attachment, emotion regulation difficulties, and ... · han and kahn 1069 the role of emotion...

25
The Counseling Psychologist 2017, Vol. 45(8) 1066–1090 © The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0011000017744884 journals.sagepub.com/home/tcp Regular Manuscript Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and Disordered Eating Among College Women and Men Suejung Han 1 and Jeffrey H. Kahn 1 Abstract In this study, we examined a structural equation model in which attachment anxiety and avoidance were associated with binge eating and restricted eating behaviors through distinct emotion regulation difficulties of emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff among college women (n = 744) and men (n = 200). As predicted, attachment anxiety was associated with emotional reactivity which, in turn, was associated with binge eating. Attachment avoidance was associated with emotional cutoff which, in turn, was associated with restricted eating, both in college women and men. Two gender differences were found: the association between emotional cutoff and binge eating was statistically significant only for women, and the association between emotional reactivity and binge eating was stronger for men than for women. Clinical implications are suggested, specifically the importance of addressing attachment insecurities and distinct emotion regulation difficulties related to serving college women and men with binge eating and/or restricted eating behaviors. Keywords attachment insecurity, emotion regulation, binge eating, restricted eating, gender 1 Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA Corresponding Author: Suejung Han, Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Campus Box 4620, Normal, IL 61790-4620, USA. Email: [email protected] 744884TCP XX X 10.1177/0011000017744884The Counseling PsychologistHan and Kahn research-article 2017 The Division 17 logo denotes that this article is designated as a CE article. To purchase the CE Test, please visit www.apa.org/ed/ce

Upload: others

Post on 03-Jun-2020

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000017744884

The Counseling Psychologist2017, Vol. 45(8) 1066 –1090

© The Author(s) 2017Reprints and permissions:

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0011000017744884

journals.sagepub.com/home/tcp

Regular Manuscript

Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and Disordered Eating Among College Women and Men

Suejung Han1 and Jeffrey H. Kahn1

AbstractIn this study, we examined a structural equation model in which attachment anxiety and avoidance were associated with binge eating and restricted eating behaviors through distinct emotion regulation difficulties of emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff among college women (n = 744) and men (n = 200). As predicted, attachment anxiety was associated with emotional reactivity which, in turn, was associated with binge eating. Attachment avoidance was associated with emotional cutoff which, in turn, was associated with restricted eating, both in college women and men. Two gender differences were found: the association between emotional cutoff and binge eating was statistically significant only for women, and the association between emotional reactivity and binge eating was stronger for men than for women. Clinical implications are suggested, specifically the importance of addressing attachment insecurities and distinct emotion regulation difficulties related to serving college women and men with binge eating and/or restricted eating behaviors.

Keywordsattachment insecurity, emotion regulation, binge eating, restricted eating, gender

1Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA

Corresponding Author:Suejung Han, Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Campus Box 4620, Normal, IL 61790-4620, USA. Email: [email protected]

744884 TCPXXX10.1177/0011000017744884The Counseling PsychologistHan and Kahnresearch-article2017

The Division 17 logo denotes that this article is designated as a CE article. To purchase the

CE Test, please visit www.apa.org/ed/ce

Page 2: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1067

Disordered eating behaviors such as binge eating and restricted eating among college students are highly prevalent, carrying serious physical and mental health consequences. For example, 25% to 48% of college women and 6% to 17% of college men binge eat (e.g., Matthews, 2004), and 44% of college women and 27% of college men are on a diet to lose weight (American College Health Association, 2010). Even at a subclinical level, binge eating is associated with obesity and illnesses such as diabetes (Schwarze, Oliver, & Handal, 2003); excessive restricted eating is associated with gastrointestinal issues and amenorrhea (Selzer, Caust, Hibbert, Bowes, & Patton, 1996). Both binge eating and restricted eating are also associated with mental health con-cerns such as depression and anxiety, and may precede the development of eating disorders at a clinical level (Agras, 2001).

Attachment theory (Bowlby, 1988; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007) may explain why some individuals engage in disordered eating. It has been sug-gested that people with insecure attachment may have emotion regulation difficulties that are associated with disordered eating (e.g., Tasca et al., 2009). Thus, in this study we examined how insecure attachment is distinctively related to binge eating and restricted eating through two emotion regulation difficulty pathways: emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff. In addition, we examined whether these interrelationships differed between college women and men.

Insecure Attachment and Disordered Eating

Attachment security develops among children if the primary caregiver reli-ably responds to the children’s need for protection and soothing (Bowlby, 1988). These securely attached children subsequently internalize how the caregiver soothed them and develop their own distress regulating capacity. Children who perceive the caregiver to be inconsistent in responsiveness learn to exaggerate distress to seek proximity to their caregiver; this is known as attachment anxiety because the child feels anxiety about rejection and abandonment, and therefore engages in a hyperactivating emotion regulation strategy (Fraley & Shaver, 2000; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Children who perceive the caregiver to be consistently rejecting or unavailable learn to sup-press distress (i.e., a deactivating emotion regulation strategy) and avoid proximity to the caregiver to prevent rejection; these children develop attach-ment avoidance, or discomfort with, and avoidance of, attachment figures (Fraley & Shaver, 2000; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). The attachment secu-rity or insecurity that develops in childhood persists as a trait-like quality into adult relationships (e.g., romantic relationships) with relative stability (Fraley, 2002), internalized as mental representations of self, others, interpersonal relationships, and distress regulation strategies (Bowlby, 1988).

Page 3: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1068 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

Attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance are two forms of attach-ment insecurity; in the past several decades, attachment insecurity has been investigated in relation to disordered eating. Some clinicians have suggested that disordered eating (e.g., binge eating) could be a symbolic way of seeking comfort from foods rather than from unavailable or rejecting attachment fig-ures (e.g., parents, partner; Orzolek-Kronner, 2002) or is a direct reflection of underlying attachment patterns (e.g., restricted eating as distancing from the self, and binge eating as underregulated affects; Ringer & Crittenden, 2007). A meta-analysis of 154 empirical studies on the link between psychosocial factors and disordered eating showed that insecure attachment was the sec-ond strongest correlate of disordered eating with a large effect size (d = 1.31), with negative self-evaluation being the strongest correlate (d = 2.27) among 11 psychological variables, such as perceived low parental care and sensitiv-ity to social dominance (Caglar-Nazali et al., 2013). In a review of 50 empiri-cal studies, Tasca and Balfour (2014) reported that 80% to 100% of individuals with eating disorders had an insecure attachment style. These two reviews did not specify the gender of the participants in the studies reviewed.

Although the link between insecure attachment and disordered eating is well established, the distinct relationships between the two dimensions of insecure attachment—attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance—and disordered eating are less clear. When general disordered eating symptoms were examined, attachment anxiety was associated with disordered eating consistently, whereas attachment avoidance showed mixed findings. For example, attachment anxiety was strongly associated with disordered eating symptoms among a community sample of women (Eggert, Levendosky, & Klump, 2007) and a clinical sample of women (Tasca et al., 2009), yet the association between attachment avoidance and disordered eating symptoms was weak for women in the clinical sample (Tasca et al., 2009) and nonsig-nificant among community women (Eggert et al., 2007). Findings were also mixed when eating disorder diagnoses were examined. A review article reported distinct associations between attachment anxiety and bulimic-spec-trum disorders, and between attachment avoidance and restricting-type anorexia nervosa (Zachrisson & Skarderud, 2010) in women with eating dis-orders, although other research found no such associations among clinical samples of women (Broberg, Hjalmers, & Nevonen, 2001; Zachrisson & Kulbotten, 2006). Perhaps these mixed findings may be because attachment anxiety and avoidance are distinctively associated with different disordered eating behaviors, not diagnostic categories (Zachrisson & Skarderud, 2010). Thus, we hypothesized that attachment anxiety would be associated with binge eating behavior and that attachment avoidance would be associated with restricted eating behavior because of the distinct emotion regulation dif-ficulties associated with attachment anxiety versus avoidance.

Page 4: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1069

The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties

Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating behav-iors as efforts to regulate dysfunctional emotions. For example, binge eating could be viewed as an effort to escape from negative self-consciousness (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991), self-medication for distress with foods (Schlundt & Johnson, 1990), or the replacement of painful emotions with less painful emotions (Kenardy, Arnow, & Agras, 1996). Research supports these perspectives. For example, a review reported that state negative affect tended to precede binge eating episodes among women, suggesting that when some people feel overwhelmed by negative emotions they binge eat as an immedi-ate emotion regulatory effort (Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011). Also, emotion reg-ulation difficulties, such as finding it hard to stay goal directed when feeling emotionally overwhelmed, and having difficulty gaining a clear understand-ing of one’s feelings, have been associated with binge eating among a com-bined sample of college men and women (Han & Pistole, 2014). Thus, a tendency to feel intense negative emotions may be related to binge eating.

Anorexic symptoms (e.g., restricted eating), on the other hand, have been conceptualized as an effort to avoid experiencing emotions (Wildes & Marcus, 2011). Clinical observations and research suggest that restricted eat-ing may serve to numb negative emotions (Harrison, Sullivan, Tchanturia, & Treasure, 2009), avoid experiencing or expressing emotional states (Wildes & Markus, 2011), or soothe anxiety (Strober, 2004) among women. Consistently, anorexic symptoms have been associated with low interocep-tive awareness (Pollatos et al., 2008), difficulty recognizing emotions in one-self and others (Bydlowski et al., 2005), alexithymia (Rastam, Gillberg, Gillberg, & Johansson, 1997), and emotional inhibition (Holliday, Uher, Landau, Collier, & Treasure, 2006), in samples of women. Thus, whereas emotional reactivity (i.e., underregulated emotions and difficulty controlling overwhelming emotions) may be associated with binge eating, emotional cutoff (i.e., overregulated emotions and difficulty experiencing and express-ing emotions) may be associated with restricted eating, at least among women.

These two emotion regulation difficulties—emotional reactivity and emo-tional cutoff—correspond to attachment anxiety’s hyperactivating strategy and attachment avoidance’s deactivating strategy, respectively (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Thus, attachment anxiety is conceptually related to emotional reactivity, which is also related to binge eating; by contrast, attachment avoidance is conceptually related to emotional cutoff, which is also related to restricted eating. A few studies have examined the mediating roles of emotion regulation difficulties between attachment insecurity and disordered eating, but they either did not compare binge eating versus restricted eating

Page 5: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1070 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

separately or did not distinguish emotional reactivity from emotional cutoff. For example, general emotion regulation difficulties mediated the link between both attachment anxiety and avoidance and binge eating in a com-bined sample of college men and women (Han & Pistole, 2014), and another study found mediation with general disordered eating symptoms as the dependent variable among college women (Ty & Francis, 2013). In the only study of which we are aware that examined distinct mediators of emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff for attachment anxiety and avoidance, emo-tional reactivity mediated the link between attachment anxiety and disordered eating symptoms, but emotional cutoff did not mediate the link between attachment avoidance and disordered eating among a clinical sample of women (Tasca et al., 2009). Thus, we aimed to extend this research to differ-ent forms of disordered eating (i.e., binge eating and restricted eating).

Gender Differences in the Proposed Model

As reviewed previously, most research on the links among attachment, emo-tion regulation, and disordered eating has focused on women. Research on men’s disordered eating has increased recently, however, demonstrating that a substantial minority of men report disordered eating behaviors (Wade, Keski-Rahkonen, & Hudson, 2011). The current literature on disordered eat-ing in men has largely focused on their unique body image issues of desire for muscularity (vs. thinness in women) and associated behaviors such as exces-sive protein consumption or exercise (O’Dea & Abraham, 2002). Thus, scholars have argued that symptom constellations of eating disorder diagno-ses may be different for men from women, and thus current eating disorder measures may not validly capture eating disorders among men (e.g., Strother, Lemberg, Stanford, & Turberville, 2012). However, men and women may be similar in how specific behaviors (i.e., not symptom constellations for diag-noses) of binge eating and restricted eating are manifested. Research also shows that it is not dissatisfaction with less muscle but the fear of gaining fat that was associated with disordered eating among men (Smith, Hawkeswood, Bodell, & Joiner, 2011). This suggests that men may engage in fasting and binge eating in a similar manner to women—to avoid gaining fat.

There have been inconclusive findings among the few existing studies that examined gender differences on the association between attachment and dis-ordered eating. In one study, the authors found gender similarity in that inse-cure attachment was associated with pathological eating behaviors and attitudes among both college men and women (Huprich, Stepp, Graham, & Johnson, 2004). Other researchers reported some gender differences. For example, fearful attachment (characterized by both high attachment anxiety

Page 6: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1071

and high attachment avoidance) was associated with bulimic symptoms in college women, whereas secure attachment (characterized by both low attachment anxiety and low attachment avoidance) was associated with drive for thinness in college men (Elgin & Pritchard, 2006). Attachment anxiety was also associated with dieting in men and with general disordered eating symptoms, dieting, and bulimia in women, whereas attachment avoidance was associated with general disordered eating symptoms in men and with only dieting in women (Koskina & Giovazolians, 2010). In sum, attachment anxiety and avoidance seem to be associated with disordered eating in both women and men, but it is far from clear whether attachment anxiety and avoidance are distinctively associated with binge eating versus restricted eat-ing behaviors between genders.

Gender differences on the association between emotion regulation diffi-culties and disordered eating are not conclusive either. Some researchers have reported gender similarities. For example, general emotion regulation difficulties were associated with disordered eating among college men (Lavender & Anderson, 2010) and college women (Ty & Francis, 2013). Impulsivity was associated with disordered eating behaviors and attitudes both in college men and women (Lundahl, Wahlstrom, Christ, & Stoltenberg, 2015). Similarly, negative urgency—the propensity to act rashly when dis-tressed—was associated with binge eating in both men and women (e.g., Davis-Becker, Peterson, & Fischer, 2014). In contrast, Perry and Hayaki (2014) found that alexithymia—difficulty recognizing and describing one’s feelings, similar to emotional cutoff—was associated with bulimic symptoms among college men, whereas emotional impulse strength (similar to emo-tional reactivity) was associated with bulimic symptoms among college women. Given the inconclusive findings related to gender differences in emotion regulation difficulties and disordered eating, it is necessary for future work to further clarify these relationships.

The Present Study

Using the proposed model, we hypothesized that attachment insecurity and disordered eating would be associated through emotion regulation difficulties (i.e., emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff; see Figure 1). We tested all possible pathways but were particularly interested in two pathways. Specifically, we predicted that (a) attachment anxiety would be associated with emotional reactivity which, in turn, would be associated with binge eat-ing, and that (b) attachment avoidance would be associated with emotional cutoff which, in turn, would be associated with restricted eating, as the pri-mary indirect relations based on attachment theory. To test the model, we

Page 7: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1072 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

used nonclinical samples of college women and men because binge eating and restricted eating are highly prevalent in nonclinical populations, yet affect mental and physical health negatively (Stice, Marti, Shaw, & Jaconis, 2009). Also, subclinical disordered eating behaviors appear to be similar in their repercussions and correlates except for the severity (e.g., Jansen, van den Hout, & Griez, 1990), and warrant psychological intervention and pre-ventive efforts.

We also explored potential gender differences in this model. We could not find a consistent conceptual or empirical rationale for a gender difference between insecure attachment and binge eating versus restricted eating. However, based on Perry and Hayaki’s (2014) finding that emotional expres-sivity and alexithymia were associated with disordered eating in men and women, respectively, we predicted that emotional reactivity may be more strongly associated with disordered eating for women than men, whereas emotional cutoff may be more strongly associated with disordered eating for men than women.

Method

Participants

A total of 1,048 college students at a U.S. Midwestern university who took psychology courses were recruited from the psychology department research participation pool. Among the initial respondents, 104 who either did not

Figure 1. Structural equation model of the relations among attachment insecurities, emotion regulation, and disordered eating. Coefficients for women (n = 744) are not italicized above, and coefficients for men (n = 200) are in italicized typeface below. Standardized coefficients are displayed.*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Page 8: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1073

complete more than half of the survey, did not provide sufficient demographic information, or appeared to respond randomly (e.g., reporting age as 2) were removed. The final sample of 944 included 744 (79%) women and 200 (21%) men with a mean age of 19.54 (SD = 2.38) for women and 19.94 (SD = 3.56) for men. The majority of participants identified as European American (76%; 577 women, 143 men), with African American individuals comprising 11% (80 women, 22 men), Latinos 7% (52 women, 16 men), Asian Americans 2% (10 women, six men), and the remaining 4% endorsing other or multiple ethnic identities. The sample reflected the ethnic composition of the student population at the university.

Instruments

Attachment. The 12-item Experiences in Close Relationships-Short Form (Wei, Russell, Mallinckrodt, & Vogel, 2007) measured adult attachment. It includes attachment anxiety (6 items; e.g., “I need a lot of reassurance that I am loved by my partner”) and attachment avoidance (6 items; e.g., “I am nervous when partners get too close to me”) dimensions. Participants rate each item on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly). Higher scores indicate higher levels of attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for this study were .76 for men and .78 for women on attachment anxiety, and .80 for men and .86 for women on attachment avoidance.

Emotion regulation difficulties. Two subscales of the Differentiation of Self Inventory (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998) measured emotion regulation dif-ficulties. The 11-item emotional reactivity subscale measures the degree to which a person responds to environmental stimuli with emotional flooding, emotional lability, or hypersensitivity (e.g., “At times I feel as if I’m riding an emotional roller-coaster”). The 12-item emotional cutoff subscale measures the degree to which a person feels threatened by intimacy and avoids intense experiences (e.g., “I have difficulty expressing my feelings to people I care for”). Each item is rated on a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all true of me) to 6 (very true of me). Higher scores indicate higher levels of emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff, respectively. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were reported as .88 and .79 for scores on emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff, respectively (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were .87 for men and .86 for women on emotional reactiv-ity and .92 for men and .90 for women on emotional cutoff, respectively.

Binge eating. The 16-item Binge Eating Scale (BES; Gormally, Black, Das-ton, & Rardin, 1982) measured binge eating behaviors and associated

Page 9: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1074 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

feelings and cognitions (e.g., feeling a lack of control). Each of the 16 items lists three to four statements that range from normal to problematic eating. For example, an item has four statements such as: (a) “I rarely eat so much food that I feel uncomfortably stuffed afterwards” (weighted 0); (b) “Usually about once a month, I eat such a quantity of food, I end up feeling very stuffed” (weighted 1); (c) “I have regular periods during the month when I eat large amounts of food, either at mealtime or at snacks” (weighted 2); and (d) “I eat so much food that I regularly feel quite uncomfortable after eating and sometimes a bit nauseous” (weighted 3). Scores of the 16 items are summed such that higher scores indicate more severe binge eating. The BES demon-strated concurrent validity by differentiating people with severe binge eating from people with mild or no binge eating (Gormally et al., 1982). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for this study was .88 for men and .89 for women.

Restricted eating. The 10-item Restrained Eating subscale of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (van Strien, Frigters, Bergers, & Defareds, 1986) measured a restricted-eating tendency to control weight (e.g., “Do you delib-erately eat less in order not to become heavier?”). The response format is a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). The internal consis-tency of scores from the subscale has been reported to range from .92 to .95 among various samples (van Strien et al., 1986). The subscale scores also have high factorial validity and concurrent validity as indicated by correla-tions with other restrained eating scales (e.g., the restraint scale of the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire; Stunkard & Messick, 1985). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was .93 for men and .94 for women in this study.

Procedure

A brief study description and URL to the online survey were posted on the research participation management site as an option for students to complete for research credit. After reading the consent form on the first page of the survey, participants clicked the ‘agree to participate’ button to proceed to complete the survey.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Means, standard deviations, and correlation coefficients of the study vari-ables are presented in Table 1. The strongest correlations for both women and men were between attachment anxiety and emotional reactivity and between attachment avoidance and emotional cutoff. Based on Gormally et al.’s

Page 10: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1075

(1982) cutoffs, 26.8% of college women (n = 199) and 18.5% of college men (n = 37) in our study reported mild to severe binge eating. Although there is no cutoff score provided for restricted eating, restricted eating scores were significantly higher than the mean score reported for the community samples with which the scale was developed, t = 4.37, p < .001, d = .24, for women, t = 6.19, p < .001, d = .52, for men (van Strien et al., 1986).

Given our use of structural equational modeling (SEM), we created item parcels for each of the variables in our hypothesized model. The item-to-construct method was used to create four parcels as indicators of binge eating and three parcels for all the other latent variables (Little, Cunningham, Shahar, & Widaman, 2002). There were some missing data at the parcel level, yet nearly all participants (97%) had no more than one missing parcel; the maximum number of missing parcels for any given participant was six (out of 19). Little et al.’s missing completely at random test was not significant, χ2(745) = 792.59, p = .11, thus supporting the possibility that the data were missing completely at random. We used the Mplus MLR estimator, which accounts for missing data using full-information maximum likelihood. Minimum covariance coverage was .95, further suggesting that missing data were not problematic.

Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 M SD

1. Attachment anxiety

— .18* .51*** .32*** .32*** .12 3.68 1.14

2. Attachment avoidance

.28*** — .12 .53*** .28*** .17* 2.93 1.13

3. Emotional reactivity

.54*** .05 — .60*** .31*** .06 3.02 0.96

4. Emotional cutoff

.33*** .62*** .25*** — .41*** .14 2.48 1.07

5. Binge eating .30*** .18*** .28*** .30*** — .49*** 9.76 8.116. Restricted

eating.19*** .15*** .18*** .27*** .47*** — 2.25 0.84

M 4.06 2.73 3.74 2.40 12.97 2.71 — —SD 1.15 1.18 0.97 1.00 8.55 0.94 — —Possible range 1–7 1–7 1–6 1–6 1–46 1–5 — —

Note. Data for men (n = 200) are above the diagonal; data for women (n = 744) are below the diagonal.*p < .05. ***p < .001.

Page 11: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1076 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

Distributions of the item parcels showed neither univariate skewness nor kurtosis, with maximum skewness of 1.18 and maximum kurtosis of 1.30. Nevertheless, the MLR estimation method provides standard errors and a chi-square statistic that are robust to nonnormality (Muthén & Muthén, 2010). Sizes of the two samples were adequate to detect a medium effect size with six latent variables with 19 indicators (minimum required sample size = 177; Westland, 2010).

Overview of Model Testing

We first tested the fit of the measurement model both separately by gender and in a multiple-groups model. This allowed us to examine measurement invariance, that is, whether the indicator variables mapped onto the latent variables in the same way for women as for men. We also included a test of latent mean structures to assess whether the means of the latent variables dif-fered by gender. We then assessed the fit of the structural model in Figure 1 to data from women and men separately, and we determined which indirect effects were statistically significant. We also tested this structural model in a multiple-groups analysis that allowed us to determine whether the structural paths differed by gender. In all cases, acceptable model fit was indicated by values of .95 or greater on the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), values of .06 or lower on the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and values of .08 or lower on the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual. Difference in fit between pairs of nested models was determined with the chi-square difference test as well as the difference in the values of the other fit indices.

Measurement Model

In the measurement model, all latent variables were allowed to correlate with one another. The fit indices for the measurement model for the sample of women indicated that the 19 indicators mapped onto the six latent constructs very well (see Table 2). Standardized factor loadings ranged from .59 to .96, p < .001. Correlations among latent variables ranged from .10 to .67. For the sample of college men, the measurement model’s fit to the data was margin-ally adequate (see Table 2). Standardized factor loadings among men ranged from .61 to .97, and correlations among latent variables ranged from .10 to .65.

Measurement invariance. To compare the fit of the measurement model as a function of gender, we followed Byrne’s (2012) recommended procedures

Page 12: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1077

for conducting multiple groups analyses. First, we tested a configural model in which the same pattern of fixed and free parameters was specified for both groups. Except for fixing all factor variances to 1 (to facilitate model identi-fication), no equivalence constraints were imposed on any parameters. This configural measurement model provided a good fit to the data (see Table 2).

Next, we tested a model with equality constraints on the factor loadings such that the associations between the 19 indicator variables and the six latent variables were the same for women as for men. This constrained model also provided a good fit to the data (see Table 2), and the other fit indices were no worse than in the prior model. Moreover, a chi-square difference test, which accounts for the scaling correction factor given the robust estimation method,

Table 2. Structural Equation Model Fit Indices

Model χ2 df CFI TLI RMSEA 90% CI SRMR

Measurement models Women only (n = 744) 420.59 137 .97 .96 .05 [.05, .06] .04 Men only (n = 200) 277.08 137 .94 .93 .07 [.06, .08] .07 Configural 699.58 274 .96 .95 .06 [.05, .06] .05 Constrained factor

loadings710.31 293 .96 .96 .06 [.05, .06] .05

Prior model + constrained factor covariances

730.57 308 .96 .96 .05 [.05, .05] .06

Prior model + constrained intercepts

755.81 327 .96 .96 .05 [.05, .06] .06

Prior model + varying factor means and variances

746.40 315 .96 .96 .05 [.05, .06] .06

Structural models Women only (n = 744) 432.95 141 .97 .96 .05 [.05, .06] .05 Men only (n = 200) 278.85 141 .95 .93 .07 [.06, .08] .07 Configural 713.75 282 .96 .95 .06 [.05, .06] .05 Constrained factor

loadings, intercepts, covariances, and structural coefficients

765.23 331 .96 .96 .05 [.05, .06] .06

Prior model + two varying structural paths (post hoc)

751.40 329 .96 .96 .05 [.05, .06] .06

Note. df = degrees of freedom; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CI = confidence interval; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.

Page 13: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1078 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

indicated that the model with constrained factor loadings did not provide a significantly worse fit to the data than the configural model, Δχ2(19) = 10.74, p = .93. Thus, the fit of indicators to latent variables did not differ by gender.

The next model added additional between group constraints, this time for factor covariances. Because testing the invariance of the error covariances is considered unnecessary (Byrne, 2012), residual variances of indicators were allowed to vary. Intercepts were also free to vary between groups. This model also provided a good fit to the data (see Table 2) and did not provide a signifi-cantly worse fit than either the previous model, Δχ2(15) = 20.26, p = .16, nor the configural model, Δχ2(34) = 30.99, p = .62. The CFI, TLI, and RMSEA indicated as good of a fit in this new model as in the configural model. Thus, measurement invariance was supported.

Structured means modeling. Because the fit of the measurement model did not differ by gender, the meaning of the latent variables likely did not differ by gender. We therefore examined whether the means of these latent variables differed. We estimated a model that, in addition to specifying equal factor loadings and factor covariances, specified that the intercepts of the indicator variables were equal. This model also provided a good fit to the data (Table 2). Moreover, we concluded that it did not provide a significantly worse fit to the data than the previous model based on the chi-square difference test, Δχ2(19) = 25.24, p = .15, and the fact that other fit indices did not change.

Latent variable means and variances were constrained to be equal between groups in all prior models. Thus, we next allowed these six means and six vari-ances to differ between groups. Comparing this model to the prior model tested the null hypothesis that the means and variances of the latent variables did not differ by gender. Once again, this was a good-fitting model (see Table 2). Moreover, it did not provide a significantly worse fit to the data than the previ-ous model, Δχ2(12) = 9.41, p = .67, and the other fit indices did not change from the previous model. The differences in means (whereby negative differences indicate higher means for women) were estimated to be -.04 for attachment anxiety, p = .64; -.17 for attachment avoidance, p = .06; -.11 for emotional reactivity, p = .25; -.03 for emotional cutoff, p = .71; -.03 for binge eating, p = .74; and -.02 for restricted eating, p = .79. Thus, there was no reason to believe that the means of the latent variables differed between genders.

Structural Model

Our primary goal was to test the fit of the hypothesized model in Figure 1 for women and men. Among women, the hypothesized model fit the data well (see Table 2), yet the structural model fit significantly worse than the

Page 14: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1079

Table 3. Estimates of Standardized Bias-Corrected Bootstrapped Indirect Effects

Independent variable

Mediatorvariable

Dependent variable

Women Men

Indirect effect 99% CI

Indirect effect 99% CI

Attachment anxiety

Emotional reactivity

Binge eating .16* [.07, .23] .31* [.14, .48]

Attachment anxiety

Emotional cutoff Binge eating .04* [.01, .08] .01 [–.03, .04]

Attachment anxiety

Emotional reactivity

Restricted eating

.09* [.02, .17] .09 [–.08, .25]

Attachment anxiety

Emotional cutoff Restricted eating

.03* [.00, .05] .03 [–.03, .09]

Attachment avoidance

Emotional reactivity

Binge eating −.03* [–.06, .00] −.07 [–.17, .02]

Attachment avoidance

Emotional cutoff Binge eating .20* [.13, .27] .03 [–.10, .16]

Attachment avoidance

Emotional reactivity

Restricted eating

.02 [–.04, .00] −.02 [–.07, .03]

Attachment avoidance

Emotional cutoff Restricted eating

.11* [.05, .18] .15* [.02, .28]

Note. CI = confidence interval.*p < .01.

measurement model, Δχ2(4) = 12.36, p = .01. Nevertheless, other fit indices were virtually identical between the measurement and structural models. Standardized coefficients are displayed in Figure 1. Attachment anxiety was positively associated with emotional reactivity, and attachment avoidance was negatively associated with emotional reactivity, R2 = .38. Emotional cut-off (R2 = .47) was positively associated with both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. The stronger paths by far were between (a) attachment anxiety and emotional reactivity and (b) attachment avoidance and emotional cutoff. Binge eating (R2 = .20) was positively associated with both emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff. Finally, restricted eating (R2 = .07) was posi-tively associated with both emotional cutoff and emotional reactivity.

Eight indirect effects are implied by the structural model (see Table 3). We tested the significance of these indirect effects by bootstrapping 10,000 sam-ples with replacement. Table 3 displays the bias-corrected estimates of the standardized indirect effects. Because of the large number of indirect effects, we used a conservative alpha level of .01 to report the 99% confidence inter-vals of the indirect effects in Table 3. Seven of the eight indirect effects were statistically significant among women (with the exception being the indirect

Page 15: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1080 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

effect from attachment avoidance to restricted eating via emotional reactiv-ity). Three of these indirect effects were substantial in size (i.e., larger than .10), and two of these were the primary pathways we hypothesized: (a) attachment anxiety to binge eating via emotional reactivity and (b) attach-ment avoidance to restricted eating via emotional cutoff. The third substantial indirect effect was attachment avoidance to binge eating via emotional cutoff.

For men, the structural model provided a reasonable, if not ideal, fit (see Table 2). The structural model did not provide a significantly worse fit to the data than the measurement model based on the values of the other fit indices and the chi-square difference test, Δχ2(4) = 1.97, p = .74. Standardized coef-ficients are displayed in Figure 1 (italicized typeface). Attachment anxiety was positively, and attachment avoidance was negatively, associated with emotional reactivity, R2 = .45. Emotional cutoff (R2 = .43) was positively associated with attachment avoidance, but attachment anxiety was not associ-ated with emotional cutoff. Binge eating (R2 = .20) was positively associated with emotional reactivity but not with emotional cutoff. Finally, restricted eating (R2 = .10) was associated with emotional cutoff but not emotional reactivity.

As with the sample of women, we tested the significance of the eight pos-sible indirect effects via bootstrapping (see Table 3). Only two indirect effects were significant among men, and these were the two that we hypothesized would be primary pathways between attachment and disordered eating: (a) attachment anxiety to binge eating via emotional reactivity and (b) attach-ment avoidance to restricted eating via emotional cutoff. The substantial indi-rect effect between attachment avoidance and binge eating (via emotional cutoff) that was significant among women was not significant among men.

Our final step was to determine whether the model in Figure 1 provided a similar fit to women and men. We estimated a configural model in which the same pattern of coefficients was specified for both women and men, but the estimates of these parameters were allowed to vary across gender. The con-figural structural model provided a good fit to the data (see Table 2). Thus, the multigroup representation of the model in Figure 1 was empirically supported.

The comparison model constrained the 19 factor loadings, 19 intercepts, eight structural paths (i.e., betas), and three factor covariances to be equal between groups. This heavily constrained model also provided a good fit to the data with fit indices that were comparable to those from the configural model, and it did not provide a significantly worse fit to the data than the structural configural model, Δχ2(49) = 51.48, p = .38. Thus, the model in Figure 1 provided a similar fit for both women and men. Nevertheless, an

Page 16: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1081

inspection of the structural paths indicated potentially meaningful differences in the magnitude of the paths between emotional reactivity and binge eating (.23 for women, .43 for men) and between emotional cutoff and binge eating (.32 for women, .05 for men). We therefore tested an additional, post hoc model similar to the fully constrained structural model but in which these two paths were allowed to vary by gender. This model provided a good fit to the data (with comparable fit indices as compared to the prior model), and it also provided a significantly better fit to the data than the fully constrained model, Δχ2(2) = 13.83, p < .001. Thus, although the model as a whole did not differ across gender, these two structural paths did.

Discussion

We examined the differential associations between (a) attachment anxiety and avoidance and (b) binge eating and restricted eating, via distinct emotion regulation difficulties of emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff, among college women and men. Our results revealed that, as hypothesized, attach-ment anxiety was associated with binge eating via emotional reactivity, and attachment avoidance was associated with restricted eating via emotional cutoff, in both college women and men. Theoretically, attachment anxiety’s hyperactivating strategy of intensifying emotional reactions (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007) may demand immediate regulatory efforts such as binge eat-ing. One the other hand, attachment avoidance’s deactivating strategy of sup-pressing emotional reactions and maintaining restricted emotionality could contribute to restricted eating motivations and behaviors.

Our results may explain some mixed findings on the associations between attachment and different eating disorder diagnoses. Some researchers reported the distinct associations between attachment anxiety and bulimia nervosa and between attachment avoidance and anorexia nervosa (e.g., Zachrisson & Skarderud, 2010), whereas others did not find such distinct associations between insecure attachment dimensions and eating disorder diagnostic categories (Broberg et al., 2001; Zachrisson & Kulbotten, 2006). We showed that it might not be the diagnostic categories but the disordered eating behaviors that are distinctively associated with attachment anxiety and avoidance (Orzolek-Kronner, 2002; Ringer & Crittenden, 2007).

Also, Tasca et al. (2009) did not find the indirect role of emotional cutoff between attachment avoidance and general disordered-eating symptoms among a clinical sample of women. By differentiating binge eating and restricted eating, we were able to show that, consistent with the theory, emo-tional cutoff did mediate the association between attachment avoidance and restricted eating, in addition to replicating the mediating role of emotional

Page 17: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1082 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

reactivity in the association between attachment anxiety and binge eating. This was the first study, to our knowledge, that demonstrated the distinct indirect pathways by emotional reactivity and cutoff between insecure attach-ment and disordered eating.

It is noteworthy that our results supported the hypothesized paths among both college women and men. Despite the dearth of research on men, these results are consistent with the few existing studies reporting that negative affect (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003) and insecure attachment (Elgin & Pritchard, 2006; Huprich et al., 2004; Koskina & Giovazolians, 2010) are associated with disordered eating among men.

However, we did find several important gender differences on the links between emotion regulation difficulties and disordered eating. In addition to the distinct pathways that were hypothesized, other indirect pathways were significant in only women. Specifically, attachment anxiety was associated with binge eating also via emotional cutoff and with restricted eating via emotional reactivity. Attachment avoidance was also associated with binge eating via emotional cutoff. Perhaps attachment insecurity is a general risk factor for various emotion regulation difficulties and disordered eating among women, whereas attachment anxiety versus avoidance serves distinct contri-butions to binge eating versus restricted eating. The generalized pathways partly came from the significant association between emotional cutoff and binge eating among women. Based on Perry and Hayaki’s (2014) finding that showed alexithymia as being associated with disordered eating among men, we had predicted that emotional cutoff would be more strongly associated with disordered eating in men than in women. Emotional cutoff was associ-ated with restricted eating in women and men to a similar degree, but it was not associated with binge eating in men. Binge eating has been conceptual-ized as an effort to escape from negative emotions such as painful self-aware-ness (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991), which may be related to the tendency to avoid confronting negative emotions (i.e., emotional cutoff). This specula-tion was supported only for women, perhaps because relying on binge eating to regulate negative emotions is more culturally acceptable for women than for men. Men may be more likely to engage in other ways of suppressing negative emotions, such as drinking, that are more socially acceptable (Capraro, 2000).

Also, attachment avoidance is associated with low levels of self-reported distress (Mikulincer, Florian, & Tolmacz, 1990) but high physiological indi-cators of distress (e.g., Sonnby-Borgstrom & Jonsson, 2004). It is possible that women with attachment avoidance cut off their emotional distress at the explicit level, but the “suppressed” emotional distress may still demand regu-latory effort of binge eating at the implicit level. In other words, emotional

Page 18: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1083

suppression might not be as effective of a coping strategy for women as for men because they experience more intense negative emotions compared to men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012).

Based on Perry and Hayaki’s (2014) finding that impulse strength was associated with disordered eating in women, we had predicted that emotional reactivity would be more strongly associated with disordered eating in women than in men. Emotional reactivity was associated with both binge eat-ing and restricted eating in women, but emotional reactivity was more strongly associated with binge eating in men than in women. Noteworthy was that there was no significant mean difference on emotional reactivity between men and women, which is inconsistent with the previous research that shows that women tend to experience more intense emotions than men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012). Perhaps the level of emotional reactivity between men and women is not the critical difference; instead, the important gender difference may be the implication that emotional reactivity has for disordered eating. In other words, because men are usually less reactive emotionally than women, when men do experience emotional reactivity, they are at greater risk for engaging in binge eating than women (Perry & Hayaki, 2014). Because there are mixed findings on whether men are less emotionally reactive than women, future research may investigate the ways men experience emotional reactiv-ity and the relationship to various disordered eating behaviors.

Clinical Implications

More than 20% of our nonclinical participants reported mild to severe levels of binge eating, and the mean level of restricted eating among our samples was higher compared to that in the sample (van Strien et al., 1986) used in previous research. Although the cutoff score for clinical levels of restricted eating is not available, and the sample size of those with a clinical level of binge eating did not allow for group comparisons, the apparent high preva-lence of disordered eating in our samples warrants discussion of clinical implications of our findings.

Counseling psychologists who serve college men and women with disor-dered eating may address attachment insecurity, different emotion regulation difficulties, and their contributions to different disordered eating behaviors. First, for people who binge eat, it may be useful to explore their attachment anxiety issues (e.g., fear of rejection in their attachment relationship), result-ing negative emotions, and subsequent compelling urges to binge eat to soothe those negative emotions. Second, for people who restrict their eating, it may be useful to explore attachment avoidance issues (e.g., anticipated rejection and fear for intimacy in their attachment relationship), resulting

Page 19: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1084 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

emotional and interpersonal suppression, and subsequent generalization of such suppressive tendencies in the form of restricted eating. Third, for people who engage in both binge eating and restricted eating alternately, it may be useful to explore distinct attachment-related antecedents for these two disor-dered eating behaviors (e.g., perceived rejection versus anticipated rejection) and intervening dysfunctional emotion regulation (e.g., intensifying versus suppressing). As such, clients may (a) gain insight about potential attachment meaning of foods and eating behaviors, (b) develop a more functional and realistic view of and relationship with foods that do not reflect their underly-ing attachment relationship (e.g., appreciating foods without feeling guilty but not overindulging in them like they want affection from the attachment figure; Bacon, 2010), and (c) learn how to address negative emotions result-ing from perceived insecurity in the attachment relationships.

Working on their attachment relationships and emotion regulation strate-gies directly may also help. Establishing the therapeutic alliance as a secure attachment relationship, improving romantic and family relationships, or developing secondary secure attachment relationships (e.g., friendships) may be useful by reducing the need for excessive proximity seeking to, or avoid-ance of, foods (e.g., Mallinckrodt, Daly, & Wang, 2010). Also, helping clients with becoming aware of their emotions, confronting them, naming them, understanding and accepting them, and therefore not being overwhelmed by them may be useful (e.g., Robinson, Dolhanty, & Greenberg, 2015) in reduc-ing the emotion regulatory needs for disordered eating behaviors. In particu-lar, addressing different mental health implications of emotional reactivity versus emotional cutoff for men and women would provide a gender-sensi-tive intervention for disordered eating.

Limitations and Future Research Ideas

There are limitations in this study that warrant future research. First, in this study we did not examine gender-specific body image issues or the sociocul-tural factor of body objectification (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) as contrib-uting factors to disordered eating. Future research should address interrelationships between these variables and the current study’s variables to provide a comprehensive picture for how disordered eating arises. Perhaps the larger social context that presses objectification of one’s body may increase body dissatisfaction and body image issues, which, in turn, may lead to disordered eating. In doing so, internalizing the thin body ideal may lead someone to engage in disordered eating behavior (e.g., dieting and then rebound binge eating) to compensate for the felt insecurity in the adult attach-ment relationship (e.g., perceived rejection by the partner). Future studies

Page 20: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1085

should test these speculations. Second, we did not assess disordered eating behaviors typically associated with men, such as protein consumption or excessive exercise. Future research should address whether the proposed path model is supported when examining these disordered eating behaviors among men. Third, this study was cross-sectional and correlational. No interpreta-tions of causal relationships among the variables should be made. Future research using experience sampling or daily diary methods should test the temporal or causal order of these relationships. Such designs would also allow for the examination of possible within-person variations of binge eat-ing and restricted eating as a function of attachment anxiety versus avoidance and various emotion regulation difficulties and emotional states. For exam-ple, within one individual, with some situational changes (e.g., perceived rejection), attachment anxiety could exacerbate emotional distress, compel-ling that person to seek quick self-medication of binge eating as symbolic proximity seeking. On the other hand, with other situational changes (e.g., anticipated rejection), attachment avoidance could exacerbate the discomfort with intimacy and suppression efforts, leading to restricted eating. Fourth, we used a nonclinical sample of college students. The study results may not be generalized to clinical or community samples nor college samples with dif-ferent demographic factors. Demographic factors such as social status, race, or ethnicity need to be examined as potential moderators.

Conclusion

This study showed that attachment anxiety and avoidance were associated with binge eating and restricted eating through distinct emotion regulation difficulties. This result suggests that clinicians may consider how specific attachment insecurities and emotional reactivity or suppression tendencies contribute to binge eating or restricted eating distinctively among their women and men clients. Importantly, clinicians and researchers could attend to disordered eating behaviors (versus diagnostic categories) and how they manifest themselves over time depending on situationally activated attach-ment insecurities and emotion regulation difficulties.

Authors’ Note

Portions of this paper were presented at the 2016 Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Page 21: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1086 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This study was supported by the Illinois State University New Faculty Initiative Grant awarded to the first author.

ORCID iD

Suejung Han http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1016-8179

References

Agras, W. S. (2001). The consequences and costs of the eating disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24, 371–379. doi:10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70232-X

American College Health Association. (2010). National college health assessment: Reference group data report. Retrieved from http://www.acha-ncha.org/docs/ACHA-NCHA-II_ReferenceGroup_DataReport_Spring2010.pdf

Bacon, L. (2010). Health at every size: The surprising truth about your weight. Dallas, TX: BenBella Books.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human devel-opment. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Broberg, A. G., Hjalmers, I., & Nevonen, L. (2001). Eating disorders, attachment and interpersonal difficulties: A comparison between 18- to 24- year-old patients and normal controls. European Eating Disorders Review, 9, 381–396. doi:10.1002/erv.421

Bydlowski, S., Corcos, M., Jeammet, P., Paterniti, S., Berthoz, S., Laurier, C., … Consoli, S. M. (2005). Emotion-processing deficits in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 37, 321–329. doi:10.1002/eat.20132

Byrne, B. M. (2012). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. New York, NY: Routledge.

Caglar-Nazali, H. P., Corfield, F., Cardi, V., Ambwani, S., Leppanen, J., Olabintan, O., … Treasure, J. (2014). A systematic review and meta-analysis of “Systems for Social Processes” in eating disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 55–92. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.002

Capraro, R. L. (2000). Why college men drink: Alcohol, adventure, and the para-dox of masculinity. Journal of American College Health, 48, 307–315. doi:10.1080/07448480009596272

Davis-Becker, K., Peterson, C. M., & Fischer, S. (2014). The relationship of train negative urgency and negative affect to disordered eating in men and women. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 9–14. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.010

Eggert, J., Levendosky, A. A., & Klump, K. L. (2007). Relationships among attach-ment styles, personality characteristics, and disordered eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40, 149–155. doi:10.1002/eat.20351

Elgin, J., & Pritchard, M. (2006). Adult attachment and disordered eating in under-graduate men and women. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 21, 25–40. doi:10.1300/J035v21n02_05

Page 22: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1087

Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 123–151.

Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical devel-opments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4, 132–154. doi: 10.1O37//1089-2680.4.2.132

Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: toward under-standing women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x

Gormally, J., Black, S., Daston, S., & Rardin, D. (1982). The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addictive Behaviors, 7, 47–55. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(82)90024-7

Haedt-Matt, A. A., & Keel, P. K. (2011). Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating: A meta-analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 660–681. doi:10.1037/a0023660

Han, S., & Pistole, M. C. (2014). College student binge eating: Insecure attachment and emotion regulation. Journal of College Student Development, 55, 16–29. doi:10.1353/csd.2014.0004

Harrison, A., Sullivan, S., Tchanturia, K., & Treasure, J. L. (2009). Emotion recogni-tion and regulation in anorexia nervosa. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 16, 348–356. doi:10.1002/cpp.628

Heatherton, T. E., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-aware-ness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86–108. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86

Holliday, J., Uher, R., Landau, S., Collier, D., & Treasure, J. (2006). Personality pathology among individuals with a lifetime history of anorexia nervosa. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20, 417–430. doi:10.1521/pedi.2006.20.4.417

Huprich, S. K., Strepp, S. D., Graham, A., & Johnson, L. (2004). Gender differences in dependency, separation, object relations, and pathological eating behavior and attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 801–811. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00153-3

Jansen, A., van den Hout, M., & Griez, E. (1990). Clinical and non-clinical binges. Behavior Research & Therapy, 28, 439–444. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(90)90164-E

Kenardy, J., Arnow, B., & Agras, W. S. (1996). The aversiveness of specific emo-tional states associated with binge-eating in obese subjects. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 30, 839–844. doi:10.3109/00048679609065053

Koskina, N., & Giovazolias, T. (2010). The effect of attachment insecurity in the development of eating disturbances across gender: The role of body dissatisfac-tion. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 144, 449–471. doi:10.1080/00223980.2010.496651

Lavender, J. M., & Anderson, D. A. (2009). Contribution of emotion regulation dif-ficulties to disordered eating body dissatisfaction in college men. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 43, 352–357. doi:10.1002/eat.20705

Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To par-cel or not to parcel: Exploring the question and weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–173. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1

Page 23: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1088 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

Lundahl, A., Wahlstrom, L. C., Christ, C. C., & Stoltenberg, S. F. (2015). Gender dif-ferences in the relationship between impulsivity and disordered eating behaviors and attitudes. Eating Behaviors, 18, 120–124. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.05.004

Mallinckrodt, B., Daly, K., & Wang, C. D. C. (2010). An attachment approach to adult psychotherapy. In J. H. Obegi, & E. Berant (Eds.), Attachment theory and research in clinical work with adults (pp. 234–268). New York, NY: Guilford.

Matthews, C. R. (2004). Examining problem drinking and eating disorders from a gendered perspective. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 3, 67–80. doi:10.1300/J069v23n03_06

McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2003). Sociocultural influences on body image and body changes among adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 5–26. doi:10.1080/00224540309598428

Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 58, 273–280. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.273

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynam-ics, and change. New York, NY: Guildford.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 161–187. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109

O’Dea, J. A., & Abraham, S. (2002). Eating and exercise disorders in young college men. Journal of American college health, 50, 273-278. doi:10.1080/07448480209603445

Orzolek-Kronner, C. (2002). The effect of attachment theory in the development of eating disorders: Can symptoms be proximity-seeking? Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 19, 421–435.

Perry, R. M., & Hayaki, J. (2014). Gender differences in the role of alexithymia and emotional expressivity in disordered eating. Personality and Individual Differences, 71, 60–65. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.029

Pollatos, O., Kurz, A. L., Albrecht, J., Schreder, T., Kleemann, A. M., Schopf, V., … Schandry, R. (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 9, 381–388. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.02.001

Rastam, M., Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I. C., & Johansson, M. (1997). Alexithymia in anorexia nervosa: A controlled study using the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 95, 385–388. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb09650.x

Ringer, F., & Crittenden, P. M. (2007). Eating disorders and attachment: The effects of hidden family processes on eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 15, 119–130. doi:10.1002/erv.761

Robinson, A. L., Dolhanty, J., & Greenberg, L. (2015). Emotion-focused family ther-apy for eating disorders in children and adolescents. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 22, 75–82. doi:10.1002/cpp.1861

Schlundt, D. J., & Johnson, W. G. (1990). Eating disorders: Assessment and treat-ment. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

Page 24: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

Han and Kahn 1089

Schwarze, N. J., Oliver, J. M., & Handal, P. J. (2003). Binge eating as related to nega-tive self-awareness, depression, and avoidance coping in undergraduates. Journal of College Student Development, 44, 644–652. doi:10.1353/csd.2003.0058

Selzer, R., Caust, J., Hibbert, M., Bowes, G., & Patton, G. (1996). The association between secondary amenorrhea and common eating disordered weight control practices in an adolescent population. Journal of Adolescence Health, 19, 56–61. doi:10.1016/1054-139X(95)00229-L

Skowron, E. A., & Friedlander, M. L. (1998). The Differentiation of Self Inventory: Development and initial validation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 235–246. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.45.3.235

Smith, A. R., Hawkeswood, S. E., Bodell, L. P., & Joiner, T. E. (2011). Muscularity versus leanness: An examination of body ideals and predictors of disordered eating in heterosexual and gay college students. Body Image, 8, 232–236. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.03.005

Sonnby-Borgstrom, M., & Jonsson, P. (2004). Dismissing-avoidant pattern of attach-ment and mimicry reactions at different levels of cognitive control. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44, 141–151.

Stice, E., Marti, N., Shaw, H., & Jaconis, M. (2009). An 8-year longitudinal study of the natural history of threshold, subthreshold, and partial eating disorders from a community sample of adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 587–597. doi:10.1037/a0016481

Strother, E., Lemberg, R., Stanford, S. C., & Turberville, D. (2012). Eating disorders in men: Underdiagnosed, undertreated, and misunderstood. Eating Disorders, 20, 346–355. doi:10.1080/10640266.2012.715512

Strober, M. (2004). Managing the chronic, treatment-resistant patient with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 245–255. doi:10.1002/eat.20054

Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 71–83. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8

Tasca, G. A., & Balfour, L. (2014). Attachment and eating disorders: A review of current research. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47, 710–717. doi:10.1002/eat.22302

Tasca, G. A., Szadkowski, L., Illing, V., Trinneer, A., Grenon, R., Demidenko, N., … Bissada, H. (2009). Adult attachment, depression, and eating disorder symptoms: The mediating role of affect regulation strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 662–667. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.006

Ty, M., & Francis, A. J. P. (2013). Insecure attachment and disordered eating in women: The mediating processes of social comparison and emotion dysregula-tion. Eating Behaviors, 21, 154–174. doi:10.1080/10640266.2013.761089

van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295–315. doi:10.1002/1098-108X(198602)5:2<295::AID-EAT2260050209>3.0.CO;2-T

Page 25: Attachment, Emotion Regulation Difficulties, and ... · Han and Kahn 1069 The Role of Emotion Regulation Difficulties Emotion regulation theorists have conceptualized disordered eating

1090 The Counseling Psychologist 45(8)

Wade, T. D., Keski-Rahkonen, A., & Hudson, J. (2011). Epidemiology of eating dis-orders. In M. Tsuang, & M. Tohen (Eds.), Textbook in psychiatric epidemiology (3rd ed., pp. 343–360). New York, NY: Wiley.

Wei, M., Russell, D. W., Mallinckrodt, B., & Vogel, D. L. (2007). The Experiences in Close Relationship Scale (ECR)-short form: Reliability, valid-ity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 88, 187–204. doi:10.1080/00223890701268041

Westland, J. C. (2010). Lower bounds on sample size in structural equation modeling. Electronic Commerce Research & Applications, 9, 476–487.

Wildes, J. E., & Marcus, M. D. (2011). Development of emotion acceptance behav-ior therapy for anorexia nervosa: A case series. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 44, 421–427. doi:10.1002/eat.20826

Zachrisson, H. D., & Kulbotten, G. R. (2006). Attachment in anorexia nervosa: An exploration of associations with eating disorder psychopathology and psychiatric symptoms. Eating and Weight Disorders, 11, 163–170. doi:10.1007/BF03327567

Zachrisson, H. D., & Skarderud, F. (2010). Feelings of insecurity: Review of attach-ment and eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 18, 97–106. doi:10.1002/erv.999

Author Biographies

Suejung Han is an assistant professor in the Department of Psychology at Illinois State University. Her research interests include attachment and emotion regulation in disordered eating, motivations in health behaviors, and diversity issues.

Jeffrey H. Kahn is a professor in the Department of Psychology at Illinois State University. His research interests are in the areas of emotion, self-disclosure, scien-tific development, and quantitative methods. He is a past member of the editorial board of The Counseling Psychologist and a current member of the editorial board of the Journal of Counseling Psychology.