bulletin personality and social psychology

16
http://psp.sagepub.com/ Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology http://psp.sagepub.com/content/37/3/350 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/0146167211398139 2011 37: 350 Pers Soc Psychol Bull MarYam G. Hamedani, Hazel Rose Markus and Alyssa S. Fu My Nation, My Self: Divergent Framings of America Influence American Selves Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology can be found at: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Additional services and information for http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://psp.sagepub.com/content/37/3/350.refs.html Citations: at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011 psp.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Upload: truongkhanh

Post on 03-Jan-2017

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

http://psp.sagepub.com/Bulletin

Personality and Social Psychology

http://psp.sagepub.com/content/37/3/350The online version of this article can be found at:

 DOI: 10.1177/0146167211398139

2011 37: 350Pers Soc Psychol BullMarYam G. Hamedani, Hazel Rose Markus and Alyssa S. Fu

My Nation, My Self: Divergent Framings of America Influence American Selves  

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of: 

  Society for Personality and Social Psychology

can be found at:Personality and Social Psychology BulletinAdditional services and information for     

  http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

 

http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:  

http://psp.sagepub.com/content/37/3/350.refs.htmlCitations:  

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 2: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Article

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin37(3) 350 –364© 2011 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, IncReprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0146167211398139http://pspb.sagepub.com

My Nation, My Self: Divergent Framings of America Influence American Selves

MarYam G. Hamedani1, Hazel Rose Markus1, and Alyssa S. Fu1

Abstract

Current public discourse calls for America to act more interdependently in the world or act more like a conjoint agent. America and American selves, however, are typically associated acting independently or disjoint agency. Since nation is a significant sociocultural source of self, the authors examine what happens to American selves if America is instead associated with conjoint agency. Study 1 surveyed participants in America and nine nations (N = 610) about America’s role in the world and found that although people currently associate America with disjoint agency, they overwhelmingly prefer America to be a conjoint agent. Studies 2–4 demonstrated that framing America’s role in the world with conjoint agency rather than disjoint agency led Americans to see themselves more positively (Studies 2 and 3) and be less individualistic in their self-descriptions and actions (Study 4). The results reveal how changes in the sociocultural context can catalyze a corresponding change in the selves that inhabit that context.

Keywords

self, nation, America, disjoint agency, conjoint agency

Received December 23, 2009; revision accepted October 7, 2010

Clearly, America must continue to lead the world we did so much to make.

U.S. President William Clinton (First inaugural address, January 20, 1993)

We are stronger when we act together. . . . We will listen care-fully, bridge misunderstanding, and seek common ground.

U.S. President Barack Obama (Speech following G-20 Summit, April 6, 2009)

In an ongoing cycle of mutual constitution, cultures and selves make each other up, and when cultures change, so do the selves that constitute them (Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, & Nisbett, 1998; Shweder, 1991; Taylor, 1989). One significant socio-cultural source of self is nation of origin, and in these studies, we examine what happens to the self when the prevalent view of one’s nation undergoes change. Both America as a nation and selves in American contexts are characterized by acting independently and exerting influence—leading the world, as former president Bill Clinton puts it. In contempo-rary world opinion, however, this very self and nation defining feature—what previous research has called disjoint agency (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2003)—has been viewed

as problematic or even undesirable. Many politicians, aca-demics, and social commentators claim that America would fare better if it behaved more interdependently with and res ponsively toward other nations in the global community—what previous research has called conjoint agency (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2003). In fact, as in the quote above, many of President Obama’s speeches contain a call for America to act more like a conjoint agent, emphasizing the importance of listening to, seeking common ground with, and working cooperatively with other nations.

In four studies we examine what can happen to American selves if America becomes prevalently associated with con-joint agency instead of disjoint agency. Specifically, we ask whether Americans and people around the world would like America to behave more like a conjoint agent and how framing America with conjoint agency versus disjoint agency influ-ences Americans’ perceptions and evaluations of themselves.

1Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA

Corresponding Author:MarYam G. Hamedani, Stanford University, Department of Psychology, Jordan Hall, Bldg. 420, Stanford, CA 94305-2310Email: [email protected]

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 3: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Hamedani et al. 351

In doing so, we extend understanding of how ideas and prac-tices prevalent in the sociocultural context can dynamically influence individual selves (cf. Chiu & Hong, 2006; Heine, 2008; Kitayama, Ishii, Imada, Takemura, & Ramaswamy, 2006; Oyserman & Lee, 2007).

America and Me: Disjoint AgentsAlthough the definition of America as a nation has varied across time and place, numerous analyses cite independence as America’s persistent signature (Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, & Tipton, 1985; Hochschild, 1995; Lipset, 1996; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Quinn & Crocker, 1999; Sampson, 1988; Tocqueville, 1969; Triandis, 1989). Historically, America has been powerfully and positively depicted as a beacon of liberty endowed with the responsibility of spread-ing independence and freedom across the continent (e.g., Manifest Destiny) and the globe (e.g., the Cold War; Coles, 2002; Lipset, 1996; Schlesinger, 1986; Turner, 1920). As a consequence, disjoint agency (Markus & Kitayama, 2003)—or acting as an independent agent that shapes its own destiny as well as influences the environment and oth-ers in that environment—has become normatively and posi-tively associated with America as a nation.

Disjoint agency is also associated with how to be a good, normatively successful self in mainstream American contexts (Heine, 2001; Leung & Cohen, 2007; Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2003; Morling, Kitayama, & Miyamoto, 2002; Triandis, 1989). Many prevalent ideas, practices, institutions, and prod-ucts in American contexts—including ideas about America as nation—foster the notion that people should individuate themselves, express their thoughts, emotions, and preferences, and exert control over their social environments and relations with others (Kim & Markus, 1999; Kitayama et al., 2006; Markus & Kitayama, 2003; Morling et al., 2002). In fact, European Americans perform better, and are happier and healthier, when they act as disjoint agents (Brim, Ryff, & Kessler, 2004; Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Ji, Peng, & Nisbett, 2000; Morling et al., 2002; Oishi & Diener, 2001). In sum, being a good nation and being a good self in American con-texts are both associated with the same defining feature: dis-joint agency.

America and Changing AssociationsDespite the traditional, positive association between America and disjoint agency, this association has currently fallen on hard times. Emblematic of this change, the “cowboy,” tradi-tionally invoked to symbolize America’s robust connection to disjoint agency (e.g., Hochschild, 1995), has been used to disparage America’s way of interacting with other nations in the world during recent years (e.g., Hertsgaard, 2002). Indeed, extensive survey research assessing public opinion around the globe indicates that much of the world dislikes America’s penchant for unilateral action, fears its unrivaled power, and

believes that it seeks to control the international community (Kohut & Stokes, 2006; Zogby, 2002). As encapsulated by Kohut and Stokes (2006), in their comprehensive review of years of polling research conducted by the Pew Research Center, people “from nearby Canada to the far reaches of Africa and Asia . . . believe that there is too much America in their world” (p. 18). Moreover, this perception of America’s disjoint agency is not simply the result of recent American foreign policy initiatives: It has been steadily increasing since America emerged from the Cold War as the world’s hyperpower (Buruma & Margalit, 2004; Friedman, 2000; Hertsgaard, 2002; Kohut & Stokes, 2006; Sardar & Davies, 2002; Sweig, 2006).

Given that disjoint agency has been so pervasively associ-ated with what it means to be America and what it means to be a good self in American contexts, we anticipate that when disjoint agency is criticized at the national level, the rever-berations will affect how Americans actually perceive them-selves. Due to the increased prevalence of messages promoting America’s interdependence and responsiveness to others, as reflected in the opening quote from President Obama, will conjoint agency become a valued way of being for America and change how Americans perceive and evaluate them-selves? Or, given the enduring links with disjoint agency, will Americans maintain their preference for disjoint agency? To begin to examine these questions, we frame America as either a disjoint agent (i.e., a global leader) or a conjoint agent (i.e., a global partner) and test how framing America as a nation influences how individual Americans see themselves and America.

In Study 1 we ask whether people want America to become interdependent and behave like a conjoint agent, as implied by current polls and public discourse. Specifically, we ask samples of Americans and people from other nations to choose among a variety of representations of America’s role in the world, indicating their view of America’s actual role as well as their view of America’s ideal role. We anticipate a divergence between the actual and the ideal. With respect to America’s actual role, we hypothesize that people will asso-ciate America with disjoint agency. With respect to America’s ideal role, we predict that people will associate America with conjoint agency.

Given the hypothesized link between nation and self, we anticipate that if the desired role for America as a nation shifts from being a disjoint agent to a conjoint agent, this shift will also occasion changes in how individual Americans actually perceive themselves. In Studies 2–4, we examine how fram-ing America as a disjoint agent or conjoint agent influences Americans’ self-evaluations and self-perceptions. Since nation is a significant sociocultural source of self, we anticipate that as conjoint agency becomes increasingly valued for America as a nation, American selves will, in turn, reflect this cultural shift toward conjoint agency. In Studies 2 and 3, we predict that Americans will evaluate themselves more positively when America is framed as a conjoint agent. In Study 4, we

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 4: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

352 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3)

predict that Americans will behave less individualistically when America is framed as a conjoint agent.

Study 1Overview

In Study 1 we examine the extent to which people associate America with disjoint or conjoint agency. We created three schematic models that vary in terms of how much they emphasize America’s role as a conjoint or disjoint agent in the world. These models were derived from theorizing about international relations and globalization (Brecher & Costello, 1994; Held, McGrew, Goldblatt, & Perraton, 1999; Tomlinson, 1999; Wallerstein, 1979). To quickly and vividly convey these differences in America’s role in the world, we used the American flag—an extremely pervasive cultural product that symbolizes America—and positioned it relative to other nations in the world (see Figure 1).

We asked participants from America and nine other nati ons to choose which model actually and ideally represents America’s role in the world. The models depicted America as (a) a global leader or disjoint agent, (b) a global co-leader or modified disjoint agent, or (c) a global partner or conjoint

agent. The models were pretested to ensure that people inter-preted them as intended.

HypothesesWe anticipate that participants’ judgments will differ depen-ding on whether they rate America’s actual or ideal role in the world. With respect to America’s actual role, we hypoth-esize that, overall, participants in America as well as in other nations will be more likely to associate America with dis-joint agency (the Leader model), rather than conjoint agency (the Partner model). This pattern is consistent with the prev-alent association between America and disjoint agency. With respect to America’s ideal role, we anticipate that partici-pants in America as well as other nations will be more likely to associate America with conjoint agency (the Partner model) than disjoint agency (the Leader model), reflecting the cur-rent preference for America to be more interdependent and behave like a conjoint agent.

MethodParticipants. Participants were 610 adults aged 20 to 60 years

from the following nations: United States = 145, Bermuda = 35,

America: Global Leader(Leader)

America: Global Co-Leader(Co-Leader) America: Global Partner

(Partner)

Figure 1. Schematics representing America’s role in the world

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 5: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Hamedani et al. 353

England = 76, France = 36, Germany = 49, Hong Kong = 33, India = 49, Japan = 52, Singapore = 88, South Africa = 47. Our goal was to draw a wide range of community samples from countries around the world that vary in international status, and our final set of participants reflected the sites of current collaborators. England, France, Germany, and Japan were classified as relatively high status nations (n = 213), whereas the remainder of international countries sampled were classified as relatively low status nations (n = 252). We operationalized high status nations as those that are rela-tively more influential in the global community—for exam-ple, leading members of prominent international organizations such as the United Nations who have possessed, in recent history, significant economic and political clout in the world. Likewise, we operationalized low status nations as those that did not fit these criteria. Participants were approached in common, public urban spaces, such as transit stations and shopping centers, and asked to complete the survey. They were given a small gift as compensation. Recruiters were ins tructed to avoid American-owned businesses (e.g., Star-bucks in Japan).

Materials. Participants from France, Germany, India, and Japan all completed the survey translated into French, German, Hindi, and Japanese, respectively. Participants from Bermuda, England, Hong Kong, Singapore, and South Africa completed the survey in English, which was the most common language in those contexts. Translations were conducted by a professional translation service, and all surveys were translated and back-translated according to standard procedures (e.g., Brislin, Lonner, & Thorndike, 1973).

Procedure. Participants viewed the three models simulta-neously and without labels. They were then asked (a) “Which model best represents how the world actually is today?” and (b) “Which model best represents how you

would ideally like the world to be in the future?” After mak-ing their selection, participants were asked to briefly explain their choices.

ResultsActual model. A chi-square analysis, c2(4, N = 610) = 20.24,

p < .001, fc = .13, revealed that, overall, when rating America’s actual role in the world, participants across nations were very unlikely to select the Partner model and were decidedly more likely to select one of the Leader models (see Figure 2). In addition, nation status affected model choice. Specifically, high status nation participants were most likely to choose the Leader model (61.3%), whereas American participants were equally likely to select the Leader (46.2%) and Co-Leader (42.8%) models. Low status nation participants were also equally likely to choose the Leader (42.9%) and Co-Leader (46.8%) models. Of the few who chose the Partner model to represent America’s actual role, 11.0% were American par-ticipants, 10.3% were low status nation participants, and 3.8% were high status nation participants. Overall, these results confirm that the current prevalent meaning of America is that of a disjoint agent and not a conjoint agent.

Ideal model. When rating America’s ideal role in the world, participants across nations were highly likely to select the Part-ner model and very unlikely to select one of the Leader models, c2(4, N = 610) = 26.26, p < .001, fc = .15 (see Figure 3). In particular, American participants (57.6%), as well as high status nation participants (75.0%) and low status nation participants (67.7%), associated America with the Partner model. Of those who chose one of the Leader models to rep-resent America’s ideal role in the world, 30.6% were Ameri-can participants, 19.0% were low status nation participants, and 9.1% were high status nation participants.

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

Leader Co-Leader Partner

Per

cen

t (%

) C

ho

osi

ng

Mo

del

America n = 145

High Status n = 213

Low Status n = 252

Figure 2. Actual model: Percentage of participants by nation choosing which model actually represents America’s role in the world

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 6: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

354 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3)

Discussion

Although Americans and people around the world associate America with being a global leader, they overwhelmingly pre-fer America to be a global partner, confirming widespread public opinion. Overall, these results are consistent with the idea that many prefer America to be a conjoint agent and not a disjoint agent. Furthermore, international status moderated how people from high status and low status nations view America’s actual role in the world. Participants from high sta-tus nations were likely to associate America with the Leader model, whereas participants from low status nations were equally likely to associate America with the Leader and Co-Leader models. Since high status nations confront and com-pete with America’s independence and influence in the international community more directly than low status nations, people engaging in such contexts may have a stronger associa-tion between America and disjoint agency. American partici-pants, however, were also equally likely to associate America’s actual role with both the Leader and Co-Leader models. These results may reflect Americans’ current concern with Ameri-ca’s role as an influencing and dominating leader, and desire for America to become more of a partner with the rest of the world. In the subsequent studies, we directly manipulate expo-sure to positive Leader and Partner framings of America to test whether a change in how America should act in the world influences how Americans actually perceive themselves.

Pilot Study: Two Framings of America’s Role in the World for Studies 2–4Overview

In Studies 2–4 we examine how framing America as either a disjoint or conjoint agent influences Americans’ self-evaluations

and self-perceptions. Instead of using the schematics from Study 1, we created two speeches that outlined a general, guiding vision for America’s role in the world for these priming studies. Speeches are powerful vehicles for making meaning, and political leaders and policy makers frequently use them to communicate and disseminate ideas about national identity and America’s role in the world.

Inspired by Quinn and Crocker (1999), who developed primes that drew on actual speeches of former presidents, we used phrases from speeches given by American presidents and United Nations secretaries-general. Our goal was to construct two equally engaging and positive framings of America’s role in the world: one speech described America as an independent, influential disjoint agent (i.e., the Leader framing) and one speech described America as an interdepen-dent, adjusting conjoint agent (i.e., the Partner framing; see Figure 4). The speeches were matched in terms of the number of words and sentences as well as the number of inclusive words and phrases (e.g., we, us, our, my fellow Americans).

MethodParticipants. A diverse group of 26 participants from Stan-

ford University rated the speeches on a variety of attributes assessing speech likability and effectiveness. Using a second diverse group of 125 participants from the same university, we evaluated how memorable participants found each speech, which type of role in the world for America was communi-cated in each speech, and how positively participants identi-fied with being American following each speech. Participants received course credit for their participation.

Materials. The first group of 26 participants rated the spee ches for how likable, positive, powerful, and compelling they were, as well as how much rhetorical skill they exhibited. Participants rated these items using 7-point Likert-type scales.

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

Leader Co-Leader Partner

Per

cen

t (%

) C

ho

osi

ng

Mo

del

America n = 145

High Status n = 213

Low Status n = 252

Figure 3. Ideal model: Percentage of participants by nation choosing which model ideally represents America’s role in the world

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 7: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Hamedani et al. 355

For the second group of 125 participants, we created a brief memory test to see if participants found the Partner and Leader framings of America communicated in each speech to be comparably memorable. The test consisted of eight phrases that could have been in the speech—half of which were correct—and participants selected the phrases that they thought were present in the speech.

Participants’ level of identification with being American was measured using three items from Davies, Steele, and Markus (2008). These items assessed the extent to which participants identified with being American, how important being American was to them, and how much they thought of themselves as American. Participants rated these items using 7-point Likert-type scales. Since these items were highly intercorrelated (a = .86), we collapsed them into one scale by summing across items.

Finally, we asked participants to rate the extent to which the speech they were exposed to communicated an interde-pendent, adjusting role for America in the world versus an independent, influencing role on a 7-point Likert-type scale.

Role-type anchored the scale, providing a continuum from 1 (global partner) to 7 (global leader).

Procedure. Participants were run in small group sessions. On entering the lab, they were told that they were going to read a brief speech given by an American senator in the mid-1990s,1 which outlined a general, guiding vision for America’s role in the world. Participants were thereby exposed to either the Leader or the Partner framing of America. They then completed a questionnaire including the dependent measures.

Results and DiscussionSuggesting that we succeeded in creating two comparable speeches, participants’ ratings did not differ in terms of how likable, positive, powerful, or compelling they thought the speeches were, nor in how much rhetorical skill they exhib-ited (n = 26, all ts < 1.00). Participants also remembered each speech equally well (t < 1.00, items correct M = 4.12, SD = 1.34). Furthermore, no significant differences emerged by speech for the American identification measure (t < 1.50, identification score M = 15.93, SD = 3.64), indicating that the speeches did not affect participants’ level of identification with their American identity.

Notably, however, the two frames did create different imp ressions of America’s role in the world. Participants found the Partner frame to portray America as having more of an interdependent, adjusting role in the world and the Leader frame to portray America as having a more indepen-dent, influencing role in the world (Partner: M = 2.61, SD = 1.64; Leader: M = 6.31, SD = 0.75), t(123) = 16.39, p < .001, d = 2.96. Taken together, these results reveal that we were successful in creating two positive and engaging frames that mirrored actual ways in which America’s role in the world could be communicated by politicians and policy makers.

Study 2Overview and Hypothesis

Given the hypothesized link between nation and self, we anticipate that as the desired role for America as a nation shifts from being a disjoint agent to a conjoint agent, this shift will also influence individual Americans’ self-evaluations and self-perceptions. As an initial test of this idea we asked par-ticipants to read one of the two speeches created in the pilot study, which framed America as either an influencing global leader (the Leader frame) or an adjusting global partner (the Partner frame), and then examined how Americans feel about themselves and about being American. Given that America’s ideal role in the world is that of a conjoint agent rather than a disjoint agent, we anticipate that thinking about America as a global partner will be associated with more positive feel-ings about the self and about being American than thinking about America as a global leader.

America: Global Leader

What is America’s role in our interconnected world? In the past century America became the world’s mightiest power, saving the world from tyranny in two world wars and a long cold war. America’s obligation to be a great leader and model nation is undisputed. We must continue to lead the world we did so much to make. This path is rarely, however, easy or straightforward. Through our independence and leadership we must work to exert proper influence in order to make our world better. Our can-do spirit and American dream position us to inspire, help, and lead others to realize their dreams. My fellow Americans, we must continue to be a beacon of hope to all who long for freedom, a better life, and a better tomorrow.

America: Global Partner

What is America’s role in our interconnected world? Though our world contains a diversity of nations and cultures, we are one global community with a common destiny. Our political, economic, and social challenges are interconnected, and no nation can be autonomous. We must work together to broker differences in power, culture, size, and interests among states. It is imperative that we adjust to others, and America’s interests and perspective may not always prevail. America will need to adapt to the leadership and influence of others in the international community. My fellow Americans and citizens of the world, our global community requires a new sense of interdependence and together we can create and foster inclusive solutions. America must build bridges and form partnerships to foster a global consciousness.

Figure 4. Speeches framing America’s role in the world: Global leader (disjoint agency) versus global partner (conjoint agency)

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 8: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

356 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3)

Method

Participants. A diverse group of 125 undergraduates (all U.S. citizens, 84 female, age M = 19.87 years) from Stanford University participated in this study.2 Participants received either course credit or monetary compensation.

Materials and procedure. To assess how participants felt about themselves, we measured state affect using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), in which participants rated their current level of each emotion using 5-point Likert-type scales. Participants’ feel-ings about being American were measured using five items, which assessed the extent to which participants were proud, happy, and comfortable to be an American as well as how criti-cal they were of America and how embarrassed they were by America. Since these items were highly intercorrelated (a = .83), we collapsed them into one scale by summing across items (after reverse coding the last two items). We also included a measure of political orientation, composed of two items assessing participants’ self-reported level of political liberalness and conservativeness. The global feelings about being American and political orientation items were both rated using 7-point Likert-type scales.

Participants were run in small group sessions using the same Leader and Partner frames and study setup outlined in the pilot study. After exposure to the two speeches framing America’s role in the world, participants completed a ques-tionnaire including the dependent measures.

ResultsTo assess how the speeches generally made participants feel, we computed an affective balance score by subtracting par-ticipants’ standardized negative affect score from their stan-dardized positive affect score (Watson et al., 1988). Confirming our hypothesis, participants exposed to the Partner frame (M = 0.51, SD = 1.53) reported more positive affect than did participants exposed to the Leader frame (M = –0.49, SD = 1.52), t(123) = 3.67, p < .001, d = 0.67. Also lending some support to our prediction, a marginal effect of speech emerged for participants’ global feelings about being American. Partici-pants exposed to the Partner frame (M = 23.00, SD = 5.38) reported feeling somewhat more positively about being American than participants exposed to the Leader frame (M = 21.28, SD = 5.23), t(123) = 1.78, p = .08, d = 0.32.

To examine whether political orientation influenced how participants responded to the speeches, we summed partici-pants’ self-reported levels of liberalness and conservative-ness (the conservativeness item was reverse coded; higher overall scores indicate greater liberal political attitudes). Par-ticipants’ political orientation did not emerge as a significant covariate across these findings, indicating that the speeches affected liberals and conservatives similarly (Fs < 1, M = 10.03, SD = 2.19, range = 3.00–14.00).3

Discussion

Coupled with the results from Study 1, these data suggest that as conjoint agency is increasingly preferred to disjoint agency at the national level, the reverberations will affect individual Americans’ own self-evaluations. Study 2 lends support to our hypothesis that participants feel more posi-tively about themselves when America’s role in the world is framed in terms of conjoint agency (Partner) rather than dis-joint agency (Leader). Participants felt good and felt better about being American when America was framed as a global partner rather than as a global leader. As the desired role for America as a nation shifts from being a disjoint agent to a conjoint agent, this shift can also influence individual Americans’ self-evaluations.

Study 3Overview and Hypotheses

Study 2 suggests that Americans feel better about them-selves when thinking about America as a global partner rather than as a global leader. In Study 3, we seek to support these findings using another measure of self-evaluation. To further test the idea that the Partner frame will lead to more positive self-evaluations than the Leader frame, in Study 3 we asked participants to describe how they feel about them-selves using words from Osgood and colleagues’ three dimensions of meaning that people apply to make judg-ments: good/bad (Evaluation), active/passive (Activity), and strong/weak (Potency; Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957). We hypothesize that, lending support to our prior findings and reflecting the current desire for America to act more like a conjoint agent, framing America as a Partner will lead Americans to report feeling “good” compared to fram-ing America as a Leader. Since the frames are comparably engaging and arousing, we do not expect participants to describe themselves differently in terms of how “strong” or “active” they feel.

MethodParticipants. A diverse sample of 125 undergraduates (all

U.S. citizens, 65 female, age M = 19.79 years) from Stanford University participated in this study. Participants received either course credit or monetary compensation.

Materials. We used the Leader and Partner frames cre-ated in the pilot study. To assess how participants feel along Osgood et al.’s (1957) dimensions of meaning (good, strong, active), we developed a “me/not-me” task (e.g., Markus, 1977) in which participants were presented with a word list (20 word trials per dimension) and asked to rate (on a 5-point Likert-type scale) how much each word “describes me right now.” All words were between five and eight letters in length

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 9: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Hamedani et al. 357

and were pilot tested to ensure that they were comparably easy to understand. Each dimension scale was reliable: good, a = .93; strong, a = .79; active, a = .86.

Procedure. Participants were approached around campus and asked to complete a brief survey. The study was intro-duced using the same procedures outlined in the pilot study. Participants read either the Leader or Partner speech and then completed the self-description task.

ResultsWe computed an average rating score per meaning dimension and analyzed participants’ self-description ratings using a 2 (speech: Leader vs. Partner) × 3 (dimension: good vs. strong vs. active) ANOVA, with repeated measures on the second variable. A significant speech by meaning dimension interac-tion emerged, F(2, 246) = 13.06, p < .0001, hp

2 = .10. Consis-tent with our Study 2 results, participants described themselves as feeling more “good” after reading the Partner versus Leader speech (see Figure 5). This predicted difference was con-firmed by a simple effects test: good, t(123) = 2.38, p < .05,d = 0.68. Participants did not feel divergently “strong” (Part-ner: M = 3.45, SD = 0.46 vs. Leader: M = 3.42, SD = 0.51; t < 1.00) or “active” (Partner: M = 3.15, SD = 0.46 vs. Leader: M = 3.22, SD = 0.55; t < 1.00) after exposure to the speeches.

We also ran a control condition in which participants read the same introduction to the task but completed the self-description ratings before being exposed to the speeches. There was a significant main effect of dimension for the

control condition, F(2, 94) = 52.56, p < .0001, hp2 = .53.

Participants’ mean levels per dimension were: good, M = 3.75, SD = 0.55; strong, M = 3.25, SD = 0.56; active, M = 3.05, SD = 0.53. These data suggest that the direction of the framing effects may be due to the Leader frame evoking negative self-perceptions.

DiscussionSupporting the results from Study 2, Study 3 further shows that participants report relatively more positive self-evaluations when America is framed as a global partner rather than as a global leader. Reflecting the current desire for America to act more like a conjoint agent, framing America as a Partner led Americans to report feeling “good” compared to framing America as a Leader. Participants did not differ in how “strong” or “active” they felt, indicating that the frames are compara-bly engaging and arousing.4

Study 4Overview and Hypotheses

Studies 2 and 3 indicate that framing America as a conjoint agent (global partner) or a disjoint agent (global leader) inf luences Americans’ self-evaluations. These studies dem-onstrate that as America’s ideal role in the world shifts from that of a disjoint agent to a conjoint agent, such a change in what it means to be America will be reflected in the self. In

2.00

2.25

2.50

2.75

3.00

3.25

3.50

3.75

Leader Partner

Mea

n “

Go

od

” S

elf-

Des

crip

tio

n R

atin

gs

(1-5

)

Speech

Figure 5. Self-description means for the “good” dimension, by condition. Error bars represent standard errors.

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 10: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

358 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3)

particular, participants evaluated themselves more positively following the Partner frame compared to the Leader frame. In Study 4, we test how the two frames influence Americans’ self-perceptions by examining the structure of the self after exposure to each frame.

In the present studies we both draw on and extend the growing body of cultural priming literature, which demon-strates how selves can be dynamically influenced by the con-text and can change to reflect ideas about the self or the type of self-concept that is cued in the moment (e.g., Gardner, Gabriel, & Lee, 1999; Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martínez, 2000; Oyserman & Lee, 2007; Trafimow, Triandis, & Goto, 1991). Here, we do not manipulate how people think about themselves by cuing different ideas about the self (e.g., “I” vs. “we”); instead, we see if the self can be influenced by cuing different ideas about an important sociocultural source of self—America. In particular, we investigate how American selves are affected when America—which has been perva-sively and chronically associated with disjoint agency—is paired or framed with conjoint agency.

American contexts are characterized by ideas, practices, institutions, and products that promote and foster disjoint agency (Kitayama, Duffy, & Uchida, 2007; Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2003). As a consequence, as selves are exp osed to and engage in American contexts, behaviors and actions are likely to take a particular individualistic, self-focused form (Heine, 2001; Markus & Kitayama, 2010; Triandis, 1989). Americans as a whole and European Ameri-cans in particular—compared to people in multiple other sociocultural contexts—are likely to describe themselves with a relatively high proportion of abstract, trait-like terms (Heine, 2001; Rhee, Uleman, Lee, & Roman, 1995; Wang, 2004) and also behave according to their personal prefer-ences (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Kim & Markus, 1999; Savani, Markus, & Conner, 2008). Framing America as a conjoint agent (Partner) instead of a disjoint agent (Leader) may inhibit these normatively appropriate ways of character-izing the self and activate alternative ways of understand-ing the self (cf. Higgins, 2005). In this study we examine whether European Americans behave less individualistically when America is framed as a conjoint agent rather than a disjoint agent. We predict that following the Partner frame, compared to the Leader frame, European American partici-pants should be relatively less likely to (a) characterize themselves in terms of abstract personal attributes and (b) choose to behave according to their preferences.

In addition, one important factor that may moderate whether a change in America from a disjoint agent to a con-joint agent could lead to a change in self is prior exposure to sociocultural sources of self that promote conjoint agency. Using East Asian American ethnic background as a proxy for sustained exposure to East Asian ideas and practices, which frequently foster conjoint agency (Markus & Kitayama, 2003),

we include Asian Americans as a comparison group. Asian Americans are likely to have been exposed not only to main-stream American contexts that foster disjoint agency but also to East Asian family and community contexts that foster conjoint agency (cf. Hong et al., 2000; Tsai, Ying, & Lee, 2000).5 Because Asian American selves are frequently shaped by contexts that foster both disjoint and conjoint agency, changing the meaning of one significant source of self—that is, America—may not be as impactful. Therefore, we antici-pate that—unlike European Americans—Asian Americans will be less likely to change their behavior when America is framed as a conjoint agent.

MethodParticipants. A community sample of 145 adults from the

San Francisco Bay Area (all U.S. citizens, 93 female, age M = 26.48 years) participated in this study. Of the sample, 54% were European American and 46% were East Asian Ameri-can. Participants were recruited through online ads posted on craigslist.org and completed the study online. Participants received $5 gift certificates as compensation.

Materials and procedure. In the current study, we used the same Leader and Partner framings of America’s role in the world and study setup explained in the pilot study. To index the tendency to behave individualistically and reveal the extent to which participants characterize themselves in terms of abstract personal attributes, we asked them to complete an open-ended, modified possible selves measure (Dunkel & Kerpelman, 2006; Oyserman & Markus, 1990). Participants were asked to describe what they expect to be like and what they expect to be doing in the next year. We selected this measure because previous research has found that the possi-ble selves measure is a particularly good indicator of contex-tual variation in self (Oyserman & Markus, 1990). Moreover, since our goal was to assess a potential change in self, we wanted to focus participants on how they see themselves in the near future (Oyserman & Fryberg, 2006).

To evaluate participants’ tendency to behave according to their preferences, we designed a choice task in which par-ticipants were provided with a series of brief vignettes about everyday interpersonal situations and were asked to make a choice between acting on their personal preferences and adjusting to the preferences of others (e.g., Kim & Markus, 1999; Savani et al., 2008). For example, in one vignette, a participant could indicate that when selecting a movie to watch with a group of her friends, she would choose the movie (personal preference) or let the group decide the movie (adjust to group’s preference). These options anchored 7-point Likert-type scales, and participants rated their inclination to behave one way or the other along the scale. Higher scores indicate a greater desire to act according to one’s own per-sonal preferences.

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 11: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Hamedani et al. 359

Results

Emphasizing personal attributes. Two independent raters coded participants’ possible selves for mentions of personal attributes (e.g., motivated, outgoing, optimistic; e.g., Cousins, 1989; Oyserman & Markus, 1990; Trafimow et al., 1991) and attained a high level of reliability: k = .95 (Landis & Koch, 1977). We then computed a proportion score for each partici-pant, which was the number of possible selves described in terms of personal attributes relative to the total number of pos-sible selves. Using a 2 (speech: Leader vs. Partner) × 2 (eth-nicity: European American vs. Asian American) ANOVA, we found a significant speech by ethnicity interaction: F(1, 141) = 4.26, p < .05, hp

2 = .03. Confirming our hypothesis, we found that European American participants were less likely to describe themselves in terms of personal attributes after expo-sure to the Partner frame than the Leader frame, t(77) = 2.71, p < .05, d = 0.62, whereas Asian American participants did not change their behavior, t < 1.00 (see Figure 6).6

Behaving according to personal preference. We created an average rating score for participants’ tendency to behave according to their personal preferences across the vignettes. We found a main effect by speech, F(1, 141) = 5.87, p < .05, hp

2 = .04, as well as another significant speech by ethnicity interaction, F(1, 141) = 5.56, p < .05, hp

2 = .04. Participants were overall less likely to indicate a desire to act according to their personal preferences after exposure to the Partner frame compared to the Leader frame, t(143) = 2.64, p < .01, d = 0.44; however, this effect appears to be driven by the European American participants. As hypothesized, we found that Euro-pean American participants were less likely to indicate a desire

to act according to their personal preferences after exposure to the Partner frame compared to the Leader frame, t(77) = 3.54, p < .01, d = 0.81, whereas Asian American participants did not change their behavior, t < 1.00 (see Figure 7).7

DiscussionStudy 4 revealed that the Partner framing of America inhib-ited several key individualistic tendencies that typically characterize selves in American contexts. That is, European American participants were less likely to emphasize their individuality or to express their preferences when exposed to the Partner frame compared to the Leader frame. Notably, Asian American participants did not reveal a change in self, suggesting that sustained prior exposure to contexts that pro-mote and foster conjoint agency may moderate this effect. Taken together, these findings suggest that if the desired role for America as a nation shifts from being a disjoint agent to a conjoint agent, this shift could result in less individualistic American selves. Given the assumed link between “my nation” and “my self,” changes in what characterizes America as a nation could lead to changes in what it means to be a norma-tively appropriate, successful self in American contexts.

General DiscussionSummary of Findings

Given that nation is a significant sociocultural source of self, our results suggest that if America as a nation becomes more

.00

.05

.10

.15

.20

.25

.30

European American Asian American

Pro

po

rtio

n o

f P

erso

nal

Att

rib

ute

Sel

f-D

escr

ipti

on

s

Ethnicity

Leader Partner

Figure 6. Proportion of self-descriptions that mention personal attributes, by ethnicity and condition. Error bars represent standard errors.

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 12: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

360 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3)

interdependent, American selves will, in turn, reflect this cultural shift toward conjoint agency. In Study 1 we found that Americans and people around the world demonstrate a preference for American interdependence, confirming public discourse and polling data. Although Americans and people around the world currently see America as an independent, disjoint agent, they ideally prefer America to be more inter-dependent and act like a conjoint agent. Moreover, in Studies 2–4 we found that when Americans are primed with an inter-dependent America, self-perception and the structure of the self change. Specifically, framing America as a conjoint agent rather than a disjoint agent leads Americans to per-ceive themselves more positively as well as inhibit individu-alistic tendencies (e.g., in self-description and behavior). In Study 4 we also found that, among Americans, Asian Ameri-cans did not reveal this change in self, suggesting that the link between nation and self may depend on the nature of prior, sustained exposure to contexts that promote and foster conjoint agency.

Implications, Limitations, and Future Directions

My nation, my self. The central question we ask in this article is whether American selves will change if America changes. Here, we investigate what can happen to American selves if a concept that has traditionally structured American sociocultural contexts—disjoint agency—becomes less pre-ferred and valued in the public sphere than conjoint agency.

Given the dynamic process by which sociocultural contexts shape individual selves, when important elements of the socio-cultural context—ideas, institutions, practices, and products—change, individual selves will, in turn, reflect these changes. If alternate meanings can be pervasively built into the world, the selves inhabiting that world will change accordingly.

Previous research has examined the ways in which selves differ across sociocultural contexts (see Kitayama et al., 2007, for a recent review), how selves can change when they move from one cultural context to another (e.g., Heine & Lehman, 2004; Oishi, Lun, & Sherman, 2007), and how selves can change to reflect ideas about the self that are primed or cued in the moment (see Oyserman & Lee, 2007, for a recent review). The focus of this article, however, is one that has been relatively unexamined in the sociocul-tural psychology literature—how the self can be influenced by macro-level changes within a given sociocultural con-text. Here, we investigate how changes in self can result from changes in the ideas associated with a powerful, but typically invisible or implicit, sociocultural source of self—for example, nation. The present studies reveal how a change in the meanings that typically structure sociocultural con-texts can catalyze a corresponding change in self.

Relationship between nation and self. These studies are the first to link changes in the meanings associated with one’s nation to changes in self. They examine how changes in a significant sociocultural source of self—such as nation—can lead to changes in self-perception and self-evaluation. Future studies should examine exactly how this link between nation

2.00

2.50

3.00

3.50

4.00

4.50

5.00

European American Asian American

Mea

n T

end

ency

to

Beh

ave

Acc

ord

ing

to

Pre

fere

nce

(1-

7)

Ethnicity

Leader Partner

Figure 7. Mean tendency to behave according to personal preference, by ethnicity and conditionError bars represent standard errors.

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 13: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Hamedani et al. 361

and self operates. For example, Studies 2 and 3 suggest that the link between nation and self might involve some type of discrepancy reduction, in that people feel good about them-selves following a speech that endorses a view of nation that they prefer or value. Yet Study 4 reveals that framing America as a conjoint agent does more than just induce values-congruent good feelings; it also leads participants to change themselves—that is, to inhibit individualistic tendencies.

Given that the primary goal of this article was to examine how selves can be influenced by macro-level changes in the sociocultural context, we decided to use actual cultural products—speeches about America’s role in the world—to begin to investigate this process. Notably, these studies did not manipulate nation versus non-nation in the primes. Subsequent studies should test how priming the concept of agency (e.g., disjoint and conjoint agency, independence and interdependence, leadership and partnership) in the context of nation influences the self compared to priming agency outside the context of nation. The results from Study 4, in which participants responded differently to the primes, also suggest that the relationship between self and nation can vary by exposure to and experience with disjoint and conjoint models of agency.

Future priming studies might also consider whether the manner in which America is described—for example, with patriotic speech compared to more neutral speech—changes how self can be influenced by nation. Another important direc-tion is to investigate possible mediators that may affect the relationship between nation and self, such as attachment to American identity, constructive patriotism, endorsement of individualistic values, and beliefs about America’s ideal role in the world. Future work can also examine how other aspects of the self may be influenced by the conjoint and disjoint agency framings of America. For example, does framing America with conjoint agency lead Americans to behave like conjoint agents (e.g., adjust to others, cooperate with others, and listen more to others) compared to framing America with disjoint agency?

In addition, the tight coupling between nation and self examined here may be fairly specific to American contexts because, in America, nation may be a particularly significant sociocultural source of self. America’s historical story involves a very explicit process of defining what it means to be America and American that highlights the importance of dis-joint agency. Born out of revolution, and encapsulated in the founding documents of the nation, independence, individual-ism, and disjoint agency have been the explicit, defining fea-tures of “Americanness” throughout much of the nation’s history (e.g., Lipset, 1996; Tocqueville, 1969; Turner, 1920). Although what it means to be a good self and nation are linked—through disjoint agency—in American contexts, such a relationship between nation and self may vary substantially across other national contexts with different historical sto-ries. Future work should examine the relationship between

nation and self across other sociocultural contexts. This line of research will expand prior work on national identity, which has primarily focused on examining how national identity functions through intragroup and intergroup processes such as identity threat and in-group bias (for recent work, see Davies et al., 2008; Li & Brewer, 2004; Mummendey, Klink, & Brown, 2001).

Change the world, change the self. Despite the powerful public discourse favoring greater interdependence, as well as the positive attitudes toward an interdependent America reported here, shifting America and American selves from disjoint agency to conjoint agency may not be quick or easy. Since disjoint agency is so foundational for America and Americans, the links among disjoint agency, America, and American selves are both pervasive and enduring. Disjoint agency is widely valued and practiced across the major insti-tutions of American life—for example, home, school, work, media—and deeply engrained in American selves (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 2003). Alternatively, conjoint agency—acting interdependently and responsively toward others—although regarded as positive and necessary, has not been equally pervasive in mainstream American contexts and has not shaped the self to the same extent as disjoint agency (e.g., Adams, Anderson, & Adonu, 2004; Greenfield, Keller, Fuligni, & Maynard, 2003; Markus & Kitayama, 2003). In fact, recent studies have shown that conjoint agency can result in negative outcomes for American selves (Hamedani, Markus, & Fu, 2011). Hamedani and colleagues (2011) found that when primed with conjoint agency, European Americans’ performance and persistence lagged compared to when they were primed with disjoint agency. In addition, they found that in American contexts, acting interdependently and adjusting to others is less likely to be associated with strength, power, and success than acting indepen dently and exerting influence.

Changing American selves in an enduring way, therefore, may occur only if more ideas, institutions, practices, and products in American contexts—that is, more important sociocultural sources of self—pervasively promote, foster, and sustain the ideas and practices of conjoint agency. Given the powerful ways in which disjoint agency structures American worlds and American psyches, heeding President Obama’s call for an interdependent America will require more than positive attitudes and associations toward interde-pendence, although this may be an essential first step. It will require that interdependence and conjoint agency are increas-ingly built into the American sociocultural context. Conjoint agency, for example, may become more pervasive as “eco-consciousness” becomes part of everyday living, uni-versities foster “public service,” workplaces and schools award employees and students for “fitting in” and “being cooperative,” urban planners resurrect city neighborhoods to “build community,” and politicians and policy makers nur-ture America’s role as a “global partner.” If American ideas

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 14: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

362 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3)

and practices pervasively promote interdependence and con-joint agency, American selves will change accordingly.

Acknowledgments

We thank our reviewers for their helpful feedback and Jeanne Tsai and the Culture Collaboratory at Stanford University for their com-ments on earlier versions of this article. We also thank all of the research assistants who participated in this research for their valu-able contributions.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article.

Financial Disclosure/Funding

The author(s) received the following financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article:

This research was supported by National Science Foundation Research Grant 0092018, awarded to Hazel Rose Markus.

Notes

1. The speech was introduced as having taken place in the mid-1990s, to indicate that it was not from the current administra-tion and to activate general perceptions of American identity.

2. The ethnic breakdown of our samples in Studies 2 and 3 is typi-cal of the Stanford undergraduate community, which is as fol-lows: approximately 10% African American, 10% Latino, 25% Asian American, and 55% European American.

3. The student sample used in Study 3 and the community sample used in Study 4 exhibited similar political attitudes; the com-munity sample was somewhat less liberal (Study 3: M = 9.55, SD = 2.77, range = 2.00–12.00; Study 4: M = 8.63, SD = 1.64, range = 5.00–14.00). Given that political orientation also did not emerge as a significant covariate in Studies 3 and 4 (Fs < 2.00), this variable is not discussed further. Although we found that political orientation did not influence how participants respon ded to the speeches, our student and community samples demonstrated fairly liberal political attitudes, thereby restrict-ing the range of possible attitudes.

4. We also conducted a follow-up study using an implicit version of the self-description task. Controlling for baseline response time, we conducted a 2 (speech: Leader vs. Partner) × 3 (di-mension: good vs. strong vs. active) mixed models ANCOVA on participants’ self-description response times. A significant speech by meaning dimension interaction emerged, F(2, 88) = 7.11, p < .001. Notably, participants were slower to describe themselves as “strong” after exposure to the Partner vs. Leader speech, F(2, 88) = 12.70, p < .0001, b = .04 (Partner: M = 1236 ms, SD = 547 vs. Leader: M = 1156 ms, SD = 554). Although conjoint agency is presently positively regarded in relation to America as a nation, it may also be associated with weakness. This suggestive finding, worthy of further study, may occur because conjoint agency has not been as extensively elaborated

and societally inscribed for America or for individual selves in American contexts as disjoint agency.

5. Asian Americans have been found to highly identify as American as well as Asian and include both identities in the self (e.g., Chery-an & Monin, 2005; Devos, 2006).

6. We also coded participants’ tendency to describe themselves in terms of interpersonal roles and relationships, which has been used to assess whether people perceive themselves as interde-pendent with others (e.g., Cousins, 1989; Oyserman & Markus, 1990; Trafimow, Triandis, & Goto, 1991). Participants’ re-sponses did not differ between conditions.

7. We tested an additional set of covariates in this study: atten-tion to domestic media, attention to international media, and independent/interdependent self-construal. These variables did not yield any significant effects. We also asked participants to rate the extent to which others’ opinions about America affect how they feel about themselves on a 7-point Likert-type scale, to investigate whether Americans are aware of a connection between nation and self. We found that participants did not endorse this idea, M = 2.90, SD = 0.14. Speech did not affect participants’ responses.

References

Adams, G., Anderson, S. L., & Adonu, J. K. (2004). The cultural grounding of closeness and intimacy. In D. J. Mashek & A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 321-339). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. M. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Brecher, J., & Costello, T. (1994). Global village or global pillage: Economic restructuring from the bottom up. Boston, MA: South End.

Brim, O. G., Ryff, C. D., & Kessler, R. C. (2004). How healthy are we? A national study of well-being at midlife. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Brislin, R. W., Lonner, W. J., & Thorndike, R. M. (1973). Cross-cultural research methods. New York, NY: John Wiley.

Buruma, I., & Margalit, A. (2004). Occidentalism: The West in the eyes of its enemies. New York, NY: Penguin.

Cheryan, S., & Monin, B. (2005). “Where are you really from?”: Asian Americans and identity denial. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 717-730.

Chiu, C.-Y., & Hong, Y.-Y. (2006). Social psychology of culture. Principles of social psychology. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Coles, R. L. (2002). War and the contest over national identity. Sociological Review, 50, 586-609.

Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 124-131.

Davies, P. G., Steele, C. M., & Markus, H. R. (2008). A nation challenged: The impact of foreign threat on America’s tolerance

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 15: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

Hamedani et al. 363

for diversity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 308-318.

Devos, T. (2006). Implicit bicultural identity among Mexican Amer-ican and Asian American college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12, 391-402.

Dunkel, C., & Kerpelman, J. (2006). Possible selves: Theory, research, and applications. New York, NY: Nova.

Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psy-chology (4th ed., pp. 915-981). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Friedman, T. L. (2000). The Lexus and the olive tree: Understand-ing globalization. New York, NY: Anchor Books.

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value free-dom, but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mir-rors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10, 321-326.

Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461-490.

Hamedani, M. G., Markus, H. R., & Fu, A. S. (2011). In the land of the free, interdependent action undermines motivation. Manu-script submitted for publication.

Heine, S. J. (2001). Self as cultural product: An examination of East Asian and North American selves. Journal of Personality, 69, 881-906.

Heine, S. J. (2008). Cultural psychology. New York, NY: Norton.Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (2004). Move the body, change the

self: Acculturative effects of the self-concept. In M. Schaller & C. S. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 305-332). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Held, D., McGrew, A., Goldblatt, D., & Perraton, J. (1999). Global transformations: Politics, economics and culture. Cambridge, UK: Polity.

Hertsgaard, M. (2002). The eagle’s shadow: Why America fasci-nates and infuriates the world. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Higgins, E. T. (2005). Value from regulatory fit. Current Direc-tions in Psychological Science, 14, 209-213.

Hochschild, J. L. (1995). Facing up to the American dream: Race, class, and the soul of the nation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Uni-versity Press.

Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709-720.

Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349-366.

Ji, L., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and per-ception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Person-ality and Social Psychology, 78, 943-955.

Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, har-mony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 785-800.

Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., & Uchida, U. (2007). Self as cultural mode of being. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cul-tural psychology (pp. 136-174). New York, NY: Guilford.

Kitayama, S., Ishii, K., Imada, T., Takemura, K., & Ramaswamy, J. (2006). Voluntary settlement and the spirit of independence: Evidence from Japan’s “northern frontier.” Journal of Person-ality and Social Psychology, 91, 369-384.

Kohut, A., & Stokes, B. (2006). America against the world: How we are different and why we are disliked. New York, NY: Times Books.

Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159-174.

Leung, A. K., & Cohen, D. (2007). The soft embodiment of culture: Camera angles and motion through time and space. Psychologi-cal Science, 18, 824-830.

Li, Q., & Brewer, M. B. (2004). What does it mean to be an American? Patriotism, nationalism, and American identity after 9/11. Political Psychology, 25, 727-739.

Lipset, S. M. (1996). American exceptionalism: A double-edged sword. New York, NY: Norton.

Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models of agency: Socio-cultural diversity in the construction of action. In V. Murphy-Berman & J. J. Berman (Eds.), Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self: Volume 49 of the Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 18-74). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 420-430.

Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural prac-tices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 311-323.

Mummendey, A., Klink, A., & Brown, R. (2001). Nationalism and patriotism: National identification and out-group rejection. Brit-ish Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 159-171.

Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2001). Goals, culture, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1674-1682.

Oishi, S., Lun, J., & Sherman, G. D. (2007). Residential mobility, self-concept, and positive affect in social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 131-141.

Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The mea-surement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

Oyserman, D., & Fryberg, S. (2006). The possible selves of diverse adolescents: Content and function across gender, race, and national origin. In C. Dunkel & J. Kerpelman (Eds.), Possible selves: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 17-39). New York, NY: Nova.

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 16: Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

364 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3)

Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. (2007). Priming “culture”: Culture as situated cognition. In S. Kitayama (Ed.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 255-279). New York, NY: Guilford.

Oyserman, D., & Markus, H. R. (1990). Possible selves and delin-quency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,112-125.

Quinn, D. M., & Crocker, J. (1999). When ideology hurts: Effects of belief in the Protestant ethic and feeling overweight on the psychological well-being of women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 402-414.

Rhee, E., Uleman, J. S., Lee, H. K., & Roman, R. J. (1995). Spon-taneous self-descriptions and ethnic identities in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 142-152.

Sampson, E. E. (1988). The debate on individualism: Indigenous psychologies of the individual and their role in personal and societal functioning. American Psychologist, 43, 15-22.

Sardar, Z., & Davies, M. W. (2002). Why do people hate America? New York, NY: Disinformation Company.

Savani, K., Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. L. (2008). Let your pref-erence be your guide? Preferences and choices are more tightly linked for North Americans than for Indians. Journal of Person-ality and Social Psychology, 95, 861-876.

Schlesinger, A. M. (1986). The cycles of American history. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Sweig, J. E. (2006). Friendly fire: Losing friends and making enemies in the anti-American century. New York, NY: Public Affairs.

Taylor, C. (1989). Sources of the self: The making of the modern identity. Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge Press.

Tocqueville, A. D. (1969). Democracy in America (J. P. Mayer, Ed., G. Lawrence, Trans.). Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

Tomlinson, J. (1999). Globalization and culture. Chicago, IL: Uni-versity of Chicago Press.

Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649-655.

Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506-520.

Tsai, J. L., Ying, Y., & Lee, P. A. (2000). The meaning of “being Chinese” and “being American”: Variation among Chinese American young adults. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 302-322.

Turner, F. J. (1920). The significance of the frontier in American his-tory. In J. Phelan (Ed.), Readings in rural sociology (pp. 29-33). Madison, WI: Macmillan.

Wallerstein, I. (1979). The capitalist world economy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical memory and self-description in European American and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 3-15.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070.

Zogby, J. (2002). The 10 nation “impressions of America” poll report. Utica, NY: Zogby International.

at Stanford University Libraries on March 18, 2011psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from