autonomy, rights, and parenting in changing chinese ... · contexts kim lun sharon to department of...

157
AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE CULTURAL CONTEXTS by Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development Ontario Institute for Studies in Education University of Toronto © Copyright by Kim Lun Sharon To (2017) `

Upload: others

Post on 05-Aug-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

AUTONOMY,RIGHTS,ANDPARENTINGINCHANGINGCHINESECULTURALCONTEXTS

by

KimLunSharonTo

DepartmentofAppliedPsychologyandHumanDevelopment

UniversityofToronto

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Graduate Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development Ontario Institute for Studies in Education

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Kim Lun Sharon To (2017)

`

Page 2: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

ii

AUTONOMY,RIGHTS,ANDPARENTINGINCHANGINGCHINESECULTURALCONTEXTS

KimLunSharonTo

DepartmentofAppliedPsychologyandHumanDevelopment

UniversityofToronto

Abstract

Drawing on both a universalistic perspective (self-determination theory) and a cultural

psychological perspective (Greenfield’s theory of cultural change), the papers presented

in this dissertation examine the joint role of universal psychological processes and

cultural transformation in accounting for variations and similarities in parenting,

children’s rights attitudes, and their outcomes in diverse settings within China. One

hundred and twenty-eight Chinese adolescents (12-16-year-olds) and their mothers from

urban and rural China - two settings that have been rapidly changing at a different pace

over the past decades with the rise of modernization - participated in the current study.

The first paper focuses on how cultural parenting practices are transmitted across

generations, and their impact on Chinese adolescents’ psychological well-being. The

second paper narrows the focus and examines how the socialization environment and

other socio-demographic factors may contribute to the development of children’s rights

conceptions amongst Chinese mothers and adolescents. Comparing mothers and

adolescents from different settings coexisting within the same nation, encompassing

urban social ecologies of the Gesellschaft form to the rural social ecologies of the

Gemeinschaft form (Greenfield, 2009), these studies provide insight into how the extent

of urbanization impacts parenting practices, beliefs and attitudes, and consequently

affects children’s development in a non-Western cultural setting. Overall, findings from

Page 3: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

iii

both of the studies demonstrated converging evidence supporting the universalist claim

that children in a variety of cultural contexts benefit from family environments which are

responsive and promote children’s needs to exercise autonomy. Findings also suggested

that there has been a shift in cultural parenting practices in China (with a different pace of

change in urban versus rural areas), consistent with Greenfield’s theory of cultural

change.

Page 4: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

iv

Acknowledgements

I am indebted to a great number of people for their assistance with this thesis. There

are many people towards whom I would like to express my gratitude for their

contribution towards the completion of this thesis.

To my supervisor, Dr. Charles Helwig, you have been a constant source of

encouragement and mentorship over the course of my undergraduate and graduate career.

I feel extremely fortunate to have worked so closely with you for so many years. You

have been a source of great support for my research and my professional development, as

well as of sound advice. Your outstanding example, teaching and mentoring was crucial

for the completion of this dissertation, and will have far reaching impacts in my academic

and professional life.

To my thesis committee, Dr. Michele Peterson-Badali and Dr. Kang Lee, thank you

for your incisive feedback, stimulating discussion, and practical guidance and support.

To my dear colleagues and friends who undertook this journey with me. It would

have been impossible or me to express how much your constant support, passion for the

field, and friendship meant to me throughout these years.

To Raymond and my family, thank you for your unfailing faith in me. I owe all of

you my deepest gratitude for your unconditional love and unbridled support to allow me

to pursue my dream.

Last but not the least, I would also like to thank all the children and parents who

participated in this study, as well as our research collaborators and assistants in China.

Without all of you, accomplishing this dissertation project would not have been possible.

Page 5: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

v

Table of Contents

Acknowledgements............................................................................................................ iv Table of Contents ................................................................................................................v List of Tables................................................................................................................... viii List of Figures.................................................................................................................... ix List of Appendices...............................................................................................................x

General Introduction............................................................................................................1 Debate of Universality versus Cultural Relativism.................................................1 Grand Divide of Individualism-Collectivism versus Variations and Movements within Culture..........................................................................................................3 Cultural Transmission versus Constructivist Approaches to Values and Beliefs....4 Modernization, Social and Cultural Changes and Human Development................5 Overview of Research Studies.................................................................................9 Anticipated Contribution.........................................................................................9 Paper 1: Socio-Cultural Change and Parenting in China: Autonomy Support, Democratic Family Climate, and Adolescents’ Psychological Well-Being Abstract..................................................................................................................11 Introduction............................................................................................................12 Baumrind’s Parenting Typologies.............................................................12 Dimensional Theoretical Approach as Motivated by Self-determination Theory........................................................................................................15 Intergenerational Transmission of Parenting.............................................19 Greenfield’s Theory of Sociocultural Shift................................................20 Social Change in China and Human Development....................................22 Different Pace of Development in Urban versus Rural China...................26 Increased Rural-to-Urban Migration..........................................................27 Objectives of the Present Study and Hypotheses.......................................28 Method...................................................................................................................31 Participants and Research Sites..................................................................31 Procedures..................................................................................................34 Measures....................................................................................................35 Results....................................................................................................................38 Chinese Mothers’ Report of Their Current Parenting................................39 Adolescents’ Report of Their Mothers’ Parenting.....................................39 Comparisons Between and Correspondence Among Mothers’ and Adolescents’ Report of Parenting..............................................................40 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among Variables.........................42 Associations Between Parenting and Adolescents’ Psychological Well-

Page 6: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

vi

Being..........................................................................................................45 Chinese Mothers’ Retrospective Recall of the Way They Were Parented48 Comparisons of Chinese Mothers’ Current Parenting with Their Retrospective Recall of How They Were Parented...................................48 Correlates of Mothers’ Current Parenting..................................................51 Discussion..............................................................................................................54 Parenting in Contemporary China.............................................................56 Correlates of Mothers’ Current Parenting..................................................59 Mothers’ versus Adolescents’ Reports......................................................60 Parenting in Future China..........................................................................61 Paper 2: Children's Rights Attitudes Amongst Adolescents and Mothers in China Abstract..................................................................................................................63 Introduction............................................................................................................64 Parents’ and Children’s views on Children’s Rights.................................68 Socialization Correlates of Children’s Rights Attitudes............................71 The Present Study and Hypotheses............................................................74 Method...................................................................................................................76 Participants and Procedures.......................................................................76 Measures....................................................................................................77 Results....................................................................................................................80 Initial Investigation of Potential Oppositional or Reactive Patterns..........80 Descriptive and Correlational Analysis.....................................................83 Adolescents’ Perspectives on Children’s Rights.......................................84 Mothers’ Perspectives on Children’s Rights.............................................85 Adolescents’ versus Mothers’ Perspectives on Children’s Rights............85 Hierarchical Regression Analyses: Predicting Adolescents’ and Mothers’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Rights..........................................................88 Discussion..............................................................................................................92 Mothers’ and Adolescents’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Rights..............92 Socialization Correlates of Adolescents’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Rights.........................................................................................................98 Correlates of Mothers’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Rights...................100 Conclusions..........................................................................................................102

General Discussion..........................................................................................................105 Socialization Pathways to Well-Being and the Development of Rights Conceptions..........................................................................................................105 From Global Cultural Orientations To Dynamic Shifts and Movements within Culture and Over Time.............................................................................107

Page 7: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

vii

Constructivist Approaches to Values andBeliefs.................................................109 Different Pace in Value Versus Behavioral Changes..........................................110 Mothers’ Versus Adolescents’ Reports................................................................112 Implications for Research and Future Directions........ .......................................114 From Cross-Sectional to Longitudinal Studies........................................114 From Self-reports to Observational Reports of Autonomy- Supportive Parenting................................................................................116 From Quantitative to Qualitative Studies................................................117 Conclusion...........................................................................................................118References........................................................................................................................120 Appendices.......................................................................................................................141

Page 8: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

viii

List of Tables

Table 1. Parental education and occupational levels (in years and in categories).............33

Table 2. Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD) of POPS and Democratic Climate Scale as reported by (a) Mothers for their Current Parenting, (b) Adolescents for their Mothers’ Current Parenting, and (c) Mothers for How They were Parented (Retrospectively).......41

Table 3. Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD) and Correlations Among Mother Self-reported and Adolescent Reported Parenting and Family Democratic Climate................42

Table 4. Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD), and Pearson Correlations of All Study Variables Related to Family Socialization Environment and Adolescents’ Psychological Well Being For The Full Sample.......................................................................................44

Table 5. Regressions for Adolescent-reported Parenting and Family Climate Variables on Adolescents’ Psychological Well-Being............................................................................47

Table 6. Regression Examining Correlates between Mothers’ Retrospective Reports of how They were Parented and Mothers' Current Parenting................................................53

Table 7. Means, Standard deviations (SD), Pearson Correlations of Adolescents’ and Mothers’ Reported Family Socialization Environment and Children’s Rights Attitudes.82

Table 8. Relations between mothers’ attitudes toward children’s rights and their self-reported parenting..............................................................................................................89

Table 9. Relations between Mothers’ Children's Rights Attitudes and their Self-reported Parenting............................................................................................................................90

Table 10. Mothers' Retrospective Recall of Parenting and Current Attitudes Toward Children's Rights................................................................................................................91

Page 9: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

ix

ListofFigures

Figure 1. Mean score of (a) autonomy support, (b) responsiveness, (c) democratic climate, and (d) tolerance of dissent as reported by urban, rural-to-urban and rural mothers (retrospective recall of parenting and self-report of current parent….................50 Figure 2. Rights X Age Group X Respondent Interactions……………………………...87

Page 10: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

x

List of Appendices

Appendix A. Perceptions of Parents Scales (POPS)…………………………...……….141

Appendix B. Democratic Environment Questionnaire (DEQ)………...……………….144

Appendix C. Children's Rights Attitudes (CRA) questionnaire……………......………145

Page 11: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

1

General Introduction

This dissertation comprises two separate, but related, studies. The first one

focuses on how cultural parenting practices are transmitted across generations, and their

impact on Chinese adolescents' psychological well-being. The second study narrows the

focus and examines how the socialization environment and other socio-demographic

factors may contribute to the development of children’s rights conceptions amongst

Chinese mothers and adolescents. In both studies, the unique cultural setting of

contemporary China is the background upon which the research is set. In conjunction

with the intergenerational comparisons and the consideration of developmental pathways,

the impact of socio-cultural changes on these processes will be explored.

Debate of Universality versus Cultural Relativism

In the past few decades, developmental psychologists have conducted extensive

research on the socialization factors that are essential for positive and healthy

development. Generally, responsiveness and autonomy-supportive parenting are arguably

the most central qualities in parenting that have been found to be associated with a range

of beneficial psychological, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes for children (e.g.,

Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2005; Weiss & Schwarz, 1996). Proponents of self-

determination theory, for example, maintain that autonomy (i.e., one’s ability to act

autonomously in accordance with ones’ values), along with relatedness and competence,

are basic universal human needs whose satisfaction are essential for well-being (Ryan &

Deci, 2011). Emerging literature on relationships between perceptions of personal

autonomy, democratic climate and psychological well-being further suggests that a

socialization environment which promotes personal choice, freedom of expression, and

Page 12: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

2

meaningful involvement in decision making, may promote positive human development

(Jia et al., 2009; To, Helwig, & Yang, 2017).

However, it has been argued that most research studies conducted thus far are

based on samples drawn entirely from Western, educated, industrialized, rich and

democratic (“WEIRD”) societies (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). Notions of

autonomy and rights as well as theories like self-determination theory have been

criticized as originally derived from Western cultural settings that may have little

relevance to, or may not be compatible with, cultural beliefs in other cultures. For

example, some researchers adhering to theoretical perspectives within cultural

psychology (e.g., Greenfield, Suzuki, & Rothstein-Fisch, 2006; Markus & Kitayama,

1991; Shweder & Bourne, 1984; Triandis, 1989) have used a number of different but

related and dichotomous terms to characterize cultural variations in self-construals and

self-other relations. To name a few, individualism versus collectivism, independent

versus interdependent developmental pathways, and egocentric versus sociocentric

selves, are amongst some of the common cultural dichotomies that have been popularized

in cross-cultural research. As proposed by cultural psychologists, different conceptions of

the self and morality are believed to be held by individuals within individualistic and

collectivistic societies. In individualistic societies, the self is seen as separated from

others with an orientation towards principles or notions of individual rights and personal

autonomy. In contrast, in collectivistic societies, the self is seen as sociocentric with an

orientation towards duty and obedience to authority, preservation of social harmony, and

the upholding of traditional roles (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). It has been proposed that

individuals from collectivist cultures place a much stronger emphasis on interdependence

Page 13: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

3

and conformity, and hence often put group goals and harmony over personal goals and

desires. These theorists have therefore called into question whether the support of

autonomy, emphasis on individual freedom and rights, as well as the promotion of

democratic participation within social institutions such as the family are relevant for

people whose core values and beliefs differ from those of individualistic cultures. These

debates between universalists and cultural relativists have generated a ripple of emerging

research examining whether theories and notions of rights and autonomy are universally

applicable and relevant for individuals from cultures other than the mainstream Western

context. This subject will be further explored in the following papers.

Grand Divide of Individualism-Collectivism versus Variations and Movements

within Culture

The dichotomous constructs (e.g., individualism-collectivism) discussed above

are based on the underlying assumption that cultures are distinctly divided, and that self-

construals are stable and homogeneous within culture. While such general cultural

characterizations have been popular and influential in research in the 1980s, cultural

dichotomies of this sort have increasingly received criticism as oversimplifying the

complexities of culture and human cognition (see, for example, Helwig, 2005; Hermans

& Kempen, 1998; Smetana, 2002; Turiel, 2002; Turiel, Killen, & Helwig, 1987). In fact,

it has been proposed that cultural orientations may not be mutually exclusive or stable

over time (Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). With the globalizing trend of

social, economic and political transformation, it has been suggested that a culture’s

position on the cultural orientation spectrum may shift over time in accordance with a

society’s socioeconomic development (Greenfield, 2009). Variations within cultures and

Page 14: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

4

amongst different social contexts and domains should also be considered (Nucci, Camino,

& Sapiro, 1996; Smetana, 2002). Therefore, there has been a call for shifting attention

from studying cultures and individuals using such broad, static global constructs to the

study of cultural processes, particularly in the face of globalization and social changes

(Hermans & Kempen, 1998).

Cultural Transmission versus Constructivist Approaches to Values and Beliefs

A related issue that will be addressed in the following papers is how values and

beliefs are developed. On the one hand, the cultural transmission model suggests that

conceptions of self and morality are largely transmitted directly to individuals through

social participation in cultural practices and shared rituals (e.g., Shweder, 1999; Shweder

& Sullivan, 1993). This perspective suggests that there would be little change within

cultures, as individuals tend to internalize prevailing cultural practices and ideologies

rather than sometimes being in opposition to them (Helwig, 2005). It is therefore assumed

that children and parents alike would share similar views and beliefs, both of which

would be largely consistent with the prevailing cultural norms. This perspective of the

direct transmission of cultural values is likely congruent with cultural psychologists’

argument that there would be distinct differences in the conceptions of autonomy and

rights as well as parental beliefs and practices amongst individuals from different cultural

orientations.

On the other hand, the constructivist approach (Nucci, 2001; Turiel, 1998)

proposes that children construct their social and moral concepts from their own

experiences and in interaction with their socialization contexts. The diverse social

contexts and situations experienced by an individual do not only serve as a function of

Page 15: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

5

transmitting values and beliefs, but also as a context that may instantiate tensions or

conflicts with individuals’ developing sense of autonomy. It is through their daily

experiences in social encounters and reflections on their experiences that children

develop their own reasoning and judgments, which may or may not be in accordance with

the existing cultural beliefs systems, norms, and practices (Turiel, 2002). This approach

views individuals as active agents in their own socialization; thus, the construction of

beliefs and reasoning must be seen as an active and emerging process.

Modernization, Social and Cultural Changes and Human Development

In light of the debates in the literature cited above, the two papers presented in

this dissertation aim to contribute to our understanding of how autonomy, rights, and

parenting may be understood in the cultural settings of urban and rural China.

Specifically, two main themes related to cultural parenting practices (Paper 1) and the

development of children’s rights conceptions (Paper 2) will be explored. These two topics

were chosen as we expected that parenting practices and children’s rights attitudes are

both issues that are dynamically influenced by factors such as culture (general cultural

contexts and social change), socialization contexts (e.g., family environment), as well as

individual experience (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Although developmental psychologists

have studied both of these topics in Western and non-Western cultural settings, with

parenting being much more extensively researched than children’s rights conceptions, the

fundamental processes of how development interacts with social changes in non-Western

cultural contexts is much less understood. In fact, most studies have assumed constancy

in the environment and individual’s beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors. However, with the

global movement towards urbanization and industrialization, the understanding and

Page 16: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

6

conceptualization of children’s rights and well-being has been evolving (Stearns, 2016).

This is the result of the changing definition of “basic” needs for children’s well-being and

the shifting attitudes and views of the parental and societal role as it relates to children’s

positive and healthy development. This shift in societal beliefs about children has been

gradual, accompanied by modernization and multiple significant events that marked these

shifts which are reflected in policy implementation (Stearns, 2016). For example,

children used to be viewed as “property” belonging to their parents before the 18th

century. With the rise of rights advocacy groups, the beginning of rights language in

public discourse about children, and the ratification of the United Nations Convention on

the Rights of the Child (UNCRC) in 1989, new attention has been paid to children’s

development (Aries, 1996; Stearns, 2016). This was then followed by the shift from an

emphasis on rights that are mainly nurturance-based to the extension of autonomy-based

self-determination rights to children (Stearns, 2016). This perspective portrays children as

autonomous individuals with evolving capabilities who can freely express themselves and

decide in matters affecting them. This shift from parental authority to a nurturance-based

approach, followed by a child-centered approach, in which children are given

opportunities and freedom that their parents may have never had as children, marks a

tipping point in history and has implications for children’s social, cognitive, and

psychological development (Stearns, 2016).

Adolescents in China, especially those from urban settings, have grown up in a

climate of vast social and economic transformation that is much different from the social

environment in which their parents grew up. It is therefore of particular interest to explore

whether these parents, despite their very different experience of socialization, have

Page 17: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

7

changed their parenting practices in accordance with these shifts in views of children, or

whether they have largely continued parenting practices congruent with the way they

were raised. Very few studies, however, have explored the effects of social change on

these shifts in parenting beliefs and behaviors, attitudes about children’s rights, and

children’s changing socialization environment and development (e.g., opportunities to

experience rights, development of rights conception, psychological health). Some recent

research has begun to investigate the dynamic interactions between the influence of social

norm change as resulting from modernization and the continual influence of long-

standing traditional values and practices (Inglehart & Baker, 2000). Others have

attempted to redefine the cultural dichotomies of individualism-collectivism by further

differentiating the constructs (e.g., the normative and relational I-C (Kagitcibasi, 1997),

and horizontal and vertical I-C (Triandis, 1994)). Greenfield’s (2009) theory of social

change and human development proposed different developmental pathways found in

different societies according to their level of modernization and socioeconomic status and

delineates how a society adapts its values to these changing environments. With its

consideration of movement within culture, this theory provides a useful framework for

understanding the implications of socio-environmental change on shifting cultural values

and parenting practices.

China affords a unique cultural context ideally suited to explore the above

questions. The Confucianist culture has dominated China for thousands of years. Its

ideologies have become a central part of the Chinese cultural spirit and continue to

influence present Chinese society as well as the way Chinese people think and act in their

everyday lives (Wang, 2014). Traditional Confucian ethics, for example filial piety, has

Page 18: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

8

governed not only how people view relationships and interact within the family, but also

how they behave in other systems (e.g., school) and larger society (Pye, 1992). Given the

rapid and unprecedented economic growth and demographic transformations in the post-

Mao era, it is vital to investigate how Chinese families and societies have evolved with

this modernization, and at the same time how they may have maintained a continuity with

traditional values and practices. I am also interested in examining how urbanization and

social changes have influenced societal norms, as reflected in parental and adolescents’

attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors.

While modernization has drastically changed the landscape in urban areas, many

rural areas within China remain poor, traditional, and mainly agricultural economies

(Peerenboom, 2002). Moreover, the loosening of policy in migration has led to a dramatic

increase of rural to urban movement, with many rural youth venturing into the cities in

search of opportunities (Knight & Song, 1999). As a result, China provides a distinctive

setting where we could study families who are from a highly modern, developed, and

fast-changing urban areas, along with those who are from a much more traditional, rural

setting with much less exposure to Western influences, as well as those who are in

transition. Thus, the different pace of change in urban and rural China allows us to

examine how parenting and children’s rights conceptions may differ in different settings

coexisting within the same nation. The research findings are expected to shed light on

questions about the universal aspects of the development of autonomy and children’s

understandings of their own rights within varied cultural settings.

Page 19: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

9

Overview of Research Studies

The following two papers present studies of adolescents and mothers in both rural

and urban China. Paper 1 examines how cultural parenting practices transmit across

generations, and their impact on adolescents' psychological well-being. It is proposed that

parenting practices change over time as they adapt to the needs of the changing

socioeconomic conditions (Greenfield, 2009). Traditional parenting practices and values

face new environmental demands that emerge with social change, and may not be

adaptive to the demands of the new socioeconomic conditions. However, autonomy

supportive and responsive parenting is hypothesized to be universally beneficial for

children regardless of the cultural backgrounds and the changing environments. This

research aims to study the impact of social changes as manifested in the shifts in

parenting practices and their consequences on human development, as reflected in the

self-reports of adolescents regarding their psychological well-being.

Paper 2 narrows the focus to issues specific to children’s rights. While shifts in

societal views on children’s rights have been reflected in the ratification of the UNCRC

(1989), to this date, we still know very little about the fundamental processes in the

development of children’s rights conceptions in diverse cultural settings. In this paper, we

examine how the family socialization environment and sociodemographic factors (e.g.,

origins, current setting, gender) may play a role in the development of children’s rights

attitudes.

Anticipated Contribution

To date, research on the implications of parenting and children’s rights attitudes

was conducted mostly in Western cultural contexts. Through an investigation of values,

Page 20: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

10

attitudes, beliefs and practices in both mothers and adolescents in urban and rural China,

these studies will allow us to have a glimpse of past and current Chinese parenting,

providing insight into how parents form their parenting practices, and how thinking about

rights is developed. Together, these two papers will contribute to the emerging literature

on how socio-environmental factors, including social changes, need to be considered in

conjunction with developmental pathways and the fulfillment of autonomy needs in

diverse cultural settings. We believe that a developmental and ecological approach to

research that takes into account developmental and social changes is a potent tool that can

inform the design of parenting and institutional programs and policy that connects

individual, family, society, and culture.

Page 21: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

11

Paper 1

Socio-Cultural Change and Parenting in China: Autonomy Support, Democratic

Family Climate, and Adolescents’ Psychological Well-Being

Abstract

This study examined urban and rural Chinese adolescents’ (12-16 years) and their

mothers’ reports of maternal autonomy support and responsiveness, overall family

democratic climate and tolerance of dissent (N =256, with 64 mother-adolescent dyads

per setting). Relations between these dimensions of parenting and adolescents’

psychological well-being were examined. Drawing on both a universalistic perspective

(self-determination theory) and a cultural psychological perspective (Greenfield’s theory

of cultural change), this study examined the joint role of universal psychological

processes and cultural transformation in accounting for variations and similarities in

parenting and its outcomes in diverse settings within China.

Using retrospective recalls of how mothers were parented, changes of parenting

over a generation were also examined. We found that urban mothers reported parenting in

ways that are more autonomy supportive, democratic, and tolerant of dissent than both

rural mothers and their own mothers, whereas rural mothers only reported being more

tolerant of dissent compared to their own mothers. Regarding to relations between

parenting and adolescents’ psychological well-being, democratic family climate, as

perceived by adolescents, was found to be associated with higher adolescent life

satisfaction and fewer depressive symptoms in both urban and rural settings of China.

Page 22: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

12

Introduction

Baumrind’s Parenting Typologies

Since Baumrind's (1966, 1971) seminal work classifying different parenting

styles, there has been an exponential amount of research conducted over the last several

decades exploring the correlates of these parenting styles. Parenting style that is

categorized as authoritative involves practices that value and encourage autonomy, open

communication, and children’s ideas and input while also adhering to the firm

enforcement of rules (Grolnick, 2002). Authoritative parenting, characterized by high

levels of both responsiveness (warmth) and demandingness (control), has been

consistently found to be associated with a range of beneficial social, emotional, and

academic outcomes for children. Conversely, parenting that is characterized as

authoritarian or “psychologically controlling” (Barber, 1996) involves overcontrol in the

form of intruding and manipulating the child’s emotional development (e.g., through the

use of shaming or excessive guilt), or stresses blind obedience over rational discipline.

These forms of parenting, characterized by high demandingness yet low responsiveness,

have been found to negatively affect healthy child development and to be related to both

internalizing symptoms (such as depressive symptomology and low self-esteem) as well

as externalizing problems such as delinquency and low internalization of moral norms

(e.g., Assor, Roth, & Deci, 2004; Barber, 2002).

One of the criticisms of Baumrind’s classifications of parenting styles concerns

the applicability of typologies in different cultures (Chao, 1994; Grolnick, 2003).

Findings from cross-cultural studies suggested that optimal parenting may vary

depending on culture due to the different cultural values and socialization goals of

Page 23: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

13

parents. Hence, the question of whether the positive outcomes of authoritative parenting

and the negative sequelae associated with authoritarian parenting hold across cultures has

been a topic of debate. For example, scholars adhering to the theoretical perspectives

within cultural psychology have taken a culturally relativistic perspective on the self and

parenting (Greenfield, Keller, Fuligni, & Maynard, 2003; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). In

these approaches, conceptions of self and morality are depicted as largely transmitted to

individuals through cultural ideologies and participation in shared rituals and practices

(Shweder et al., 2006). The individualism-collectivism (I-C) distinction, for example,

arguably has been the most widely used construct in cross-cultural psychology to explain

cultural differences in attitudes, behaviors, self-construals, and family dynamics

(Kâğitçibaşi, 1994; Kim, 1997; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). For instance, building upon

the I-C distinction, Markus and Kitayama (1991) suggested that people in individualistic

societies, such as those in North America and Europe, tend to have more independent

self-construals and hence view the self as autonomous and independent from others,

leading to a moral focus on personal choice and the assertion of individual needs. In

contrast, people in collectivistic societies, which include much of Asia, South America,

and Africa, have a higher interdependent or sociocentric conception of the self. The

sociocentric self is believed to be more adaptive to the more hierarchical structure of

many traditional societies, and is associated with a corresponding moral orientation

emphasizing maintenance of harmony, strict adherence to the existing social norms and

roles, as well as respect for authority and hierarchy.

Supporting these cultural relativist perspectives, some scholars have suggested

that parental control/authoritarian parenting may have different meanings across cultures

Page 24: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

14

and hence may not carry the same negative consequences for children's development

amongst children from interdependent and collectivist cultures (Chao & Tseng, 2002).

For instance, Chao (1994, 1996) pointed out that in Chinese culture, there is a notion of

guan (“training”) in parenting, which places a strong emphasis on children’s obedience,

adherence to socially desirable behaviors, and respect for parental authority. While it has

some overlapping components with Baumrind’s conceptualization of authoritarian

parenting (e.g., parental strictness and control, rejection of the child’s autonomy) and

hence often has been misinterpreted as “authoritarian,” it also differs in important ways

including its emphasis on parental involvement and support. Moreover, unlike

authoritarian parenting, guan is seen as motivated by love and concern by the parent and

is often regarded as a highly important component of Chinese parenting in raising a well-

adjusted child (Chao, 1994). Particularly due to an understanding of the competitive

academic environment that they are in, Chinese adolescents may view and interpret

moderate strictness and control as an expression of parental concern, care, and positive

involvement for the sake of ensuring their future well-being (Wang, 2014). Hence, it has

been suggested that the notion of guan is commonly viewed as having a positive

connotation in Chinese culture (Tobin, Wu, & Davidson, 1989). Accordingly,

Baumrind’s original conceptualization of parenting styles, based on her observations of

typical Western cultural parenting, has been criticized as not fully capturing the cultural

nuances of parenting in Chinese or other non-Western cultures (e.g., Chao, 1994, 1996).

However, several recent studies attempting to examine these cross-cultural

theories have failed to demonstrate the hypothesized relationship between authoritarian

parenting and positive outcomes for children, as well as the positive interpretation of

Page 25: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

15

parental control in collectivist cultures, especially when authoritarian parenting is defined

as including a harsh, rigidly punitive, or psychologically-controlling parental manner. In

contrast to the assertion of Chao (1994), recent studies show that children and

adolescents, including those from collectivistic cultures, criticize several forms of

parental control commonly used in Asian cultures, such as psychological control (e.g.,

shaming, use of social comparisons) and they do not view these practices as a sign of

parental care and concern (Helwig, To, Wang, Liu, & Yang, 2014; Sorkhabi, 2012). In

fact, in a recent and comprehensive review, it was concluded that there were consistent

associations between authoritarian parenting and negative and conflictual family

dynamics, as well as poor maternal mental health across different ethnic groups

(Sorkhabi, 2012). However, more directive forms of parenting (including parental

monitoring and communication of clear boundaries) have been associated with positive

child outcomes in both Western and non-Western cultures (Sorkhabi, 2012), so long as

these forms of parenting are instantiated in a responsive and autonomy-supportive way

(e.g., with rational discipline, warmth and concern for the child).

Dimensional Theoretical Approach as Motivated by Self-determination Theory

Whereas Baumrind’s model of parenting typology was based on two dimensions

(responsiveness and demandingness), parenting styles are in fact multi-dimensional

constructs, as the debates over parenting style and culture reviewed in the last section

suggest (Chao, 1994; Sorkhabi, 2012). Authoritative parenting, for example, involves a

constellation of characteristics including parental acceptance, warmth, involvement,

psychological autonomy, and behavioral control (Steinberg, 1990). Trends in recent

parenting research hence have moved away from studying parenting using global

Page 26: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

16

constructs, and instead have called for more specificity in defining the components that

make up these parenting styles, and research examining the distinct contribution of

individual parenting dimensions to outcomes for children in different cultures (Darling &

Steinberg, 1993). This move to disaggregate the parenting typology has led to a dramatic

increase in research examining the correlates and outcomes of different specific parenting

dimensions.

One of the key dimensions of parenting comprising authoritative parenting is the

notion of autonomy support. Authoritative parents support children’s autonomy in several

ways, including being attentive and responsive to children’s perspectives and emotional

needs and individuality, and providing them with meaningful choices over key aspects of

their lives (Rudy & Grusec, 2001). A theoretical perspective that highlights the

importance of autonomy is self-determination theory (SDT). SDT proposes that

psychological autonomy (i.e., the freedom of an individual to self-determine their own

behavior according to their own values and interests), together with a sense of

competence and relatedness, are basic, universal psychological needs holding across

culture and time (Deci & Ryan, 2013). As autonomy is the focus of this dissertation, the

subsequent review will focus on research findings regarding this need (although

relatedness and competence have shown similar general patterns, see Ryan & Deci,

2011). Hence, socialization practices that foster children’s needs for psychological

autonomy, characterized by parenting practices that respect children as autonomous

agents, are hypothesized as crucial in promoting psychological well-being (Ryan & Deci,

2001).

Page 27: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

17

In line with the propositions of SDT, an emerging body of research conducted in

diverse settings, including Western and non-Western, individualist and collectivist

cultures, and urban and rural societies, consistently suggests that adolescents who

perceive their parents to be autonomy supportive and responsive also reported better

psychological adjustment (see Ryan & Deci, 2006). For example, Vansteenkiste and

colleagues (2005) examined the implications of parental autonomy support on students’

well-being and adjustment in China, a cultural context often characterized as

collectivistic and in which autonomy is often believed to be less important than in the

West (Pye, 1992). Contrary to this view, Vansteenkiste et al. (2005) found autonomy to

be positively correlated with psychological well-being and negatively correlated with

depression amongst Chinese university students. Similarly, Chirkov et al. (2003) moved

beyond simple dichotomies between individualistic versus collectivistic cultures by

adding another dimension, comparing vertical versus horizontal societies (i.e., societies

which value equality and mutual respect versus those that emphasize hierarchy and

obedience). Despite differences in cultural practices engaged in by individuals from four

diverse cultures (i.e., South Korea, Russia, Turkey, and the United States), the

relationship between perceived autonomy need satisfaction and psychological well-being

was maintained across cultures and gender. Both studies found that although there was a

stronger emphasis on interdependence and conformity in collectivist cultures or more

vertical societies, personal autonomy was nevertheless important and had the benefits of

enhancing psychological well-being. Similarly, perceived parental responsiveness

(caring) has been consistently found to predict less psychological distress and behavioral

problems among adolescents (Bogenschneider & Pallock, 2008). Indeed, responsiveness

Page 28: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

18

was found to be a complementary dimension that often goes hand in hand with parental

autonomy support; parents who are more caring or responsive to their children also are

more responsive to their developmental needs for autonomy (Peterson-Badali, Morine,

Ruck, & Slonim, 2004; To et al., 2017).

Moreover, recent studies have found that not only is autonomy supportive and

responsive parenting related to adolescents’ psychological functioning, but specifically

democratic aspects of autonomy make a unique contribution to the psychological well-

being of adolescents; these findings apply even to those from non-Western cultural

settings such as China, where democratic values are not commonly promoted (To et al.,

2017). That study examined specific aspects of democratic family environment as

reflected in the promotion of children’s freedom of expression, participation in family

decision-making, and respect for due process in instances where the child is accused of

wrongdoing. These aspects of democratic family environment were found to be

associated with urban and rural Chinese adolescents’ reports of greater life satisfaction

and lower levels of depression (To et al., 2017). These relations between democratic

features of social organization, such as support for autonomy and personal choice, and

psychological well-being, have also been replicated in other settings, such as schools (Jia

et al., 2009; Way, Reddy, & Rhodes, 2007). Opportunities for choice and decision

making in the classroom were significantly associated with adolescents’ psychological

health, self-esteem and fewer depressive symptoms among both American (Way et al.,

2007) and urban Chinese adolescents (Jia et al., 2009). Taken together, these findings

provide support for self-determination theory’s claim that adolescents from a non-

Page 29: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

19

Western, collectivistic culture also may benefit from socialization environments that are

autonomy supportive, responsive and democratic.

Intergenerational Transmission of Parenting

Given that parents who are more responsive, autonomy supportive and promote a

more democratic family environment contribute to better psychological outcomes in their

children, in the next sections we will look further into the factors that are related to the

development of parenting practices, including the role of social-cultural context and,

especially, the issue of cultural change. One question addressed by this thesis is how

cultural parenting practices are transmitted across generations, and the impact of socio-

cultural change on this process, as exemplified by the case of China.

Traditionally, two prominent perspectives have guided most research on the

intergenerational transmission of parenting: social learning theory and attachment theory.

Both theories take an enculturation view, emphasizing the effects of social transmission,

and have been widely used to explain why many children grow up replicating the

parenting styles in which they were raised. From the social learning perspective, children

learn and internalize parenting practices through experiential learning, observation and

imitation of parental models (Bandura, 1977). Through the lens of attachment theory,

Bowlby (1988) suggested that a child’s experience with their parents or primary caregiver

leads to the development of an internal working model that forms the foundation for

future relationships. It is hypothesized that this internal representation will then act as a

prototype and influence the way one interacts with others (e.g., their own children).

According to these theories, we would then expect that parents would likely adopt similar

parenting practices that they themselves experienced while growing up.

Page 30: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

20

On the other hand, more recent ecological frameworks (e.g., Bronfenbrenner &

Morris, 2006; Crockett & Silbereisen, 2000) posit that there could be various stable and

dynamic factors occurring on multiple, interacting levels that can shape cognition and

behaviors (including the way we parent). In addition to the direct influence and modeling

from parents, there are different levels of influencing factors at the biological,

psychological, social, cultural and historical levels that interact dynamically with each

other and change over time as a person develops and becomes a parent. Although there

are some relatively stable contextual factors (e.g., residing in an urban vs. rural setting,

socioeconomic status and parental education levels) that appear to play a significant role

in predicting the quality and style of parenting (Campbell & Gilmore, 2007; Chen et al.,

2000; Hoff, Laursen, & Tardif, 2002), recent studies have called for research into more

dynamic factors, such as modernization and sociocultural shift, that might influence

societal views of appropriate parenting (Conger, Belsky, & Capaldi, 2009). Research

from this perspective has shown that, instead of being a passive recipient of their

socialization and simply replicating their own experience, individuals play an active role

in constructing their parenting beliefs and practices as they experience changes in their

social environment (Campbell & Gilmore, 2007; Chen & Chen, 2010; Elder, 1994;

Greenfield, 2009).

Greenfield’s Theory of Sociocultural Shift

Moreover, it has been argued that society, culture, values and human behaviors

(including parenting) are not static. Greenfield’s (2009) theory of social change and

human development postulates relationships between ecological change and cultural

change, and that cultural orientations are adaptive to and influenced by the environmental

Page 31: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

21

conditions. Specifically, she argues that collectivistic values, such as emphasis on the

welfare of the group/community over individuals, are adaptive to Gemeinschaft

environments (“community”; characterized by relatively poor, rural, subsistence-based

environments with low levels of technology and formal education; Tönnies, 1887/1957).

On the other hand, individualistic values, such as a focus on personal choice, privacy and

materialism, are adaptive to Gesellschaft societies (“society”; characterized by relatively

wealthy, urban environments with high levels of technology and formal education;

Tönnies, 1887/1957). As urbanization has been accompanied by advancement in

technological development and increased higher education in urban populations, the

world has generally and gradually moved away from the Gemeinschaft toward the

Gesellschaft direction. Greenfield’s (2009) theory hence provides a framework for

understanding the implications of urbanization for shifts in cultural values and

orientations, as well as corresponding parenting practices.

It has been proposed that the boundaries between the two distinct cultural

orientations are more fluid and dynamic than what was previously theorized, especially

within developing societies (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). Indeed, many have begun to

examine the coexistence of individualism and collectivism at the macro-societal level, as

well as the integration of independent and interdependent socialization goals at the micro

level – in which both might vary across societal contexts and development (Tamis-

LeMonda et al., 2008). Similarly, different parenting practices may co-exist as families

respond and adapt to the global changing environment toward an urban lifestyle. Hence,

there has been a call to move away from polarizing cultures and toward understanding

how social and cultural changes, as a result of globalization and different policy

Page 32: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

22

implementations, may have shaped parental socialization goals, beliefs, and practices. In

the face of the rapid social, cultural, and economic changes that are currently underway in

contemporary China, this shift in cultural conceptualization is particularly relevant for

understanding how personal and sociocultural factors may interact and influence the

parenting of today’s children in China, and in turn, influence children’s psychological

well-being. These new perspectives also point to the importance of considering cultural

contexts and the effects of social change when studying the universality and function of

autonomy supportive and democratic socialization environments.

Social Change in China and Human Development

The impact of social change appears to operate at multiple levels. As Greenfield’s

theory of social change and human development and Bronfenbrenner’s ecological

framework suggest, adolescent development is affected by the dynamics of the immediate

social contexts that they experience on a daily basis, also known as microsystems (e.g.,

families), which are in turn affected by macro-level (e.g., societal) environment and

changes. Therefore, in order to understand how social changes may have impacted

adolescent development in a particular sociocultural context, we need first to understand

the sociocultural and historical context of the adolescents’ environment.

Within traditional China, Confucianism-prescribed parenting has been argued to

be adaptive in an agricultural and socialist environment in which collective efforts are

essential for the common good of the community (Lin, Huang, & Wang, 2015). Qualities

like obedience and following orders from authority are not only consistent with

traditional cultural values, but have also been highly valued in settings where most

workers would secure their life-long employment in the work units of a state-owned

Page 33: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

23

company, which was the case in the past for most workers in China (Cai & Wang, 2010).

However, the economic reform and open-up policy (started in 1978) has begun a new

historical chapter in China. Under the reform policy, China has undergone a dramatic

transition from state socialism into a market-oriented economy. This economic change

comes along with the demand for very different qualities and skills in the labor market, as

well as a culture that is influenced by a myriad of social forces and ideologies. Other

values and ideologies such as capitalism, individual choice, and freedom were introduced

and began to coexist with traditional Chinese values (Wang, 2014). This confluence of

ideologies has likely influenced Chinese parents to reflect upon the way they were raised

and, by extension, to possibly adapt their child-rearing practices (Wang, 2014). As

Flanagan (2000) suggested, social change challenges established patterns of behaviors

and thinking, leading individuals to question and reflect upon their current practice.

Parents in China, especially those who reside in urban areas, witnessed drastic

intergenerational change as they became parents of today’s children. It is likely that, as

Chinese parents became aware of changing societal demands in a competitive and

market-oriented society, they also became alert to the fact that traditional Chinese

parenting (that aims to instill cooperative, obedient behaviors) is no longer compatible

with the qualities of labour that the market demands (which value characteristics like

individual initiative-taking, competitiveness, creativity, and independence in order to

thrive (Naftali, 2016).

As parents react to social and cultural change by questioning and re-evaluating

established parenting practices and beliefs, they construct their own way of parenting that

is more attuned and sensitive to the needs of their children as well as societal demands

Page 34: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

24

(Greenfield, 2009). Indeed, parenting in contemporary China has been observed to be

gradually shifting away from the traditional stereotypical image of authoritarian parenting

to practices that increasingly emphasize the cultivation of children’s own sense of self

and qualities like creativity and independence (e.g., Chen & Chen, 2010; Way et al.,

2013). In a cohort-design study, Chen and colleagues studied parental childrearing

attitudes between parents of elementary school children in two cohorts (1998 and 2002)

in urban China. While parents of both cohorts did not differ from each other regarding

their emphasis on academic achievement (both highly valued its importance), parents in

the later cohort reported themselves to be higher in parental warmth and autonomy

support, as well as lower on power assertion than those in the earlier cohort (Chen &

Chen, 2010). It appears that there is a gradual shift in parenting in China, especially in

fast-changing urban settings, that is consistent with Greenfield’s proposition that

parenting is adapting to changing social ecologies. Indeed, when Lu and Chang (2013)

conducted semi-structured interviews with urban Chinese parents, none of the parents

reported beliefs or practices that are fully congruent with traditional Chinese parenting.

Instead, the majority of parents reported parent-child relationships that were egalitarian,

democratic and child-centered.

This transformation of parenting beliefs and behaviors, especially in the urban

areas, also has been accompanied and affected by the state’s implementation of the one-

child policy in 1980. After this policy was enacted, Chinese parents expressed feeling the

pressure to invest all of their resources into their sole offspring to ensure their success

(Naftali, 2016). To achieve this goal, many urban parents turned to various resources,

from magazine articles by parenting experts to social media, for answers on how to best

Page 35: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

25

parent their only children. Messages that were widely spread through these media were

heavily influenced by Western ideologies and much of the content was about encouraging

parents to expand their childrearing goals beyond the behavioral and psychological

qualities that are traditionally valued (Naftali, 2016). Nevertheless, the shift in parenting

practices and beliefs was not purely one sided. In fact, parents in China have been

receiving a mixture of different kinds of messages from the popular media. On the one

hand, academic scholars and educators in China have been advocating the importance of

parents’ attending to children’s psychological needs and promoting their

individualization. On the other hand, extreme authoritarian parenting emphasizing the

importance of maintaining hierarchical intergenerational relationships has been

popularized by the writers of books such as Battle Hymn of the Tiger Mother and

Brothers and Sisters of Peking University (originally titled Beat Them Into Peking

University), which advocated authoritarian forms of parenting as essential for raising

successful children (Chua, 2011; Xiao, 2011). Moreover, Chinese parents expressed

continuing support for the virtue of filial piety and traditional collectivistic values that

emphasize relatedness as being equally important to cultivate in their children (Lin et al.,

2015). These mixed and, at times, contradictory views and beliefs about parenting

contribute to contemporary Chinese parents’ uncertainty about the “proper” way of

raising their children (Naftali, 2014). Consequently, as scholars (e.g., Tamis-LeMonda et

al., 2008) suggested, parenting in China was observed to have shifted from the pure

traditional form of practice and is now characterized by the dynamic coexistence of the

individualistic and collectivistic developmental goals of autonomy, relatedness, and the

importance of child obedience.

Page 36: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

26

While it is impossible to go back in time to examine what parenting was like in

China before the social and economic reforms, in the present study, I attempted to obtain

a glimpse of parenting in the previous generation through mothers’ retrospective recall of

how they were parented. I am interested in examining whether and how mothers are

incorporating their experience of social changes into their current parenting practices. As

Chinese mothers begin to adapt and change their parenting practices as a result of macro-

level societal changes, it is also of particular interest to examine whether and how these

changing parental practices may also impact adolescents’ development. Putting together

the proposition of SDT and the shift in focus toward more autonomy supportive

parenting, I am interested in exploring how these changes in parenting recently observed

in contemporary China may also contribute to the well-being of today’s Chinese

adolescents.

Different Pace of Development in Urban versus Rural China

The socioeconomic, ideological (i.e., parenting beliefs and behaviors) and

demographic changes described above have, however, mainly taken place in urban China.

Rural areas, especially cities which are far away from the coastal region, have remained

undeveloped, economically disadvantaged, highly traditional, and lacking in higher

educational opportunities (Knight & Song, 1999), corresponding well to Greenfield’s

(2009) Gemeinschaft environment. Compared to urban China, rural areas experienced

much less impact brought about by the reform of the market economy. Moreover, rural

Chinese parents are allowed to have more than one child to help the families with farm

work. In research with Western samples, social context has been found to play a

significant role in predicting both parenting and adolescents’ outcomes. For example,

Page 37: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

27

socioeconomic disadvantage has been found to be associated with less warm and more

controlling parenting (Scaramella, Neppl, Ontai, & Conger, 2008). In research conducted

in China, Chen and his colleages (2010) found that as urban parents perceived more

social change (including work-related opportunities, self-improvement in work) than their

counterparts in the rural area, their urban adolescents also correspondingly reported lower

levels of parental control and greater encouragement of independence in comparison to

the rural adolescents. The current study sought to examine potential differences between

perceptions of parenting in urban and rural areas within China, thus examining both a

Gesellschaft and a Gemeinschaft environment (Greenfield, 2009) under the umbrella of a

shared cultural context. The different pace of change in the urban and rural areas would

thus allow us to examine how parenting may differ in different settings coexisting within

the same nation that broadly correspond to the Gesellschaft and Gemeinschaft distinction,

and in turn, how these differences may impact adolescents’ psychological well-being.

Increased Rural-to-Urban Migration:

In addition to the distinctly different experience between people in the urban and

rural regions of China, the full-scale reform towards a market economy and the loosening

of restrictions on migration have together led to a dramatic increase of rural-to-urban

migration (Ma, 2002). Since the implementation of the reform and open door policy,

there has been a significant amount of foreign investment into the manufacturing industry

in China (Ma, 2002). In response to the unprecedented growth of the economy and an

increased demand for labor in the urban cities, many rural residents moved to urban areas

to seek greater job opportunities and the possibilities of a better life. Therefore, parents of

todays’ urban children are not a homogenous group. Instead, they are composed of

Page 38: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

28

parents from different origins, with many of them having moved from rural regions to the

urban cities at some point in their life (Ma, 2002). While some previous studies (e.g.,

Fuligni & Zhang, 2004; Wen & Lin, 2012) have differentiated and identified setting

differences in urban and rural China (or examined a specific migrant population), no

studies that I am aware of have also examined the impact of social migration for parents

who have already settled and reside in the urban setting, and how parents’ place of origin

(urban versus rural) may have an impact on their current parenting behaviors. Whereas I

expect that rural mothers would be exposed to the least influence of the societal reforms,

and urban mothers born and raised in the urban areas to be exposed to the greatest impact

of the reforms, mothers who moved from rural areas to urban cities may be in an

intermediate phase, having had a mixed socialization experience. Mothers who moved

from rural to urban areas as a child may still be using more traditional parenting practices

and upholding socialization goals that are more consistent with the traditional values and

beliefs in their daily interactions at home. In the meantime, they also would have

experienced the most drastic change in their surrounding societal environment as they

develop and become parents themselves within the milieu of fast-changing urban China.

Objectives of the Present Study and Hypotheses

The purpose of the current research was twofold. First, I desired to investigate

what parenting in urban and rural China looks like in contemporary China. As discussed

earlier, I am interested in discerning how mothers from different origin-current setting

combinations (urban, rural-to-urban, and rural) may differ from each other in their

childrearing practices. I expected that the three groups would differ from each other in

terms of autonomy supportive and democratic parenting, with urban mothers being the

Page 39: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

29

most autonomy supportive and democratic, and rural mothers being the least supportive

of these types of parenting. On the other hand, I expected that mothers of the three groups

would not differ from each other in terms of their nurturance and responsiveness (Chen et

al., 2010).

Previous studies either have focused on adolescents' perceptions of parenting, or

examined parents' self-reports of their parenting practices. Yet, few studies have looked

at both reporters (e.g., mothers and their children) and compared their perceptions of

parenting and the relative contribution of these perceptions to adolescents' psychological

well-being. The current study examined the perspectives of both the mother and the

adolescent respondents, and explored whether and how mothers and their adolescent

children may differ in their views of parenting. The little extant prior research measuring

both mothers and adolescents’ report on parenting has shown non-significant to moderate

correspondence between their reports of parenting, with mothers reporting themselves to

be more autonomy supportive and less controlling than their children’s reports. In these

studies, adolescents’ ratings generally have been found to be more valid than those of

their mothers (Cheung, Pomerantz, Wang, & Qu, 2016; Gonzales, Cauce, & Mason,

1996). Given that adolescents’ perceptions of parenting have been found to be of central

importance within SDT accounts of psychological well-being (Chirkov & Ryan, 2001), I

expected that adolescents’ reports, as coming from individuals who directly experience

the socialization practices, would be more highly correlated with their psychological

well-being than would their mothers’ reports.

Controlling for the effects of demographic variables, I also aimed to advance the

understanding of the role of mothering and democratic family climate on adolescents’

Page 40: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

30

psychological functioning. Based on SDT, I expected that both responsive parenting and

the provision of personal autonomy would be related to adolescents' psychological well-

being. As suggested by findings of recent research (e.g., To et al., 2017), I further

expected that democratic family environment would have unique functional significance,

above and beyond general autonomy support and responsiveness, for adolescents’

psychological well-being.

Given the hypothesized relationships between parenting and adolescents’ health

and well-being, the second goal of the current study was to examine how different

parenting dimensions may have changed over time and how different demographic and

socialization variables of the mothers may influence the way they currently parent. On

the one hand, according to social learning and attachment theory, I expected that mothers’

own socialization experience would serve as a prototype and predict how they currently

parent. On the other hand, I also expected that other variables (e.g., the experience of

urbanization for urban-residing mothers) may lead mothers to reflect on their own

experience and construct parenting beliefs that are more in line with the developmental

needs of adolescents in a changing society.

Specifically, I looked at the mothers’ current setting (i.e., whether they currently

live in urban versus rural areas of China), their origins (i.e., whether they were born in

urban versus rural areas of China), parental education level, and their retrospective recall

of parenting and family climate, to examine which variables may predict their current

parenting practice. While there are no studies I am aware of examining how parents’

origin and their migration from rural to urban areas may affect parenting, I hypothesized

that rural-to-urban mothers would continue to be influenced by the traditional Chinese

Page 41: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

31

parenting and hence would parent in a less autonomy-supportive fashion than their urban

counterparts. In comparison, urban mothers who were born and grew up in a modern,

urban setting may have more exposure to a Gesellschaft social ecology, and hence may

be more responsive to their children’s autonomy needs. Regardless of urban/rural setting,

I also expected that the way mothers were parented would directly influence their current

parenting practices, with mothers who were raised by autonomy supportive parents being

more autonomy supportive as they themselves become parents, and those who recalled

responsive parenting would show more current responsiveness to their children.

However, given the dramatic changes that have taken place in urban China in the past

few decades, I also expect that contemporary Chinese parenting, especially in the urban

areas, overall would demonstrate a significant shift from the traditional Chinese parenting

these mothers themselves may have experienced in the previous generation.

Method

Participants and Research Sites

The total sample consisted of 128 Chinese adolescents and their mothers from two

research sites (in urban and rural China), with 64 adolescent-mother dyads from each

setting. Among the adolescent participants, 64 were early-adolescents (M = 12.45, SD =

.95) and 64 were mid-adolescents (M = 15.28, SD = .66), with gender evenly distributed

within each setting and age group combination. Because the urban sample contained

dyads that were approximately evenly distributed between those whose mothers were

originally from urban Chinese settings (30 dyads) and those whose mothers migrated to

urban settings from rural areas at some point in their lives (34 dyads), I was able to

compare maternal origin (i.e., urban versus rural-to-urban) in our analyses. The

Page 42: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

32

adolescents in both the urban and rural-to-urban maternal origin group were born and

raised in the urban area, whereas those in the rural group were born and raised in the rural

area, as was true of their mothers.

The sample was drawn from schools in two distinct locales in China: the city of

Nanjing in Jiangsu province and the rural area of Feicheng in Shandong Province. These

research sites were selected to represent different milieux within China and to provide a

contrast between adolescents and mothers who were from a modern, economically

developed city and those who were from a much more traditional, agriculturally-based,

rural community with far less exposure to Western influences. The urban Chinese

subsample was drawn from schools serving a largely middle-class population located in

the city of Nanjing, the capital city of Jiangsu province and one of the largest commercial

centers in the East China region. Residents of Nanjing, like those in other modern cities

in China, overall have a much higher standing of living, level of education, and exposure

to foreign cultures than people living in rural areas. Average per capita wage for residents

of Nanjing is 40, 286 yuan, or US$6,486 (Nanjing Statistical Yearbook, 2013). Parental

education (in years) and occupational levels were listed in Table 1. Parental occupation

(both mothers and fathers) was scored on a 5-point scale (Ren & Edwards, 2015), with

“1” indicating an individual who is unemployed (including being a homemaker), “2” as

non-technical or semi-technical worker (e.g., farmers, factory workers), “3” as technical

worker (e.g., owners of small stores, drivers, technicians and mechanics), “4” as semi-

professional (e.g., elementary teachers, managers and public servant), “5” as professional,

officer and administrator (e.g., doctors, engineers, lawyers, teachers of higher education,

researchers, middle- and high-rank government officials). The average occupational level

Page 43: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

33

for the urban group was 4.03 (SD = 1.18) and the average occupational level for the rural-

to-urban group was 3.67 (SD = 1.11).

The rural sample came from two schools located in the rural area of Feicheng in

Western Shandong Province. The region from which the sample was drawn was chosen

to be representative of a typical rural and agricultural region in China, and thus

substantially less developed economically and more traditional in character than the

larger urban centers. The average occupational level was 2.19 (SD = .42). The majority

(76%) of Chinese rural parents were farmers by occupation. Overall, mothers and fathers

in Nanjing (both urban and rural-to-urban parents) had attained a significantly higher

educational and occupational level than their counterparts in the rural area (see Table 1).

Table 1. Parental education and occupational levels (in years and in categories).

Urban Urban-to-Rural Rural Mother’s Educational Level (years) 14.75a

(SD = 2.86) 12.79b

(SD =3.32) 7.87c

(SD =2.13) Father’s Education Level (years) 14.69a

(SD = 3.34) 14.06a

(SD = 2.32) 9.84b

(SD=2.03) Education

Never received any education 0% 0% 1% Grade school only 2% 5% 25% Middle school only 8% 10% 52% High school only 5% 22% 19% Post-secondary education (e.g., college/associate degree) 20% 31% 2% University degree 40% 27% 0% Master’s/Doctoral degree 13% 4% 0% Unknown education level 12% 1% 1%

Occupational level 4.03a (SD = 1.18)

3.67a (SD = 1.11)

2.19b

(SD = .42) Note.Superscriptsareusedtoindicatepost-hoccomparisonsbetweenthethreeorigin-settingsintermsoftheireducationallevels.Ratingssharingthesameletterswithineachrowdidnotdiffersignificantlyfromoneanother,whereasratingshavingdifferentletterswithineachrowdifferedsignificantlyfromoneanother.

Page 44: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

34

Procedures

Participants were recruited from participating public schools in China but tested

within their homes in order to allow for more extensive data collection sessions and the

involvement of parents. Parents who had expressed interest in participating in the study

were contacted by telephone and details of the research study were explained during the

phone call. A visit was paid to the home of interested families at a time convenient to

them. Questionnaires were administered in two separate rooms for each mother-

adolescent dyad. Parental informed consent and adolescents’ assent (verbal and written)

was obtained prior to data collection. All participants were told that participation would

be on a wholly volunteer basis, that confidentiality would be guaranteed, and that

participation could be discontinued at any time.

Participants (adolescents and their mothers) were given, first, an interview

examining their spontaneous conceptions of salient children’s rights issues. Interviews

were conducted in Chinese (Mandarin) by our Chinese collaborator or trained research

assistants. Participants were told that there were no right or wrong answers. All

interviews were audio-recorded with the permission of the interviewees, and

subsequently transcribed verbatim in Chinese for coding. While the interviews conducted

were not part of the current research, these results have been reported in a conference

presentation (To & Helwig, 2015). Subsequently, participants completed a set of self-

report questionnaires. During completion of the questionnaire, a trained native research

assistant was present to clarify any questions that might arise. Urban Chinese families

received two movie tickets and rural Chinese families received RMB150 (approximately

24 USD) as a token of appreciation for their participation. All procedures in this study

Page 45: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

35

were reviewed and approved by the University of Toronto Research Ethics Board. On

average, adolescents took approximately 30 minutes and mothers took approximately 40

minutes to complete the questionnaires.

Measures

To ensure accurate understanding and readability of the questionnaires (especially

for the rural parents), Chinese translations of all measures used in this study were first

pilot tested with four urban and four rural Chinese adolescent-mother dyads.

Incorporating feedback from the participants, we then modified some of the wording in

order to improve readability and comprehension of the questionnaires. Standard back-

translation techniques (Brislin, 1970) were then employed to ensure the accuracy of the

translations.

Autonomy support and responsiveness. Adolescent participants were given the

Perception of Parents scales (POPS, originally derived from Robbins, 1995; see

Appendix A) to assess their perceived maternal autonomy support and responsiveness

(warmth and involvement). The scales consisted of nine items about autonomy support

(e.g., “My mother allows me to decide things for myself”) and twelve items about

responsiveness (e.g., “My mother puts time and energy into helping me”). Items in this

questionnaire were rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (not at all true) to

7 (very true). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s alphas) for the adolescent reported

autonomy support and responsiveness subscales were acceptable (alpha for autonomy

support = .81 and for responsiveness = .83).

Mothers were asked to fill out two versions of the Perception of Parents scales

(POPS). One was modified to measure mothers’ self-report of their own current

Page 46: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

36

parenting, and another one measured mothers’ retrospective report of how they perceived

that they were parented when they themselves were adolescents. Since the POPS were

originally created for children to report on their parents’ parenting, minor wording

changes were made in the mother self-reported POPS to minimize social desirability

concerns (e.g., one of the reversed items, “My mother doesn’t seem to think of me often”,

was not reversed in the self-report scale and rephrased as “I think of my child often”.)

Since there is no differentiation between past and present tense in Chinese language, the

POPS used by mothers when reporting their experiences of how they perceived being

parented was the same as the one filled out by their adolescents. The only difference was

that mothers were asked to recall how they were parented when they were at their

adolescents’ age. The internal consistencies (Cronbach’s alphas) for the mothers’ self-

reported current autonomy support and responsiveness were .69 and .76 respectively. The

internal consistencies for mothers’ retrospective recall of how they were parented were

.81 (autonomy support) and .85 (responsiveness).

Democratic climate and tolerance of dissent. To examine the unique role of

democratic family environment, both adolescents and mothers filled out the five-item

democratic family climate measure (To et al., 2017; see Appendix B). This scale

measures particular democratic dimensions of family life and structure, such as children’s

freedom of expression (e.g., “There is mutual respect between me and my parents even in

areas in which we disagree”), perceptions of due process (e.g., “My parents will listen to

my explanation and offer a fair hearing if I am accused of wrongdoing”), and

involvement in decision making (e.g., “In my family, I am given the chance to help make

decisions”). Each item is assessed on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all

Page 47: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

37

true) to 7 (very true). Similar to the POPS, mothers were asked both to self-report their

current family democratic climate, as well as their retrospective perceived family

democratic climate when they were adolescents. Reliabilities of the 5-item democratic

family climate scale were .71 (for adolescent report), .62 (for mother self-report) and .80

(for mothers’ retrospective report). An additional item measuring tolerance of dissent (“It

is possible to criticize my parents’ decisions”) was presented in conjunction with the

democratic climate measure but was analyzed separately, given the unique predictive

power of this item as found in prior studies (e.g., Khoury-Kassabri & Ben-Arieh, 2009;

To et al., 2017).

Psychological Well-being. Finally, adolescent participants completed three

measures of psychological well-being. Given the hypothesized relations between parental

control of autonomy and internalizing symptomology (Chirkov & Ryan, 2001; Hasebe,

Nucci, & Nucci, 2004), participants were given the 10-item version of the Children’s

Depression Inventory (CDI short; Kovacs, 2011). Each item on the CDI consists of a set

of three statements describing a symptom of depression. Responses were scored in order

of increasing severity from 0 to 2. As another measure of common internalizing

symptomology, participants were also given the short form of the Revised Children’s

Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS-2; Reynolds & Richmond, 2008). RCMAS-2 short

form is a 10-item Likert-type questionnaire used to measure child trait anxiety.

Participants answered questions about their worries on a 4-point scale, ranging from 1

(Never) to 4 (Always) for each question. As a more general measure of well-being,

participants were given the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, &

Griffin, 1985). Participants indicated agreement with each of the statements (e.g., “I am

Page 48: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

38

satisfied with my life”) on a 7-point scale, ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7

(Strongly Agree). All of these measures have been determined to be valid for mainland

Chinese populations in prior research (e.g., Qin, Pomerantz, & Wang, 2009; To et al.,

2017; Yang, Ollendick, Dong, & Xia, 1995). Reliabilities of these measures in the current

study were acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .64 for CDI, .81 for RCMAS-2, and .73 for

the Life Satisfaction Scale).

Results

In this study, while I tried to categorize the heterogeneous group of urban mothers

into several different groups according to the age at which they migrated to urban areas,

preliminary analyses showed that groups that migrate at different ages did not

significantly differ from one another. Hence, to increase the statistical power for analyses,

I only differentiated urban mothers who were born and raised in urban areas from those

who were born in rural areas and migrated to urban areas.

While parental education and occupational levels may act as confounds when

comparing the three origin-setting groups in the following analyses, these variables are

largely orthogonal amongst the groups. In fact, these demographic variables are central to

the characteristics of Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft forms of social ecologies and hence

are intended to be part of the categorical comparisons. Therefore these demographic

variables were not treated as covariates in the following between-group comparisons.

Subsequent analyses examined Chinese mothers’ and adolescents’ reports of

current parenting, correspondences between mothers’ and adolescents’ reports,

correlations between adolescents’ and mothers’ reports and adolescent psychological

Page 49: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

39

well-being, relations between mothers’ current reports of parenting and their retrospective

reports of how they were parented, and lastly, correlates of mothers’ current parenting.

Chinese Mothers’ Report of Their Current Parenting

To examine the current landscape of parenting and family climate in

contemporary China, we first looked at how mother participants reported on how they are

currently parenting their adolescents. Univariate analysis of variances (ANOVAs, see

Table 1a) were conducted and revealed that mothers from the three different origin-

setting groups (urban, rural-to-urban, and rural) differed significantly from one another in

their self-reported parenting. As seen in Table 2a, post-hoc analyses comparing mothers

from the three origin-setting groups indicated that urban mothers reported themselves to

be significantly more autonomy supportive and responsive than rural-to-urban and rural

mothers did. Urban mothers also reported themselves to be significantly more democratic

and tolerant of dissent than rural mothers, whereas rural-to-urban mothers did not differ

from the other two groups on these variables. Taken together, these findings confirm the

expectations that urban mothers would parent in a more autonomy supportive and

democratic way than rural mothers, and that rural-to-urban mothers would generally

parent in ways that were in between the other groups on these measures.

Adolescents’ Report of Their Mothers’ Parenting

Adolescents’ reports of their mothers’ current parenting practices were

investigated with univariate analysis of variance (ANOVAs) in order to determine

whether adolescents of mothers from different origin-setting groups differed in their

reports. As shown in Table 2b, the three groups only differed significantly in the area of

tolerance of dissent. Specifically, adolescents of mothers from the urban and rural-to-

Page 50: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

40

urban groups reported their mothers to be more tolerant of dissent than did rural

adolescents. However, the three groups did not differ from each other in their reports of

parental autonomy support, responsiveness, and democratic family climate.

Comparisons Between and Correspondence Among Mothers’ and Adolescents’

Report of Parenting

In order to answer the question of whether reports of parenting from mothers and

adolescents differed significantly from each other, repeated measures analyses of

variance (ANOVAs) were conducted (with setting, maternal origin, adolescent’s age

group and gender as between subject factors). Findings for mother-adolescent dyadic

comparisons showed that mothers' self reports of their parenting differed significantly

from adolescents' reports (see Table 3). Mothers consistently rated themselves to be more

autonomy supportive (F(1, 123) = 3.99, p < .05), democratic (F(1,123) = 4.67, p < .05),

and tolerant of dissent (F(1, 123) = 21.00, p < .001) than the way their adolescents

reported them to be (see Tables 2a and 2b). Mothers and adolescents did not differ,

however, in their reports of responsiveness.

The extent to which adolescents’ reports of parenting corresponded with mothers’

self-reports was evaluated using simple correlations. The correspondence between

adolescents’ and mothers’ reports of autonomy supportive parenting and tolerance of

dissent was in the low-moderate range (rs = .20 and .18 respectively, p < .05), whereas

the correspondence between the reports was non-significant for responsive parenting and

democratic family climate (rs = .08 and .12 respectively, ns) (Table 3).

Page 51: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

41

Table 2. Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD) of POPS and Democratic Climate Scale as reported by (a) Mothers for their Current Parenting, (b) Adolescents for their Mothers’ Current Parenting, and (c) Mothers for How They were Parented (Retrospectively)

(a)Mother(Self-reportCurrent) (b)Adolescent-report(mother)

Urban

Rural-to-Urban Rural F

ηp2

Urban

Rural-to-Urban

Rural

F

ηp2

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

AutonomySupport 6.08a .62

5.54b .83

5.22b .73

10.48***

.15 5.58 0.96

5.41 1.16

5.12 .95

1.94(ns)

.03

Responsiveness 6.29a .66

5.59b .84

5.75b .68

6.08** .10

5.82 1.09

5.83 .99

5.61 .82

.84(ns) .01

DemocraticClimate 6.10a .91

5.49ab .79

5.49b 1.00

3.51*

.06

5.7 1.26

5.15 1.47

5.32 .9

1.46(ns)

.02

ToleranceofDissent 5.64a 1.27 4.86ab 1.67 4.32b 1.51 6.07** .10 4.30a 1.95 4.68a 1.77 3.14b 1.76 9.12*** .14

(c)Mother(Retrospective)

Urban

Rural-to-Urban

Rural F ηp2

M SD M SD

M SD

AutonomySupport 4.73 1.15 4.8 1.24 5.22 0.89

2.73(ns) .05

Responsiveness 4.94a 0.84

5.00a 1.16

5.83b 0.82

12.32***

.18

DemocraticClimate 4.67a 1.09 5.09ab 1.40 5.49b 0.97

4.32* .07

ToleranceofDissent 3.96 1.38 4.11 1.41 3.48 1.81 1.84(ns) .03

Note.Ratingscalesareona7-pointscale,with1=“notatalltrue”to7=“verytrue.”Note.Superscriptsareusedtoindicatepost-hoccomparisonsbetweenthethreeorigin-settingswithineachparentingdimension.Ratingssharingthesameletterswithineachparentingdimensiondidnotdiffersignificantlyfromoneanother,whereasratingshavingdifferentletterswithineachparentingdimensiondifferedsignificantlyfromoneanother.Note.*p≤.05,**p≤.01,***p≤.001.

Page 52: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

42

Table 3. Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD) and Correlations Among Mother Self-reported and Adolescent Reported Parenting and Family Democratic Climate

Mother Self-report Adolescent Report t (Cohen's d) r

M SD M SD Autonomy Support 5.49 .79

5.28 1.03 * (d = 0.18) .20*

Responsiveness 5.81 .76

5.68 .92 ns (d = 0.11) .08 Democratic Climate 5.61 .94

5.34 1.16 * (d = 0.20) .12

Tolerance of Dissent 4.77 1.59 3.84 1.91 *** (d = 0.41) .18* Note. *p ≤.05, **p ≤.01, ***p ≤.001.

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among Variables

Pearson-product moment correlations were used to examine relations among

variables. Table 4 shows the means, standard deviations, and correlations of all study

variables related to mothers’ current parenting and adolescents’ psychological well-being

for the full sample. The univariate distributions for each measures of psychological well-

being were approximately normal. Skewness and kurtosis for each variable was examined

and determined to be within acceptable limits (Tabachnick & Fidel, 2001).

In the present study, we hypothesized that parenting variables would be associated

with adolescents’ psychological well-being. These hypothesized relations were generally

supported. Adolescent-reported autonomy support, responsiveness and democratic

climate were all correlated positively with depression (reversed) and life satisfaction.

Adolescents’ anxiety (reversed) was positively correlated with their report of family

democratic climate, but negatively correlated with their report of tolerance of dissent at

home. However, mother-reported family environmental variables were found not to be

significantly correlated with any measures of adolescent-reported psychological well-

being. Since only adolescent-reported (but not mother-reported) parenting and family

Page 53: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

43

environmental variables were associated with adolescent-reported psychological

adjustment, only adolescent-reported variables were used in the following regression

analyses.

Page 54: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

44

Table 4.

Mean (M), Standard Deviations (SD), and Pearson Correlations of All Study Variables Related to Family Socialization Environment

and Adolescents’ Psychological Well Being For The Full Sample

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Autonomy Support (A) --

2. Responsiveness (A) .84*** --

3. Democratic Climate (A) .71*** .67*** --

4. Tolerance of Dissent (A) .27** .19* .20* --

5. Autonomy Support (M) .20* .17 .27** .25** --

6. Responsiveness (M) .08 .08 .27** .02 .61*** --

7. Democratic Climate (M) .03 .01 .12 .09 .62*** .58*** --

8. Tolerance of Dissent (M) .05 .04 .09 .18* .46*** .26** .33*** --

9. Depression (R) .36*** .32*** .39*** -.07 .15 .16 .09 -.04 --

10. Anxiety (R) .15 .13 .18* -.20* .14 .15 .12 -.09 .44*** --

11. Life Satisfaction .42*** .36*** .41*** .03 .17 .13 .13 .08 .38*** .36*** --

Mean 5.28 5.68 5.34 3.84 5.49 5.81 5.61 4.77 1.72 2.85 4.94

SD 1.03 .92 1.16 1.91 .79 .76 .94 1.59 .23 .51 1.08

Note. (M) = mother-reported; (A) = adolescent-reported; (R) = reverse scored. *p ≤.05, **p ≤.01, ***p ≤.001.

Page 55: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

45

Associations Between Parenting and Adolescents’ Psychological Well-Being

In order to control for demographic variables in the regression model, we first

investigated which of the demographics were significantly related to the outcome

variables. Symptoms of depression were found to vary with gender, female participants

(M = 1.77, SD = .21) reported a higher level of depression symptoms compared to the

male participants (M = 1.66, SD = .24); F(1, 116) = 6.44, p < .05, ηp2 = .05. In addition,

early-adolescents (M = 2.95, SD = .58) displayed more anxiety symptoms than mid-

adolescents (M = 2.74, SD = .42); F(1, 116) = 5.32, p < .05, ηp2 = .04. A main effect of

setting was not found, demonstrating that neither levels of depression, anxiety nor life

satisfaction varied according to the urban or rural setting. Hence setting was not included

in the following hierarchical regression analyses.

Next, several hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to examine

relations between family environment variables and adolescents’ psychological well-

being. Demographic variables (e.g., adolescents’ gender, age group) were entered in the

first step as controls and also to allow evaluation of the predictive power of the parenting

variables over and above that of the demographic variables. In order to avoid over-fitting

of the model, only demographic variables that have significant relations with the outcome

variable were included in the first step of the regression model. All of the variables of

interest (i.e., all parenting variables) were included in the following steps to explore the

relation between various parenting dimensions and adolescents’ psychological well-

being. Demographic family climate was entered at Step 3 in order to determine whether

this family environment variable would have predictive power with respect to

psychological well-being above and beyond what would be predicted by general

Page 56: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

46

measures of parental autonomy support and responsiveness that were entered in Step 2.

To check for multi-collinearity, tolerance statistics and variance inflation factors were

examined for all regression models and none was > 5.0. Thus, the multiple regression

analyses were not considered to be biased (Neter, Wasserman, & Kutner, 1990).

The regression models of adolescents’ reported life satisfaction and depression

using maternal autonomy support and responsiveness as predictors in Step 2 were

significant, with autonomy supportive parenting predicting greater life satisfaction and

lower levels of depression. In the third step of the regression analyses, democratic family

climate also was a significant predictor in the positive direction, predicting greater life

satisfaction and lower depression, above and beyond what was predicted by general

measures of parental autonomy support and responsiveness (Maternal autonomy support

was no longer significant after the addition of democratic family climate in Step 3 in both

models, see Table 5). For the regression model for anxiety symptoms, neither the addition

of parental autonomy support and responsiveness nor democratic family climate in the

second and third step improved the predictive power of the model. Together, the

predictor variables accounted for 21% of the variance in adolescents’ life satisfaction

scores, 19% of their depression scores, and 9% of their anxiety scores (Table 5).

Page 57: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

47

Table 5. Regressions for Adolescent-reported Parenting and Family Climate Variables on Adolescents’ Psychological Well-Being

Life Satisfaction Depression (R) Anxiety (R) Variables ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β Step 1

.05 6.10*

.04* 5.33* Adolescents' Gender

.16

Age Group -.23*

Step 2 .18 13.18***

.16 7.90*** .07 1.89 Autonomy Support .28

.19 .05

Responsiveness -.03 -.04

.01 Step 3 .21 3.95*

.19 4.72* .09 1.81

Democratic Family Climate .23* .26*

.17 Total R2 .21 .19 .09 Note. *p ≤.05, **p ≤.01, ***p ≤.001.

Page 58: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

48

Chinese Mothers’ Retrospective Recall of the Way They Were Parented

To obtain a glimpse into the “traditional” Chinese parenting and family climate in

the previous generation, mothers were asked to recall how they were parented as

adolescents. Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVAs) were used to test whether

mothers from the three different origin-setting groups (urban, rural-to-urban, and rural)

differed from one another in their reports of how they were parented. As shown in Table

2c, mothers from the three groups differed significantly from one another in terms of

their recall of parental responsiveness and democratic family climate, but not in terms of

their recall of their parental autonomy support and tolerance of dissent. Post-hoc analyses

indicated that both urban and rural-to-urban mothers recalled their parents to be

significantly less responsive to their needs compared to rural mothers’ recall. In terms of

family climate, urban mothers recalled their parents to be significantly less democratic

compared to the rural mothers’ reports, but reports from the rural-to-urban mothers did

not differ significantly from the other two groups.

Comparisons of Chinese Mothers’ Current Parenting with Their Retrospective

Recall of How They Were Parented

To compare how parenting in urban and rural China may have changed over time

(as reflected in maternal self-reports), we compared Chinese mothers’ current parenting

with their retrospective recall of parenting (see Tables 2a and 2c). Given their greater

exposure to modern or Western ideologies regarding childrearing practices, we

hypothesized that urban mothers would be more critical of traditional parenting practices

(i.e., practices that are more authority-oriented and less autonomy supportive or

democratic than more modern styles of child-rearing). This was expected to lead to a

Page 59: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

49

greater shift in perceptions of parenting among urban mothers in comparison to their

counterparts from the rural areas. Whereas both urban and rural-to-urban mothers

reported themselves to be significantly more autonomy supportive (urban: t(19) = -4.39,

p <.000, Cohen’s d =1.02; rural-to-urban: t(33) = -3.62, p =.001, Cohen’s d = 0.65), more

responsive (urban: t(19) = -7,17, p <.000, Cohen’s d = 1.63; rural-to-urban: t(33) = -3.36,

p <.005, Cohen’s d = 0.60) and more democratic (urban: t(19) = -5.67, p <.000, Cohen’s

d = 1.28; rural-to-urban: t(33) = -2.00, p =.05, Cohen’s d = 0.39), rural mothers did not

see themselves to be significantly different from their own parents in these parenting

dimensions (autonomy support: t(63) = .02, p = .98, Cohen’s d = 0.003; responsiveness:

t(63) = .90, p = .37, Cohen’s d = -0.11; democratic climate: t(63) = -.02, p = .99, Cohen’s

d = -0.002) (see Figure 1a, b, c). On the other hand, all groups (urban, rural-to-urban and

rural) of mothers reported themselves to be significantly different from their own parents

in terms of their tolerance of dissent (urban: t(19) = 3.46, p < .005, Cohen’s d = 0.77;

rural-to-urban: t(33) = 2.91, Cohen’s d = 0.51, p < .01; rural: t(63) = 3.88, p < .001,

Cohen’s d = 0.49) (see Figure 1d).

Page 60: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

50

Figure 1. Mean score of (a) autonomy support, (b) responsiveness, (c) democratic climate, and (d) tolerance of dissent as reported by urban, rural-to-urban and rural mothers (retrospective recall of parenting and self-report of current parent.

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Urban Rural-to-Urban Rural

MeanRespnosiveness

Mothers'retrospectivereportMothers'self-report*** **

(b)

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Urban Rural-to-Urban Rural

MeanToleranceofDissent

Mothers'retrospectivereportMothers'self-report

*******

(d)

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Urban Rural-to-Urban Rural

MeanAutonomySupport

Mothers'retrospectivereportMothers'self-report*** **

(a)

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Urban Rural-to-Urban Rural

MeanDem

ocraticClimate

Mothers'retrospectivereportMothers'self-report*

****(c)

Page 61: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

51

Correlates of Mothers’ Current Parenting

In order to examine how mothers’ demographic background and socialization experience

may predict the way they currently parent, four hierarchical regression analyses were conducted.

Measures of parental autonomy support, responsiveness, democratic family climate, and

tolerance of dissent were regressed on four blocks of independent variables separately.

Demographic variables (i.e., their current setting (1 = urban), origins (1 = urban), and educational

levels (in years)) were entered at the first step in the model. In all regression analyses,

background variables contributed significantly to the model. Specifically, significant main effects

were found for mothers’ origin in all models (see Table 6). As shown in Table 2a, mothers who

were born in urban areas reported themselves to be significantly more autonomy supportive,

responsive, democratic, and tolerant of dissent than their counterparts from the rural areas.

Mothers’ current setting contributed significantly only to the regression models of autonomy

supportive and responsive parenting. Urban mothers, regardless of their origins, were

significantly more autonomy supportive and responsive than rural Chinese mothers.

Controlling for demographic variables, maternal socialization experience (i.e., mother’s

recall of how autonomy supportive and responsive their parents were) accounted for additional

variance in the prediction of mothers’ current parenting. As shown in Table 6, recalled parental

autonomy support significantly predicted mothers’ current autonomy supportive and democratic

parenting, as well as their tolerance of dissent, whereas recalled parental responsiveness

significantly predicted their current responsiveness to their own adolescent children at the second

step. Neither the addition of perceived democratic family climate nor tolerance of dissent in the

third and fourth step improved the predictive power of the models for mothers’ current autonomy

supportive parenting, responsive parenting, and democratic family climate, and hence were

Page 62: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

52

removed from the final models. For mothers’ reported current tolerance of dissent, although their

recall of democratic family climate was not a significant predictor in the third step, their recall of

parental tolerance of dissent in the fourth step was found to be a significant predictor, above and

beyond what was predicted by more general measures of parental autonomy support and

responsiveness. Together, the predictor variables accounted for 29% of the variance in mothers’

current autonomy supportive parenting, 36% of their responsiveness parenting, 26% of the family

democratic climate, and 33% of their tolerance of dissent (Table 6).

Page 63: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

53

Table 6.

Regression Examining Correlates between Mothers’ Retrospective Reports of how They were Parented and Mothers' Current Parenting

Autonomy

Responsiveness (C) Democratic Climate (C) Tolerance of

Support (C) Dissent (C)

Variables ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β

Step 1 .17 7.51*** .12 5.09** .07 2.92* .11 4.55** Current Setting .39** .27* .22 .11 Mothers’ Origin .27** .33*** .24* .23* Mothers’ Educational Level -.13 -.20 -.09 -.01

Step 2 .12 8.89*** .24 20.61*** .19 13.87* .12 8.10*** Autonomy Support (R) .31* -.01 .34** .34* Responsiveness (R) .07 .56*** .15 -.20 Step 3 (.00) .05 (.02) 2.75 (.02) 2.70 .00 .00 Democratic Climate (R) -.04 Step 4 (.00) .44 (.01) 1.75 (.01) 1.93 .10 15.55*** Tolerance of Dissent (R) .35*** Total R2 .29 .36 .26 .33

Note. (C) Mothers’ self-reported current parenting; (R) = Mothers’ retrospective reports of how they were parented. *p ≤.05, **p ≤.01, ***p ≤.001.

Page 64: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

54

Discussion

This study examined several dimensions of socialization in the family, including

parental autonomy support and responsiveness and family democratic climate, in

contemporary China as well as in retrospective recalls of parenting in a previous

generation. Relations between these dimensions of socialization in contemporary China,

both urban and rural areas, and Chinese adolescents’ psychological well-being were

examined. In the second part of the study, we examined how different parenting

dimensions may have changed over the generation, as well as the demographic and

socialization factors that may have contributed to contemporary Chinese mothers’

parenting.

China has often been characterized as a collectivistic country, with traditional

cultural values that emphasize hierarchy and group harmony over personal autonomy

(Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Shweder, 1999). This has led to a debate on whether

theoretical propositions about parenting derived from self-determination theory (SDT),

such as the role of autonomy supportive and responsive parenting in children’s optimal

development and psychological well-being, are universally valid and applicable to non-

Western cultures. The results of the present study, together with the empirical evidence

yielded by research conducted in diverse cultural settings (for a review, see Deci & Ryan,

2013), provide converging evidence suggesting that autonomy supportive and responsive

family environments are associated with positive psychological outcomes and

adjustment. These findings were further replicated across rural social ecologies of the

Page 65: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

55

Gemeinschaft form and urban social ecologies of the Gesellschaft form in a non-Western

cultural environment in this study (Greenfield, 2009).

Moreover, the current study both confirms and extends the findings of previous

research of SDT by investigating the unique contribution of democratic family climate to

adolescents’ psychological well-being. We found that family environments that are

perceived as more democratic, as indicated by adolescents who perceive their parents as

recognizing their freedom of expression, or taking into account their opinions in family

decisions, and as demonstrating respect for due process and fairness, are associated with

adolescents’ reports of greater satisfaction with their lives and fewer depressive

symptoms. These findings persisted even after controlling for demographic variables and

general parental autonomy support and responsiveness in the regression analyses,

demonstrating the unique contribution of democratic family environment to adolescents’

healthy psychological functioning. This is consistent with evidence found in longitudinal

studies conducted in urban China (e.g., Cheung, Pomerantz, Wang & Qu, 2016; Wang,

Pomerantz & Chen, 2007), providing converging evidence to a causal relation between

autonomy support and psychological well-being.

Despite the different pace of modernization in different parts of China, we found

that parental autonomy support and responsiveness as well as democratic family

environment were all highly correlated with psychological health in both our urban and

rural samples, supporting the SDT proposition that autonomy is a universal need and that

its functional significance for psychological well-being is not restricted to Western or

urban cultural contexts with Gesellschaft social ecology. These findings are particularly

intriguing since discussions related to notions of democracy and personal autonomy are

Page 66: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

56

often discouraged in China, where the political atmosphere is ostensibly Communist.

Nevertheless, the current research points to the converging findings that socialization

practices that support the fulfillment of children’s needs for autonomy and personal

freedoms, and that empower children to have their own voice in matters related to them,

may be an important avenue to enhancing children’s psychological well-being, regardless

of variations in social ecologies (Gesellschaft and Gemeinschaft) or the general cultural

normativeness of these practices.

Parenting in Contemporary China

Unlike the distinct cultural stereotype of China as characterized by Confucianism

or as reflected in some dichotomous perspectives on Individualism/Collectivism, we have

found dynamic variations in parenting within Chinese culture. Rather than being

homogenous, Chinese parenting appears to fall along the spectrum with collectivism or

traditional practices on one end and more modern, autonomy supportive practices on the

other end. These variations are associated with a variety of factors, including mothers’

place of origin, their current environment, and their own socialization experiences.

The current research suggested that modernization, particularly in the urban areas,

is swiftly shifting and changing the norms of Chinese parenting practices. Even though

the influence of Confucius emphasizing patriarchy and filial piety in parenting has

prevailed and greatly influenced daily family and social interactions in China throughout

most of its history, the norms and dynamics of family interactions appear to be shifting.

For example, both urban and rural contemporary Chinese mothers’ accounts of their

childhood revealed that they recalled their parents to be less tolerant of their

disagreements, compared to their own current parenting. This may be taken as further

Page 67: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

57

evidence of a macro shift in parenting in China, in both urban and rural regions,

characterized by some movement away from the hierarchical, authoritarian ways of

interacting with their children towards a more egalitarian, authoritative parenting style

that is more accepting of criticism and different opinions amongst the family members.

Indeed, many have suggested that since the introduction of the One-Child Policy in 1979,

parenting has drastically changed across the country (Naftali, 2016). With attention and

resources in the family all focused on the one and only child, these single children have

often been portrayed as a generation of “little emperors,” having a strong say in many

family decisions (Jing, 2000). Even though the one-child limit was mostly enforced in

urban areas and rural parents were allowed two children, the family size is still

significantly smaller than what it used to be. This may help explain parents’ movement

away from the traditional, more authoritarian style of parenting toward more acceptance

of opposing views from their adolescent children.

In contrast, there were no differences between rural mothers’ reports of their

current parenting and their reports of their mothers’ parenting in terms of autonomy

support, responsiveness, and democratic family climate. The difference pace of change in

parenting uncovered in the current study across urban and rural settings suggests that this

shift toward more autonomy supportive and democratic parenting in urban areas may be a

result of, and response to, the drastic modernization and changing social conditions

evident in urban China and other Gesellschaft social ecologies (Greenfield, 2009). These

findings appear to be consistent with Greenfield’s proposition that urban mothers

recognize that the current Gesellschaft environment in urban China requires a different

type of parenting, and hence they have adopted parenting practices that are more adaptive

Page 68: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

58

to the Gesellschaft environment and hence also more responsive to children’s need for

autonomy. It appears that they also have started to recognize and respect children’s

democratic rights in the family as they become parents themselves (Naftali, 2014).

As expected, we found that socialization practices that support the fulfillment of

autonomy needs were more common and were endorsed to a higher extent in the urban

than the rural environment. As reflected in mothers’ self-reports, the parenting of urban

mothers was more responsive to children’s needs, more supportive of their autonomy,

more democratic, and more tolerant of dissent than that reported by rural mothers. These

differences are consistent with the different socialization requirements of the more

modern, Gesellschaft social ecology of the urban area where individualistic values are

adaptive, when compared to the relatively unchanged, Gemeinschaft rural environment

where children’s autonomy of self-actualization is traditionally less valued (Greenfield,

2009). It appears that the changes brought about by the economic and social reform have

created ripple effects, influencing the cultural norms. To summarize, these changes have

led urban Chinese mothers to practice in a manner that is more sensitive to the

psychological needs of their children, more receptive to egalitarian discussions in the

family, and more open to regarding their children as part of family decision making,

despite their reports that they experienced such parenting to a much lesser extent when

growing up.

The rural-to-urban mothers represent a unique group who both continue to be

influenced by Confucian traditions as well as being exposed to modern and Western

ideologies as they became parents, thus setting them apart from either the urban mothers

or the rural mothers. Positioned within such a mixed socialization experience, this group

Page 69: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

59

of mothers appear to be in an intermediate stage between traditional parenting and more

modern values and socialization goals, pointing to the dynamic nature of parenting

practices and beliefs in contemporary China. The differences that were found in parenting

attitudes due to both setting and place of origin suggest that categorical or dichotomous

distinctions between cultures (as reflected, for example, in Individualism and

Collectivism) are an oversimplification that is no longer applicable in a changing society

undergoing drastic transformation such as contemporary China. Instead, more

contextually-sensitive ecological models are needed in order to properly account for the

variations in parenting found in the current research (Greenfield, 2009; Tamis-LeMonda

et al., 2008).

Correlates of Mothers’ Current Parenting

In addition to the influence of demographic variables (e.g., maternal origin,

current setting), we hypothesized that mothers’ current parenting would be associated

with their own socialization experience (i.e., how they were parented). Several

predictions about the correlates of mothers’ current parenting practices were supported.

We found that parenting followed a straightforward pattern in which mothers’ current

autonomy supportive and democratic parenting, as well as their tolerance of their

children’s opposing views, was facilitated by socialization experiences that were also

supportive of their autonomy. Responsive parenting was related to mothers’ perception of

their own parents’ responsiveness. Perceived tolerance of dissent from one’s own parents

was found to be a significant predictor of mothers’ own tolerance of dissent, even after all

other variables were controlled for. Overall, although perceived parenting accounted for

some variances of mothers’ current parenting, a significant proportion of variance

Page 70: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

60

remained to be accountable by other factors. Mothers are not simply replicating the

parenting model that they themselves had experienced. Instead, parenting beliefs and

practices that are endorsed by today’s Chinese mothers appeared to be dynamic

byproducts of both the external sociocultural environment and their own socialization

experiences; Chinese mothers are active agents in creating a socialization environment

that is conducive to the optimal development of their children and adapted to their current

social ecology. Shifts in parenting are not only happening across generations, but also

dynamically and internally as parents witness and reflect on the constant changes that are

unfolding within their immediate sociocultural environment.

Mothers’ versus Adolescents’ Reports

Research on parenting has often relied on the reports of a single reporter (usually

child reported data) for behaviors of other family members and in drawing conclusions

about the family climate. However, the present study investigated mothers’ and their

adolescent children’s reports of parenting behaviors and family climate and found that the

associations between the two reporters were generally small. The non-significant to

moderate levels of agreement between mothers and their adolescents is consistent with

findings in the existing literature conducted in the United States and China (e.g., Cheung

et al., 2016; Gonzales et al., 1996; Pettit, Laird, Dodge, & Bates, 2001), pointing to the

very different perceptions of parenting that children and mothers may have. Comparing

the two reporters’ ratings, mothers in the current study consistently reported their

parenting to be more autonomy supportive, and the family climate as more democratic

and tolerant of dissent (but not in terms of responsiveness), than do their adolescent

children. These differences in reports can be attributed to a number of possible factors,

Page 71: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

61

including social desirability (the desire of mothers to be perceived as more positive than

their actual parenting practices), normative disagreements between mothers and

adolescents, as well as adolescents’ developmental progression toward greater concern

with autonomy issues. These interpretations will be discussed in turn.

On the one hand, mothers might tend to present themselves in a more favorable

light in terms of their parenting practices and family dynamics (e.g., Gonzales et al.,

1996; Schwarz, Barton-Henry, & Pruzinsky, 1985). Due to these potential biases,

mothers might not be as good a reporter of their own parenting. Similar to findings of the

prior studies using mothers as informants (e.g., Cheung et al., 2016; Lamborn, Mounts,

Steinberg, & Dornbusch, 1991; Steinberg, Lamborn, & Darling, 1994), mothers’ reports

in our study were not as predictive of child outcomes as those assessed from the

children’s perspective. On the other hand, as children get older and claim increasing

autonomy over a wider range of issues, they may desire more autonomy and perceive

their mothers in a more negative fashion if they are not being granted the autonomy that

they desire (Smetana, 2005). Most importantly, however, the results of the present

investigation indicated that adolescents’ reports of parenting, but not mothers’ own

reports, were related to adolescents’ psychological well-being. Thus, no matter how these

parent-adolescent discrepancies are interpreted in terms of their accuracy, adolescents’

own perceptions of parenting, and not mothers’ perceptions, appears to be of most

relevance when predicting their psychological well-being (see also Cheung et al., 2016).

Parenting in Future China

Although studies of parenting and family socialization experience on adolescents’

psychological well-being are abundant, this study is one of the first to investigate

Page 72: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

62

correlates of parenting and democratic features of socialization in a non-Western setting.

A strength of the current study is that it has included three origin-setting groups of

parent-adolescent dyads – an urban sample from a setting that is fast-changing in its

modernization, a rural-to-urban sample representing parents who have moved from rural

to urban areas as they became parents of their urban children, and a rural sample of

mother-adolescent dyads from a traditional rural setting in China that has experienced

relatively minimal exposure to modern or Western parenting ideologies. This has allowed

us to explore how parenting and development varies within a culture undergoing an era of

social change.

Due to the cross-sectional nature of the current study, causality cannot be

established. While it is impossible to go back in time to longitudinally examine how

parenting has shifted with the social and economic reforms that have taken place in urban

China, we have instead utilized Chinese mothers’ retrospective reports of the parenting

they have received to assess the generational change. Social change, however, has not yet

come to an end. With the Chinese government’s modernization plan to move the majority

of the country’s population into urban city life in the next decade, many rural regions of

China are currently undergoing rapid changes that are quickly transforming the landscape

of rural areas. Children who are currently living in the rural areas are quickly urbanizing

as they become parents themselves. This has therefore given us a time-sensitive

opportunity to utilize longitudinal designs in subsequent research to obtain an accurate

depiction of the changes in socialization and its sequelae that are happening as the effects

of globalization continue to unfold in the next decades.

Page 73: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

63

Paper 2

Children's Rights Attitudes Amongst Adolescents and Mothers in China

Abstract

This study examined urban and rural Chinese adolescents’ (12-16 years) and their

mothers’ attitudes toward children's rights (N =256, with 64 mother-adolescent dyads per

setting). We explored how different dimensions of family socialization environment,

namely responsiveness and autonomy supportive parenting, democratic family

environment, and parental tolerance of dissent, as well as sociodemographic factors may

contribute to the development of these rights conceptions in diverse settings within

China.

Chinese participants (particularly amongst the adolescents) demonstrated support

for both rights to nurturance (i.e., rights to be protected and cared for) as well as

entitlement to self-determination (i.e., rights to participate and make autonomous

decisions). Regarding to relations between parenting and children's rights attitudes, father

responsiveness emerged as a particularly important predictor of adolescents' attitudes

about their nurturance rights; whereas parental tolerance of dissent was found to be an

important predictor for both nurturance and self-determination rights. On the other hand,

only mothers' sociodemographic factors, but none of their retrospective family

socialization experience, emerge as significant predictors for their current attitudes

toward children's rights issues. The current study points to the importance of examining

environmental factors and social change in conjunction with developmental pathways and

the changing roles in one's lifespan to fully understand rights attitudes and beliefs.

Page 74: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

64

Introduction

In study 1, I examined the relationship between socialization environment and

well-being and found that the perceptions of an autonomy supportive, responsive, and

democratic family environment are associated with Chinese adolescents’ healthy

psychological well-being. In Study 2, I will narrow the focus and examine specific

autonomy and nurturance issues, namely, how Chinese children and mothers endorse

different types of rights implicating these issues, as well as how different dimensions of

socialization environment may contribute to the development of these rights conceptions.

Over the last century, the notion of children’s rights has become prevalent along

with other historical trends including the emergence of academic initiatives in child

development and scholastic and public discourse on children’s rights (Vandenhole,

Desmet, Reynaert, & Lembrechts, 2015). Since then, there has been increased recognition

and support for the notion that children and adolescents, just like adults, are individuals

with the same basic human rights. This expanded recognition is reflected in the adoption

of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child (UNCRC), established by

the United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) in 1989. As the first legally binding

agreement on children’s rights, UNCRC listed a wide range of rights that focuses on

protecting the health, welfare, and well-being of individuals under 18 years of age.

Researchers generally agree that there are two main categories encompassing the range of

rights covered in the UNCRC – nurturance rights and self-determination rights (Rogers &

Wrightman, 1978; Ruck, Abramovitch, & Keating, 1998). Nurturance rights pertain to

the satisfaction of basic needs that are required to maintain children’s healthy

development, both physically and psychologically, whereas self-determination rights

Page 75: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

65

refer to the right of children to have a voice when making decisions that are relevant to

their lives. This balance and integration of providing rights for nurturance and self-

determination to children and adolescents goes hand in hand with the two fundamental

tenets that frame the Convention: the best interest of the child and the evolving capacities

of the child (Peterson-Badali & Ruck, 2008).

While the UNCRC was ratified and approved by nearly all members of the United

Nations “on paper,” children’s and parents’ views and attitudes regarding these rights,

especially in non-Western cultures, are however not well-studied. As young people’s

knowledge and attitudes about their rights is likely an important determinant of whether

they are able to effectively utilize their rights (Peterson-Badali, Ruck, & Ridley, 2003),

examining their attitudes toward their rights also would allow us to ensure that the

UNCRC is actually achieving its protective goals (Day, Peterson-Badali, & Ruck, 2006;

Peterson-Badali et al., 2004). Similarly, it is equally important for parents, who are the

primary provider of these rights to children, to recognize and support these rights for

them to be realized (Ruck, Peterson-Badali, & Day, 2002). Parental views about

children’s rights also may influence children’s views about their own rights, either

through the influence of their sociopolitical attitudes (e.g., conservatism/liberalism) or

their parenting practices (Day et al., 2006; Peterson-Badali et al., 2004). Generally,

research in North American cultural contexts examining adults’ and young people’s

views and attitudes toward children’s rights has consistently found that both types of

rights are endorsed, with higher levels of endorsement of children’s nurturance rights

than self-determination rights (e.g., Day et al., 2006; Peterson-Badali et al., 2004;

Peterson-Badali et al., 2003; Rogers & Wrightsman, 1978; Ruck et al, 2002).

Page 76: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

66

However, the notion of children’s rights often has been identified with

individualism and criticized as a Western cultural construction that has functional

relevance only for people from Western, democratic societies (Kaime, 2011; Murphy-

Berman, Levesque, & Berman, 1996). Specifically, cultural psychologists argued that for

individuals from traditional, non-Western societies, characterized by a collectivistic

moral orientation toward authority and the group, issues of autonomy and individual

rights are much less salient (Kaime, 2011). China is a prime example of such a

collectivist society, with its cultural ideals of filial piety and Confucianism that inform

Chinese adolescents’ and parents’ traditional moral beliefs (Peerenboom, 2002). This

question of whether children’s self-determination is considered to be important is

particularly relevant to countries where rights (especially rights to self-determination and

freedom of expression) are not strongly emphasized. Specifically, although children’s

rights to nurturance may be widely recognized across cultures, recognition of children’s

rights to full social participation and expressing their own views remains a topic with

varying perspectives and support (e.g., Ben-Arieh, Khoury-Kassabri, & Haj-Yahia, 2006;

Lahat, Helwig, Yang, Tan, & Liu, 2009). It has been suggested that in cultures that place

a strong emphasis on obedience and respect for hierarchy, children’s entitlement to

autonomy may be perceived as a threat to parental authority (Keshavarz & Baharudin,

2009). Hence, children’s self-determination rights may be more likely to be opposed,

particularly when the fulfillment of these rights are in conflict with parental wishes or

societal practices.

Cross-cultural research has attempted to elucidate these debates by examining the

views of rights of adolescents from different non-Western cultural settings. Emerging

Page 77: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

67

research conducted across diverse cultural contexts (e.g., rural and urban China, Africa,

The Middle East, India, Korea) has indicated that rights are similarly endorsed by

children and adults in these settings, demonstrating support for a number of nurturance

and self-determination rights (e.g., Ben-Arieh & Khoury-Kassabri, 2008; Hoppe-Graff &

Kim, 2005; Lahat et al., 2009; Neff & Helwig, 2002; Ruck, Tenenbaum, & Willenberg,

2011). Departing from the presumed cultural stereotypes, Chinese adolescents were

remarkably similar to their Western counterparts in their responses and social reasoning

about their rights (Lahat et al., 2009). For example, Lahat and her colleagues (2009)

found that urban and rural Chinese adolescents endorsed both nurturance and self-

determination rights (including freedom of speech and religion), notions that are often

associated with Western ideologies, and made references to children’s personal choice

and autonomy in their justifications for endorsements of self-determination rights.

Similarly, Helwig, Arnold, Tan, and Boyd (2003) found that adolescents from different

regions in mainland China, including those from rural traditional regions, favored

decision making procedures that made room for children’s autonomy (such as majority

rule or consensus) over decision making by adult authorities for decisions involving

children in different social contexts (e.g., in peer and family contexts). In contrast to the

stereotypical construals of Chinese psychology as oriented toward filial piety and

obedience to authority, Chinese adolescents appealed to children’s freedom from the

interference of authority, their rights as persons, and the beneficial developmental

outcomes when children are given opportunities to make autonomous decisions (Helwig

et al., 2003). These findings reflect that adolescents’ thinking about rights is much more

complex and multifaceted than what can be accounted for through dichotomizing cultural

Page 78: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

68

orientations of individualism and collectivism, or modernity and traditionalism.

Parents’ and Children’s views on Children’s Rights

Cross-generational studies comparing parents’ and young people’s views on

rights in Western cultural settings have shown that while parents are more likely to

endorse nurturance rights than their adolescents, they are less likely to favor recognition

of children’s rights to self-determination (e.g., Day et al., 2006; Peterson-Badali et al.,

2004; Peterson & Ruck, 2003; Rogers & Wrightsman, 1978; Ruck, Keating,

Abramovitch, & Koegl, 1998; Ruck et al., 2002). Moreover, children’s support for self-

determination rights generally increase with the child’s age, with older adolescents

endorsing more rights related to self-determination and also endorsing these rights more

strongly when compared to younger adolescents (Lahat et al., 2009; Peterson-Badali et

al., 2003; Walker, Brooks, & Wrightsman, 1999).

Although recent studies have begun to examine the rights attitudes, knowledge

and reasoning of children in non-Western cultural contexts (e.g., Cherney & Shing, 2008;

Lahat et al., 2009; To et al., 2017), virtually nothing is known about the relationship

between parents’ and children’s attitudes toward rights in these settings. In one of the few

published studies on this topic conducted in non-Western cultural settings, Ben-Arieh,

Khoury-Kassabri, and Haj-Yahi (2006) compared attitudes toward children’s rights in

four generations (adolescents, young mothers of toddlers, middle-aged mothers of

adolescents, and grandmothers) of Jews and Muslims residing in Israel and the

Palestinian Authority. As opposed to previous research findings conducted in Western

contexts, they did not find any significant developmental or cohort effects. Instead they

found uniform objection to children’s rights when they conflicted with parental rights

Page 79: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

69

(across nationality and age group), showing the deeply embedded respect for parental

authority and the difference in pace in recognizing children’s rights in more traditional

societies. However, it is important to note that participants were asked to indicate their

general support for children’s rights in situations where they infringe on authority using a

binary response (either “yes” or “no”). Use of dichotomous judgments such as these,

along with general questions regarding unspecified situations, may not capture the true

variances in participants’ responses.

In contrast, Lahat et al. (2009)’s study examined adolescents’ judgments and

reasoning about children’s rights by asking participants to evaluate different scenarios in

which children’s rights issues are involved on a 6-point Likert scale. They found

universal developmental patterns across settings that included both modern urban areas as

well as traditional rural areas within China. Specifically, both rural and urban Chinese

adolescents increasingly endorsed self-determination rights with age, both in

straightforward situations and in situations where these rights conflicted with children’s

welfare interests (nurturance rights). The fact that these two studies demonstrated

contrasting findings in terms of rights endorsement in non-Western cultural contexts may

lie in the way the questions were asked. It appears that when participants were asked in a

general manner to choose between a child’s rights versus parental rights, the results were

very different from a methodology in which they were given a particular scenario to

consider. These differences in findings point to the importance of conducting more cross-

cultural studies on this topic. Indeed, both authors called for more studies of this topic

across generations (i.e., mothers and their adolescents) in order to inform our

understanding of how different socio-historical experiences encountered by different

Page 80: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

70

generations in different cultures may influence their attitudes toward children’s rights

(Ben-Arieh et al., 2006; Lahat et al., 2009).

In light of the very few existing studies and these contrasting findings in non-

Western cultural contexts, I sought to contribute to the research literature in this area by

investigating the relationship between urban and rural Chinese adolescents’ and their

mothers’ attitudes toward children’s rights. The current generation of Chinese

adolescents (particularly those from urban settings) has come of age during a time when

China has undergone dramatic changes both socially and economically (Ma, 2002).

These adolescents have experienced a corresponding loosening of constraints in terms of

their personal autonomy and as a result, their experiences coming of age are very

different from those of their parents (Peerenboom, 2002). Hence, I expected that there

would be both generation and role differences in adolescents’ and their mothers’ attitudes

toward nurturance and self-determination rights.

Despite the developmental pattern in children’s rights attitudes cited above

(revealing significant differences between younger versus older participants in their

views of self-determination rights), the findings are sometimes confounded with the age

of the ‘target’ child (i.e., age of the story character or child of which participants were

asked to make judgment) (Day et al., 2006). While some studies specify the age of the

target child, other studies match the age of the target child with the participants’ age,

making it difficult to delineate whether the differences found between adolescents of

different age groups are a result of the developmental progression or variations in how

rights are endorsed for children of different target ages. I set the age of the target child in

our research as 12 years old in order to eliminate this confound. Lastly, gender findings

Page 81: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

71

have been mixed, with some studies finding stronger support for nurturance rights

amongst females than males (e.g., Day et al., 2006; Peterson-Badali et al., 2004) whereas

others found little evidence of gender differences (e.g., Ruck et al., 1998; Ruck et al.,

2002).

In addition to the general descriptives of children’s rights attitudes, I was also

interested in examining the effects of socialization variables on adolescents and mothers’

thinking about rights. To this end, an overview of relevant literature on factors that

contribute to attitudes toward children’s rights will be presented in the following section,

with an emphasis on family correlates of children’s rights attitudes.

Socialization Correlates of Children’s Rights Attitudes

The emerging evidence suggests that attitudes about children’s rights are

multifaceted and influenced by a variety of factors. These factors include not only the

specific rights in question and the domain of the issue involved, but also children’s

socialization experiences with rights and the opportunity to exercise these rights in social

contexts such as the family and the school (Ben-Arieh et al., 2006; Ruck et al., 1998).

Specifically, two pathways have been hypothesized through which socialization may

exert its influence on attitudes toward rights issues. One of the pathways proposed is

based on findings which suggest that parenting that is sensitive to children’s

developmental needs is predictive of adolescents’ attitudes about their rights (Peterson-

Badali et al., 2004; To et al., 2017). Hence, how children experience rights in their own

lives may be reflected in the way they think about rights. For instance, children’s

experience with autonomy supportive parenting entailing making their own decisions

over matters of importance to the child was found to predict children’s awareness of their

Page 82: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

72

self-determination rights, whereas their experiences of being cared for and protected were

associated with their support of nurturance rights (Peterson-Badali et al., 2004; To et al.,

2017). Moreover, To et al. (2017) studied variables beyond the general parental

autonomy support and responsiveness by investigating the unique contribution of a

democratic environment as well as the role of authority’s tolerance of dissent to

children’s attitudes toward their rights. Similar to the findings from prior research

conducted at school (e.g., Khoury-Kassabri & Ben-Arieh, 2009), parental tolerance of

dissent was found positively to predict adolescents’ support for self-determination rights,

as well as their psychological well-being, over and beyond what is predicted by other

general parenting dimensions (To et al., 2017).

On the other hand, an oppositional or reactive pathway also has been proposed,

with emerging evidence that youth who perceive especially low levels of autonomy

support--hence indicating that their basic need for autonomy is severely restricted by

agents of socialization--may form an oppositional perspective against the socialization

practices that have not met their needs (Peterson-Badali, Ruck, & Bone, 2008). This may

in turn heighten their awareness of and support for self-determination rights (Lo, Helwig,

Chen, Ohashi, & Cheng, 2011; To et al., 2017; Vansteenkiste, Soenens, Petegem, &

Duriez, 2014). Thus, there is a need for further research that is sensitive to these different

potential pathways to inform theory building in this area.

Whereas findings of developmental research suggest relationships between young

people’s socialization experiences and their attitudes toward children’s rights, virtually

nothing is known about whether these socialization effects would continue to influence

rights attitudes as one becomes a parent. In Study 1, mothers’ retrospective recall of how

Page 83: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

73

they were parented was associated with their current parenting. Moreover, findings from

studies examining the relationship between parenting and attitudes about rights suggested

that there are positive relations between mothers’ self-perceived parenting (e.g.,

responsiveness, involvement of children in family decision-making process) and maternal

support for self-determination rights (Peterson-Badali et al., 2004). However, these

variables did not emerge as significant predictors in the regression predicting maternal

rights attitudes in the cited study. Therefore, it remains unclear how parental attitudes

toward rights and their parenting practices in the home (as well as children’s actual

experiences of rights in the home) are related.

With its long history and influence of Confucianism and filial piety, as well as its

political backdrop of Communism, which all stress the subordination of the individual to

group goals and the emphasis of subsistence rights over civil rights, China offers a unique

cultural context in which to explore these issues. Furthermore, China has undergone

tremendous economic, social and cultural transformation in the last few decades, as it

moved toward a market economy (Mackerras, 2001). These sociocultural changes also

have brought about changes in social norms and values, which have been reflected in the

everyday life of the Chinese family, influencing parenting style, family dynamics and

daily interactions, as well as beliefs about children’s rights (Wang, 2014).

The development and modernization, however, has been disproportionately

allocated to coastal urban cities, with many of the rural areas remaining traditional and

undeveloped. This has resulted in a stark contrast between the massively transformed,

developed, urban parts of China and the much more traditional, collectivistic, mainly

agricultural and rural regions of China. Comparing and contrasting parental conceptions

Page 84: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

74

of rights from these two diverse settings would allow us to delineate the effects of

dramatic economic and social transformation on individuals’ values (including their

conceptions of children’s rights). In the mean time, studying different age groups of rural

and urban Chinese adolescents’ conceptions of rights would allow us to study the relative

effect of sociocultural environment and developmental factors on children’s attitudes

toward their rights. Lastly, exploring concepts of autonomy and rights and their correlates

in both mothers and adolescents would provide insight into how conceptions of rights are

developed and formed amongst different generations.

The Present Study and Hypotheses

The present study was designed to extend previous research on children’s rights

attitudes by comparing the perspectives of mothers and their adolescent children from

two age groups (early and mid-adolescents) and within two non-Western geographical

contexts (traditional rural and modern urban China). The current study also contributes to

the identified gaps in the literature by examining the role of family factors (e.g.,

responsiveness and autonomy supportive parenting, democratic family climate, parental

tolerance of dissent) in adolescents’ and mothers’ attitudes toward children’s rights in

diverse settings within China. Based on the findings of research conducted with North

American children and mothers (e.g., Day et al., 2006; Peterson-Badali et al., 2004; Ruck

et al., 2002), as well as recent literature examining rights understanding amongst Chinese

adolescents (e.g., Lahat et al., 2009; To et al., 2017), a number of research predictions

were generated.

Although the pace of development differed at times, Chinese adolescents were

found to follow a similar developmental pattern as their counterparts in Western cultural

Page 85: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

75

contexts (To et al., 2017). Hence, I surmised that Chinese adolescents, regardless of age,

would endorse self-determination rights to a greater extent than would their mothers,

whereas mothers would endorse greater support for nurturance rights compared to their

adolescent children. With the Chinese Communist political climate that stresses

subsistence rights and children’s welfare interests over individual autonomy

(Peerenboom, 2002), I expected that both Chinese mothers and adolescents generally

would endorse nurturance rights to a higher extent than self-determination rights, as

found in prior research conducted in Chinese cultural settings (e.g., Lahat et al., 2009; To

et al., 2017). Consistent with prior research suggesting the influence of urbanization and

modernization on support for rights and autonomy (e.g., Lahat et al., 2009; To et al.,

2017), I expected that children’s nurturance and self-determination rights would be

endorsed more strongly by urban mothers and adolescents compared to their counterparts

in rural areas. Based on the null gender findings of To and her colleagues’ (2017) studies

of Chinese adolescents’ attitudes about rights, I did not anticipate any gender differences

in children’s rights attitudes in the present study.

With respect to correlates of attitudes toward rights, I hypothesized that children’s

attitudes regarding their rights would be related to their own experience with rights.

Hence, children’s perceptions of parental responsiveness would be related to their support

for nurturance rights, whereas their perceptions of parental autonomy support would be

related to their endorsement of self-determination rights (To et al., 2017). Findings from

To et al. (2017) suggested that parental tolerance of dissent also may have a unique

contribution to adolescents’ endorsement of their self-determination rights and hence I

expected the same pattern to emerge from our current study. In addition to the

Page 86: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

76

straightforward, positive relations between socialization variables and children’s rights

attitudes, I would also pay attention to the potential for reactive pathways that might

result from low levels of perceived autonomy support and responsiveness (Lo et al.,

2011; To et al., 2017; Vansteenkiste et al., 2014). Regarding parental attitudes toward

children’s rights, I surmised that parental attitudes toward children’s rights would be

associated with both their retrospective recall of socialization experience and their current

social environment (urban, rural-to-urban, rural).

Method

Participants and Procedures

Study participants and corresponding demographics are the same as those

reported in Paper 1. In short, the sample consisted of 128 Chinese adolescents and their

mothers from two research sites (in urban and rural China), with 64 adolescent-mother

dyads from each setting. Among the adolescent participants, 64 were early-adolescents

(12-13 years old) and 64 were mid-adolescents (15-16 years old). Because the urban

sample contained dyads that were approximately evenly distributed between those whose

mothers were originally from urban Chinese settings (30 dyads) and those whose mothers

migrated to urban settings from rural areas at some point in their lives (34 dyads), I was

able to compare maternal origin (i.e., urban versus rural-to-urban) in our analyses. The

adolescents in both the urban and rural-to-urban maternal origin group were born and

raised in the urban area, whereas those in the rural group were born and raised in the rural

area, as was true of their mothers.

The recruitment method and procedures are the same as those reported in Paper 1;

see the Methods section in Paper 1 for more information.

Page 87: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

77

Measures

To ensure accurate understanding and readability of the questionnaires (especially

for the rural parents), Chinese translations of all measures used in this study were first

pilot tested with four urban and four rural Chinese adolescent-mother dyads.

Incorporating feedback from the participants, some of the wording were then modified in

order to improve readability and comprehension of the questionnaires. The final versions

of all questionnaires were back translated into English by a different native Chinese

bilingual translator. This back-translated English version of the questionnaire was

compared with the original version and any discrepancies corrected in order to ensure the

accuracy of the translation (Brislin, 1970).

Children's rights attitudes. To assess adolescents’ and mothers’ attitudes to

various rights issues, the Children’s Rights Attitudes (CRA) questionnaire (Peterson-

Badali et al., 2003; originally developed by Rogers & Wrightsman, 1978; see Appendix

C) was used. Among the 40 items of CRA, 18 correspond to children’s nurturance rights

and involve issues regarding children’s entitlement to care and protection from harm

(e.g., “Children should have the right to receive medical treatment when necessary”).

Twenty-two items correspond to self-determination rights, i.e., the right of children to

exercise control over various facets of their lives (e.g., “Children should have the right to

decide which recreational activities they will participate in”). Since child care is mostly

done by extended family members in China, in contrast to the paid day care arrangements

common in North America, two items related to the right of quality child care (under the

category of nurturance rights) were modified to the right for children with special needs

to receive adequate education and assistance.

Page 88: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

78

Using the same methods applied in previous studies of CRA (e.g., Day et al.,

2006; Peterson-Badali et al., 2004), participants were asked to respond to the children’s

rights questions by using a 12-year-old as a referent. Participants then rated the extent of

their support for each of the 40 statements of children’s rights issues on a scale from 1

(strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). An average score for each type of right was

computed, with higher scores indicating stronger endorsement of rights.

The internal consistency reliabilities attained in our present sample was

acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha for self-determination rights = .82 (adolescent), .77

(mother); and for nurturance rights = .70 (adolescent), .79 (mother)).

Autonomy support and responsiveness. Adolescent participants were given the

Perception of Parents scales (POPS, originally derived from Robbins, 1995; see

Appendix A) to assess their perceived parental (maternal and paternal) autonomy support

and responsiveness (warmth and involvement). The scales consisted of nine items about

autonomy support (e.g., “My mother allows me to decide things for myself”) and twelve

items about responsiveness (e.g., “My mother puts time and energy into helping me”). In

order to add to the understanding of father’s roles in the development of children’s rights

understanding, this study explored the adolescents’ perceptions of both mothers and

fathers’ parenting. Each item in this questionnaire was rated on a 7-point Likert-type

scale, ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true). The internal consistency (alphas)

for the adolescent reported autonomy support = .81 and responsiveness = .83.

Mothers were asked to fill out two versions of the Perception of Parents scales

(POPS). One was modified to measure mothers’ self-report of their own current

parenting, and another one measured mothers’ retrospective report of how they perceived

Page 89: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

79

that they were parented when they themselves were adolescents. Since the POPS were

originally created for children to report on their parents’ parenting, minor wording

changes were made in the mother self-reported POPS to minimize social desirability

concerns (e.g., one of the reversed items, “My mother doesn’t seem to think of me often”,

was not reversed in the self-report scale and rephrased as “I think of my child often”.)

Since there is no differentiation between past and present tense in Chinese language, the

POPS used by mothers when reporting their experiences of how they perceived being

parented was the same as the one filled out by their adolescents. The only difference was

that mothers were asked to recall how they were being parented when they were at their

children’s age. The internal consistencies (alphas) for the mothers’ self-reported current

autonomy support and responsiveness were .69 and .76 respectively. The internal

consistencies for mothers’ retrospective recall of how they were parented were .81

(autonomy support) and .85 (responsiveness).

Democratic climate and tolerance of dissent. To examine the unique role of

democratic family environment, both adolescents and mothers filled out the five-item

democratic family climate measure (To, et al., 2017; see Appendix B). This scale

measures particular democratic dimensions of family life and structure, such as children’s

freedom of expression (e.g., “There is mutual respect between me and my parents even in

areas in which we disagree”), perceptions of due process (e.g., “My parents will listen to

my explanation and offer a fair hearing if I am accused of wrongdoing”), and

involvement in decision making (e.g., “In my family, I am given the chance to help make

decisions”). Each item is scored on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all

true) to 7 (very true). Similar to the POPS, mothers were asked both to self-report their

Page 90: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

80

current family democratic climate, as well as their retrospective perceived family

democratic climate when they were adolescents. Reliabilities of the 5-item democratic

family climate scale were .71 (for adolescent report), .62 (for mother self-report) and .80

(for mothers’ retrospective report). An additional item measuring tolerance of dissent (“It

is possible to criticize my parents’ decisions”) was presented in conjunction with the

democratic climate measure but was analyzed separately, given the unique predictive

power of this item as found in prior studies (e.g., Khoury-Kassabri & Ben-Arieh, 2009;

To et al., 2017).

Results

Initial Investigation of Potential Oppositional or Reactive Patterns

Means, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations among all study variables

are presented in Table 7. Since previous studies (e.g., To et al., 2017) have cautioned

against the existence of reactive cases, I first explored the data for potential oppositional

patterns in the form of high levels of endorsement of self-determination rights when

perceived autonomy support and responsiveness were low. Although the relations

between support for self-determination rights and parental responsiveness (r = -.12

(mother), -.12 (father)), parental autonomy support (r = -.04 (mother), -.10 (father)), and

home democratic climate (r = -.12) were all negative, none of the correlations was

significant. Moreover, after five cases from the urban sample and one from rural sample

were identified as reactive cases based on To et al. (2017)’s criteria, the patterns of

correlations and regression analyses were no different from the full sample analyses.

Thus, the full sample analyses are presented in the rest of the results section.

Subsequent analyses examined Chinese adolescents' and mothers' attitudes toward

Page 91: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

81

children's rights issues, correlates of adolescents' and mothers' attitudes toward children's

rights, and lastly, relations between mother's retrospective reports of how they were

parented and their children's rights attitudes.

Page 92: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

82

Table 7.

Means, Standard deviations (SD), Pearson Correlations of Adolescents’ and Mothers’ Reported Family Socialization Environment and Children’s Rights Attitudes

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

1. Maternal Autonomy Support (A) --

2. Paternal Autonomy Support (A) .62*** --

3. Maternal Responsiveness (A) .84*** .63*** --

4. Paternal Responsiveness (A) .59*** .81*** .69*** --

5. Home Democratic Climate (A) .71*** .53*** .67*** .60*** --

6. Home Tolerance of Dissent (A) .27** .05 .19* -.02 .20* --

7. Autonomy Support (M) .20* .10 .17 .13 .27** .25** --

8. Responsiveness (M) .08 .07 .08 .16 .27** .02 .61*** --

9. Home Democratic Climate (M) .03 -.01 .01 -.02 .12 .09 .62*** .58*** --

10. Tolerance of Dissent (M) .05 -.01 .04 .09 .09 .18* .46*** .26** .33*** --

11. Support for Nurturance Rights (A) .27** .21* .21* .29*** .19* .24** .00 -.04 -.09 .08 --

12. Support for Self-Determination Rights (A) -.04 -.10 -.12 -.12 -.12 .27** .01 -.00 -.03 .07 .14 --

13. Support for Nurturance Rights (M) .13 .06 .16 .15 .20* .24** .52*** .50*** .37*** .42*** .14 -.09 --

14. Support for Self-Determination Rights (M) .11 -.08 .05 -.04 .12 .15 .33*** .06 .18* .34*** .02 -.00 .23* --

Mean 5.28 5.23 5.68 5.31 5.34 3.84 5.49 5.81 5.61 4.77 4.94 4.09 4.90 3.57

SD 1.03 1.09 .92 1.04 1.16 1.91 .79 .76 .94 1.59 .43 .59 .59 .58

Note. (A) = Adolescent-reported; (M) = mother self-report. *p ≤.05, * *p ≤.01, ***p ≤.001.

Page 93: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

83

Descriptive and Correlational Analysis

Bivariate correlations of all study variables are presented in Table 7. Regarding

socialization correlates of rights attitudes, adolescents’ endorsement of nurturance rights

was positively associated with their ratings of parental autonomy support, responsiveness,

democratic family climate and tolerance of dissent. Adolescents’ endorsement of self-

determination rights was positively associated with tolerance of dissent at home, but did

not correlate with any other family environment variables. In contrast, adolescents’

attitudes toward children’s rights were not related to any of the mothers’ self-reported

parenting and family climate measures. Regarding the relationships between maternal

attitudes toward children’s rights and their self-reported parenting, maternal support for

both nurturance and self-determination rights was associated with their reports of

autonomy support, democratic family climate, and tolerance of dissent. Maternal

responsiveness also was correlated with their support for nurturance rights, but not self-

determination rights. On the other hand, when adolescents’ reports of parenting were

used, only democratic family climate and tolerance of dissent were associated with

maternal attitudes toward nurturance rights, and none of the adolescent-reported

parenting variables were significantly related to maternal attitudes toward self-

determination rights. It is also interesting to note that significant correlations between

nurturance and self-determination rights attitudes were found only for mothers (r = .23, p

< .05), but not for adolescents (r = .14, p = .13) and not between mothers’ and

adolescents’ attitudes toward rights (r = .14, p = .12 for nurturance rights and r = .00, p =

.99 for self-determination rights). The univariate distributions for all measures of

children’s rights attitudes were approximately normal. Skewness and kurtosis for each

Page 94: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

84

variable was examined and determined to be within acceptable limits (Tabachnick &

Fidel, 2001).

Similar to Paper 1, while parental education and occupational levels may act as

confounds when comparing the three origin-setting maternal groups in the following

analyses, these variables are largely orthogonal amongst the groups. In fact, these

demographic variables are central to the characteristics of Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft

forms of social ecologies and hence are intended to be part of the categorical

comparisons. Therefore these demographic variables were not treated as covariates in the

following between-group comparisons.

Adolescents’ Perspectives on Children’s Rights

Children’s rights attitudes were examined among adolescents using a 2 (type of

right: nurturance or self-determination) X 2 (setting: urban or rural) X 2 (adolescents'

gender) X 2 (adolescents' age group: early or mid-adolescence) repeated-measures

analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with type of right as a repeated measure. As expected,

significant main effects for the type of right indicated that there was significantly stronger

support for nurturance rights (M = 4.95 (out of 6), corresponding to agree somewhat)

than self-determination rights (M = 4.09 (out of 6), corresponding to agree slightly),

F(1,116) = 221.56, p <.001, η2 = .43).

Significant right X age group interactions indicated that, whereas children’s

support for nurturance rights was stronger in the mid-adolescent group (M = 4.85 for 12-

year-olds vs. M = 5.05 for 16-year-olds), support for self-determination rights was

stronger in the early-adolescent group (M = 4.22 for 12-year-olds vs. M = 3.95 for 16-

year-olds), F(1,116) = 16.03, p <.001, η2 = .05.

Page 95: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

85

Mothers’ Perspectives on Children’s Rights

Mothers’ attitudes toward children’s rights also were examined using a 2 (type of

right: nurturance or self-determination rights) X 3 (origin-setting: urban, rural-to-urban,

rural) X 2 (adolescents' age group: 12-year-olds or 16-year-olds) X 2 (adolescents'

gender) repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs), with the type of right as a

repeated measure. Similar to the findings with adolescents, significant main effects for

the type of right indicated that mothers supported nurturance rights (M = 4.89 (out of 6),

corresponding to agree somewhat) to a significantly higher extent than their support for

self-determination rights (M = 3.56 (out of 6), corresponding to agree slightly), F(1,106)

= 275.71, p <.001, η2 = .52. Significant main effects for origins also were found, F(1,106)

= 7.50, p =.001, η2 = .14. Post-hoc comparisons between the three origin-settings

combinations indicated that both urban and rural-to-urban mothers were more supportive

of children’s rights issues than rural mothers (self-determination rights: M = 3.78, SD =

.64 for urban and M = 3.65, SD = .57 for rural-to-urban vs. M = 3.44, SD = .55 for rural

mothers; nurturance rights: M = 5.30, SD = .53 for urban and M = 4.93, SD = .72 for

rural-to-urban vs. M = 4.74, SD = .50 for rural mothers).

Adolescents’ versus Mothers’ Perspectives on Children’s Rights

Adolescents’ and mothers’ attitudes toward children’s rights were compared using a

2 (respondent: adolescents or mothers) X 2 (type of right: nurturance or self-

determination rights) X 2 (setting: urban or rural) X 2 (adolescents' age group: early- or

mid-adolescence) repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs), with respondent

and type of right as repeated measures. A main effect of respondent indicated that

adolescents endorsed rights (M = 4.52) to a higher extent compared to their mothers (M =

Page 96: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

86

4.24), F(1,120) = 26.99, p <.001, η2 = .10. A significant right X respondent interaction

revealed that, whereas adolescents (M = 4.94) and their mothers (M = 4.91) did not differ

in their support for nurturance rights, adolescents (M = 4.09) supported self-

determination rights to a significantly greater extent than did mothers (M = 3.57), F(1,

120) = 34.84, p < .001, η2 = .10.Main effects for setting indicated that urban participants,

across respondents, age groups, and types of rights, endorsed rights more positively than

their rural counterparts, F(1, 116) = 17.36, p < .001, η2 = .15.

Significant age group X respondent interactions also were qualified by a significant

three-way right X age group X respondent interaction, F(1, 120) = 7.79, p < .01, η2 = .02,

indicating that the right X age group interaction described above only applied to

adolescent, not mother, respondents. Regardless of the age of their adolescents, mothers

of adolescents of both age groups did not differ from each other in terms of their support

for nurturance and self-determination rights (see Figure 1). Moreover, significant age

group X respondent interactions only applied to self-determination rights. Specifically,

views between mothers and adolescents regarding self-determination rights were most

discrepant amongst the early-adolescent group. Although mothers’ and adolescents’

endorsements of self-determination rights were still discrepant from one another amongst

the mid-adolescent group, this discrepancy was less than that found among the younger

age group (see Figure 1). On the other hand, mothers and adolescents did not differ from

one another in their support for nurturance rights in either age group. There were no

significant differences in terms of how adolescents or mothers responded as a function of

the adolescents’ gender. Adolescent gender was therefore omitted in subsequent

regression analyses.

Page 97: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

87

Figure2.Rights X Age Group X Respondent Interactions.

1

2

3

4

5

6

NurturanceRightsSelf-determinationRights

NurturanceRightsSelf-determinationRights

Adolescents Mothers

MeanChildren'sRightsAttitudes

12-year-olds16-year-olds

**

**ns

ns

*** **

Page 98: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

88

Hierarchical Regression Analyses: Predicting Adolescents’ and Mothers’ Attitudes

Toward Children’s Rights

To assess how adolescents’ perceptions of parenting contribute to their attitudes

toward children rights, I performed two hierarchical regression analyses on adolescents’

endorsement of nurturance and self-determination rights, respectively. To check for

multi-collinearity, tolerance statistics and variance inflation factors were examined for all

regression models and none was > 5.0. Thus, the multiple regression analyses were not

considered to be biased (Neter et al., 1990).

Adolescents’ attitudes toward nurturance and self-determination rights. In

order to avoid over-fitting of the model, only demographic variables that have significant

relations with the outcome variable were included in the first step of the regression model

as controls. Specifically, when nurturance and self-determination rights were analyzed

separately (instead of together in the repeated ANOVAs above), age group and setting

were found to be significant for nurturance rights, whereas only age group was found to

be significant for self-determination rights. In addition to the significant age group effects

described above, it was found that urban adolescents endorsed nurturance rights to a

significantly higher extent than their rural counterparts. These demographic variables

were entered in the first step of the regression analyses accordingly (Table 8). All

parenting variables (including both maternal and paternal responsiveness and autonomy

support) were included in the second step to explore the relationship between different

parenting dimensions, as perceived by the adolescents, and their attitudes toward

children’s rights.

Page 99: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

89

As Table 8 shows, in addition to age group, father responsiveness significantly

predicted higher levels of endorsement of nurturance rights. None of the parenting

variables significantly predicted adolescents’ support for self-determination rights. The

introduction of democratic family climate variables in the third block did not contribute

significantly to either of the regression models. In the fourth step of the regression

analyses, adolescents’ perception of tolerance of dissent at home emerged as a significant

predictor, which was positively related to support for both nurturance and self-

determination rights. The regression models accounted for 24% and 17% of the variances

in nurturance and self-determination rights attitudes, respectively (see Table 8).

Table 8.

Regression Examining Correlates between Family Socialization Environment and Adolescents’ Children’s Rights Attitudes

Nurturance Rights Self-Determination Rights Variables ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β Step 1 .10 6.81** .05 6.91** Age Group .19* -.22* (Setting) .16 Step 2 .11 3.88** .03 .90 M Autonomy Support .26 .22

F Autonomy Support -.25 -.27 M Responsiveness -.14 -.10 F Responsiveness .48** .15 Step 3 .00 .31 .01 .95 Democratic Family Climate -.10 -.17

Step 4 .03 4.55* .08 11.42*** Tolerance of Dissent .20* .31*** Total R2 .24 .17

Note. M = mother; F = father. *p <.05, **p <.01, ***p < .001.

Relations between mothers’ attitudes toward children’s rights and their self-

reported parenting. Next, I assessed whether mothers’ current parenting practices

predicted their attitudes toward children’s rights. Similar to the regression analyses

Page 100: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

90

conducted for adolescents, I included only the significant main effects of origin-setting

into the mothers’ regression models as a control (Table 9). Mothers’ self-perceived

parenting in the second step was significant for both models, with maternal autonomy

support significantly predicting both higher levels of endorsement of nurturance rights

and self-determination rights (Maternal autonomy support was no longer significant in

the nurturance rights model after the addition of tolerance of dissent in Step 4, see Table

3). Maternal responsiveness also significantly predicted support for nurturance rights.

Democratic family climate was not a significant predictor in Step 3 in either models. In

step 4, mothers’ perceptions of their tolerance of dissent emerged as significant positive

predictors of their support for nurturance and self-determination rights, above and beyond

what was predicted by more general measures of parental autonomy support and

responsiveness and democratic family climate. Together, the regression model accounted

for 40% of the variance in nurturance rights attitudes and 19% of the variance in self-

determination rights attitudes.

Table 9.

Relations between Mothers’ Children's Rights Attitudes and their Self-reported Parenting

Nurturance Rights Self-Determination Rights Variables ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β Step 1 .11 14.10*** .06 6.71* Origin-Setting .13 .08 Step 2 .25 22.41*** .09 5.77** Autonomy Support .18 .28* Responsiveness .34*** -.20 Step 3 .00 .00 .00 .26 Democratic Family Climate -.02 .04 Step 4 .04 7.63** .04 5.95* Tolerance of Dissent .23** .24* Total R2 .40 .19 Note. *p <.05, **p <.01, ***p < .001.

Page 101: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

91

Relations between mothers’ retrospective recall of parenting and their

children’s rights attitudes. Lastly, I also assessed whether mothers’ retrospective recall

of how they were parented as adolescents predicted their current attitudes toward

children’s rights. Their origin-setting in Step 1 was significant for both models, with

urban mothers (from both the urban and rural-to-urban group) endorsing both rights to a

significantly greater extent than rural mothers. Controlling for demographic variables,

none of the parenting nor family environment variables as recalled by mothers in the

second, third, and fourth step was predictive of their current attitudes toward children’s

rights for nurturance and self-determination (see Table 10).

Table 10.

Mothers' Retrospective Recall of Parenting and Current Attitudes Toward Children's Rights

Nurturance Rights Self-Determination Rights Variables ΔR2 ΔF β ΔR2 ΔF β Step 1 .11 14.10*** .06 6.71* Origin-Setting

.33*** .23*

Step 2 (.05) 1.48 (.04) 1.13 M Autonomy Support F Autonomy Support M Responsiveness F Responsiveness Step 3 (.01) 1.12 (.01) .63 Democratic Family Climate Step 4 (.00) .26 (.01) 1.59 Tolerance of Dissent Total R2 .11 .06 Note. M = mother; F = father. *p <.05, **p <.01, ***p < .001.

Page 102: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

92

Discussion

The debate about the universality of children’s rights has been a growing topic of

discussion in the past decades, which has set in motion a small body of cross-cultural

research examining attitudes about children’s rights. Studies across a range of settings

and contexts have consistently found that children and adults recognize and endorse

children’s rights to both nurturance and self-determination. However, very little research

has examined concurrently both children’s and their mothers’ attitudes toward children’s

rights in non-Western cultural settings. This is a particularly important area for research

given the debates between universalists and relativists about the relevance of rights issues

for culturally non-Western societies and the criticism of imposing Western liberal notions

of individualism and rights onto other non-Western societies (Markus & Kitayama,

1991). The current study fills this gap in the literature by examining children’s rights

attitudes amongst children and mothers in a non-Western cultural context (China) and

includes investigation of the correlates of rights attitudes in both a fast-changing, modern

urban setting and a traditional rural setting.

Mothers’ and Adolescents’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Rights

The present study found that adolescents and their mothers across diverse settings

within China demonstrated support for both nurturance and self-determination rights. In

addition to supporting the rights to be protected and cared for, Chinese respondents

(particularly amongst the adolescents) also demonstrated that they highly value

opportunities for children to exercise their rights to participation and to make personal

autonomous decisions, even though these practices are not traditionally valued in their

societies or may even run counter to existing cultural practices.

Page 103: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

93

Consistent with previous research, both Chinese adolescents and their mothers

supported nurturance rights to a greater extent than self-determination rights, and hence

viewed the rights of children to be cared for and protected from harm as more important

than children’s entitlement to self-determination. On the other hand, whereas most of the

prior studies comparing children’s and parents’ attitudes toward rights found that adults

held more favorable attitudes about children’s nurturance rights (e.g., Day et al., 2006;

Peterson-Badali et al., 2004; Rogers & Wrightsman, 1978), adolescents and their mothers

in our current study did not differ from one another in terms of their attitudes about

nurturance rights. In fact, Chinese mothers and their adolescents in both age groups

highly endorsed nurturance rights (consistent with the rights findings of South African

mixed-race children and their mothers; Ruck, Tenenbaum, & Wallenberg, 2011).

Moreover, this is also consistent with the cultural emphasis in China on the role of

parents and authority to provide care and protection to children.

Our hypothesis that adolescents would support self-determination rights to a

greater extent than their mothers was supported. Both early and mid-adolescents endorsed

their rights to exercise personal choices and autonomy across the variety of contexts and

situations included in our measure (including freedom of expression, privacy and making

choices over areas that matter to them like friendships). However, contrary to our

expectation that the endorsement of self-determination rights would be stronger in the

mid-adolescent group, we found that younger adolescents in the current study were found

to be more likely than older adolescents to favor children’s self-determination rights. This

finding is not consistent with a previous study conducted in China in a different province

that held the target child constant at 12 years of age, in which support for both self-

Page 104: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

94

determination and nurturance rights was found to be higher in older adolescents (To et

al., 2017). Further investigation showed that the adolescents in both age groups in our

current research were younger than the two age groups represented in the To and

colleagues’ (2017) study. Indeed, the mid-adolescent group in the current study is closer

in age and in their endorsement of nurturance and self-determination rights to the early

adolescent group in To et al. (2017)’s study. With the target child set at twelve years old,

which is very close in age to the early adolescent group represented in the current study,

it is possible that the heightened endorsement of self-determination rights was the result

of the aspiration of these adolescents for more autonomy rights for themselves (Peterson-

Badali et al., 2008). Hence, it is possible that the discrepancies between the findings of

the present and previous studies may lie in the methodological factors, i.e. the relative

correspondence between the participants’ age and the target age of the character child

being examined. Indeed, the findings of greater endorsement of self-determination rights

by early adolescents in this study is consistent with those of prior studies (e.g., Day et al.,

2006), which, like the present study, found that younger participants who are similar in

age as the target child, were more likely to support self-determination rights.

Further, it is possible that older Chinese adolescents may be responding more like

the mother participants when evaluating a target child who is younger than they are.

Similar to their mothers’ reasoning in prior studies (e.g., Peterson-Badali & Ruck, 2008;

Ruck et al., 2002), older adolescents may include maturity and competence as part of

their evaluation and view the target child in the current study as one who is still in need

of parental protection and care, and not yet fully capable of self-determination.

Page 105: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

95

A similar explanation may account for why the differences between adolescents’

and mothers’ attitudes toward self-determination rights were most discrepant amongst the

younger age group, with the discrepancies gradually narrowing in the mid-adolescent

group. This finding is noteworthy given that previous studies have reported that

adolescent-mother conflicts peak during early adolescence (usually between 12 and 13

years of age), when adolescents attempt to expand their personal autonomy and are often

met with parental resistance (Collins, 1990; Peterson-Badali, Ruck, & Bone, 2008).

These findings provide converging evidence that while differences between parents and

their children are common during adolescence, it is during early adolescence, when the

focus of developmental needs may be different amongst adolescents and parents, that

these differences are most salient. Although the parents’ focus remains mainly on

protection and nurturance needs at this stage of development, young adolescents’ desire

for autonomy appears to be ahead of parental readiness and may be actively driving their

needs into realization (Smetana, 2011).

I did not identify a subgroup of adolescents in our current study who distinctly

show a reactive pattern in their endorsement of rights, unlike in our prior research (To et

al., 2017). However, younger adolescents in this study appear to be demonstrating a

similar pattern as they strive for and put considerable attention on rights that are deemed

just as important to them, yet may not have been fully met in their lives. These findings

of adolescents’ heightened awareness of rights that have not been fully fulfilled were also

echoed by the qualitative findings of Peterson-Badali and her colleagues' (2008) study of

maltreated children in Canada. Previously maltreated children were asked to generate

rights scenarios in order to examine the rights that were most salient for them. Similar to

Page 106: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

96

the current study’s findings, they found that the youngest adolescents, who were often not

yet given much decision making opportunities compared to their older counterparts,

mentioned the right to make autonomous decisions more than other type of right in their

self-generated responses. As older adolescents are usually given more opportunities to

exercise these rights, they were also observed to be less concerned about such issues and

hence mentioned them less often. Peterson-Badali et al. (2008) suggested that these self-

generated rights by young adolescents have an “aspirational” aspect as they are striving

for opportunities that are not yet fully realized, which makes these rights issues

particularly salient for them. These findings are also in line with that of Helwig et al.

(2003) and Lau (1992), who found that values which are perceived as being insufficiently

realized in one’s social environment tend to be more highly valued compared to those

that are customarily granted.

The position of the older adolescents in regard to children’s self-determination

rights may reflect their greater sophistication in balancing considerations of children’s

maturity, autonomy, and the conflicting goals of social contexts, rather than simply

deference to adult authority. Previous studies conducted in Western and non-Western

settings (e.g., Khoury-Kassabri & Ben-Arieh, 2009; Lahat et al., 2009) of children’s

reasoning about rights (rather than their attitudes, as measured in the present study) has

shown that older adolescents’ thinking about self-determination rights appears to be more

complex and differentiated. Instead of simply endorsing children’s rights to make

autonomous decisions across social contexts, older adolescents seem to be more attuned

to the socialization goals of different social institutions, distinguishing among different

social contexts (e.g. family, school) when making decisions (e.g., Lahat et al., 2009).

Page 107: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

97

However, despite less strong endorsement of self-determination rights, older adolescents

are not simply subordinating children’s autonomy rights to adult authority. Instead, the

prior literature on reasoning about rights suggests that with age, Chinese adolescents, like

their Western counterparts, made more differentiated social and moral judgments based

on social context and the type of decision under consideration (Helwig et al., 2003;

Helwig & Kim, 1999). For example, although Chinese adolescents justified the need to

rely on the superior competence of authorities to make appropriate choices regarding

certain issues (e.g., decisions about school curriculum), they appealed to children’s

autonomy and viewed children as having equal rights to contribute to decision making in

other contexts (e.g., in the peer group, or for certain family decisions), where children

were believed to have the maturity to make these decisions on their own (Helwig et al.,

2003; Helwig & Kim, 1999). Thus, with age, adolescents increasingly make more

multifaceted and differentiated social judgments, characterized by balancing concerns

with the best interests of the child along with attunement to the evolving capacities of the

child. The gradual development of adolescents’ ability to make these more nuanced

distinctions during adolescence may explain the growing rapprochement between older

adolescents and their mothers in their support of children’s rights to self-determination.

The significant correlations between mother-reported support for adolescents’

nurturance and self-determination rights indicates that mothers who are sensitive to the

developmental needs of their children are attuned to both their developing needs for

autonomy as well as their ongoing needs for care and protection. Interestingly,

adolescents’ support for nurturance and self-determination was not significantly

correlated, showing that adolescents regard these two types of rights as distinct

Page 108: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

98

constructs. Indeed, prior studies on adolescents’ social reasoning regarding their

endorsement of these rights demonstrated that they provided distinctly different

justifications depending on the type of right (e.g., Ruck, 1998). Moreover, the current

study suggests that there may be differential pathways in the development of rights

conceptions for these two types of rights, which will be discussed in further detail in the

next section.

Socialization Correlates of Adolescents’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Rights

The hypothesis that parenting and family democratic climate are related to

attitudes toward children’s nurturance and self-determination rights was generally

supported; results revealed that adolescents’ endorsement of nurturance rights was

predicted by their perceptions of parental autonomy support and responsiveness as well

as a democratic family environment. It is particularly surprising that father

responsiveness emerged as an especially important predictor of adolescents’ attitudes

about their nurturance rights. Given that mothers are often the primary caregivers in

China, child responsiveness and nurturing may be regarded as part of the role assumed by

mothers. On the other hand, Chinese fathers often assume a figure of authority and

discipline in the household (Lamb, 2004). Being responsive and nurturing may not fit

with the traditional stereotype. As a result, the experience of having responsive fathers

who are attuned to the developmental needs of the children may be particularly salient to

their children, and in turn may play an important role in the development of the

awareness of their rights. Indeed, a few studies have shown that Chinese fathers’

parenting is predictive of children’s development and outcomes, above and beyond the

effects of mothers’ parenting (e.g., Ang, 2006; Chen, Liu, & Li, 2000; Shek, 1999).

Page 109: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

99

These findings regarding the unique role of Chinese fathers in the development of

children’s understanding and endorsement of nurturance rights issues is noteworthy and

warrants further research to determine whether those findings could be replicated in other

studies and in other, non-Western cultural settings.

Contrary to my hypotheses, I did not find that children’s perception of autonomy

supportive parenting and democratic family climate predicted their endorsement of self-

determination rights. The current findings thus suggest the possibility of a differential

pathway regarding the development of different types of children’s rights issues.

Specifically, children's support for nurturance is more closely tied to their family

socialization experience, which shapes their beliefs and expectations of what to expect

from adults in terms of being cared for and protected from harm. On the other hand,

awareness and support for self-determination rights appears to arise from children’s

innate developmental needs for autonomy. Children appear to be aware of its dynamic

and negotiated nature and regard it as something that they claim over time with

development and through negotiation (Smetana, 2011).

However, tolerance of dissent seems to be an important general predictor, as it

was positively associated with and significantly predicted both attitudes toward

nurturance and self-determination rights amongst Chinese adolescents, even after

controlling for general parental responsiveness and autonomy support in the regression

analyses. Consistent with prior research among different ethnicities in the Middle East as

well as Chinese adolescents in urban and rural China (Khoury-Kassabri & Ben-Arieh,

2009; To et al., 2017), our findings suggest that children’s own experience of being able

to openly express their opinions, whether the same or different from parental views,

Page 110: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

100

facilitates stronger endorsement of children’s rights issues. Having the experience of

freely expressing themselves also shapes their belief that other children should have these

rights. It is noted that these associations between tolerance of dissent and both nurturance

and self-determination rights were found across both urban and rural settings, and for

males and females. This is striking as there are considerable differences in modernization

and traditionalism between these environments, with more traditional gender

socialization norms obtaining in rural settings (Zhang, Zheng, & Wang, 2003),

suggesting that similar processes may be at work. Given that being able to openly

criticize and express opposition to parental views is generally incompatible with

traditional Chinese cultural norms or may even be frowned upon by parental authority,

children’s experience with parents who are tolerant of dissent may be particularly salient

in promoting children’s own rights conceptions. These findings provide support for the

proposition that across diverse cultural contexts, socialization agents who are sensitive to

children’s developmental needs—especially their needs for expressing their own views—

may enhance children’s appreciation of their rights.

Since no other studies I am aware of have examined the relationship between

children’s rights attitudes and tolerance of dissent in cultural settings in which opposing

views from children are more commonly welcomed, further studies are needed to

determine whether the unique relationship between tolerance of dissent and rights

attitudes is found in other cultural settings.

Correlates of Mothers’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Rights

As expected, an examination of relations between maternal views of children’s

rights and maternal parenting practices revealed strong relationships between these

Page 111: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

101

constructs, indicating that maternal attitudes toward children’s rights to nurturance and

self-determination generally translate into their parenting behavior. Specifically, mothers’

self-reported responsive parenting predicted maternal endorsement of children’s

nurturance rights, whereas autonomy supportive parenting positively predicted their

attitudes about self-determination rights. Moreover, maternal endorsement of both rights

was related to their self-reported tolerance of dissent. The shared associations between

tolerance of dissent and both types of rights reflect that there may be similar underlying

processes, i.e., that parents who are more open to criticism may view the developmental

needs of children to take precedence over parental authority, leading to stronger

endorsement of both types of children’s rights.

Although there are clear relationships between adolescents’ socialization

experience and their thinking about rights, it is interesting to note that none of the

retrospective family socialization variables emerge as significant predictors for mothers’

current attitudes toward children’s rights issues. It appears that while socialization

experiences (e.g., experience of autonomy, exercising rights and expressing their views

freely) are related to adolescents’ current conceptions of rights, with time and as one

becomes a parent oneself, other factors (e.g., their current socio-cultural environment)

may become more directly predictive of mothers’ thinking about children’s rights.

Further research, perhaps of a longitudinal nature, should examine other contextual

factors that may predict parental attitudes and facilitate their construction of children’s

rights issues over time.

While most studies tend to depend on only one source of informants, one

particular strength of the current study was the simultaneous investigation of multiple

Page 112: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

102

informants (i.e., mothers and adolescents) to examine the relationships and whether there

are any significant differences amongst different informants. Our current findings show

that adolescents’ perceptions and experiences of rights and autonomy appear to be quite

different from mothers’ self-report of their own parenting practices. Generally, when

family socialization variables were rated by the adolescent participants, the associations

between these variables and the child’s endorsement of rights were much stronger than

the relationship between these variables when they were rated by the parents. These

findings are similar to those of Yeung and Chan (2015), who found that the relationships

between family processes and parenting practices and child mental health were much

stronger when they were rated from the perspective of children (instead of from their

parents). Although this may be partially explained by the shared variance when the

variables were reported by the same respondents, these findings also demonstrate that it is

the child’s subjective perception, as the individual who experiences the parenting first-

hand, that matters the most when investigating the correlates of children’s rights attitudes.

Conclusions

Similar to other research with participants of Western cultural background (e.g.,

Day et al., 2006; Peterson-Badali et al., 2004; Rogers & Wrightsman, 1978), Chinese

adolescents and mothers in the current study endorsed a broad range of children’s rights

including rights issues related to both nurturance and self-determination. Despite the

differences between mothers and their adolescents in their support for children’s self-

determination rights, this gap slowly narrows with adolescents’ age. As China’s efforts of

modernization continue, the shifts in sociopolitical environment likely would become an

impetus for changes in social norms, attitudes, beliefs and practices. Both Chinese

Page 113: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

103

mothers and adolescents appear to be actively constructing their own views about rights

and adapting their behaviors as they experience continuous changes in their immediate

environment and evolving norms. Longitudinal studies would be warranted in the future

in order to systematically examine how individuals’ attitudes may change and evolve

over time during periods of social change and especially as children become parents

themselves. Hence, for a complete understanding of this issue, environmental factors and

social changes would need to be considered in conjunction with developmental pathways

and the changing roles in ones’ lifespan. The current period is in fact a prime time to

study this topic. With China’s continual effort to reform and reduce the rural-urban gap,

drastic social change will likely continue to happen, particularly in the rural areas in the

next decades (Guthrie, 2012).

Although the present study extends previous research on children’s rights by

considering both adolescents’ and mothers’ conceptions of rights in two contrasting

settings within China, there are several limitations that should be noted. First, due to the

cross-sectional design of the current studies, direction of relationships cannot be inferred

from the findings. As noted earlier, longitudinal research in future studies would be

warranted to elucidate the causality of relationships and allow examination of changes in

attitudes about rights over time, human development, and social change. Secondly,

instead of using a rights attitudes measure that is derived from a Western cultural setting,

qualitative research to elicit indigenous and spontaneous rights responses from Chinese

adolescents and parents would allow us to identify rights issues that are most salient to

their lives without potentially imposing a Western cultural framework on their responses.

Moreover, findings of the present study show that tolerance of dissent is a particularly

Page 114: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

104

prominent predictor of adolescents’ attitudes toward rights; future research would benefit

from incorporating this variable when examining family socialization processes. It would

be particularly important to examine whether the unique contribution of this variable

found in the current study would be replicated and generalizable to other settings. Lastly,

given that the current studies have shown the importance and unique role of fathers’

parenting in shaping children’s attitudes about their rights, further studies need to

consider fathers’ beliefs and attitudes toward children’s rights to self-determination and

nurturance, and how these beliefs may shape the rights attitudes, beliefs, and practices of

our next generation.

Page 115: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

105

General Discussion

The papers presented in this dissertation aim to contribute to our understanding of

how autonomy, rights, and parenting may be understood in the cultural settings of urban

and rural China: two settings that have been rapidly changing at a different pace over the

past decades with the rise of modernization.

The two studies examine the relationship between different types of cultural

parenting practices and adolescents’ psychological well-being, as well as the types of

socialization practices that encourage the development of understandings of rights in the

family context. Comparing mothers from three different origin-setting groups (urban,

rural-to-urban, and rural China), encompassing urban social ecologies of the Gesellschaft

form to the rural social ecologies of the Gemeinschaft form (Greenfield, 2009), these

studies provide insight into how the extent of urbanization impacts parenting practices,

beliefs and attitudes, and consequently affects children’s development in a non-Western

cultural setting.

Socialization Pathways to Well-Being and the Development of Rights Conceptions

Adolescents’ socialization experiences appear to be important determinants

affecting their psychological, cognitive, and moral development (e.g., Baumrind, 1966;

Hasebe et al., 2004; Jia et al., 2009; Vansteenkiste et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2007).

Findings from both of our studies demonstrated converging evidence supporting the

universalist claim that children in a variety of cultural contexts benefit from family

environments which are responsive and promote children’s needs to exercise autonomy.

Consistent with the proposition of SDT, the significant associations between autonomy

supportive parenting and children’s positive psychological well-being, across settings of

Page 116: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

106

modern urban and traditional rural China, suggest universal processes in the development

of autonomy, a basic psychological need.

In addition to parenting that is responsive to children’s developmental need for

personal autonomy and rights, I also found that democratic family environments with

features like the opportunity to participate in decision making, and the ability to freely

express one’s voice—especially when the perspectives are in disagreement with the

parents’—can be an important pathway in supporting optimal child development and in

facilitating an appreciation of children’s rights. These findings are particularly intriguing

given that China is governed by the Communist party in which freedom of thought,

tolerance for dissent, and discussions related to notions of democracy are often

discouraged. Moreover, despite globalization and the widespread use of the Internet

within China, publications and digital forms of information regarding politics and

democracy are tightly restricted by the state and the Community Party of China (Guillén

& Suárez, 2005). Nevertheless, Chinese children develop notions of personal autonomy

and rights that encompass the importance of democratic participation and expressions of

dissent or criticism (see Ruck, Peterson-Badali, & Helwig, 2014 for a review). These

conceptions of autonomy, democracy and rights hence are not merely products of

Western civilizations nor are they only relevant for individuals with an independent view

of the self (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Shweder & Sullivan, 1993). Instead, these notions

appear to have functional relevance in the realization of psychological well-being and are

important for the development of rights conceptions for children in non-Western cultural

settings, even though the political atmosphere may not be fully compatible with these

ideas.

Page 117: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

107

From Global Cultural Orientations To Dynamic Shifts and Movements within

Culture and Over Time

Putting together the findings of the two studies, it is evident that social and

economic changes in China within the past few decades have brought about changes in

social norms and created ripple effects challenging traditional beliefs and practices.

Despite the long history of Confucianism in China which emphasizes hierarchy,

obedience to authority, and the subordination of the self to the social group, these

elements of the more traditional, historical collectivist culture are now combined with

values that are adaptive to a modern urban economy (Wang, 2014). Dynamic shifts in

values and cultural orientations were noted as the social change unfolds (Chen et al.,

2010). Due to the economic reforms and opening up policy that was initiated in 1978,

China, especially the urban coastal areas, has experienced rapid urbanization and

improvements in living standards and education (Mackerras, 2001). With these

socioeconomic changes came the demand for very different qualities and skills in the

labour market. This corresponds with a shift in parenting style emphasizing assertiveness,

independence, creativity, and autonomy, and a shift in attitudes toward children’s rights

that was becoming more common amongst urban parents (Naftali, 2016).

In the current studies, the findings appear to be consistent with Greenfield’s

proposition that mothers may reflect on how they were parented and modify their current

parenting in response to the new requirements of the changing social ecologies

(Greenfield, 2009). Indeed, a shift in cultural parenting practices was reported, with

mothers reporting themselves to be more autonomy supportive and more tolerant of

different opinions and criticism from their children when compared to their own parents.

Page 118: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

108

This parallels shifts in the societal beliefs and views regarding the raising of children

(Stearns, 2016). Similarly, it was found that Chinese mothers’ attitudes toward children’s

rights are no longer predicted by their own socialization experiences. Other contextual

factors like their current socio-cultural environment (e.g., the modern urban setting)

appear to be influencing their thinking about children’s autonomy and attitudes toward

children’s rights issues over time.

Hence, rather than adhering to a purely global and stable cultural orientation of

collectivism, Chinese adolescents and parents took into consideration sociocultural

change and its corresponding shift in societal demands, in their own parenting and when

making judgments about children’s rights (e.g., Wang, 2014). Moreover, variations

within Chinese culture are also evident in our studies. The impact of social change is

revealed in the different pace of attitudinal and behavioral shifts observed in parents of

different setting-origin groups. As hypothesized, the drastic reform in urban China may

have led urban parents to reflect on traditional practices and values and to integrate

modern values into their parenting to meet the new demands of a capitalist and globalized

economy. However, due to the uneven socioeconomic development and different pace of

change in rural areas, there is much less of a degree of value shift and corresponding

change in parenting practices amongst rural Chinese parents. Moreover, the finding that

rural-to-urban mothers were in the intermediate stage of this process, that is, between

urban and rural parents in terms of their rights attitudes and socialization practices, points

to the dynamic and transitional nature of parenting practices and attitudes during social

change. It is evident that shifts in attitudes do not happen overnight. The process is

gradual and likely requires active reflection on varied experiences, as reflected in the

Page 119: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

109

different pace of change in thinking and attitudes amongst rural to urban parents, who

had experienced both types of social environments (Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft). The

dynamic shifts in parenting practices and beliefs within culture and over time in

contemporary China also point to the fallacy of oversimplifying and dichotomizing

cultures as static constructs (for instance in the form of individualism and collectivism).

Constructivist Approaches to Values and Beliefs

A consistent theme that arose in both papers was adolescents’ and mothers’ active

role in their construction of values, beliefs, and in the socialization environment they

provide to their children. Instead of simply transmitting and accepting traditional cultural

practices and values from one generation to another, Chinese adolescents and mothers

appear to be actively constructing their own views about rights and parenting practices

based on several factors, including reflection on their own socialization experience, their

recognition of the innate developmental needs for autonomy, and an acknowledgement of

the socialization demands in their current social environment.

Chinese adolescents and their mothers demonstrated that they value opportunities

for children to exercise rights for participation and to make autonomous decisions (even

though these practices are not traditionally valued in their societies or may even run

counter to existing cultural practices). Moreover, adolescents were found to hold beliefs

and to endorse practices that not only reflected values that are traditionally shared by

Chinese cultures, but that also integrate their developing conceptions of autonomy, and

they endorsed rights that may be contested or insufficiently realized in their prevailing

socialization milieu. Hence, adolescents are seen as active agents within their

socialization environment as they strive for an environment that meets their

Page 120: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

110

developmental needs.

Similarly, urban mothers appeared to endorse more autonomy-related rights when

compared to what they perceived that they had received during adolescence. This is in

stark contrast to rural mothers’ reports, of which there were no differences between their

self-reported parenting and their retrospective reports of their mother’s parenting. This

suggests that urban mothers may have reflected upon the way they were raised and

integrated their knowledge of what is required in the new economy and society, which

may have differed from what they had experienced growing up. Hence, instead of

directly passing along traditional parenting beliefs and practices, this finding of shifts in

urban parenting highlights the active role of mothers in constructing their beliefs of

parenting practices in response to changes in their social and cultural milieu.

Different Pace in Value Versus Behavioral Changes

Another interesting observation derived from the two papers is the finding that

while modernization and socio-environmental change is happening at a speedy pace,

values, beliefs, attitudes, and behavioral change may occur at a different pace. On the

one hand, findings in Paper 1 illustrated that mothers’ current parenting practices remain

quite heavily influenced by the way mothers recalled they were parented (in addition to

the differential influence of urban and rural environments discussed above). On the other

hand, findings from Paper 2 illustrated that mothers’ recall of their own prior

socialization experience was not related to their current attitudes toward children’s rights.

It appears that while their own experiences of how they were parented influences their

parenting behaviors, mothers’ current environmental circumstances appear to be the most

important factor having ramifications for their explicit attitudes regarding children’s

Page 121: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

111

rights. It is possible that living in an urban environment has heightened their awareness of

the changing societal demands and led them to recognize their children’s needs for

autonomy to a greater extent than rural parents whose social environment has changed

relatively less. Moreover, there has been an increase in the general population's,

especially urban parents,' access to information about children's rights as well as an

increase in public awareness of the importance of children's rights to overall healthy

development (Naftali, 2015).

The findings that mothers’ endorsements of children’s rights, unlike their current

parenting practices, were not predicted by their own socialization experience suggest that

values, attitudes, and behavioral change may be impacted by social change in a different

fashion and at a different pace. It is possible that behaviors like parenting practices are

often more visceral and less conscious as they result from reactions to parent-child

situations. During those interactions, the continual impact of cultural traditional parenting

practices is more evident, and it is probably more likely for one to replicate the way they

were raised. This process is consistent with expectations from the social learning and

attachment theory about transmission of parenting styles across generations, as discussed

in the Introduction of Paper 1. On the other hand, acquisition of values, and specifically,

the development of rights conceptions, would be expected to involve a more reflective

process. These understandings require one to actively reflect on one’s own socialization

experiences with regard to children’s welfare and autonomy needs, and to integrate this

knowledge with current socio-cultural demands. Hence, with time, mothers’ attitudes

may no longer be predicted by their socialization experience (especially when cultural

change produces relevant new material for reflection). These findings suggest that

Page 122: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

112

different processes may underlie the changes in attitudes (children’s rights attitudes in

this case) and behaviors (e.g., parenting practices).

Moreover, social psychology research has found that measures of general

attitudes often are poor predictors of actual behaviors (Wicker, 1969). The

inconsistencies between general attitudes and specific behaviors have led many social

psychologists to question the relevance and utility of attitudes measures (e.g., Campbell,

1963; Wicker, 1969). Researchers have often attributed response biases (i.e., the tendency

to give socially desirable responses on attitudes) as one of the reasons that may have led

to the failure of capturing true attitudes, and hence led to the attitude-behavior

inconsistences. More recent approaches, however, have found that when measures of

attitudes and behaviors are at compatible levels of specificity, the correlations between

the two measures are often much higher (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2005). For the current

studies, while the measures of children’s rights were rather specific (asking about rights

in specific contexts), the measures of parenting behaviors used were comparatively

general (asking general perception of autonomy support and responsiveness in the

family). Hence, specific children’s rights attitudes may not provide a good basis for

predicting and explaining general parenting behaviors. Future studies may consider

matching the levels of specificity of the attitude and behavior measures, for example, by

asking more specific parenting behaviors that are relevant to specific children’s rights

issues to further explore the relationship between the two constructs.

Mothers’ Versus Adolescents’ Reports

Research on parenting has often relied on the reports of a single reporter (usually

child reported data) for behaviors of other family members and in drawing conclusions

Page 123: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

113

about the family climate. However, the present study investigated parents’ and their

adolescent children’s reports of parenting behaviors and family climate and found that the

associations between the two reporters were generally modest. The non-significant to

moderate levels of agreement between parents and their adolescents is consistent with

findings conducted in the United States and China, pointing to the very different

perceptions of parenting that children and parents may have (e.g., Cheung et al., 2016;

Gonzales et al., 1996). Across studies examining differences in perceptions of family

climate, adolescents consistently perceive families more negatively, more conflictual and

less cohesive than their parents do (e.g., Noller et al., 1992; Ohannessian, Lerner, Lerner,

& Von Eye, 1995). Similarly, when comparing the two reporter’s ratings, adolescents in

the current study consistently reported their mothers’ parenting to be less autonomy

supportive, and the family climate as less democratic, than their mothers did. This finding

may reflect adolescents’ increased expectations regarding autonomy, leading to

discrepancies between adolescent expectations and those of their parents, as has been

well-documented in parenting research across cultures (see Smetana, 2011, for a review).

Moreover, on the one hand, adolescents may base their reports of parenting on both

observable parenting behaviors and what they perceive as appropriate for them as they

assert their growing autonomy and attempt to achieve individuation during adolescence.

On the other hand, parents’ self-reports may be based on not only their parenting

behavior, but also their cognition and intentions, which may not always be manifested in

their observable behaviors (Schifield et al., 2016).

In addition, within-rater reports were found to be generally higher than cross-rater

(i.e., between mothers’ and adolescents’) reports in the current study. Hence, while

Page 124: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

114

significant relations were found when only adolescent reports or only mother reports

were used; relations between adolescent reports and mother reports were generally non-

significant. These findings may be due to the shared-method variance and as mentioned

previously, may reflect the different viewpoints of the respondents.

Nevertheless, despite the discrepancies, Schofield and his colleagues (2016)

proposed that the low correlations between different raters of parenting is a strength

(instead of a problem), as each report is “uniquely biased” and “equally valid” in

contributing their perspectives to the constructs. Future research examining measures of

family socialization should explore whether the discrepancies between reporters depends

on the parenting construct under consideration and explore the potential of leveraging the

power of reporters discrepancies by understanding the discrepancy patterns among

multiple raters to predict outcomes (Schofield et al., 2016).

Implications for Research and Future Directions

- From Cross-Sectional to Longitudinal Studies

The current research findings also suggest some specific avenues for further

research to guide policy and practice. To date, much of the work has focused on Western

cultural contexts in a set time. The current studies, despite utilizing diverse settings in a

non-Western cultural context, like most work conducted thus far, is cross-sectional. The

disadvantage with cross-sectional studies is that the direction of causality cannot be

inferred.

For example, while our focus was on the effects of parenting on adolescents'

psychological well-being in Paper 1, recent theoretical advances and studies indicate that

Page 125: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

115

parenting is no longer understood as a one-way process. Instead, bidirectional effects

have been found, and adolescents also had a strong effect on parenting styles (Smetana,

2017). Conceptualizing parenting in isolation, without the consideration of children as

agents within their family, would not be able to capture the dynamic and interactive

nature of the socialization processes (Nixon & Halpenny, 2010).

Similarly, the relationship between social change and the development and shift in

attitudes, values, and behaviors is not a simple one-way direction of causality. Instead,

there is likely an interactive process involving the social environmental impact on

people's thinking, norms and beliefs about “proper” parenting practices and children’s

rights attitudes. This in turn further drives societal development and modernization.

Hence, in order to better understand the dynamic and interactive nature of development

and social change, it would be essential to examine social changes along with the

expansion of autonomy needs during development in adolescence, as well as the role of

social change in the socialization processes.

Moreover, virtually nothing about the factors leading to the development of

parenting practices and beliefs as one becomes parents in non-Western cultural contexts

is known. The developmental period between adolescence and young adulthood would be

a particularly interesting one to explore, as this is often a time when values and attitudes

are further developed as individuals transition to more independent thinking and possibly

parenthood. For these reasons, longitudinal studies examining socialization processes

across time, particularly during this important period of transition, would allow us to

further delineate the effects of development and modernization, and study the effects of

cultural transformation and change on parental behaviors and their development of

Page 126: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

116

children’s rights attitudes.

- From Self-reports to Observational Reports of Autonomy-Supportive Parenting

The two studies presented in this dissertation used mother and adolescent self-

reports. As discussed prior, the potential impacts of reporter effects were evident in the

current studies. To address these issues, the use of observational reports of parenting may

likely help to establish the exact way in which autonomy-supportive parenting is

expressed within Chinese socialization contexts. There remains a huge gap in

observational studies of parenting in China and especially rural settings when compared

to the large number of such studies conducted in Western cultures.

Similarly, although our study uncovered clear associations between autonomy-

supportive parenting and psychological well-being and endorsement of children’s rights

attitudes, there may be differences in the particular ways in which adolescents construe

autonomy support (or democratic family climate) across urban and rural settings in

China. Adolescents may gauge the “proper” amount of autonomy they believe they

should be given in relation to the particular opportunities for autonomy afforded by their

environments. For example, research in Western cultural settings has shown that

adolescents’ expectations for autonomy are influenced by the amount of autonomy that

they believe that their peers have (Daddis, 2011). Moreover, particular dimensions of

autonomy, such as the appropriate level of involvement of children in family decision

making or the types of child expression that may be permissible or reasonably restricted

by parents, may not be perceived in exactly the same way in urban and rural China.

Accordingly, research has found that Chinese rural adolescents sometimes do not endorse

as strongly certain self-determination rights for children (i.e., those relating to their

Page 127: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

117

decision making autonomy) as those in urban setting, or they have later expectations

about why they should receive certain forms of decision making autonomy than their

urban peers (Lahat et al., 2009; To et al., 2017; Zhang & Fuligni, 2006). Thus, future

research is needed to better illuminate the specific types of parenting practices that are

most important to adolescents’ perceptions of autonomy support, and corresponding

psychological well-being and children’s rights attitudes outcomes, in different socio-

cultural settings.

- From Quantitative to Qualitative Studies

The current studies were carried out against the backdrop of changing trends and

social norms in Chinese society. Beliefs and values are continuously shifting, and old

practices that used to be effective for children may no longer be appropriate or beneficial

for their well-being. To truly inform beneficial and effective parenting practices, there is

clearly a need for greater attention and understanding of these issues from children’s

perspectives. Hence, in addition to the use of preset questionnaires that have been derived

from Western research studies and translated into the corresponding languages, future

research should integrate qualitative components and allow the active participation of

children to express their indigenous views and thoughts on parenting and to identify

rights issues that are salient for them (Peterson-Badali & Ruck, 2008; Ruck et al., 2014).

For example, questions soliciting their perspectives on how they would parent in the

future when they themselves become parents would be a particularly interesting topic for

investigation. This would allow for investigations of reflections on current experience

while they also imagine taking on a different role in their response. Would their parenting

practices be different from their own parents, and if so, how? Are there any rights that are

Page 128: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

118

not recognized by their own parents that they would consider important to provide for

their future children? Would they take into account social change in their thinking and

foresee a need for shifts in parenting practices in the future?

This form of qualitative study involving the active engagement of children allows

children's voices to be heard and acknowledges their right to play an active role in

research that affects their lives (Peterson-Badali & Ruck, 2008; Ruck et al., 2014). It also

empowers them as active social agents with the power to make decisions in matters

related to themselves, and to influence their environment and policy about children (Ben-

Arieh, 2005). This will also allow us to gain invaluable insight into children’s

experiences of parenting practices and is imperative in our efforts in improving children’s

well-being. Additionally, parenting programs may also integrate such knowledge and

enhance parents and socialization agents’ awareness of children’s perspectives on

parenting and rights.

Similarly, while we speculate that changes in urban parenting are the byproducts of

adaptions to sociocultural environmental change, it would be important to explore this

hypothesis through direct interviews with parents of adolescents. Having parents from

different origin-settings to reflect on their own socialization experiences and to share

their thoughts on the socialization and socio-cultural factors that may have influenced

their current parenting beliefs and practices would allow us to gain a better understanding

of the Chinese parents’ experiences as they continue to face significant social change

over the next decade.

Conclusion

China has made tremendous gains in improving the condition of its children's

Page 129: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

119

lives in the past decades. In response to the changing sociocultural context in

contemporary China, a new type of citizen with critical thinking, initiative, creativity, and

social and cultural awareness would be needed for the new global economy. Investments

in adapting the socialization climate (e.g., by parent education and teacher training) to

promote an environment that would encourage and guide children to develop capacities

to make their own rational decisions, promote children’s active role in their own agency,

and harness their potential through full participation would be an important avenue to the

realization of healthy development and flourishing in the next generation.

Page 130: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

120

References

Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on behavior. In D. Albarracin,

B.T. Johnson & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), Handbook of Attitudes and Attitude Change:

Basic Principles (pp. 173-221). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum

Ang, R. P. (2006). Effects of parenting style on personal and social variables for Asian

adolescents. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76(4), 503–511.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.76.4.503

Aries, E. (1996). Men and women in interaction: Reconsidering the differences. New

York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Assor, A., Roth, G., & Deci, E. L. (2004). The emotional cost of parents’ conditional

regard: a self-determination theory analysis. Journal of Personality, 72(1), 47–88.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00256.x

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.

Psychological Review, 84(2), 191.https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.84.2.191

Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct.

Child Development, 67(6), 3296–3319. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1996.tb01915.x

Barber, B. K. (2002). Intrusive parenting : how psychological control affects children

and adolescents. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

https://doi.org/10.1037/10422-000

Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child

Development, 37(4), 887-907. https://doi.org/10.2307/1126611

Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology,

Page 131: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

121

4(1), 1-103. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030372

Ben-Arieh, A. (2005). Where are the children? Children’s role in measuring and

monitoring their well-being. Social Indicators Research, 74(3), 573-596.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-004-4645-6

Ben-Arieh, A., & Khoury-Kassabri, M. (2008). Attitudes toward and understanding of

children’s rights among middle school students in Jerusalem: the role of family

values and patterns, nationality, and religion. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,

78(3), 359-368. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014183

Ben-Arieh, A., Khoury-Kassabri, M., & Haj-Yahia, M. M. (2006). Generational, ethnic,

and national differences in attitudes toward the rights of children in Israel and

Palestine. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76(3), 381–388.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.76.3.381

Bogenschneider, K., & Pallock, L. (2008). Responsiveness in parent-adolescent

relationships: Are influences conditional? Does the reporter matter? Journal of

Marriage and Family, 70(4), 1015–1029. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-

3737.2008.00543.x

Bowlby, J. (1988). Attachment, communication, and the therapeutic process. In J.

Bowlby, A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory, (pp. 137-157).

London, UK: Routledge.

Brislin, R. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural

Psychology ,1(3), 185-216. https://doi.org/10.1177/135910457000100301

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). Contexts of child rearing: Problems and prospects. American

Psychologist, 34(10), 844-850.https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.34.10.844

Page 132: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

122

Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human

development. In R. Lerner, Handbook of child psychology: Vol 1, Theoretical

models of human development (pp. 793–828). New York, NY: Wiley.

Cai, F., & Wang, M. (2010). Growth and structural changes in employment in transition

China. Journal of Comparative Economics, 38(1), 71-81.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jce.2009.10.006

Campbell, D. T. (1963). Social attitudes and other acquired behavioral dispositions. In S.

Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 6, pp. 94–172). New York:

McGraw-Hill.

Campbell, J., & Gilmore, L. (2007). Intergenerational continuities and discontinuities in

parenting styles. Australian Journal of Psychology, 59(3), 140–150.

http://doi.org/10.1080/00049530701449471

Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style :

Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training beyond

parental control and authoritarian parenting style : Understanding Chinese parenting

through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65(4), 1111–1119.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00806.x

Chao, R. K. (1996). Chinese and European American mothers’ beliefs about the role of

parenting in children’s school success. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27(4),

403-423. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022196274002

Chao, R.K., & Tseng, V. (2002). Parenting of asians. Handbook of Parenting, 4, 59-93.

Chen, X., Bian, Y., Xin, T., Wang, L., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2010). Perceived social

change and childrearing attitudes in China. European Psychologist, 15(4), 260–270.

Page 133: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

123

http://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000060

Chen, X., & Chen, H. (2010). Children’s socioemotional functioning and adjustment in

the changing Chinese society. In R. K. Silbereisen & Xi. Chen (Eds.), Social change

and human development : concept and results (pp. 209–226). London, England:

Sage. http://doi.org/10.4135/ 9781446252161.n10

Chen, X., Liu, M., Li, B., Cen, G., Chen, H., & Wang, L. (2000). Maternal authoritative

and authoritarian attitudes and mother–child interactions and relationships in urban

China. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(1), 119–126.

http://doi.org/10.1080/016502500383557

Chen, X., Liu, M., & Li, D. (2000). Parental warmth, control, and indulgence and their

relations to adjustment in Chinese children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Family

Psychology, 14(3), 401–419. http://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.14.3.401

Chen, Y., & Chen, H. (2010). Children’s socioemotional functioning and adjustment in

the changing Chinese society. In Social change and human development : concept

and results (pp. 209–226). London, England: Sage Publications.

Cherney, I. D., & Shing, Y. L. (2008). Children’s nurturance and self-determination

rights : A cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 64(4), 835–856.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00591.x

Cheung, C., Pomerantz, E., Wang, M., & Qu, Y. (2016). Controlling and autonomy‐

supportive parenting in the United States and China: Beyond children’s reports.

Child Development, 87(6), 1992-2007. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12567

Chirkov, V. I., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Parent and teacher autonomy-support in Russian

and U.S. adolescents common effects on well-being and academic motivation.

Page 134: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

124

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(5), 618–635.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0022022101032005006

Chirkov, V., Ryan, R. M., Kim, Y., & Kaplan, U. (2003). Differentiating autonomy from

individualism and independence: a self-determination theory perspective on

internalization of cultural orientations and well-being. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 84(1), 97–110. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.97

Chua, A. (2011). Battle hymn of the tiger mother. New York, NY: Penguin Press.

Collins, W. (1990). Parent-child relationships in the transition to adolescence: Continuity

and change in interaction, affect, and cognition. In R. Montemayor, G. Adams, and

T. Gullotta (Eds.), Advances in adolescent development: Volume 2. The transition

from childhood to adolescence. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Conger, R. D., Belsky, J., & Capaldi, D. M. (2009). The intergenerational transmission of

parenting: closing comments for the special section. Developmental Psychology,

45(5), 1276–1283. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0016911

Crockett, L. J., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2000). Negotiating Adolescence in Times of Social

Change. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511600906

Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model.

Psychological Bulletin, 113(3), 487-496. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-

2909.113.3.487

Day, D. M., Peterson-Badali, M., & Ruck, M. D. (2006). The relationship between

maternal attitudes and young people’s attitudes toward children’s rights. Journal of

Adolescence, 29(2), 193–207. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.05.002

Page 135: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

125

Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (2013). The importance of autonomy for development and well-

being. In B. W. Sokol, F. M. E. Grouzet, & U. Muller (Eds.), Self-Regulation and

Autonomy: Social and developmental dimensions of human conduct (pp. 19–46).

New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale.

Journal of Personality, 49(1), 71–75.

https://doi.org/10.1007/springerreference_184631

Elder, G. (1994). Time, human agency, and social change: Perspectives on the life

course. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57(1), 4–15. https://doi.org/10.2307/2786971

Flanagan, C. A. (2000). Negotiating adolescence in times of social change. In L. J.

Crockett & R. K. Silbereisen (Eds.), Negotiating Adolescence in Times of Social

Change (pp. 191–198). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511600906

Fuligni, A. J., & Zhang, W. (2004). Attitudes toward family obligation among

adolescents in contemporary urban and rural China. Child Development, 75(1), 180–

192. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00662.x

Gonzales, N., Cauce, A., & Mason, C. (1996). Interobserver agreement in the assessment

of parental behavior and parent‐adolescent conflict: African American mothers,

daughters, and independent observers. Child Development, 67(4), 1483-1498.

https://doi.org/10.2307/1131713

Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental change: shifting

pathways of human development. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 401–418.

http://doi.org/10.1037/a0014726

Page 136: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

126

Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways

through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 461–490.

http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145221

Greenfield, P. M., Suzuki, L. K., & Rothstein-Fisch, C. (2006). Cultural pathways

through human development. Handbook of Child Psychology, 4, 655–699.

https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0417

Grolnick, W. (2003). The psychology of parental control: How well-meant parenting

backfires. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410606303

Guillen, M. F., & Suarez, S. L. (2005). Explaining the global digital divide: Economic,

political and sociological drivers of cross-national internet Use. Social Forces,

84(2), 681–708. http://doi.org/10.1353/sof.2006.0015

Guthrie, D. (2012). China and globalization: The social, economic and political

transformation of Chinese society. Great Britain, UK: Routledge.

Hasebe, Y., Nucci, L., & Nucci, M. S. (2004). Parental control of the personal domain

and adolescent symptoms of psychopathology: A cross-national study in the United

States and Japan. Child Development, 75(3), 815–828. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-

8624.2004.00708.x

Helwig, C. (2005). Culture and the construction of concepts of personal autonomy and

democratic decision-making. In J. E. Jacobs & P.A. Klaczynski (Eds.), The

development of judgment and decision making in children and adolescents (pp. 181-

212). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Helwig, C., Arnold, M., Tan, D., & Boyd, D. (2003). Chinese adolescents’ reasoning

about democratic and authority‐based decision making in peer, family, and school

Page 137: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

127

contexts. Child Development, 74(3), 783-800. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-

8624.00568

Helwig, C., & Kim, S. (1999). Children’s evaluations of decision‐making procedures in

peer, family, and school contexts. Child Development, 70(2), 502-512.

https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00036

Helwig, C., To, S., Wang, Q., Liu, C., & Yang, S. (2014). Judgments and reasoning about

parental discipline involving induction and psychological control in China and

Canada. Child Development, 85(3), 1150-1167. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12183

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Beyond WEIRD: Towards a broad-

based behavioral science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 111–135.

http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000725

Hermans, H., & Kempen, H. (1998). Moving cultures: The perilous problems of cultural

dichotomies in a globalizing society. American Psychologist, 53(10), 1111-1120.

https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.53.10.1111

Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardif, T. (2002). Socioeconomic status and parenting. In M. H.

Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 231–252). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Hoppe-Graff, S., & Kim, H. (2005). Understanding Rights and Duties in Different

Cultures and Contexts: Observations From German and Korean Adolescents. In N.

J. Finkel & F. M. Moghaddam (Eds.), Human rights and duties: Empirical

contributions and normative commentaries (pp. 49–73). Washington, DC: APA

Press.

Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of

Page 138: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

128

traditional values. American Sociological Review, 65(1), 19-51.

https://doi.org/10.2307/2657288

Jia, Y., Way, N., Ling, G., Yoshikawa, H., Chen, X., Hughes, D., Ke, X., & Lu, Z.

(2009). The influence of student perceptions of school climate on socioemotional

and academic adjustment: a comparison of chinese and american adolescents. Child

Development, 80(5), 1514–1530. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01348.x

Jing, J. (2000). Feeding China’s little emperors : food, children, and social change.

Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Kagitcibasi, C. (1997). Individualism and collectivism. In J. W. Berry, M. H. Segall, & C.

Kagitcibasi (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Vol. 3. Social behavior

and applications (pp. 1–49). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Kagitcibasi, Ç. (1994). A critical appraisal of individualism and collectivism: Toward a

new formulation. . In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S.-C. Choi, & G. Yoon

(Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 52–

65). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kaime, T. (2011). The convention on the rights of the child - A cultural legitimacy

critique. Groningen, The Netherlands: Europa Law Publishing.

Keshavarz, S., & Baharudin, R. (2009). Parenting style in a collectivist culture of

Malaysia. European Journal of Social Sciences, 10(1), 66-73.

Khoury-Kassabri, M., & Ben-Arieh, A. (2009). School climate and children’s views of

their rights: A multi-cultural perspective among Jewish and Arab adolescents.

Children and Youth Services Review, 31I(1), 97-103.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2008.06.002

Page 139: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

129

Kim, U. (1997). Asian collectivism: An indigenous perspective. In H. S. R. Kao & D.

Sinha (Eds.), Asian perspectives on psychology (pp. 147–163). New Delhi, India:

Sage.

Knight, J., & Song, L. (1999). The Rural-Urban Divide: Economic Disparities and

Interactions in China. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Kovacs, M., & Staff, M. (2011). Children’s Depression Inventory 2 (CDI2). North

Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Lahat, A., Helwig, C. C., Yang, S., Tan, D., & Liu, C. (2009). Mainland Chinese

adolescents’ judgments and reasoning about self-determination and nurturance

rights. Social Development, 18(3), 690–710. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-

9507.2008.00507.x

Lamb, M. (2004). The role of the father in child development (3rd ed.). New York, NY:

Wiley.

Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Authoritarian ,

indulgent , and neglectful families patterns of competence and adjustment among

adolescents from authoritative , authoritarian , indulgent , and neglectful families.

Child Development, 62(5), 1049–1065. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131151

Lau, S. (1992). Collectivism’s individualism: Value preference, personal control, and the

desire for freedom among Chinese in Mainland China, Hong Kong, and Singapore.

Personality and Individual Differences, 13(3), 361-366.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(92)90115-6

Lin, L., Huang, C., & Wang, Q. (2015). Parenting in contemporary China: The dynamics

of interdependence and independence. In Parenting across Cultures: A Global

Page 140: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

130

Perspective on Parenting Practices. London, UK: Routledge Press.

Lo, C., Helwig, C. C., Chen, S. X., Ohashi, M. M., & Cheng, C. M. (2011). The

psychology of strengths and weaknesses: Assessing self-enhancing and self-critical

tendencies in Eastern and Western cultures. Self and Identity, 10(2), 203–212.

http://doi.org/10.1080/15298861003751272

Lu, H. J., & Chang, L. (2013). Parenting and socialization of only children in urban

China: An example of authoritative parenting. The Journal of Genetic

Psychology,174(3), 335-343. http://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2012.681325

Ma, L. J. (2002). Urban transformation in China, 1949–2000: A review and research

agenda. Environment and planning A, 34(9), 1545-1569.

https://doi.org/10.1068/a34192

Mackerras, C. (2001). The new Cambridge handbook of contemporary China.

Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition,

emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.98.2.224

Murphy-berman, V., Levesque, H. L., & Berman, J. J. (1996). The convention: On the

rights of the child. The Philippine Journal of Nursing, 60(3), 40.

http://doi.org/10.2307/4065371

Naftali, O. (2014). Children, rights and modernity in China: raising self-governing

citizens. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

Naftali, O. (2016). Children in China. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.

Neff, K., & Helwig, C. (2002). A constructivist approach to understanding the

Page 141: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

131

development of reasoning about rights and authority within cultural contexts.

Cognitive Development, 17(3), 1429-1450. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0885-

2014(02)00126-0

Neter, J., Wasserman, W., & Kutner, M.H. (1990). Applied Linear Statistical Models:

Regression, Analysis of Variance, and Experimental Designs (3rd ed.). Homewood,

IL: Irwin.

Nixon, E., & Halpenny, A. (2010). Children’s perspectives on parenting styles and

discipline: a developmental approach. Dublin: The National Children’s Strategy

Research Services.

Noller, P., Seth-Smith, M., Bouma, R., & Schweitzer, R. (1992). Parent and adolescent

perceptions of family functioning: A comparison of clinic and non-clinic

families. Journal of Adolescence, 15(2), 101-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/0140-

1971(92)90041-3

Nucci, L. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University

Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511605987

Nucci, L., Camino, C., & Sapiro, C. M. (1996). Social class effects on northeastern

Brazilian children’s conceptions of areas of personal choice and social regulation.

Child Development, 67(3), 1223–1242. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131889

Ohannessian, C. M., Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., & von Eye, A. (1995). Discrepancies in

adolescents' and parents' perceptions of family functioning and adolescent emotional

adjustment. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 15(4), 490-516.

Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and

collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses.

Page 142: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

132

Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 3–72. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3

Peerenboom, R. (2002). China’s long march toward rule of law. New York, NY:

Cambridge University Press.

Peterson-Badali, M., Morine, S. L., Ruck, M. D., & Slonim, N. (2004). Predictors of

maternal and early adolescent attitudes toward children's nurturance and self-

determination rights. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 24(2), 159–179.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0272431603262667

Peterson-Badali, M., & Ruck, M. D. (2008). Studying children's perspectives on self-

determination and nurturance rights: issues and challenges. Journal of Social

Issues, 64(4), 749-769. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00587.x

Peterson-Badali, M., Ruck, M., & Bone, J. (2008). Rights conceptions of maltreated

children living in state care. The International Journal of Children’s Rights, 16(1),

99–119. http://doi.org/10.1163/092755608X278902

Peterson-Badali, M., Ruck, M. D., & Ridley, E. (2003). College students’ attitudes

toward children’s nurturance and self-determination rights. Journal of Applied

Social Psychology, 33(4), 730–755. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-

1816.2003.tb01922.x

Pettit, G., Laird, R., Dodge, K., & Bates, J. (2001). Antecedents and behavior‐problem

outcomes of parental monitoring and psychological control in early adolescence.

Child Development, 72(2), 583-598. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00298

Pye, L. (1992). The spirit of Chinese politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Qin, L., Pomerantz, E., & Wang, Q. (2009). Are gains in decision‐making autonomy

during early adolescence beneficial for emotional functioning? The case of the

Page 143: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

133

United States and China. Child Development, 80(6), 1705-1721.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01363.x

Ren, L., & Pope Edwards, C. (2015). Pathways of influence: Chinese parents'

expectations, parenting styles, and child social competence. Early Child

Development and Care, 185(4), 614-630.

https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.944908

Rogers, C., & Wrightsman, L. (1978). Attitudes toward children’s rights: Nurturance or

self-determination? Journal of Social Issues, 34(2), 59-68.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1978.tb01029.x

Ruck, M. D., Abramovitch, R., & Keating, D. P. (1998). Children’s and adolescents’

understanding of rights: balancing nurturance and self-determination. Child

Development, 69(2), 404–417. https://doi.org/10.2307/1132174

Ruck, M. D., Keating, D. P., Abramovitch, R., & Koegl, C. J. (1998). Adolescents’ and

children’s knowledge about rights: some evidence for how young people view rights

in their own lives. Journal of Adolescence, 21(3), 275–289.

http://doi.org/10.1006/jado.1998.0153

Ruck, M. D., Peterson-Badali, M., & Day, D. M. (2002). Adolescents’ and mothers’

understanding of children’s rights in the home. Journal of Research on Adolescence,

12(3), 373–398. http://doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.00038

Ruck, M. D., Tenenbaum, H., & Willenberg, I. (2011). South African mixed-race

children’s and mothers’ judgments and reasoning about children’s nurturance and

self-determination rights. Social Development, 20(3), 517–535.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00607.x

Page 144: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

134

Ruck, M., Peterson-Badali, M., & Helwig, C. (2014). Children’s perspectives on

nurturance and self-determination rights: Implications for development and well-

being. In A. Ben-Arieh, F. Casas, I. Frones, & J. Korbin (Eds.), The handbook of

children’s well being: Theories, methods and policies in global perspective (pp.

2537–2560). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-

481-9063-8_186

Rudy, D., & Grusec, J. E. (2001). Correlates of authoritarian parenting in individualist

and collectivist cultures and implications for understanding the transmission of

values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 202-212. doi:

10.1177/0022022101032002007

Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (2006). Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: does

psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? Journal of Personality, 74(6),

1557-1586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00420.x

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potential: a review of

research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52,

141–166.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2011). A self-determination theory perspective on social,

institutional, cultural, and economic supports for autonomy and their importance for

well-being. In V.I. Chirkov, R.M. Ryan, & K.M. Sheldon (Eds). Human autonomy

in cross-cultural context (pp.45-64). New York: Springer.

https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9667-8_3

Scaramella, L. V., Neppl, T. K., Ontai, L. L., & Conger, R. D. (2008). Consequences of

socioeconomic disadvantage across three generations. Journal of Family

Page 145: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

135

Psychology, 22(5), 725–733. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0013190

Schofield, T. J., Parke, R. D., Coltrane, S., & Weaver, J. M. (2016). Optimal assessment

of parenting, or how I learned to stop worrying and love reporter

disagreement. Journal of Family Psychology, 30(5), 614-624.

Schwarz, J., Barton-Henry, M., & Pruzinsky, T. (1985). Assessing child-rearing

behaviors: A comparison of ratings made by mother, father, child, and sibling on the

CRPBI. Child Development, 56(2), 462-479. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129734

Shek, D. T. (1999). Paternal and maternal influences on the psychological well-being of

Chinese adolescents. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 125(3),

269–296.

Shweder, R. (1999). Why cultural psychology? Ethos, 27(1), 62-73.

Shweder, R., & Bourne, E. (1984). Does the concept of person vary cross-culturally. In

R.A. Schweder & R.A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and

emotion (pp.158-199). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Shweder, R., & Sullivan, M. (1993). Cultural psychology: Who needs it? Annual Review

of Psychology, 44(1), 497-523. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.44.1.497

Smetana, J. (2005). Adolescent-parent conflict: Resistance and subversion as

developmental process. In L. Nucci (Ed.), Conflict, contradiction, and contrarian

elements in moral development and education (pp. 69-91). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Smetana, J. (2011). Adolescents’ social reasoning and relationships with parents:

Conflicts and coordinations within and across domains. In E. Amsel & J. Smetana

(Eds.), Adolescent vulnerabilities and opportunities: Constructivist and

development perspectives (pp.139-158). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Page 146: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

136

https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139042819.009

Smetana, J. G. (2002). Culture, autonomy, and personal jurisdiction in adolescent-parent

relationships. Advances in child development and behavior, 29, 51-87.

https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2407(02)80051-9

Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Antecedents and outcomes of self-

determination in 3 life domains: The role of parents’ and teachers’ autonomy

support. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34(6), 589–604.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-005-8948-y

Sorkhabi, N. (2012). Parent socialization effects in different cultures: Significance of

directive parenting. Psychological reports, 110(3), 854-878.

http://doi.org/10.2466/10.02.17.21.PR0.110.3.854-878

Stearns, P. (2016). Childhood in world history. New York, NY: Routledge.

https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315561363

Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conflict, and harmony in the family relationship. In S.

Feldman & G. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent. (pp. 255-

276). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1994).

Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among adolescents from

authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development,

65(3), 754. doi:10.2307/1131416

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using Multivariate Statistics (4th ed.).

Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Way, N., Hughes, D., Yoshikawa, H., Kalman, R. K., & Niwa,

Page 147: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

137

E. Y. (2008). Parents’ goals for children: The dynamic coexistence of individualism

and collectivism in cultures and individuals: Topic review. Social Development,

17(1), 183–209. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00419.x

To, S., Helwig, C., & Yang, S. (2017). Predictors of children’s rights attitudes and

psychological well-being among rural and urban mainland Chinese adolescents.

Social Development, 26(1), 185-203. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12195

Tobin, J., Wu, D., & Davidson, D. (1989). Preschool in three cultures: China, Japan, and

the United States. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Tönnies, F. (1957). Community and society (Gemeinschaft und Gesellschaft, C. P.

Loomis, Ed. & Trans.). East Lansing: Michigan State University Press. (Original

work published in German, 1887)

Triandis, H. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts.

Psychological Review, 96(3), 506-520. http://doi.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-

295X.96.3.506

Triandis, H. (1994). Culture and social behavior. Trafalgar House Publishing.

Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. Handbook of Child Psychology. III:13.

https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0313

Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: Social development, context, and conflict.

Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511613500

Turiel, E., Killen, M., & Helwig, C. (1987). Morality: Its structure, functions, and

vagaries. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young

children (pp. 155-243). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Vandenhole, W., Desmet, E., Reynaert, D., & Lembrechts, S. (Eds). (2015). Routledge

Page 148: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

138

international handbook of children’s rights studies. London, UK: Routledge.

https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315769530

Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., Petegem, S. Van, & Duriez, B. (2014). Longitudinal

associations between adolescent perceived degree and style of parental prohibition

and internalization and defiance. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 229–236.

http://doi.org/10.1037/a0032972

Vansteenkiste, M., Zhou, M., Lens, W., & Soenens, B. (2005). Experiences of autonomy

and control among Chinese learners: Vitalizing or immobilizing? Journal of

Educational Psychology, 97(3), 468–483. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-

0663.97.3.468

Walker, N., Brooks, C., & Wrightsman, L. (1999). Children’s rights in the United States:

In search of a national policy. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452232911

Wang, Y. C. (2014). In search of the Confucian family: Interviews with parents and their

middle school children in Guangzhou, China. Journal of Adolescent Research, 29,

765-782. doi: 10.1177/0743558414538318

Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E.M., & Chen, H. (2007). The role of parents' control in early

adolescents' psychological functioning: A longitudinal investigation in the United

States and China. Child Development, 78, 1592-1610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.2007.01085.x

Way, N., Okazaki, S., Zhao, J., Kim, J. J., Chen, X., Yoshikawa, H., Jia, Y., & Deng, H.

(2013). Social and emotional parenting: Mothering in a changing Chinese society.

Asian American Journal of Psychology, 4(1), 61–70.

Page 149: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

139

http://doi.org/10.1037/a0031204

Way, N., Reddy, R., & Rhodes, J. (2007). Students’ perceptions of school climate during

the middle school years: Associations with trajectories of psychological and

behavioral adjustment. American Journal of Community, 40(3-4), 194-213.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-007-9143-y

Weiss, L. H., & Schwarz, J. C. (1996). The relationship between parenting types and

older adolescents’ personality, academic achievement, adjustment, and substance

use. Child Development, 67(5), 2101–2114. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1996.tb01846.x

Wen, M., & Lin, D. (2012). Child development in rural China: Children left behind by

their migrant parents and children of nonmigrant families. Child Development,

83(1), 120–136. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01698.x

Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship of verbal and overt

behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 41–78.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1969.tb00619.x

Yang, B., Ollendick, T., Dong, Q., & Xia, Y. (1995). Only children and children with

siblings in the People’s Republic of China: Levels of fear, anxiety, and depression.

Child Development, 66(5), 1301-1311. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131648

Yeung, J. W. K., & Chan, Y.-C. (2015). Parents’ religiosity, family aocialization and the

mental health of children in Hong Kong: Do raters make a difference? Journal of

Family Studies, 22(2), 140-161. http://doi.org/10.1080/13229400.2015.1049195

Zhang, W. & Fuligni, A. J. (2006). Authority, autonomy, and family relationships among

adolescents in urban and rural China. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 527-

Page 150: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

140

537. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00506

Zhang, X., Zheng, X., & Wang, L. (2003). Comparative research on individual modernity

of adolescents between town and countryside in China. Asian Journal of Social

Psychology, 6(1), 61–73. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-839X.t01-1-00010

Page 151: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

141

Appendix A

Perceptions of Parents Scales (POPS)

Autonomy Support Questions about your mother.

1. My mother seems to know how I feel about things.

2. My mother tries to tell me how to run my life.

3. My mother, whenever possible, allows me to choose what to do.

4. My mother listens to my opinion or perspective when I've got a problem.

5. My mother allows me to decide things for myself.

6. My mother insists upon my doing things her way.

7. My mother is usually willing to consider things from my point of view.

8. My mother helps me to choose my own direction.

9. My mother isn't very sensitive to many of my needs.

Questions about your father.

10. My father seems to know how I feel about things.

11. My father tries to tell me how to run my life. (R)

12. My father, whenever possible, allows me to choose what to do.

13. My father listens to my opinion or perspective when I've got a problem.

14. My father allows me to decide things for myself.

15. My father insists upon my doing things his way.

16. My father is usually willing to consider things from my point of view.

17. My father helps me to choose my own direction.

18. My father isn't very sensitive to many of my needs.

Page 152: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

142

Responsiveness

Questions about your mother.

1. My mother finds time to talk with me.

2. My mother accepts me and likes me as I am.

3. My mother doesn't seem to think of me often.

4. My mother clearly conveys her care for me.

5. My mother spends a lot of time with me.

6. My mother makes me feel very special.

7. My mother often seems too busy to attend to me.

8. My mother is often disapproving and unaccepting of me.

9. My mother is not very involved with my concerns.

10. My mother is typically happy to see me.

11. My mother puts time and energy into helping me.

12. My mother seems to be disappointed in me a lot.

Questions about your father.

13. My father finds time to talk with me.

14. My father accepts me and likes me as I am.

15. My father doesn't seem to think of me often.

16. My father clearly conveys his care for me.

17. My father spends a lot of time with me.

18. My father makes me feel very special.

19. My father often seems too busy to attend to me.

20. My father is often disapproving and unaccepting of me.

21. My father is not very involved with my concerns.

Page 153: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

143

22. My father is typically happy to see me.

23. My father puts time and energy into helping me.

24. My father seems to be disappointed in me a lot.

Page 154: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

144

Appendix B

Democratic Environment Questionnaire (DEQ)

Questions about your family.

Freedom of expression/tolerance of dissent 1. It is possible to criticize my parents’ decisions. (“tolerance of dissent” item)

2. There is mutual respect between me and my parents even in areas in which we disagree.

Participation 3. My parents make family decisions without taking my opinion into account

4. In my family, I am given the chance to help make decisions.

Rule of law/ Due process

5. My parents often seem to consider themselves above the rules.

6. My parents will listen to my explanation and offer a fair hearing if I am accused of wrongdoing.

Page 155: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

145

Appendix C

Children’s Rights Attitudes (CRA) questionnaire

Self-determinationRights 1. Children should have the right to decide which recreational activities they will

participate in

2. Decisions about a child's medical treatment should be made by the child, even if the

child's life is at stake

3. Children should have the right to express ideas or opinions in school newspapers,

even if many teachers may not agree with them

4. Children should have to get permission from their parents before taking a job

5. Children should have the right to choose what they wear even if their parents

disapprove

6. Children should have the right to decide which school they will attend

7. Children should have the right to choose which religion they practice, even if it is

different from their parents

8. Parents should be able to enter a child's room without the child's permission

9. Children should have the right to choose their friends

10. Children should have the right to keep their diary private from their parents

11. Parents should be able to keep children from seeing television shows dealing with

content that parents consider inappropriate

12. Parents should be able to read their child’s diary if they have concerns about their

child

13. Children should have the right to keep their room locked even if their parents are

concerned about what might be in it

14. Children should have the right to practice their religion

15. Children should have the right to wear what they want

16. Children should have the right to work if they choose

17. It is up to parents to decide which recreational activities are too rough or dangerous

for their children to play

18. Children should have the right to choose what they watch on television

Page 156: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

146

19. Children should have the right to express their ideas and opinions

20. Decisions about a child's routine medical treatment, e.g., getting a needle, should be

made by the child

21. Children should have the right to choose which school to attend, even if their parents

disagree

22. Children should have the right to choose their friends even if their parents don’t

approve

Nurturance Rights 1. Parents should be required to take their children for counselling when they are

having emotional problems at school

2. Money from taxes should be used to ensure that children receive quality child care

3. Even if parents are extremely busy, they should always make themselves available to

talk with their child if the child has a problem

4. Children should have the right to adequate clothing

5. Children should have the right to a quality education

6. Children should have the right to receive adequate dental care

7. Children should have the right to quality child care

8. Children should have the right to an adequate breakfast

9. Adequate clothing for children may be a goal of our society, but it should not be a

right for every child

10. Children should have the right to an adequate home environment

11. Dental care should be provided to children only when their parents can pay for it

12. Money from taxes should be spent to ensure that all children receive a quality

education whether they are rich or poor

13. Children should have the right to receive medical treatment even if their parents

forbid it on religious grounds

14. The government should be required to help parents provide an adequate home

environment for children

15. Children should have the right to receive medical treatment when necessary

16. Schools should be required to provide breakfasts for those children who don't get

adequate breakfasts at home

Page 157: AUTONOMY, RIGHTS, AND PARENTING IN CHANGING CHINESE ... · CONTEXTS Kim Lun Sharon To Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto Abstract Drawing

147

17. Children should have the right to have their emotional needs taken care of by their

parents

18. Children should have the right to counselling when they are having emotional

problems at school