“the reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and...

35
“The reversed gender gap and the education gradient in health: A cohort perspective” Katrijn Delaruelle, Veerle Buffel, and Piet Bracke HeDeRa (Health and Demographic Research), Department of Sociology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium Presented in session: “Gender and health” at the 3rd International ESS Conference, 13-15th July 2016, Lausanne, Switzerland Abstract Taking a cohort approach, we expect the health returns from schooling for women to have changed against the background of the reversal of the gender gap in higher education. Using information from the European Social Survey (seven waves: 2002– 2014) for individuals 25 to 85 years of age (Nmen = 114,797; Nwomen = 136,179) in 30 European countries, we observe diminishing direct health returns from education across female cohorts. This trend has driven the reversal of the gender advantage in direct health benefits from schooling to the detriment of women. Second, we see changes in the indirect health benefits of educational attainment, through its impact on the labor and marriage market situation of women. These indirect effects suppress the trend of the decreasing direct relevance of education for women’s health. Although our findings support the female-favorable gap in the total health-education linkage, they also point to its historical embeddedness. Keywords education, gender, health, marriage market, labor market, credentialism Acknowledgements (if relevant)

Upload: others

Post on 07-Jul-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

“The reversed gender gap and the education gradient in health: A cohort perspective”

Katrijn Delaruelle, Veerle Buffel, and Piet Bracke

HeDeRa (Health and Demographic Research), Department of Sociology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

Presented in session: “Gender and health” at the 3rd International ESS Conference, 13-15th July 2016, Lausanne, Switzerland Abstract Taking a cohort approach, we expect the health returns from schooling for women to have changed against the background of the reversal of the gender gap in higher education. Using information from the European Social Survey (seven waves: 2002–2014) for individuals 25 to 85 years of age (Nmen = 114,797; Nwomen = 136,179) in 30 European countries, we observe diminishing direct health returns from education across female cohorts. This trend has driven the reversal of the gender advantage in direct health benefits from schooling to the detriment of women. Second, we see changes in the indirect health benefits of educational attainment, through its impact on the labor and marriage market situation of women. These indirect effects suppress the trend of the decreasing direct relevance of education for women’s health. Although our findings support the female-favorable gap in the total health-education linkage, they also point to its historical embeddedness. Keywords education, gender, health, marriage market, labor market, credentialism Acknowledgements (if relevant)

Page 2: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

INTRODUCTION The existence of an education gradient in health is well documented (Ross & Wu, 1996).

Higher-educated people generally experience better health, less psychological distress, and a

lower prevalence of depressive symptoms compared with the lower educated (Bracke et al.,

2013; Mirowsky & Ross, 2003; Ross & Wu, 1996).

During recent decades, a growing body of empirical research has provided

specifications for the analyses of health returns from education by gender. The subsequent

results, nonetheless, paint a somewhat inconsistent picture. Whereas a number of studies

show that the link between education and health is more pronounced among women (Ross &

Mirowsky, 2006, 2010), other research reveals similar patterns in the education-health

association for both men and women (Marmot et al., 1997). By contrast, in mortality research,

the educational gradient is found to be stronger for men (Ross et al., 2012).

These inconsistencies may be due to several factors, one of which is the existence of

temporal differences (Zajacova & Hummer, 2009). To shed light on this potential confounder,

public health researchers have recently begun to examine the interplay between gender and

education from a life course perspective (Ross & Mirowsky, 2006, 2010). However, against

the background of the gendered expansion of tertiary education, we argue that it might be

fruitful for research to incorporate an additional time-related perspective that goes beyond the

individual level: A cohort approach, which is designed to capture social change (Ryder,

1965).

In the current study, the focus is on a major social transformation that has taken place in

the educational field during recent decades in the Western world and beyond. Specifically,

from the 1960s onward, the number of women in higher education has increased substantially

(Vincent-Lancrin, 2008), to such an extent that men now generally lag behind women on

several key educational benchmarks (DiPrete & Buchmann, 2013). As statistics of the

Page 3: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

European commission (Eurydice, 2012) illustrate, women accounted for 46% of the

participants in tertiary education in 1985, but currently represent approximately 55% of the

students in higher education.

Earlier research shows that the new gender distribution of education has consequences

for a variety of social processes (Blossfeld et al., 2015; Chiappori et al., 2009; Klesment &

Van Bavel, 2015). It seems to be related to an increased presence of women in the labor force

(Blossfeld et al., 2015, p. xviii), and changed patterns of educational assortative mating

(Schwartz & Han, 2014) and union formation (Klesment & Van Bavel, 2015).

However, to the best of our knowledge, theoretical and empirical knowledge concerning

the impact of the reversal of the educational gender gap and subsequent contextual changes on

the association between education and self-rated health for men and women is lacking. It is

unclear whether, and how, these historical transformations have affected gender-specific

health returns from education.

Accordingly, our study aims to expand knowledge about gender differentials in the link

between education and self-rated health, by systematically examining inter-cohort variations.

For this purpose, we analyze data from seven cross-sectional waves (2002–2014) of the

European Social Survey (ESS), by applying hierarchical age-period-cohort analysis (HAPC),

under the assumption of certain period effect restrictions (Bell & Jones, 2013).

BACKGROUND

Education, health, and gender

Obtaining an educational degree affects self-rated health through both socialization and

allocation mechanisms (Bracke et al., 2013; Bracke et al., 2014). The socialization function of

education is related to the theory of learned effectiveness (Ross & Mirowsky, 1999).

According to this theory, education–as a form of human capital–provides people with

Page 4: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

effective skills and learned competence, which enable individuals to control various life

events, including health. The values and knowledge gained from schooling act as a root cause

of good health. The allocation function of education refers to the processes that assign

individuals to social positions, and hence, affect health-relevant life chances and opportunities

(Chiappori et al., 2009; Huijts et al., 2010). Two of these life areas are of particular interest.

The first concrete example comprises the marriage market returns from education (Chiappori

et al., 2009). Compared with those who have little schooling, the well-educated are more

likely to become married and to stay married (Carlson et al., 2004), and accordingly, to

experience intimate support. In this regard, marriage constitutes a buffer against ill health

(Schoenborn, 2004). Second, formal schooling contributes to the position of individuals in the

labor market (Allmendinger, 1989). This results in accruing economic resources that help to

improve an individual’s standard of living (Mirowsky & Ross, 2005). The higher educated

generally are at a lower risk of being unemployed, and are more likely to have access to high-

status and well-paid work. Because of their better job opportunities, they tend to have lower

exposure to stress caused by economic hardship, and are more likely to have better access to

healthcare than their lower-educated counterparts. In conclusion, whereas the socialization

function adds direct value to people’s health (Johnston et al., 2015) through the provision of

knowledge that is useful to tackle general life problems (Bracke et al., 2014), the allocation

function indirectly affects health through work and family conditions (Zapata Moya et al.,

2015).

In recent decades, researchers have specified the relationship between education and

health for gender (e.g. Ross & Mirowsky, 2006, 2010), thereby paying attention to the fact

that both the aforementioned socialization and allocation functions of education might be

conditioned by gender. However, we argue that such studies are not complete without

considering the socio-historical context in which the gender-specific linking pathways unfold.

Page 5: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Cohort differences

According to Ryder (1965, p. 845), “each cohort has a distinctive composition and character

reflecting the circumstances of its unique origination and history.” Successive birth cohorts

grow up in disparate socio-historical settings and are therefore exposed to different historical

and social forces (Yang, 2008).

With regard to the focus of this study, one cohort-related transformation is highly

significant. For the greater part of the twentieth century, men have been higher educated than

women (Vincent-Lancrin, 2008). The male enrollment rate in higher education far outpaced

that of women. However, since the 1990s the gender gap in education has generally been

reversed (DiPrete & Buchmann, 2013; Klesment & Van Bavel, 2015). In most European

countries, women now outnumber men in participation in, and graduation from, tertiary

education. This is not to say that men are now less likely to enter into higher education than

they were in the past: Quite the contrary (Vincent-Lancrin, 2008). The reversal of the gender

gap is primarily attributable to the considerably higher growth rate of the number of women

in tertiary education, compared with that of men.

This ‘catching-up and passing-over effect’ has had far-reaching consequences, not least

for the female population. Due to the dramatic increase in their participation rates, younger

cohorts of women have been faced with fundamental transformations in the returns from

education (DiPrete & Buchmann, 2013; Schwartz & Han, 2014). Both direct advantages

(those brought about by the socialization process) and indirect benefits (those brought about

by the allocation process) from education seem to have changed over time for women. Taking

this a step further, it is not unreasonable to assume that the female-specific association

between education and self-rated health will fluctuate across cohorts, which in turn makes the

gender differential in the education-health link highly dependent on the socio-historical

context. We should point out that we do not preclude the possibility of male cohorts

Page 6: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

exhibiting different patterns regarding the educational gradient in health. However, it is more

likely that female cohorts do, given their steeper increase. Therefore, in the following section,

we pay particularly close attention to changes among women.

Changes over time in the relationship between education and health among women

In general, there are reasons to assume that the direct relationship between education and

health has decreased across female cohorts. This expectation can be derived from Collins’

(1979) framework of credential inflation. For over four decades, Collins has pointed out that

the worldwide process of educational expansion has gone hand-in-hand with the devaluation

of diplomas. In meritocratic societies, credentialism has brought people to value educational

degrees, irrespective of the skills and knowledge acquired (Van de Werfhorst & Andersen,

2005). In line with this view, many social scientists speak now of education as an ‘artificial

good’, representing a symbolic value rather than being of functional use (Ross & Mirowsky,

1999).

Because an additional year of education no longer provides real skills, the health

advantages induced by the education-related socialization function might have become small.

In a recent gender-neutral study, Delaruelle and colleagues (2015) indeed show that the

massive growth in higher education has watered down the beneficial effect of this mechanism,

as the exploratory power of the theory of learned effectiveness was observed to be relatively

low among more-recent cohorts. In the current paper, we presume that this pattern of

diminishing direct health returns from education is more pronounced among female cohorts,

because of women’s steeper increase in participation in tertiary education. To conclude, this

logic brings us to hypothesize that:

Direct health returns from education have decreased across female cohorts (Hypothesis 1).

Page 7: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

By contrast, arguments concerning the indirect pathways point in a different direction.

Changes over time in the mediating roles of marriage and employment may have altered the

trend of diminishing direct health returns from education.

Our first focus is on changes in the allocation role of education regarding the marriage

market. In recent decades, several researchers have reported negative correlations between

education and marriage opportunities among older female cohorts (Torr, 2011). Evidence

seems to indicate that better-educated women born in the first half of the twentieth century are

less likely to have married than their lower-educated counterparts. We can interpret this

finding within the framework of the gender specialization theory (Becker, 1981), which

stereotypes men as ‘breadwinners’ and women as ‘primary homemakers and caregivers’. In

contexts characterized by such traditional concepts, highly-educated women are less attractive

because of their greater focus on professional life (Kalmijn, 2013; Torr, 2011). In addition,

they have better economic prospects, which makes it less problematic to remain single.

This marital gradient has reversed, however, from one birth cohort to another

(Goldscheider et al., 2015). Along with the substantial increase in female participation in

tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away.

More and more women became working mothers, and the higher educated were financially

even better off than before (Kalmijn, 2013). Intuitively, we can assume that the latter factor

would strengthen the negative relationship between education and marriage. However, studies

show that in recent years the ‘economic independence effect’ has been overshadowed by an

‘income effect’ (Heard, 2011). Among more-recent cohorts, men are increasingly attracted to

women who can bring resources to the union (Goldscheider et al., 2015). Accordingly, better-

educated women’s marriage opportunities have improved over time, while for the lower

educated, they have deteriorated sharply.

Page 8: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

In relation to health, one must also consider changes in the likelihood of staying

married, as the experience of divorce is shown to be related negatively to mental and physical

well-being (Prigerson et al., 1999). Here again, a remarkable reversal is apparent (Schwartz &

Han, 2014). Whereas in the past, women with a higher level of education were more likely to

divorce, marriage dissolution is now more common among the lower educated. One possible

explanation for this trend relates to changes in the marriage market condition. A well-

recognized principle in modern societies is that of ‘educational assortative mating’ (Kalmijn,

1998): Individuals tend to match with regard to their educational background. For the greater

part of the twentieth century, educational hypergamy (unions in which women have a lower

educational attainment than their partner) was the dominant pattern of assortative mating

(Klesment & Van Bavel, 2015). However, this traditionally observed trend is no longer

compatible with the new gender-specific composition of education levels. Women’s increased

access to higher education has shifted the cultural norm towards educational hypogamy

(unions in which women have a higher educational attainment than their partner). This shift

may have indirectly affected the divorce rates of lower-educated women, as a result of the

shrinking pool of potential mates for men who prefer to ‘marry down’ in terms of education

(Schwartz & Han, 2014). These men have significantly less freedom in partner choice, which

may reduce the probability of finding the ideal match, and hence, may increase the rates of

marital dissolution.

Our second focus is on changes in the allocation role of education regarding the labor

market. In relevant literature, it is widely recognized that employment rates have evolved very

differently over time for distinct education levels (Schwartz, 2010). To reveal the mechanisms

that drive these changes, one must not only consider women’s own human capital, but also–if

applicable–their partners’ socioeconomic status.

Page 9: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

With regard to the effect of women’s own education level, previous studies reveal a

pattern of increasing importance (Montez et al., 2014): The educational gradient in

employment participation appears to be more pronounced among more-recent cohorts. One

explanation refers to the ‘crowding out’ hypothesis (Wolbers, 2011): The process of being

forced downwards on the occupational ladder (Gesthuizen & Wolbers, 2010). In the context

of educational expansion, a substantial number of workers have become over-educated. In

many European countries, the massive growth of tertiary education has not been accompanied

by an equivalent upgrade to the labor market (Bracke et al., 2013; Bracke et al., 2014),

forcing highly-educated employees into jobs that were previously carried out by

intermediately-educated workers (Gesthuizen & Wolbers, 2010). The latter, in turn, have put

the low educated at a disproportionally higher risk of being pushed out of the labor market,

and accordingly, of being more likely to experience poor health. Given the gender-segregated

employment conditions and the steeper increase in highly-educated women, crowding out

might have occurred more among the female than among the male population (Klein, 2015).

An alternative sociological explanation attributes the diverging pattern to the emergence

of a stigma effect (Solga, 2002). Low-educated women in younger cohorts are more likely to

be labeled as ‘deviating from the norm’ than their counterparts in older cohorts, because the

group they belong to holds a social minority position (Schofer & Meyer, 2005). Their lack of

education is stigmatized as an ‘individual failure to succeed’. Accordingly, particularly in

meritocratic societies, employers are less willing to hire low-educated women. Although a

similar dynamic is found among lower-educated men, it hits their female counterparts harder

(Gesthuizen, 2004). Women with lower qualifications not only have to fight against

education-related prejudices, but also against gender stereotypes (e.g. the absence of a career

orientation). These reduced job opportunities may adversely affect low-educated women’s

self-rated health.

Page 10: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Perhaps even more important to consider is the changing role of the spouse’s

employment status (Brynin & Francesconi, 2004). Among older female cohorts, labor-force

participation is shown to be related negatively to the partner’s earnings (England et al., 2012).

Notwithstanding the fact that highly-educated women are expected to have higher levels of

productivity, it is this group in particular who exited the labor market once they were married.

Given the pattern of assortative mating, better educated women had fewer incentives to work.

Their partners’ financial success enabled them to focus on homemaking or volunteering. By

contrast, lower-educated women needed to have a job to compensate for their spouses’ lower

earnings. However, this ‘need for income’ process has weakened with each successive birth

cohort, eventually leading to a reversal of the educational gradient in employment (Schwartz,

2010).

Researchers have observed strong complementarities in couples’ employment status

among more recent cohorts (Verbakel & De Graaf, 2009), a finding that can partly be

understood in the larger context of educational expansion. Alongside education, come gender-

egalitarian attitudes (England et al., 2012). Accordingly, the more women received higher

credentials, the more they were likely to strive for equal employment opportunities. In line

with the ‘opportunity-cost’ argument, higher-educated women are now more career oriented,

as they can gain more from being employed (i.e. relatively high wages, identity, and the

satisfaction of having meaningful work) than from staying at home. By contrast, low-

educated women face a completely different situation. Their added economic value to the

household is relatively weak, which encourages them to stay at home.

From what is detailed above, it appears clear that marriage market and labor market

decisions are often interwoven (Brynin & Francesconi, 2004). Therefore, we conceive them

as two sides of one process; a process that we expect to indirectly strengthen the relationship

between education and health among women. To sum up, we hypothesize that:

Page 11: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

The increased level of educational inequality in marriage and labor market outcomes

suppresses the trend of diminishing direct health returns from education (Hypothesis 2).

Last, to conclude our thought experiment, we may assume that if the relationship

between education and health has changed across female cohorts, the gender differential in

the educational gradient in health can in no way be considered as time-invariant.

Controlling for compositional change: parental education

When estimating changes over time in the association between education and health, one has

to control for compositional changes (Goesling, 2007). From one birth cohort to another, the

composition of the group of both the lower and the higher educated changes substantially. At

the lowest end of the educational spectrum, the population has become more negatively

selected in terms of social background (Solga, 2002). At the other end of the spectrum, the

population has become less exclusive (Schofer & Meyer, 2005).

Due to the expansion of democracy and human rights, lower-status groups have

claimed an increasing proportion of the academic degrees. Not controlling for these

compositional changes could lead to misinterpretations of the results: Real differences might

be masked or confounded by changes in the composition of the population (Goesling, 2007).

In order to overcome this possible pitfall, our models control for parental education; the prime

indicator for social origin effects (Groot & van den Brink, 2007).

DATA AND METHODS

Data

For our analysis, we use data from seven different waves (2002–2014) of the biennial

European Social Survey (ESS). The ESS data is obtained from face-to-face interviews and is

representative for the non-institutionalized population of 15 (wave 7) to 31 (wave 4)

Page 12: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

countries. National response rates vary considerably by countries and waves, from 31.4% for

Germany in 2014 to 81.43% for Bulgaria in 2010. We restrict the present sample to the

population aged 25–85 years. We exclude respondents from Albania, Latvia, Romania, and

Kosovo from the analysis due to the insufficient availability of wave observations. Because of

the substantial cultural and geographical differences, we also exclude respondents from

Turkey and Israel. Additionally, respondents for whom we lack information on self-rated

health, education, gender, employment, and marital status are omitted from the sample (52%,

N = 13,712). In total, the pooled dataset contains 250,976 respondents.

Measurements

Dependent variable. Health is assessed by the question “How good is your health in

general?” Respondents could choose from the following answer categories: very good, good,

fair, bad, or very bad. Earlier studies have underscored the validity of self-reported

assessments (Christiaens & Bracke, 2014). Answers are coded from 0 to 4, with higher scores

indicating better health.

Independent variables. To enhance the comparability of educational systems between

countries, education is assessed continuously by years of full-time education completed

(Bracke et al., 2013). Additionally, the impact of outliers is minimized by equalizing all

scores higher than the country-year specific mean plus three standard deviations. For gender,

men form the reference category. Age is measured in years. Lastly, cohort effects are assessed

using generally 3-year bands, with higher values denoting younger cohorts.

Control variables and mediators. To control for parental educational attainment, five

response categories are derived from the European survey version of the ISCED-97,

indicating the highest level of education completed by father or mother: Less than lower-

Page 13: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

secondary education (ISCED 0-1) (reference category), lower-secondary education (ISCED

2), upper-secondary education (ISCED 3), post-secondary non-tertiary education (ISCED 4),

and tertiary education (ISCED 5-6). We add an extra category for respondents who had not

provided information about either of their parents’ educational attainment.

Employment status, economic hardship, marital status, and the number of children

living in the household are considered as individual-level mediators (allocation variables).

Employment status contrasts employed people (reference category) with those who are not;

the latter categorized as, student; unemployed, looking for work; unemployed, not looking for

work; permanently sick or disabled; retired; housework; and a remaining category for

community or military services and ‘other’. Economic hardship is assessed as equivalent

household income, using the modified OECD scale (OECD, 2013). This scale assigns a

weight of 1 to the first adult in the household, 0.5 to all other adults (>14 years old), and 0.3

to children (<14 years old). In the next step, this mediator is grouped into five categories to

enhance the comparability of the weighted household incomes between countries: Lowest

income (<50% of the median income), modest income (>50% and <80% of the median), high

income (>80% and <120% of the median), highest income (>120% of the median [reference

category]), and missing values. Marital status indicates whether someone was married or in a

civil partnership (reference category), divorced or separated, widowed, or single. The number

of children living in the household is split into two variables, one representing children aged

below 12 years and the other representing children aged between 12 and 21 years.

Analysis

To investigate whether gender-specific health returns from education change across cohorts,

we conduct a hierarchical age-period-cohort analysis (HAPC). In its initial original

specification, the HAPC model–which considers individuals as concurrently nested in periods

and cohorts–assesses simultaneously the age, period, and cohort components of change

Page 14: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

(Yang, 2006; Yang & Land, 2006). This model, however, has been criticized by Bell and

Jones1 (Bell & Jones, 2013, 2014a). According to them, it is impossible to estimate net effects

of time-derived changes accurately, without making the assumption that at least one of the

APC effects is equal to zero. To decide which one of the three must be left out of the model,

theoretical grounds are needed (Bell & Jones, 2014b). We removed the period effect from the

model, based on the fact that given the limited time span available, theoretically it is unlikely

to assume any substantial linear (or higher polynomial) period effect in the education-health

association.2

The HAPC modeling framework requires a multilevel, cross-classified design. We

discern three different levels (Figure 1). First, the individual level consists of 114,797 male

respondents and 136,179 female respondents. Second, individuals are simultaneously nested

in cohorts and periods. On the one hand, at the cohort level we distinguish between 25 birth

cohorts, most with a 3-year duration, although the youngest and oldest cohorts are calculated

with an interval of two years. In sum, birth cohorts range between 1917–1918 and 1988–

1989. At the period level, on the other hand, we had to find a solution for the small number of

periods covered by the data. Simulation studies show that in order to perform Bayesian

estimation procedures in an accurate and reliable way, approximately 15 higher-level units

must be available (Stegmueller, 2013). The ESS data consists of only seven periods of

observation. To address this problem, we apply the clustering technique proposed by

Fairbrother (2014): In order to create a sufficient number of second-level units, we examine

the different periods as clustered within countries. Considering that not all countries

participated in all waves, 158 different country-periods are defined. Last, the country-periods

are again nested in 30 countries.

Figure 1: Multilevel analysis design

Country

N = 30

Page 15: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

All models are estimated in MLwiN version 2.31. The Markov Chain Monte Carlo

(MCMC) estimation procedure is applied to account for the cross-classified structure

(Browne, 2012). Furthermore, age, cohort, and education effects are respectively divided by

ten and grand-mean centered in order to facilitate the interpretation of the unstandardized

coefficients. Design weights are used, but we do not include population weights. We estimate

both gender-stratified and gender-combined models. We only present the results of the former

in the main text, as they are more straightforward to interpret, however, the latter (see

Appendix B) allow us to test whether the results differ significantly between men and women.

The analyses are built up stepwise, starting with a baseline model that assumes the education

effect to be time-invariant. In the second model, we assess cohort changes in the total effect of

education on health. In the third model, potential mediators are added. In this way, we can

obtain an insight into changes over time in the direct effect of education on health.

Additionally, this allows us to shed light on possible suppression processes, which may be

indicated in situations where the coefficient of the direct effect becomes larger than the

coefficient of the total effect (Sobel, 1987). The Deviance Information Criterion (DIC) gives

an indicator of the overall goodness of the model fit, with lower scores indicating a better

model (Spiegelhalter et al., 2002).

Cohorts Country*Period N = 25 N = 158

Individuals

Nmen = 114,797

Nwomen = 136,179

Page 16: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

RESULTS

Appendix A gives an overview of the descriptive statistics of the gender-specific

samples. We see that the mean self-rated health scores for men and women are respectively

2.77 (SD = 0.90) and 2.64 (SD = 0.94) (range 0–4). In addition, Figure 2 depicts the average

years of education completed by birth cohort, for men and women separately. As can be seen,

average education levels have increased across cohorts, although not at a similar pace for men

and women. Female averages have risen faster than those of males, resulting in the reversal of

the gender gap in education for the cohorts born between 1964 and 1966.

With regard to the variance decomposition analysis (not shown, but available upon

request), we obtained the following results: First, we see that a substantial part of the variance

in self-rated health is determined by higher-level processes (i.e. ρ =

[σ²country+σ²country*period+σ²cohort]/[σ²country+σ²country*period + σ²cohort +

σ²individual]). For men, 31.3% of the total variance is at a contextual level and for women,

35.3%. Second, among both men and women, intra-cohort differences account for the largest

share of the contextual variance, which supports the feasibility of using a cohort approach.

Respectively for men and women, 20.5% and 23.7% of the differences in self-rated health are

attributable to differences in the socio-historical context in which individuals are embedded.

Figure 2: Average years of education completed by gender and cohort

Page 17: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Table 1 and Table 2 show the results of the cross-classified multilevel analyses of self-

rated health for men and women. All models are controlled for parental education, which is

linked positively to health. Respondents with parents who completed upper-secondary

education are particularly likely to report good health.

In both tables, Model 1 clearly shows a positive relationship between educational

attainment and self-rated health. Other things being equal, highly-educated people tend to

have better health (bmen = 0.287; bwomen = 0.350). However, this overall educational effect

seems to be significantly stronger among the female population (p<.001). Furthermore, we

see temporal differences in health. Among both men and women, younger people report

higher self-rated health scores than older people do (bmen = -0.122; bwomen = -0.137). In

addition, we observe cohort differences in health that are similar among both genders (bmen =

0.146; bwomen = 0.127). In general, more-recent cohorts are healthier.

As can be seen from Model 2 (Table 1 and Table 2), the education effect on health

differs with age. The health gap across education levels seems to be more pronounced among

older men and women (beducation*age-men = 0.068; beducation*age-women = 0.047). By contrast, there

are no significant cohort differences in the total effect of education for either gender.

Furthermore, both gender-stratified regressions reveal significant effects of the

mediating variables. Higher self-rated health scores are found among employed people,

4

6

8

10

12

14

16A

vera

ge y

ears

of e

duca

tion

com

plet

ed

Cohorts

Men

Women

Page 18: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

married people, people with higher household income, and with each additional child living in

the household. All these effects are observed for both men and women, though some of them

seem to be stronger for one gender than the other. The health gap between the retired and the

employed appears to be more pronounced among men than among women (p<.001). A

similar pattern is obtained with regard to homemakers compared with the employed (p<.01)

and the widowed compared with the married (p<.05). The number of children living in the

household aged below 12 years seems to affect women’s health more than men’s (p<.01).

Page 19: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Table 1: Self-rated health regressed on education, age, and cohort. Total male population aged 25–85 (Ncohorts = 25; Nindividuals = 114,797, weighted sample)

(full models) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

b

b b

(SE) (SE) (SE) Intercept 2.700 *** 2.714 *** 2.981 ***

(0.056) (0.057) (0.057)

Education (/10) 0.287 *** 0.273 *** 0.167 ***

(0.007) (0.007) (0.007)

Age (/10) - 0.122 *** -0.123 *** -0.058 ***

(0.011) (0.012) (0.011)

Cohort (/10) 0.146 *** 0.139 *** 0.142 ***

(0.034) (0.037) (0.039)

Education x Age (/100) 0.068 *** 0.035 *

(0.016) (0.016) Education x Cohort (/100) 0.062 -0.022

(0.048) (0.046) Parental education (less than lower-secondary education = reference)

Lower-secondary education 0.025 ** 0.020 * 0.005 (0.008) (0.008) (0.007) Upper-secondary education 0.091 *** 0.088 *** 0.051 *** (0.008) (0.008) (0.008) Post-secondary non-tertiary education

0.073 *** 0.072 *** 0.029 *

(0.013) (0.012) (0.012) Tertiary education 0.076 *** 0.083 *** 0.044 *** (0.009) (0.009) (0.009) Education level unknown -0.068 *** -0.070 *** -0.053 *** (0.014) (0.014) (0.013)

Employment status (employed = reference) Student 0.004 (0.020) Unemployed, looking for work -0.121 *** (0.012) Unemployed, not looking for work -0.212 *** (0.019) Permanently sick or disabled -1.219 *** (0.015) Retired -0.342 *** (0.009) Housework -0.179 *** (0.018) Others -0.208 *** (0.025)

Page 20: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Controlling for these predictors does affect the results. Among the male population

(Table 1, Model 3), the age effect on the association between education and health is partly

explained by the addition of the mediators. Nevertheless, direct health benefits from education

continue to increase with age (beducation*age-men = 0.035). Additionally, similar to the total effect,

no significant cohort changes are observed in the direct effect of education. Among the

Marital status (married = reference) Divorced or separated -0.056 *** (0.008) Widowed -0.122 *** (0.011) Single – never married/civil partnership -0.052 *** (0.007)

Household income (>120% of median income = reference)

< 50% of median income -0.232 *** (0.009) 50%–80% of median income -0.157 *** (0.007) 80%–120% of median income -0.089 *** (0.006) Missing values -0.048 *** (0.007) Number of children aged below 12 0.024 *** (0.004) Number of children aged between 12 and 21 0.027 *** (0.004) Variance Country 0.093

0.094

0.085

(0.027)

(0.027)

(0.024) Country x Period 0.002

0.002

0.002

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.000) Cohort 0.001

0.001

0.006

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.002) Individual 0.629

0.629

0.576

(0.003) (0.003) (0.002) DIC 272657.73 272509.18 262484.32 *p < .05; **p < .01; *** p < .001 (two-tailed test). All continuous variables are centered on their grand mean.

Page 21: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

female population (Table 2, Model 3), the age-related interaction effect in the prediction of

self-rated health is no longer significant after adjusting for the mediators. By contrast,

educational differences in health appear to be structured in a cohort-specific fashion. The

direct effect of education on health is found to be stronger in older cohorts (beducation*cohort-women

= -0.142).

That the education-cohort interaction term becomes statistically significant in Model 3

(Table 2) implies the presence of a suppression effect. It suggests that younger cohorts are

characterized by larger educational inequalities in marriage and employment compared with

older cohorts, cancelling out the diminishing direct effect of education on health across

female cohorts. Put differently, were it not that education favors women’s health-relevant life

chances more in younger cohorts than it does in older cohorts, the interaction between

education and cohort in Model 3 would be even more negative.3

Page 22: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Table 2: Self-rated health regressed on education, age, and cohort. Total female population aged 25–85 (Ncohorts = 25; Nindividuals = 136,179, weighted sample) (full models)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

b

b b

(SE) (SE) (SE) Intercept 2.600 *** 2.622 *** 2.865 ***

(0.059)

(0.060) (0.060)

Education (/10) 0.350 *** 0.338 *** 0.249 ***

(0.007)

(0.007) (0.007)

Age (/10) - 0.137 *** -0.136 *** -0.080 ***

(0.011)

(0.012) (0.011)

Cohort (/10) 0.127 *** 0.120 *** 0.123 ***

(0.035)

(0.036) (0.037)

Education x Age (/100) 0.047 ** 0.015

(0.015) (0.015) Education x Cohort (/100) -0.033 -0.142 **

(0.045) (0.044) Parental education (less than lower-secondary education =

reference)

Lower-secondary education 0.030 ** 0.024 * 0.016 * (0.007) (0.007) (0.007) Upper-secondary education 0.089 *** 0.087 *** 0.061 *** (0.007) (0.007) (0.007) Post-secondary non-tertiary education

0.080 *** 0.082 *** 0.055 *

(0.012) (0.011) (0.011) Tertiary education 0.081 *** 0.090 *** 0.060 *** (0.008) (0.008) (0.008) Education level unknown -0.072 *** -0.073 *** -0.055 *** (0.012) (0.012) (0.012) Employment status (employed = reference) Student -0.016 (0.018) Unemployed, looking for work -0.113 *** (0.012) Unemployed, not looking for work -0.196 *** (0.017) Permanently sick or disabled -1.197 *** (0.014) Retired -0.263 *** (0.008) Housework -0.103 *** (0.007) Others -0.224 *** (0.022) Marital status (married = reference) Divorced or separated -0.057 *** (0.007) Widowed -0.074 ***

Page 23: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Lastly, we need to bring the gender-stratified results together in order to shed light on

cohort changes in the gender differential in the education-health association. In total, female

cohorts seem constantly to receive greater health benefits from education than male cohorts

do. This can be inferred from the insignificant cohort interactions in Model 2, for both men

and women. However, when we look at the direct effect of education, the picture becomes

more differentiated. To describe this result more precisely, we plot the predicted self-rated

health scores by cohorts and education level for both men and women (Based on Model 3 in

Table 1 and Table 2).

(0.007) Single – never married/civil partnership -0.047 *** (0.007) Household income (>120% of median income = reference) < 50% of median income -0.230 *** (0.008) 50%–80% of median income -0.162 *** (0.007) 80%–120% of median income -0.090 *** (0.006) Missing values -0.062 *** (0.007) Number of children aged below 12 0.037 *** (0.004) Number of children aged between 12 and 21 0.034 *** (0.004) Variance Country 0.105

0.106

0.099

(0.030)

(0.030)

(0.027) Country x Period 0.003

0.003

0.002

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.000) Cohort 0.000

0.000

0.003

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.000) Individual 0.636

0.634

0.590

(0.002) (0.003) (0.002) DIC 324750.01 324508.20 314722.70 *p < .05; **p < .01; *** p < .001 (two-tailed test). All continuous variables are centered on their grand mean.

Page 24: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Figure 3: Educational health differences across cohorts, controlling for marital status, number

of children living in the household, employment status, and household income

As shown in Figure 3, the educational gradient in health in older cohorts is significantly

steeper among women than among men. The opposite holds true for more-recent cohorts.

From the birth cohort 1976–1978 onwards, health inequalities between the education levels

seem to be larger among men. This finding implies a reversal of the direct health benefits

from education to the detriment of women. Figure 3 additionally shows that this shift is solely

due to changing patterns among female cohorts.

Sensitivity checks

Several sensitivity checks were performed on the robustness of our results (not shown, but

available upon request). First, we assessed the models using different birth-interval lengths

(5-year and 7-year birth cohorts). Second, we ran the analyses only on the middle cohorts, in

order to exclude possible bias induced by the end-of-cohort values. Last, we used a

hierarchically-ordered logit model to control for a ‘ceiling effect bias’, which is likely to

occur given the ordinal nature of the dependent variable. All these additional analyses

confirmed our main findings.

1.75

2.25

2.75

3.25

3.7519

17-1

918

1919

-192

119

22-1

924

1925

-192

719

28-1

930

1931

-193

319

34-1

936

1937

-193

919

40-1

942

1943

-194

519

46-1

948

1949

-195

119

52-1

954

1955

-195

719

58-1

960

1961

-196

319

64-1

966

1967

-196

919

70-1

972

1973

-197

519

76-1

978

1979

-198

119

82-1

984

1985

-198

719

88-1

989

Self-

rate

d he

alth

Cohorts

Lower-educated men

Higher-educated men

Lower-educated women

Higher-educated women

Page 25: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

DISCUSSION

In this study, we investigated whether the gender differential in the education-health

association has changed against the background of the reversed gender gap in higher

education.

From a total point of view, we can conclude that the social transformation in the

educational realm has left the gender balance untouched. Across all the birth cohorts

observed, education has constantly favored women’s health more than that of men. However,

ending the discussion at this point would be somewhat misleading. When separating the total

effect of education into its gender-specific direct and indirect contributions, more complex

dynamics come to light, which contribute to a more nuanced and detailed understanding.

The female side of the story outlines the most important findings, which justifies our

plea to focus on women. We observe opposite birth cohort trends in the linking pathways of

the education-health association. On the one hand, we observe that the direct relevance of

schooling for women’s health decreases across cohorts. Consistent with Collins’ (1979)

framework on credential inflation, education’s socialization function appears to be of lesser

importance with regard to producing good health among women in younger cohorts compared

with those in older cohorts. In other words, the massive growth in the number of women in

higher-education enrollment has led to a devaluation of the content of educational degrees,

thereby creating a need to modify the theory of learned effectiveness to account for social

change. Although credential inflation may be the most likely explanation for the pattern of

diminishing direct health returns from education, we must bear in mind that other

explanations cannot be ruled out completely, especially given that we did not operationalize

the socialization variables. One alternative suggests the possible existence of a spillover

effect. The pivotal idea is that the increased level of educational enrollments has created a

Page 26: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

climate of societal openness (Smits, 2003), which women from lower-status groups could

benefit the most from with regard to health (Huijts et al., 2010).

On the other hand, we see the rising importance of education for women’s health

through their positions on the labor and marriage market. We can derive this finding from the

suppression effect found in the analysis. After adjusting for the allocation variables, the

diminishing trend of direct health returns from schooling is seen to be completely cancelled

out. Put differently, it appears that the rapid rise in higher-education completion among

women has shifted the female-specific balance between the direct and the indirect health

effects of education toward the indirect effects. We are relatively optimistic regarding this

point. This evolution, in fact, implies that women with higher credentials have become

increasingly able to catch up with highly-educated men’s health, through their improved

capability to translate schooling into equal economic and marital payoffs. Given that women’s

education level keeps rising at a faster pace than that of men (Vincent-Lancrin, 2008), it

remains an open question where this trend will end.

The male side of the story is of less relevance with regard to social change effects,

though it appears worthwhile to discuss it in some depth. The analysis indicates that for men,

educational inequalities in health–either produced through direct or indirect channels–operate

on an age, rather than on a cohort basis. This evidence may suggest that men’s increase in

higher education has not been substantial enough to significantly affect their health returns

from schooling. Men in older cohorts benefit from an additional year of education to an equal

extent as men in younger cohorts.

Although the results appear robust, some limitations of this research need to be

acknowledged. First, health is self-reported, which may result in an over or under estimation

of the actual prevalence of poor health (Subramanian et al., 2010). However, the validity of

self-rated health as an overall measurement of population health has been widely confirmed in

Page 27: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

relevant literature (e.g. Zajacova et al., 2012). Future research should additionally address this

concern by incorporating objective health indicators, such as chronic conditions or functional

limitations. Second, this study could suffer from selection biases. The repeated cross-sectional

nature of our data restricts our capacity to fully distinguish social causation from health

selection processes. Nevertheless, as we control for parental education, we can be reasonably

certain that the greater part of the association between education and health is causal. In order

to be completely sure, further studies could make use of the expansion of compulsory

education as a social experiment to test the direction of the relationship (Brunello et al., 2009;

Oreopoulos et al., 2003). In addition to health-selection bias, mortality selection could affect

the results concerning the older cohorts, more so if this is not a gender-neutral phenomenon.

This source of potential bias stems from the structure of our dataset. Due to the short time

span of our measurements, each cohort covers only a small subset of the full age range.

Accordingly, the earliest cohorts are completely made up of the older population, which

makes them vulnerable to selective survival bias. Although our analysis on the mid-range

cohorts partially moderates this shortcoming, we need more complex models that allow us to

weight for cohort, country, gender, and education specific mortality rates in order to

overcome this issue completely. Last, our results depend on the theoretical assumption of

restricted period effects. However, as mentioned by Bell and Jones (2014a), such premises are

inevitable when attempting to move on to a better understanding of temporal changes.

Even with the limitations noted, this study makes a significant contribution to

existing literature concerning the contextualization of educational inequalities in health

among men and women. We hope to have contributed to the further development of a macro-

sociology of education and health, by showing that education is not only an acquired social

characteristic of individuals. Education is a societal institution, the expansion of which

transforms societies, permeates various life domains, and hence profoundly affects population

Page 28: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

health. All macro-sociological theories deal with transformations in societies. With regard to

the social positions of women and men, the reversal of the gender gap in education is

probably one of the most pervasive transformations in recent history, having consequences for

the health-relevant life chances of both genders for decades to come.

NOTES

1 We acknowledge recent debate concerning Bell and Jones’ remark (see: Bell & Jones, 2015;

Reither, Land, et al., 2015; Reither, Masters, et al., 2015). The prevailing consensus seems to

be that researchers need to gain insight into the descriptive age-period-cohort statistics.

Accordingly, we performed exploratory curve-fitting analyses, which show that age and

cohort are approximately linear functions of health. Hence, we integrated the model

specification of Bell and Jones into our analyses.

2 One may argue the presence of an economic recession effect. Although we agree with this

reasoning, we assume this crisis effect to be of temporary duration, rather than to affect self-

rated health continuously.

3 To make the claim of a suppression effect more persuasive, we performed ancillary analyses.

Using multilevel multinomial logistic models, we regressed marital status (married =

reference category) and employment status (employed = reference category) on education,

age, and cohort. The results (not shown, but available upon request) indicate that the

educational differences in the likelihood of respectively marriage and employment vary

substantially from one birth cohort to another. Whereas a higher level of education negatively

affected women’s odds of being married (versus being single or divorced) in older cohorts, it

now positively influences their likelihood of marriage. A similar pattern can be observed with

regard to being employed (versus being a homemaker).

Page 29: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Funding for this research was provided by the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO)

(G0B8714N).

Page 30: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

REFERENCES

Allmendinger, J. (1989). Educational systems and labor market outcomes. European sociological review, 5(3), 231-250.

Becker, G. (1981). A treatise on the family. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Bell, A., & Jones, K. (2013). The impossibility of separating age, period and cohort effects. Social

Science & Medicine, 93, 163-165. Bell, A., & Jones, K. (2014a). Another 'futile quest'? A simulation study of Yang and Land's

Hierarchical Age-Period-Cohort model. Demographic Research, 30, 333-360. Bell, A., & Jones, K. (2014b). Don't birth cohorts matter? A commentary and simulation exercise on

Reither, Hauser, and Yang's (2009) age-period-cohort study of obesity. Social Science & Medicine, 101, 176-180.

Bell, A., & Jones, K. (2015). Should age-period-cohort analysts accept innovation without scrutiny? A response to Reither, Masters, Yang, Powers, Zheng and Land. Social Science & Medicine, 128, 331-333.

Blossfeld, H.-P., Skopek, J., Triventi, M., & Buchholz, S. (2015). Gender, education and employment: An international comparison of school-to-work transitions. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Bracke, P., Pattyn, E., & von dem Knesebeck, O. (2013). Overeducation and depressive symptoms: diminishing mental health returns to education. Sociology of Health & Illness, 35(8), 1242-1259.

Bracke, P., Van de Straat, V., & Missine, S. (2014). Education, mental health, and education-labor market misfit. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 55(4), 442-459.

Browne, W. (2012). MCMC Estimation in MLwiN v2. 26. Centre for multilevel modelling, Bristol. Brunello, G., Fort, M., & Weber, G. (2009). Changes in compulsory schooling, education and the

distribution of wages in Europe. The Economic Journal, 119(536), 516-539. Brynin, M., & Francesconi, M. (2004). The material returns to partnership: the effects of educational

matching on labour market outcomes and gender equality. European Sociological Review, 20(4), 363-377.

Carlson, M., McLanahan, S., & England, P. (2004). Union formation in fragile families. Demography, 41(2), 237-261.

Chiappori, P.-A., Iyigun, M., & Weiss, Y. (2009). Investment in schooling and the marriage market. American Economic Review, 99, 1689-1714.

Christiaens, W., & Bracke, P. (2014). Work–family conflict, health services and medication use among dual‐income couples in Europe. Sociology of health & illness, 36(3), 319-337.

Collins, R. (1979). The credential society: An historical sociology of education and stratification. New York: Academic Press.

Delaruelle, K., Buffel, V., & Bracke, P. (2015). Educational expansion and the educational gradient in health: A hierarchical age-period-cohort analysis. Social Science & Medicine, 145, 79-88.

DiPrete, T. A., & Buchmann, C. (2013). The rise of women: The growing gender gap in education and what it means for American schools. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

England, P., Gornick, J., & Shafer, E. F. (2012). Women's employment, education, and the gender gap in 17 countries. Monthly Labor Review, 135(4), 3-12.

Eurydice. (2012). Key data on education in europe 2012. Brussels: Eurydice. Fairbrother, M. (2014). Two multilevel modeling techniques for analyzing comparative longitudinal

survey datasets. Political Science Research and Methods, 2(01), 119-140. Gesthuizen, M., & Wolbers, M. H. (2010). Employment transitions in the Netherlands, 1980–2004:

Are low educated men subject to structural or cyclical crowding out? Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 28(4), 437-451.

Gesthuizen, M. J. W. (2004). The life-course of the low-educated in the Netherlands: Social and economic risks. Nijmegen: ICS Dissertation.

Goesling, B. (2007). The rising significance of education for health? Social Forces, 85(4), 1621-1644. Goldscheider, F., Bernhardt, E., & Lappegård, T. (2015). The gender revolution: A framework for

understanding changing family and demographic behavior. Population and Development Review, 41(2), 207-239.

Page 31: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Groot, W., & van den Brink, H. M. (2007). The health effects of education. Economics of Education Review, 26(2), 186-200.

Heard, G. (2011). Socioeconomic marriage differentials in Australia and New Zealand. Population and Development Review, 37(1), 125-160.

Huijts, T., Monden, C. W. S., & Kraaykamp, G. (2010). Education, educational heterogamy, and self-assessed health in Europe: A multilevel study of spousal effects in 29 European countries. European Sociological Review, 26(3), 261-276.

Johnston, D. W., Lordan, G., Shields, M. A., & Suziedelyte, A. (2015). Education and health knowledge: evidence from UK compulsory schooling reform. Social Science & Medicine, 127, 92-100.

Kalmijn, M. (1998). Intermarriage and homogamy: Causes, patterns, trends. Annual review of sociology, 24, 395-421.

Kalmijn, M. (2013). The educational gradient in marriage: A comparison of 25 European countries. Demography, 50(4), 1499-1520.

Klein, M. (2015). The increasing unemployment gap between the low and high educated in West Germany. Structural or cyclical crowding-out? Social science research, 50, 110-125.

Klesment, M., & Van Bavel, J. (2015). The reversal of the gender gap in education and female breadwinners in Europe. Families and Societies. Working paper series(26).

Marmot, M., Ryff, C. D., Bumpass, L. L., Shipley, M., & Marks, N. F. (1997). Social inequalities in health: next questions and converging evidence. Social science & medicine, 44(6), 901-910.

Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2003). Education, social status, and health. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2005). Education, learned effectiveness and health. London Review of Education, 3(3), 205-220.

Montez, J. K., Sabbath, E., Glymour, M. M., & Berkman, L. F. (2014). Trends in work–family context among US women by education level, 1976 to 2011. Population Research and Policy Review, 33(5), 629-648.

OECD. (2013). Guidelines for micro statistics on household wealth. Paris: OECD Publishing. Oreopoulos, P., Page, M. E., & Stevens, A. H. (2003). Does human capital transfer from parent to

child? The intergenerational effects of compulsory schooling. Cambridge, Massachusetts: National Bureau of Economic Research.

Prigerson, H. G., Maciejewski, P. K., & Rosenheck, R. A. (1999). The effects of marital dissolution and marital quality on health and health service use among women. Medical care, 37(9), 858-873.

Reither, E. N., Land, K. C., Jeon, S. Y., Powers, D. A., Masters, R. K., Zheng, H., et al. (2015). Clarifying hierarchical age–period–cohort models: A rejoinder to Bell and Jones. Social Science & Medicine, 145, 125-128.

Reither, E. N., Masters, R. K., Yang, Y. C., Powers, D. A., Zheng, H., & Land, K. C. (2015). Should age-period-cohort studies return to the methodologies of the 1970s? Social Science & Medicine, 128, 356-365.

Ross, C. E., Masters, R. K., & Hummer, R. A. (2012). Education and the gender gaps in health and mortality. Demography, 49(4), 1157-1183.

Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (1999). Refining the association between education and health: The effects of quantity, credential, and selectivity. Demography, 36(4), 445-460.

Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2006). Sex differences in the effect of education on depression: resource multiplication or resource substitution? Social science & medicine, 63(5), 1400-1413.

Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2010). Gender and the health benefits of education. The Sociological Quarterly, 51(1), 1-19.

Ross, C. E., & Wu, C. L. (1996). Education, age, and the cumulative advantage in health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37(1), 104-120.

Ryder, N. B. (1965). The cohort as a concept in the study of social-change. American Sociological Review, 30(6), 843-861.

Schoenborn, C. A. (2004). Marital status and health, United States 1999-2002 Advance data from vital and health statistics, No. 351. Hyattsville, MD: U.S. National Center for Health Statistics.

Page 32: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Schofer, E., & Meyer, J. W. (2005). The worldwide expansion of higher education in the twentieth century. American sociological review, 70(6), 898-920.

Schwartz, C. R. (2010). Earnings inequality and the changing association between spouses’ earnings. American journal of sociology, 115(5), 1524-1557.

Schwartz, C. R., & Han, H. (2014). The reversal of the gender gap in education and trends in marital dissolution. American sociological review, 79(4), 605-629.

Smits, J. (2003). Social closure among the higher educated: Trends in educational homogamy in 55 countries. Social Science Research, 32(2), 251-277.

Sobel, M. E. (1987). Direct and indirect effects in linear structural equation models. Sociological Methods & Research, 16(1), 155-176.

Solga, H. (2002). 'Stigmatization by negative selection': Explaining less-educated people's decreasing employment opportunities. European sociological review, 18, 159-178.

Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., & Van Der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology), 64(4), 583-639.

Stegmueller, D. (2013). How many countries for multilevel modeling? A comparison of frequentist and Bayesian approaches. American Journal of Political Science, 57(3), 748-761.

Subramanian, S. V., Huijts, T., & Avendano, M. (2010). Self-reported health assessments in the 2002 World Health Survey: how do they correlate with education? Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 88(2), 131-138.

Torr, B. M. (2011). The changing relationship between education and marriage in the United States, 1940-2000. Journal of family history, 36, 483-503.

Van de Werfhorst, H. G., & Andersen, R. (2005). Social background, credential inflation and educational strategies. Acta Sociologica, 48(4), 321-340.

Verbakel, E., & De Graaf, P. M. (2009). Partner effects on labour market participation and job level: opposing mechanisms. Work, Employment & Society, 23(4), 635-654.

Vincent-Lancrin, S. (2008). The reversal of gender inequalities in higher education: An on-going trend. In OECD (Ed.), Higher education to 2030 (Vol. 1, pp. 265-298). Paris: OECD Publishing.

Wolbers, M. H. (2011). Dynamiek in overscholing en verdringing op de arbeidsmarkt. Tijdschrift voor Arbeidsvraagstukken, 27, 398-413.

Yang, Y. (2006). Bayesian inference for hierarchical age-period-cohort models of repeated cross-section survey data. Sociological Methodology, 36, 39-74.

Yang, Y. (2008). Social inequalities in happiness in the United States, 1972 to 2004: An age-period-cohort analysis. American Sociological Review, 73(2), 204-226.

Yang, Y., & Land, K. C. (2006). A mixed models approach to the age-period-cohort analysis of repeated cross-section surveys, with an application to data on trends in verbal test scores. Sociological Methodology, 36, 75-97.

Zajacova, A., & Hummer, R. A. (2009). Gender differences in education effects on all-cause mortality for white and black adults in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 69(4), 529-537.

Zajacova, A., Rogers, R. G., & Johnson-Lawrence, V. (2012). Glitch in the gradient: Additional education does not uniformly equal better health. Social Science & Medicine, 75(11), 2007-2012.

Zapata Moya, A., Buffel, V., Yáñez, C. N., & Bracke, P. (2015). Social inequality in morbidity, framed within the current economic crisis in Spain. International journal for equity in health, 14(1), 131.

Page 33: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

APPENDIX

Appendix A: Descriptive statistics (Nmen = 114,797; Nwomen = 136,179) Men Women

Range Mean SD Mean SD Health 0–4 2.77 0.90 2.64 0.94 Years of education 0–30 12.5 3.9 12.1 4.1 Age (in years) 25–85 51 15.5 52 15.8 Cohort (birth year) 1917–1987 1957 15.8 1956 16.1

% N % N Parental education

Less than lower-secondary education 29.6% 33999 31.6% 43056 Lower-secondary education 19.1% 21976 19.6% 26678 Upper-secondary education 28.1% 32293 26.4% 36008 Post-secondary non-tertiary education 4.9% 5600 4.8% 6579 Tertiary education 13.6% 15631 12.9% 17612 Education level unknown 4.6% 5298 4.6% 6246

Employment Employed 60.9% 69961 46.5% 63335 Student 1.4% 1609 1.4% 1913 Unemployed, looking for work 4.3% 4917 3.3% 4497 Unemployed, not looking for work 1.6% 1868 1.6% 2119 Permanently sick or disabled 2.7% 3141 2.4% 3296 Retired 26.5% 30430 27.0% 36757 Household 1.6% 1877 16.9% 23031 Others 0.9% 994 0.9% 1231

Household income < 50% of median income 9.2% 10543 11.9% 16154 50–80% of median income 15.3% 17538 17.7% 24061 80–120% of median income 24.6% 28188 24.3% 33024 > 120% of median income 34.2% 39253 27.5% 37479 Missing values household income 16.8% 19275 18.7% 25461

Marital status Married or civil partnership 63.5% 72895 56.2% 76478 Divorced or separated 9.5% 10923 12.4% 16927 Widowed 4.7% 5376 15.5% 21100 Single 22.3% 25603 15.9% 21674

Number of children aged below 12 0 80.2% 92082 78.5% 106963 1 10.8% 12402 11.8% 16084 2+ 9.0% 10313 9.7% 13132 Number of children aged between 12 and 21 0 83.1% 95342 80.6% 109788 1 10.9% 12517 12.7% 17234 2+ 6.0% 6938 6.7% 9157

Page 34: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

Appendix B: Self-rated health regressed on education, age, and cohort. Total population aged 25–85 ( Ncohorts = 25; Nindividuals = 250,976, weighted sample) (full models)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

b

b b

b

(SE) (SE) (SE) (SE) Intercept 2.685 *** 2.690 *** 2.696 *** 2.894 ***

(0.058)

(0.058) (0.059)

(0.057)

Education (/10) 0.324 *** 0.266 *** 0.256 *** 0.150 ***

(0.005)

(0.007) (0.007)

(0.007)

Gender -0.079 *** -0.082 *** -0.072 *** -0.049 *** (0.003) (0.003) (0.004) (0.004) Age (/10) -0.133 *** -0.132 *** -0.128 *** -0.070 ***

(0.010)

(0.011) (0.011)

(0.010)

Cohort (/10) 0.129 *** 0.127 *** 0.130 *** 0.149 ***

(0.030)

(0.032) (0.034)

(0.033)

Education x Gender (/10) 0.107 *** 0.100 *** 0.115 *** (0.008) (0.009) (0.009) Education x Age (/100)

0.071 *** 0.052 **

(0.017)

(0.016)

Education x Cohort (/100) 0.078

0.038

(0.049)

(0.046)

Age x Cohort -0.007 -0.040 *** (0.005) (0.006) Age x Gender -0.009 -0.007 (0.008) (0.008) Cohort x Gender -0.020 -0.019 (0.024) (0.024) Education x Gender x Age -0.020 -0.023 (0.022) (0.021) Education x Gender x Cohort -0.121 -0.165 ** (0.063) (0.061) Parental education (less than lower-secondary education = reference)

Lower-secondary education 0.028 *** 0.027 *** 0.023 *** 0.012 * (0.005) (0.005) (0.005) (0.005) Upper-secondary education 0.090 *** 0.090 *** 0.087 *** 0.056 *** (0.005) (0.005) (0.005) (0.006) Post-secondary non-tertiary education

0.076 *** 0.076 *** 0.077 *** 0.043 ***

(0.009) (0.008) (0.009) (0.008) Tertiary education 0.078 *** 0.079 *** 0.087 *** 0.052 *** (0.006) (0.006) (0.006) (0.006) Education level unknown -0.073 *** -0.073 *** -0.074 *** -0.056 *** (0.009) (0.009) (0.009) (0.009) Employment status (employed = reference)

Student -0.012 (0.013) Unemployed, looking for work -0.116 *** (0.008) Unemployed, not looking for work -0.203 ***

Page 35: “The reversed gender gap and the education …...tertiary education, gender specialization and gender interdependency gradually faded away. More and more women became working mothers,

(0.012) Permanently sick or disabled -1.205 *** (0.010) Retired -0.297 *** (0.006) Housework -0.118 *** (0.006) Others -0.215 *** (0.016) Marital status (married = reference)

Divorced or separated -0.058 *** (0.005) Widowed -0.094 *** (0.006) Single – never married/civil partnership -0.053 *** (0.005)

Household income (>120% of median income = reference)

< 50% of median income -0.230 *** (0.006) 50%–80% of median income -0.160 *** (0.005) 80%–120% of median income -0.090 *** (0.004) Missing values -0.055 *** (0.005) Number of children aged below 12 0.031 *** (0.003) Number of children aged between 12 and 21 0.032 *** (0.003) Variance

Country 0.099

0.100

0.100

0.094

(0.028)

(0.029)

(0.029)

(0.027)

Country x Period 0.003

0.003

0.003

0.002

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.000)

Cohort 0.000

0.000

0.001

0.001

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.000)

(0.000)

Individual 0.634

0.634

0.633

0.585 (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) (0.002)

DIC 597974.06 597798.61 597367.46 577640.28 *p < .05; **p < .01; *** p < .001 (two-tailed test).

All continuous variables are centered on their grand mean.