blyth-1997-the modern language journal

17
A Constructivist Approach to Grammar: Teaching Teachers to Teach Aspect CARL BLYTH Department OfFrench &Italian University of Texas at Austin Awtin, TX 78712-1197 Email: cblyth@mail,utexas.edu This article demonstrates how a constructivist approach to teacher education helps inex- perienced teachers understand the learning and teaching of aspect, a core grammatical concept. By consciously experiencing the process of narration (i.e., how a speaker per- ceives real or imagined events and then organizes perceptions into a coherent recounting of events), apprentice teachers construct a deeper awareness of the form/meaning aspec- tual correlations of the target language. More generally, this study argues that a constructiv- ist approach to teacher education facilitates the development of an innovative grammar pedagogy by challenging TAs’s traditional beliefs about the nature of grammar. MOST FOREIGN LANGUAGE TEACHERS gradually develop a personal set of eclectic be- liefs about the teaching and learning of gram- mar based on a variety of sources: their lan- guage learning and teaching experiences, teacher education courses, participation in conferences and workshops, and frequent dis- cussions with other foreign language teachers. Although many teaching practices are taught explicitly and learned consciously in teacher ed- ucation programs, others are simply “picked up” or constructed in the process of becoming a teacher, a process akin to acculturation (Caz- den, 1988). In this respect, teachers of foreign languages are no different than teachers of other subjects or disciplines who also tend to develop their eclectic beliefs and practices over time (Clark, 1988; Cohen & Ball, 1990). Although such pedagogical eclecticism may represent a healthy skepticism of educational innovations, it is not always enlightened. Many foreign language teachers hold traditional be- liefs about explicit grammar instruction that are no longer supported by current research in The Moda Language Juuntal, 81, i (1997) 01997 The Moda Language Juurnal 0026-7902/97/50-66 $1.50/0 linguistics and second language acquisition (Lee & VanPatten, 1995; VanPatten, 1996). In particular, many teachers wedded to traditional methods of grammar instruction resist learner- centered or constructivist approaches to learn- ing. These teachers persist in their beliefs that the sequence of a grammatical syllabus can be derived unproblematically and in a priori fash- ion from a given language and that communica- tive skills and metalinguistic awareness can be taught adequately through teacher explanation of grammatical rules, followed by mechanical drills and an occasional communicative exercise. Given the misconceptions underiying the teaching of grammar, how can teacher prepara- tion programs help apprentice teachers shift their focus from teaching to learning? How can teachers-in-training construct pedagogical practices that are more firmly based on what is currently known about the way foreign lan- guages are learned rather than on orthodox, nor- mative beliefs about how languages should be taught? In an attempt to answer these questions, this essay examines the teaching and learning of aspect, specifically the distinction between the preterit and imperfect tenses of the Romance languages, for example, the wsi compare’ and the impurfuit in French; the pretkto and the imperfect0 in Spanish.’

Upload: sharonquek-tay

Post on 07-Sep-2015

25 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

DESCRIPTION

Modern Language Journal

TRANSCRIPT

  • A Constructivist Approach to Grammar: Teaching Teachers to Teach Aspect CARL BLYTH Department OfFrench &Italian University of Texas at Austin Awtin, TX 78712-1197 Email: cblyth@mail,utexas.edu

    This article demonstrates how a constructivist approach to teacher education helps inex- perienced teachers understand the learning and teaching of aspect, a core grammatical concept. By consciously experiencing the process of narration (i.e., how a speaker per- ceives real or imagined events and then organizes perceptions into a coherent recounting of events), apprentice teachers construct a deeper awareness of the form/meaning aspec- tual correlations of the target language. More generally, this study argues that a constructiv- ist approach to teacher education facilitates the development of an innovative grammar pedagogy by challenging TAss traditional beliefs about the nature of grammar.

    MOST FOREIGN LANGUAGE TEACHERS gradually develop a personal set of eclectic be- liefs about the teaching and learning of gram- mar based on a variety of sources: their lan- guage learning and teaching experiences, teacher education courses, participation in conferences and workshops, and frequent dis- cussions with other foreign language teachers.

    Although many teaching practices are taught explicitly and learned consciously in teacher ed- ucation programs, others are simply picked up or constructed in the process of becoming a teacher, a process akin to acculturation (Caz- den, 1988). In this respect, teachers of foreign languages are no different than teachers of other subjects or disciplines who also tend to develop their eclectic beliefs and practices over time (Clark, 1988; Cohen & Ball, 1990).

    Although such pedagogical eclecticism may represent a healthy skepticism of educational innovations, it is not always enlightened. Many foreign language teachers hold traditional be- liefs about explicit grammar instruction that are no longer supported by current research in

    The M o d a Language Juuntal, 81, i (1997)

    01997 The M o d a Language Juurnal 0026-7902/97/50-66 $1.50/0

    linguistics and second language acquisition (Lee & VanPatten, 1995; VanPatten, 1996). In particular, many teachers wedded to traditional methods of grammar instruction resist learner- centered or constructivist approaches to learn- ing. These teachers persist in their beliefs that the sequence of a grammatical syllabus can be derived unproblematically and in a priori fash- ion from a given language and that communica- tive skills and metalinguistic awareness can be taught adequately through teacher explanation of grammatical rules, followed by mechanical drills and an occasional communicative exercise.

    Given the misconceptions underiying the teaching of grammar, how can teacher prepara- tion programs help apprentice teachers shift their focus from teaching to learning? How can teachers-in-training construct pedagogical practices that are more firmly based on what is currently known about the way foreign lan- guages are learned rather than on orthodox, nor- mative beliefs about how languages should be taught? In an attempt to answer these questions, this essay examines the teaching and learning of aspect, specifically the distinction between the preterit and imperfect tenses of the Romance languages, for example, the wsi compare and the impurfuit in French; the pretkto and the imperfect0 in Spanish.

  • Curl Blyth

    Aspect is a core grammatical concept, yet frequently it is poorly understood by students of French and Spanish (Connor, 1992; Ozete, 1987). Kaplan (1987) states that this aspectual opposition is perceived by teachers as partic- ularly problematic for English-speaking learn- ers of French (p. 53)* and Garcia and van Putte (1988) claim that it constitutes one of the major difficulties encountered by native speakers of Germanic languages in the learning of Spanish (p. 263). Graduate teaching assis- tants (TAs) often find aspect difficult to teach because they themselves lack a clear under- standing of it. What I shall argue below is that constructivism provides a beneficial framework for teacher education programs because it al- lows teachers to gain essential technical knowl- edge about grammar while also gaining insight into beliefs concerning pedagogical practices.

    51

    development, learning is development; (b) dis- equilibrium facilitates learning; (c) reflective abstraction is the driving force of learning; (d) dialogue within a community engenders fur- ther thinking; (e) learning proceeds towards the development of . . . central organizing prin- ciples that can be generalized across experi- ences and that often require the undoing or reorganizing of earlier conceptions (pp. 29- 30). These general principles point to a learner- centered pedagogy in which the teacher acts as a facilitator of active and personalized learning rather than as an expert dispensing prepared information.

    CONSTRUCTIVISM

    Constructivism is a theory of learning and knowledge closely associated with the work of several well-known psychologists: Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky, Jerome Bruner, and Howard Gard- ner. The most fundamental and radical epis- temological principle of constructivism holds that knowledge does not and can not have the purpose of producing representations of an in- dependent reality, but instead has an adaptive function (von Glaserfeld, 1996, p. 3). In other words, constructivism rejects the idea that hu- man knowledge is a direct reflection of an ob- jective reality. A constructivist would argue that every human being constructs his or her own version of reality. As a consequence, multiple realities or multiple ways of knowing are to be expected in the classroom. Fosnot (1996a) states the following:

    [the constructivist perspective is viewed as] a self- regulatory process of struggling with the conflict between existing personal models of the world and discrepant new insights, constructing new repre- sentations and models of reality as a human meaning-making venture with culturally developed tools and symbols, and further negotiating such meaning through cooperative social activity, dis- course, and debate. (p. ix)

    Although constructivism has become an im- portant poststructuralist psychological theory of learning, it does not translate neatly into a set of pedagogical practices. Nevertheless, Fosnot (1996b) suggests five general principles of con- structivism with obvious applications to educa- tional practice: (a) Learning is not the result of

    TRADITIONAL GRAMMAR TEACHING

    Largely unchanged for decades, the presenta- tion of grammar in foreign language textbooks and classrooms continues to be based on an outdated combination of behaviorism, struc- turalist linguistics, and versions of audiolingual- ism and cognitive-code theory. In general, these traditional presentations of grammar are characterized by a strict sequence of drills as formulated by Paulston (1972): First learners practice grammatical structures via mechanical drills, then meaningful drills, and last, commu- nicative drills. To perform a mechanical drill, students do not need to attach any meaning to the grammatical form being practiced. The classic example of a mechanical drill is the transformational exercise in which students manipulate or transform an isolated gram- matical item, typically a verb conjugation or a nominal inflection. Like mechanical drills, meaningful drills have only one correct re- sponse, however, they require students to un- derstand both the stimulus and the response. Communicative drills also require the student to understand the meaning of the stimulus and the response, but differ from meaningful drills in that there is no single correct answer.

    Lee and VanPatten (1995) note that Paul- stons grammatical sequence was in keeping with the period of language teaching domi- nated by behaviorism, which emphasized observ- able behavior, avoidance of errors, and habit formation (p. 91). Today, however, such a tradi- tional presentation of grammar is at odds with what is known and widely accepted about the role of input in first and second language acqui- sition. Traditional approaches pay little, if any, attention to the effects of input on the develop- ing grammatical system of a learner and instead focus almost entirely on output or linguistic

  • 52

    production. VanPatten (1996, p. 59) claims that the most serious problem for traditional ap- proaches to grammar is the mismatch be- tween theory and practice, between the impor- tant role attributed to input in current theory and the lack of input in current practice:

    With its emphasis on output practice, a traditional approach to grammar instruction ignores the cru- cial role of input in second language acquisition- and the definition of input in second language ac- quisition does not include instructors explanations about how the second language works. The defini- tion of input is limited to meaning-bearing input, language that the learner hears or sees that is used to communicate a message. Thus, in traditional in- struction, learners practice a form or structure, but they are not getting the input that is needed to construct the mental representation of the struc- ture itself. (VanPatten, 1996, p. 6)

    Despite recent theoretical developments that question a strict sequence of grammar drills, the emphasis on output practice remains wide- spread largely because it is supported by entrenched beliefs among foreign language teachers: the belief that the grammar of a lan- guage consists of a series of isolated facts called grammar points, and the conviction that gram- mar is eminently teachable, that is, profitably taught through explanations of grammar rules. Unfortunately, in their attempt to capture grammatical knowledge in the form of explicit rules, teachers frequently mistake the rule itself for what it actually represents-the mental proc- ess of the speaker. Current textbook rules and classroom practices used in the teaching of grammar pay scant attention to the speakers mental processes and thus forfeit any chance of attacking meaning on its home ground (Langacker, 1987, p. 99). In fairness to teachers, however, some degree of reification is common to virtually all approaches to grammar. For ex- ample, Langacker (1994) points out that key terms commonly used in linguistic discussions are invariably nouns: language, thought, con- cept, cognition, structure, construction, and so forth. Nevertheless, Langacker maintains that when he uses a term like concept in his linguistic analyses, he does not envisage a fixed, static entity like a physical object lodged somewhere in the brain (p. 25) but rather, a dynamic men- tal process.

    On the other hand, Rutherford (1987) argues that a fixed, static entity is exactly what most traditional language teachers envisage when they use the term grammar. He refers to the common-place reification of grammar in for-

    The Modern Language Journal 81 (1997)

    eign and second language instruction as the accumulation of entities (p. 17). According to Rutherford, traditional approaches to the teaching of grammar are predicated on the be- liefs that language is composed of discrete en- tities and that the essential characteristics of the entities (e.g., the rules for their formation) can be directly imparted to the learner: For pur- poses of teaching language form, it would seem, one has to get a handle on something, and the most natural kind of thing to try to grasp in this way is a solid, stable, fixed piece of the total language product-something with edges to it . . . in other words, a language con- struct (p. 56).

    There are many teachers, however, who have grown wary of a traditional, teacher-centered approach to explicit grammar instruction. Re- jecting the traditional belief that grammar is acquired through an explicit examination of a rule followed by application of the rule in the form of a mechanical drill, many teachers have opted instead for a so-called comprehension- based pedagogy in which students come to know the grammar through exposure to com- prehensible input (Krashen, 1982). Although comprehension-based approaches to instruc- tion may vary greatly in how instructors attempt to render the input comprehensible, they are similar in their reduced emphasis on formal grammatical analysis. Consequently, compre- hension-based approaches are often described as shifting the pedagogical focus from form to meaning. Today, the terms focus on form and focus on meaning are frequently understood as a shorthand for two competing ideologies within the foreign language teaching profes- sion, the two extremes of an ongoing debate over the efficacy of grammar instruction. Un- fortunately, the competing camps and the de- bate itself promote a dichotomous conception of grammar instruction as described by Connor (1992):

    On the one hand, we have concentrated on linguis- tic form with explicit instruction in grammar rules at the level of individual, decontextualized sen- tences, accompanied by some drill of learned forms and by the fervent hope that these forms would prove accessible if the student found himself in a true, communicative situation. On the other hand, spurning traditional grammar instruction as irrele- vant to acquisition, we have endeavored to offer comprehensible input and guidance in the some- what inscrutable art of communicative competence, all the while hoping that comprehensible, profi- cient output would emerge. (p. 31)

  • Grl Bhth

    RECONCEPTUALIZING GRAMMAR TEACHING

    VanPatten and Cadierno (1993) claim that the debate between traditional and nontradi- tional approaches to grammar misses the point. The question is not whether grammar should be taught, but how it should be taught. Before teachers can address this important question, they must understand the complexity and het- erogeneity of grammatical concepts. The either /or approach to grammar instruction-either you teach it or you dont-indicates a profound misconception of grammar as a monolithic and homogeneous phenomenon, either wholly am- enable to instruction or not amenable at all. Rather, a pedagogical grammar is more profita- bly conceived of as a heterogeneous group of linguistic and psychological phenomena that may be amenable to formal instruction, but to varying degrees and in varying ways. Such a conception of grammar would require teachers to determine more thoughtfully how grammar should be taught, including which grammatical phenomena respond best to form-focused in- struction, and which will be acquired without explicit focus if learners have adequate expo- sure to the language (Lightbown & Spada, 1993, p. 99).

    In order to construct a more effective gram- mar pedagogy, teachers must first address the deleterious effects of their traditional concep- tion of grammar instruction as an accumula- tion of entities. Although such an approach has a certain heuristic appeal, it ultimately conceals the dynamic relationship between grammar and the mind. Garrett (1986) argues that the perennial debate concerning explicit grammar instruction rarely addresses the crucial ques- tion of the psychological status of linguistic rules:

    When we complain that teaching students grammar rules does not enabIe them to communicate, we only confirm what linguistic theory has implied all along: the rules which describe the system attested to by competence are abstract descriptive generaliz- ations that do not per se describe the mental proc- ess by which a speaker formulates or comprehends any particular utterance. (p. 138)

    Garrett advocates a processing approach to grammar instruction and argues for the rele- vance of psycholinguistic theory to pedagogical praxis. Similarly, Rutherford (1987) calls for a process-oriented conceptualization of grammar- grammar as a mental strategy for the process- ing of discourse (p. 153).

    53

    Based on studies of input processing, the cog- nitive processes that learners employ to com- prehend meaning-bearing input, VanPatten (1996) suggests that instruction be based on structured input activities in which learners are given the opportunity to process form in the input in a controlled situation so that better form-meaning connections might happen com- pared with what might happen in less con- trolled situations (p. 60). Structured input is the centerpiece of what VanPatten refers to as processing instruction, an approach to gram- mar instruction that combines a traditional focus on form with comprehensible input in an attempt to alter the processing strategies that learners take to the task of comprehension and to encourage them to make better form- meaning connections than they would if left to their own devices (p. 60).

    OBSTACLES TO INNOVATION IN GRAMMAR TEACHING

    What would happen if teacher education pro- grams took the suggestions of Garrett, Ruther- ford, and VanPatten seriously and attempted to promote a more process-oriented grammar in- struction? Unfortunately, current textbooks and current models of teacher education often hinder rather than promote pedagogical inno- vation. Is such an approach to grammar even feasible given current textbooks? How much can an inexperienced teacher be expected to teach against a traditional textbook? Further- more, can teacher educators reasonably expect inexperienced teachers to adopt an approach to grammar instruction that they have never ex- perienced themselves? What would it take to convince teachers of the efficacy and appro- priateness of a more learner-centered, process- oriented approach to grammar instruction? What obstacles prevent the adoption of innova- tive ways to teach aspect?

    Textbooks as Obstacles

    Hubbard (1994) claims that the conception and presentation of pedagogical grammar has remained virtually unchanged for centuries. Replacing a static conception of grammar with a more dynamic one will not be easy given the current treatment of grammar in textbooks: a brief grammar explanation followed by a se- quence of form-focused drills. Textbooks have a particularly strong influence on inexperienced teachers, shaping their teaching practices and

  • 54

    even their beliefs about language learning. Of- ten an inexperienced teachers metalinguistic knowledge is simply a reflection of the text- books grammatical explanations. Insecure about their grammatical knowledge, inex- perienced teachers tend to view linguistic rules as the business of the professional linguist and not of the practitioner. As a consequence, they accept the textbooks rules at face value, rarely examining the criteria used in their formula- tion: clarity, simplicity, predictive value, con- ceptual parsimony, and relevance ( Westney, 1994, p. 72). Hubbard (1994) notes, however, that the few recent improvements to pedagogi- cal grammars have come primarily from lan- guage teachers rather than from theoretical lin- guists (p. 49).

    Many textbooks not only present grammar ac- cording to Paulstons outdated taxonomy of drills but frequently contain highly misleading and, at times, inaccurate grammatical informa- tion. For example, Herschensohn (1988) found that in many first-year French textbooks, dis- course phenomena, such as the choice of defi- nite and indefinite articles, were routinely ex- plained in sentential terms. Another common problem with textbooks is the confusion be- tween the separate but related grammatical cat- egories of tense and aspect. Garrett (1986) notes that traditional labels used in textbooks to discuss aspect are seriously misleading as explanations, sometimes actually wrong. She cites the example of the verb forms imperfect and present perfect which are said to repre- sent different tenses, but in fact they distinguish aspect (p. 140).

    Textbook explanations of aspect may be problematic for an inexperienced teacher, yet they present even greater problems for the lin- guistically naive student. For example, Dan- sereau (1987) blames vague, incomplete, con- tradictory, and generally poor explanations found in most beginning textbooks (p. 35) for much of the confusion surrounding aspectual choice. The most serious problem with text- book explanations, Dansereau argues, is that they are exception-ridden. Leaky rules are a common problem in grammatical analysis, es- pecially in pedagogical grammars, which are ill- suited to capturing the variability inherent in language perf~rmance.~ In this regard, Westney (1994) distinguishes between low-level rules of formation and high-level rules of use. Low-level rules present few problems for the learner be- cause they are axiomatic-easy to understand, easy to apply. By contrast, high-level rules de-

    The Modern Language Journal 81 (1997)

    scribing aspectual choice are essentially proba- bilistic statements that prove difficult, if not im- possible, to apply in any principled way.

    It can be frustrating for both student and teacher alike to try to apply aspectual rules that are based on descriptive terms such as con- tinuing event, durative event, punctual event, single event, repeated event, and so forth. Dansereau (1987) argues that textbook rules based on these descriptive terms often have trouble accounting for even ordinary sen- tences such as the following:

    1. Le mi a rignipendant soixante am.

    2. A huit hares, jitais dam nwn bureau. (The king reigned (preterit) for sixty years.)

    (At eight oclock, I was (imperfect) in my office.)

    (He often came (preterit) to see me.) 4. Cet it&& il ne manpait que o h x fois par jour.

    (That summer, he only ate (imperfect) two times a day.) (p. 34-5)

    When encountering sentence 1, the student wonders why an event that continued for sixty years is encoded in the preterit and not the im- perfect, since the imperfect is prescribed for durative or continuing events. In sentence 2, the student puzzles over the use of the imper- fect, which is supposedly reserved for situations without any reference to an exact moment of time. In sentence 3, the student finds the pret- erit used with an event repeated an indetermi- nate number of times (often) even though the textbook rule states that the preterit encodes an event repeated a determinate number of times. In sentence 4, the imperfect is used even though the number of times is clearly stated (two times a day), a contradiction of the text book rule prescribing the imperfect for an event repeated an indeterminate number of times. Dansereau (1987) concludes from these examples that the traditional descriptive termi- nology dooms the student to confusion, frus- tration, and incorrect usage (p. 37).

    An even more serious charge is leveled by Garrett (1986) who claims that a traditional presentation of rules found in most textbooks hinders not only students direct processing of meaning but even their realization of how such processing might be undertaken (p. 142). In other words, textbook rules often lead students to draw the wrong conclusions about how aspec- tual meaning maps onto linguistic form in the minds of native speakers. For example, Dan- sereau (1987) notes that the use of the terms

    3. n at souvent venu me V O ~ Z

  • Carl Blyth

    state versus action, as in the imperfect is used primarily for states and the preterit is used primarily for actions, (p. 36) inevitably leads students to confuse inherent lexical aspect, the intrinsic nature of an event, with grammatical aspect. Inherent lexical aspect and grammatical aspect are often distinguished poorly in text- books because they exhibit a strong correlation in actual usage-inherently punctual verbs are most frequently encoded in perfective aspect and inherently stative verbs are most frequently encoded in imperfective aspect. Of course, stu- dents must learn that regardless of its inherent lexical aspect, any verb may be grammatically encoded for perfectivity or imperfectivity. Given that textbooks are not likely to change their presentation of aspect in the near future, it is largely the responsibility of teacher educa- tion programs to help teachers construct new practices and explanations for the teaching of aspectual distinctions.

    55

    Teaching is understood to be analogous to skilled activity in general; it develops primarily through practice and exposure to the activity of experts (Kinginger, 1995, p. 125). For TAs who hold a reductionist view of teaching as a set of techniques to be mastered, the craft model is often preferred for its perceived practicality. In the craft model, TAs apprenticing with master teachers are encouraged to follow demon- strated teaching practices as closely as possible. Kaplans (1993) autobiographical account of her own professional development as a French TA learning the standard rites of pedagogy provides a good example of several specific practices designed to teach the difference be- tween the passt? composi and the imparfait;

    You learn to draw a time line. You go up to the blackboard, and its dramatic, and you say, this is the imperfect: the imperfect is for description; its for events that havent finished. The time it takes to say this is just about the time it takes to drag your chalk line, slowly, all the way across the board. You pick up your chalk and you explain, chalk in hand, that the imperfect is used to describe feelings, states of being; its used to describe background, landscape, and ongoing thoughts. All sorts of things with no definite beginning and end. Then you pause, take hold of your chalk piece like a weapon, and you stab that blackboard line at one point, then at another. This is the passe compose, this staccato: a point on the imperfect line of expe- rience, a discrete action in the past with a begin- ning and an end that you can name. (p. 142)

    One of the major problems with the craft model is that the TA may learn how to perform such rituals flawlessly although never gaining an understanding of their motivation. In the case of the practices described by Kaplan, inex- perienced teachers rarely stop to consider why aspect is typically represented in visual mne- monics while most grammar points are ex- plained primarily in words. Since the role of the apprentice is to imitate practice not critique it, the craft model does little to promote a critical evaluation of pedagogy. As a consequence, the craft model, when employed by itself as the basis for teacher education, actually reinforces pedagogical tradition by passing on long-held yet unexamined practices.

    The craft model by itself also fails to provide TAs with important pedagogical content knowl- edge about linguistic structure and language learning. A recent survey of graduate TAs in French at various universities found that TAs generally lacked important metalinguistic knowledge despite a strong emphasis on gram-

    Teacher Educaticm Programs as Obstach

    It seems ironic that teacher education pro- grams may actually prove to be obstacles to sub- stantive pedagogical change. But it is never an easy task to convince teachers to adopt new practices and, given the constraints imposed on many teacher education programs, profound change is often unrealistic. Because of institu- tional pressures, those involved in university language program direction feel obliged to focus methods courses on the immediate needs and concerns of the institution rather than on long-term professional development of TAs (Gore11 & Cubillos, 1993; Pons, 1993). Thus, teacher education frequently becomes teacher training with little time devoted to a critical evaluation of teaching practices and the devel- opment of more imaginative or sophisticated styles of being language teachers (Marks, 1993,

    Kinginger (1995) claims that two common models of teacher education-the craft model and the applied science model-are poorly suited to engendering lasting pedagogical change because they lack the necessary ele- ments of personal experience and reflection: the uaft model . . . emphasizes imitation and emulation of the experts professional wisdom, and the applied science model . . . focuses on a professions received knowledge, the facts to be found in journals, textbooks-and courses on education (p. 125). The craft model of teacher education is essentially atheoretical:

    p. 3).

  • 56

    matical analysis throughout their own language learning experience (Fox, 1993). In particular, Foxs survey revealed that the model of lan- guage with which TAs begin their careers ig- nores discourse competence as a distinct level of grammatical organization. As a result, TAs are prone to conceive of grammar as comprised of distinct entities that are adequately de- scribed at a sentential level. The results of Foxs finding have particularly disturbing implica- tions for the teaching of aspect: Despite years of traditional grammar instruction, TAs are often unable to give a succinct and coherent explana- tion of aspect because they lack an understand- ing of its discourse basis. To fill the knowledge gap, Fox suggests that TAs receive an introduc- tion to linguistic description of the target lan- guage as part of their methods course in order to raise their awareness about grammatical phe- nomena governed by discourse principles.

    Scholars have recently called for TA training programs to include a greater emphasis on sec- ond language acquisition theory and applied linguistic research (Fox, 1992, 1993; Rankin, 1994). In an effort to introduce TAs to the latest theoretical research and its implications for classroom practice, some educators adopt a ver- sion of the applied science model. Kinginger refers to this model as top-down since teachers are envisioned as the consumers of research produced by a group of experts. Unfortunately, methods courses based on the applied science model tend to reinforce the gap between theory and practice in the minds of many teachers who frequently judge such courses as too theoretical and therefore too impractical. More important, the suggested pedagogical applications of the research go unheeded because the TAs are given little, if any, practical experience.

    The M h Language Journal 81 (1997)

    with current research is to challenge their tradi- tional beliefs about the nature of grammar- what grammar is and how it is learned. A con- structivist approach is particularly appropriate for TA education in this regard because it allows TAs to acquire essential linguistic and ped- agogical content knowledge-the technical facts-as well as an awareness of their own be- liefs about foreign language learning. Fosnot (1996~) argues that the primary goal of a con- structivist approach to teacher education is to facilitate new ways of knowing:

    If understanding the teaching/learning process from a constructivist view is itself constructed, and if teachers tend to teach as they were taught, rather than as they were taught to teach, then teacher edu- cation needs to begin with these traditional beliefs and subsequently challenge them through activity, reflection, and discourse in both coursework and field work through the duration of the program. Most importantly, participants need experiences as learners that confront traditional views of teaching and learning in order to enable them to construct a pedagogy that stands in contrast to older, more tra- ditionally held views. (p. 206)

    If personal experience ultimately plays the most important role in facilitating change in a teachers practices, then instead of being told about practices, TAs should directly experience teaching practices. Rather than observe a mas- ter instructor who demonstrates practices on other students, TAs should experience new practices as a learner would. Thus, TA educa- tors should demonstrate a given practice on the TAs themselves who in turn come to understand the practice from the learners perspective. Fi- nally, TAs must be given the opportunity to re- flect on their experiences as learners. The goal is to help TAs integrate the scientific facts concerning aspect with their personal experi- ences through a period of critical reflection (Wallace, 1991). A CONSTRUCTIVIST APPROACH TO TA EDUCATION

    Kinginger (1995), following Wallace (1991), uses the term reflective practitioner to charac- terize the ideal teacher who integrates research, theory, and practical experiences through in- formed, critical reflection. This synthesis of dif- ferent ways of knowing is central to a constructiv- ist approach to learning. Implementing more effective ways to teach grammar requires teacher education programs to do more than simply provide models to be emulated (the craft model) and research to be applied (the applied science model). Rather, the key to persuading TAs to adopt a grammar pedagogy consonant

    TEACHING TAs ABOUT ASPECT

    In order to construct practices that more ef- fectively aid students in their understanding of aspectual choice, TAs must have a clear under- standing of aspectual phenomena including the conceptual knowledge underlying linguistic performance. To gain a more thorough under- standing of the relationship of aspect to cogni- tion, TAs benefit from a review of three related research areas: studies of L2 acquisition, cogni- tive linguistics, and gestalt visual perception. Although technical knowledge is essential for the

  • Carl Blyth

    construction of effective grammar instruction, TA educators must keep in mind that appren- tice teachers are unlikely to adopt a given prac- tice based solely on a review of research find- ings, however pertinent they may be. Thus, the presentation of basic linguistic knowledge should be seen as a preliminary stage in foreign language teacher development, a means to an end.

    57

    use present tense, then pretknto, and finally imper- fecto.6 Andersen notes, however, that when learners of Spanish first begin to employ the pret- erit and the imperfect, they do so according to inherent lexical aspect and prototypicality, using the pretkito for prototypical punctual events and the imperfecto for prototypical states.

    A prototype is identified by a set of charac- teristic features, which define it as the best ex- emplar of its category. For example, events may be characterized by three semantic features as seen in Table 1: dynamic, telic, and punctual. A dynamic event requires some energy to sustain it; a telic event describes an activity with a clear terminal point; and a punctual event is instan- taneous or momentary.

    Aspect in Second L a n p g z Acquisition Research

    TAs routinely confuse aspect with tense. Therefore, a discussion of aspect should begin by distinguishing these two grammatical cate- gories. Tense is the grammatical category com- monly used in linguistic analysis to refer to the way a language encodes the time at which an action denoted by a verb takes place. Thus, tense is concerned with situating events in ref- erence to other events, in other words, with or- dering events along a timeline. Aspect, on the other hand, is not concerned with temporal points of reference, but rather with the differ- ent ways of viewing the internal temporal con- stituency of a situation (Comrie, 1976, p. 3). In most approaches to aspect, the differences be- tween the perfective aspect and imperfective aspect are explained in terms of the speakers perspective. Perfective aspect is equated with an external perspective from which the speaker perceives the event as a self-contained whole. From such an external perspective, one may envision an events boundaries or outlines-its beginning and end. In contrast, imperfective aspect reflects a situation as seen from an internal perspective; the speaker views the situa- tion from within and is unable to distinguish temporal boundaries.4

    TAs need to understand that students who confuse inherent lexical aspect (i.e., the intrin- sic nature of an event) with grammatical aspect have posited an incorrect hypothesis about the morphological system of the target language: the passt? compmt?/petirito tense encodes actions and the impa.f.it/zmperficto tense encodes states. This hypothesis is widely attested in early stages of interlanguage development and is known as the Defective Tense Hypothesis following Weist et al. (1984) because verb tense morphology is not used to encode tense or grammatical as- pect, but rather inherent lexical aspect (An- dersen, 1990, p. 307).5 Based on studies of Span- ish interlanguage, Andersen (1990) claims that learners pass through stages in the acquisition of the Spanish tense/aspect system; learners first

    TABLE 1 Semantic Feature Analysis of Events

    ~ ~ ~

    to to to paint to have run a picture recognize

    + punctual - - -

    dynamic - + + + telic - - + +

    Note. From Anderson, 1990, pp. 310-311.

    Based on these three features, events can be arranged along a continuum. On one end of the continuum, to have possesses none of the rel- evant semantic features while at the opposite end, to recognize possesses all three features. Andersens claim is that the usage of the pretirito in second language learning spreads from situa- tions characterized by all three semantic fea- tures (to recognize) to situations charac- terized by two features (to paint a picture), then to situations characterized by only one fea- ture (to run), and finally, at a relatively ad- vanced stage, to situations characterized by none of the relevant features (to have). In a similar fashion, the marking of the imperfecto be- gins with situations lacking all three semantic features and spreads in the opposite direction, eventually to situations possessing all three fea- tures. Thus, the two maximally differentiated events in Table 1-to have and to recognize- are prototypes of the learners incipient aspec- tual categories-imperfictu and petirito.

    Andersens (1993) finding concerning Spanish L2 discourse is reflective of the discourse of na- tive Spanish speakers as well. Fully proficient na- tive speakers exhibit a similar distributional bias in the classes of verbs to which they attach pret- erit and imperfect verbal inflections. For exam- ple, there is a much higher frequency of imper-

  • 58

    fect than preterit usage with stative verbs and there is a much higher frequency of preterit use than imperfect use with punctual verbs. TAs must understand that statistical properties of language performance are caused by a speakers underlying conceptual kndwledge. Any attempt to explain perfectivity and imperfectivity in terms of correlations with verb classes in the speech of native speakers (a common practice of many TAs) ultimately confuses cause with effect.

    The M o d a Language Journal 81 (1997)

    sequently, a cognitive grammar takes the speakers perception of events as the natural point of departure for explaining aspectual choice: Within this framework, meanings are defined relative to conceptual domains, partic- ular linguistic choices are often found to hinge upon the vantage point from which a given situ- ation is viewed, and category boundaries are seen as fluctuating and dependent on, among other things, the conceptualizers experience or purpose (Rudzka-Ostyn, 1988, p. ix).7 Lan- gacker (1987) argues that experiences that are basic, recurrent, and sharply differentiated have a special perceptual saliency and as a con- sequence emerge as archetypes. Goldberg (1995) notes that these archetypes are first equated with prototypes and then are extended beyond the original prototypical values. An- dersens (1990) findings about Spanish inter- language, that prototypical uses of the preterit and imperfect are learned before their less pro- totypical uses, is thus in keeping with the basic claims of cognitive linguistics.

    Aspect in Cognitive Linguistic Research

    The challenge for any pedagogical grammar of aspect is to capture a speakers underlying conceptual knowledge of events or situations in a rule whose usage is clear, simple, and highly pre- dictable. Capturing these abstractions in a ped- agogical rule that proves comprehensible to stu- dents requires a radical reformulation of our notion of grammar and of the operation of grammatical concepts . . . (Garrett, 1986, p. 145). Fortunately for language teachers, the outlines of such a reformulation already exist in cognitive-functionalist grammars. The cognitive- functionalist approach reflects a shift from the prevailing conception of linguistic knowledge characterized by Chomskyan competence as for- mal, abstract, and modular toward a conception of language as the reflection of human con- sciousness (Bolinger, 1977; Chafe, 1993; Faucon- nier, 1985; Foley & Van Valin, 1984; Givbn, 1984, 1989; Haiman, 1985; Hopper & Thompson, 1980; Lakoff, 1987; Lambrecht, 1994; Langacker, 1987, 1991; Wierzbicka, 1988). Neither a standard nor a formalized linguistic theory, the cognitive-func- tionalist approach is characterized by the follow- ing premises: (a) languages are systems primarily used for communication, (b) linguistic forms are best studied in terms of their semantic and prag- matic functions, and (c) actual language use consists of multipropositional speech whose organization is guided by discourse principles (Cooreman & Kilborn, 1990; Tomlin, 1994). In general, cognitive-functionalist approaches to language attempt to determine the semantic or pragmatic conditions that lead to the selection of alternative grammatical structures such as the perfective and the imperfective aspect. Tomlin (1994) claims that such an approach to grammar is consonant with the basic assumptions of com- municative language teaching.

    Scholars working within this framework theo- rize that language structure is directly associ- ated with conceptual structure, which in turn reflects scenes basic to human experience. Con-

    Aspect in Visual Perception Research

    Why do aspectual distinctions lend them- selves to a visual representation? How does the visual representation of a grammar rule help or hinder the learning of that rule? What is the semiotic relationship between Kaplans lines and dashes drawn on the blackboard and the concepts of perfectivity and imperfectivity? Vir- tually every discussion of aspect invokes meta- phors of visual perception (Andrews, 1992; Con- nor, 1992; Fleischman, 1990; Lunn, 1985; Ozete, 1988; Paprotti, 1988; Terry, 1981; Thogmartin, 1984). And in cognitive linguistic theory, the existence and comprehensibility of metaphors linking aspect and perception are meant to be taken as evidence that the aspect-perception link is real (Lunn, 1985, p. 52). Some of the strongest evidence for the reality of the link be- tween aspect and perception is described by Rein- hart (1984) who claims that narrative structure is an artifact of visual perception:

    . . . [narrative] organization is a temporal extension of the principles governing the spatial organization of the visual field into figure and ground, proposed by the gestalt theory, and [ . . . ] there is a striking correlation between the perceptual criteria deter- mining the figure and those determining the narra- tive foreground. (p. 779)

    Andersen (1993) makes a similar claim in his explanation of the distributional bias of verbal inflections in Spanish interlanguage discourse:

  • Carl Blyth

    The perceptual systems of humans and other ani- mals allow or perhaps we might say force us to dis- tinguish an important or foregrounded entity or event from all of the unimportant or less important events . . . According to this basic notion of distin- guishing figure from ground, we would say that the learner perceives the punctual or telic events as key, important, foreground, and learns to mark them as such and to not mark the background events or situations. (p. 328)

    59

    [Figures 1 through 5 are from finczpks of Gestalt Psychology by Kurt Koffka, copyright 1935 by Harcourt Brace & Company and renewed in 1963 by Elizabeth Koffka, reproduced by per- mission of the publisher.]

    FIGURE 1 Continuous Contours/Privileged Interpretation

    Reinharts and Andersens basic claim is that some experiences are perceptually more salient than others. These salient events are conceived of in terms of a foreground that stands out against a background of less salient, out-of- focus events. In gestalt theory, visual perception of a figure defined as an intuitive notion of recognizable form, depends on the relevant background (PaprottC, 1988, p. 458). In other words, we are able to recognize a figure or per- ceive a form because the background enables it to stand out. The functional dependency of the figure on the ground may be conceived of like a black dot on a computer screen. As one black- ens the screen, the dot becomes less and less perceptible, eventually disappearing altogether into the background.

    Following Labov (1972), Reinhart equates the foregrounded events of a narrative with the se- quence of chronologically ordered main clauses encoded in perfective aspect, the so-called back- bone or plotline of the story. In essence, all nar- ratives depend on perfective events to advance the plot. In contrast, backgrounded clauses in the imperfective aspect do not advance the plot since they are not temporally ordered and may be displaced within the text without changing the temporal order of the story. Reinhart argues that the three criteria of foregrounded events- sequentiality, punctuality, and perfectivity-are related to principles of gestalt perception of the figure. For example, the criterion of temporal sequentiality finds a spatial analogue in the ge- stalt principle of good continuity. This principle states that we organize shapes according to con- tours and that continuous contours are given highest priority in visual perception. In Figure 1, we see a white stripe on black stripes rather than four black figures, since the white stripe is the continuous shape. Similarly, events that are tem- porally sequenced are easy to identify on the grounds of good continuity. This phenomenon, the perception of sequential events in terms of a continuous contour, is apparent in the plotline and backbone metaphors so common to discus- sions of narrative.

    According to gestalt theory, smaller areas are perceived more readily as figures and larger areas are perceived more readily as back- ground. The temporal equivalent of spatial size is the duration of an event. Punctual events are temporally smaller and are therefore readily perceived as figures against the temporally larger durative event. In Figure 2, the image is ambiguous because the black and white areas are approximately the same size. Is it a black cross on a white background or vice versa? The mind readily entertains both interpretations.

    FIGURE 2 Ambiguous Figure/Ground Relationship

    In Figure 3, however, the mind entertains only one interpretation-a smaller white cross (the figure) is distinguished from a larger black background.

    FIGURE 3 Unambiguous Figure/Ground Relationship

    The principle of size is related to the princi- ple of closure, which states that the more enclosed an area is, the more it is figure-like. Note that completed or perfective events are

  • 60

    bounded or closed on both ends (their begin- ning and ends are in-focus). In Figure 4, we see three thin figures and a remaining line al- though other interpretations are possible (e.g., three thick figures).

    The M0ah-n Language Journal 81 (1997)

    to restore that balance and are ready to enter- tain alternative approaches to explaining as- pectual choice. Through a series of experience- based activities followed by personal reflection (reflective abstraction is the driving force of learning), I help TAs understand how gram- matical form is linked to aspectual meaning. I introduce the TAs to a technique described in Connor (1992), which extends already estab- lished, discourse-level notions of aspectual con- trast based on plot versus background (Dan- sereau, 1987; Thogmartin, 1984 ) or intrigue vs. arrike plan by rendering such notions concrete through visualization (p. 323). The greatest ad- vantage of this technique is that it reduces the various contradictory rules found in most text- books into one concrete, visualizable template in which figure vs. ground equals plot vs. back- ground equals [preterit] vs. [imperfect] (p. 323).

    Beginning foreign language students typ- ically decide between the two competing past tenses based on the most immediate of con- texts, the inherent aspect of the verb, thereby ignoring the larger narrative discourse (Garcia &van Putte, 1988). TAs can distinguish an inap- propriate or infelicitous aspectual choice, but nevertheless have difficulty explaining its inap- propriateness or infelicity due to their lack of a transparent metalanguage and to their limited understanding of the role of the preterit and the imperfect in the construction of a narrative text. To overcome these problems, I discuss the preterit and imperfect tenses with TAs almost exclusively in terms of the role these aspectual categories play in creating the foreground/ background structure of narrative. This rela- tively simple distinction is easily demonstrated by asking a TA to give a plot summary of a film (preferably a mystery). With the plotline events written on the board, TAs soon understand that the temporal sequence of events implies causal- ity and that a change in the chronology of events would lead to a change in the meaning of the story, sometimes destroying the storys co- herence altogether. Establishing the actual order of the events is of utmost importance to the plot of any mystery.

    In order to contrast the sequentiality of fore- ground events to the nonsequentiality of back- ground events, I have TAs read short newspaper articles about recent crimes or accidents. Not only is this type of text characterized by a straight forward plotline, but it also includes crucial supporting information (e.g., the driver was drunk at the time of the accident; the burg- lar was wearing a black ski mask; the assailant

    FIGURE 4 Ambiguous Partially Bounded Figures

    II (I II I When the space is more fully enclosed as in

    Figure 5, the ambiguity is eliminated; we see only three rectangular figures with one remain- ing bracket on the far left.

    FIGURE 5 Unambiguous Fully Bounded Figure

    TEACHING TAs TO TEACH ASPECT

    The review of pertinent research on aspect should help TAs gain a better understanding of the link between a speakers subjective percep- tion of events and his or her aspectual choices. My ultimate goal as a TA educator, however, is to help the TA translate the newly acquired technical understanding of aspect into teaching practice. In order to accomplish this, appren- tice teachers must be allowed to raise their own questions about the instruction of aspect and to test their own hypotheses about what is and is not an effective practice. In keeping with a con- structivist approach to TA education, my role is not so much to teach teachers how to teach as- pect, but rather to facilitate and guide TAss own construction of teaching practices.

    Disequilibrium and Rejbctiue Abstraction

    I begin my lessons on the instruction of as- pect by challenging the TAss received wisdom concerning the preterit/imperfect distinction (disequilibrium facilitates learning). Recalling Dansereaus (1987) critique of textbook expla- nations of aspect, I give counterexamples to ev- ery so-called usage rule that the TAs are able to formulate. With their received wisdom fully un- balanced (disequilibrium), TAs naturally seek

  • Carl Bbth

    was known to one of his victims, etc.). Visual mnemonic devices are used to further reinforce the narrative functions of the foreground and background (Sharwood Smith, 1988c; Westfall & Foerster, n.d.). While reading the stories, TAs must draw an arrow (+) above all verbs that move the plot forward and a circle (0) above all verbs that do not advance the plot. Next, the TAs list the foreground events (e.g., the events indicated by an arrow) in chronological order and determine the scope of the background events. Upon reflection, TAs discover that some background events have scope over the entire story, while others have scope over a sequence of events, and still others have scope over a sin- gle event. TAs also discover during this activity that the wider the scope of a background event, the easier it is to displace the event to other points in the narrative.

    61

    Fleischman (1990) clarifies this point with her definition of narrative event as a hermeneutic construct for converting an undifferentiated continuum of the raw data of experience, or of the imagination, into the verbal structures we use to talk about experience: narrative, stories (p. 99). To make these ideas more concrete, I play a 60-second video clip (a television com- mercial with an obvious plotline) and ask the TAs to jot down their own versions of the story. The TAss narratives are compared and the dif- ferences in narrative structure are noted and analyzed. TAs must justify their aspectual choices to one another using the foreground/ background distinction as well as a visual meta- language borrowed from cinematography (close- up, wide angle shot, out-of-focus, etc.) rather than the traditional grammatical terminology. The sharing of viewpoints within the commu- nity of learners helps TAs to see how real world events (here, video events) are perceived by dif- ferent narrators and then transposed into sig- nificantly different narratives (dialogue within a community engenders further thinking). When TAs share with each other their different aspectual choices, they begin to understand why it is possible for students to represent the same events in different ways.

    Next, I hand out several brief narrative ver- sions of the same 60-second commercial, this time written by native speakers who had previ- ously viewed the video clip. When the TAs read the narratives written by native speakers, they have not only seen the narrated events with their own eyes, but they have discussed various ways of grounding these events (i.e., packaging the events as either part of the foreground or background). Other video clips are used in the same manner to help the TAs comprehend the steps by which native speakers express and com- prehend meaning in narrative discourse (Gar- rett, 1986, p. 138).

    Dialogue Within a Community of h a m

    When TAs are proficient at distinguishing background from foreground events in narra- tive, I ask them to remember a personal anec- dote that they would be willing to share in class. While replaying the anecdote in their minds eye, the TAs identify critical moments appear- ing in the foreground of the stream of events. Connor (1992) contends that perceived events are stored in memory as film-like images:

    elements in the film-like substrate of mental imag- ery which appeared closer, brighter, clearer, more in focus in the minds eye would be matched with plot-advancing events and thus the pass6 compose (Boyer, 1985) while those elements which appeared hazier, vaguer . . . less salient (Wallace, 1982, p. 205) would be matched to background states or events and hence, to the imparfait. (p. 322)

    Connor reports that students who use this visu- alization technique significantly increase the accuracy of their usage of the preterit and of the imperfect in oral and written production. As soon as TAs have replayed the incident and categorized the events as foreground or back- ground, they each tell their story to the class in the target language and explain their aspectual choices in terms of discourse structure.

    Although TAs begin to understand the link between aspectual choice and speaker perspec- tive, they may fail to grasp narration at a deeper level. It is important for TAs to understand that a narrative is the creation of a narrator who intentionally, although largely unconsciously, chooses what to attend to when perceiving the seamless web of history (Ong, 1982, p. 12).

    G?ntral Organizing Rinciples

    These activities are followed by a period of informed reflection during which TAs are en- couraged to derive general principles about language learning based on their personal ex- periences and their newly acquired scientific knowledge. TAs generally conclude that learn- ers must experience narrative events for them- selves in order to construct the concept of nar- rativity and the correlated concept of aspect. Reflecting on the hypothesis in cognitive lin- guistics that the encoding of any event is di-

  • 62

    rectly linked to the subjective perception of that event, TAs come to realize that the teach- ing of aspect must be phenomenologically grounded such that percepts of a given event (the impression of the event as perceived by the senses) must be linked to concepts of perfec- tivity and imperfectivity (the abstractions de- rived from specific instances). Thus, by self- consciously experiencing the process of narra- tion, TAs naturally develop central organizing principles of aspectual choice-the notions of cognitive processing and discourse-that closely reflect Rutherfords (1987) conceptualization of grammar as a mental strategy for the proc- essing of discourse.

    The quintessentially constructivist view of as- pect formulated by the TAs themselves contra- dicts long-standing pedagogical practices, most notably the common use of cloze passages to teach aspectual choice to beginning students. TAs realize that cloze passages and third person narratives are mediated by someone elses sub- jectivity and thus are inherently problematic. It matters little that the author is a famous writer and that the passage is from a well-known work of literature; a cloze passage essentially requires the student to reconstruct the mental processes of someone elses mind, to see events through someone elses eyes, a tricky task even for native speakers. Forced to fill in the correct aspec- tual choice without access to the authors mind or to the real life events, students inevitably re- sort to a strategy of playing the probabilities based on inherent lexical aspect and proto- typicality (Oh, its a stative verb so it must be imperfective.). As a consequence, the use of cloze passages in beginning language classes unwittingly reinforces the erroneous Defective Tense Hypothesis posited by most beginning language learners. Rather than cloze passages of fictional narratives or even nonfictional third person accounts of past experience, TAs now understand why first person true narrative representations or personal accounts of lived experience are more transparent for the learner and thus more effective for teaching aspect (Oller, 1993).

    Another key principle for the teaching of as- pect, which the TAs invariably adopt following a constructionist approach, is the importance of visual input. Ideally, visual information should accompany linguistic input so that learners may establish their own pragmatic mappings be- tween the visual concepts of figure and ground, the discourse concepts of foreground and back- ground, and the grammatical concepts of per-

    The Modern LanguageJournal81 (1997)

    fectivity and imperfectivity. Current models of cognition now recognize what language teach- ers have long taken for granted-that a given mental representation is linked to other repre- sentations in the mind in such a way that words are associated with images (Paivio, 1986). Al- though TAs are generally aware of the impor- tant role visual aids play in listening compre- hension and in vocabulary learning, they frequently do not consider the role visual repre- sentations might play in the acquisition of grammatical knowledge. Sharwood Smith (1988b) argues that . . . maximizing visual representa- tions to accompany, illustrate, and explain lin- guistic items may well improve learning a great deal . . . I (p. 213). This principle contradicts most textbooks reliance on written texts to ex- emplify aspectual distinctions. If aspect is based on gestalt principles of visual perception as Rein- hart claims, then learners must see the events in order to construe the foreground/background relationship between the events in a narrative. This means that the teaching of aspect should make as liberal a use of visual input as possible, and in particular, of films and video. Although recent trends in video and interactive multi- media appear promising, the importance of vis- ual input for the acquisition of grammatical concepts remains to be fully explored by most commercial publishing companies, two excep- tions being the videobased programs French In Action (Capretz, 1994) and Destinos (1992).

    CONCLUSION

    The constructivist approach to TA education advocated in this article is similar to the notion of raising grammatical consciousness as expli- cated by Sharwood Smith (1988a) and Ruther- ford (1987). In such an approach, all pedagogical materials and teaching practices are properly viewed as aids to learning but never as the ob- jects to be learned, an important distinction. Thus, TAs should not focus exclusively on mem- orizing specific rules and practices during their teacher preparation courses in order to repro- duce or perform them later in their classrooms, rather they should strive to understand the proc- ess by which grammatical forms map onto meanings. To teach aspect, TAs must self- consciously experience narration in order to envision aspect both as a formal system and as a process for creating meaning. The development of effective practices and materials for the teaching of aspect requires an understanding of the form/meaning correlations of a language

  • Carl Bhth

    but, more important, it requires an understand- ing of how native and nonnative speakers per- ceive events and then selectively construct a narrative out of their perceptual experiences.

    Teacher educators may worry that TAs will find the cognitive notions discussed here to be too abstract and too removed from the realities of classroom practice. In response to such rea- sonable objections, Rutherford (1987) reminds teachers and teacher trainers of the nature of language learning:

    . . . what happens inside ones head, as concepts are formed and transmitted in what we know as lan- guage, is an absolutely crucial concern for any edu- cational discipline that takes the nature of lan- guage itself as its point of departure. And language teaching is such a discipline. The sort of cognitive change that language teaching is intended to bring about-namely, the learning of a language-is one that is ultimately explainable only by recourse to the kinds of thmtical abstractionr that are needed for research into how the mind actually works. (p. 2, emphasis added)

    If teacher educators are serious about helping TAs reconstruct traditional grammar practices along cognitive lines, then they must accept the inevitable transfusion of psychology into the profession as recognized by Sharwood Smith (1988b): It has become increasingly evident in recent years that what is by convention termed applied linguistics, in that it has to do with for- eign language learning and instruction, should be as much applied psychology as applied lin- guistics . . . (p. 206).

    Lee and VanPatten (1995) observe that de- spite research that calls into question a tradi- tional presentation of grammar, most textbooks have changed very little in the past few decades:

    If we glance at language textbooks (including those that are described as communicative and or profi- ciency oriented), it appears that instruction in grammar adheres to several tenets rooted in behav- iorism (the belief in reinforcing good habits) and historical inertia (The common argument of Thats the way its been done for years and years and years). (p. 90)

    As long as textbooks remain unchanged, teach- ers will have a hard time changing how they teach grammar. Thus, teacher education pro- grams must clearly demonstrate the benefits of nontraditional materials and practices that are more closely informed by research insights from such allied fields as second language ac- quisition, psycholinguistics, sociolinguistics, and psychology. More specifically, the challenge is to help apprentice teachers understand the

    63

    advantages of an alternative approach to grammar instruction, one which seeks to link meaning with structure or form, moves the learner from input to output, and is more learner centered than what we see in traditional grammar instruction (Lee & VanPatten, 1995,

    The key to meeting this challenge lies in what our teachers believe about the nature of gram- mar, for without a transformation in teachers beliefs about grammar, there will be little change in grammar instruction. A constructiv- ist approach to teacher education is well- suited to instructional reform because it helps teachers deconstruct the traditional concep- tion of grammar and in its place reconstruct a new one, a conception of grammar as a mental process.

    p. 90).

    NOTES

    The term tense as commonly used in pedagogi- cal grammars of French and Spanish is a misnomer since the relevant distinction is aspectual and not temporal. See Nehls (1992) for a crosslinguistic anal- ysis of aspect as it relates to language pedagogy.

    2 In a report on the Canadian French immersion programs, Harley (1986) noted that students who had received from 1000 to 3500 hours of content instruc- tion had made minimal progress in marking aspect in the verb in the past (p. 73). Swain (1985) makes similar observations of French immersion students who continued to struggle with the correct choice of tense when narrating the events of a story.

    On the other hand, GivBn (1984) argues that it is unrealistic to expect rules based on natural language usage to be categorical (i.e. without exception): Cat- egories conform to their basic definitions in the ma- jority of cases, and rules obey their strict description more likely than not. But there is always a certain amount of messy residue left, one that does not seem to fit into the category/rule in the strictest sense of their definition. This is an old dilemma with cogniz- ing and perceiving organisms, having to do with how to process input categorically while allowing for fuzzy edges (p. 12). One attempt to systematize language variation is the variable rule, a statistical description of the relative influences of factors involved in the production of any given utterance. The variable rule was originally used for sociolinguistic research but has been extended to the analysis of interlanguage variability ( Tarone, 1988), although its psychological status remains controversial (Gregg, 1990).

    The linguistic distinction between the terms per- fective and imperfective is clarified by their ety- mologies. The term per-ctive comes from the past par- ticiple perfecturn of the Latin verb perficere (to do to

  • 64

    completion). The modern French infinitive parfaire and the modern English infinitive to pcrjit have the same sense (e.g., to make perfect and to complete). Although most English speakers are familiar with the evaluative sense of the verb, to improve something to the point of perfection, they are frequently unaware of the second sense intended by linguists, to do some- thing to the point of completion.

    This same hypothesis has been shown to be opera- tive in the acquisition of both the L1 and L2 verb inflectional system (Antinucci and Miller, 1976; Bronck- art and Sinclair, 1973; Economides, 1985) as well as in the evolution of creole languages (Bickerton, 1975, 1981; Givh, 1982).

    Kaplan (1987) claims an identical developmental sequence for the acquisition of French as a second language from prisent to

    For a cognitive linguistic approach to tense and aspect, see Lunn (1985) and Paprottt (1988).

    composi to impatfoit

    The Moakrn Language Journal 81 (1997)

    Chafe, W. (1993). Lliswurse, ~ I ~ U U W S S , and time: The frow and duplammnt of m c i o u c expmence in speuk- ing and writing. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Clark, C. (1988). Asking the right questions about teacher preparation: Contributions of research on teacher thinking. EduGafirmalZhxm~ 17,5- 12.

    Cohen, D. K., & Ball, K. L. (1990). Policy and practice: An overview. Educational Evaluation and Policy

    Comrie, B. (1976). Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni- versity Press.

    Connor, M. (1992). A processing strategy using visual representation to convey the pass6 compost/ imparfait distinction in French. ZnternutionalRe- vue of Applied Linguistics, 30, 321-328.

    Cooremen, A., & Kilborn, K. (1990). Functionalist lin- guistics: Discourse structure and language proc- essing in second language acquisition. In T. Huebner and C. Ferguson (Eds.), C70sscumts in second language acquisitwn and linguistic theories (pp. 195-224). Amsterdam and Philadelphia:

    John Benjamins. Dansereau, D. (1987). A discussion of techniques used

    in the teaching of the pass6 composcYimparfait distinction in French. The French Review, 61, 33- 38.

    Destim: An Znhoductiun to Spanish. (1992). Anneberg/ CPB Collection.

    Economides, P.J. (1985). The erpwsUm of tenre and aspect in the Englrsh interlanguage of a Viehurmese child Un- published master's thesis, UCLA.

    Fauconnier, G. (1985). Ma& spaces: Aspects of meaning umstncctMn in natural lan- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Fleischman, S. (1990). Tenre and narrativity: Fnrm medi- cvalperfmnce to moda fiction. Austin, TX: Uni- versity of Texas Press.

    Foley, W. A., &Van Valin, R. Jr. (1984). FunctMnal syntax and universd p m w . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Fosnot, C. T. (1996a). Preface. In C. Fosnot (Ed.), Ca-

    Analy~i~, 12, 327-345.

    5-hduim: zhq peqwtmq rmd pmdrre (pp. ix- xi). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Fosnot, C. T. (1996b). Constructivism: A psychological theory of learning. In C. Fosnot (Ed.), Cbnstrw tivirm: T?amy, perspectives, andjrrmtice (pp. 8-33). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Fosnot, C. T. (1996~). Teachers construct constructiv- ism: The center for constructivist teaching/ teacher preparation project. In C. Fosnot (Ed.), Constructivism: Theory, perspectives, and practice (pp. 205-216). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Fox, C. A. (1992). Toward a revised model of TA train- ing. In J. Walz (Ed.), Development and StLpnVrna of teaching assistunts in forkgn languap. AAUSC issues in language program direction (pp. 191- 207). Boston: Heinle.

    REFERENCES

    Andersen, R. (1990). Developmental sequences: The emergence of aspect marking in second lan- guage acquisition. In T. Huebner & C. Fer- guson (Eds.), Crosscurrents in second language

    Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Andersen, R. (1993). Four operating principles and

    input distribution as explanations for under- developed and mature morphological systems. In K. Hyltenstam &A. Viberg (Eds.), Aogression and regression in language: sociocultural, neuro- psychological, and linguistic perspectives (pp. 309- 339). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Andrews, B. (1992). Aspect in past tenses in English and French. Zntmnafional RNlle of A f l U Linguis-

    Antinucci, F., & R. Miller. (1976). How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child Language

    Bickerton, D. (1975). @namics ofa creole s y s t m . Cam- bridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of hnguage. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma Publishers.

    Bolinger, D. (1977). Meaning rand f m . London: Long- man.

    Boyer, H. (1985). L'tconomie des temps verbaux dans le discours narratif. L.e&n& nwobne, 53, 73- 89.

    Bronckart, J. P., & H. Sinclair. (1973). Time, tense, and aspect. coglutiun, 2,107-130.

    Capretz, P. J. (with Abetti, A., Germain, M. A., & Wy- lie, L.) (1994). French in actiun: A bepnning mrse in language and culture. (2nd ed.). New Haven, C T Yale University Press.

    Cazden, C. (1988). Clarmxmr discourn: The language .f teaching and /earning. Portsmouth, NH: Hei- nemann Educational Books, Inc.

    a tqu i s ih and linguistic thaories (pp. 305-324).

    tics, 30, 281-297.

    3, 169-189.

  • Carl Blyth

    Fox, C. A. (1993). Communicative competence and beliefs about language among graduate teach- ing assistants in French. The Modern Language

    Garcia, E. C., & van Putte, F. C. (1988). The value of contrast: contrasting value of strategies. Inter-

    Garrett, N. (1986). The problem with grammar: What kind can the language learner use? The Modern

    Givbn, T. (1982). Tense-aspect-modality: The creole prototype and beyond. In P. Hopper (Ed.),

    163). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Ben- jamins.

    Givbn, T. (1984). Syntax, vol. I. Amsterdam and Phila- delphia: John Benjamins.

    Givbn, T. (1989). Mind, code, and context: Essays in pmg- matics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Goldberg. A. (1995). Conshuctimrr: A colnshuGfion gram- mar appmach to argument stmcture Chicago: Uni- versity of Chicago Press.

    Gorell, L. C., & Cubillos, J. (1993). TA programs: The fit between foreign language teacher prepara- tion and institutional needs. In D. Benseler (Ed.), The dynumics of language pmgram directh. AAUSC issues in language program direction (pp. 91-109). Boston: Heinle.

    Gregg. K. (1990). The variable competence model of second language acquisition, and why it isnt. Applted Linguistics, 11,364-383.

    Haiman, J. (1985). Natural syntax: Iwnzcity and Ooswn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Harley, B. (1986). Age in second hnguage acyuisition. Avon, England Multilingual Matters.

    Herschensohn, J. (1988). Linguistic accuracy and text- book grammars: The determiner system in French. The Modern Language J m d , 72, 409- 414.

    Hopper, P., & Thompson, S. (1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Languagv, 56, 251-299.

    Hubbard, P. L. (1994). Non-transformational theories of grammar: Implications for language teach- ing. In T. Odlin (Ed.) Pmpectivcs on pedagogical grammar (pp. 49-71). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Kaplan, A. (1993). French hm: A memoir. Chicago: Chicago University Press,

    Kaplan, M. (1987). Developmental patterns of past tense acquisition among foreign language learners of French. In B. VanPatten, T. Dvorak, & J. Lee, (Eds.), Foreign languugv learning: A re- search Pe~pecCriVe (pp. 52-60). Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.

    Kinginger, C. (1995). Toward a reflective practice of TA education. In C. Kramsch (Ed.), Redefining the hndmies of language study. AAUSC issues in language program direction (pp. 123-144). Boston: Heinle.

    Krashen, S. D. (1982). Principles and practice in secund l a m aquisitim Oxford, England Pergamon.

    J o u ~ , 77, 313-324.

    ~ t i d Reuu of Applied Linguistics, 26, 263-280.

    Language JOUTTL~, 70, 133-148.

    T- Bebuaa se))~& ad- (pp. 115-

    65

    Labov, W. (1972). The transformation of experience in narrative syntax. In Language in thc i n w city (pp. 354-396). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Lakoff, G. (1987). Woma,fire, and other dangnmcs things: What catepia m d about the mind Chicago: Chi- cago University Press.

    Lambrecht, K. (1994). ln$mn&m shvctzlrc and senhce f017n:Afhemyofbfic,fonyandthemcntaln@zw&tm ofdismm @wn& Cambridge studies in linguis- tics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Langacker, R. W. (1987). Fmndationr of cognitive gram- mar: Themetical perspzctivcs. Stanford, CA: Stan- ford University Press.

    Langacker, R. W. (1991). F a c n d a h of cognitive gram- mar: Desmpiw afllicatia. Stanford, CA: Stan- ford University Press.

    Langacker, R. W. (1994). Culture, cognition, and grammar. In M. Piitz (Ed.), Language contact and language conjhct (pp. 25-53). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Lee, J., & VanPatten, B. (1995). Making communicative language teaching happen New York: McGraw Hill.

    Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (1993). How languap are Cearned Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Lunn, P. V. (1985). The aspectual lens. Htganic Linguis-

    Marks, E. (1993). Memory, desire, and pleasure in the classroom: La grande mademoiselle. MLA News-

    Nehls, D. (1992). An analysis of verbal aspect as a cross-linguistic category: Implications for lan- guage teaching. InternuCiollalRevueofApplred Lin-

    Oller, J. W. (1993). Reasons why some methods work. In J. W. Oller, Jr. (Ed.), Mcthodc that work: Idcatfor literacy and language tachers (pp. 374-386). Bos- ton: Heinle.

    Ong, W. (1982). Oral remembering and narrative structure. In D. Tannen (Ed.), spaken and written language: ExpIoring orality and literacy (pp. 12-24). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Ozete, 0. (1988). Focusing on the preterite and im- perfect. H e n i n , 71, 687-691.

    Paivio, A. (1986). Mental ngresmtah : A dual coding a*. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Paprotti, W. (1988). A discourse perspective on tense and aspect in standard modern Greek and English. In B. Rudzka-Ostyn (Ed.) Topicr in cog- nitive linguistics (pp. 447-506). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Paulston, C. B. (1972). Structural pattern drills: A classification. In H. Allen & R. Campell (Eds.), TmhingEngllSh as a second language (pp. 129-138). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Pons, Cathy R. (1993). TA supervision: Are we prepar- ing a future professoriate? In David P. Benseler (Ed.) The dynamics of language program directinn. AAUSCissves an languagvpmgmm direction (pp. 19- 31). Boston: Heinle.

    Rankin, J. (1994). Hit or miss, or missing: The role of

    tics, 2, 49-61.

    letur; 25, 3-4.

    g ~ i s l i c ~ , 30,255-280.

  • 66

    second language acquisition in language teacher training. Die Untenichtspaxis, 27, 18-27.

    Reinhart, T. (1984). Principles of gestalt perception in the temporal organization of narrative texts. Linguistics, 22, 779-809.

    Rudzka-Ostyn, B. (Ed.), (1988). Toplcs in mgtzitiue lin- guistics. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Rutherford, W. (1987). second languagegrammar: Learn- ing and teaching. New York: Longman.

    Sharwood Smith, M. (1988a). Consciousness raising and the second language learner. In W. Ruther- ford and M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Grummar andseamdlanguqeknching:A b m k o f d v ( p p . 51- 60). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Sharwood Smith, M. (1988b). Applied linguistics and the psychology of instruction: A case for trans- fusion? In W. Rutherford and M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Grammar and secund language teach- ing: A book of readings (pp. 206-223). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Sharwood Smith, M. (1988~). Imperfective versus progressive: An exercise in contrastive ped- agogical linguistics. In W. Rutherford and M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Grammar and second lan- guage teaching: A h k of readings (pp. 224-230). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible inputs and compre- hensible output in its development. In S. Gass & C. G. Madden (Eds.), Znput in second language acquisition (pp. 236-254). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Tarone, E. (1988). Variation in intmlanguage. London: Edward Arnold.

    Terry, R. (1981). Concepts of pastness: The @si com- posi and the imperfect. Foreign Language Annals,

    Thogmartin, C. (1984). Tense, aspect, and context. 14, 105-110.

    The French Revkw, 57, 344-349.

    The Modern Language Journal 81 (1997)

    Tomlin, R. (1994). Functional grammars, pedagogical grammars, and communicative language teach- ing. In T. Odlin (Ed.), Pmpctivar on pedagogical grammar (pp. 140-178). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    VanPatten, B. (1996). Input procRFsing and grammar in- struction in second language acquisition. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    VanPatten, B., & T. Cadierno. (1993). Explicit instruc- tion and input processing. Studies in second Lan- guage Acquisitiun, 15, 225-243.

    von Glaserfeld, E. (1996). Introduction: Aspects of constructivism. In C. Fosnot (Ed.), Constructiv- ism: Thewy, perspectives, and practice (pp. 3-7). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Wallace, M. (1991). Training fmign language teachers: A refictive approach Cambridge: Cambridge Uni- versity Press.

    Wallace, S. (1982). Figure and ground: The interrela- tionships of linguistic categories. In P. Hopper (Ed.), Tase-mfxct: Betwen semantics and pagmatics (pp. 201-223). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Weist, R. M., Wysocka, H., Witkowska-Stadnik, K., Buczowska, W., & Konieczna, E. (1984). The de- fective tense hypothesis: On the emergence of tense and aspect in child Polish. Journal of Child Language, 11, 347-374.

    Westfall, R., 8c Foerster, S. (n.d.). Beyond aspect: New strategies for teaching the preterite and the im- perfect. Unpublished manuscript, University of Texas.

    Westney, P. (1994). Rules and pedagogical grammar. In T. Odlin (Ed.), Pe@ectiues on pedapgical gram- mar (pp. 72-96). Cambridge: Cambridge Uni- versity Press.

    Wierzbicka, A. (1988). The semantics ofgramkr . Am- sterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    MLJ Offers No-Fee Copying for Course Packs

    THE MLJIS PLEASED TO REMIND READERS THERE IS NO COPYRIGHT FEE FOR USING ITS articles in course packs for students. We hope that this policy will encourage learning and scholar- ship by helping instructors make materials available to students. To constitute permission to use MLJ material just copy this announcement and place it in your course pack. There will continue to be a copyright charge for other use of MLJmaterial. For further information, contact:

    Margaret Walsh The University of Wisconsin Press 114 N. Murray Street Madison, WI 53715-1199 Tel: (608) 262-4925 Fax: (608 262-7560 Email: [email protected]