berk - macro-micro relationships in durkheim’s analysis of egoistic suicide

24
PDFlib PLOP: PDF Linearization, Optimization, Protection Page inserted by evaluation version www.pdflib.com – [email protected]

Upload: pete-sampras

Post on 04-Dec-2015

222 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

DESCRIPTION

Durkheim, Suicidio, Micro-Macro, Sociología

TRANSCRIPT

PDFlib PLOP: PDF Linearization, Optimization, Protection

Page inserted by evaluation versionwww.pdflib.com – [email protected]

Macro-Micro Relationships in Durkheim’s Analysis of Egoistic Suicide*

BERNARD B. BERK

California State University, Los Angeles

Contemporary theory is increasingly concerned with macro-micro integration. An

attempt is made to integrate these levels of analysis in Durkheim’s theory of egoistic

suicide. Does Durkheim’s theory, which is a social system analysis designed to explain

differences in suicide rates between groups, have micro implications for specifying

which particular individuals within the group will take their lives? In attempting to

answer this question by exploring the causal linkages between integration and suicide,

Durkheim’s theory of egoistic suicide was revealed not to be a singular theory but

rather contained several different explanations. The numerous interpretations have

resulted from his incompletely specified, inconsistent, unsystematized, and inade-

quately tested theory. These ambiguities also account for the historically inconsistent

research findings that have limited sociologists’ ability to advance beyond Durkheim’s

century-old formulations. Further areas for new research and illustrative hypothesis

are suggested.

Durkheim’s theory of egoistic suicide is a classic example of a social system analysisthat is designed to explain differences in rates between groups.1 The question exam-ined here is: ‘‘Does his theory also have implications for specifying which particularindividuals within the group will take their lives?’’ Exploring the macro-microrelationships revealed numerous explanations embedded in the theory.

Although Durkheim’s work on egoistic suicide is viewed as a singular theory, closeranalysis revealed several explanations contained within it, each about different socialprocesses and intervening variables between integration and suicide. Analysis hasrevealed an incompletely specified theory, which contained alternative interveningprocesses or that have evolved out of an associated body of research. This may partlyaccount for some of the inconsistent research findings of subsequent studies.

The importance and centrality of Durkheim’s Suicide needs little documentation.Scholars describe it as: ‘‘one of the greatest pieces of sociological research conductedby anyone’’ (Merton 1968:63); ‘‘the cornerstone of the whole approach taken by mostsociologists in the twentieth century’’ (Douglas 1967:xiii); and ‘‘Suicide remains amonument’’ (Pope 1976:204). Although Durkheim’s work remains unsurpassed,2 an

Address correspondence to: Bernard B. Berk, Sociology Department, California State University, LosAngeles, 5151 State University Drive, Los Angeles, CA 90032. Tel.: þ1 818 766 9887; Fax: þ1 323 343 5155;E-mail: [email protected]. *The author acknowledges the helpful suggestions of Thomas J. Scheff.

1The other types of suicide, altruistic, anomic, and fatalistic, are not specifically dealt with in this article,which is also limited by the omission of literature in other languages. Durkheim believed egoistic was themost widespread type of suicide (1951:356). Johnson (1965) and Pope (1976) have argued that egoisticsuicide is not only basic to Durkheim’s theory, but that the other causes can be reduced to egoism—aposition not shared by this author (see also Heyns 1975). Breault and Barkey (1982) state egoistic suicide hasreceived the most attention, and the other types were added only for completeness sake (Lester 1994:13).

2Subsequent research has pursued empirical relationships but has been, with few exceptions, woefullyinadequate in theoretical development.

Sociological Theory 24:1 March 2006# American Sociological Association. 1307 NewYork Avenue NW,Washington, DC 20005-4701

increasing number of scholars have revealed ambiguities, conflicting interpretations,and methodological shortcomings (Pickering and Walford 2000; Lester 1994;Pescosolido and Georgianna 1989; Stack 1980; Pope 1976; Douglas 1967; Johnson1965).

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS

Relationships between macro and micro analysis are explored to illuminate ambigu-ities in Durkheim’s theory. Gibbs (1968:8) states that two questions dominate researchon suicide: (a) Why rates of suicide differ between groups? and (b) Why one individualrather than another commits suicide? He posits that separate explanations arerequired to answer each of these questions, presumably on the basis that macro andmicro explanations of suicide are unrelated.

Durkheim’s work is a classic example of a social system analysis of behaviorordinarily regarded as a highly personal act. Although his theory was designed toexplain differences in suicide rates between groups, the theory might also have con-sequences for specifying which individuals within the group will commit suicide. If thetheory does possess both macro and micro implications, then several importantquestions are generated. First, does macro theory have consequences for microbehavior?3 Specifically, does Durkheim’s theory have implications for predictingindividual instances of suicide even if he did not intend to explain those aspects ofsuicide?4 Or, conversely, must separate explanations be proposed to explain micro andmacro phenomena, as Gibbs suggests?

Durkheim partially developed a micro analysis within his macro theory. The posi-tion set forth here is that macro theories can often best be tested by their microimplications. Only by identifying the underlying social mechanisms through which thevariables accomplish their effects can the questions of ‘‘how’’ and ‘‘why’’ suicideoccurs be answered. By investigating the causal nexus between integration and suicide,the meanings of Durkheim’s theory, its ambiguities, its incomplete specifications, andthe alternative explanations contained within the theory can be brought into focus.

Disagreement exists over the theory’s ability to predict individual instances ofsuicide. Robinson’s (1950) ‘‘ecological fallacy’’ occurs as a result of inferring relationsbetween variables for individuals from relationships between variables obtained fromthe aggregate.5 Some critics have leveled such accusations against Durkheim on theassumption that his theory was concerned with explaining the correlates of individualinstances of suicide. The criticism remains valid, whatever position is taken withrespect to the controversy of the applicability of the theory to individual instances,when leveled at Durkheim’s arguments in discrediting insanity as a cause of suicide

3Inkeles asserts that adequate sociological analysis is either impossible or severely limited without theexplicit use of psychological theory (1959:250). This analysis seeks to examine the extent to which asociological theory may have implications for the prediction of individual’s behavior. It is possible thatcertain laws describe the behavior of collectivities and that special laws are required to describe who in thecollectivity will be likely to be influenced. Blau (1960) set forth criteria to differentiate ‘‘structural’’ or‘‘group’’ effects from those influences that result from ‘‘individual characteristics’’ in the analysis of socialbehavior. Breault also makes a case for incorporating psychological variables into sociological theories ofintegration (Lester 1994:24).

4Both Giddens (1972) and Turner (1990) assert Durkheim’s works were evolving toward an integration ofmicro with macro analysis. Turner attempted to trace interconnections between macro theories of socialorganization and micro analysis of interaction and ritual.

5Hammond (1973) attempts to specify the conditions under which it is possible to infer correlationsbetween variables for individuals from aggregate data. See also Selvin (1965) and Duncan and Davis (1953).Bogue and Bogue (1976) have also argued that when rigorous standards are followed, aggregate analysescan provide reliable predictions about an individual’s behavior.

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 59

(1951:ch. 1). For interpretations that suggest individual instances of suicide can bespecified, see Douglas (1967:39) as well as forms of social causes (1951:277–94),although Durkheim is careful to point out that a particular suicide can reflectmixed causes and will also be stamped by the unique character of the person.Others have taken the position similar to Inkeles (1959:225) that Durkheim’s intentwas to explain suicide only sociologically. Johnson argues that Durkheim ‘‘intends histheory to explain variation among social environments in the incidence of suicide, notthe suicides of particular individuals’’ (1965:876). However, he continues: ‘‘The ques-tion of Durkheim’s unit of analysis occasionally troubles readers of Suicide. Onesometimes wonders whether it is societies and groups, or social conditions (forexamples, widowhood or poverty) or perhaps even individuals. The feeling thatimplicitly Durkheim’s unit of analysis is the individual arises especially in his discus-sion of ‘Individual Forms of Different Types of Suicide’’’ (Book II, ch. 6). Othersargue that Durkheim’s theory is purely sociological, oriented toward explainingdifferences in rates between groups, and consequently has no relevance for predictingindividual instances of suicide. Gibbs (1968:9), for example, argues that ‘‘urbaniza-tion’’ is a group property and therefore has no application to the individual level ofanalysis. In short, does Durkheim’s theory have implications for predicting individualbehavior whether he intended them or not? Does the hypothesis that suicide varieswith the degree of integration in a group also imply that those persons who are leastintegrated are also the ones most likely to commit suicide? The response is thatdissimilar causal nexi involve different explanations of suicide and thus providedifferent answers to this question.

DURKHEIM’S SUICIDE

Durkheim regarded the suicide rate as a ‘‘social fact’’ that could be investigated apartfrom the individual acts that comprised the overall rate. The total volume of suicidesin a society was a fact to be accounted for and reflected the magnitude and characterof social forces. He hoped to establish empirical support for the conclusion that thecharacter of society had a demonstrable effect on even highly personal actions, whichwould demonstrate that individuals were influenced by a collective morality.

Durkheim set forth three objectives for Suicide: (a) to identify the nature of socialcauses; (b) to show how these causes produce effects; and (c) the relation of socialcauses to the individual’s reactions as associated with suicide (1951:52). Both (b) and(c) illustrate Durkheim’s belief that interrelationships between social levels of analysisand individual reactions could be identified.

Durkheim’s primary concern throughout was the basis of social cohesion and notsuicide per se. Durkheim initially viewed suicide as a manifestation of the lack ofsocial cohesion and the suicide rate a convenient index of weak social bonds.However, his analysis of altruistic suicide questioned the notion because suicidecould also result from strong social bonds in a group. Whether suicide is a manifesta-tion of weak or strong bonds must be determined by the context and social meaningof the act. An important question is whether Durkheim shifted his focus frominvestigating the sources of cohesion, viewing suicide as an index of the lack ofcohesion, to explaining variations in suicide rates? Did he lose sight of his originalaim, or change it during the writing of Suicide, or were both concerns pursued in thework? Subsequent research has been oriented toward accounting for the variance insuicide rates rather than testing Durkheim’s theory of social cohesion. Furthermore,

60 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

Durkheim never explicated the concept of social bonds although both shared beliefsand social relationships have been examined in the literature as cohesive forces.

Suicide therefore was a poor index of cohesion because it could reflect strong aswell as weak social bonds. In addition, the official statistics upon which his analysisand subsequent conclusions rested were flawed.6

Scholars have also criticized studies that failed to explore the most powerful sourcesof variance in suicide rates, such as gender or age, including variables that may havelittle relevance to cohesion, because they view the point of Durkheim’s study toaccount for the variation in suicide rates. If this is how his work is to be judged, heaccounted for very little of the variation in suicide rates. But Durkheim’s focus onintegration and regulation dealt primarily with the social control over behavior andaspirations that kept suicide in check, thus putting his major emphasis on the impactof social control on suicide rather than accounting for all the variation in suicide rates.

If examination of cohesive forces was the focus of Durkheim’s work, then otherindices of cohesion such as the permanence of bonds could have been examined inaddition to or in place of suicide. Studies of durability of marriage bonds, linked tohomogamy, religious orthodoxy, and role complementarity, are similar to Durkheim’sconclusions on suicide in that they focus on the role of shared values and socialregulation in creating cohesion in marital bonds. Other studies of marital stabilitylinking it to need-complementarity, communication, and satisfaction in the relation-ship focus on the nature of the marital relationship itself as a cohesive force thatcontributes to the strength of social bonds. Studies of longevity of friendship ties,organizational turnover, geographical mobility, and so on could also shed light oncohesion. The existence, strength, and nature of the bond (intimate versus distant)could be explored to shed light on cohesive forces in groups. Scheff and Rettinger(1991) have explored the role of emotions in creating social bonds and cohesion.

Durkheim’s acknowledgment of flaws in the suicide statistics should have forcedhim to select other indices of integration than suicide and kept him on track with hisprimary interest on the nature of social cohesion.

JUSTIFICATIONS FOR BELIEVING SUICIDE SOCIALLY CAUSED

Durkheim argued that regularities in suicide rates were evidence that suicide wassocially caused. Statistics indicated: (a) each society had a stable rate of suicide fromyear to year and therefore it could not just be a property of the particular individualscontained within society at any specific point; (b) each society had its own character-istic rate of suicide; (c) each society had its own rate of acceleration; (d) fluctuation inrates was concomitant with transformations in social organization or crises; (e)

6Official statistics underrepresent the actual rate of suicide, as many are never detected and others arehidden to avoid shame, which may lead to systematic bias. Furthermore, officials who create the statisticshave no common agreement on the definition of suicide; certainly, it is not the same definition employed byDurkheim (Gibbs 1968:13). Officials also have no clear method for determining whether a suicide hasoccurred. Suicide rates are thus dependent on the actions of the officials who create them, and as Douglas(1967:152–231) has shown, secular officials create different statistics than religious functionaries.Pescosolido and Mendelson (1986) argue that this has little impact on the relationships between sociologicalvariables. Definitions and determinations vary by district. Yet despite these problems, Durkheim becamemore concerned with explaining variations in suicide rates than with testing hypotheses about socialcohesion. Pope (1976) not only questions the validity of the facts upon which the theory rests but assertsthat Durkheim selectively presented only those facts that agreed with his hypotheses, incorrectly interpretedthe statistics, switched definitions to fit his preconceptions, and failed to control for relevant variables.Subsequent research (Simpson and Conklin 1989; Breault and Barkey 1982; Danigelies and Pope 1979;Stack 1978, 1980) has shown inconsistent support for Durkheim’s conclusions.

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 61

societies at similar levels of civilization exhibited similar suicide rates; and (f) sub-groups within a society had their own characteristic rates (1951:Book III, ch. 1).

He assumed that if suicides were primarily the result of independent individualactions, there would be considerable irregularity in acts of suicide and, consequently,in the suicide rates of a particular society. He concluded, therefore, that some extra-individual forces were responsible for the regularities in the suicide rates. He postu-lated that at each moment of its history, each society had a definite characteristicaptitude for suicide.

Believing he had established suicide rates were a property of the society, andprovided a theory to account for variation in the suicide rates between groups,Durkheim shifted his analysis to identifying the specific factors in society that con-tributed to the overall suicide rate.7

IDENTIFICATION OF ONE SOCIAL CAUSE: EGOISM

Egoistic Suicide

Durkheim sought to identify the types of suicide based on the similarity of theircauses. ‘‘Egoistic’’ suicides resulted from the weakening of the group’s control over theindividual’s behavior. ‘‘The more weakened the groups bond . . . (the more he/she)depends only on himself/herself and recognizes no other rules of conduct than what arefounded on self-interest . . . egoism (is when) the individual ego asserts itself to excess inthe face of the social ego and at its expense, we may call . . . egoistic the special type ofsuicide springing from excessive individualism’’ (1951:209) (italics added).

Durkheim relied on earlier studies to identify regularities in the suicide rates. Onlyregularities would reflect the social forces at work and allow him to separate socialcauses from idiosyncratic suicidal acts. After exploring differences in suicide ratesbetween different religious confessions, marital conditions, and states of the polity,Durkheim concluded that the differences could be explained by the degree of integra-tion of the various groups. Low levels of integration were associated with high rates of‘‘egoistic’’ suicide. The social law was accordingly stated: ‘‘suicide varies inversely withthe degree of integration of the group’’ (1951:209).

Definition of Integration

A major difficulty with the theory is that Durkheim never clearly and explicitlydefined ‘‘integration.’’ Not only did he fail to define integration conceptually, but healso never specified any precise operations to measure this aspect of group life.8

Therefore, his conclusions in regard to the relationship between integration andsuicide are unsupported—lacking any empirical measurement of the level of integra-tion of religious confessions, family organization, and political states.

This shortcoming led to different interpretations of ‘‘integration’’ and consequentlyto considerable differences among scholars testing his theory. ‘‘Integration’’ has beenvariously used to refer to shared beliefs, social interaction, social relationships, socialorganization, feelings of cohesiveness, the vitality of society, and to the balance ofegoistic and altruistic forces within a group. Thus, Durkheim’s ambiguous and

7Douglas asserts ‘‘the only disagreement between Durkheim and his predecessors was over the question ofjust how the moral state of society was related to individual actions of suicide’’ (1967:16). See also Heyns(1975).

8Gibbs and Martin (1958, 1964). See also Stack (1983).

62 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

sometimes conflicting use has led to a number of different referents for integration inthe literature.

‘‘Integration’’ as ‘‘Shared Beliefs and Practices.’’ Integration here refers to the systemof norms, beliefs, and customs of the group—that is, to its culture. In his analysis ofdifferences in suicide rates between religious confessions, Durkheim (1951) stressedthe system of shared beliefs and practices as central:

The only essential difference between Catholicism and Protestantism is the latterpermits free inquiry to far greater degree than the first (1951:157). Protestantism. . . has fewer common practices . . . the more extensive is the credo, the more unifiedand strong is the society. (1951:159, italics added)

A religious society . . . (attaches) them completely to an identical body of doctrine. . . in proportion as the body is extensive and firm . . . Judaism . . . consists (in)practices minutely governing all details of the individuals’ life and leaving no roomfor individual judgment. (1951:160)

. . . religion protects man . . . because it is a society. What constitutes this society isthe existence of a certain number of beliefs and practices common to all thefaithful, traditional and thus obligatory. The more numerous and strong thesecollective states of the mind are, the stronger the integration of the religiouscommunity, and also the greater its preservative value. (1951:170)

Similarly, Durkheim asserts:‘‘the suicidal tendency in educated circles is due . . . tothe weakening of traditional beliefs and to the state of moral individualism from this’’(1951:168).

Durkheim also examined the impact of the family, particularly the consanguinealunit, which like religious society has a preservative effect. Specifically with respect tothe issue of density of the family, he argued:

The number of children may be of no significance if they do not actually andcontinually share in group life (1951:201) . . . the density of a group cannot sinkwithout its vitality diminishing. Where collective sentiments are strong, it is becausethe force with which they affect each individual conscience is echoed in all othersand reciprocally. The intensity they attain therefore depends on the number ofconsciences which react to them in common (1951:201–02). Consequently, in afamily of small numbers, common sentiments . . . cannot be very intense, for thereare not enough consciences in which they can be represented and reinforced bysharing them. No powerful tradition can be formed there as united members of asingle group . . . Small families are also inevitably short-lived and without durationno society can be stable. Not only are collective states weak . . ., but they cannot benumerous; for their number depends on the active interchange . . . In a sufficientlydense society, this circulation is uninterrupted; for some social units are always incontact . . . Likewise when the family is small, few relatives are ever together; sothat domestic life languishes. (1951:202)

But for a group to be said to have less common life than another means that it isless powerfully integrated; for the state of integration of a social aggregate canonly reflect the intensity of collective life circulating in it (1951:202).

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 63

Durkheim also explored the impact of crises and wars that ‘‘rouse collective sentiments,stimulate . . . patriotism, political and national faith, alike, concentrating activity toward asingle end, at least temporarily cause a stronger integration of society’’ (1951:208).

In Durkheim’s analysis of religion, education, family, and the polity, the common-ality of beliefs and practices is stressed as a manifestation of ‘‘integration.’’ Hisanalysis follows from his earlier conception of ‘‘mechanical solidarity’’ where cohesionresults from sharing common norms, values, beliefs, and rituals. The larger the scopeof these common values or beliefs, the more powerful the force exerted to bindindividuals to one another and to the group. By contrast, the more a culture permitsindividual judgment, the less the force exerted by the system to dominate the lives ofits members, and the fewer the strings that tie individuals to the group, and subsequentlyto life itself. Newcomb’s (1953) ABX theory describes the strengths of social ties at theindividual level resulting from similar attitudes or values. Durkheim never explicates thenature of social bonds, much less measures cohesion. He uses suicide as an example of aweakened social bond, that formed the basis of his interest in suicide. See Scheff (1990,1997) for a serious attempt to examine social bonds and the role of micro processes increating social cohesion. Turner (1990) and Collins (1988:ch.6, 2004) also examine therole of micro processes in creating cohesion. Homans focuses on the frequency ofinteraction leading to more positive sentiment and presumably closer bonds.

Durkheim identifies common beliefs and practices as the essential aspect of integra-tion. The more they pervade various aspects of the individual’s life, and the greater theextent to which they regulate behavior, the greater the integration of the group.Adherence to norms is the hallmark of a strongly integrated society, whereas individualreflection or discussion with respect to action characterizes a loosely integrated group.In this normative sense, integration refers to the group’s culture; it is indicated by theextent to which the culture is: shared, pervasive, strongly held, and a guide for behavior.Durkheim does not focus on the content of norms, just that they are shared and regulatebehavior. Presumably, any shared norms cause a tightening of social bonds.

‘‘Integration’’ as ‘‘Social Interaction.’’ When Durkheim discusses integration as it isrelated to family size, he focuses on social interaction as a key variable. He states thatthe intensity and vitality of a family is a function of increased interaction among themembers—an augmentation associated with greater family size and increased socialdensity. Social interaction reestablishes the sense of the collectivity and its authority,thereby intensifying common understandings and tightening social bonds. Alongthese lines, Gibbs and Martin (1958, 1964) have interpreted ‘‘integration’’ to refer toa quality of social relationships. They have reinterpreted Durkheim’s hypothesis intheir own terms: ‘‘the suicide rate of a population varies inversely with the stabilityand durability of social relationships within that population’’ (1958:141). Althoughother factors related to social interaction, such as the volume, frequency, or intimacyof the interaction, could also have been considered, Gibbs and Martin did not addressthem.

However, Durkheim asserts that interaction was important because it increasedcommon sentiments. Interaction, per se, was not part of the definition of integration. Inhis discussion of family integration, interaction is important only to the extent itintensifies the common sentiments of the group. ‘‘Consequently in a family of smallnumbers common sentiments cannot be very intense, for there are not consciences inwhich they can be represented and re-enforced by sharing them’’ (1951:202).Throughout his analysis, Durkheim continually refers to shared beliefs and practicesas the central factor of integration.

64 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

A justification for Gibbs and Martin’s interpretation of integration exists whenDurkheim discusses the role of ‘‘mutual support’’ as a key element in suicide(1951:210). At the support level, social interaction is crucial to that analysis.9

However, throughout Durkheim’s analysis, social relationships take on a secondaryrole. Even when discussing the restraining aspects of marriage in suicide, he empha-sizes the importance of the institutional features of marriage and the social authorityinvested in the marital union as primary rather than the interpersonal relationshipbetween the husband and the wife. What actually matters in fact is not only thatregulation should exist, but that it should be accepted by the conscience. Otherwise,because this regulation no longer has moral authority and continues only through theforce of inertia, it can no longer play any useful role. It chafes without accomplishingmuch (1951:272). Durkheim continually emphasized the primacy of culture oversocial relationships or social interaction in his analyses.

The conflict in Gibbs and Martin’s interpretation results from Durkheim’s lack ofclarity in distinguishing defining characteristics of ‘‘integration’’ from factors thatincrease integration. Durkheim obscures matters further by failing to distinguishculture from social organization or interaction by using the concept of ‘‘integration’’to refer to both aspects of group life.10

Pescosolido and Georgianna (1989) in a thoughtful empirically-based reformulationof Durkheim’s theory of the role of religion in suicide also focus on social relation-ships as the basis for both integration and regulation. They state that ‘‘moral rulesmust be backed by a cohesive community to endure . . . We do not dismiss theimportance of beliefs nor claim a causal ordering between beliefs and networks’’(1989:40). However, they do not determine the relative contribution of social tiesversus shared beliefs. Pescosolido and Georgianna (1989) supplant Durkheim’s for-mulations with network analysis and replace the concept of society with networks.Durkheim’s emphasis was on the reality of society as sui generis and emergent, and thepowerful force of the collective conscience and culture. The role of social interactionwas augmentary rather than a primary determinant of suicide. Pescosolido doessuggest that continued interaction is essential to render beliefs strong and viable andthat cultural configurations must be continually sustained and renewed by socialinteraction, but places the primary emphasis on social relationships. Stack’s (1980)finding that divorce was more important than religion, Travis’s (1990) finding thatsocial isolation was more predictive than social disorganization, and Giddens’s, (1972)association of social isolation, depression, and suicide all support a network ratherthan a cultural definition of integration.

It is a question of whether ambiguity in Durkheim’s theory created these incon-sistencies or if they represent an alternative explanation. Research contrasting therelative contribution of social control (the degree beliefs are shared and regulatebehavior) versus social networks (interaction and relationships) must be undertaken.Pescosolido and Georgianna, outside of church attendance, do not measure therichness of the social networks and infer them only indirectly; in the same fashion,Durkheim presents no empirical data that Catholics have more similar beliefs thanProtestants. Durkheim was aware of the contexts of religious organizations andexamined variance within denominations by comparing the Church of England,organizationally similar to the Catholic Church, with other Protestant denominations,

9Turner also identifies social interaction as a factor creating social cohesion (1990:1097). Breault asserts‘‘Durkheim plainly means the degree to which people are attached, bonded, or connected to each other.Durkheim is talking about social and emotional ties and the amount of intensity of such bonds’’ (1994:13).

10Pope asserts Durkheim also fails to distinguish belief from actions (1967:15). See also Bellah (1959:460).

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 65

and examining the minority status of a religious group in different societal contexts.Pescosolido’s work points to the importance and interrelatedness of both macrostructures and micro processes in the analysis of suicide. Mutual support performsintegrative functions for social structures. Social relationships, they assert, also act asrestraints (in anomic suicides). However, Durkheim focused solely on the ‘‘restraint ofaspirations’’ from normative control rather than the restraints implicit in socialrelationships as the primary determinant of anomic suicide. Henry and Short’s(1954) analysis of suicide also focused on the strength of external control throughrelationships rather than by norms. There are real differences between Durkheim’sand Pescosolido’s theories that need to be empirically examined. Micro analysiswould also reveal the underlying dynamics and has implications for their macrodifferences.

Landecker (1950) identified several types of integration and distinguished ‘‘norma-tive’’ (i.e., cultural) from ‘‘communicative’’ (social) integration, a distinctionDurkheim failed to recognize. Douglas noted a similar confusion in Durkheim’sanalysis of the primacy of ‘‘internal’’ versus ‘‘external’’ causes of social behavior(1967:47–51).

Durkheim’s lack of clarity in defining the concept, his failure to operationalize andundertake measurement of integration, his confusion of definitional components withboth the causes and consequence of integration, and his use of the same term to referto divergent aspects of group life have all contributed to the confusion surroundingthis concept and his theory.

‘‘Integration’’ as ‘‘A Balance of Opposing Forces.’’ Douglas (1967) similarly arguesthat Durkheim did not use ‘‘integration’’ consistently; in the early chapters it referredto ‘‘shared meanings’’ but in the later chapters to ‘‘an equilibrium of opposing forces(egoism and altruism).’’ Douglas identifies three possible interpretations ofintegration: (a) the number of shared meanings; (b) the dimension of egoism–altruism, i.e., of noninvolvement or involvement in society; and (c) the equilibriumof the two opposing forces of egoism and altruism (1967:54). Interpretation (a) impliesany shared meaning, inasmuch as it constitutes a form of social regulation, can act torestrain suicide, which he rejects in light of the larger meaning of Durkheim’s work.(b) Egoism or altruism refers to specific sets of moral beliefs that foster aloofness,involvement, or submission to society, which he also rejects as he regards egoism oraltruism as orientations to the primary social (moral) meanings rather than to specificsocial meanings as Durkheim ignored the content of the moral beliefs as a factorcontributing to integration. The ‘‘cult of individualism’’ could be an exception toDouglas’s analysis, and Durkheim linked the content of norms to altruistic suicideand identified specific norms requiring suicide. Also see Pope (1976:34–38). In egoisticsuicide, noninvolvement in group life emerges as a consequence of the lack of sharedbeliefs rather than from specific norms. Societies also form collective judgments aboutthe value of life or individualism, but, according to Durkheim, these wereepiphenomenal and evolved as a consequence of the lack of shared beliefs (lowintegration). Once in existence, however, such collective representations canreinforce individual feelings of meaninglessness, thereby intensifying suicidalimpulses in individuals, but have only a secondary reinforcing effect on decreasinginvolvement in society.

These collective representations, when out of balance, exert a force upon theindividual that produces individual states (such as melancholy) which, in turn,

66 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

cause suicide if individual representations are not strong enough to contradict theforce of collective representation. (1967:54)

In his Third interpretation, (c), Douglas asserts that the larger meaning ofDurkheim’s formulation should be extended to apply to the equilibrium of the oppos-ing forces of anomie-fatalism, as well as to the balance of the two opposing dimensionsof egoism-altruism and anomie-fatalism to achieve an overall equilibrium in society.Egoism and altruism are not specific sets of morals, but orientations toward moralmeanings that constitute society, and represent the general orientations of submissive-ness, aloofness, and rebelliousness—all of which are necessary for adequate function-ing of any group. ‘‘But an imbalance or upset in the equilibrium between the three leadsto certain individual states in a certain proportion of the individuals in the group andthence to a given suicide rate. Each force when not restrained by the other forces leadsto suicidogenic forces within individuals’’ (Douglas 1967:53).

Durkheim may have changed his conception of the causes of suicide as Parsons(1949) contends, or Douglas’s attempt to systematize the theory may have bent themeaning of integration, cohesion, and strength of social bonds to an unrecognizableform (a balance in the society of opposing forces).

There are problems with Douglas’s interpretations. (a) If integration refers to a‘‘balance’’ between two opposing forces, how could an inverse relationship betweensuicide and integration exist as Durkheim postulates? (b) A way of unifying egoismand altruism is to posit a ‘‘U’’-shaped relationship between integration and suicidewhere both high and low degrees of integration are associated with high rates ofsuicide and an optimum balance between them minimizes suicide rates. Although thisinterpretation is commonly found in the literature (Lester 1992:388; Pescosolido andGeorgianna 1989:34, 45; Breault and Barkey 1982; Pope 1976:14, 57; Heyns 1975;Douglas 1967), it is not in accord with Durkheim’s theory of altruistic suicide.

High levels of integration do not automatically produce high rates of suicide.Altruistic suicide occurs in highly integrated societies only when there are specificnorms requiring individuals to take their lives. Highly integrated societies that did notmake suicide obligatory were not necessarily characterized by high rates of suicide. Incontrast, low degrees of integration always generated high rates of suicide. This is onereason for the inconsistent findings with respect to religion and suicide rates. Findingson highly integrated religious groups and suicide are inconsistent and would only beexpected to be high among religious groups that required such sacrifices or where thelikelihood of group condemnation from failure or shame was also high. Durkheim’sanalysis of altruistic suicide was also more directed toward identifying the differentforms, processes, and causes of suicide than in predicting actual suicide rates.However, his comparison of suicides in the army with those among the civilianpopulation clouds the issue. Nonetheless, a ‘‘U’’-shaped function or equilibriumconception is not in accord with the theory. Lester also suggested that little attentionhas been given to whether particular measures of integration and regulation are valid(1989:237). The link between the empirical variables employed in specific studies andthe theoretical concept of integration is often tenuous.

(c) Douglas’s interpretations rest on the belief that egoism-altruism and anomie-fatalism are interrelated and act as a force upon each other, and integration refers to abalance between the two dimensions. This would imply that a tightly knit society thatrequires suicide is somehow not integrated, contrary to Durkheim’s conclusion.Durkheim viewed these dimensions as unrelated: egoism-altruism refers to the com-monality of social meanings and the degree to which they regulate conduct, whereas

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 67

anomie-fatalism refers to the degree to which norms regulate aspirations—dimensionsthat are independent of each other. Failing an interdependence of the dimensions,there is little justification for Douglas extending the integration concept to refer to anoverall equilibrium. There is some limited justification for Douglas’s position in thelater part of Suicide where Durkheim states: ‘‘No moral idea exists which does notcombine in proportions varying with the society involved, egoism altruism, and acertain anomie. For social life assume both that the individual has a certain person-ality, that he is ready to surrender it if the community requires, and finally, that he isto a certain degree sensitive to ideas of progress. This is why there are no peopleamong whom these three currents of opinion do not coexist, bending men’s inclina-tions in three different and even opposing directions. Where they offset one another,the moral agent is in a state of equilibrium which shelters him against any thought ofsuicide. But let one of them exceed a certain strength to the detriment of the others,and as it becomes individualized, it also becomes suicidogenic, for the reasonsassigned’’ (1951:321). Durkheim distinguished ‘‘integration’’ from ‘‘equilibrium’’—the former referring to the cohesive aspects of society resulting from shared socialmeanings and the latter to a state of organization in society that minimizes suicide.When Douglas equates these two concepts, he creates further confusion as well asglossing over the different processes at work that produce different forms of suicide.Although integration plays a role in both egoistic and altruistic suicide, its role is verydifferent in eliciting the two forms of suicide. The substitution of ‘‘equilibrium’’ for‘‘integration’’ leads to a belief that a synthesis has taken place when there is evidencethat Durkheim had quite different notions regarding the causes of suicide that cannotbe synthesized, as Douglas might suppose by changing the meaning of a term.

‘‘Integration’’ as ‘‘Organization’’ or ‘‘Unity.’’ Landecker (1950) used ‘‘integration’’ torefer to unification or unifying processes; organization and unity are the key elementsof integration. It can also refer to the organization of ‘‘smaller units into largerwholes,’’ a part to whole analysis. Landecker further specified types of elements thatcould be unified: cultural standards, behaviors, and persons. Three types ofintegration were derived from the combination of these elements.11 ‘‘Cultural’’integration referred to the degree to which cultural standards were consistent withone another.12 ‘‘Normative’’ integration referred to the degree to which a person’sbehaviors were consistent with the cultural standards. ‘‘Communicative’’ integrationreferred to the degree to which interaction occurred with respect to cultural meanings(similar to Gibbs and Martin, 1964). Landecker believed social isolation was theprimary cause of egoistic suicide, whereas Durkheim gave social isolationimportance only because it decreases normative integration.13

Landecker focused on the ‘‘unification process,’’ which is related to the concept of‘‘system,’’ whereas Durkheim’s concept of integration was linked to ‘‘cohesion’’ and ‘‘socialbonds.’’ On the one hand, Durkheim was concerned with how systems are held together,and on the other hand, more importantly, the ‘‘extent the individual’s behavior wasregulated by the group—how social systems control behavior.’’ Landecker’s focus wason ‘‘systemic’’ properties, whereas Durkheim’s was on social control and its contribution

11Not all logical combinations were explored, nor was logical consistency maintained in his analysis.12Linton (1936:282) defined ‘‘integration’’ similarly to refer to the proportion of alternatives to universals

and specialties. The lower the proportion of alternatives, the higher the cultural integration.13A fourth type, ‘‘functional’’ integration, referred to mutual dependence resulting from specialization in

the division of labor. Durkheim’s analysis of egoistic suicide was based on mechanical solidarity, cohesionresulting from shared values, norms, and beliefs (i.e., shared culture) and not organic solidarity.

68 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

to cohesion. Durkheim (1954) explored social bonds in The Division of Labor, identifyingtwo bases of social cohesion: mechanical and organic solidarity. Organic solidarity referredto cohesion resulting from mutual dependence fostered by the division of labor. Exceptthat it purportedly decreased mechanical solidarity, this type of bond was of little concernin his analysis of egoistic suicide. However, mechanical solidarity, where persons arebound together by their common sentiments, formed the core of his conception ofintegration used in his analysis of egoistic suicide.14 Douglas ignores cohesion and socialcontrol, factors that Durkheim considered crucial. Douglas’s attempt to substitute ‘‘equili-brium’’ for ‘‘integration’’ merely focuses on the balance of submissiveness, aloofness, andrebelliousness without directing attention to the forces in society that produce cohesion.

‘‘Integration’’ as ‘‘Attunement’’ or ‘‘Intersubjectivity.’’ The last and most micro-macrotheory of integration has been offered by Scheff (1997). His approach defines socialsolidarity in terms of ‘‘attunement’’ or intersubjectivity. This approach is parallel, butmore clearly defined, than Durkheim’s idea of ‘‘collective conscience.’’ There is anambiguity in the meaning of the French word ‘‘conscience’’ that has troubledAnglophone interpreters: the word means both conscience and consciousness. InScheff’s definition of solidarity, individuals can momentarily share beliefs and feelingscollectively; they literally share a collective consciousness: be it religious or politicalbeliefs, on the one hand, or collective emotions like grief or anger, on the other.

Scheff goes on to outline three basic degrees of collective consciousness: isolation(little or no shared beliefs/feelings), engulfment (little or no individual identity apartfrom the group), and solidarity (a balance between collective and autonomous indi-vidual consciousness). In his analysis, isolation is the basis for egoistic-anomic suicide,and engulfment is the basis for fatalistic-altruistic suicide. The isolated person feelsalone in the world, to the extent that life may not seem worth living. The engulfedperson has lost his or her individual identity, so the person’s own life means little.

To summarize, at least five different definitions of ‘‘integration’’ can be identified inthe literature associated with Durkheim’s theory: (a) shared beliefs and practices; (b)social interaction; (c) a balance of opposing forces such as egoism-altruism andanomie-fatalism; (d) organization/unity; and (e) attunement. Other terms equatedwith ‘‘integration’’ such as ‘‘feelings of cohesiveness’’ or the ‘‘vitality of a group’’employed by Durkheim can be viewed as consequences of integration and not apart of the meaning of that term.

Thus, cohesion and control form the basis of Durkheim’s conception of ‘‘integra-tion.’’ The strength of social bonds is a function of the degree to which collectivesentiments are shared, pervasive, and serve as a guide to behavior. Intense interactioncan increase the strength of the normative system to regulate behavior. A sequencedemonstrating the consequences of low social interaction may be conceived as follows:

low social interaction! few shared meanings!weak social bonds! high suicide(egoistic)

If Durkheim is referring to the degree to which society regulates the individual’sconduct (the amount of nonconformity would be a negative index), the shared mean-ings may increase the likelihood of conformity to the normative system.

14Durkheim did not believe the cohesiveness wrought by functional integration was (a) either sufficient or(b) of the right character to reduce suicide in a group.

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 69

Not only does ‘‘integration’’ have different meanings, but it can also refer todifferent units and levels of analysis. Sometimes, the unit refers to properties of agroup and other times to properties at the individual level. Different levels of abstrac-tion are often involved in different types of integration. Douglas’s concept of integra-tion, a balance of opposing forces, is conceived at the societal level. Socialrelationships can apply at the individual as well as the collective level. This createsproblems in the determination of integration because subgroups within a society maybe tightly knit, whereas cleavages or conflicts exist at a societal level.

Before levels of analysis in the theory can be explored properly, an examination ofthe causal nexus between integration and suicide must be undertaken. Why does thelack of integration or the absence of shared meanings produce suicide according toDurkheim?

WHY LOW INTEGRATION CAUSES SUICIDE: THE CAUSAL NEXUS

Does Durkheim’s hypothesis of an inverse relationship between suicide and integra-tion also imply that the least integrated persons will commit suicide? This cannot beanswered until the intervening variables and causal nexus are examined to see pre-cisely how and why low integration causes suicide. This is one of the least rigorousaspects of Durkheim’s theory, as he fails to specify with precision the nature of therelationship between intervening variables.

Durkheim offers several reasons why the lack of integration results in suicide. Eachinvolves different causal links.

Individual Feelings of Meaninglessness

The foremost reason, according to Durkheim, is that low integration (few commonbeliefs or practices) leads to the feelings of meaningless or purposelessness in indivi-duals, which, in turn, cause them to take their lives.

If the individual isolates himself, it is because the ties uniting him with others areslackened or broken, because society is not sufficiently integrated at points he isin contact with it. The gap between one and another individual conscience,estranging them from each other are authentic results of the weakening of thesocial forces. (1951:281, italics added)

Durkheim states that society cannot disintegrate without the individuals detachingthemselves from social life. And as they detach themselves from social life, they losethe very attachments that make life worthwhile.

He argues that the ‘‘being’’ of the civilized person is primarily created by society,which fills the individual with religious, political, or moral beliefs that control theiractions. These beliefs not only have a collective origin, but their very purpose is toserve collective or societal ends. Individuals have no inborn needs to use language,marry, work, or be moral. But, for society to persist, social sentiments must evolve.These sentiments are then directed toward the purpose of group life. These beliefs aresociety incarnate—individualized in each of us. We cling to these forms of activityonly to the extent we cling to society itself. As we become detached from society, webecome detached from those internalized social aspects of ourselves, which is societyinternalized. If we are not tied to these social ends, then behavior loses the meaningand purpose for which it evolved. And the purpose toward which moral, religious,

70 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

and political beliefs are oriented—the collective goals—is lost to us as well. Only abeliever firm in his or her faith or an individual strongly bound by family or politicalties can feel purpose to this life. Uncommitted to social purpose, life loses all meaning.

Because we are socialized into a social existence, its fading leaves us unable to besatisfied without one and results in feelings of helplessness and emptiness. In such astate of confusion, the least discouragement may give birth to desperate resolutions. Iflife is not worth the trouble of being lived, everything becomes a pretext to ridourselves of it.

Collective Sentiments of Meaninglessness

Durkheim further asserts that groups also evolve a collective evaluation of the valueof existence ‘‘inclining people to sadness or cheerfulness, making them see things inbright or somber lights’’ (1951:213). ‘‘Currents of depression and disillusionmentemanating from no particular individual but expressing society’s state of disintegra-tion’’ (1951:214) are formed that reflect the relaxation of societal bonds. Belief systemsevolve in the group, which attest to the senselessness of life, and new moralitiescommend suicide or minimal existence.

As these currents are collective, they have, by virtue of their origin, and authoritywhich they impose upon the individual and drive him more vigorously on the wayto which he is already inclined by the state of moral distress directly aroused inhim by the disintegration of society. (1951:214)

Ironically, at the same moment the individual frees him/herself from the socialenvironment by suicide, he/she submits to the social influence.

The bond attaching people to life relaxes because the bond that attaches them tosociety has slackened. The incidents of private life that seem to be the direct inspira-tion of suicide and are considered its determining causes are, in reality, only incidentalcauses. The individual yields to the slightest shock of circumstances because society’sstate of disintegration has made him or her ready prey to suicide. The more social theperson is, the more points of support outside of himself or herself become necessary. Yetwhether these feelings cause suicide, or whether they merely intensify suicidal feelingsthat arise from other sources, or whether both components are necessary and sufficientconditions for the individual’s taking his or her life is not specified by Durkheim.

Durkheim also discusses secondary reasons why the lack of integration can lead tosuicide.

Intensification of Misfortunes

According to this hypothesis, the individual’s misfortunes, in which the suicidalimpulses are rooted, become more intensely felt under conditions of low integration.Thus, the suicidal impulse is intensified.

This intensification is brought about in several ways. First, when the individual hasno purpose outside his/her own existence, any misfortune that occurs is dramaticallyintensified because of excessive self-concern—the result of there being little else to lifebut the individual themselves.

The individual has no reason to endure life’s suffering patiently. For they cling tolife more resolutely when belonging to a group they love, so as not to betray

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 71

interests they put before their own . . . and the lofty goal they envision preventstheir feeling personal troubles so deeply. (1951:209–10)

Misfortune and personal troubles come to absorb total attention because theindividual is fixated on the self. An individual who undergoes a misfortune and whohas little involvement in collective goals will experience more deprivation and tormentthan a similar individual who has a greater involvement in group life. Second,personal misfortunes also become intensified when group support is lacking.

There is, in short, in a cohesive and animated society a constant interchange ofideas and feelings from each to all, something like a mutual moral support, whichinstead of throwing the individual on his own resources leads him to share in thecollective energy and supports his own when exhausted. (1951:210)

Here Durkheim refers to the network of interpersonal relationships as ‘‘integration.’’Groups act as ‘‘shock absorbers.’’ When group support is available for individualsduring crises, they are more able to cope with them successfully. If two individualssuffer the same misfortune, it will be experienced less intensely and devastatingly by aperson who has group support. Rituals such as funerals provide support and reestab-lish solidarity among the group members.

Weakened Restraints

‘‘A collective force is one of the obstacles best calculated to restrain suicide; itsweakening involves a development of suicide. When society is strongly integrated, itholds individuals under its control and considers them at its service and forbids themto dispose willfully of themselves. Accordingly it opposes evading one’s dutiesthrough death. But how could society impose its supremacy upon them when theyrefuse to accept its subordination as legitimate’’ (1951:209).

Durkheim also sees integration as a restraining force upon the individual’s desire totake his/her own life. The theoretical basis lies in the restraining force of society in theform of its culture and social organization.

An important point not explicated by Durkheim is the basis upon which the group‘‘controls’’ the individual’s behavior. If he is asserting that the culture of the groupwith its system of norms and legitimate authority controls the individual’s behavior,then integration refers to the shared meanings and their ability to regulate conduct.If, however, he is asserting that the restraint of the individual’s behavior comes fromthe social organization and social structure of the group, then ‘‘integration’’ refers tothe structure of interpersonal relationships. Two different intervening processes arepossible, and Durkheim never makes explicit the role of each process.

Isolation and Loneliness

An explanation attributed to Durkheim (Landecker 1950) is that low integration(weak communicative integration or the lack of intimate relationships) leads tofeelings of isolation and loneliness that are painful and, in turn, lead to suicide. Asimilar interpretation is that social individuation, fostered by the division of labor orcultural diversity (i.e., normative and functional integration), results in differences invalues, practices, and beliefs, which fragments the sense of community and, in turn,leads to social isolation and loneliness. Both these states in society directly produce

72 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

suicidal impulses in individuals. However, this argument is more imputed toDurkheim than is actually found in his work Suicide. Travis (1990) linked isolationto Halbwachs’ rather than Durkheim’s theory of suicide.

DIFFERENT EXPLANATIONS CONTAINED WITHIN DURKHEIM’STHEORY AND THEIR SIGNIFICANCE FOR PREDICTIONS OFINDIVIDUAL SUICIDE

Numerous explanations are contained within Durkheim’s theory of egoistic suicide.Five different definitions of integration can be found in the literature (shared mean-ings or culture, interaction and intimate relationships or social structure, and abalance of egoistic-altruistic and anomic-fatalistic forces or equilibrium of socialsystem, and attunement or intersubjectivity or social-psychological processes).Furthermore, five different conceptions of integration (organization of the part tothe whole and synchronicity, unity, strength of the social bond or cohesion, balance orequilibrium of forces, and social control) can refer to three different units (persons,group standards, or behavior) at several levels of analysis (society, community, group,and individuals) that could involve numerous intervening processes or causal nexibetween low integration and suicide (individual sentiments and collective representa-tions of meaninglessness, intensification of misfortunes, weakened restraints, andloneliness-isolation).

Permutations of these variables can be generated as possible hypotheses to accountfor egoistic suicide. Not all permutations are logically consistent, and several inter-pretations in the literature are not supported by a close reading of Durkheim’s work.Nonetheless, several possible explanations are contained within the theory of egoisticsuicide.

Ambiguity is heightened by Durkheim’s failure to specify adequately the nature of theintervening variables and the mechanisms by which low integration contributes to suiciderates. The lack of integration can (a) produce suicidal impulses, (b) intensify alreadyexisting suicidal impulses, or (c) merely restrain individuals from acting on alreadyexisting suicidal impulses. Thus, in some explanations, integration is a necessary andsufficient condition for suicide, whereas in others, integration only alters the probabilityof suicide resulting, providing suicidal impulses already in existence in a population.

Only permutations reasonably plausible within the structure of Durkheim’s theoryare examined here. We shall also explore whether they have implications for predict-ing individual suicidal tendencies.

Individual Sentiments of Meaninglessness

The most plausible interpretation of Durkheim’s theory is that the lack of integrationrefers to a lack of shared sentiments. In this sense, the lack of integration leads toweakened social regulation due to weaker orientation toward social goals, creatingsentiments of meaninglessness in individuals, which cause suicidal behavior. The mostreasonable interpretation of integration in this instance would be ‘‘unity’’ of the groupwith respect to social meanings or social regulation. Groups lacking unity produce inindividuals the impulse to commit suicide. Unity is a group attribute—a groupcharacterized by many shared social meanings. At the group level, the hypothesisstates that suicide rates would be high in groups that exhibit few shared meanings.Predictions could also be made at the individual level in that sentiments of mean-inglessness would be most strongly felt in individuals who were culturally dissimilar,

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 73

and those would be the most likely to commit suicide. The individual not bound tofamily, religious, or political ties would feel little purpose to life, only emptiness. Here,precipitating events would not be the cause of suicide; anything could trigger thesuicidal impulse. A hypothesis could be proposed:

HYPOTHESIS 1A can be expressed: SM/TM! nIm! SR

SM¼ shared meanings, TM¼ total number of meanings, n¼ number of persons,Im¼ intensity of suicidal impulses, SR¼ suicide rate

This hypothesis postulates at the group level of analysis that the proportion ofshared meanings in the group will generate a specific number of persons with suicidalimpulses sufficient to induce suicidal acts, which, in turn, will lead to a specific suiciderate. The relationship between increments in shared meanings and increases in suiciderates can be examined.

HYPOTHESIS 1B: This interpretation of Durkheim’s theory can also generate predic-tions at the individual level of analysis: those individuals most dissimilar in socialmeanings will be the ones most likely to commit suicide within the group.

Collective Sentiments of Meaninglessness

This explanation emphasizes group definitions that incline individuals to feel sadnessand hopelessness as a result of collective representations. If shared beliefs can inde-pendently produce inclinations toward suicide in the individual, predictions could bemade only at the group level and not for specific individuals. Or if collective senti-ments cause suicide, then those individuals most tied to the group’s culture wouldcommit suicide, as opposed to Hypothesis 1B focusing on those who were culturallydissimilar. However, it appears that Durkheim gave collective representations only asecondary reinforcing role in his analysis of egoistic suicide.

HYPOTHESIS 2A: PM/TM! nIm! SR

PM¼ pessimistic meanings, TM¼ total meanings, n¼ number of persons,Im¼ intensity of suicidal impulses, SR¼ suicide rate

This hypothesis postulates at a group level that as the proportion of pessimistic orhopeless meanings increase in a group, the rate of suicide will also increase.

HYPOTHESIS 2B: This hypothesis at an individual level would suggest that those mostintegrated would be the most likely to internalize such pessimistic meanings andtherefore most likely to commit suicide.

Intensification of Misfortunes

This approach, which Durkheim also regarded as secondary, views the role of societynot as creating suicidal impulses, but as amplifying the individual’s misfortunes.Suicidal impulses do not necessarily have social origins.

‘‘Excessive self-concern’’ increases the intensity of suffering. Cultural diversity (lowintegration) leads to increased individualism, which heightens self-concern. Because indi-vidualism intensifies misfortunes when they arise, it amplifies suicidal impulses in theindividual. This interpretation rests upon the notion of integration as shared meanings.

74 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

Group support reduces the intensity of misfortunes and individual sufferings. Here,Durkheim uses ‘‘integration’’ to refer to the network of interpersonal relationshipsthat provide the individual the support and energy necessary to cope with misfortunessuccessfully.

In both cases, lack of integration does not produce the suicidal impulse; it onlyintensifies an impulse arising from other forces. Integration is used in two differentsenses: (a) shared meanings and (b) strong interpersonal relationships. Both canrefer to properties of groups (e.g., highly integrated groups) and individuals (e.g.,persons culturally similar to others or those with relationships in the group). Itwould not be possible, however, to predict individual instances of suicide from thisexplanation. Even though integration can refer to an individual level, its predictivevalue is limited, as only those who (a) experience a misfortune and (b) have a lowdegree of integration will commit suicide. There is no way from the theory todetermine who will experience a misfortune. The origins of suicidal impulses arenot related to integration.

In addition, for Durkheim’s theory to predict differences in suicide rates betweengroups, he must either assume a similar degree of misfortune among groups or assumethat the forces of amplification were primarily responsible for determining thestrength of suicidal impulses. In both cases, the theory could not predict whichindividuals in a loosely integrated group would take their lives because the primaryimpetus comes from misfortune, not integration. Durkheim says little concerning therole of society in creating misfortunes. An exception is found in his analysis of anomicand fatalistic suicide where the role of society is emphasized in creating individualsuffering. Another hypothesis can be generated:

HYPOTHESIS 3A: nTm! (nIm) A! SR

n¼ number of instances, Tm¼magnitude of tragedy or misfortune, Im¼ intensity ofsuicidal impulse, A¼ amplification factor,15 SR¼ suicide rate

Hypothesis 3A applies only at a group level and states that the frequency andintensity of misfortunes in a society will cause a certain number of persons toexperience suicidal impulses with a range of intensities. It also indicates that a lowSM/M (normative integration) or a high percentage of isolates (communicative inte-gration) will increase the magnitude of suicidal impulses in the population. Thequalifications stated above apply with respect to predicting suicide rates in a society,and no hypothesis could be generated at an individual level of analysis. An analogy isriver of troubles; the slings and arrows of outrageous fortune flowing throughsocieties that have many strong social bonds will drag few over the abyss of suicide.

Weakened Restraints

This explanation views the role of society as neither a producer nor an amplifier ofsuicidal impulses, but rather as a restrainer of such impulses. It is not concerned withthe origin of the suicidal impulse but views society’s role of holding the impulse incheck as primary. Whatever the sources of suicidal inclinations, groups differ in thedegree of restraints they can exercise to check these individual impulses. Durkheimgave this form of explanation a secondary role as well.

15The amplification factor is a function of either SM/M or proportion of isolates in the population.

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 75

Here again, two different definitions of integration are used by Durkheim: (a)integration as ‘‘shared culture’’ that controls the individual’s behavior throughnorms and legitimate authority and (b) the ‘‘strength of the relational system’’whereby the influence of the group is exerted on the individual through interpersonalrelationships, obligations, responsibility, and the claims of others (Henry and Short1954) from a strongly integrated group (or network of social relationships).

Even though shared meanings can refer to both group and individual character-istics, no individual predictions can be made on this basis because the origins ofsuicidal impulses are unconnected to integration. At the group level, Durkheim isasserting that Group A has a weaker system of norms and authority than Group B.Consequently, persons motivated toward suicide are more likely to be restrained inGroup B than Group A. However, if Durkheim argues that groups with strongerrestraints will have lower suicide rates, he must assume that there are similar propor-tions of persons with suicidal impulses in both groups and that the strength of thesuicidal impulses in individuals are roughly the same in the two groups. Group B mayhave stronger restraints, but it may also have more people with suicidal impulses orstronger impulses. In this case, Group B would exhibit a higher rate of suicide thanGroup A even though it had stronger restraints. This explanation rests on theassumption that either there are similar proportions or impulses with similar degreesof intensity in different groups—perhaps a questionable assumption—or the strengthof the resistance resulting from group integration is the only critical factor that needbe considered in the suicide rate.

HYPOTHESIS 4A: nib! (–R)! SR

nib¼ number of persons with suicidal impulses of a certain magnitude, R¼ resistancefactor for a given society,16 SR¼ suicide rate

Hypothesis 4A states the suicide rate is primarily a function of the degree to whichgroup restraints operate to check suicidal impulses. The amount of restraint is afunction of either the degree of normative integration (percent shared meanings) orthe strength of the relational system or communicative integration (percent isolates).In both Hypotheses 2A and 3A, nib may be constant and its inclusion in each caseindicates that the SR is primarily a function of either amplification of suicidalimpulses or resistance to suicidal impulses. Empirical research is necessary to testwhich factor was operative, as statistical analysis of rates would be identical undereither hypothesis. Without independent investigation, it would not be possible todiscern whether amplification or resistance was operating.

Because there is no way to identify an individual as being prone to suicidal impulsesfrom this perspective, no individual prediction could be made. Furthermore, unlesscultural dissimilarity was employed as the index, there would be no way of knowingwhich persons participated in shared meanings.

If restraint evolves from the network of social relationships, then individuals withthe fewest social ties would be the least subject to restraint or social control.Individual predictions could only be made in this instance if one could identifythose with suicidal impulses. However, if one assumed similarity in suicidal impulsesin both integrated and nonintegrated populations, then those most integrated wouldhave the lowest suicide rates.

16The R factor is a function of SM/M or proportion of isolates in the group.

76 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

An alternative is to view the strength of the barrier as being so strong that restraintis the significant factor regardless of the frequency or intensity of suicidal impulses inthe group. In both cases, no individual predictions can be made.

The four possibilities are as follows:

(a) suicidal impulse – no resistance – suicide(b) suicidal impulse – resistance – no suicide(c) no suicidal impulse – no resistance – no suicide(d) no suicidal impulse – resistance – no suicide

Integration as Social Relationships or Interaction

Here, integration can refer to cohesion or the strength of the social bond thatincreases cohesion. Groups that have high rates of interaction or intimate relation-ships produce more meaning or provide more support. Those isolated from suchinteraction would be those most likely to commit suicide.

An Equilibrium Between E–A and A–F Forces

With Douglas’s interpretation, it would not be possible to identify individualinstances of suicide, as the concept only refers to group attributes. It also refers toequilibrium in the total society, not just subgroups. Furthermore, it would not bepossible to develop operational measures of ‘‘balance’’ or ‘‘equilibrium’’ other than byworking back from the suicide rate. To use this ex post facto measure as an explana-tion of the rates in society would be circular reasoning, and thus the empiricalfruitfulness of this concept is limited.

SUMMARY

Whether Durkheim’s hypothesis of the relationship between suicide and integrationhas implications for the prediction of individual suicide depends on which interpreta-tions are selected. Although some imply that individuals who are the least integratedshould be the ones most likely to commit suicide, others do not permit such individualpredictions. Clearly, the relationships between macro and micro analysis are complexand depend on the particular theoretical formulation of causal linkages. Durkheimclearly gave some of the variables a secondary or minor role (e.g., weakened restraintsor amplification of misfortunes). Feelings of meaninglessness resulting from socialdifferentiation were the primary factor in the early chapters. This did permit indivi-dual predictions to be made. However, in the later chapters, Durkheim speaks ofsocial currents imposing somber views on persons, emphasizing the effects of collec-tive meanings on individuals. This position is contradictory to the one outlined in theearlier chapters. Depending on which chain of intervening variables was selected,some analyses lead to individual predictions. Yet even here there are problematicissues because the most integrated persons (i.e., those most in touch with socialmeanings of pessimism or the emptiness of life) would be the most likely to commitsuicide in Hypothesis 2B, whereas just the opposite would be true in Hypothesis 1B.The inconsistencies in Durkheim’s theory have not been addressed or resolved andcause difficulty in interpreting inconsistent findings in this area.

It should be noted, however, that individual manifestations would reflect the socialcauses of suicide (1951:146). According to Durkheim, not all suicides were social,

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 77

some resulted solely from the person’s own nature. Even in social types, victims givetheir act a personal stamp that expresses their temperament and the social conditionsin which they are involved. Suicides resulting from social causes also leave a shadingand special mark expressive of the individual. Egoistic suicides are acts devoid ofpassion, reflecting detachment, emptiness, and melancholy.

Another source of confusion is that Suicide grapples with three distinct problems: (a)variation in rates between groups (i.e., why one group had a higher suicide rate than another);(b) why stability existed in the suicide rate of a specific group; and (c) a problem not directlyconfronted, why one person rather than another committed suicide within a group.

In his analysis of the variation of suicide rate between groups, Durkheim identifiedthe dimension of ‘‘integration’’ and ‘‘restraint’’ (related to the four types of suicide) asthe critical factors. These are properties of the culture of the group, specifically itsnormative system and the degree to which (a) there are unified beliefs and (b) itregulates aspirations in members of the group.

However, when he deals with the issue of overall stability of the suicide rate, hefocuses on the social organization or the manner of associations among individuals(1951:321). He speaks of a ‘‘collective current’’ that reflects the collective inclinationflowing down the channels of social organization. The intensity of the current deter-mines the volume of suicides. The character and intensity of the moral constitutionestablishes the contingent of voluntary deaths. The collective force is a property of theobjective state of society, external to the members in the society, and is not to bemeasured in individual inclinations.

The proper way to measure any element of a collective is not to measure itsmagnitude within individual’s consciences and to take the average of them all.Rather it is their sum that must be taken. Even this method would be much belowreality, for this would give us the social sentiment reduced by all its lossesthrough individuation. (1951:319)

The social organization or patterns of associations are the channels through whichthe current flows, and its overall configuration determines the magnitude of suicides,and its paths determine which particular persons are affected by the currents.

It is rare, if not impossible, for one of the currents to succeed in exerting suchpreponderant influence over all points of society. It always reaches this degree ofenergy in the midst of restricted surroundings containing conditions especially favor-able to its development. One or another social condition, occupation, or religiousfaith stimulates it more especially. Yet the sum of all these individual cases has its ownunity. That is, although these particular environments where suicide occurs mostfrequently are separated from one another, dispersed in thousands of ways over theentire territory, they are nevertheless closely related, for they are parts of a singlewhole organism (1951:322).

With respect to which specific individuals take their lives, Durkheim also believedthat individuals also have to be susceptible to social currents and suggested thatthe civilization that breeds the anomic and egoistic tendencies also produces delicatenervous systems particularly vulnerable to anomie and egoism. ‘‘Society thereforecannot lack the material for its needs, for it has, so to speak, kneaded it withits own hand’’ (1951:323). Thus, when exploring the question of which personsin a social environment are most likely to commit suicide, Durkheim ironicallyreintroduces individual psychological susceptibility, rather than sociologicalvulnerability.

78 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY

The role of individual-level factors in the origin of suicide can now be precisely put.If in a given moral environment, for example, in the same religious faith or in thesame occupation, certain individuals are affected and others not—this is undoubtedly,in great part because the former’s mental constitution offers less resistance to suicide.But although these conditions may share in determining the particular persons inwhom its current becomes embodied, neither the special qualities nor the intensity ofthe current depends on these conditions. A given number of suicides are not foundannually in a group because it contains a given number of neuropathic persons.Neuropathic conditions only cause the suicides to succumb with a greater readinessto the current. No society exists in which the various forms of nervous degenerationdo not provide suicide with more than the necessary number of candidates. Onlycertain ones are called. These are the ones who through circumstances have beennearer the pessimistic currents and who consequently have felt these influences morecompletely (1951:323–24).

This last statement suggests that those most touched by the social currents (i.e.,those most integrated) would be the most subject to suicide. Introducing psychologi-cal variables such as depression that suggest it is an independent cause of suicideoverlooks Durkheim’s conception that they are the ones most likely to be effected bythe larger social forces and without these forces suicide may not occur within suchindividuals. Only comparative studies of societies where egoistic forces are weak willdepressives have less suicide.

Durkheim reflects that a year is needed to produce the full degenerative effectbecause the conditions of social activity change in both intensity and directionaccording to the season. An annual period is required for the same combination ofactivities to reoccur. The bill for potential suicides is paid in annual installmentsbecause the collective current requires a certain amount of exposure to penetrateindividuals.

In laying out some of the ambiguities, inconsistencies, and alternative hypothesescontained within Durkheim’s analysis of egoistic suicide, it is hoped that furtherempirical investigation can shed light on which avenues may be fruitful in the analysisof social suicide.

REFERENCES

Bellah, R. N. 1959. ‘‘Durkheim and History.’’ American Sociological Review 24:447–61.

Blau, P. M. 1960. ‘‘Structural Effects.’’ American Sociological Review 25:178–93.

Bogue, D. J. and E. J. Bogue. 1976. ‘‘Ecological Correlation Reexamined.’’ In Essays in Human Ecology.

Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Breault, K. D. and K. Barkey. 1982. ‘‘A Comparative Analysis of Durkheim’s Theory of Egoistic Suicide.’’

Sociological Quarterly 23:321–31.

Collins, R. 1988. Theoretical Sociology. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.

———. 2004. Interaction Ritual Chains. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Danigelis, N. and W. Pope. 1979. ‘‘Durkheim’s Theory of Suicide as Applied to the Family: An Empirical

Test.’’ Social Forces 57:1081–1106.

Douglas, J. D. 1967. The Social Meaning of Suicide. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Duncan, O. and B. Davis. 1953. ‘‘An Alternative to Ecological Correlation.’’ American Sociological Review

18:665–66.

Durkheim, E. 1951. Suicide. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

———. 1954. The Division of Labor in Society. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

Gibbs, J. P., ed. 1968. Suicide. New York: Harper & Row Co.

Gibbs, J. P. and W. T. Martin. 1958. ‘‘A Theory of Status Integration and Its Relationship to Suicide.’’

American Sociological Review 23:14–147.

———. 1964. Status Integration and Suicide. Eugene, OR: University of Oregon Press.

MACRO-MICRO RELATIONSHIPS 79

Giddens, A. 1972. Emile Durkheim: Selected Writings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hammond, J. L. 1973. ‘‘Two Sources of Error in Ecological Correlations.’’ American Sociological Review

38:764–77.

Henry, A. F. and J. F. Short, Jr. 1954. Suicide and Homocide: Some Economic, Sociological and

Psychological Aspects of Aggression. New York: Free Press.

Heyns, E. 1975. ‘‘Suicide and Homo Duplex: An Interpretation of Durkheim’s Typology of Suicide.’’

Sociological Quarterly 16:87–104.

Homans, G. C. 1950. The Human Group. NewYork: Harcourt.

Inkeles, A. 1959. ‘‘Personality and Social Structure.’’ Pp. 249–76 in Sociological Today, edited by

R. K. Merton, L. Broom, and L. S. Cottrell. New York: Basic Books.

Johnson, B. D. 1965. ‘‘Durkheim’s One Cause of Suicide.’’ American Sociological Review 30:875–86.

Landecker, W. S. 1950. ‘‘Types of Integration and Their Measurement.’’ American Journal of Sociology

56:332–40.

Lester, D. 1989. ‘‘A Test of Durkheim’s Theory of Suicide Using Data from Modern Nations.’’ International

Journal of Comparative Sociology 30:3–4.

———. 1992. ‘‘A Test of Durkheim’s Theory of Suicide in Primitive Societies.’’ Suicide and Life-Threatening

Behavior 22:388–94.

———. 1994. Emile Durkheim: Le Suicide 100 Years Later. Philadelphia: Charles Press.

Linton, R. 1936. The Study of Man. New York: Appleton-Century Co.

Merton, R. K. 1968. Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: Free Press.

Newcomb, T. M. 1953. ‘‘An Approach to the Study of Communicative Acts.’’ Psychological Review

60:393–404.

Parsons, T. 1949. The Structure of Social Action. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

Pescosolido, B. A. and S. Georgianna. 1989. ‘‘Durkheim, Suicide, and Religion: Toward a Network Theory

of Suicide.’’ American Sociological Review 54:33–48.

Pescosolido, B. A. and R. Mendelson. 1986. ‘‘Social Causation or Social Construction? An Investigation

into the Social Organization of Official Rates.’’ American Sociological Review 51:80–101.

Pickering, W. S. F. and G. Walford. 2000. Durkheim’s Suicide: A Century of Research and Debate. London:

Routledge.

Pope, W. 1976. Durkheim’s Suicide: A Classic Analyzed. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Robinson, W. S. 1950. ‘‘Ecological Correlations and the Behavior of Individuals.’’ American Sociological

Review 15:351–57.

Scheff, T. J. 1990. Microsociology: Discourse, Emotion, and Social Structure. Chicago, IL: University of

Chicago Press.

———. 1997. Emotion, the Social Bond, and Human Reality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Scheff, T. J. and Rettinger. 1991. Emotions & Violence: Shame & Rage in Destructive Conflict. Lexington,

MA: Lexington Books.

Selvin, H. 1965. ‘‘Durkheim’s Suicide: Further Thoughts on a Methodological Classic.’’ Pp. 113–36 in Emile

Durkheim, edited by R. A. Nisbet. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Simpson, M. E. and G. H. Conklin. 1989. ‘‘Socioeconomic Development, Suicide and Religion: A Test of

Durkheim’s Theory of Religion and Suicide.’’ Social Forces 67(4):949–64.

Stack, S. 1978. ‘‘Suicide: A Comparative Analysis.’’ Social Forces 57:644–53.

———. 1980. ‘‘Religion and Suicide: A Reanalysis.’’ Social Psychiatry 15:65–70.

———. 1983. ‘‘A Comparative Analysis of Durkheim’s Theory of Egoistic Suicide: A Comment.’’

Sociological Quarterly 24:625–27.

Travis, R. 1990. ‘‘Halbwachs and Durkheim: A Test of Two Theories of Suicide.’’ British Journal of

Sociology 41:225–43.

Turner, J. H. 1990. ‘‘Emile Durkheim’s Theory of Social Organization.’’ Social Forces 68(4):1089–1103.

80 SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY