author's personal copy...to test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical...

11
ORIGINAL ARTICLE Effects of PER3 clock gene polymorphisms on aging-related changes of the cerebral cortex Delphine Dewandre 1 Mercedes Atienza 1 Mayely P. Sanchez-Espinosa 1 Jose L. Cantero 1 Received: 17 December 2016 / Accepted: 7 September 2017 Ó Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2017 Abstract Considerable evidence suggests that circadian rhythmicity is progressively disrupted in senescence. Among clock genes, Period3 (PER3) has been associated with circadian phenotypes, homeostatic regulation of sleep, and cognitive performance in young adults. However, the effects of PER3 genotype on aging-related changes in both cognitive function and cortical integrity remain largely unknown. To shed light into this issue, we have investi- gated differences in cognitive performance, patterns of cortical thickness, and cortical glucose consumption in normal elderly subjects homozygous carriers of the short (PER3 4/4 , n = 32) and long repeat alleles (PER3 5/5 , n = 32). Relationships between cognitive performance and cortical thickness/metabolism were further explored for each PER3 genotype. We found that PER3 5/5 carriers had poorer cognitive performance (attention, executive func- tion, semantic memory, and verbal fluency) and lower cortical integrity (structural and functional) than PER3 4/4 . PER3 5/5 further showed thinning of temporo–parietal areas, and reductions of glucose consumption in fronto– temporo–parietal regions bilaterally. Moreover, PER3 5/5 subjects exhibited significant correlations between decreased glucose metabolism in fronto–parietal regions and poorer cognitive flexibility, though only correlations with lower glucose consumption of the supramarginal gyrus distinguished PER3 5/5 from PER3 4/4 groups. Overall, these findings enhance our understanding on the gene– brain interaction in aging, and may have further implica- tions for the detection of subclinical cognitive decline associated with PER3 genotypes in late life. Keywords PER3 Aging Cortical thickness Cortical metabolism PET Cognitive function Introduction Aging is a heterogeneous phenomenon that involves a progressive loss of physiological reserve and deregulation of multiple biological systems, resulting in different levels of resilience and physical health outcomes. Dysfunctions of the master circadian pacemaker, i.e., the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) of the hypothalamus, are common in aging, likely due to alterations in synaptic transmission and cel- lular physiology (Farajnia et al. 2014). Several lines of evidence have further supported a link between circadian dysfunctions and the severity of aging-related pathological conditions, such as neurodegenerative, cardiovascular, metabolic, and mood disorders (Kondratova and Kondratov 2012). Circadian rhythms are regulated by a set of canonical clock genes highly conserved across species, with allelic variants influencing individual rhythms at different levels of organization (Brown et al. 2008). In mammals, the clock machinery of the SCN is maintained by complex tran- scriptional–translational feedback loops involving core clock genes as period (PER1, PER2, and PER3), cryp- tochrome (CRY1 and CRY2), CLOCK, brain and muscle ARNT-like proteins 1 and 2 (BMAL1 and BMAL2), and basic helix–loop–helix transcription factors DEC1 and DEC2 (Reppert and Weaver 2001). A recent body of evi- dence supports that clock gene expression also occurs in & Jose L. Cantero [email protected] 1 Laboratory of Functional Neuroscience, Spanish Network of Excellence for Research on Neurodegenerative Diseases (CIBERNED), Pablo de Olavide University, Ctra. de Utrera Km 1, 41013 Seville, Spain 123 Brain Struct Funct DOI 10.1007/s00429-017-1513-0 Author's personal copy

Upload: others

Post on 25-Sep-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Effects of PER3 clock gene polymorphisms on aging-relatedchanges of the cerebral cortex

Delphine Dewandre1 • Mercedes Atienza1 • Mayely P. Sanchez-Espinosa1 •

Jose L. Cantero1

Received: 17 December 2016 / Accepted: 7 September 2017! Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2017

Abstract Considerable evidence suggests that circadianrhythmicity is progressively disrupted in senescence.

Among clock genes, Period3 (PER3) has been associated

with circadian phenotypes, homeostatic regulation of sleep,and cognitive performance in young adults. However, the

effects of PER3 genotype on aging-related changes in both

cognitive function and cortical integrity remain largelyunknown. To shed light into this issue, we have investi-

gated differences in cognitive performance, patterns of

cortical thickness, and cortical glucose consumption innormal elderly subjects homozygous carriers of the short

(PER34/4, n = 32) and long repeat alleles (PER35/5,

n = 32). Relationships between cognitive performance andcortical thickness/metabolism were further explored for

each PER3 genotype. We found that PER35/5 carriers had

poorer cognitive performance (attention, executive func-tion, semantic memory, and verbal fluency) and lower

cortical integrity (structural and functional) than PER34/4.

PER35/5 further showed thinning of temporo–parietalareas, and reductions of glucose consumption in fronto–

temporo–parietal regions bilaterally. Moreover, PER35/5

subjects exhibited significant correlations between

decreased glucose metabolism in fronto–parietal regions

and poorer cognitive flexibility, though only correlationswith lower glucose consumption of the supramarginal

gyrus distinguished PER35/5 from PER34/4 groups. Overall,

these findings enhance our understanding on the gene–

brain interaction in aging, and may have further implica-tions for the detection of subclinical cognitive decline

associated with PER3 genotypes in late life.

Keywords PER3 ! Aging ! Cortical thickness ! Corticalmetabolism ! PET ! Cognitive function

Introduction

Aging is a heterogeneous phenomenon that involves aprogressive loss of physiological reserve and deregulation

of multiple biological systems, resulting in different levels

of resilience and physical health outcomes. Dysfunctions ofthe master circadian pacemaker, i.e., the suprachiasmatic

nuclei (SCN) of the hypothalamus, are common in aging,

likely due to alterations in synaptic transmission and cel-lular physiology (Farajnia et al. 2014). Several lines of

evidence have further supported a link between circadian

dysfunctions and the severity of aging-related pathologicalconditions, such as neurodegenerative, cardiovascular,

metabolic, and mood disorders (Kondratova and Kondratov

2012).Circadian rhythms are regulated by a set of canonical

clock genes highly conserved across species, with allelicvariants influencing individual rhythms at different levels

of organization (Brown et al. 2008). In mammals, the clock

machinery of the SCN is maintained by complex tran-scriptional–translational feedback loops involving core

clock genes as period (PER1, PER2, and PER3), cryp-

tochrome (CRY1 and CRY2), CLOCK, brain and muscleARNT-like proteins 1 and 2 (BMAL1 and BMAL2), and

basic helix–loop–helix transcription factors DEC1 and

DEC2 (Reppert and Weaver 2001). A recent body of evi-dence supports that clock gene expression also occurs in

& Jose L. [email protected]

1 Laboratory of Functional Neuroscience, Spanish Network ofExcellence for Research on Neurodegenerative Diseases(CIBERNED), Pablo de Olavide University, Ctra. de UtreraKm 1, 41013 Seville, Spain

123

Brain Struct Funct

DOI 10.1007/s00429-017-1513-0

Author's personal copy

Page 2: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

the cerebral cortex (Rath et al. 2013; Li et al. 2013; Lim

et al. 2013; Cermakian et al. 2011; Yang et al. 2007). Thisfinding is of paramount importance for aging because

cortically dependent phenomena such as higher cognitive

functions (Wright et al. 2012; Reid et al. 2011) and solubleamyloid-beta levels (Huang et al. 2012; Kang et al. 2009),

one of the primary neurotoxic agents in Alzheimer’s dis-

ease pathology (Hardy and Selkoe 2012), have showncircadian fluctuations that are largely affected by age.

The PER gene family plays a key role in determining thelength of the circadian oscillation through reversible

phosphorylation and regulated degradation of PER proteins

(Chiu et al. 2008; Virshup et al. 2007). Human PER3contains a variable number of tandem repeat (VNTR)

polymorphism consisting of either 4 or 5 repeated 54-bp

sequences encoding 18 amino acids (PER34 or PER35)(Ebisawa et al. 2001). PER3 polymorphisms have been

associated with circadian phenotypes, cognitive perfor-

mance, and homeostatic regulation of sleep (Dijk andArcher 2010). Thus, previous studies have shown that

young homozygous carriers of the long repeat allele

(PER35/5) have diurnal preference, greater sleep propen-sity, and poorer cognitive performance after sleep depri-

vation than those carrying the two short repeat alleles

(PER34/4) (Maire et al. 2014; Lazar et al. 2012; Groegeret al. 2008; Viola et al. 2007).

The influence of PER3 allele status on the aging brain is

largely unknown, as revealed by the lack of research attissue or macroscopic level. Several studies performed in

peripheral blood cells have shown that the expression

pattern of PER3, but not PER1 and PER2, is altered inolder adults (Ando et al. 2010; Hida et al. 2009), likely

contributing to aging-related changes in sleep homeostasis

and circadian rhythmicity (Viola et al. 2012). As dailyrhythms of PER3 expression are ubiquitous in the human

cerebral cortex (Li et al. 2013; Lim et al. 2013) and PER35/5

carriers are more vulnerable to sleep loss throughout life,in terms of higher homeostatic sleep response and poorer

cognitive performance (Maire et al. 2014; Lazar et al.

2012; Groeger et al. 2008; Viola et al. 2007), our firstprediction is that the structural–functional integrity of the

cerebral cortex should be less preserved in elderly PER35/5

individuals, and hence, cognitive deficits should be moreevident in PER35/5 than in PER34/4 carriers. Given that

executive functions and their neural correlates in the pre-

frontal cortex are highly vulnerable to aging (Buckner2004) and that frontal areas are especially compromised in

sleep-deprived young PER35/5 carriers during executive

performance (Vandewalle et al. 2009), we further predictthat the relationship between decreased frontal integrity

and lower executive function should be stronger in older

PER35/5 than in PER34/4 groups.

To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of

cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography

(FDG-PET), between normal elderly subjects with PER34/4

and PER35/5 polymorphisms, and then evaluated whetherdifferences in cortical integrity (structural and/or func-

tional) between both homozygous genotypes were associ-

ated with variations in cognitive performance.

Materials and methods

Subjects

This study included 180 healthy older (HO) subjects who

participated in different research programs conducted inthe Laboratory of Functional Neuroscience at Pablo de

Olavide University. Participants were primarily recruited

from senior citizen’s associations, health screening pro-grams, and hospital outpatient services. Spanish was the

native and the primary language of all participants. They

showed normal cognitive performance in the neuropsy-chological tests relative to appropriate reference values for

age and education level. Individuals with medical condi-

tions and/or history of conditions that may affect brainstructure or function (e.g., stroke, coronary heart diseases,

obstructive sleep apnea, diabetes, head trauma, neurode-

generative diseases, depression, hydrocephalus, intracra-nial mass, MRI infarcts, and/or the use of psychoactive

medication) were not included in the study. All subjects

showed a global score of 0 (no dementia) in the clinicaldementia rating (CDR) as well as normal independent

function—assessed by the Spanish version of the Interview

for Deterioration in Daily Living Activities (Bohm et al.1998). Depression was excluded (scores B5) by the shorter

version of the Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage et al.

1983). The Ethics Committee of the Pablo de OlavideUniversity approved the study, and participants gave their

written informed consent.

PER3 genotyping

All subjects were genotyped for the PER3 VNTR poly-morphism. Genomic DNA was extracted from whole blood

using the salting-out method (Miller et al. 1988) and stored

at -80 "C until analysis. PER3 genotypes were obtainedwith an allele-specific polymerase chain reaction (PCR)

using the following primers: 50-GTGTCTTTTCA

TGTGCCCTTACT-30 (forward) and 50-TACCCCAATATACCTGACAAAAA-30 (reverse). The PCR was carried

out in a final volume of 30 lL, consisting of 150 ng of

genomic DNA, 10 pM of each primer, 0.20 mM of each

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 3: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

dNTP, 1 9 Green GoTaq# Flexi buffer (Promega), 2 mM

of MgCl2, and 2.5 units of GoTaq# Hot Start Polymerase(Promega). Conditions of amplification included an initial

denaturation step at 95 "C for 2 min, followed by 40 cycles

of denaturation at 95 "C for 1 min, annealing at 58 "C for 1min, and extension at 72 "C for 1 min. The resulting PCR

products were separated by electrophoresis in 2% agarose

gel, allowing us to distinguish the 4-repeat polymorphismwith 453 bp and the 5-repeat polymorphism with 507 bp.

As part of the quality control, we confirmed that the minorallele frequency was above 0.01, and the genotype distri-

bution of the total sample (n = 180) was in Hardy–

Weinberg Equilibrium (v2 = 0.29, p = 0.58). Of screenedelderly subjects (n = 180), 56 (31.1%) were homozygous

carriers of the short repeat allele (PER34/4), 92 (51.1%)

were heterozygous carriers (PER34/5), and 32 (17.8%) werehomozygous carriers of the long repeat allele (PER35/5).

This distribution is relatively similar to the previous studies

for the European population (Lazar et al. 2012; Benedettiet al. 2008).

Results showed in the present study were obtained

comparing a subgroup of normal elderly subjects withPER34/4 and PER35/5 genotypes matched in age and sex

(n = 32 in each group) to rule out potential confounds of

these variables. Although similar analyses were performedincluding all PER3 polymorphisms, results showed that the

PER34/5 group did not differ significantly from the two

other PER3 genotypes (results not shown) in either cog-nitive function or cortical integrity (structural and/or

functional).

Cognitive assessment

A neuropsychological battery covering memory, attention,executive functioning, and language was administered to

all participants in the morning (10:00–12:00). Episodic

memory was assessed with the Spanish version of theverbal paired associates subtest (immediate and delayed

forms) from the Wechsler Memory Scale-III, and

semantic memory was evaluated with the Spanish adaptedversion of the Camel and Cactus test (Bozeat et al. 2000).

The digit span subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelli-

gence Scale was used as an index of working memory,while attention and executive functions were evaluated

with the Spanish version of Stroop test and the Trail

Making Test (parts A and B), respectively (Pena-Casa-nova et al. 2009). Language was assessed with the

Spanish version of the Boston Naming Test (Jahn et al.

2013) and semantic fluency with the verbal fluency task(fruit and vegetable category). Finally, the global cogni-

tive status was evaluated with the Spanish version of the

Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Lobo et al.1979).

MRI and FDG-PET acquisition

Structural cerebral images were acquired on a PhilipsAchieva 3T MRI scanner equipped with an eight-channel

head coil. T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gra-

dient echo (MP-RAGE) cerebral images were obtained foreach participant. Acquisition parameters were empirically

optimized for gray/white contrast (repetition

time = 2300 ms, echo time = 4.5 ms, flip angle = 8",matrix dimensions = 320 9 320, 0.8 isotropic voxel, no

gap between slices, and time per acquisition = 9.1 min).

FDG-PET cerebral images were acquired on a whole-body PET-TAC Siemens Biograph 16 HiREZ scanner

(Siemens Medical Systems, Germany) with in-plane reso-

lution of 4.2 mm and point-spread function of 6 mm.Subjects fasted for at least 8 h before PET examination,

and all participants were scanned at the same time of the

day (8:00–9:00 am). Intravenous lines were placed10–15 min before tracer injection of a mean dose of

370 MBq of 2-[18F]fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose (FDG).

Participants stayed during 30 min in a dimly lit room withtheir eyes closed to minimize external stimuli during the

FDG uptake period. PET scans lasted approximately

30 min. A transmission scan was used for attenuationcorrection, and FDG-PET cerebral images were recon-

structed with 2.6 9 2.6 9 2 mm voxel resolution using the

standard 2D back-projection filters. MRI-based correctionof FDG-PET data for partial volume effects was performed

with the PMOD software v3.208 (PMOD Technologies

Ltd., Switzerland). The FDG-PET scanning protocol wasidentical in all participants.

Estimation of surface-based cortical thicknessand cortical metabolism

MRI data were processed using the analysis pipeline ofFreesurfer v5.3 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) that

involves intensity normalization, registration to Talairach,

skull stripping, segmentation of white matter (WM), tes-sellation of the WM boundary, and automatic correction of

topological defects (Fischl and Dale 2000). Pial/WM

boundaries were manually corrected on a slice-by-slicebasis in each participant to enhance the reliability of cor-

tical thickness measures. Particular attention was paid to

cortical regions at the border with cerebrospinal fluid toavoid partial volume effects. Manual editing of cortical

boundaries was performed by experienced laboratory staff

blinded to PER3 genotypes and cognitive results. Corticalthickness maps were finally smoothed with non-linear

spherical wavelet-based de-noising schemes, which havedemonstrated enhanced specificity and sensitivity at

detecting local and global changes in cortical thickness

(Bernal-Rusiel et al. 2008).

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 4: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

To map volumetric FDG activity on the cortical surface,

we first co-registered individual FDG-PET images to T1-weighted images using PMOD tools. Next, volumetric

partial volume-corrected FDG-PET cortical images were

sampled onto the subject’s native surface, transformed tothe Freesurfer standard surface space, and smoothed with

non-linear spherical wavelet-based de-noising schemes.

Finally, FDG activity assigned to each surface vertex wasnormalized by the FDG activity of the entire cortex using

an iterative vertex-based statistical method that excludedgroup-dependent vertices in the calculation of global

activity (Park et al. 2006).

Statistical analyses

Before applying any statistical test on cognitive variables,we tested whether they deviated from normality by

applying the Shapiro–Wilk test (Table 1). Those cognitive

tests that fulfilled this assumption were introduced in amultivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA). The

equality of error variances and variance–covariance

matrices were tested with the Levene test and Box’sM tests, respectively. Differences between PER34/4 and

PER35/5 groups for the remaining non-normally distributed

cognitive variables were assessed with the Mann–WhitneyU test. As these variables were not correlated with each

other, the Bonferroni test was used to adjust confidence

intervals. The above statistical analyses were performedwith SPSS v22 (SPSS Inc. Chicago, IL).

Differences in cortical thickness and cortical metabo-

lism between PER34/4 and PER35/5 groups were assessedby applying vertex-wise analysis of variance (ANOVA) for

each cortical hemisphere. Results were corrected for mul-

tiple comparisons using a previously validated hierarchicalstatistical model (Bernal-Rusiel et al. 2010). This model

first controls the family-wise error rate at the level of

clusters by applying random field theory over smoothedstatistical maps, and next controls the false discovery rate

(FDR) at the level of vertex over unsmoothed statistical

maps limited to significant clusters. In the present study,the FDR was controlled at the 0.05 level, meaning that on

average, only 5% of the significant vertices were falsepositives.

Vertex-wise linear regression analyses were further

performed in each homozygous PER3 group to determinewhether changes in cognitive performance were associated

with variations in cortical thickness/relative cortical

metabolism. Regression analyses, corrected for multiplecomparisons by applying the same hierarchical method,

were restricted to only those regions that showed signifi-

cant differences in cortical thickness/metabolism betweenPER34/4 and PER35/5 genotypes. If correlations for any of

the analyses reached significance in at least one of the two

groups, we then assessed group differences betweenregression slopes using the hierarchical method mentioned

above.

Results

Demographic and cognitive performance

Table 1 shows mean values in demographic and cognitivescores for PER34/4 and PER35/5 groups matched in age and

Table 1 Demographic andcognitive function in PER34/4

and PER35/5 groups matched inage and sex

Normality PER34/4 PER35/5 p

Age Yes 67.7 ± 3.8 67.6 ± 4.9 n/a

Gender (M/F) n/a 18/14 18/14 n/a

MMSE No 29.1 ± 1.2 28.4 ± 1.4 0.02

Word pairs (delayed) No 5.2 ± 2 4.6 ± 2.4 0.34

Digit span No 13 ± 3.6 12.1 ± 3.1 0.31

Camel and Cactus Yes 54.1 ± 4.1 49.7 ± 3.5 0.00002*

Stroop (words) No 105.4 ± 17.3 96.2 ± 11.8 0.01

Stroop (colors) Yes 74.3 ± 11 65.7 ± 8.7 0.001*

Stroop (word-colors) Yes 49 ± 10 40.7 ± 6.4 0.0002*

TMT-A Yes 34.9 ± 8.9 43.4 ± 10.2 0.001*

TMT-B No 103 ± 52.9 127.3 ± 54.3 0.04

Boston Naming Test No 52.6 ± 5.8 51.8 ± 4.2 0.22

Verbal fluency No 18.3 ± 3.5 15.4 ± 4.6 0.002*

Results are expressed as mean ± standard deviation of scores obtained in each group. Normalityassumption was assessed with the Shapiro–Wilk test

M males, F females. TMT Trail making test; MMSE Mini Mental State Examination, n/a not applicable

*p\ 0.05 in the MANOVA (normally distributed variables) or p\ 0.0083 in the Mann–Whitney U testwith Bonferroni correction (non-normally distributed variables)

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 5: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

sex. Only those cognitive variables that were normally

distributed were introduced in the MANOVA (i.e., Cameland Cactus test, Stroop test for colors and word-colors, and

the TMT-A test). The MANOVA showed a significant

main effect of genotype (F4,59 = 6.95; p\ 0.0002; Wilk’sK = 0.68). Neither the Levene test nor the Box’s M tests

yielded significant results. Univariate analyses indicated

that elderly PER35/5 carriers showed lower scores insemantic memory as revealed by the Camel and Cactus test

(F1,62 = 21.6; p\ 0.00002), attention as derived from theStroop for colors (F1,62 = 12.4; p\ 0.001) and word-col-

ors (F1,62 = 15.7; p\ 0.0002), and executive function as

revealed by the TMT-A test (F1,62 = 12.8; p\ 0.001).Among non-normally distributed cognitive variables, only

verbal fluency scores were significantly lower in PER35/5

as compared with PER34/4 after applying the Bonferronicorrection (p\ 0.002).

Group differences in cortical thickness

Differences in cortical thickness between PER34/4 and

PER35/5 genotypes are summarized in Table 2 and Fig. 1.Compared to older PER34/4 carriers, PER35/5 exhibited

significant patterns of thinning over bilateral temporopolar

cortex (left: pcorrected\ 10-3; right: pcorrected\ 10-4), leftsuperior parietal cortex (pcorrected\ 10-3), and right lin-

gual, entorhinal, and parahippocampal gyrus (pcor-

rected\ 10-4). Although the standard deviation of theclusters located in the temporopolar cortex and left superior

parietal lobe was markedly different, likely due to the small

local changes, the Levene test did not achieve statisticalsignificance for any of these clusters.

Group differences in cortical metabolism

Table 2 and Fig. 2 depict differences in cortical metabo-lism between PER34/4 and PER35/5 groups matched in age

and sex. In comparison with elderly PER34/4 carriers,

PER35/5 showed significantly lower glucose consumptionin middle frontal (left: pcorrected\ 10-4; right: pcor-rected\ 10-4), inferior parietal (left: pcorrected\ 10-4;

right: pcorrected\ 10-5), and middle temporal regions (left:pcorrected\ 10-4; right: pcorrected\ 10-6).

Correlations between cortical thickness/metabolismand cognitive performance

Next, we sought to determine whether patterns of corticalthinning and/or cortical metabolism were correlated with

cognitive performance in each group. Analyses showed that

TMT-B scores negatively correlated with glucose metabo-lism in the left supramarginal gyrus (R2 = 0.32, pcor-rected\ 0.003) and left middle frontal regions (R2 = 0.59,

pcorrected\ 10-6) across PER35/5 carriers (Fig. 3), thisrelationship being significantly stronger in PER35/5 than in

PER34/4 elderly subjects in the left supramarginal gyrus

(pcorrected\ 0.002). No significant associations betweenother cognitive tests and structural/functional cortical

changes were detected in either PER35/5 or PER34/4 groups.

Discussion

Growing evidence suggests that the impact of disadvanta-

geous genotypes on cognition and cerebral integrity is

magnified in late life, likely due to the combined effects of

Table 2 Patterns of corticalthinning and lower corticalmetabolism in elderly PER35/5

carriers

Cortical region CS (mm2) Mean ± SD cluster (mm) % Change pcorrected

PER34/4[PER35/5 PER34/4 PER35/5

Cortical thickness (MRI)

L temporal 132 4.09 ± 0.34 3.36 ± 0.83 21 10-3

L superior parietal 280 2.61 ± 0.16 2.28 ± 0.29 14 10-3

R temporal/entorhinal 272 3.79 ± 0.29 3.14 ± 0.63 20 10-4

R lingual/entorhinal parahippocampal 1539 2.21 ± 0.13 2.01 ± 0.14 9 10-4

Cortical metabolism (FDG-PET)

L middle frontal 3281 1.3 ± 0.06 1.22 ± 0.07 6 10-4

L inferior parietal 705 1.15 ± 0.08 1.03 ± 0.1 11 10-4

L middle temporal 454 0.96 ± 0.04 0.9 ± 0.05 6 10-4

R middle frontal 2088 1.33 ± 0.08 1.25 ± 0.11 6 10-4

R inferior parietal 4176 1.19 ± 0.06 1.09 ± 0.09 9 10-5

R middle temporal 530 0.97 ± 0.04 0.89 ± 0.07 9 10-6

Mean ± SD shows the mean and standard deviation of significant clusters for each group. pcorrected: p valuecorrected with the hierarchical method (Bernal-Rusiel et al. 2010)

L left, R: right; SD: standard deviation; CS: cluster size defined as the area of change (in mm2)

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 6: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

aging and genetic vulnerability on neuroanatomical and

neurochemical resources (Lindenberger et al. 2008). Inmost previous studies, target genes were related to synaptic

plasticity, neurotransmitters involved in executive func-

tioning, and propensity in developing AD (Papenberg et al.2015). However, little research has been conducted on the

contribution of circadian clock genes to inter-individual

differences in aging. Here, we provide the first evidence inhumans that PER35/5, a polymorphism of the PER3 gene

associated with alterations of sleep structure (Viola et al.

2007) and higher vulnerability to sleep loss in young adults(Maire et al. 2014; Vandewalle et al. 2009; Groeger et al.

2008; Viola et al. 2007), is also related to lower cognitive

performance and reduced structural–functional integrity ofthe cortical mantle in normal elderly subjects. PER35/5 also

showed stronger associations between reduced relative

metabolism over parietal regions and decreased cognitiveflexibility than PER34/4 carriers. Although preliminary due

to the small sample size, these findings contribute to

enhance our understanding on inter-individual variabilityin aging due to circadian genetic factors, an aspect that may

be particularly relevant in vulnerable aged population

carriers of the PER35/5 genotype.

In line with the vast literature supporting the circadian

regulation of human cognitive processes (e.g., Santhi et al.2016; Burke et al. 2015; Krishnan and Lyons 2015; Mulder

et al. 2013; Dijk et al. 1992), the previous studies have

shown that PER3 allele status modulates different aspectsof executive function in young adults (Groeger et al. 2008;

Viola et al. 2007). In our study, elderly PER35/5 carriers not

only showed decreased executive function compared toPER34/4, but also poorer semantic memory and verbal

fluency. Interestingly, we also found that these cognitive

variables were related to one another, likely indicating thatboth semantic memory and verbal fluency impairment

resulted from reduced cognitive control in older PER35/5

subjects (Berberian et al. 2016). Decreased executivefunction is expected to further influence memory, because

many encoding and retrieval processes rely on controlled

processing (Simons and Spiers 2003). However, whereasexecutive function and episodic memory are strongly

affected by aging (Buckner 2004), verbal fluency and

semantic memory are relatively preserved (Spaniol et al.2006; Allen et al. 2002). The disparity of these findings is

likely due to the influence of other genes on these cognitive

aspects (Erickson et al. 2008) and/or the impact of different

Fig. 1 Pattern of cortical thinning in elderly PER35/5 subjects. Left(L) and right (R) panels correspond to differences in the left and rightcortical hemispheres, respectively. Significant patterns of corticalthinning are displayed on inflated cortical surfaces (left superiorcorner of each panel) and on flattened cortical surfaces (right superiorcorner of each panel). Squares with colored borders limited thelocation of significant regional changes. The surface of the square waszoomed on flattened cortical maps (bottom panels) displayingcytoarchitectonic delimitation of affected regions. Abbreviations forthe left superior parietal (square with blue borders): PCL posteriorparacentral lobule, CiS cingulate sulcus, PrC precuneus, SuPSsubparietal sulcus, CPD cingulate postdorsal, POS parieto-occipital

sulcus, CS central sulcus (Scheperjans et al. 2005). Abbreviations forthe left temporal lobe (square with yellow borders): In insula, PIparainsular cortex, TA temporal area, TAr temporal area rostral, TGtemporal pole, TAp temporal area polysensory, TEd temporal areadorsal (Ding et al. 2009). Abbreviations for the right temporal lobe(square with orange borders): LG lingual gyrus, FG fusiform gyrus,PHC parahippocampal cortex, EC entorhinal cortex, iTC inferior-temporal cortex, TE temporal area, TEd temporal area dorsal, TAptemporal area polysensory, TA temporal area, TAr temporal arearostral, TG temporal pole, In insula, PI parainsular cortex (Ding et al.2009)

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 7: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

experimental approaches (i.e., cross-sectional vs. longitu-dinal) on studying the influence of aging, environment, and

genes on cognitive decline (Jagust 2009).

Reduced metabolic activity in regions of the fronto–parietal network of elderly PER35/5 individuals was asso-

ciated with decreased cognitive flexibility, an important

aspect of executive function (Arbuthnott and Frank 2000).Although the capacity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

to maintain executive functions has shown to be compro-

mised in young PER35/5 carriers (Vandewalle et al. 2009),the present study revealed that the supramarginal gyrus

contributes to a greater extent to account for differences

between PER34/4 and PER35/5 older adults. This is notsurprising given that the supramarginal gyrus has been

largely considered as part of a widely distributed fronto-

posterior network involved in the flexible shifting andupdating of information in working memory (Nyhus and

Barcelo 2009).

The most striking finding of the present study is thereduced structural and functional integrity of the cortical

mantle showed by older PER35/5 individuals. While PER3

polymorphisms are not associated with AD (Pereira et al.2016), our results showed that cortical regions affected in

PER35/5 match with those affected in preclinical AD

(Driscoll et al. 2012; Rowe et al. 2007; Gomez-Isla et al.1996). Mechanisms by which the PER35/5 polymorphism is

associated with the loss of cortical integrity remain elusive.

PER35/5 subjects live under a higher sleep pressurethroughout life (Maire et al. 2014) accompanied by

increased slow wave activity (SWA) after sleep loss—a

Fig. 2 Patterns of cortical hypometabolism in elderly PER35/5

subjects. Left (L) and right (R) panels correspond to differences inthe left and right cortical hemispheres, respectively. Significantpatterns of lower cortical metabolism are displayed on inflatedcortical surfaces (on the top of each panel) and on flattened corticalsurfaces (left and right superior corners of each panel). Squares withcolored borders limited the location of significant regional changes.The surface of the square was zoomed on flattened cortical mapsdisplaying cytoarchitectonic delimitation of affected regions. Abbre-viations for the middle frontal region (square with orange borders):MFG middle frontal gyrus, 8Ad dorsal part of area 8A, 8Av ventralpart of area 8A, 9 superior frontal gyrus, 9/46d dorsal part of middlefrontal gyrus, 9/46v ventral part of middle frontal gyrus, 10 frontalpole, IFs inferior frontal sulcus, PCs paracingulate sulcus, ORs orbitalsulcus (Petrides and Pandya 1999). Abbreviations for the inferiorparietal region (square with yellow borders): 1 area 1 primarysomatosensory cortex, 2 area 1 primary somatosensory cortex, 3a area

3a primary somatosensory cortex, 3b area 3b primary somatosensorycortex (Geyer et al. 1997); OP1 caudal operculum, OP4 rostraloperculum, PF area parietalis, PFop area parietalis tenui corticalisopercularis, PFt parietal rostral area of inferior parietal lobe withtenuicorticalis neurons, PFm area parietalis magnocellularis, PGarostral region in the caudal inferior parietal cortex, PGp caudal regionin the caudal inferior parietal cortex, 7A anterior region of thesuperior parietal lobe, 7PC area posterior to the post-central sulcus ofthe superior parietal lobe, POS parieto-occipital sulcus, PrC pre-cuneus, PCL paracentral lobe (Caspers et al. 2006); hIP3 intraparietalarea 3, hOc4d dorsal occipital, area 4, V1 visual area 1, V2 visual area2 (Malikovic et al. 2016). Abbreviations for the middle temporal lobe(square with blue borders): LG lingual gyrus, FG fusiform gyrus,PHC parahippocampal cortex, EC entorhinal cortex, iTC inferior-temporal cortex, TE temporal area, TEd temporal area dorsal, TAptemporal area polysensory, TA temporal area, TAr temporal arearostral, TG temporal pole (Ding et al. 2009)

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 8: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

putative marker of sleep homeostasis occurring during slowwave sleep (SWS) (Borbely 1982). SWS, in turn, promotes

memory consolidation processes orchestrated by cortical

slow oscillations that determine changes in synaptic

weights underlying memory storage (Wei et al. 2016;Walker and Stickgold 2006). Both the reduced glucose

metabolism over frontal regions and the lower memory

performance observed in PER35/5 elderly individuals might

Fig. 3 Correlations between relative cortical metabolism and TMT-Bscores in elderly PER34/4 and PER35/5 carriers. a Left panel showsresults of linear regressions represented on inflated cortical surfacesbetween relative cortical metabolism and TMT-B scores in PER35/5

carriers. As illustrated in the scatterplot (right panel), only PER35/5

subjects showed significant associations between reduced relativemetabolism in left middle frontal and poorer performance in theTMT-B test, though PER34/4 individuals showed a trend towardssignificance. b Compared to older PER34/4 subjects, PER35/5 carriersshowed a significantly stronger association between reduced meta-bolism in left supramarginal gyrus and poorer performance in theTMT-B test (left panel). Note that the regression lines of each groupdepicted in the scatterplot (right panel) showed opposite directions,but only correlations in the PER35/5 group reached significance.

Results derived from vertex-wise regression analyses were correctedfor multiple comparisons (p\ 0.05) using a previously validatedhierarchical statistical model (Bernal-Rusiel et al. 2010). Whilecortical surface-based correlations were performed in a vertex-wisemanner at the whole-cortex level (left panel), scatterplots (right panel)were computed using the peak vertex of significant clusters displayedin the cortical surface maps (left panel). FDG-PET measuresdisplayed in scatterplots (a, b) correspond to metabolic rate ofcortical glucose consumption normalized to the subject’s meanuptake, and TMT-B scores were computed as the time (in seconds)required to complete the test. Variables included in the scatter plots(a, b) correspond to the standardized residuals from a linearregression analysis with age as covariate

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 9: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

stem from the higher homeostatic sleep pressure experi-

enced by these subjects on a daily basis, which may turndysfunctional in late life, leading to loss of functional

integrity in frontal regions—the main source generator of

SWA (Murphy et al. 2009)—and consequently affectingSWA-dependent mechanisms of memory consolidation.

Elderly PER35/5 subjects further showed cortical thin-

ning of the entorhinal cortex and lower relative metabolismof middle temporal regions relative to their counterparts

carrying the two short repeat alleles. Both regions are ofparticular relevance for cognitive aging due to several

considerations. First, layer II neurons of the entorhinal

cortex represent a vulnerable cell type with the potential toinitiate downstream cascades leading to aging-related

functional decrements among hippocampal neurons

(Stranahan and Mattson 2010). Second, studies performedin rats and monkeys have shown that hippocampal synaptic

responses to stimulation of their afferent fibers from the

entorhinal cortex fluctuate in a circadian manner, theintegrity of this circuit being essential for time-dependent

aspects of memory acquisition (Barnes et al. 1977). Finally,

the entorhinal cortex also contributes to memory retrievalby means of reciprocal connections with the hippocampus

and frontal regions (Takehara-Nishiuchi 2014), suggesting

that selective damage of the entorhinal cortex may speedup cognitive decline in asymptomatic elderly subjects.

This study has limitations that should be addressed in

future experiments. First, we conducted a cross-sectionalstudy, which prevented determining the role of PER35/5

genotype in accelerated aging. Future longitudinal studies

should compare the rate of cognitive decline and corticalintegrity (structural and functional) between older PER34/4

and PER35/5 carriers to establish whether the PER35/5

genotype is indeed associated with vulnerable trajectoriesof aging. Second, a potential bias that could have influ-

enced our results is the time of day chosen for PET scan-

ning (8:00–9:00), since the interaction between alertnesslevel and PER3 chronotype may have an effect on cortical

metabolism. However, this may be questionable, since

patterns of cortical hypometabolism were limited to elderlyPER35/5 carriers, a genotype that has been associated with

diurnal preference in young (Archer et al. 2003) but not in

older adults (Jones et al. 2007). Finally, the small size ofthe sample employed in the present study may lead to an

increased risk of inflated effect sizes. Therefore, these

results should be considered as preliminary and replicatedin further experiments with a larger and independent

sample.

Acknowledgements This work was supported by research grantsfrom the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness(SAF2011-25463, PSI2014-55747-R), the Regional Ministry ofInnovation, Science and Enterprise, Junta de Andalucia (P12-CTS-

2327), CIBERNED (CB06/05/1111), and the Spanish Sleep Society.The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

Allen PA, Sliwinski M, Bowie T, Madden DJ (2002) Differential ageeffects in semantic and episodic memory. J Gerontol B PsycholSci Soc Sci 57:P173–P186

Ando H, Ushijima K, Kumazaki M, Takamura T, Yokota N, Saito T,Irie S, Kaneko S, Fujimura A (2010) Influence of age on clockgene expression in peripheral blood cells of healthy women.J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 65:9–13

Arbuthnott K, Frank J (2000) Trail making test, part B as a measure ofexecutive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm.J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 22:518–528

Archer SN, Robilliard DL, Skene DJ, Smits M, Williams A, Arendt J,von Schantz M (2003) A length polymorphism in the circadianclock gene Per3 is linked to delayed sleep phase syndrome andextreme diurnal preference. Sleep 26:413–415

Barnes CA, McNaughton BL, Goddard GV, Douglas RM, Adamec R(1977) Circadian rhythm of synaptic excitability in rat andmonkey central nervous system. Science 197:91–92

Benedetti F, Dallaspezia S, Colombo C, Pirovano A, Marino E,Smeraldi E (2008) A length polymorphism in the circadian clockgene Per3 influences age at onset of bipolar disorder. NeurosciLett 445:184–187

Berberian AA, Moraes GV, Gadelha A, Brietzke E, Fonseca AO,Scarpato BS, Vicente MO, Seabra AG, Bressan RA, Lacerda AL(2016) Is semantic verbal fluency impairment explained byexecutive function deficits in schizophrenia? Rev Bras Psiquiatr38:121–126

Bernal-Rusiel JL, Atienza M, Cantero JL (2008) Detection of focalchanges in human cortical thickness: spherical wavelets versusGaussian smoothing. Neuroimage 41:1278–1292

Bernal-Rusiel JL, Atienza M, Cantero JL (2010) Determining theoptimal level of smoothing in cortical thickness analysis: ahierarchical approach based on sequential statistical threshold-ing. Neuroimage 52:158–171

Bohm P, Pena-Casanova J, Aguilar M, Hernandez G, Sol JM, BlesaR, NORMACODEN Group (1998) Clinical validity and utility ofthe interview for deterioration of daily living in dementia forSpanish-speaking communities. Int Psychogeriatr 10:261–270

Borbely AA (1982) A two process model of sleep regulation. HumNeurobiol 1:195–204

Bozeat S, Lambon-Ralph MA, Patterson K, Garrard P, Hodges JR(2000) Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia.Neuropsychologia 38:1207–1215

Brown SA, Kunz D, Dumas A, Westermark PO, Vanselow K,Tilmann-Wahnschaffe A, Herzel H, Kramer A (2008) Molecularinsights into human daily behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA105:1602–1607

Buckner RL (2004) Memory and executive function in aging and AD:multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors thatcompensate. Neuron 44:195–208

Burke TM, Scheer FA, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP Jr (2015)Sleep inertia, sleep homeostatic and circadian influences onhigher-order cognitive functions. J Sleep Res 24:364–371

Caspers S, Geyer S, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Amunts K, Zilles K(2006) The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonicparcellation and interindividual variability. Neuroimage33:430–448

Cermakian N, Lamont EW, Boudreau P, Boivin DB (2011) Circadianclock gene expression in brain regions of Alzheimer’s diseasepatients and control subjects. J Biol Rhythms 26:160–170

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 10: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

Chiu JC, Vanselow JT, Kramer A, Edery I (2008) The phospho-occupancy of an atypical SLIMB-binding site on PERIOD that isphosphorylated by DOUBLETIME controls the pace of theclock. Genes Dev 22:1758–1772

Dijk DJ, Archer SN (2010) PERIOD3, circadian phenotypes, andsleep homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev 14:151–160

Dijk DJ, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA (1992) Circadian and sleep/wakedependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive perfor-mance. J Sleep Res 1:112–117

Ding S, Van Hoesen GW, Cassell MD, Poremba A (2009) Parcel-lation of human temporal polar cortex: a combined analysis ofmultiple cytoarchitectonic, chemoarchitectonic, and pathologicalmarkers. J Comp Neurol 514:595–623

Driscoll I, Troncoso JC, Rudow G, Sojkova J, Pletnikova O, Zhou Y,Kraut MA, Ferrucci L, Mathis CA, Klunk WE, O’Brien RJ,Davatzikos C, Wong DF, Resnick SM (2012) Correspondencebetween in vivo (11)C-PiB-PET amyloid imaging and post-mortem, region-matched assessment of plaques. Acta Neu-ropathol 124:823–831

Ebisawa T, Uchiyama M, Kajimura N, Mishima K, Kamei Y, KatohM, Watanabe T, Sekimoto M, Shibui K, Kim K, Kudo Y, OzekiY, Sugishita M, Toyoshima R, Inoue Y, Yamada N, Nagase T,Ozaki N, Ohara O, Ishida N et al (2001) Association of structuralpolymorphisms in the human period3 gene with delayed sleepphase syndrome. EMBO Rep 2:342–346

Erickson KI, Kim JS, Suever BL, Voss MW, Francis BM, Kramer AF(2008) Genetic contributions to age-related decline in executivefunction: a 10-year longitudinal study of COMT and BDNFpolymorphisms. Front Hum Neurosci 2:11

Farajnia S, Deboer T, Rohling JH, Meijer JH, Michel S (2014) Agingof the suprachiasmatic clock. Neuroscientist 20:44–55

Fischl B, Dale AM (2000) Measuring the thickness of the humancerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl AcadSci USA 97:11050–11055

Geyer S, Schleicher A, Zilles K (1997) The somatosensory cortex ofhuman: cytoarchitecture and regional distributions of receptor-binding sites. Neuroimage 6:27–45

Gomez-Isla T, Price JL, McKeel DW Jr, Morris JC, Growdon JH,Hyman BT (1996) Profound loss of layer II entorhinal cortexneurons occurs in very mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci16:4491–4500

Groeger JA, Viola AU, Lo JC, von Schantz M, Archer SN, Dijk DJ(2008) Early morning executive functioning during sleepdeprivation is compromised by a PERIOD3 polymorphism.Sleep 31:1159–1167

Hardy J, Selkoe DJ (2012) The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer’sdisease: progress and problems on the road to therapeutics.Science 297:353–356

Hida A, Kusanagi H, Satoh K, Kato T, Matsumoto Y, Echizenya M,Shimizu T, Higuchi S, Mishima K (2009) Expression profiles ofPERIOD1, 2, and 3 in peripheral blood mononuclear cells fromolder subjects. Life Sci 84:33–37

Huang Y, Potter R, Sigurdson W, Santacruz A, Shih S, Ju YE, KastenT, Morris JC, Mintun M, Duntley S, Bateman RJ (2012) Effectsof age and amyloid deposition on Ab dynamics in the humancentral nervous system. Arch Neurol 69:51–58

Jagust W (2009) Genes and cognitive aging. Front Neurosci3:161–163

Jahn DR, Mauer CB, Menon CV, Edwards ML, Dressel JA, O’BryantSE (2013) A brief Spanish–English equivalent version of theBoston Naming Test: a Project FRONTIER study. J Clin ExpNeuropsychol 35:835–845

Jones KH, Ellis J, von Schantz M, Skene DJ, Dijk DJ, Archer SN(2007) Age-related change in the association between apolymorphism in the PER3 gene and preferred timing of sleepand waking activities. J Sleep Res 16:12–16

Kang JE, Lim MM, Bateman RJ, Lee JJ, Smyth LP, Cirrito JR, FujikiN, Nishino S, Holtzman DM (2009) Amyloid-beta dynamics areregulated by orexin and the sleep-wake cycle. Science326:1005–1007

Kondratova AA, Kondratov RV (2012) The circadian clock andpathology of the ageing brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:325–335

Krishnan HC, Lyons LC (2015) Synchrony and desynchrony incircadian clocks: impacts on learning and memory. Learn Mem22:426–437

Lazar AS, Slak A, Lo JC, Santhi N, von Schantz M, Archer SN,Groeger JA, Dijk DJ (2012) Sleep, diurnal preference, health,and psychological well-being: a prospective single-allelic-vari-ation study. Chronobiol Int 29:131–146

Li JZ, Bunney BG, Meng F, Hagenauer MH, Walsh DM, Vawter MP,Evans SJ, Choudary PV, Cartagena P, Barchas JD, SchatzbergAF, Jones EG, Myers RM, Watson SJ Jr, Akil H, Bunney WE(2013) Circadian patterns of gene expression in the human brainand disruption in major depressive disorder. Proc Natl Acad SciUSA 110:9950–9955

Lim AS, Myers AJ, Yu L, Buchman AS, Duffy JF, De Jager PL,Bennett DA (2013) Sex difference in daily rhythms of clockgene expression in the aged human cerebral cortex. J BiolRhythms 28:117–129

Lindenberger U, Nagel IE, Chicherio C, Li SC, Heekeren HR,Backman L (2008) Age-related decline in brain resourcesmodulates genetic effects on cognitive functioning. FrontNeurosci 2:234–244

Lobo A, Escolar V, Ezquerra J, Seva A (1979) Mini-ExamenCognoscitivo: un test sencillo, practico, para detectar alter-aciones intelectuales en pacientes psiquiatricos. Actas Luso EspNeurol Psiquiatr Cienc Afines 3:189–202

Maire M, Reichert CF, Gabel V, Viola AU, Strobel W, Krebs J,Landolt HP, Bachmann V, Cajochen C, Schmidt C (2014) Sleepability mediates individual differences in the vulnerability tosleep loss: evidence from a PER3 polymorphism. Cortex52:47–59

Malikovic A, Amunts K, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Kujovic M,Palomero-Gallagher N, Eickhoff SB, Zilles K (2016) Cytoar-chitecture of the human lateral occipital cortex: mapping of twoextrastriate areas hOc4la and hOc4lp. Brain Struct Funct221:1877–1897

Miller SA, Dykes DD, Polesky HF (1988) A simple salting outprocedure for extracting DNA from human nucleated cells.Nucleic Acids Res 16:1215

Mulder CK, Gerkema MP, Van der Zee EA (2013) Circadian clocksand memory: time-place learning. Front Mol Neurosci 6:8

Murphy M, Riedner BA, Huber R, Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, TononiG (2009) Source modeling sleep slow waves. Proc Natl Acad SciUSA 106:1608–1613

Nyhus E, Barcelo F (2009) The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and thecognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: a criticalupdate. Brain Cogn 71:437–451

Papenberg G, Lindenberger U, Backman L (2015) Aging-relatedmagnification of genetic effects on cognitive and brain integrity.Trends Cogn Sci 19:506–514

Park HJ, Lee JD, Chun JW, Seok JH, Yun M, Oh MK, Kim JJ (2006)Cortical surface-based analysis of 18F-FDG PET: measuredmetabolic abnormalities in schizophrenia are affected by corticalstructural abnormalities. Neuroimage 31:1434–1444

Pena-Casanova J, Quinones-Ubeda S, Quintana-Aparicio M, AguilarM, Badenes D, Molinuevo JL, Torner L, Robles A, BarqueroMS, Villanueva C, Antunez C, Martinez-Parra C, Frank-GarciaA, Sanz A, Fernandez M, Alfonso V, Sol JM, Blesa R,NEURONORMA Study Team (2009) Spanish MulticenterNormative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): norms for verbalspan, visuospatial span, letter and number sequencing, trail

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy

Page 11: Author's personal copy...To test these predictions, we first compared patterns of cortical thickness and cortical metabolism, examined with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission

making test, and symbol digit modalities test. Arch ClinNeuropsychol 24:321–341

Pereira PA, Alvim-Soares A, Bicalho MA, de Moraes EN, Malloy-Diniz L, de Paula JJ, Romano-Silva MA, Miranda DM (2016)Lack of association between genetic polymorphism of circadiangenes (PER2, PER3, CLOCK and OX2R) with late onsetdepression and Alzheimer’s disease in a sample of a Brazilianpopulation (Circadian Genes, Late-Onset Depression andAlzheimer’s Disease). Curr Alzheimer Res 13:1397–1406

Petrides M, Pandya DN (1999) Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex:comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and themacaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. Eur JNeurosci 11:1011–1036

Rath MF, Rohde K, Fahrenkrug J, Møller M (2013) Circadian clockcomponents in the rat neocortex: daily dynamics, localizationand regulation. Brain Struct Funct 218:551–562

Reid KJ, McGee-Koch LL, Zee PC (2011) Cognition in circadianrhythm sleep disorders. Prog Brain Res 190:3–20

Reppert SM, Weaver DR (2001) Molecular analysis of mammaliancircadian rhythms. Annu Rev Physiol 63:647–676

Rowe CC, Ng S, Ackermann U, Gong SJ, Pike K, Savage G, CowieTF, Dickinson KL, Maruff P, Darby D, Smith C, Woodward M,Merory J, Tochon-Danguy H, O’Keefe G, Klunk WE, Mathis CA,Price JC, Masters CL, Villemagne VL (2007) Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging and dementia. Neurology 68:1718–1725

Santhi N, Lazar AS,McCabe PJ, Lo JC,Groeger JA,DijkDJ (2016) Sexdifferences in the circadian regulation of sleep and wakingcognition in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:E2730–E2739

Scheperjans F, Grefkes C, Palomero-Gallaguer N, Schleicher A,Zilles K (2005) Subdivision of human parietal area 5 revealed byquantitative receptor autoradiography: a parietal region betweenmotor, somatosensory, and cingulate cortical areas. Neuroimage25:975–992

Simons JS, Spiers HJ (2003) Prefrontal and medial temporal lobeinteractions in long-term memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:637–648

Spaniol J, Madden DJ, Voss A (2006) A diffusion model analysis ofadult age differences in episodic and semantic long-termmemory retrieval. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 32:101–117

Stranahan AM, Mattson MP (2010) Selective vulnerability of neuronsin layer II of the entorhinal cortex during aging and Alzheimer’sdisease. Neural Plast 108190

Takehara-Nishiuchi K (2014) Entorhinal cortex and consolidatedmemory. Neurosci Res 84:27–33

Vandewalle G, Archer SN, Wuillaume C, Balteau E, Degueldre C,Luxen A, Maquet P, Dijk DJ (2009) Functional magneticresonance imaging-assessed brain responses during an executivetask depend on interaction of sleep homeostasis, circadian phase,and PER3 genotype. J Neurosci 29:7948–7956

Viola AU, Archer SN, James LM, Groeger JA, Lo JC, Skene DJ, vonSchantz M, Dijk DJ (2007) PER3 polymorphism predicts sleepstructure and waking performance. Curr Biol 17:613–618

Viola AU, Chellappa SL, Archer SN, Pugin F, Gotz T, Dijk DJ,Cajochen C (2012) Interindividual differences in circadianrhythmicity and sleep homeostasis in older people: effect of aPER3 polymorphism. Neurobiol Aging 33:1010.e17–1010.e27

Virshup DM, Eide EJ, Forger DB, Gallego M, Harnish EV (2007)Reversible protein phosphorylation regulates circadian rhythms.Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 72:413–420

Walker MP, Stickgold R (2006) Sleep, memory, and plasticity. AnnuRev Psychol 57:139–166

Wei Y, Krishnan GP, Bazhenov M (2016) Synaptic mechanisms ofmemory consolidation during sleep slow oscillations. J Neurosci36:4231–4247

Wright KP, Lowry CA, Lebourgeois MK (2012) Circadian andwakefulness-sleep modulation of cognition in humans. FrontMol Neurosci 5:50

Yang S, Wang K, Valladares O, Hannenhalli S, Bucan M (2007)Genome-wide expression profiling and bioinformatics analysisof diurnally regulated genes in the mouse prefrontal cortex.Genome Biol 8:R247

Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O (1983) Development andvalidation of a geriatric depression scale: a preliminary report.J Psychiatr Res 17:37–49

Brain Struct Funct

123

Author's personal copy