child eyewitnesses: seeing is believingchild eyewitnesses: seeing is believing c. a. elizabeth luus...

10
Journal of Applied Psychology 1995, Vol. 80, No. 2,317-326 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-9010/95/$3.00 Child Eyewitnesses: Seeing Is Believing C. A. Elizabeth Luus University of Victoria Gary L. Wells Iowa State University John W. Turtle York University Two studies examined perceptions of child and adult eyewitnesses' credibility. In Study 1, college students evaluated transcribed testimonies of 8-year-old and adult witnesses to a videotaped staged crime. Half were misinformed about the witness's age (i.e., either believing a child's testimony was provided by an adult or vice versa). Neither actual age nor ostensible age affected participants' evaluations. In Study 2, adults (N= 85) viewed videotaped testimonies of 8-year-old, 12-year-old, and adult eyewitnesses. Half viewed 1 witness's testimony and then evaluated his or her credibility. The others viewed only a still frame of 1 witness, then imagined the testimony the witness had provided, and finally evaluated his or her credibility without having actually heard the testimony. Young chil- dren were judged more favorably when their entire testimony, rather than a still frame, was viewed. This was not true for the older eyewitnesses. What cues do jurors use to judge the credibility of child eyewitnesses? Are jurors' evaluations of children's testi- mony biased by negative stereotypes of children's eyewit- ness capabilities? This article addresses these questions by comparing participant jurors' evaluations of their imagined versus the actual eyewitness capabilities of samples of child and adult eyewitnesses. The legal community has historically regarded the tes- timony of children with distrust, believing children are too suggestible or prone to incorporate misleading sug- gestions into their memory reports (Whipple, 1911). Surveys of adults' beliefs about child eyewitnesses have suggested that the general population shares this pessi- mistic view of children's eyewitness capabilities (e.g., Leippe & Romanczyk, 1987; Ross, Dunning, Toglia, & Ceci, 1991; Yarmey & Jones, 1983). For example, re- C. A. Elizabeth Luus, Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada; Gary L. Wells, Department of Psychology, Iowa State University; John W. Tur- tle, Department of Psychology, York University, North York, Ontario, Canada. John W. Turtle is now at Ryerson Polytechnical Institute. This work was supported by grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC 410 92 0820) and the National Science Foundation ( SES 90-22182). Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- dressed to C. A. Elizabeth Luus, Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, P.O. Box 3050, Victoria, British Co- lumbia, Canada V8W 3P5. Electronic mail may be sent via Bitnet to [email protected]. spondents to Ross et al.'s survey reported that they be- lieved a child eyewitness would be less accurate and more suggestible than an adult eyewitness. They also reported that they would assign less weight to testimony if it was provided by a child rather than an adult. Experimental research has confirmed that child eye- witnesses are more suggestible than their adult counter- parts (e.g., Ceci, Ross, & Toglia, 1987; Cohen & Harnick, 1980; King &Yuille, 1987; Wells, Turtle, & Luus, 1989). For example, Wells et al. had samples of children (8- and 12-year-olds) and adults watch a simulated-crime video- tape and then respond to direct- and cross-examination questioning about the crime. Cross-examination differs from direct examination in several ways. Direct exami- nation is conducted by the attorney who calls the witness to the stand. It is generally a friendly exchange in which the examiner tries to make the witness look credible while observing the rule against leading the witness. Cross-examination is conducted by the opposing coun- sel, whose interest is in undermining the perceived credi- bility of the eyewitness. The cross-examiner is allowed to use leading questions. Wells et al.'s (1989) direct examination consisted of a series of straightforward, nonleading questions; the cross-examination included a number of leading items. The children were as accurate as the adults in responding to direct-examination questioning. However, the children were significantly less accurate than were the adults un- der cross-examination. These findings are consistent with other studies that have found that the accuracy of child and adult eyewitnesses is about the same under nonlead- 317

Upload: others

Post on 22-May-2020

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Journal of Applied Psychology1995, Vol. 80, No. 2,317-326

Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.0021-9010/95/$3.00

Child Eyewitnesses: Seeing Is Believing

C. A. Elizabeth LuusUniversity of Victoria

Gary L. WellsIowa State University

John W. TurtleYork University

Two studies examined perceptions of child and adult eyewitnesses' credibility. In Study1, college students evaluated transcribed testimonies of 8-year-old and adult witnesses toa videotaped staged crime. Half were misinformed about the witness's age (i.e., eitherbelieving a child's testimony was provided by an adult or vice versa). Neither actual agenor ostensible age affected participants' evaluations. In Study 2, adults (N= 85) viewedvideotaped testimonies of 8-year-old, 12-year-old, and adult eyewitnesses. Half viewed 1witness's testimony and then evaluated his or her credibility. The others viewed only astill frame of 1 witness, then imagined the testimony the witness had provided, and finallyevaluated his or her credibility without having actually heard the testimony. Young chil-dren were judged more favorably when their entire testimony, rather than a still frame,was viewed. This was not true for the older eyewitnesses.

What cues do jurors use to judge the credibility of childeyewitnesses? Are jurors' evaluations of children's testi-mony biased by negative stereotypes of children's eyewit-ness capabilities? This article addresses these questionsby comparing participant jurors' evaluations of theirimagined versus the actual eyewitness capabilities ofsamples of child and adult eyewitnesses.

The legal community has historically regarded the tes-timony of children with distrust, believing children aretoo suggestible or prone to incorporate misleading sug-gestions into their memory reports (Whipple, 1911).Surveys of adults' beliefs about child eyewitnesses havesuggested that the general population shares this pessi-mistic view of children's eyewitness capabilities (e.g.,Leippe & Romanczyk, 1987; Ross, Dunning, Toglia, &Ceci, 1991; Yarmey & Jones, 1983). For example, re-

C. A. Elizabeth Luus, Department of Psychology, Universityof Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada; Gary L. Wells,Department of Psychology, Iowa State University; John W. Tur-tle, Department of Psychology, York University, North York,Ontario, Canada.

John W. Turtle is now at Ryerson Polytechnical Institute.This work was supported by grants from the Social Sciences

and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC 410 920820) and the National Science Foundation ( SES 90-22182).

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to C. A. Elizabeth Luus, Department of Psychology,University of Victoria, P.O. Box 3050, Victoria, British Co-lumbia, Canada V8W 3P5. Electronic mail may be sent viaBitnet to [email protected].

spondents to Ross et al.'s survey reported that they be-lieved a child eyewitness would be less accurate and moresuggestible than an adult eyewitness. They also reportedthat they would assign less weight to testimony if it wasprovided by a child rather than an adult.

Experimental research has confirmed that child eye-witnesses are more suggestible than their adult counter-parts (e.g., Ceci, Ross, & Toglia, 1987; Cohen & Harnick,1980; King &Yuille, 1987; Wells, Turtle, & Luus, 1989).For example, Wells et al. had samples of children (8- and12-year-olds) and adults watch a simulated-crime video-tape and then respond to direct- and cross-examinationquestioning about the crime. Cross-examination differsfrom direct examination in several ways. Direct exami-nation is conducted by the attorney who calls the witnessto the stand. It is generally a friendly exchange in whichthe examiner tries to make the witness look crediblewhile observing the rule against leading the witness.Cross-examination is conducted by the opposing coun-sel, whose interest is in undermining the perceived credi-bility of the eyewitness. The cross-examiner is allowed touse leading questions.

Wells et al.'s (1989) direct examination consisted of aseries of straightforward, nonleading questions; thecross-examination included a number of leading items.The children were as accurate as the adults in respondingto direct-examination questioning. However, the childrenwere significantly less accurate than were the adults un-der cross-examination. These findings are consistent withother studies that have found that the accuracy of childand adult eyewitnesses is about the same under nonlead-

317

318 E. LUUS, G. WELLS, AND J. TURTLE

ing, free-recall formats (e.g., Mar in, Holmes, Guth, &Kovac, 1979; Saywitz, 1987), but child eyewitnesses areless accurate than adults in response to leading questions(e.g., Ceci et al., 1987; Cohen & Harnick, 1980; King &Yuille, 1987; Leippe, Manion, & Romanczyk, 1992).

Do adult observers perceive these differences in the ac-curacy of cross-examination testimony provided by chil-dren versus adults? That is, are the children providingdiscernible cues to the weakness of the cross-examinationtestimonies? Empirical investigations of this questionhave yielded mixed results. Some studies have found thatchild eyewitnesses are perceived as less credible thanadults (e.g., Goodman, Golding, & Haith, 1984; Good-man, Golding, Helgeson, Haith, & Michelli, 1987;Leippe et al., 1992; Leippe & Romanczyk, 1987, Study2 of 1989). Others have found that child witnesses areperceived as more credible than adults (e.g., Study 4 ofLeippe & Romanczyk, 1989; Ross, Miller, & Moran,1987). Still others have found no age-related differencesin the perceived credibility of child and adult eyewit-nesses (Wells et al., 1989). Wells et al. found that for bothadult and child eyewitnesses, participant jurors tended tobelieve those eyewitnesses whom they perceived as con-fident. Thus, 8-year-old witnesses perceived as highlyconfident were rated as more credible than many adultwitnesses who apparently lacked confidence.

How can these contradictory findings be recon-ciled? We see the inconsistencies as linked to severaldiscrepancies in the methods used in the studies.First, the studies vary in terms of the comprehensive-ness of the testimony participant jurors were asked toevaluate. Some studies assessed jurors' stereotypes ofchildren's eyewitness capabilities by providing partic-ipant jurors with (a) a description of a case with onlya statement of the witness's age, (b) a written trialtranscript (and manipulated participant jurors' as-sumptions about the age of the witness), or (c) a vid-eotape of a child or an adult actor reciting a testimonyscript. Other studies (Leippe et al., 1992; Wells et al.,1989) have gone beyond the question of adults'stereotypes of children's eyewitness capabilities bypresenting participant jurors with samples of childand adult eyewitnesses who actually witnessed anevent and then responded to questions concerningtheir memory for the event.

The stereotype studies have obtained dramaticallydifferent results from one another. For example, whereasGoodman et al. (1987) found their videotaped child eye-witness to be perceived as less credible than their adulteyewitness, Ross et al. (1987) found their child eyewit-ness to be perceived as more credible than their adult eye-witness. To explain these inconsistent findings, we notethat previous research has revealed profound differencesacross eyewitnesses in their perceived credibility (e.g.,

Wells, Lindsay, & Ferguson, 1979; Wells et al., 1989).The idea that there is variance in the perceived credibilityof eyewitnesses within ages suggests the possibility thatsome child eyewitnesses might be perceived as more cred-ible than some adult eyewitnesses and vice versa (asfound by Wells et al., 1989). It then follows that a singleexample of a child's or an adult's testimony cannot beconsidered representative of that age group's testimonyin general. Thus, the specific child or adult actor who de-livered the testimony could have made all the differencein the varying results obtained by the stereotype studies.

Provision of reports from children who are genuinelytrying to recall a witnessed event from memory usingtheir own words is, in our opinion, an important featureof any research concerned with perceptions of children'seyewitness credibility. To date, only two studies (Leippeet al., 1992; Wells et al., 1989) have included this featurein their research. These studies varied in terms of thestimulus materials used (Wells et al. had participantsview a simulated-crime film, whereas Leippe et al. testedparticipants' memory for a live interaction involving abogus skin-sensitivity test), the questions included in thememory test, and the age of participants tested (Leippeet al. included 5-6-year-olds in their sample; Wells et al.'syoungest participants were 8 years old). Although thetwo studies were not entirely consistent in their overallfindings, they both found a tremendous amount of varia-tion in perceived credibility within their samples of childand adult participant witnesses. "On average, adults mayappear more credible, but some significant number ofchildren may look just as credible—or moreso—thansome adults" (Leippe et al., 1992, p. 190). This was cer-tainly the case with the children who served as witnessesin Wells et al.'s study.

Wells et al. (1989) found that eyewitness age was unre-lated to any of the credibility assessments made by par-ticipant jurors. Participant jurors were sensitive to themain effect difference between direct-examination andcross-examination performance (appropriately estimat-ing lower accuracy for cross-examination than for directexamination) but were not sensitive to the interaction be-tween type of examination (direct vs. cross) and eyewit-ness age. For both adult and child eyewitnesses, partici-pant jurors tended to believe those eyewitnesses whomthey perceived as confident. Thus, interestingly, 8-year-old witnesses who were perceived as highly confidentwere rated as more credible than those adult witnesseswho displayed lower levels of confidence.

These results lend credence to Leippe and Romanc-zyk's (1989) conclusion that "adults' negative precon-ceptions about children's memory will not dispose themto reject a child's memory message if the message's qual-ity is sufficiently 'mature' to belie the stereotype" (p.127). These researchers presented participant jurors

CHILD EYEWITNESSES 319

with a description of a case involving a child or adult wit-ness or they had participant jurors read a transcript ofthe child or adult witness's testimony. In general, theirresults showed that participant jurors tended to show abias against the child's testimony in the description casesin which they merely knew the child's age but a bias fa-voring the child's testimony when they read the testi-mony transcript. This finding is consistent with the no-tion of augmentation in attribution theory (Kelley, 1972)in which a behavior that occurs in spite of some inhibi-tory factor (rather than because of a facilitory factor) isperceived to have an especially strong underlying cause.In this case, a child's young age may have been perceivedas an inhibitory factor, and hence, perceptions of thechild's memory may have been augmented relative toadults' memory.

Leippe and Romanczyk's (1989) findings havesuggested that it is possible that jurors may imaginechildren to be incapable of delivering accurate, con-vincing testimony. However, they may come to re-consider these preconceived negative views of childeyewitnesses upon actually observing a child testify.Our research builds on Leippe and Romanczyk's find-ings in addressing this possibility.

In our first experiment, we presented participant ju-rors with testimony transcripts from a sample of 8-year-old eyewitnesses and a sample of adults to see if theycould discern differences in the verbal portions of the tes-timony. By using transcripts rather than videotapes, wewere able to manipulate the actual age of the witnessesindependently of the ostensible age of the witnesses.Thus, some participant jurors evaluated actual 8-year-olds' testimonies, thinking that the witnesses were 25years old, whereas others evaluated actual 25-year-olds'testimonies, thinking that the witnesses were 8 years old,and so on.

In our second experiment, a set of 42 testimony video-tapes of witnesses who took part in a preliminary study(described below) served as stimulus materials. Partici-pant jurors either viewed an 8-year-old, a 12-year-old, oran adult witness and evaluated his or her credibility orviewed only a still frame of one witness and evaluated hisor her credibility without hearing the actual testimony. Inthe latter condition, participants were instructed to imag-ine the testimony the witness would provide. This proce-dure allowed us to directly compare participant jurors'perceptions of real versus imagined eyewitness capabili-ties of child and adult eyewitnesses.

Preliminary Study: Creating Stimulus Materials

We had 8-year-olds, 12-year-olds, and adults (n = 14per age group) individually witness a filmed abduction ofa child from a playground. Participants were not fore-

warned of an upcoming memory test. We were sensitiveto the fact that this event might evoke fear among thechildren. The video we created depicted a nonviolentscene in which a man approached a young boy, talked tohim in a friendly manner, then offered him some moneyand coaxed him to go for a drive in his car. The boy hesi-tantly but willingly accompanied the man.

The parents of all child participants were fully in-formed of the content of the video through a letter thatwas sent home with all students in participating class-rooms. Parents indicated their willingness to allow theirchild to participate by signing and returning an attachedconsent form. The principals and the parent-teacher as-sociations of the participating schools viewed the crimevideo before granting their approval of the study. In de-briefing the young children, we discussed playgroundsafety and reviewed some measures the children might *take to remember details of events they suspect may bedangerous to themselves or others (e.g., try to write downthe license number of suspicious cars).

The next day, each eyewitness responded to direct- andcross-examination questioning while being videotaped.The direct examination consisted of straightforwardquestions (e.g., "How many children were at theplayground?"). The cross-examination questions werenot of this straightforward nature but rather constitutedan attempt to undermine the credibility of the witness, toelicit inconsistent testimony, or both. For example, oneof the cross-examination questions was "\bu claimed be-fore that the playground was fairly crowded, is that cor-rect?" In fact, only three people were at the playground(and nearly every witness had previously made thisassertion). Both the direct- and cross-examination ques-tions were scripted in a way that allowed the questionersto follow their examination format regardless of how thewitness responded. These testimony videotapes served asstimulus materials in our research.

The children were sampled from four different publicschools located in different areas of a medium-sized city.We chose schools that varied in terms of the affluence ofthe surrounding neighborhoods to sample a diversity ofsocioeconomic backgrounds. Furthermore, we avoidedselecting children who were not representative of theirage group in that we did not sample the children onlyfrom advanced-achievement or only from lower achieve-ment school programs.

Our decision to study 8- and 12-year-olds in compari-son with adults was based primarily on current knowl-edge of children's understanding of the legal system andtheir cognitive capabilities. Studies of reality monitoring(e.g., Johnson & Foley, 1984; Johnson & Raye, 1981)have suggested that children develop the ability to distin-guish fantasy from reality between 6 and 8 years of age.By age 8, children have acquired an understanding of

320 E. LUUS, G. WELLS, AND J. TURTLE

rights (Melton, 1980) and a view of both their generalself-worth and their competence in diiferent domains(Harter, 1982). At this age, children also begin to com-prehend concepts of court and to understand the rolesplayed by judges, lawyers, and witnesses (Saywitz, 1989).However, the function of the jury and a sense of the legalsystem's role in society are not appreciated until approx-imately 12 years of age (Saywitz, 1989).

All witnesses responded to 17 questions: 10 direct-ex-amination questions and 7 cross-examination questions.Answers to these questions were scored by determiningdefinite boundaries on the range of possible replies toeach question. For example, "burgundy" and "purple"were the only acceptable descriptions for the color of thegetaway car. Interjudge reliability was virtually perfectbecause the scoring criteria were very explicit. Univariateanalyses of variance (ANOVAs; with age as the factor) onthe total number of questions answered correctly indi-cated that accuracy did not differ significantly with agefor the direct-examination responses, F(2, 39) = 2.23, p= .12. There was, however, a significant age difference inthe accuracy of the cross-examination responses, F(2,39) = 8.91, p = .007. A subsequent Newman-Keuls anal-ysis revealed that the 8-year-old children were signifi-cantly less accurate under cross-examination (meannumber of correct responses = 3.07, SD = 0.92) thanwere the 12-year-old and adult witnesses (p < .01). The12-year-old and adult eyewitnesses did not differ in theaccuracy of their cross-examination responses (for the12-year-olds, the mean number of correct responses =4.57, SD = 1.28; for the adults, the mean number of cor-rect responses = 4.28, SD = 1.07).

An item analysis of the cross-examination questionsrevealed that the young children evidenced a pattern oferring in response to the leading questions. Theadolescents and adults did not show a similar patternof question-specific errors. For example, in response tothe query, "Was the man carrying his wallet in his righthand or his left hand?" 100% of the 8-year-olds repliedeither "left" or "right." The man, in fact, did not havea wallet. Thus, a correct response required correctingthe questioner. For both the adolescents and the adults,30% correctly asserted that the man had not been car-rying a wallet.

The large decline in the accuracy of the 8-year-olds'responses under cross-examination is consistent withCeci et al.'s (1987) findings regarding young children'ssusceptibility to misleading information. Ceci et al.found that very young children were significantly morelikely than were adolescents to incorporate misleading in-formation into their responses when the misinformationwas conveyed by an adult. However, when the misleadinginformation was conveyed by a 7-year-old rather than anadult, the young children were significantly less likely to

Table 1Perceived Credibility of Participant Witnesses:Preliminary Study

Dependent measure M SD

Direct-examination perceived accuracy8-year-olds 6.73 1.1312-year-olds 6.87 1.24Adults 6.87 1.13

Cross-examination perceived accuracy8-year-olds 6.10 1.1812-year-olds 6.16 1.29Adults 6.35 1.14

Direct-examination believability8-year-olds 4.96 1.3012-year-olds 4.99 1.22Adults 5.08 1.26

Cross-examination believability8-year-olds 4.57 1.0912-year-olds 4.44 1.38Adults 4.59 1.22

Overall believability8-year-olds 4.64 1.1512-year-olds 4.73 1.27Adults 4.65 1.39

Direct-examination confidence8-year-olds 4.33 1.4012-year-olds 4.43 1.26Adults 4.81 1.09

Cross-examination confidence8-year-olds 3.96 1.1112-year-olds 3.99 1.20Adults 4.06 1.28

Overall confidence8-year-olds 4.30 1.4212-year-olds 4.22 1.31Adults 4.33 1.33

include it in their reports. This finding suggests that theyoung children's suggestibility was in part due to theirreadiness to comply with an adult authority figure. Ourcross-examination questioning can be considered analo-gous to Ceci et al.'s misleading-questions environment inthat in both situations, an accurate response required dis-agreeing with or contradicting an adult. Thus, the re-duced accuracy of our 8-year-old participants' cross-ex-amination testimonies likely stemmed from the socialpressure inherent in that questioning environment.

To assess the credibility of the videotaped testimonies,we had participant jurors (N= 294) view the testimoniesand assess the credibility of each eyewitness (see Table 1for a summary of these credibility assessments). Eyewit-ness age proved to be unrelated to any of the eight credi-bility measures. For our first experiment, we transcribedthe testimonies of a subset of the 8-year-olds and a subsetof the adults. Details concerning these subsets of testimo-nies are provided below.

Experiment 1Overview

When evaluating the credibility of a child eyewitnessversus an adult eyewitness, a person may invoke different

CHILD EYEWITNESSES 321

norms (other children and other adults, respectively),which serve as anchors for judging the credibility of thesewitnesses. This suggestion is consistent with the idea ofa norm-comparison effect (Kahneman & Miller, 1986).Consider the sentence "The small man held the largenewborn baby." This sentence is understood withoutdifficulty because it simultaneously evokes two differentnorms for size. The baby is not assumed to be bigger thanthe man, even though the baby is described as being largeand the man is described as being small, because the babyis assumed to be compared with a different norm (otherbabies) than the man (other men). Our first experimentallowed us to test for norm-comparison effects in judg-ments of eyewitness credibility by disentangling partici-pant jurors' age-related expectations from any actual age-related differences in witnesses' testimonies.

Method

Participants and design. Introductory psychology students(N = 216), participating in partial fulfillment of a course re-quirement, were randomly assigned to 1 of 18 conditions in a 2(actual age of eyewitness: 8 years old vs. adult) X 3 (ostensibleage of eyewitness: 8 vs. 12 vs. 25 years old) X 3 (testimony cred-ibility: high vs. medium vs. low) between-subjects factorial de-sign. The latter factor, testimony credibility, was a categoricalclassification of the transcripts based on the composite credibil-ity scores that the raters in our preliminary study assigned tothe videotaped testimonies. The purpose of this factor was toallow us to sample from the transcripts (rather than use all 42)while at the same time to perform an ANOVA test to see if theparticipants who were judged most and least credible withineach age group by raters in our preliminary study (on the basisof evaluations of their videotaped testimonies) were also themost and least credible when participant jurors were evaluatingonly written transcript versions of their testimony.

Procedure. Participants were asked to read 1 of the 18 testi-mony transcripts provided by eyewitnesses of the filmed abduc-tion. Three transcripts were randomly selected from Grade 3students who had been assigned a high-, medium-, or low-cred-ibility score by participant jurors (as described above). Threetranscripts were selected from adult witnesses who were judgedby participant jurors in our preliminary study to be high, mod-erate, or low in terms of credibility. Three versions of each tes-timony were created by the written assertion that the witnesswas 8, 12, or 25 years old. Participants were asked to respondto questions concerning (a) the number of questions answeredcorrectly by the witness under direct examination, (b) the num-ber of questions answered correctly by the witness under cross-examination, (c) the believability of the witness under directexamination, (d) the believability of the witness under cross-examination, (e) the confidence of the witness under direct ex-amination, ( f ) the confidence of the witness under cross-exam-ination, (g) the overall believability of the eyewitness, and (h)the overall confidence of the eyewitness. All confidence and be-lievability assessments were made on 7-point scales rangingfrom 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely).

Results and Discussion

We conducted univariate ANOVAs on each of theeight measures. A significant main effect for the credi-bility classification emerged on seven of the eight mea-sures (.01 < ps < .045). The only measure that did notshow significant differences for the credibility-classifi-cation variable was the measure of believability underdirect examination (p < .40). Recall that the credibil-ity-classification variable refers to the overall perceivedcredibility of the witnesses as evaluated by participantjurors who viewed their videotaped testimonies in ourpreliminary study. This result, therefore, shows thatparticipant jurors in this experiment were able to seethe same kinds of credibility differences between wit-nesses merely on the basis of the transcripts that theparticipant jurors saw in the videotaped testimonyfrom which these transcripts were drawn.

There were no interaction effects associated with thecredibility-classification variable. There also were nomain effects or interaction effects associated with ac-tual age (all ps > A). Thus, the 8-year-olds' transcriptswere judged to be just as credible as the adults'transcripts.

More important, there were also no main effects orinteraction effects associated with ostensible age (all p s> .5). Participant jurors did not judge the witness to beless credible when described as 8 years old than whendescribed as 12 or 25 years old. These results refutethe suggestion that norm-comparison effects underlieparticipant jurors' assessments of eyewitness credibil-ity. If the stated age of the witness had prompted par-ticipant jurors to use different standards for judgingtestimony credibility, then we should have found an os-tensible-age effect. We conclude, therefore, that a wit-ness's age plays a less important role in jurors' assess-ments of the witness's credibility than does the mannerin which the witness answers questions under direct ex-amination and cross-examination.

Experiment 2

Overview

The results of our first experiment support the viewsof Leippe and Romanczyk (1989) that there may be astereotyped bias against the child eyewitness, but the pro-vision of children's actual testimony seems sufficient toovercome these preconceptions. Our second experimentaddressed the question of the fit between jurors' imaginedversus actual perceptions of the credibility of child andadult eyewitnesses.

Previous research has examined either adults' stereo-types of children's testimony (e.g., Ross et al., 1987) oradults' evaluations of videotaped testimony actually pro-

322 E. LUUS, G. WELLS, AND J. TURTLE

vided by child eyewitnesses (Leippe et al., 1992; Leippe& Romanczyk, 1989; Wells et al., 1989). The design ofthe current research, however, permits a direct compari-son of adults' stereotypes of children's eyewitness capa-bilities with adults' evaluations of actual testimony pro-vided by child versus adult eyewitnesses.

The entire set of testimony videotapes (n - 14 per agegroup: 8-year-olds, 12-year-olds, and adults) served asstimulus materials in this experiment. Participants wereasked either to view a videotape of the testimony of onewitness and to evaluate his or her credibility or to viewonly a still frame of one witness and to evaluate his or hercredibility without hearing the actual testimony. In thelatter condition, participants were instructed to imaginethe testimony the witness would provide. This procedureallowed us to directly compare participant jurors' per-ceptions of real versus imagined eyewitness capabilitiesof child and adult eyewitnesses.

Method

Participants. Eighty-five undergraduate students partici-pated in exchange for extracredit in an undergraduate psychol-ogy course.

Procedure. Participants took part in 2-4-person groups.They were seated alone in a cubicle with a television monitorand were told that they would be shown an eyewitness to a sim-ulated-crime film. Half of the participants were told that theywould be given the opportunity to view this witness's testimonyconcerning the crime. The other half of the participants weretold that they would be shown a still frame of the witness andwould be asked to imagine the testimony this witness had pro-vided. After either viewing or imagining the witness's testimony,participants were asked to judge (a) the number of questionsanswered correctly by the witness under direct examination, (b)the number of questions answered correctly by the witness un-der cross-examination, (c) the believability of the witness underdirect examination, (d) the believability of the witness undercross-examination, (e) the confidence of the witness under di-rect examination, ( f ) the confidence of the witness under cross-examination, (g) the overall believability of the eyewitness, and(h) the overall confidence of the eyewitness. All confidence andbelievability assessments were made on 7-point scales rangingfrom 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely).

Results

The dependent variables were highly intercorrelated(see Table 2). A multivariate analysis of variance(MANOVA) on the eight dependent measures witheyewitness age and presentation mode as between-subjects variables indicated a significant main effect ofpresentation mode, Wilks's lamda = 0.81, F(8, 77) =2.21, p < .05, and a significant Eyewitness Age X Pre-sentation Mode interaction, Wilks's lamda = 0.71,F( 16, 156) = 1.79, p < .05. Eyewitness age failed to

produce a significant effect, p > . 1. A summary of theperceived-credibility ratings showing significant effectsis provided in Table 3.

Given the strong intercorrelation among the depen-dent measures, we computed a single composite depen-dent variable for analysis. (Note that we first convertedthe ratings to z scores to control for the different indicesused, i.e., a 10-point scale for direct-examination ac-curacy estimates and 7-point scales for the remainingdependent measures.) An ANOVA on this compositescore yielded a significant Eyewitness Age X Presenta-tion Mode interaction, F(2, 84) = 226.25, p < .001.Eyewitness age and presentation mode did not yieldsignificant effects (ps> . 1).

Post hoc t tests yielded no significant differences be-tween participant jurors' evaluations of the still frameversus full testimony of the 12-year-olds and the adults.Participant jurors who viewed the full testimony of an 8-year-old rated the witness as more credible than did par-ticipant jurors who only imagined an 8-year-old witness'stestimony, t(84) = 3.60, p < .01. These results are dis-played in Figure 1.

General Discussion

The results of Experiment 2 suggest that adults' esti-mates of the credibility they would assign to a child eye-witness fall short of the credibility they assign upon actu-ally viewing a child testify. We found that eyewitness agetypically interacted with testimony presentation mode(i.e., whether participants viewed the full testimony ora still frame of a participant witness). Participants whoevaluated the still frame of an 8-year-old and then imag-ined that witness's testimony assigned significantly lowertestimony credibility ratings than did participants whoactually viewed the 8-year-old's testimony. Participantswho evaluated the still frame of a 12-year-old or an adultgenerally assigned comparable or slightly higher credibil-ity ratings to them when asked to imagine rather thanactually view their testimonies.

Some legal jurisdictions have started to allow child eye-witnesses to testify without being subject to cross-exami-nation (e.g., Germany and Israel). There is an assump-tion in these jurisdictions that cross-examination pre-sents special problems for children—that it willinappropriately confuse them and shatter their apparentconfidence and credibility. We found no support for thecontention that cross-examination serves to shatter theperceived confidence or credibility of children's oral tes-timony. In fact, participant jurors who had the opportu-nity to view the witnesses' full testimonies rated the 8-year-olds as more accurate and equally confident and be-lievable under cross-examination as the adults. These re-sults were obtained in spite of the fact that the participant

CHILD EYEWITNESSES 323

Table 2Correlations Among Measures: Study 2

Dependent measure 1

1 . Direct-examination accuracy2. Direct-examination believability3. Direct-examination confidence4. Cross-examination accuracy5. Cross-examination believability6. Cross-examination confidence7. Overall believability8. Overall confidence

.65

.61

.43

.34

.33

.49

.40

—.69.37.47.41.64.49

—.32.41.49.55.65

—.77.73.69.65

—.78 —.86 .70 —.67 .81 .72 —

Note. All correlations are significant at p < .01.

jurors were highly discriminating in the way that theyevaluated the eyewitnesses. There was considerable vari-ance across the 42 witnesses in the ways that participantjurors perceived the confidence, believability, and accu-racy of the witnesses. Furthermore, as evidenced in Ex-periment 1, there was good interjudge reliability as towhich witnesses were thought to be the most confident,believable, and accurate.

There is some cause for concern, perhaps, that partici-pant jurors found the 8-year-old witnesses to be as credi-

ble as the 12-year-old and adult witnesses in their cross-examination testimony when, in fact, the 8-year-oldswere significantly less accurate. This is yet another exam-ple of the difficulty that participant jurors seem to havein evaluating the accuracy of eyewitness testimony (e.g.,Wells, 1993; Wells & Leippe, 1981; Wells et al., 1979).In part, the difficulty stems from the fact that eyewitnessconfidence does not closely track eyewitness accuracy(e.g., Brigham & Cairns, 1988; Leippe, Wells, & Ostrom,1978; Parker & Caranza, 1989; Wells & Murray, 1983),

Table 3Perceived Credibility of Participant Witnesses: Study 2

Testimony presentation mode

Still frame Full testimony

Dependent measure M SD M SD '(84)

Direct-examination confidence8-year-olds12-year-oldsAdults

Cross-examination accuracy8-year-olds1 2-year-oldsAdults

Cross-examination confidence8-year-olds12-year-oldsAdults

Cross-examination believability8-year-olds12-year-oldsAdults

Overall confidence8-year-olds12-year-oldsAdults

Overall believability8-year-olds12-year-oldsAdults

3.475.204.43

3.404.273.53

3.004.533.73

4.025.204.00

3.405.204.00

4.004.934.33

1.211.321.14

1.301.491.06

1.131.851.22

1.281.691.16

1.391.27.85

1.311.391.11

4.404.074.47

5.074.403.80

4.933.733.67

5.704.004.20

4.934.004.20

5.134.474.60

1.341.281.26

1.581.121.52

1.911.221.40

1.411.221.45

1.391.071.57

1.311.131.30

2.13*2.58*

<1

3.36**<1<1

3.57**<1<1

2.81**<1<1

3.40**2.67*

<1

2.46**<1

<1

Note. Ratings were made on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). 7" test comparesstill frame and full testimony.*p<.05. **p<.01.

324 E. LUUS, G. WELLS, AND J. TURTLE

8 years1 2 yearsAdult

-6STILL FULL

PRESENTATION

Figure 1. Perceived credibility of participant witnesses. Still = still frame; full = fulltestimony.

whereas the believability of eyewitness testimony doesclosely track eyewitness confidence (e.g., Wells et al.,1979). Recent research has suggested that the difficultymight also derive from the malleable nature of eyewitnessconfidence (Luus & Wells, 1994).

The current data suggest that the low diagnosticvalue of eyewitness confidence holds for children aswell as adults. Indeed, the perceived confidence of the8-year-olds' testimony under cross-examination wasequal to that of adults, whereas accuracy was lower, andthe overall believability of the witnesses correspondedclosely to the witnesses' overall confidence (r = .79).We believe that if the 8-year-olds' confidence undercross-examination had been lower than that of theadults, the participant jurors would have attributedless accuracy and believability to the 8-year-olds' cross-examination testimony.

There are several possible solutions to the problem ofcross-examination producing lower accuracy but notlower perceived credibility for the 8-year-old witnessesversus the adult witnesses. One type of solution is to trainor educate jurors to be sensitive to the problems thatyoung children have with cross-examination comparedwith direct-examination testimony. Another possibility isto follow the lead of some jurisdictions in discouragingor disallowing the use of cross-examination with youngchildren and using only the nonleading styles of question-ing. We find neither of these proposed solutions satisfac-tory. The disallowance of cross-examination tends to in-terfere with the rights of the accused and violates some

basic tenets of the adversarial system. The juror-sensitiv-ity proposal suffers from the general concerns expressedby researchers over trying to estimate the accuracy of wit-nesses rather than improve the accuracy of witnesses. Al-though we acknowledge recent experimental success insensitizing jurors to eyewitness problems (e.g., Cutler,Penrod, & Dexter, 1989) and we agree in principle thatexpert testimony on these matters should be accepted bythe courts (see Kassin, Ellsworth, & Smith, 1989), wefavor the development of techniques that can improve theaccuracy and the completeness of eyewitness testimony(e.g., see Fisher, Geiselman, & Amador, 1989; Saywitz &Snyder, 1993; Warren-Leubecker, Hulse-Trotter, &Tubbs, 1991). Perhaps these children could benefit froma precourtroom testimony session in which they weretaught to recognize discrepancies between their memoryand external suggestions, thereby allowing them to resistsome of the debilitating effects of cross-examination. Ourdata clearly demonstrate that there is enough variation inperceived credibility within ages to preclude the assump-tion that any one person could represent the perceivedcredibility of that age group.

Conclusion

We argue that research on the perceived credibility ofchild versus adult eyewitnesses must use designs in whicheyewitnesses are a sampled variable within age levels andthe eyewitnesses are testifying in their own words. Suchdesigns are necessary to capture the natural variance

CHILD EYEWITNESSES 325

within ages and thereby avoid the unwarranted assump-tion that a single child is representative of all children ofthat child's age.

In general, our results support the contention ofLeippe and Romanczyk (1989) that there is an age-basedbias or stereotype against the child eyewitness, but whenobservers view the actual testimony of the children, thebias is suspended. The reader is cautioned, however, toconsider several limitations to this work for purposes ofgeneralization.

First, our conclusions are limited to the boundaries ofthe ages that were used in these studies. Had we used 4-year-old or 6-year-old children, for example, we mighthave found considerable differences between children ofsuch young age and adult eyewitnesses, even with the pro-vision of their full testimony (see Leippe et al., 1992).

Second, we acknowledge several differences betweenactual courtrooms and the setting in which our child par-ticipants were questioned. Recent research has suggestedthat children may be less accurate in testifying under con-ditions with high forensic realism rather than a labora-tory setting (e.g., Hill & Hill, 1987; Saywitz & Na-thanson, 1993; Tobey& Goodman, 1992). Testimony ofthe children and the adults in this research was taken ina familiar, friendly setting (their own schools) and did notinclude spectators. Under these conditions, the 8-year-oldand adult eyewitnesses were generally perceived to beequally confident and credible in testifying. In an actualcourtroom, however, the average 8-year-old may be rela-tively more nervous and appear less confident than theaverage adult, which, in turn, might confirm rather thandisconfirm an observer's preconception of the child as be-ing less likely to be correct.

These results provide an answer to the question of thefit between the imagined versus actual eyewitness capa-bilities of children and adults. Our findings suggest thatjurors may enter the courtroom with a negative biasagainst child witnesses. However, qualities of the wit-ness's testimony seem to play a more important role thandoes the witness's age in judgments of eyewitness credi-bility. That is, people might imagine children to beincapable of delivering accurate, convincing testimony.However, these preconceived negative views of child eye-witnesses may be quickly discarded on actually observinga child testify.

References

Brigham, J. C, & Cairns, D. L. (1988). The effect of mugshotinspections on eyewitness identification accuracy. Journal ofApplied Social Psychology, 18, 1394-1410.

Ceci, S. J., Ross, D. E, & Toglia, M. P. (1987). Age differencesin suggestibility: Narrowing the uncertainties. In S. J. Ceci,M. P. Toglia, & D. E Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness mem-ory (pp. 79-91). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Cohen, R. L., & Harnick, M. A. (1980). The susceptibility ofchild witnesses to suggestion: An empirical study. Law andHuman Behavior, 4, 201-210.

Cutler, B. L., Penrod, S. D., & Dexter, H. R. (1989). The eye-witness, the expert psychologist, and the jury. Law and Hu-man Behavior, 13, 311-332.

Fisher, R. P., Geiselman, R. E., & Amador, M. (1989). Fieldtest of the cognitive interview: Enhancing the recollections ofactual victims and witnesses of crimes. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 74, 722-727.

Goodman, G. S., Golding, J. M., & Haith, M. M. (1984). Ju-rors' reactions to child witnesses. Journal of Social Issues,40, 139-156.

Goodman, G. S., Golding, J. M., Helgeson, V. S., Haith, M. M.,& Michelli, J. (1987). When a child takes the stand: Jurors'perceptions of children's eyewitness testimony. Law and Hu-man Behavior, 11, 27-40.

Harter, S. (1982). The Perceived Competence Scale for Chil-dren. Child Development, 53, 87-97.

Hill, P. E., & Hill, S. M. (1987). Videotaping children's testi-mony: An empirical view. Michigan Law Review, 85, 809-833.

Johnson, M. K., & Foley, M. A. (1984). Differentiating factfrom fantasy: The reliability of children's memory. Journalof Social Issues, 40, 33-50.

Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psy-chological Review, 88, 67-85.

Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Compar-ing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136-153.

Kassin, S. M., Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, V. L. (1989). The"general acceptance" of psychological research on eyewitnesstestimony. American Psychologist, 44, 1089-1098.

Kelley, H. H. (1972). Attribution in social interaction. InE. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S.Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causesof behavior (pp. 1-26). Morristown, NJ: General LearningPress.

King, M. A., & Yuille, J. C. (1987). Suggestibility and the childwitness. In S. J. Ceci, M. P. Toglia, & D. E Ross (Eds.), Chil-dren 's eyewitness memory (pp. 24-35). New \fork: Springer-Verlag.

Leippe, M. R., Manion, A. P., & Romanczyk, A. (1992). Eye-witness persuasion: How and how well do fact finders judgethe accuracy of adults' and children's memory reports?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 181 -197.

Leippe, M. R., & Romanczyk, A. (1987). Children on the wit-ness stand: A communication/persuasion analysis of jurors'reactions to child witnesses. In S. J. Ceci, M. P. Toglia, &D. F. Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness memory (pp. 155-177). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Leippe, M. R., & Romanczyk, A. (1989). Reactions to child(versus adult) eyewitnesses. Law and Human Behavior, 13,102-132.

Leippe, M. R., Wells, G. L., & Ostrom, T. M. (1978). Crimeseriousness as a determinant of accuracy in eyewitness iden-tification. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 345-351.

Luus, C. A. E., & Wells, G. L. (1994). The malleability of eye-

326 E. LUUS, G. WELLS, AND J. TURTLE

witness confidence: Co-witness and perseverance effects.Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 714-723.

Marin, B. V., Holmes, D. L., Guth, M., & Kovac, P. (1979).The potential of children as eyewitnesses: A comparison ofchildren and adults on eyewitness tasks. Law and Human Be-havior, 3, 295-306.

Melton, G. (1980). Children's concepts of their rights. Journalof Clinical Child Psychology, 9, 186-190.

Parker, J. E, & Caranza, L. E. (1989). Eyewitness testimony ofchildren in target-present and target-absent lineups. Law andHuman Behavior, 13, 133-149.

Ross, D. F., Dunning, D., Toglia, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1991). Thechild in the eyes of the jury: Assessing mock juror perceptionsof the child witness. Law and Human Behavior, 14, 5-23.

Ross, D. F., Miller, B. S., & Moran, P. B. (1987). The child inthe eyes of the jury: Assessing mock jurors' perceptions of thechild witness. In S. J. Ceci, M. P. Toglia, & D. F. Ross (Eds.),Children's eyewitness memory (pp. 142-154). New York:Springer-Verlag.

Saywitz, K. J. (1987). Children's testimony: Age-related pat-terns of memory errors. In S. J. Ceci, M. P. Toglia, &D. F. Ross (Eds.), Children's eyewitness memory (pp. 36-52). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Saywitz, K. J. (1989). Children's conceptions of the legal sys-tem: "Court is a place to play basketball." In S. J. Ceci,D. F. Ross, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.), Perspectives on children'stestimony (pp. 131-157). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Saywitz, K. J., & Nathanson, R. (1993). Children's testimonyand their perceptions of stress in and out of the courtroom.International Journal of Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 613-622.

Saywitz, K. J., & Snyder, L. S. (1993). Improving children'stestimony with preparation. In G. Goodman & B. Bottoms(Eds.), Child victims, child witnesses: Understanding and im-

proving children's testimony (pp. 117-146). New York:Guilford.

Tobey, A. E., & Goodman, G. S. (1992). Children's eyewitnessmemory: Effects of participation and forensic context. ChildAbuse and Neglect, 16, 779-786.

Warren-Leubecker, A., Hulse-Trotter, K., & Tubbs, E. (1991).Inducing resistance to suggestibility in children. Law and Hu-man Behavior, 15, 273-286.

Wells, G. L. (1993). What do we know about eyewitness iden-tification? American Psychologist, 48, 553-571.

Wells, G. L., & Leippe, M. R. (1981). How do triers of fact inferthe accuracy of eyewitness identification? Using memory fordetail can be misleading. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66,682-687.

Wells, G. L., Lindsay, R. C. L., & Ferguson, T. J. (1979). Accu-racy, confidence, and juror perceptions in eyewitness identi-fication. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 440-448.

Wells, G. L., & Murray, D. M. (1983). What can psychologysay about the Neil v. Biggers criteria for judging eyewitnessaccuracy? Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 347-362.

Wells, G. L., Turtle, J. W, & Luus, C. A. E. (1989). The per-ceived credibility of child eyewitnesses: What happens whenthey use their own words? In S. J. Ceci, D. F. Ross, & M. P.Toglia (Eds.), Perspectives on children's testimony (pp. 23-46). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Whipple, G. W. (1911). The psychology of testimony. Psycho-logical Bulletin, 8, 307-309.

Yarmey, A. D., & Jones, H. P. T. (1983). Is the study of eyewit-ness identification a matter of common sense? In S. Lloyd-Bostock & B. R. Clifford (Eds.), Evaluating witness evidence(pp. 13-40). Chichester, England: Wiley.

Received June 28, 1993Revision received August 1, 1994

Accepted August 2, 1994 •