Analysis intervals determinedfrom them. This illustration is an average of the data from the lastfour persons in the study; in practicer the peaks and analysisintervals were determined separately for each person.

Figure 4-a - ERPs for Move trials (610 trials from P's #6-9).

Figure 4 - ERPs for No-Move trials (604 trials from P's #6-9).

Figur 5 - Subtraction ERP's (No-Move minus Move, 6 P's) showing thenegative (N2) peak at 240 msec.

Fig - Between-task NCP results obtained from single trial evokedcorrelations. The most significantly differing electrode set and itsprominent correlations are shown in each interval.

Figur 72 - Average right parietal ERP of those Move trials correctlyclassified by the NCP analysis in both the P3a and RP intervals usingcorrelation measures (195 trials from 4 people).

Figure 7b - Average ERP of correctly classified No-Move trials. P391(P3a) and P530 (P3b) peaks are larger in the correctly classifiedNo-Move trials (193 trials from 4 people).

Figure 7c - Average ERP of incorrectly classified, but behaviorallycorrect, Move trials (122 trials from 4 people).

Figure 7§ - Average ERP of incorrectly classified, but behaviorallycorrect, No-Move trials. P3a is absent and P3b is smaller, thusresembling the correct Move ERP (121 trials from 4 people).

Table I - Number of trials, ERP peak latencies, centerpoints of theNCP single trial correlation analysis intervals? and average response

47

Page 55: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

initiation latency (EMG onset) for each of the 9 participants.

Ta l - Averaged P3a peak amplitude (in micro-volts) and correlated

t-tests (dr - 9).

T&§- Response Preparation (RP) interval: averaged slope of astraight line fitted to slow negative potential shift and correlated1-tests (dfu9).

Ta4l q - J-tests of correlations for the nine participants (1612trials: 839 Move, 773 No-Hove). Only those channel pairs showing asignificant uncorrected I-value are listed. (p<.05 a 1.96, p<.01 a2.57, p<.O01 a 3.29. aBonferroni-corrected t-value of 3.58 = p<.05.)

Table § - Simplified, single discriminant function classificationequation. G(f) -1 for f>O, else G(f) =0; (X/Y) is the standardized,Fisher's z transformed correlation value of the t-! electrode pair.Individual trials whose classification function G(f) - I wereassigned to the no-move class; those whose G(f) = 0 to the moveclass.

Tab- Classification accuracy for the P3a and RP intervals foreach of the 9 participants using the equations derived from the wholegroup.

48

I i

Page 56: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 -June 7

I

or% go 4 ~

" if' i'

-A-

is is S

IVA A - a 1 ~ aI

49

.... MI

-- L i I I n. i " -- ' - - -- " . --- nl u

• . . .. . .. ..- .. . ...

Page 57: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych.. 83--June 7

Average P3a orre- ctr

ated rceA1l Itude (mv t P,9

NOVO move- - - - -

1.3 2.90 -2.95 .01

aCz -. 26 1.69 -2.91 .01

CA -.80 1.53 -3.16 .01aPz .14 2.30 -5.3* SO 4

Pz .49 2.32 -4.771 .005 ,0

aoz -. 94 0.34 -2.28 .05

-. 62 1.82 -3.82 .005

C,4 -. 17 2.12 -4.44 .005

p3 -. 39 1.94 -2.97 .01

pk -. 80I 1.1 -3.05 .01

* Sonforront t (15 comqrisons. df * 7, pGOS 4 4.35

Table 2

so

~50

, , . . . . u , v', , - ' -a

Page 58: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 - June 7

Average Corr.- Uncor-

- ?~!Pvalated r@eted

Fz .24 -. 18 I .09 M.S.

acz .17 -,48 2.80 .05

Cz .07 -.82 2.30 .05

aft .00 ..51 2.14 .05

Pz -.03 -.69 2.10 .05

sOz -.17 -.35 1.9 .05

C3 .24 -.50 3.5 .005

C4 .62 -.54 3.0 .01

P3 -.01 -.58 2.8 .05

P4 -.11 -.69 2.2 .05

* lonferroni t (15 casparIsons, df 7) p 4.05 4.35

Table 3

.5

9

51 _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _

4.

Page 59: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 - June 7

Correlation ttElectrode Prestimulus P.) P1 Rt

Pair Interval I Int.raI Interva

Fz 2.24

F2 : F7 .8

Fz 3 2.38Fz - C3 3.12 2.42Fz -C4 2.60 2.08Fz P1 2.16Fz Pz 2.58Fz - aCz 2.38Fz Cz 2.35 2.43Fz Pi 2.38Fz -aOz 2.46FS -aCz 2.08 2.37 2.70Fe - Pz 3.13FS-P 3 3.30

I P2 2.11

P4 C4 2.47 2.94aC- C3 2.6014 - T3 2.29C4 - C3 2.17F7 "C 3 3.20F7 - 13 3.72*F8-aFz 2.58 2.63F8 - Cz 2.50 3.34F8 - aPz 3.82*F8 -aOz 3.29F8 C4 3.56

a - Cz 2.09 2.80

Pz. C3 3.01P3 "T4 2.22

- 2.514 2.13 2.38T4- C4 2.70T4- P4 2.17

Table 4

*Bonferroni-€orrected t; (100 comparisons, df - 1201p < .05 .3.58)

I-4

52

_ _/

Page 60: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Paych. 83 -- June 7

NL3w~ S

U

4. 0* ff%

- N* U W'~

0'4~ S U~. %~ S

%.~ * 4. 5N S- a. ~.~

rdII if,' if' S* 4'4. * -

- - + 4. +~ 4' U -* U 1% %. N

~.. Ia. * Ia.N %. a. %.

adS ~0 -

- - * -* 4 4. if'* S . -

U ~Sb 4.U,'

'a. S-- .0

U

I * U '.0

a3

I a. a a. a

$

I

K La

Page 61: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 -- June 7

whole1 2 4 6 7 8 9 group

P3a 64.4 55.0 53.2 51.7 55.1 52.5 58.7 51.5 54.4 55.1

ftP 62.8 64.6 47.0 43.9 64.2 58.7 52.0 58.8 60.2 55.6

Table 6

5

-- 54

+/

Page 62: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 -- June 7

NMI

SI*1*0

55

4

Page 63: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 -June 7

F , 2

fig. 2

10

-1- frig. 2c

44,

Page 64: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 -- June 7

IAM!j ..... .... ....

~5

*...* .. * f l.

Sii

I *

a....

Page 65: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 - June 7

...... .. .

L

doe ..... a 0 0 0 J

S0

, :

LI

L 0 S*

*

0- - 1;f

% * . .. 9 0 0 0 0 1 -C C S

I. .U

II

We

* "*oo 0 iSe 0lQ'

* S.

° S

Page 66: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 - J~me 7

* 0* 0* S

St.... *.** 4*t*** * **S

2 S SS

S

I6~ *5S** S*SS.. *St**. *t***~~...

S

1~C.) C.

* 6 0 0* I

hi * *......................*....*.. ... I SI S

0

4j~ I

g4~ -00 0 0 z

*5*** ****** *..~** *.... ...... *.**e *.....

N N NIL C.

O 0

* 0* S

... ****t S.. ***SS*SC* *. if* 0

6

4 0 6* 0

*

*55*5* ***SS besetS

o 0.

59

________- - S - 4

Page 67: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Psych. 83 - June 7

I U

>10w

.......... .............

° N, S

*

I>

....... .......... .1...... . .

0*

Iz

E

60

.41

Page 68: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

PsYch. .83 -- June 7

u

L 0

u-i

61

Page 69: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

V. SIGNAL PROCESSING STUDIES (Also Sponsored by the Office of Naval

Research and the USAF School of Aerospace Medicine)

A. Introduction

In order to further understand the signals extracted in themove/no-move visuospatial experiment (Sections III and IV), we areattempting to determine which type of measurements and frequency bandshave the most specific task-related information. Preliminary resultsof these ongoing studies are presented in this section. We began bydigitally filtering the single-trial brain potential timeseries intofive passbands determined from Fourier analysis of each person'saverage ERPs. This was based on the strategy that mass brainpotentials of different frequency bands might have different types ofinformation. This resulted in separate 'delta', 'theta', and low-andhigh "alpha', and "beta' frequency band timeseries for each person.Then, using brief analysis intervals centered at latencies determinedfrom the peaks of each person's average ERPs, four types Ofmeasurement were made on the timeseries of each frequency band? 1)maximum covariance between a pair of channels, 2) lag-time to maximumcovariance, 3) shape of the lagged covariance function, and 4) singlechannel power. These measurements were then used as features in abetween-task pattern recognition analysis, and the relativeclassification accuracy obtained with each measure was used as anindex of its usefulness.

, B. Methods

The analysis was confined to the data of the last four of the original Inine people (see Table I in Section IV), Three of the four were AirForce fighter test pilots, while the fourth was formerly a transportpilot. All were highly-trained individuals who demonstrated aprofessional motivation towards the experiment and produced data ofsuperior quality. The analysis was confined to the time windows andchannel pairs in which highly significant between-task differences inresults had been obtained using the zero lag correlation measure: thethree right parietal (P4) electrode-pairs in the P3a interval and thethree left central (C3) electrode-pairs in the response preparation(RP) interval (see Fig. 6 in Section IV).

For the NCP analysis each of 3 training sets contained 804 trials andeach of 3 testing sets contained 402 trials (total trials - 1206).Optimal feature subsets were selected by exhaustive search. Patternclassification accuracy was the average of the performance of theequations on the 3 testing data sets. The classification accuracy foreach significance level is shown in Table 4. For each siSnificantresult, terms of the equation having weights exceeding a threshold of15% of the maximum weight of the largest term were selected fordisplay.

1. Measure

62

- - . ... . .. . .. . .... _ I Id o

Page 70: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Zero-lag correlation was replaced by several covariance-derivedmeasures to determine whether useful information was discarded in thenormalization involved in the correlation measure. The entirecovariance function, rather than just the zero-lag value, wasconsidered to test for information in the timing of the ERPi (Figures4 to 6). Each covariance function was formed by logging the signals+6 time points (48 msec). Lag values were computed from extendedreal data points, rather than from an increasing number of zeros asdone in the noncyclic fast convolution (FFT) method.

As a preliminary step, spectral analysis and bandpass filtering wereperformed on the averaged ERP for each person. Three non-overlappingFFT's were performed for the 0.5 second prestimulus and two 0.5 secondpost-stimulus windows for each averaged ERP channel for each person.A histogram was formed from spectral frequencies over each channel(Figure 7) and used to determine passband widths for each person.Unfiltered average ERPs for one person are shown in Figure Sa.Filtered ERPs, with passbands selected from Fig,,re 7, are shown inFigures Sb-h.

Covariance was computed between two filtered single trials 9 X(t) andY(t), using the raw score method:

N N NCxy(t) -W EXlt)(t). E x(t) Y t0 0

where N is the number of points in the time window. Three featureswere derived from the covariance function. The first was the maximum(absolute) value of the covariance function. The second was the lagtime at the peak of the covariance function, employed to measure the-relative timing of signals between channels. Since this peak lagmeasure was likely to be unstable, a third measure was used to assessinter-electrode timing. This was a lagged covariance function 'shapemeasure,' computed as the similarity of the lagged covariance functionto a cosine function. Since the cosine is an even function, thismeasure would indicate the tendency of one channel in a pair to leador lag the other. The frequency of the cosine used was the centerfrequency of the passband used to filter the ERP forming thecovariance function. The 'shape measure' was computed as the sum ofcross-products between the points of the covariance function and thecosine function. The sum was divided by the maximum absolutecross-product to equalize the range across frequency bands. Thismeasure was expected to be more stable than the lag-to-peak measuresince it is derived from all points in the covariance function ratherthan just one. The fourth measure was single channel power, computedas the mean square value of the filtered timeseries .over the points ofthe time window.

2, Channels

In the between-task results of the earlier analysis (Sections III andIV) correlations of the right parietal (P4) electrode distinguishedthe move vs no-move tasks in the P3a interval with greatest accuracy.

63

Page 71: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

The three correlations which contributed most to this discriminationwere P4-aOz, P4-C4, and P4-aCz. Likewise in the RP interval, C3-aOz,C3-Pz, and C3-Fz were most effective in distinguishing the tasks.Consequently these six channel pairs were used in the present study.In the P3a interval, the C3 pairs were used as controls, and in the RPinterval the P4 pairs were controls.

Single channel power was computed for the seven channels which formedthese pairs, as well as for the P3 electrode to preserve symmetry inthe eight-channel power analysis.

3. Time intervals

The P3a interval was determined individually for each of the fourpeople so that it straddled the P3a component of each individual'saveraged ERP. The RP interval was centered at the time of onset ofthe response, as measured by the average EMG onset for each person.

The width of the analysis intervals varied with the frequency band inorder to accommodate at least one full cycle of the highest frequencycomponent in a given band. For reasons of stability of the measures,100 msec was the shortest window width. For the delta band thewindows were 200 msec wide, for the theta band they were 175 msec, andfor .te three higher bands they were 100 msec.

jach single-trial timeseries was separately filtered into fivefIe- uency bands before other computations were performed. The averagebanup~os settings were 0-4 Hz (delta), 4-8 Hz (theta), 8-11 Hz (lowalpha), 11-15 (high alpha), and 15-22 Hz (beta). Exact settings wereindividually set for each person based on Fourier analysis of theiraveraged ERPs. Linear phase, Hamming FIR filters were employed. Theywere convolved with the timeseries by a fast convolution (via FFT)using the overlap-save method.

C. Results

1. Crosscovariance Measures

Using the maximum covariance measurer the pattern classificationanalysis was able to differentiate the two tasks iR t e P3a intervalusing the P4 electrode-pair covariances (p<5 x 10 ) Figure 9), butwas unable to do so using the C3 electrode-pair (control) covariances.The algorithm was allowed to combine frequency bands to achieveoptimum classification. The major features which it used were fromthe delta and theta bands. In Figures 9 and 10, a solid lineconnecting two electrode sites indicates that the value for the move

*task was grester, a dashed line that it was greater for the no-movetask. A previous analysis in this interval, using the .1-12 Hz band,zero-lag correlation for the same three P4 electrode3 pairs,discriminated the tasks with significance of only 1 x 10 . Thereforegreater discrimination was achieved by definition of the frequencyranger by using covariance rather than correlation, and by shiftingthe time series in time to find their maximum covariance.

ii 64

Page 72: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

In the RP intervaly the two tasks were sucgessfully classified by the

C3 electrode-pair covariances (p<5 x 10 ) (Figure 9), but not thecontrol (P4) electrode-pair covariances. Again the major featureswere from the delta and theta bands. Although this classification

significance was high, it was below that of the previous zero-lagcorrelation study.

The lag-to-maximum covariance was not successful in significantlydiscriminating tasks, presumably because of the instability of thismeasure without thresholding.

Using the shape lagged covariance measurl in the P3a interval, the P4pairs classified the trials (p<1 x 10 ) (Figure 10), whereas thecontrol pairs did not. Shape measures from the delta and beta bandwere the major ones used. In the RP interval, the shape measure ofthe C3 pairs classified the trials (p<10 ), but the controls did not.The major features were in the theta band.

2. Single-Channel Power Measures

For the power measures, P4, C4 and o0z were used for the P3a interval,P3, C3 and a0z for the RP interval. In the P3a interval thealgorithm was unable to discriminate the tasks with these channels.In the RP interval, the delta band was used to discriminate at p<.O01(Figure 11) compared to p<.05 for controls. In Figures 11 and 12, anupward arrow indicates that the move task power was greater, adownward arrow that no-move power was greater.

Another analysis was performed with the power measure from eightchannels. Separate analyses were performed for each of the fivefrequency bands (Figure 12).

In the 4 RP interval discrimination was achieved in the delta band atp<lxlO- ), using channels C3, Pz, aOz and aCz most strongly. For tootheta band, dis;gisination was achieved in both the P3a (p<5 x 10 )and RP (p<1 x 10 ) intervals. In the P3& interval, aOz, aCz, C3, C4and Fz were the main channels used; and in the RP interval it was P4,Pz and Fz. For the low liphe band, discrimination was achieved in theRP interval at p<5 x 10 , using Pz, C3 and P4. For &4e high alphaband discrimination in the RP interval was at p<S x 10 using aCz andFz. Discrimination was not achieved for beta band power in eitherinterval. In goneral, the direction of the between-task difference inpower was not the same for a given channel in the different frequencybands.

D. Conclusions

These preliminary results suggest somewhat that the between-tasksdifferences in inter-electrode correlations and coveriances may not bedue solely to volume conduction of potentials from a single distantgenerator. First, there was discriminatory information in the maximumcovariance measure, which was often found at non-zero lags. Second,there was information in the shape of the lagged covariance function

65

/

It

Page 73: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

which was sensitive to the phase relation between two signals. Third,when the power measure of a channel was greater for one task? power ona *highly correlated channel was seen to be lower for that task. Forexample, the C3-aOz covariance was greater for move than no-move taskin the delta band (Figure 9). In this case, aOz delta power wasgreater for the move task, but C3 delta power was lower (Figure 12).

If correlation was due solely to volume conduction from a singlegenerator, power for both correlated channels would show the samebetween-task variation. Of course, the suggestion that separategenerators contribute to the reported between-task differences inscalp correlation patterns is mere speculation. Further studies willbe required to determine if this is actually so and if pairwiseinterchannel measures are the best way to characterize the activity ofsuch generators.

Significant differences between tasks were found to be specific tocertain frequency bands. In particular, all covariance-derivedmeasures were significant only in the delta, theta and beta bands. Insome cases (e.g. the shape measure in the RP interval) a measuredifferentiated the tasks in only one band. Only for the power measurewas there any discrimination in the alpha bands.

Specificity was also achieved in the type of measure whichdistinguished the tasks. Zero-lag correlation depends on the timingof two signals as well as their similarity. These two aspects wereseparated in the lag and shape measures on the one handy and themaximum covariance on the other. These measures were found to havedifferent abilities to discriminate in the different frequency bands.Since the between-channel covariance measures differentiated tasksdifferently than the power measures, such measures may provideinformation about the brain which complements that obtained from powermeasures,

Further investigation is proceeding in several ways. An improvedshape measure which carries information as to lead or lag betweenchannel pairs has been developed. An improved single channel poweranalysis will make more explicit the similarities and differencesbetween single and multichannel measures. Additional studies willfill in the Saps in the present study from non-lagged correlations tolagged covariances.

66

'-i

I 66

t ..- ',.n.- " ,.

Page 74: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

5 x o - 2 2.

1 x 1o - 2 53.1

5x 10 53.8

1 X~o 54.5

5 x 1o 54.8

1 x 10 "14 5.

5 x 16 5 5.6

S1lo 56.2

5 x 10 6 56.

-6I 10 56.9

Table 4 - Classification accuracy at different significancelevels. (1206 £rials from 4 persons)

67 -

4

Page 75: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

iiDELTA BAND COVARIANCE- MOVE

S8 1-4. 260 TRIALS. 1-4HZ. P3a INTERVAL

aou aCz

* °

............... eo... ..................... I............. ....

01

* C

Ko ma. #"M

e C

* C

"4* -4 SC C :

DELTA BAND COVARIANCE- NO MOVE 0SSS 1-4. 260 TRIALS. 1-4 HZ, P3a INTERVAL

aOz :aCz

.. . ..... .. .... .. ... .. ...... ......

68

I " :

j------------. -

Page 76: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

THEMA BAND COARIANCE- MOVESSS 1-4. 260 TRIALS. 4-8 HZ. P3a INTERVAL

aOz : aCz

............ ......... ................ .................... ......

:4 MIS 48 AEC ,48 MiC : 48 AMEC

THETA BAND COVARIANCE- NO MOVESSS 1-4, 260TRIALS. 4-8 HZ. P3cl INTERVAL o-

aOz laCz

.. .. ...... ... ... .. .. . ... ... .. . .. ... . ...* .* •* C

how # 48 MWIIC'94 MMI m1ucl

69-

* Ci i i • - III

Page 77: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

BETA BAND COVARIANCE- MOVESSS 1-4. 260 TRIALS. 18-23 HZ P3a INTERVAL

£030

BET BADCVRANE OMV

SSS~ 1-.WTIL,1-3.Z ~ NEVLS

........ ........ ..*. ............ ......

*me S

* .70

Page 78: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

F-F I

I I 1

t a

I

TEE: .

Page 79: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

UNPLTW CAI1AS SE AI~ FLSHfl8 S *S TETM (OfTfl2)47081 I

.FKIm ... Ls a. at .e. 2*** *.d.e* ....

PhLED CtWP$'I $.~CLt~~TT4SPIf)44

___0_000_&?fee-____________s

-- It

.~~~~N~ti (3!1ew eteemts &lU.eseA7

00 0 a 000 0 000 004 6000 00 00000

ftI

Page 80: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

CDJM SS 563 FL$I41Z S S YET#4 (SPOTRES3) SI4

see. ee....e.. ..... de........

0- ISS2 % S 6E SS,-3 IET#4 (';WTIEOI) 027-S1 1

y *MO t go uq (0-1 ib.) &"=OgmN t= UW peiS.

73

Page 81: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

2- 5 NZ SS; V47.r 9.2. Se,*~s3)~

IL A CdMFLM-Lf V !.Lt'

a- '5 we C .Ii*P. Sj'SE * T.H . 14;v'1E3 S28I1IF, a CWHF~ 2.-g A TPT~~1'p~C~.A~T-f~

I N-b eT il

riour ec . Ioe thou. ffth.Wvbe" 0- f.). * a this peaxs the thetu ftoqamm

bw4 Goodtwospecral ost Is"fig.-50

74b

Page 82: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

~ :%. ~ ~*5 ;~Ig~. 4 .*I) V'27'1

Ip*I T3' a fau

P15w'~~~~~~. 00 **t "~u S- )

.0 00 0- . -.

00 'a 0 0

a-*. 00

Page 83: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

xj.4

?- 1 42 C I. Lill- d. *

I ee

?1gure BE At*& twequama band (9-11 no).10.

76

.y. ir

Page 84: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

3.:-. ':: .4 IC. V'104' TE )'74

MooIv Tlal

'I.

*o 0

Piare OF JaM bea fteqaMn baMi (11-14 8.).

77

Page 85: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

NPbAi Trials

it-o rll

A A

'FF

Page 86: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

2' .- 32 MZC.WeV** USE SSR*3 FLSHl8 SSS IETM4 (SCOT IE3 SPM471 I

0 S

2P-.2S 1 ret frequmqp bmiS (3 L.1

* 4

Ps - r

Page 87: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

too

00

LUO

CCzu(h

.0 01-4

80 ',-

Page 88: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

LU I

z UL4N

E 0..

cc x~C-

>m, Z

m

811

LUL

Page 89: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

A

amm

0u

wj00CL~

- 82

Page 90: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

83I-'P3a RP

A 3aCa

faz

FZ

e fC3r aC4t

4~OaO Pz P4

M.1S

43

IL foaca

715u?. 12 - lesm lti of suaysi of 6 cha~ l p w r am u e n13 n PA tu t va

83

Page 91: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

V1. DISTINCT BRAIN-? IA PAJTEflt ACCOMPANYING BEHAVIORALLYIETICA TRIALS (Also sponsored &M thp Of.i g. Nava Research)

In order to examine patterns used by the pattern recognition algorithmto define the move and no-move trials, the classification assigned bythe algorithm to. each trial of the testing data was noted. In allcases, the data were behaviorally correct. Trials for whichclassification was correct for both P3a and RP intervals were calledcorrect; those with incorrect classification for both intervals werecalled incorrect. This was done for both move and no-move conditions,resulting in four classes: (a) correct mover (b) correct no-move, (c)incorrect no-move, and (d) incorrect move. Unfiltered ERPs wereformed for each class for the data of the last 4 people in the study(Figure 13).

The main difference between correctly classified move and no-move ERPswas the positive P3a and P3b peaks at approximately 365 and 530 msecpost-stimulus, respectively. Comparing Figure 13c with Figure 13b,the incorrect no-move ERP is seen to lack a P3a peak and have asmaller P3b peak, thus resembling the correct move ERP. Theincorrectly classified move trials (Figure 13d) have a more distinctP3b peak than the correctly classified move trials (Figure 13a), thusresembling the correctly classified no-move ERP.

Another obvious difference between correctly and incorrectlyclassified ERP's, both move and no-move, was the strong pre-stimulusalpha 'traina in incorrectly classified ERPs. This dissimilarity isclearly seen in alpha band-pass filtered averages (Figure 14). Inboth the correct and incorrect move conditions there are alpha bandERPs which occur at the same post-stimulus time (in phase). In theincorrectly classified waveform the pre-stimulus alpha is much largerthan in the correct, and is phase reversed. The incorrect ERP appearsto undergo a phase adjustment prior to the zero-crossing atapproximately 90 msec post-stimulus, which occurs at the same time inthe correctly classified trials, and is followed by a negative peak at160 msec in both. This peak corresponds to the N163 peak in theunfiltered ERP. This could reflect a timing process which regulatesthe activity of sensory cortex in preparation for incoming stimuli(the old idea of the 'neuronic shutter'). These alpha-band filteredERP's are also clearly different in the P3a and RP intervals where theclassification was made. The high prestiomulus alpha in theincorrectly classified trials may be related to cognitive state, sothat incorrectly classified trials are qualitatively different,1perhaps due to automatic processing. Alternatively, incorrectlyclassified trials may be those with a particular alpha phase atstimulus onset, resulting in enhanced summation of pre-stimulus waves,and difference in post stimulus activity. These possibilities arebeing further investigated since the results show that differentneural patterns may accompany the same behavior.

84

t8

/ i A

Page 92: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

&)Correctly classified save trials

b) Correctly classified no-move trials

AA

c)~~ Inorcl clsife nomv trial11.10

d0 Incorrectly classified mo-ve trials

Figure 13 -Average 1RPs for trials which were correctly and Incorrectlyclassified by the NC? analysis.

85

Page 93: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

LU L.. . .. . .... .. U

LULU 10

is) - =0FADF

.. .. .....

86

.. .-----........---.

Page 94: AO A35 SINGLE TRIAL BRAIN ELECTRICAL PATTERNS OF … · ao a35 545 single trial brain electrical patterns of an auditory / and visual perceptu..(u} eeg systems lab san francisco (incassi

Ii

'~2K

IA

I

/ a