child temperament and parental personality - helda -

91
Child temperament and parental personality: continuity and transactional change Niina Komsi Department of Psychology University of Helsinki, Finland Academic dissertation to be publicly discussed, by due permission of the Faculty of Behavioral Sciences at the University of Helsinki in Auditorium XIII, Unioninkatu 34, on the 5 th of June, 2009, at 12 o’clock UNIVERSITY OF HELSINKI Department of Psychology Studies 60: 2009

Upload: others

Post on 11-Sep-2021

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

Child temperament and parental personality:

continuity and transactional change

Niina Komsi

Department of Psychology University of Helsinki, Finland

Academic dissertation to be publicly discussed, by due permission of the Faculty of Behavioral Sciences

at the University of Helsinki in Auditorium XIII, Unioninkatu 34, on the 5th

of June, 2009, at 12 o’clock

UNIVERSITY OF HELSINKI Department of Psychology

Studies 60: 2009

Page 2: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

2

Supervisor: Professor Katri Räikkönen-Talvitie Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, Finland Reviewers: Professor Sam Putnam Psychology Department, Bowdoin College, USA Professor Maria A. Gartstein Department of Psychology, Washington State University, USA Opponent: Professor Franz J. Neyer Institut für Psychologie, Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena, Germany

ISSN 0781-8254 ISBN 978-952-10-5593-5 (pbk.) ISBN 978-952-10-5594-2 (PDF)

http://www.ethesis.helsinki.fi Helsinki University Printing House

Helsinki 2009

Page 3: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

3

CONTENTS ABSTRACT 5 TIIVISTELMÄ 7 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 9 LIST OF ORIGINAL PUBLICATIONS 10 1 INTRODUCTION 11 1.1 Temperament dimensions and personality traits: theoretically, temporally and practically related constructs 11 1.2 Temperament in childhood within the developmental framework of Mary

K. Rothbart 12

1.2.1 Continuity of temperament from infancy onwards 13 1.2.2 Mothers and fathers as informants of the child’s temperament 16 1.3 Personality traits in adulthood 18 1.3.1 Stability and change in personality traits 18 1.3.2 Personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism during

adulthood 20

1.4 Transactional development of parental personality and child temperament

21

1.4.1 Implications of child temperament for parental extraversion and neuroticism

24

1.4.2 Implications of parental extraversion and neuroticism for child temperament

25

1.4.3 Child temperament and parental situational factors: the case of parental stress

27

1.4.3.1 Implications of child temperament for parental stress 28 1.4.3.2 Implications of parental stress for child temperament 30 1.5 Aims of the study 31 1.5.1 Continuity of temperament from infancy to middle childhood 32 1.5.1.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated temperament 32 1.5.1.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated temperament 34 1.5.2 Transactional development of parent and child characteristics

from infancy to middle childhood 34

1.5.2.1 Study III. Parental personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism and infant/child temperament

34

1.5.2.2 Study IV. Mother’s overall stress and infant/child temperament

35

2 METHODS 37 2.1 Outline of the study 37 2.2 Participants 37 2.3 Measures 38 2.3.1 Infant temperament 38 2.3.2 Child temperament 40 2.3.3 Parental personality traits 41 2.3.4 Perceived stress 41 2.4 Statistical analyses 42

Page 4: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

4

3 RESULTS 44 3.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated temperament from infancy to middle

childhood 44

3.1.1 Continuity of temperament subscales and superconstructs 44 3.1.2 Person-level continuity of temperament: “temperament types” 47 3.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated temperament and congruence

between mother- and father-rated continuity 49

3.2.1 Continuity of father-rated temperament subscales and superconstructs

49

3.2.2 Congruence between mother- and father-rated continuity of temperament

52

3.3 Study III. Transaction between parental personality traits and child temperament

54

3.4 Study IV. Transaction between maternal stress and child temperament 57 3.4.1 Transaction between maternal stress and individual

temperament subscales 57

3.4.2 Transaction between maternal stress and latent temperament superconstructs

58

4 DISCUSSION 59 4.1 Continuity of parent-rated temperament from infancy to middle

childhood 59

4.2 Developmental transaction between parent and child characteristics 65 4.2.1 Transactional development of parental personality traits and

child temperament 65

4.2.2 Transactional development of maternal stress and child temperament

71

4.3 General conclusions 73 4.4 Weaknesses and strengths of the study 74 4.5 Theoretical and clinical implications 76 REFERENCES 81

Page 5: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

5

Abstract Studying the continuity and underlying mechanisms of temperament change from early

childhood through adulthood is clinically and theoretically relevant. Knowledge of the

continuity and change of temperament from infancy onwards, especially as perceived by

both parents is, however, still scanty. Only in recent years have researchers become

aware that personality, long considered as stable in adulthood, may also change.

Further, studies that focus on the transactional change of child temperament and

parental personality also seem to be lacking, as are studies focusing on transactions

between child temperament and more transient parental characteristics, like parental

stress. Therefore, this longitudinal study examined the degree of continuity of

temperament over five years from the infant’s age of six months to the child’s age of

five and a half years, as perceived by both biological parents, and also investigated the

bidirectional effects between child temperament and parents’ personality traits and

overall stress experienced during that time.

First, moderate to high levels of continuity of temperament from infancy to middle

childhood were shown, depicting the developmental links between affectively positive

and well-adjusted temperament characteristics, and between characteristics of early and

later negative affectivity. The continuity of temperament was quantitatively and

qualitatively similar in both parents’ ratings. The findings also demonstrate that infant

and childhood temperament characteristics cluster to form temperament types that

resemble personality types shown in child and adult personality studies.

Second, the parental personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism were shown

to be highly stable over five years, but evidence of change in relation to parents’ views

of their child’s temperament was also shown: an infant’s higher positive affectivity

predicted an increase in parental extraversion, while the infant’s higher activity level

predicted a decrease in parental neuroticism over five years. Furthermore, initially

higher parental extraversion predicted higher ratings of the child’s effortful control,

while initially higher parental neuroticism predicted the child’s higher negative

affectivity. In terms of changes in parental stress, the infant’s higher activity level

predicted a decrease in maternal overall stress, while initially higher maternal stress

predicted a higher level of child negative affectivity in middle childhood.

Page 6: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

6

Together, the results demonstrate that the mother- and father-rated temperament of

the child shows continuity during the early years of life, but also support the view that

the development of temperament is sensitive to important contextual factors such as

parental personality and overall stress. While parental personality and experienced

stress were shown to have an effect on the child’s developing temperament, the reverse

was also true: the parents’ own personality traits and perceived stress seemed to be

highly stable, but also susceptible to their experiences of their child’s temperament.

Page 7: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

7

Tiivistelmä Temperamentin ja persoonallisuuden pysyvyyden, sekä niiden muuttumiseen

vaikuttavien mekanismien selvittäminen varhaisvuosien ja aikuisuuden aikana on sekä

teoreettisesti että kliinisesti merkityksellistä. Tieto temperamentin pysyvyydestä ja

muuttumisesta vauvaiästä eteenpäin, erityisesti molempien vanhempien arvioimana on

kuitenkin yhä vähäistä. Vasta viime vuosina tutkijat ovat tulleet tietoisiksi siitä, että

myös aikuisiässä pysyvänä pidetty persoonallisuus saattaa muuttua. Vielä ei kuitenkaan

ole tutkittu sitä miten lapsen temperamentti ja vanhemman persoonallisuus vaikuttavat

toisiinsa ajan myötä. Vanhemman tilannekohtaisempien ominaisuuksien, kuten stressin

määrän, ja lapsen temperamentin vastavuoroisia vaikutuksia ei myöskään ole vielä

riittävästi tutkittu. Tässä pitkittäistutkimuksessa tarkasteltiin lapsen temperamentin

pysyvyyttä kuuden kuukauden iästä viiden ja puolen vuoden ikään, lapsen molempien

biologisten vanhempien arvioimana. Lisäksi tarkasteltiin kaksisuuntaista vaikutusta

lapsen temperamentin ja vanhemman persoonallisuuden ja stressitason välillä näiden

viiden vuoden aikana.

Tulokset osoittivat lapsen temperamenttiominaisuuksilla olevan kohtalainen tai

vahva pysyvyys vauvaiästä viiden ja puolen vuoden ikään. Kehityksellisiä yhteyksiä

löydettiin sekä myönteiseen affektiivisuuteen ja sopeutuvuuteen liittyvien

ominaisuuksien välillä, että negatiiviseen affektiivisuuteen liittyvien ominaisuuksien

välillä. Temperamentin pysyvyys näyttäytyi samanlaisena molempien vanhempien

arvioissa. Tutkimus osoitti myös miten temperamenttipiirteet vauvaiästä lähtien

yhdistyvät persoonallisuustutkimuksessa vakiintuneita lasten ja aikuisten

persoonallisuustyyppejä vastaaviksi temperamenttityypeiksi.

Vanhempien ekstraversio- ja neurotisismi –persoonallisuuspiirteet olivat varsin

pysyviä viiden vuoden aikana, mutta tutkimus osoitti myös piirteiden muuttumisen

suhteessa vanhemman näkemykseen lapsen temperamentista: vauvan korkeammalle

arvioitu positiivinen affektiivisuus ennusti vanhemman korkeampaa ekstraversiota, ja

vauvan korkeampi aktiivisuustaso ennusti vanhemman alempaa neurotisismin tasoa

viiden vuoden jälkeen. Samaan aikaan, vanhemman alun perin korkeampi ekstraversion

taso ennusti lapsen temperamentin korkeampaa tahdonalaista itsesäätelyä, kun taas alun

alkaen korkeampi vanhemman neurotisismi ennusti lapsen korkeampaa negatiivista

affektiivisuutta viiden vuoden jälkeen. Vanhemman näkemys vauvasta aktiivisempana

Page 8: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

8

ennusti myös äidin vähäisempää stressiä viiden vuoden jälkeen, ja äidin alun alkaen

korkeampi stressitaso ennusti lapsen kasvavaa negatiivista affektiivisuutta viiden

vuoden jälkeen.

Kokonaisuudessaan tutkimus osoittaa lapsen temperamentin pysyvyyden

ensimmäisten viiden vuoden aikana, sekä äidin että isän arvioimana, mutta tukee myös

käsitystä temperamentin kehityksen alttiudesta tärkeille ympäristötekijöille, kuten

vanhempien persoonallisuuspiirteille ja vanhemman stressin määrälle. Samalla kun

vanhemman persoonallisuus ja koetun stressin määrä vaikuttivat lapsen temperamentin

kehitykseen, nämä vanhemman sinänsä varsin pysyviksi osoittautuneet ominaisuudet

olivat myös alttiita lapsen temperamenttiominaisuuksien vaikutukselle.

Page 9: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

9

Acknowledgements I would like to thank several people who have contributed to and supported me in this

endeavour.

My deepest gratitude I owe to my supervisor Professor Katri Räikkönen-Talvitie for

offering me the opportunity to undertake this task. Without her research expertise,

dedication and clarity of thought, along with her exceptional ability to solve the most

insoluble problems, this work would not have been possible.

I wish to thank the reviewers of this thesis, Professor Maria A. Gartstein and

Professor Sam Putnam, for their valuable comments that helped me improve this work.

I also thank my co-authors, Docent Anna-Liisa Järvenpää and Professor Timo E.

Strandberg, for their supportive collaboration in the Glaku- project.

I am deeply indebted to the whole Developmental Psychology research group, and

especially Docent Kati Heinonen-Tuomaala and Docent Anu-Katriina Pesonen, for their

unforgettable helpfulness and never-ending support. Sharing the joys and difficulties of

this work with them has been a privilege. I am also deeply grateful to Researcher Pertti

Keskivaara for his enthusiasm for solving statistical problems with me.

This work was carried out in the Department of Psychology at the University of

Helsinki, and was made financially possible by the Finnish Graduate School of

Psyhcology, the Academy of Finland, the University of Helsinki and the Signe and Ane

Gyllenberg, Jahnsson and Juho Vainio foundations, which I gratefully acknowledge.

I offer my heartfelt thanks to my mother Irja and my father Mauri, who have always

been there for me and have had faith in me in all my endeavours. And finally, I need to

express my love and gratitude to my husband Oki, who has been the best companion I

could ever dream of. He is also the most caring and trustworthy father to our children

Joel and Annina. Whatever I do, they are my greatest source of happiness and

inspiration, and to them I dedicate this work.

Helsinki, May 2009 Niina Komsi

Page 10: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

10

LIST OF ORIGINAL PUBLICATIONS

I Komsi, N, Räikkönen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Heinonen, K., Keskivaara, P.,

Järvenpää, A-L., & Strandberg, T.E. (2006). Continuity of temperament from

infancy to middle childhood. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 494-508.

II Komsi, N., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Keskivaara, P.,

Järvenpää, A-L., & Strandberg, T.E. (2008). Continuity of father-rated

temperament from infancy to middle childhood. Infant Behavior & Development,

31, 239-254.

III Komsi, N., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Keskivaara, P.,

Järvenpää, A-L., & Strandberg, T.E. (2008). Transactional development of parent

personality and child temperament. European Journal of Personality, 22, 553–

573.

IV Pesonen, A-K., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Komsi, N., Järvenpää, A-L., &

Strandberg, T.E. (2008). A transactional model of temperamental development:

evidence of a relationship between child temperament and maternal stress over

five years. Social Development, 17, 326-340.

Page 11: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

11

1 INTRODUCTION 1.1 Temperament dimensions and personality traits:

theoretically, temporally and practically related constructs The main assumption in personality trait theory is that traits represent partly heritable,

temperament -based individual predispositions, which show consistency across time and

situations (McCrae et al., 2000). In other words, the structure of adult personality is

presumed to emerge from early temperament that serves as a substrate for personality

development (Costa & McCrae, 2000). In recent literature, the phenomena described by

temperament and personality constructs have been concluded to be more alike than

different (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005; Mervielde, De Clercq, De Fruyt, & Van

Leeuwen, 2005), and even in the temperament dimensions emerging from infancy

research, similarities with the higher order factor structures extracted from adult studies

of personality can be seen (Rothbart & Ahadi, 1994; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000a).

Therefore, it is not surprising that research on temperament and personality has

increasingly concentrated on questions of development.

Temperament in childhood has generally been described as the constellation of

inborn traits that determines a child’s unique behavioral style and the way he or she

experiences and reacts to the world (Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart & Bates, 2006).

There is general agreement between different temperament theorists on the biologically-

based individual differences that show modest to moderate degrees of continuity during

infancy and from early childhood onwards (Lemery, Goldsmith, Klinnert, & Mrazek,

1999; Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart & Bates, 2006): temperament is thus thought to

describe those aspects of behavioral response that are not due to interactions with

caregivers or other environmental influences, but rather those aspects that are present

from birth and biologically based. Further, although temperament is traditionally

viewed as consisting of these biologically-based behavioral traits, its manifestation in

behavioral patterns is of interest: it is such patterns that influence children’s interactions

in the world, and to which parents, teachers, and peers respond (Calkins, Blandon,

Williford, & Keane, 2007).

In developmental research, the study of the underlying mechanisms that account for

stability and/or change in temperament and personality from the early years onwards

and during adulthood bears great theoretical as well as clinical relevance. From a

Page 12: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

12

theoretical point of view, research on temporal stability and change and its correlates

can shed light on the relative influence of maturational and environmental factors on

temperament and personality development. In order to be able to examine the dynamics

and correlates of developmental stability and change, the researchers are, however, first

challenged to detect and establish the degree of continuity and developmental courses of

the manifestations of temperament and personality from the very early stages onwards

(Caspi & Roberts, 1999; Goldsmith, 1996; Rothbart, 1989; Rothbart & Bates, 2006).

From a practical or clinical point of view, a better understanding of temporal stability

and change in temperament and personality characteristics can inform therapeutic

approaches to personality-related psychological problems and help to develop

interventions tailored to the needs of individual clients (Widiger, Costa, & McCrae,

2002). Given the importance of such questions, it is no wonder that recent years have

seen a surge in meta-analyses examining longitudinal stability and change in personality

traits during adulthood (see Ardelt, 2000; Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000; Roberts,

Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006), along with increasing evidence of temperament- and

personality-related antecedents and correlates of health and adjustment at all age stages

(Nigg, 2006; Hampson, 2008; Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007;

Shipley, Weiss, Der, Taylor, & Deary, 2007). However, generalizations from previous

studies on personality development are still limited. Our knowledge of the development

of early temperament in itself is also still scanty, as is our knowledge of individual

differences in change and its predictors in adult personality.

1.2 Temperament in childhood within the developmental

framework of Mary K. Rothbart According to Mary K. Rothbart and her colleagues, temperament refers to

constitutionally-based individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation (Rothbart

& Bates, 2006). Constitutional in this sense (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981) refers to the

biological basis of temperament, which becomes influenced over time by heredity,

maturation, and experience. The general factor of reactivity is seen as being directly

related to the responsivity of the nervous system to sensory stimulation, referring to the

excitability, responsivity or arousability of the physiological and behavioral systems.

Page 13: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

13

Self-regulation, in turn, refers to the neural and behavioral processes functioning to

modulate this reactivity (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981).

In Rothbart’s conceptualization, temperament in infancy refers to early differences in

motor activity, smile-proneness, soothability, attention span, anger-proneness and

fearful distress, operationalized either in a laboratory observation (Goldsmith &

Rothbart, 1991, 1996), or in parent reports (Rothbart, 1981). The subscales further form

two latent superconstructs, labeled positive and negative affectivity. Not all of these

infant temperament characteristics are present at birth (smile, fear), or are not

sufficiently matured to function as valid predictors of later temperamental development.

Therefore, temperamental continuity measured either through distinct behavioral

characteristics or through positive/negative affectivity superconstructs, is assumed to

become apparent only after the early months of infancy.

In childhood, the central concepts of temperament, such as reactivity, arousability,

and self regulation remain, but it is the individual differences in the ability to utilize

inhibitory and attentional mechanisms in the service of regulation that begin to

characterize childhood temperament (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981). By the third year

of life, initial reactivity as the dominant developmental theme is replaced by behavior

regulation (Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). That is, as the child develops, initial

reactivity and early efforts to regulate the reactivity will be supplemented by an

increasing capacity for voluntary forms of control (Rothbart & Bates, 2006).

Childhood temperament can be depicted by the individual domains of activity level,

anger-proneness, positive anticipation, attentional focusing, discomfort related to

sensory qualities of stimulation, soothability, fear, the amount of pleasure related to

high or low stimulus intensity, impulsivity, inhibitory control, low intensity pleasure,

perceptual sensitivity, sadness, shyness, and the amount of smiling and laughter

(Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 2001). These domains can be further combined

into the distinct superconstructs of extraversion, effortful control and negative

affectivity (Rothbart et al., 2001).

1.2.1 Continuity of temperament from infancy onwards

The continuity of temperamental characteristics over time and in different contexts is,

by definition, among the hallmarks of Rothbart’s theory, and the early appearing

Page 14: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

14

temperamental differences are assumed to provide a foundation for later emotional and

personality development (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997). Within this framework, the

developmental discontinuity or lawful change (Caspi & Roberts, 1999; Kagan, 1971;

Rothbart & Bates, 2006) in early temperament can also be theoretically defined and

understood. As a developing entity with increasing and differentiating manifestations

from infancy onwards (Rothbart & Bates, 2006), temperament can thus be defined in

terms of both homotypic and heterotypic continuity over time. Further, also dynamic

relations between genetically unrelated traits are acknowledged, i.e. the development of

one trait can implicate changes in the development of another, genetically unrelated trait

(Putnam et al., 2008).

By the concept homotypic continuity, the literal continuity, or stability over time of a

temperament characteristic is indicated, whereas heterotypic continuity refers to the

continuity between phenotypically different, but genotypically related attributes of

temperament (see Caspi & Roberts, 1999). To be interpreted as heterotypic continuity,

or coherence, as it has also been called, temperamental continuity has to be theoretically

defined a priori: the models of continuity require theoretically derived predictions

relating certain types of infant temperament to specific behaviors in childhood; these

predictions ought to be based upon theories of development that allow one to predict

age-appropriate changes in the phenotypic expression of a common genotype (Caspi &

Roberts, 1999). For example, one might predict that the same pattern of fear responses

to novelty observed in infancy would be found in later childhood, depicting homotypic

continuity, while heterotypic continuity would refer to relations between earlier and

later fearful behavior that are based upon age-appropriate and logical associations rather

than similarities in exact behaviors. That is, although infant negative affectivity can be

composed of fear and anger, the manifestations of negative affectivity in later childhood

are proposed to encompass more behavioral characteristics, such as proneness to

discomfort and expressions of sadness.

As the measures for infant and childhood temperament conceptualize temperament

similarly, but differ in the number of dimensions, the structural continuity of

temperament in this work is also not explored as defined by Caspi, i.e., the persistence

of correlational patterns among a set of variables across time (Caspi & Roberts, 1999),

but will rather describe associations across time between temperament constructs that

Page 15: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

15

are based on individual dimensions of temperament in infancy and on individual

dimensions of temperament in childhood.

Furthermore, these descriptions of variable-level continuity of temperament do not,

however, mirror continuity at the person (within-subject) level. While the research

focusing on temperament-variables assesses their interpersonal correlational structure,

utilizing the above-mentioned concepts, the person-centered approach examines typical

within-person configurations (Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Bergman & Magnusson,

2001; see also Robins & Tracy, 2003), and suggests yet another type of continuity, i.e.,

ipsative continuity (Caspi & Roberts, 1999). Essentially, ipsative continuity is

equivalent to structural continuity except it is considered at the level of the individual

rather than a group or population. The person-centered approach thus depicts the

development of temperament by individual temperament types or profiles, where each

temperament dimension derives its meaning from its relations to the other dimensions

within the individual.

All in all, given the fact that Rothbart’s theory is among the most prominent current

theories of temperament, and that research activity emphasizing this particular approach

is intensifying, studies focusing on the continuity of temperament within Rothbart’s

psychobiological framework are still surprisingly scarce. Rothbart’s conceptual and

operational definitions have been used in studying continuity of temperament within

developmental phases, i.e., during infancy (Carnicero, Pérez-López, Salinas, &

Martinéz-Fuentes, 2000; Denham, Lehman, Moser, & Reeves, 1995; Gartstein et al.,

2006; Lemery et al., 1999; Rothbart, 1981, 1986; Stifter, Willoughby, & Towe-

Goodman, 2008), toddlerhood (Putnam, Gartstein, & Rothbart, 2006) and childhood

(Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; Majdandzic & van den Boom, 2008; Rothbart et al.,

2001), but to my knowledge, only three studies have extended the scope across different

developmental phases from infancy to childhood (Aksan et al., 1999; Putnam, Rothbart,

& Gartstein, 2008; Rothbart, Derryberry, & Hershey, 2000b).

In the study by Rothbart and her colleagues (2000b), theoretically relevant continuity

of temperament from infancy to the child’s age of seven was shown in a sample of 26

children; however, due to the small sample size, the researchers highlighted the tentative

nature of the results (Rothbart et al., 2000b). Recently, Putnam and his colleagues

(2008) reported significant homotypic and heterotypic continuity for mother-rated

Page 16: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

16

characteristics of positive and negative affectivity from infancy to toddler and preschool

age.

Further, following the ipsative, person-level approach, temperament types in

childhood have previously been identified by Aksan and his colleagues (1999). They

placed 15% of 488 children in “expressive-noncontrolled” and “nonexpressive-

controlled” categories based on maternal ratings of the 31/2 and 41/2 year-old child’s

temperament, and considered whether these childhood types were different with respect

to infant temperament: relative to the “controlled-nonexpressive” children, the

“noncontrolled-expressive” children were found to have exhibited higher levels of

activity, distress to limitations, and fear in their infancy. However, the study did not

identify whether temperament characteristics in infancy and in middle childhood

together clustered to form profiles indicative of different temperament types (Aksan et

al., 1999).

Moreover, studies also exist where infant temperament has been used as a predictor

of temperament measured in toddlerhood, by using a measure that closely resembles

Rothbart’s conceptual definition (Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Lemery et al.,

1999). Clearly, more longitudinal research is needed to explore the developmental paths

of early temperament across different developmental phases within Rothbart’s

theoretical framework, utilizing the conceptual and operational definitions and measures

developed within this framework.

1.2.2 Mothers and fathers as informants of the child’s temperament

In temperament research, the question of objectivity in measuring child behavior

according to parental reports is open to debate (Kagan, 1994, 1998; Seifer, Sameroff,

Dickstein, Schiller, & Hayden, 2004; Stifter et al., 2008). However, parent-rated

temperament of the child has frequently shown higher age-to-age stability than

observer-rated temperament (Pauli-Pott, Mertesacker, Bade, Haverkock, & Beckmann,

2003; Rothbart et al., 2000b). Besides this greater predictive validity, recent evidence

also points to greater situational consistency of parent-ratings in comparison to more

objective perceptions (Majdandzic & van den Boom, 2007). From the developmental

point of view, it is important to further note that, within a family, especially the

Page 17: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

17

perceptions or views that parents have of their child’s characteristics are considered

essential components of development (Belsky, 1984).

Despite the encouragement to use parent-reports, the majority of the research on

temperamental continuity to date in Rothbart’s, as in other frameworks, still relies on

maternal ratings of the child’s temperament (e.g., Aksan et al., 1999; Putnam et al.,

2008; Rothbart et al., 2000b; for another framework, see also Pedlow, Sanson, Prior, &

Oberklaid, 1993), or on combinations of mother-rated and observed temperament

(Crockenberg & Leerkes, 2006; for other frameworks, see Belsky, Fish, & Isabella,

1991; Fox, Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & Schmidt, 2001; Stifter et al., 2008). The role

of fathers in the family dynamic is, however, already widely acknowledged, and

research on the quality of the early father–child dyad and its relation to the behavioral

characteristics and developmental outcomes of the child at all ages from infancy to early

adulthood is becoming more widespread (e.g., Amato, 1994; Amato & Gilbreth, 1999;

Belsky, Woodworth, Crnic, 1996; Parke, 2004; Ramchandani, Stein, & Evans, 2005;

Shannon, Tamis-LeMonda, & Cabrera, 2006).

Furthermore, there is mounting evidence of the predictive association between infant

temperament and the dynamics of father–child interaction (Belsky, 1996; Kochanska,

Friesenborg, Lange, & Martel, 2004; McBride, Shoppe, & Rane, 2002; Sirignano &

Lachman, 1985). Fathers and mothers are also increasingly considered comparable

informants about their children’s behavior (DeFruyt &Vollrath, 2003). Mother- and

father-rated temperament have, according to previous cross-sectional data, been shown

to be interrelated, and show a similar structural pattern in infancy, toddlerhood and

childhood (Auerbach et al., 2008; Goldsmith & Campos, 1990; Kochanska et al., 1998;

Leve, Scaramella, & Fagot, 2001; Majdandzic & van den Boom, 2008; Putnam et al.,

2006; Rothbart, 1981; Rothbart et al., 2001; Saudino, Wertz, & Gagne, 2004).

Nevertheless, evidence of the extent to which father-rated temperament shows

continuity during development is scanty, and to our knowledge, only a handful of

studies to date have focused on this issue. In other temperament frameworks, Van

Egeren (2004) found consistency of father-rated characteristics of negative affectivity

during the first six months, and Rubin, Nelson, Hastings, and Asendorpf (1999)

reported on moderate continuity of father-rated shyness from the age of two to four

Page 18: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

18

years. Bishop, Spence, and McDonald (2003) reported significant continuity of father-

rated behavioral inhibition from the age of three to five years.

In the framework of Rothbart, the recent study of Putnam and his colleagues (2006)

demonstrated that stability estimates for fathers ratings of temperament ranged from .26

to .73 from 18 to 36 months of age, and Rothbart and her colleagues (2001) present

stability estimates for father-ratings of temperament that ranged from .48 to .76 from

five to seven years of age, for subscales encompassing characteristics of positive and

negative affectivity and effortful control. In Majdandzic’s and van den Boom’s study

(2008), mother- and father-rated temperament showed similar stabilities over seven

months during the child’s fourth year of age. To my knowledge, no previous study has

focused on the developmental continuity of father-rated temperament over

developmental phases from infancy onwards, or on comparing the mother- and father-

rated developmental paths of early temperament.

1.3 Personality traits in adulthood 1.3.1 Stability and change in personality traits For several decades, personality research has also increasingly concentrated on

questions of development. Because the descriptions about personality remain the same

across time, the dimensions of adult personality are defined to be homotypically

continuous (homotypic referring to literal continuity or stability over time) (Caspi &

Roberts, 1999). Recent theoretical discussions have, however, focused on the relative

contributions of biological as compared to environmental factors on the homotypic

continuity, or temporal stability of personality (e.g., Ardelt, 2000; Johnson, McGue, &

Krueger, 2005; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006b); in the theoretical framework

developed by McCrae and his colleagues (2000), the association between personality

traits and environment is expressed in the person’s characteristic adaptations, while no

external influence on the basic temperament-based tendencies is acknowledged. A more

differentiated perspective has also emerged, suggesting that some environment-related

change in personality traits is possible even during adulthood (e.g., Caspi & Roberts,

1999; Helson et al., 2002; Roberts & Mroczek, 2008; Scollon & Diener, 2006).

Of the different concepts depicting personality change, normative change is

interpreted as the result of intrinsic trends based on rates of species-typical brain

Page 19: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

19

maturation and age-related changes in brain function and gene expression (e.g., McCrae

et al., 2000), or a consequence of universal patterns of life experiences and social roles

(Elder, 1994). Non-normative or rank-order change, in turn, is viewed as the result of

individual differences in genetics, injury, drug abuse, or disease processes that influence

brain chemistry on an individual level (e.g., Piedmont, 2001), or as the result of

individual life experiences such as career trajectories (Roberts, 1997) or relationship

patterns (Robins, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2002). In other words, while traits may change in

the same direction for most people (normative change in the average level on any

dimension), they may also change, in magnitude or in direction, the extent of change

owing more to individual and specific environmental determinants (non-normative

change) (Costa & McCrae, 1988; Roberts, Walton, Bogg, & Caspi 2006; see also

Tennen, Affleck, & Armeli, 2005). Similar to the variable-level homo- and heterotypic

continuity of early temperament, this differential, rank-order stability is often indexed

by cross-time correlation coefficients (stability coefficients) which, for the major

personality trait domains, have generally been shown to be substantial in magnitude

(Fraley & Roberts, 2005; Costa & McCrae, 1994). This rank-order stability also tends to

decline in magnitude as the elapsed time interval increases, and to increase

systematically with age (Caspi et al., 2005; Clark & Watson, 1999; see also Fraley &

Roberts, 2005).

With regard to demographic predictors, most researchers also agree that rank-order

stability (or test–retest stability/consistency) is considerably lower among adolescents

and young adults than among older age groups (Ardelt, 2000; Roberts & DelVecchio,

2000). However, there is some disagreement regarding the age(s) or lifestages when the

age-related increase in the magnitude of the test–retest stability coefficients peaks. Until

recently, the dominant perspective has been that once adulthood is reached, which

happens around 30 years of age, there is no subsequent change in personality (Costa &

McCrae, 1994, 1997; Terracciano, Costa, & McCrae, 2006). Other studies have reported

that stability continues to increase from early adulthood to age 50 (Roberts &

DelVecchio, 2000), while some have reported curvilinear trajectories with increases

until age 50, but declines in old age (Ardelt, 2000). The patterns of rank-order stability

for men and women have generally been found to be similar (Roberts & DelVecchio,

2000).

Page 20: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

20

1.3.2 Personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism during adulthood

The current study focuses on the two well-known personality traits, extraversion and

neuroticism (Costa & McCrae, 1992), which are included in virtually every prominent

trait model developed during the 20th century (e.g., Clark & Watson, 1999). These two

traits also have similar meanings in most models of personality, referring to positive and

negative emotionality and their regulation. Heritability estimates of extraversion and

neuroticism based on twin studies typically fall in the .40 to .60 range, with a median

value of approximately .50 (e.g., Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; Jang, McCrae, Angleitner,

Riemann, & Livesley, 1998).

As the understanding of these traits has increased, it has become clear that they

represent basic dimensions of temperament (Clark & Watson, 1999): two key features

of temperament are that they, first, are at least partly attributable to innate biological

factors and second, have emotional processes as core, defining features (Digman, 1994).

Accordingly, extraversion and neuroticism have strong and systematic links to

emotional experience, and are associated with the basic PA and NA emotions (e.g., Abe

& Izard, 1999; Carver, Sutton, & Scheier, 2000; Gross, Sutton, Ketelaar, 1998, Larsen

& Ketelaar, 1991). Also in the framework proposed by Gray (1982, 1987), links

between neuroticism and behavioral inhibition (BIS), and between extraversion and

behavioral activation (BAS), have been reported (Jorm et al., 1999; Smits & Boeck,

2006). Extraversion and neuroticism are also considered most predictive of momentary

affect (Yik & Rusell, 2001) and the affective quality of one’s perception of reality

(Uziel, 2006) and self-esteem (Watson, Suls, & Haig, 2002).

In the adult’s personality, the level of extraversion describes the quantity and

intensity of interpersonal interaction, activity level, need for stimulation, and capacity

for joy: a person scoring high on extraversion is considered sociable, active, talkative,

person-oriented, optimistic, fun-loving, and affectionate, whereas a low-scoring person

is reserved, sober, unexuberant, aloof, task-oriented, retiring, and quiet (McCrae &

John, 1992). Neuroticism, in turn, assesses adjustment versus emotional instability and

identifies individuals prone to psychological distress, unrealistic ideas, excessive

cravings or urges, and maladaptive coping responses: a person scoring high on this trait

worries a lot and is nervous, emotional, insecure, and feels inadequate, whereas a person

Page 21: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

21

scoring low is calm, relaxed, unemotional, hardy, secure and self-satisfied (McCrae &

John, 1992).

The reports of rank-order stabilities of extraversion and neuroticism during

adulthood for both genders have generally been relatively high, ranging from .61 to .80

between periods from two years to several decades (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1988;

Scollon & Diener, 2006; Vaidya, Gray, & Haig, 2002; Van Aken, Denissen, Branje,

Dubas, & Goossens, 2006). However, existing evidence shows that rank-order change

in these two temperament-based traits during adulthood also occurs, which obviously

has raised the compelling question of why these changes occur.

1.4 Transactional development of parental personality and child temperament There are many potential processes that can either support or discourage developmental

continuity in child and adult temperament- and personality-related characteristics. These

processes have been described as involving transactions between a person and the

environment. During development, these transactions may lead some individuals to

experience changes not shared with others in their cohort, the extent of change owing

more to individual and specific, rather than shared environmental determinants.

Caspi (1998) describes three transactional processes that will influence the degree of

continuity of behavior over time: reactive, evocative, and proactive transactions. Within

a family, these person-environment transactions may lead to discontinuities as well as

continuities in temperament and personality. Reactive transactions emphasize individual

characteristics that impact a person’s own experience of identical environmental

conditions. Applied to models of early temperament, temperamental traits will affect the

ways in which environmental information is processed and thereby interpreted. These

temperamentally based biases in responding to the caregiving environment may be a

powerful force in maintaining and promoting early individual differences and their

stability and/or change over time. Therefore, in order to understand the origins of

various temperamental outcomes, it is essential to pay attention to the period of infancy

and the appearance of temperamental characteristics during that time. In adulthood,

temperament-based personality characteristics that impact a person’s own experience of

Page 22: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

22

environmental conditions may also be suggested to have an effect on the way the adult

reacts to, for example, his/her child’s temperament.

Evocative transactions, in turn, refer to the manner in which unique individual

differences evoke particular responses from the environment. Those responses further

influence the manner in which the individual will respond to the environment,

establishing a self-perpetuating cycle of interaction between the individual and the

environment. This cycle may maintain continuity, but may also be suggested to promote

change in temperament and personality. For example, infants of different temperaments

may evoke different caregiving behaviors from the same parent. Or, an infant’s

temperament may evoke distinctive responses from different parents. These differences

in child and parental behaviors could serve to either maintain or diminish the expression

of temperamental and personality differences over time. The final process, proactive

transactions, refers to the manner in which individuals move beyond the environments

into which they are born and actively select and construct environments of their own

(Caspi, 1998). Within a family, the newborn infant’s environment is largely defined by

the parents, but as the child grows, both the child and the parents may be actively

constructing their mutual environment. However, within a family context, one can also

suggest that parents may try to construct an environment that is in harmony with the

child’s characteristics, even if it is not what the parents’ own personality would lead

them to choose.

Stability and change of personality characteristics and relationship qualities can thus

be considered both prerequisites and consequences of dynamic transactions between a

person and his/her relationship experiences (Lehnart & Neyer, 2006). These dynamic

transactions between individual characteristics and relationship quality are more likely

to occur in stable social environments like the family, where the individual

characteristics of parent and child represent the specific environmental determinants that

may promote stability or rank-order change in both partners of the interaction (Belsky,

1984; Caspi & Roberts, 2001; Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001). In the family, a parent and a

child can be seen as active agents who, acting according to their individual

characteristics, co-create their emerging and enduring relationship (Collins, Maccoby,

Steinberg, Hetherington & Bornstein, 2000; Kochanska et al., 2004; Lengua & Kovacs,

2005; Rubin et al., 1999; Wachs, 2006; van den Boom & Hoeksma, 1994).

Page 23: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

23

In the existing literature, the direct links between various behavioral, physiological,

and genetic characteristics in parent and child have been reasonably well established

(e.g., Crnic & Low, 2002; Propper & Moore, 2006). In terms of longitudinal causality,

the parent’s effects on child development seem better known than the effects that

children might have on their parents’ psychological characteristics. Also the

bidirectional associations between parent and child characteristics over time still remain

less studied. Transactional models in which individual characteristics of parent and

child have been shown to be mutually influential have, so far, mainly been reported in

relation to clinically relevant outcomes like difficult temperament, psychosocial

problems and compliance of the child, or adequacy of parental behavior (e.g., Ge et al.,

1996, Lengua 2006; Patterson & Fisher, 2002).

For instance, Rubin et al. (1999) reported that child shyness at the age of two

predicted a decrease in maternal and paternal encouragement of independence over two

years, whereas parenting behaviors towards 2-year-olds did not predict the 4-year-old

child’s shyness (Rubin et al., 1999). In line with this kind of directionality, Belsky et al.

(2000) reported that during the child’s third year, a child’s inhibited behavior was likely

to increase parental reactivity in terms of both discouraging and encouraging behaviors,

whereas parental reactions to the child’s inhibition were poor predictors of inhibited

behavior. However, the existing literature of associations between parental

psychological characteristics (e.g., Belsky et al., 1991; Kochanska et al., 2004) and

subsequent child behavior indicate towards parental effects as well, and raise the

question of possible bidirectional causality for these inhibition-related, as well as other

aspects of child temperament from infancy onwards.

Further, according to Belsky’s (1984) ecological model, the quality of parenting is

especially influenced by three main classes of factors: parent, child and situational

characteristics, like contextual stress and supports. Within a family system, parental

views of their own and the child’s characteristics, feelings and behavior can thereby be

understood as psychological resources or contextual components that will determine the

degree to which a parent and child will affect each other from the child’s early years

onwards (Belsky, 1984). These perceived characteristics may also be differentially

susceptible to each other’s influence. That is, parents’ own characteristics may lead to

changes in their views of their children’s temperament-related behavior, or it may be

Page 24: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

24

that parents who view their children as initially higher in some but not other

temperamental characteristics may be prone to such parenting experiences that will

eventually lead to changes in their views of their own characteristics.

In this study, the environment where the rank-order stability or change in parental

and child characteristics is examined is conceptualized in terms of child temperament

and parental personality traits and, as a more situational characteristic, the amount of

overall parental stress. Even though the existing studies regarding associations between

the child’s temperament and parental personality-related characteristics are still

relatively ambiguous in regard to causal influence, an attempt of categorizing the

studies into those where child effects on the parent are indicated and those indicating

parent effects on the child may be of help in forming a general picture of the current

knowledge.

1.4.1 Implications of child temperament for parental extraversion and neuroticism So far, research on the rank-order stability of extraversion and neuroticism has already

provided some evidence of possible environmental promoters of change: recent

evidence shows that higher life- and work-related satisfaction associates with an

increase in extraversion, while lower satisfaction, stress and unemployment relate to an

increase in neuroticism (Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003; Roberts & Chapman, 2000;

Scollon & Diener, 2006; Van Aken et al., 2006). Neyer and Asendorpf (2001), in turn,

have proposed that it is interpersonal relations, such as marital and family experiences,

that have the highest potential to affect personality change in adulthood: they found that

young adults who had recently begun dating showed greater declines in neuroticism and

shyness (a reverse facet of extraversion) and larger increases in extraversion compared

to participants whose relationship status had not changed. Similarly, getting married and

living in a more satisfying relationship has been shown to be associated with an increase

in emotional stability and a decrease in neuroticism, while marital tensions, lower

marital satisfaction and divorce associate with an increase in neuroticism (Costa,

Herbst, McCrae, & Siegler, 2000; Roberts & Chapman, 2000; Robins et al., 2002). Still,

questions regarding potential promoters of change in personality during adulthood

Page 25: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

25

(Roberts et al., 2006b; Scollon & Diener, 2006; Terracciano, McCrae, Brant, & Costa,

2005) have so far remained largely unresolved.

Of the important stages and roles of adult life, parenting a young child can be

considered a very significant and intimate interpersonal experience that has the potential

to cause enduring effects on an individual. While the parental self-concept gradually

changes during parenthood (Cowan, Cowan, Heming, & Miller, 1991), this identity

process, involving the commitment to a new social role, is also likely to involve at least

some change in parental personality over time (Roberts, Wood, & Smith, 2005).

Therefore, through their unique impact on parental behavior and on the quality of the

parent–child relationship (Calkins, Hungerford, & Dedmon, 2004; Collins et al., 2000;

Rubin et al., 1999), temperamentally different children may, in the course of time, also

have a unique impact on parents’ own personality traits.

Evidence already exists that positively affective babies evoke more positive reactions

from their caregivers than negatively affective babies (e.g., Kochanska et al., 2004;

Scarr, 1992; Van den Boom, 1989). In terms of changes in maternal psychological well

being, Gartstein and Sheeber (2004) reported that within a group of mothers having

parenting difficulties with their 3 to 6-year-old children, child externalizing behavior

was predictive of a decline in maternal self-perceived parenting competence: this, in

turn, contributed to an increase in maternal depressive symptoms over one year. Further,

associations between the child’s early temperament and changes in parents’ sense of

global efficacy, personal control, anxiety, and dispositional optimism over time have

been shown to exist (Heinonen, Räikkönen, Scheier, Pesonen, Järvenpää, & Strandberg,

2006; Sirignano & Lachman, 1985). Parenting experiences have also been shown to be

associated with changes in maternal personality-related characteristics like ego

resiliency, feelings of dependency and fearfulness (Paris & Helson, 2002). Still, the

influence that the child may actually have on the parents’ personality traits, and

especially on the two ‘best-known’ traits extraversion and neuroticism, is less known.

1.4.2 Implications of parental extraversion and neuroticism for child temperament

As Rothbart and Putnam (2002) suggested, a child’s temperament as it interacts with the

environment is likely a better predictor of developmental outcomes than temperament

Page 26: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

26

alone. Environmental pressures within the social context, especially at an early age,

provide the structure around which temperamental dispositions will be modified. In

evaluating the associations between parental personality and child temperament, it is

thus essential to pay attention to the period of infancy and the early appearance of the

child’s temperamental characteristics that will strongly affect the child’s concurrent and

subsequent behavior.

The continuity estimates of childhood temperament reported so far (Putnam et al.,

2008; Rothbart et al., 2000b) also leave room for contextual factors. In other words,

instead of conceptualizing temperament as a set of highly stable traits inherent in the

child, its continuity, especially in terms of self-regulative functions, can be seen as

relating to the child’s social environment (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Accordingly, the

development of infant temperament has consistently been shown to be affected by,

besides genetic influence, the unique environment of the child (e.g. Goldsmith, Lemery,

Buss & Campos, 1999; Propper & Moore, 2006; Stright, Gallagher, & Kelley, 2008).

Among the most significant contextual elements in a child’s environment are

parental characteristics. Given the role temperament plays in later behavior and

adjustment problems, testing the effects of parental personality on their child’s

temperament (e.g., Rothbart et al., 2000; Caspi, 2000), along with their influence in

proximal interactions (Crockenberg & Smith, 2002; van den Boom & Hoeksma, 1994)

and in various other processes in the child’s development (e.g., Kochanska, 1997) is of

particular importance.

In terms of parental extraversion and neuroticism, previous studies have revealed that

parents with high extraversion and low neuroticism display more positive, supportive,

and responsive parenting and less negative, controlling parenting (Belsky & Barends,

2002; Belsky, Crnic, & Woodworth, 1995; Clark, Kochanska, & Ready, 2000; Losoya,

Goldsmith, Callor, & Rowe, 1997; Metsäpelto & Pulkkinen, 2003). In contrast, parents’

high neuroticism is often found to be associated with less sensitive, less affective and

less stimulating parenting (Belsky et al., 1995). Associations between higher parental

extraversion and more positive evaluations and more positive outcomes of the child, as

well as between higher parental negative affectivity and neuroticism and more negative

evaluations and outcomes of the child have also been hitherto quite consistently

established (e.g., Belsky et al., 1991; Gartstein & Marmion, 2008; Kochanska et al.,

Page 27: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

27

2004; Kurdek, 2003; Propper & Moore, 2006). In general, no significant differences in

the effects of parent-related determinants on maternal and paternal behavior have been

detected (Metsäpelto & Pulkkinen, 2003; Verhoeven, Junger, Van Aken, Dekovic, &

Van Aken, 2007).

1.4.3 Child temperament and parental situational factors: the case of parental stress Besides parents’ own personality- or temperament-related psychological characteristics,

situational factors within the parents’ life may also have a significant effect on parental

feelings and behavior within a family, and especially within the parent’s relationship

with the child. Despite the origin of those situational experiences, i.e., whether they

originate from within or outside of the family, parents’ individual interpretations of and

feelings related to those experiences may further influence the parents’ and the child’s

behavior at home.

Because parents in modern societies are under various pressures, one of the most

important and influential situational factors in an adult’s life may be the amount of

perceived stress. Stress in the family context, especially when that stress is chronic and

present early in the child’s development, can further have detrimental effects on the

wellbeing of the family and the quality of parent–child relationships. Accordingly, the

existing literature shows associations between parental parenting-, daily-, life-event, and

other environment-related stress and parent- and/or teacher-reported child behavioral

problems, adjustment difficulties, and internalizing or externalizing problems in the

early childhood period (Anthony et al., 2005; Conger, McCarty, Yang, Lahey, & Kropp,

1984; Coplan, Bowker, & Cooper, 2003; Creasey & Reese, 1996; Deater-Deckard &

Scarr, 1996; Kliewer & Kung, 1998; Peterson & Hawley, 1998).

However, the majority of research in this area has mainly focused on a specific

concept, ‘parenting stress’, that is defined in terms of dysfunctional parent–child

interaction, parental anxiety or depression, and ‘difficult’ or challenging child-behavior

characteristics (Abidin, 1990; Crnic & Greenberg, 1990). This kind of parenting stress

does not necessarily reflect the more global appraisal of stress that may result from

various life domains other than parenting (e.g., work and marital discord). The existing

evidence, however, implies that it is not only stress that is roused by parenting

Page 28: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

28

experiences that may have an influence on family functioning, parental behavior and

dyadic interaction between a parent and a child at home (Crnic, Gaze, & Hoffman,

2005; Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996).

The stability of parental stress is an assumption that seems to underlie much of the

research within the field, although, due to the relative lack of longitudinal research, it

has not been established to any reliable extent. As in studies of adult personality, studies

of parental stress show greater stability for shorter time periods between assessments

(Crnic et al., 2005). In the study of Mulsow and her colleagues (2002), maternal

parenting stress was shown to be stable between the child’s ages of 15 and 36 months

(Mulsow, Caldera, Pursley, Reifman, & Huston, 2002), and in the recent Rantanen et al.

(2008) study, parenting stress of mothers and fathers with children living at home

showed moderate to high stability, but was more stable across a one-year period than

across a six-year period (Rantanen, Kinnunen, Feldt, & Pulkkinen, 2008). Further, in

Crnic et al. 2005, two indices of maternal stress, namely experiences of major life

events and parenting daily hassles, appeared to be stable over two years across the

preschool period: stressed mothers at child age three were likely to report higher stress

at child age five. However, in the study of Östberg, Hagekull and Hagelin (2007), the

individual stability of maternal parenting stress over a six-year period in a clinical

intervention group with infant sleeping and feeding problems was only moderate,

decreasing slightly over time from infancy to the child’s age of six to seven.

Similar to the development and over-time stability reported for the more

constitutionally based characteristics of child temperament and parental personality

traits, the stabilities found for the level of parental stress make it clear that there is

ample room for change. The parents’ experience of overall stress can increase or ease

off over time, and many parents do change; the intriguing question is whether these

changes can be predicted from some internal family factors. The idea of being able to

predict or define some typical correlates of change in parents’ experience of stress

makes the efforts to identify such factors within a family an exciting area for inquiry.

1.4.3.1 Implications of child temperament for parental stress

In evaluating the associations between parental stress and child temperament, periods in

infancy and early childhood are again considered to be of greatest importance.

Page 29: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

29

Concurrent associations between so called ‘difficult temperament’ and parenting stress

have provided evidence in support of the notion that infant difficultness can undermine

parental functioning (Belsky, 1984; Belsky, Rha, & Park, 2000; Calkins et al., 2004;

Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000; Gartstein & Sheeber,

2004; Heinonen et al., 2006; Kochanska et al., 2004; Rubin et al., 1999; van den Boom

& Hoeksma, 1994), although evidence concerning these effects is not altogether

coherent (Putnam, Sanson & Rothbart, 2002; Rothbart, 1986). In terms of concurrent

associations between early temperament and parental stress, Gelfand, Teti and Fox

(1992) reported a significant association between the mothers’ parenting stress and

difficult temperament among three- to 13-month-old infants. Mulsow and her

colleagues (2002) also reported a significant association between maternal parenting

stress and infant difficultness at one month, but not at six or 15 months, and Honjo and

his colleagues (1999) found maternal child-rearing stress to be related to infant-

temperamental difficulty at 18 months, but not at six months. Associations between

parental stress and child temperament have also been reported among 21-month-old

toddlers (Östberg & Hagekull, 2000) and among 48-month-old children (Coplan et al.,

2003).

However, the possible over-time effects of the child’s initial temperamental

characteristics on the parents’ stress experience remain largely unexplored. With regard

to specific caregiving-related problems during infancy, namely problems with feeding

and sleeping, it has been shown that parenting stress at the child’s age of six to seven

can, at least partly, be predicted by these early child and family problems and associated

early stress (Östberg et al., 2007). It can thus be assumed that, similar to the changes in

parental self-concept and personality characteristics induced by the parenting role

(Cowan et al., 1991; Roberts et al, 2005; Paris & Helson, 2002), the parental role may

also be accompanied by changes in the level of parental experience of stress over time.

The existing evidence further shows, that mothers and fathers are generally more

similar than different in their specific levels of stress experience (Creasey & Reese,

1996; Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996) and associated experiences (Deater-Deckard,

Scarr, McCartney, & Eisenberg, 1994). For example, Crnic and Booth (1991) reported

no differences in the absolute number or intensity of the everyday parenting hassles that

mothers and fathers experienced. The stability of parental stress and the associations

Page 30: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

30

between stress and child outcomes have also generally been shown to be independent of

the parent’s or child’s gender (Mulsow et al., 2002; Rantanen et al., 2008; Räikkönen et

al., 2006).

1.4.3.2 Implications of parental stress for child temperament

Both parental overall stress and parenting-related stress have been proven to be

significant predictors of parent and child behavior and dyadic interaction (e.g.,

Bergman, Sarkar, Glover, & O'Connor, 2008; Conger et al., 2002; Crnic et al., 2005;

Repetti & Wood, 1997), but so far only a few studies have investigated the associations

between a more global experience of parental stress and child temperament. Especially

in terms of early temperament, the causality of associations between parental stress and

infant/child temperament over time is, to a large degree, still an unexplored area.

Support for the idea that it is parenting stress that plays a causal role in the

emergence of problematic child temperament is provided by the evidence of predictive

associations between maternal prenatal stress and subsequent negative views of the

child’s temperament and mental health morbidity. Huizink and her colleagues (2002)

reported associations between increased maternal prenatal and/or postnatal stress and

infant ‘unadaptability’ (Huizink, Robles de Medina, Mulder, Visser, & Buitelaar, 2002).

In the same cohort, increased levels of maternal prenatal stress further appeared to be

associated with temperamental and behavioral problems at the child’s age of two years

(Gutteling et al., 2005). Likewise, in the study of Pesonen and her colleagues (2005),

mothers who reported continuous high stress from the pre- to the postnatal period over

six months perceived the temperament of their six-month-old infants as more negatively

and less positively tuned than did mothers who experienced increasing, decreasing or

continuous low stress over the same periods. Maternal stress across child ages two to

three years has also been shown to play a causal role in the emergence of problematic

child behavior (Belsky et al., 1996). Also in the recent studies of Robinson, Oddy et al.

(2008) and Bergman, Sarkar et al. (2008), high or increasing stress during pregnancy

was predictive of child mental health morbidity or fearfulness in toddlerhood and at the

age of five.

In terms of a more global experience of stress, evidence of the predictive link

between parental overall stress and subsequent child temperament can be drawn from

Page 31: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

31

the above-mentioned studies of prenatal stress (Bergman et al., 2008; Huizink et al.,

2002; Pesonen et al., 2005; Robinson et al., 2008). Further, Räikkönen and her

colleagues (2006) reported associations between parents’ higher perceived global stress

and more negatively tuned perceptions of the child’s temperament during infancy. In the

study of Crnic, Gaze and Hoffman (2005), the intercorrelation between two indices of

parental stress, namely more general life stress and more specific parenting daily hassles

at the child’s age of three, was low, indicating that those two indexes of stressfulness

were predominantly independent, but they were both still predictive of parent and child

behavior and dyadic interaction at the child’s age of five. The authors thus concluded

that cumulative stress, resulting from parenting hassles or other life events, may build

across developmental periods to create an increased risk for parenting and child

functioning (Crnic et al., 2005).

However, studies that focus on the predictive associations between parental overall

stress and child temperament from infancy to childhood still seem to be lacking.

Further, even though temperament is a neutral construct that covers a wide variance of

behavior, there still seems to be a lack of longitudinal studies moving from global

‘child-difficultness’, or problematic behavior –measures to linking parental overall

stress with their perceptions of a wider range of infant/child temperamental

characteristics over time.

1.5 Aims of the study The overall aim of the current study was to examine the degree of continuity of

temperament over five years from the infant’s age of six months to the child’s age of

five and a half years, and to investigate the bidirectional effects between child

temperament and parents’ personality traits and experienced overall stress during that

time. Figure 1 shows the general outline of the current study.

Page 32: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

32

Figure 1. General outline of the study. Roman numerals refer to individual substudies (please see the original list of publications on page 10) that have allowed examination of the specific research questions

1.5.1 Continuity of temperament from infancy to middle childhood

1.5.1.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated temperament

First, I addressed the question of temperamental continuity among 231 Finnish children

within the theoretical framework and operational definitions developed by Rothbart and

her co-workers (e.g., Rothbart & Bates, 2006; Rothbart et al., 2001). I assessed

differential homotypic and heterotypic continuity between individual subscales and

latent superconstructs of temperament as rated by the mother when the child was aged

six months and when the child was aged five and a half years. Based primarily on

Rothbart’s theory emphasizing developmental continuity and structural change of

temperament, and partly on the prior findings (Rothbart & Bates, 2006), I hypothesized

that all individual temperament subscales in infancy would show differential homotypic

and/or heterotypic continuity up until middle childhood.

Page 33: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

33

As for the latent temperament superconstructs, I hypothesized that significant

continuities would be found between infant positive affectivity and middle childhood

extraversion, and between infant and middle childhood negative affectivity. I also

hypothesized that infant positive affectivity would predict effortful control in middle

childhood, as the positive affectivity construct contains early characteristics of self-

regulation, i.e. duration of orienting (Kochanska et al., 1998). Additionally, based on

previous literature on the associations between early activity and later extraversion

(Eaton, 1994; Hagekull & Bohlin, 1998), specific effects of infant activity were also

tested by including specific paths from infant activity to the middle childhood

superconstructs in the model of latent superconstructs.

Moreover, by taking an ipsative, person-level approach to temperamental continuity,

I aimed to explore whether temperament from infancy to middle childhood clustered to

form profiles indicative of the personality types characterized in research on personality.

As there are both empirical and conceptual links between temperament and personality

traits, I hypothesized that temperament in infancy and in childhood would cluster to

form profiles indicative of three different temperament types that would correspond

conceptually to the personality types consistently identified in childhood and adulthood,

i.e., the resilient, undercontrolled and overcontrolled personality types (Asendorpf et al.,

2001; Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Caspi & Silva, 1995; Robins et al., 1996). Infant

activity level was included as a separate dimension in the cluster formation.

I hypothesized that, first, the temperament type corresponding to the resilient type

would be marked by a relatively high level of positive emotionality in infancy and low

levels of negative affectivity both in infancy and in childhood, and relatively high

effortful control in childhood. Second, the temperament type corresponding to the

undercontrolled type would be characterized by relatively high levels of negative

affectivity both in infancy and in childhood, and a high level of extraversion and

relatively weak self-regulative capacities in childhood. Finally, the overcontrolled

temperament type would be marked by relatively high negative affectivity in infancy

and by low extraversion and relatively high self-regulative capacities in childhood The

level of infant activity was hypothesized to be positively associated with childhood

extraversion and, accordingly, be relatively high in the second, more undercontrolled

type, and low in the third, more overcontrolled type.

Page 34: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

34

1.5.1.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated temperament

Second, I aimed to fill the gap in our knowledge of father-rated temperamental

continuity from infancy to middle childhood within a sample of 115 father-infant-child

dyads. Within this second study, I also examined whether the developmental paths in

father-rated temperament were similar to those of mother-rated temperament within a

sample of 109 father–mother–infant/child -triads. The smaller sample size in father-

ratings, however, restricted me to the variable level analyses, and did not allow a

reliable enough ipsative level analysis.

Because mother- and father-rated temperament have, according to previous cross-

sectional data, been shown to be interrelated, and show a similar structural pattern in

infancy, toddlerhood and childhood, I hypothesized that father-rated temperament

would also show significant homo- and heterotypic continuity from infancy to five and

a half years of age. I also hypothesized that paternal and maternal ratings of

temperament would show congruent continuity.

1.5.2 Transactional development of parent and child characteristics from infancy to middle childhood

After demonstrating the continuity of parent-rated temperament in itself from infancy to

middle childhood, the next step of the present study was to capture the transactions

between parental characteristics and child temperament over the same period of time.

The bidirectional effects between child temperament and parental characteristics were

investigated in terms of parents’ personality traits and overall stress experienced during

that time.

1.5.2.1 Study III. Parental personality traits extraversion and neuroticism and

infant/child temperament

In the third study, I aimed to examine rank-order stability and transactional associations

between parents’ personality traits extraversion and neuroticism (McCrae & Costa,

1987; McCrae & John, 1992) and the parents’ views of their infant’s/child’s

temperament within a sample of 109 mother-father-infant/child triads. The stability and

change in parental extraversion and neuroticism were examined in relation to parent-

rated infant activity and sumscores of positive and negative affectivity and ratings of the

Page 35: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

35

child’s temperamental sumscores extraversion, effortful control, and negative

affectivity.

In line with existing literature, parental extraversion and neuroticism were expected

to show high stability over five years. In terms of parental personality change, I

hypothesized that parental ratings of the infant’s higher positive affectivity would

predict an increase in parental extraversion and a decrease in neuroticism, and that

ratings of the infant’s higher negative affectivity would predict an increase in parental

neuroticism and a decrease in extraversion. Infant activity was examined with no

specific direction of change in parental personality hypothesized. Further, initially

higher parental extraversion was expected to predict higher parent-rated extraversion

and effortful control and lower negative affectivity in the child, and initially higher

parental neuroticism was expected to predict ratings of lower extraversion and effortful

control, and higher negative affectivity in the child.

1.5.2.2 Study IV. Mother’s overall stress and infant/child temperament

Next, I aimed to depict the over-time stability and transactional associations between

mothers’ perceived overall stress and infant/child temperament over the same time

period within a sample of 231 mothers and their infants/children. In this fourth study,

the first aim was to test the transactional associations between maternal stress and six

individual temperament subscales that represented conceptual equivalents in the infants’

as in the children’s temperament questionnaires. The subscales activity level,

anger/frustration, fear, soothability, attentional focusing, and smiling and laughter were

chosen for the analyses. Second, I aimed to test whether there were transactions

between maternal stress and the latent infant/child temperament superconstructs.

I hypothesized that temperament characteristics reflecting higher negative

emotionality and lower positive affectivity in infants would contribute to an increase in

maternal stress over five years beginning in infancy, and that higher maternal stress

would contribute to an increase in the child’s negative affectivity and a decrease in

positive affectivity characteristics and self-regulative functioning over the same period.

In sum, the current study first aimed at establishing the continuity of temperament

within a family context, as perceived by both parents, and examining whether the

developmental paths of infant/child temperament are similar in both parents’ eyes.

Page 36: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

36

Second, the study aimed to examine the transactional associations between parental

personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism and infant/child temperament, and

further, explore the transactional development of maternal perceived stress and maternal

ratings of infant/child temperament over this five-year period.

Page 37: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

37

2 METHODS 2.1 Outline of the study The ongoing Glaku project was initiated in 1998. It was designed to examine the

neonatal and early childhood predictors of hypertension development, as well as

normative psychological development in childhood. The Glaku project is a collaborative

study involving the Departments of Psychology and Medicine at Helsinki University,

and the Department of Pediatrics and Neonatology at Helsinki City Maternity hospital.

At the baseline of the study in 1998 the infants averaged six months of age (M= 6.3,

S.D. = 1.4 months). A follow-up was conducted five years later in 2003–2004, at which

point the children averaged five and a half years of age (M= 66.1, S.D. = 2.9 months).

These two time-points served as the sample for the current study. A second follow-up

was conducted in 2006, and a further follow-up will be conducted in 2009.

The initial study sample was collected between March 1st and November 30th of 1998

in Helsinki City Maternity hospital (Kätilöopiston sairaala). The hospital is one of the

principal maternity hospitals in Helsinki, with approximately 4500 births per year. The

midwives in four regular maternity wards were asked to give a questionnaire to all

women with singleton healthy births in which both parents were of Finnish origin.

During this period there were 2746 births that would have been eligible. For reasons

connected with the vacation period of the midwives, some mothers did not receive the

questionnaire (only 30 of the invited mothers refused to participate). However, it is not

likely that this involved selection bias, because the midwives were unaware of the exact

aims of the study (Strandberg, Järvenpää, Vanhanen, & McKeigue, 2001).

2.2 Participants In 1998, a total of 1049 mothers completed the questionnaire, which included questions

on antenatal stress and nutrition, and on other lifestyle variables, as well as a question

eliciting permission to examine their maternity records. Of these 1049 mothers, the first

500, along with the biological fathers of the baby, were invited to participate in a

psychological survey on child development.

Of these 500 families, a total of 328 (65.6%) family units (mother and/or father)

returned the questionnaire sent to them by mail approximately six months after the

delivery. Of these family units, data on both biological parents was simultaneously

Page 38: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

38

available on 180 units, 141 questionnaires were returned only by the mother and seven

only by the father. In total, 321 mothers and 187 fathers returned the questionnaire.

Details of the research protocol and of the recruitment of the initial sample are described

in Strandberg et al. (2001). The participants in Studies I – IV were taken from the same

sample consisting of families that participated in the Glaku project at baseline in 1998

when the infant was six months of age and again at follow-up in 2003-2004, at the

child’s age of five and a half years. The institutional Review Board at the University of

Helsinki approved this project, and the participating parents gave their informed

consent.

A total of 231 mother-infant/child dyads providing data at both data-collection points

formed the sample for Study I. A total of 115 father-infant/child dyads, providing data

at both data-collection points formed the sample for the first aim of Study II, and a total

of 109 father–mother–infant/child triads, providing data at both data-collection points

formed the sample for the second aim of Study II. A sample of 109 mother-father-

infant/child triads providing complete parental and child data at both time points formed

the sample for Study III, and a total of 231 mother–infant/child dyads providing

complete parental and child data at both time points formed the Study IV sample.

2.3 Measures 2.3.1 Infant temperament

Parental ratings of infant temperament were derived using the Infant Behavior

Questionnaire (the IBQ, Rothbart, 1981). The questionnaire is designed to refer to

specific, concrete behaviors of the infant, occurring during the past week (previous two

weeks for some items).The 96 items in the IBQ are evaluated on a seven-point scale

reflecting the relative frequency of specified infant reactions. There are six subscales

(Rothbart, 1986). Activity level describes the level of the infant’s gross motor activity,

including movement of the arms and legs, squirming, and locomotor activity. Smiling

and laughter is related to expressions of joy. Soothability (the recovery parameter of

distress) is defined as the effectiveness of soothing techniques when the child is

experiencing negative affects. Duration of orienting refers to sustained involvement

with a single object or activity when there has been no sudden change in stimulation.

Distress to limitations refers to distress when goal achievement has been blocked or a

Page 39: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

39

desirable object removed. Fear is expressed by crying or fussing and/or extended

latency to approach intense or novel stimuli.

Infant positive affectivity superconstruct has in previous studies been comprised of

slightly differing combinations of smiling and laughter, duration of orienting,

soothability and activity level (see, e.g., Goldsmith & Campos, 1990; Kochanska et al.,

1998; Rothbart, 1986; Worobey & Blajda, 1989), while distress to limitations and fear

have quite consistently been used in defining a negative affectivity superconstruct (see,

e.g., Kochanska et al., 1998; Rothbart, 1986). Infant activity has been shown to

associate, besides positive affectivity, with negative affectivity as well (Goldsmith &

Campos, 1990; Goldsmith et al., 1999; Kochanska et al., 1998; Lemery et al., 1999;

Rothbart, 1986; Worobey & Blajda, 1989).

The temperament superconstructs in the first, second and fourth study were devised

by defining the latent factors in the structural equation model, and in the third study by

computing the sumscores. The infant positive affectivity factor was defined by smiling

and laughter, duration of orienting and soothability, and infant negative affectivity was

defined by distress to limitations and fear, while infant activity was allowed to load on

both positive and negative affectivity latent factor in the structural equation model. In

the cluster analysis in Study I and in the multilevel analysis in Study III, infant positive

and negative affectivity superconstructs were computed as described in previous studies

(Kochanska et al., 1998; Rothbart et al., 2001), with the exception that infant activity

was not included in the superconstructs, but was used as a separate dimension in the

analyses.

Previous studies have reported good test-retest and alpha reliabilities for the IBQ

(Gartstein, Slobodskaya, & Kinsht, 2006; Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003; Rothbart, 1986).

Convergence between laboratory observation ratings of temperament and the IBQ scales

has also been reported (Bridges, Palmer, Morales, Hurtado & Tsai, 1993; Goldsmith &

Rothbart, 1991). In the current study the Cronbach’s alphas for the subscales ranged

from 0.74 to 0.93. Moreover, the confirmatory factor analyses conducted in Studies I

and II confirmed the construct validity in the Finnish sample.

Page 40: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

40

2.3.2 Child temperament

Parental ratings of the child’s temperament were elicited by using the Children’s

Behavior Questionnaire (the CBQ, Rothbart et al., 2001). The 195 items in the CBQ are

evaluated on a seven-point scale reflecting the relative frequency of specified child

reactions in concrete situations in previous weeks. There are 15 subscales. Activity level

describes the level of the child’s gross motor activity. Anger/Frustration refers to

negative affectivity related to the interruption of an ongoing task, or goal blocking.

Approach anticipation refers to the amount of excitement and anticipation expressed

toward expected pleasurable activities. Attentional focusing refers to the capacity to

maintain attentional focus on task-related channels. Discomfort means negative

affectivity related to sensory qualities of stimulation, including intensity, rate, and

complexities of light, movement, sound and texture. Falling reactivity and soothability

(the recovery parameter of distress) is the rate of recovery from peak distress,

excitement, or general arousal. Fear is expressed by negative affectivity including

unease, worry or nervousness, which is related to anticipated pain or distress and/or

potentially threatening situations. High intensity pleasure refers to enjoyment related to

situations involving high stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, and incongruity.

Impulsivity is the speed of response initiation. Inhibitory control is the capacity to plan

and to suppress inappropriate approach responses under instructions or in novel or

uncertain situations. Low intensity pleasure means enjoyment related to situations

involving low stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, and incongruity. Perceptual

sensitivity is the detection of slight, low-intensity stimuli from the external

environment. Sadness refers to negative affectivity and lowered mood and energy

related to exposure to suffering, disappointment, and object loss. Shyness refers to slow

or inhibited speed of approach and discomfort in social situations. Finally, Smiling and

laughter is related to positive affect in response to daily happenings.

According to Rothbart et al. (2001), high levels of impulsivity, high intensity

pleasure, activity level, smiling and laughter, and positive anticipation, and a low level

of shyness form an extraversion superconstruct; high levels of low intensity pleasure,

inhibitory control, perceptual sensitivity and attentional focusing form an effortful

control superconstruct; and high levels of fear, sadness, discomfort, and

anger/frustration and a low level of soothability form a negative affectivity

Page 41: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

41

superconstruct. These superconstructs were devised in the current study by defining the

latent factors in the structural equation models and computing the sumscores. Previous

studies have reported good test-retest and alpha reliabilities for the CBQ (Rothbart et

al., 2001). In the current study, the Cronbach’s alphas for the subscales ranged from

0.65 to 0.90. Moreover, the confirmatory factor analyses conducted in Studies I and II

confirmed the construct validity in the Finnish sample.

2.3.3 Parental personality traits Parental personality traits were assessed on the extraversion and neuroticism scales

derived from the authorized adaptation of the NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI;

Costa & McCrae, 1985; Pulver, Allik, Pulkkinen, & Hämäläinen, 1995). The self-report

questionnaire contains 48 items measuring extraversion and 48 items measuring

neuroticism, and these 96 items were included in the assessment of parental personality

at both time points. Extraversion is defined in terms of warmth, gregariousness,

assertiveness, activity, excitement-seeking and positive emotions, and neuroticism in

terms of anxiety, anger, depression, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, and

vulnerability. The items are scored on a five-point Likert scale (strongly disagree to

strongly agree), and support for the validity and reliability of the domains has been

consistent (Costa & McCrae, 1992; McCrae & Costa, 1987). In the current study, the

Cronbach’s alphas for extraversion and neuroticism ranged from .91 to .94 at both data-

collection points.

2.3.4 Perceived stress

The mothers self-reported their level of perceived overall stress approximately six

months postpartum and again five years later using the shortened version of the original

14-item perceived stress scale (the PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). PSS

is sensitive to chronic stress deriving from ongoing life circumstances, expectations

concerning future events, or from any life-events. The items are designed to tap the

degree to which respondents find their lives unpredictable, uncontrollable, and

overwhelming. These three issues have been repeatedly found to be central components

of the experience of stress (Cohen et al., 1983).

Page 42: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

42

The short version, constructed as suggested by Cohen and his colleagues (1983),

consisted of the four items that were originally most highly correlated with the 14-item

scale: the scale thus includes statements such as ‘How often have you found that you

could not cope with all the things that you had to do’ and ‘How often have you felt that

you were on top of things (a reversed item)’. In the current study, an additional fifth

question, derived from the original scale, asking directly about the level of experienced

stress was added: ‘In the last month, how often have you felt nervous and stressed’?

Mothers evaluated the degree to which they had appraised situations in their lives as

stressful (unpredictable, uncontrollable, overwhelming) in recent weeks on these five

items, using a scale ranging from never (1) to all the time (5). The scale has shown good

reliability and validity in prior studies (Cohen et al., 1983). Cronbach’s alpha in the

current study was .79.

2.4 Statistical analyses Structural equation modeling was the main statistical method used in all four studies.

Several analyses were used for comparative purposes, such as the t-tests for independent

and dependent variables. In Study I, the k-means clustering algorithm was used to test

how the study variables (sumscores) in infancy and in middle childhood clustered to

profiles. Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used in verifying that the

clusters were significantly different on the criterion variables. Structural equation

modeling and path analyses were performed using the Mplus statistical package 2.13-

4.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 - 2006). Standard model-fitting procedures and the

maximum-likelihood-estimation method were adopted. Table 1 summarizes the

information on participants, research design, measures, and main statistical analyses in

the current study.

Page 43: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

43

Table 1. Sample, research design, measures and main statistical methods used in Study I – IV Study Participants Measures Main statistical methods

I N=231 mother-

infant/child -dyads

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months: Infant Temperament Questionnaire (IBQ) Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years: Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ)

Confirmatory factor analysis, Structural equation modeling, t-test for independent samples, k-means clustering algorithm, ANOVA, post hoc t-tests (Pearson correlations)

II N=115 father-

infant/child-dyads; N=109 father–

mother–infant/child-

triads

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months: Infant Temperament Questionnaire (IBQ) Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years: Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ)

Confirmatory factor analysis, Structural equation modeling, t-tests for independent and dependent samples, (Pearson correlations)

III N=109 mother-father-

infant/child- triads

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months: Infant Temperament Questionnaire (IBQ) NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI) Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years: Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ) NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI)

Confirmatory factor analysis, Structural equation modeling multilevel technique, t-tests for independent and dependent samples, (Pearson correlations)

IV N=231 mother-

infant/child- dyads

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months: Infant Temperament Questionnaire (IBQ) Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years: Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ) Perceived Stress Scale (PSS)

Confirmatory factor analysis, Structural equation modeling, (Pearson correlations)

Page 44: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

44

3 RESULTS The main results of the four separate studies are summarized below. The details are to

be found in the original publications.

3.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated temperament from

infancy to middle childhood 3.1.1 Continuity of temperament subscales and superconstructs Pearson correlations were computed to test differential homotypic and heterotypic

continuity between the individual subscales of temperament measured in infancy (IBQ)

and in middle childhood (CBQ) over five years (Table 2). Activity level, smiling and

laughter, distress to limitations and fear showed significant homo- and heterotypic

continuity from infancy to middle childhood, whereas there was evidence only of

heterotypic continuity for soothability and duration of orienting. Heterotypic continuity

was shown, for example, between higher infant activity and later aspects of extraversion

(e.g. approach anticipation and high-intensity pleasure) and between higher infant

smiling and laughter and later low-intensity pleasure, and between higher levels of

infant distress to limitations and fear and later sadness. Associations indicating

developmental interaction between traits that are controlled by separate genetic

mechanisms were also shown, for example, between higher infant soothability and later

high-intensity pleasure, and between higher infant fear and lower levels of low-intensity

pleasure and smiling and laughter in middle childhood.

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to verify the factor structures of infant

positive and negative affectivity (Kochanska et al., 1998) and middle childhood

extraversion, effortful control and negative affectivity latent superconstructs (Rothbart

et al., 2001), and the confirmed factor structure was used as a foundation for the

structural analyses. Figure 2 shows the a priori hypothesized model including all

possible paths from the positive and negative affectivity latent superconstructs in

infancy to the extraversion, effortful control and negative affectivity latent

superconstructs in middle childhood.

The positive affectivity latent superconstruct in infancy predicted extraversion and

effortful control in middle childhood, while negative affectivity in infancy predicted

Page 45: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

45

negative affectivity in middle childhood. The model showed an adequate fit ( 2/df = 1.9;

RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.914), and temperament in infancy accounted for 4.6, 22.3 and

6.0% of the variance in extraversion, effortful control, and negative affectivity in middle

childhood, respectively.

An additional SEM involving the specific effects of infant activity, as an addition to

paths from infancy to middle childhood latent superconstructs, is presented in Figure 3.

The model with specific effects of infant activity showed an acceptable fit ( 2/df = 1.9;

RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.922), and the 2 difference test showed that the model with the

specific effects of infant activity fitted the data significantly better than the initial a

priori hypothesized model that did not involve the specific effects. The model involving

specific effects revealed significant paths from infant activity to later extraversion and

negative affectivity, while the path to effortful control was not significant. In this

model, infant temperament accounted for 8.1, 23.0, and 11.7% of the variance in

extraversion, effortful control, and negative affectivity in middle childhood,

respectively. Adding the specific paths of infant activity in the model eliminated the

significant path from infant positive affectivity to extraversion.

Page 46: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

46

Table 2. Pearson correlations between the temperament subscales measured in infancy and in middle childhood (N = 231 mother-infant/child-dyads)

p < .05. * p < .01. **

Figure 2. Structural equation model showing associations between latent temperament superconstructs measured in infancy and in middle childhood. Standardized values are given for the path coefficients. All the nonsignificant paths are also presented ( 2/df = 1.9; RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.914)

Infancy: IBQ Activity

Level Smiling / Laughter

Soothability Duration of Orienting

Distress to Limitations

Fear

Middle childhood: CBQ

Activity Level .26** .09 .05 .01 .02 .03 Anger/Frustration .02 -.05 .03 .04 .16* .12 Approach/ Anticipation

.17** .29** .20** .19** -.01 -.00

Attentional Focusing

-.10 .10 .12 .05 -.01 -.12

Discomfort -.03 -.12 -.02 -.00 .16* .10 Falling Reactivity & Soothability

-.01

.20**

.06

.07

-.34**

-.11

Fear -.05 -.04 -.04 .01 .16* .23** High-Intensity Pleasure

.18** .09 .15* .01 .02 -.08

Impulsivity .25** .08 .14* -.01 .02 .05 Inhibitory Control -.13* .02 .06 .08 -.07 -.05 Low-Intensity Pleasure

.05 .20** .11 .08 -.02 -.14*

Perceptual Sensitivity

.01 .20** .12 .26** -.09 -.03

Sadness -.05 .01 .05 .11 .14* .18** Shyness -.17* -.06 -.11 .02 .09 -.02 Smiling/Laughter .20** .34** .15* .13* -.11 -.19**

Page 47: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

47

Figure 3. Structural equation model showing associations between latent temperament superconstructs measured in infancy and in middle childhood, with specific paths from infant activity to the middle childhood superconstructs included in the model. Standardized values are given for the path coefficients. All the nonsignificant paths are also presented ( 2/df = 1.9; RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.922)

3.1.2 Person-level continuity of temperament: “temperament types”

A k- means clustering algorithm was used to test whether the temperament

superconstructs (sumscores) in infancy and in middle childhood cluster to form

theoretically meaningful and coherent profiles representing different temperament

types. The reliability of the cluster solution was tested as recommended by

Breckenridge (2000) and Mandara (2003), by examining whether a statistically

significant agreement existed in a cross-validation between two randomly divided

cluster solutions. As separate SEM and cluster analyses for girls and boys did not show

substantial differences, all the analyses were conducted with gender groups combined.

Three “temperament types”, illustrated in Figure 4, profiling infants and children

throughout the five-year-period were found: children belonging to the first cluster,

named as “resilient”, were characterized by above-average levels of activity, positive

affectivity, extraversion and effortful control, and below-average levels of negative

affectivity in infancy and middle childhood (n = 73). Those in the second cluster, named

“undercontrolled”, were characterized by an above average level of activity and a

Page 48: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

48

below-average level of positive affectivity in infancy, by above-average levels of

negative affectivity in infancy and middle childhood, by an above-average level of

extraversion, and a well below average level of effortful control in middle childhood

(n = 79). Those in the third cluster, named “overcontrolled”, were characterized by a

well below average level of activity and a below-average level of positive affectivity,

and an average level of negative affectivity in infancy. In middle childhood, they

exhibited above-average levels of effortful control and negative affectivity, and well

below the average level of extraversion (n = 79).

Figure 4. Cluster analysis identifying three temperament “types” based on sumscores of temperament superconstructs measured in infancy and in middle childhood. Note. All ps < 0.001 in ANOVAs testing the significance of differences between the clusters; all ps < 0.001 in post hoc pair-wise comparisons, except for p = 0.34 between “resilient” and “undercontrolled” in activity; p = 0.45 between “undercontrolled” and “overcontrolled” in positive affectivity; p = 0.04 between “undercontrolled” and “overcontrolled” in infant negative affectivity; ps = 1.0 between “resilient” and “undercontrolled” in extraversion and between “resilient” and “overcontrolled” in effortful control; p = 0.08 between “undercontrolled” and “overcontrolled” in childhood negative affectivity

Page 49: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

49

3.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated temperament and

congruence between mother- and father-rated continuity 3.2.1 Continuity of father-rated temperament subscales and superconstructs Pearson correlations were computed to test differential homotypic and heterotypic

continuity between the individual subscales of temperament measured in infancy (IBQ)

and in middle childhood (CBQ) over five years. Father-rated activity level, smiling and

laughter and distress to limitations showed significant homo- and heterotypic continuity

from infancy to middle childhood, whereas there was evidence only of heterotypic

continuity for soothability, duration of orienting and fear (Table 3). Heterotypic

continuity was shown, for example, between higher infant smiling and laughter and

later low-intensity pleasure and between higher infant distress to limitations and later

discomfort and fear. Associations indicating developmental interaction between

different traits were also shown: for example, higher infant activity level associated with

lower levels of inhibitory control and soothability in middle childhood, and higher

infant fear and distress to limitations associated with lower levels of control-related

abilities and less smiling and laughter in middle childhood.

The CFA presented in the first study was used in verifying the factor structures of

latent infant and child temperament superconstructs, and the confirmed factor structure

was used as a foundation for the structural analyses. In the SEM, father-rated positive

affectivity in infancy was shown to predict, as hypothesized, effortful control in middle

childhood, but it did not predict later extraversion. The SEM with fit indexes is

illustrated in Figure 5. As hypothesized, negative affectivity in infancy predicted

negative affectivity in middle childhood, but also showed a developmental trend

towards a low level of effortful control. The respective variances accounted for by

father-rated positive and negative affectivity in infancy were 5.1, 22.7, and 10.0% for

extraversion, effortful control and negative affectivity, respectively.

As in the first study, when infant activity was added to the model as a specific path,

the significance of a high level of infant activity as a specific determinant of later

extraversion was revealed (Figure 6). The predictive associations between negative

affectivity in infancy and in middle childhood and between positive affectivity and later

effortful control were not affected by the specific activity path. For fathers, adding the

Page 50: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

50

specific effect of infant activity improved the fit of the model only slightly ( ²(1) = 3.8,

p > .05).The variances accounted for by activity and positive and negative affectivity in

infancy in this additional model were 8.0, 23.7, and 9.8% for extraversion, effortful

control and negative affectivity, respectively.

Table 3. Pearson correlations between the father-rated temperament subscales measured in infancy and in middle childhood (N = 115 father-infant/child dyads)

Infancy: IBQ Activity

Level Smiling/ Laughter

Soothability Duration of Orienting

Distress to Limitations

Fear

Middle childhood: CBQ

Activity Level .19* .06 .07 -.10 .03 .01 Anger/Frustration .20* -.12 .00 -.04 .27** .14 Approach Anticipation

.14

-.09

-.03

.09

.16

-.05

Attentional Focusing

-.13

.19*

.10

.09

-.11

-.11

Discomfort .01 -.04 -.14 .08 .08 -.04 Falling reactivity & Soothability

-.20*

.20*

-.01

.02

-.23*

-.15

Fear -.08 -.05 .05 .07 .18* .10 High-Intensity Pleasure

.13

-.04

.15

-.01

.06

.02

Impulsivity .11 -.02 .17 -.16 .14 -.04 Inhibitory Control

-.27**

.17

.02

.07

-.31**

-.17

Low-Intensity Pleasure

-.01

.30**

.19*

.27**

-.14

-.19*

Perceptual Sensitivity

-.03

.16

.01

.21*

-.13

-.27**

Sadness -.05 -.18* -.16 -.02 .06 .02 Shyness .01 -.11 -.14 -.04 .05 .18 Smiling/ Laughter .04 .23* .16 .07 -.12 -.27**

p < .05. * p < .01. **

Page 51: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

51

Figure 5. Structural equation model showing associations between father-rated latent temperament superconstructs measured in infancy and in middle childhood (n = 115). Standardized values are given for the path coefficients. All the nonsignificant paths are also presented ( 2/df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05; CFI=.927)

Figure 6. Structural equation model showing associations between father-rated latent temperament superconstructs measured in infancy and in middle childhood, with the specific path from infant activity to the middle childhood extraversion superconstruct included in the model. Standardized values are given for the path coefficients. All the nonsignificant paths from infant negative and positive affectivity latent superconstructs are presented 2/df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05; CFI = .931)

Page 52: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

52

3.2.2 Congruence between mother- and father-rated continuity of

temperament Congruence between parents’ ratings was investigated within a sample of 109 mother-

father-infant/child –triads. Fathers rated their infant higher than mothers in fear,

duration of orienting and negative affectivity, and in middle childhood, they rated their

child higher in activity and impulsivity, and lower in approach anticipation, low-

intensity pleasure, perceptual sensitivity, smiling and laughter and effortful control.

Significant agreement between parents’ ratings of individual temperament subscales,

except for soothability in infancy was shown (Table 4). Paternal and maternal ratings

did not differ by child’s gender.

At the level of temperament subscales, continuity of father-rated temperament was

compared with the continuity shown for mothers in Study I. In terms of homotypic

continuity, the predictive associations in the father-ratings were similar to mother-rated

continuity in the first study. In terms of heterotypic continuity, similar developmental

tendencies were also shown, for example, between early activity and later aspects of

extraversion, between early smiling and laughter, soothability and duration of orienting

and later aspects of effortful-control, and between early distress to limitations and later

fear.

Indicators of developmental interaction between different traits were also similar, for

example, between early negative affectivity -subscales and lower levels of effortful

control -related subscales and smiling and laughter. Some difference in the salience of

associations between specific infant and middle childhood subscales could also be

detected; for example, father-rated infant distress to limitations and fear showed more

salient inverse associations with later effortful control –related subscales.

At the level of latent temperament superconstructs, no statistically significant

differences between the father- and mother-rated predictive paths between infancy and

middle childhood were found. Figure 7 displays the predictive paths between mother-

and father-rated latent temperament superconstructs from infancy to middle childhood,

including the specific effect of infant activity. The developmental links between

affectively positive and well adjusted characteristics in infancy (i.e., smiling and

laughter, orienting capacities and soothability) and later effortful control were especially

noteworthy in both father- and mother-ratings, being in line with the literature on the

Page 53: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

53

associations between positive affectivity and well functioning, adaptive control abilities.

However, even though no statistically significant difference existed between mother-

and father-rated paths, the developmental link between higher infant negative affectivity

and a lower level of effortful control in middle childhood was more pronounced in

father-ratings.

Table 4. Mean values of the infant’s (IBQ) and 5.5 -year-old child’s (CBQ) temperament variables separately for the mothers (n = 109) and the fathers (n = 109), comparisons of the variables within couples, and intraclass correlations between parents’ ratings of their child

Mothers Fathers t (paired) r (intraclass) M SD M SD Infancy: IBQ Activity level 4.54 0.79 4.59 0.69 -0.55 .45** Smiling/Laughter 5.25 0.82 5.35 0.78 -1.09 .53*** Soothability 4.95 1.03 4.80 1.15 1.09 .25 Duration of Orienting 3.77 1.10 4.10 1.17 -2.59* .46** Distress to Limitations 2.92 0.70 3.05 0.69 -1.84 .61*** Fear 2.16 0.65 2.33 0.68 -2.17* .47** Superconstruct sumscores Positive Affectivity 4.66 0.71 4.75 0.80 -1.04 .39** Negative Affectivity 2.54 0.56 2.69 0.57 -2.46* .58*** Middle childhood: CBQ Activity level 4.68 0.78 4.82 0.75 -2.13* .74*** Anger/Frustration 3.81 0.92 3.80 0.83 0.20 .66*** Approach/Anticipation 5.27 0.64 5.06 0.57 3.71*** .69*** Attentional Focusing 5.16 0.76 5.17 0.69 -.06 .64*** Discomfort 3.70 0.98 3.57 0.83 1.50 .65***

Falling Reactivity & Soothability

5.12 0.79 4.96 0.67 1.96 .46**

Fear 3.63 1.02 3.50 0.84 1.40 .64*** High-Intensity Pleasure 4.96 0.86 4.94 0.80 0.20 .72*** Impulsivity 4.21 0.87 4.40 0.75 -2.82** .77*** Inhibitory Control 5.18 0.87 5.03 0.80 1.48 .46** Low-Intensity Pleasure 5.81 0.61 5.51 0.56 4.46*** .41** Perceptual sensitivity 5.32 0.85 5.09 0.66 2.95** .67*** Sadness 3.49 0.84 3.41 0.69 0.94 .52*** Shyness 3.20 1.17 3.24 1.08 -0.53 .81*** Smiling/Laughter 5.86 0.59 5.65 0.66 2.90** .44** Superconstruct sumscores Extraversion 4.96 0.53 4.94 0.51 0.67 .85*** Effortful Control 5.37 0.56 5.20 0.49 2.98** .56*** Negative Affectivity 3.50 0.70 3.46 0.57 0.59 .62***

p < .05. * p < .01. ** p < .001***

Page 54: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

54

Figure 7. Structural equation model showing associations between latent temperament superconstructs measured in infancy and in middle childhood for mothers (n = 109) and fathers (n = 109), with the specific path from infant activity to the middle childhood extraversion superconstruct included in the model (path coefficients: mother/father). All the nonsignificant paths from infant negative and positive affectivity latent superconstructs are presented. Note. While the SEM allowing for father- and mother-rated across-time paths to form freely fitted the data well ( 2/df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05; CFI = .902), so did the model where the across-time paths were constrained ( 2/df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05; CFI = .901); the 2 difference test demonstrated no significant difference between these two models. In addition to intra-parental across-time paths, the models also specified cross-parental across-time paths. None of the cross-parental paths between the latent superconstructs were significant, and are, for the clarity of illustration, excluded from the figure

3.3 Study III. Developmental transaction between parental

personality traits and child temperament Preliminary analyses of the concurrent associations between study variables

demonstrated significant agreement between parents’ ratings of the child’s temperament

that also increased over time, while no association between maternal and paternal

personality traits extraversion and neuroticism within a family was shown. At baseline,

mothers’ and fathers’ higher extraversion correlated significantly with higher infant

positive affectivity, rs = .23 and .35, respectively, and parents’ higher neuroticism with

higher infant negative affectivity, .23 and .46, respectively. At follow-up, mothers’ and

fathers’ higher extraversion correlated significantly with higher child effortful control,

rs = .24 and .26, respectively, and for fathers also with higher child extraversion,

Page 55: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

55

r = .24 and for mothers with lower child negative affectivity, r = -.19. Mothers’ and

fathers’ higher neuroticism correlated significantly with higher child negative

affectivity, rs = 42 and .35, respectively, and for fathers, with lower child effortful

control, r = -.22 (for mothers, this association was nearly significant).

There were significant over-time Pearson correlations between parental personality

and infant/child temperament: mothers’ and fathers’ higher extraversion at baseline

associated significantly with the child’s higher effortful control five years later, rs = .24

and .20, respectively, and the parents’ higher neuroticism at baseline associated with the

child’s higher negative affectivity five years later, rs = .34 and .25, respectively. The

infants’ higher positive affectivity associated significantly with the mothers’ and

fathers’ higher extraversion 5 years later, rs = .29 and .42, respectively, and the infants’

higher negative affectivity associated with parents’ higher neuroticism, rs = .19 and .39,

respectively (for mothers, the association was nearly significant), and with lower

extraversion in fathers, r = -.25.

Multi-level analysis: Before proceeding to the multi-level analysis, a 2 -significance

test was performed showing that the cross-lagged paths between parental personality

and the infant’s/child’s temperament were similar in the maternal and paternal ratings

(p >.10). The multilevel model including statistically significant paths of the

stability/homotypic continuity of parental personality and the heterotypic continuity of

child’s temperament, and the statistically significant cross-lagged paths between the

child’s temperament and parental personality over five years are presented in Figure 8.

The model showed an excellent fit ( 2 /df =1.6; RMSEA =.05; CFI =.989).

While parental personality (ps <.001) and child temperament (ps <.01) showed

significant continuity over five years, the infant’s higher positive affectivity

significantly predicted higher parental extraversion five years later (p <.001), and higher

infant activity significantly predicted lower parental neuroticism (p <.05) after five

years. The infant’s negative affectivity did not significantly predict parental

extraversion or neuroticism after five years.

Further, initially higher parental extraversion significantly predicted the child’s

higher level of effortful control over time (p <.05), but the association between parental

and child extraversion did not reach statistical significance. Initially higher parental

Page 56: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

56

neuroticism significantly predicted the child’s higher negative affectivity (p <.001),

after controlling for the infant’s initial temperament.

In addition to these cross-lagged effects, the model further revealed parallel changes

in parental personality and the child’s temperament: decreases in parents’ views of the

levels of the child’s extraversion and effortful control (ps <.05) and an increase in views

of negative affectivity (p <.001) were significantly associated with an increase in

parental neuroticism. Changes in parental views of the child’s temperament were not

significantly associated with changes in parental extraversion at the follow-up. When

the analyses were re-run with the gender of the child as a predictor of parental

personality and the child’s temperament at the follow-up, no gender effects on the

results were found.

Figure 8. A multilevel structural equation model showing the associations between parental extraversion and neuroticism and infant/child temperament ( 2/df=1.6; RMSEA=05; CFI=.989). In the interest of clarity, only the statistically significant paths (p<.05) are presented. The values stand for unstandardized estimates and standard errors

Page 57: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

57

3.4 Study IV. Transaction between maternal stress and child

temperament 3.4.1 Transaction between maternal stress and individual temperament subscales Maternal stress measured in the infancy period correlated significantly with maternal

stress five years later, r = .48, and mother’s initially higher stress was also correlated

with the infant’s higher fear r = .18, distress to limitations, r = .31, and lower smiling

and laughter r = -.15. Maternal higher stress experience in middle childhood correlated

with the child’s higher fear, r = .21, and anger, r = .21, and with lower soothability,

r = -.18, and attentional focusing, r = -.14.

In the SEM, only one infant temperament subscale significantly predicted change in

maternal stress over five years: a higher level of infant activity predicted a decrease in

maternal stress from infancy to middle childhood when stress in the infancy period was

controlled for (p <.05). Maternal stress in the infancy period did not predict a change in

child activity from infancy to childhood when the activity level in infancy was

controlled for (Figure 9).

Maternal higher stress predicted, as hypothesized, an increase in child anger (p <.05)

and a decrease in attentional focusing (p <.05) and soothability (p <.001), when the

initial infant temperament dimensions were controlled for. Attentional focusing, anger,

and soothability did not predict any changes in the maternal stress experience when

stress in the infancy period was controlled for.

Figure 9. Change in maternal stress predicted by infant activity level. Standardized values are given for the path coefficients

Page 58: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

58

3.4.2 Transaction between maternal stress and latent temperament

superconstructs The factor structure for the latent superconstructs of positive affectivity and negative

affectivity in infancy, and for those of extraversion, effortful control, and negative

affectivity in middle childhood were confirmed in Study I and II. Figure 10 shows the

path coefficients for a transactional model of maternal stress and latent temperament

constructs in infancy and middle childhood. The model gave a good fit to the data 2/df = 1.6; RMSEA = .04; CFI = .92), and showed that maternal stress experience in

the infancy period predicted an increase in negative affectivity from infancy to middle

childhood when initial infant negative affectivity was controlled for, but it did not

predict extraversion or effortful control in middle childhood when infant temperamental

affectivity was controlled. Infant positive and negative affectivity did not predict any

changes in maternal stress experience over time.

Figure 10. A Transactional model of maternal stress and infant and child temperamental superconstructs. Straight lines are statistically significant * p < .05, *** p < .001. Note. Non-significant paths have been omitted from this final model with the exception of the path for the continuity from infant to child negative affectivity that controls for the continuity in estimating the change in negative affectivity ( 2/df = 1.6, RMSEA = .04, CFI = .92)

Page 59: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

59

4 DISCUSSION The current study aimed, first, to establish the continuity of temperament from the

infant’s age of six months to the child’s age of five and a half years as perceived by both

parents, and examine whether the developmental paths of infant/child temperament are

similar in both parents’ eyes. Second, the study aimed to explore the transactional

development of parental personality traits extraversion and neuroticism and ratings of

infant/child temperament. Further, the transactional associations between parental more

situational characteristics, namely maternal perceived overall stress, and ratings of

infant/child temperament over this five-year period were examined.

4.1 Continuity of parent-rated temperament from infancy to middle childhood The present study conceptualized temperament as constitutional individual differences

in reactivity and self- regulation, influenced over time by the interactions of heredity,

maturation, and experience (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Compared to, for example, the

early stylistic framework of Chess and Thomas (1986), and the more dimensional

approaches that identify basic temperamental traits, like Buss and Plomin’s (1984)

genetically based emotionality-activity-sociability model, Goldsmith’s primary-

emotion-based reactions (Goldsmith et al., 1987), or Kagan’s (1994) individual

differences in behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar, the present conceptualization of

temperament also included self-regulative processes that serve to modulate (enhance or

inhibit) reactivity (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). As the arousal and intensity of basic

emotions and reactions in the child’s life are influenced by the child’s regulative

capacities, defining and measuring these capacities supplements the picture of

temperament and the measurement of predictive associations between early and later

temperament and adjustment.

Besides depicting temperament as a within-person characteristic, temperament was

also presented as an interactive construct. First, the child’s expressed temperament is

assumed to affect and be affected by others; for example, the child’s level of reactivity

may not stay the same during development, but be permanently and profoundly changed

by regulative capacities that develop within interaction with the developmental context.

Second, developmental interaction between temperamental behaviors that are controlled

Page 60: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

60

by separate genetic mechanisms is also acknowledged; for example, the child’s

developing regulative capacities may affect the child’s tendency to express fear or

distress over time, or negative affectivity may also downplay the development of

effortful control.

In the present study, theoretically relevant associations between temperamental

dimensions at six months of age and at the age of five-and-a-half years were shown, and

the results of the homotypic and heterotypic continuity of individual temperament

subscales as perceived by both parents were, to a great extent, similar. Significant

homotypic continuity was shown for infant activity level, smiling and laughter, distress

to limitations and fear (nearly significant in father-ratings), while significant heterotypic

continuity was shown for infant soothability and duration of orienting in both parents’

ratings. In general, the continuity of temperament subscales from infancy to middle

childhood fulfilled the criteria of heterotypic continuity or coherence, reflecting, besides

over-time stability, also theory-based lawful change (Caspi & Roberts, 2001):

associations of infant temperament were found with phenotypically different but closely

correlated attributes, i.e., with attributes belonging to the same temperament

superconstruct in middle childhood, and subscales belonging to the broad categories of

infant positive and negative affectivity showed differential developmental paths. It

must, however, be acknowledged, that in Rothbart’s more recent work, a more fine-

grained measure of infant temperament has been developed (Gartstein & Slobodskaya,

2003); if this more recent version of IBQ with more subscales had been used, some of

the present heterotypic continuities might even have proved to be homotypic in nature.

As an example of heterotypic continuity, higher ratings of infant activity level

predicted, besides higher activity in middle childhood, also aspects of later extraversion,

e.g., approach anticipation and high-intensity pleasure, especially in mother-ratings.

However, higher infant activity also showed developmental associations with a lower

level of inhibitory control and, especially for fathers, with a higher tendency to express

anger and less ability to soothe oneself. In developmental research, a high activity level

has typically been associated with the development of extraversion, but also to

impulsivity and poor behavioral control in young children (Hagekull & Bohlin, 1998;

John, 1990; see also Caspi et al., 2005; Eaton, 1994). The present associations are thus

in line with previous research, linking the activity-aspect of surgency with anger and

Page 61: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

61

externalizing behavior (Putnam & Stifter, 2005; Rothbart et al., 2001; Rothbart et al.,

2000b). With the father, the child’s initially higher activity level may have, more often

than with the mother, resulted in manifestations of this less controlled and more

negative aspect of extraversion. For example, during a more active, outgoing and

challenging interaction with the father (Paquette, 2004), even an active and energetic

infant/child may be also susceptible to frustration and anger.

Higher infant smiling and laughter in maternal and paternal ratings also predicted

aspects of effortful control, like a stronger tendency to get pleasure from low-intensity

stimuli, and higher levels of self-soothing and attentional capacities. This sign of

positive affectivity in the infant may be a marker of self-regulatory capacity, i.e. reflect

the developmental change from a largely reactive dimension in infancy towards a more

effortfully controlled behavior in the service of cultural standards during childhood

(Ahadi et al., 2003; Putnam et al., 2001; Rothbart et al., 2001). Early positive affect may

also allow for the development of self-regulation by allowing more frequent encounters

with experiences that are cognitively and affectively challenging, leading to greater

opportunities to develop willful control, or, it may be that infants who display more

positive emotionality elicit guidance from parents that may facilitate the development of

attentional skills underpinning effortful control (Putnam et al., 2008).

For infant fear and distress to limitations, that reflect the broad construct of negative

affectivity, especially the associations of infant distress to limitations with a broader

range of characteristics of negative affectivity in middle childhood supported earlier

findings, showing different paths of development from infant fear and

frustration/distress-proneness (Derryberry & Rothbart, 2001; Goldsmith et al., 1999;

Rothbart et al., 2000b). Further, while higher infant distress/frustration predicted later

fear, depicting heterotypic continuity in both parents’ ratings, it also predicted lower

soothability, while initially higher fear, in turn, predicted also fewer signs of low

intensity pleasure and smiling and laughter in both parents’ ratings. These inverse

associations between early negative affect and later difficulties in the ability to recover

from distress or excitement and a lack of expressed positive affect are in concordance

with the suggestions of developmental interaction between traits, i.e., that

temperamental negative reactivity may compromise the effective development of

Page 62: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

62

attentional and inhibitory mechanisms, or that early negative reactivity may

compromise the development of expressions of positive affect (Putnam et al., 2008).

Further, the heterogeneity of subscale-level behaviors, reflecting common underlying

processes suggests the value of aggregating across narrowly delineated temperament

characteristics. Accordingly, at the level of latent temperament superconstructs, no

statistically significant differences between the mother- and father- rated predictive

paths between infancy and middle childhood were found. The developmental links

between affectively positive and well adjusted characteristics in infancy (i.e., smiling

and laughter, orienting capacities and soothability) and later effortful control were

especially noteworthy in both parents’ ratings, being, again, in line with the literature on

the associations between positive affectivity and well functioning, adaptive control

abilities. These early-emerging tendencies to focus attention, to enjoy calm activities,

and benefit from parental soothing may also contribute to later regulation of negative

emotions (Gaertner, Spinrad, & Eisenberg, 2008; Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003; Putnam et

al., 2008; Robins, John, Caspi, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1996; Rothbart et al.,

2001).

Continuity of negative affectivity from infancy to middle childhood was also

consistently shown in father- and mother-rated temperament. However, for the father-

rated high negative affectivity (i.e., distress to limitations and fear) in infancy, a

developmental trend towards low levels of effortful control-related characteristics (e.g.,

inhibitory control, low-intensity pleasure, perceptual sensitivity) in middle childhood

appeared, as also in subscale-level associations, somewhat more emphasized. Here, too,

early negative reactivity can be assumed to compromise the development of effortful

control (Putnam et al., 2008). It has, for example, been suggested that excess crying

during early infancy may hamper the development of self-regulatory skills (Stifter &

Spinrad, 2002).

In developmental literature, it has been shown that the child’s self-regulative

capacities develop and manifest in transaction with the caregiving environment (e.g.,

Cole, Teti, & Zahn-Waxler, 2003; Eisenberg, Zhou, & Spinrad, 2005; Feldman,

Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999), and some variance in parent ratings of the

manifestations of effortful control has been suggested to emerge due to caregivers’

unique experiences with the child (Putnam et al., 2006; Carson & Parke, 1996; Paquette,

Page 63: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

63

2004). Parental agreement of the child’s effortful control in the current study was also a

bit lower than for the other scales, a trend seen in other research as well: effortful-

control-related characteristic are suggested to be somewhat less discernible or more

variably expressed in the presence of different interaction partners (Majdandzic & van

den Boom, 2008).

While the child’s temperament is the same, where ever the child is, its overt

manifestations may, to some extent, depend on the circumstances. In that respect, it can

be suggested that mothers and fathers elicit somewhat different aspects of their child’s

temperamental behavior in their individual style of interaction with the child. Due to

these behavioral differences in the contexts of relationships, both parents can also be

“right” about their child’s temperament, while the two views may represent somewhat

different aspects of reality, especially in the characteristics of effortful control.

For example, observations comparing father–child and mother-child interaction have

found that fathers frequently engage in play behaviors aimed at making the child more

excited (see Paquette, 2004). Hence, with a playfully stimulating father, who may

actively encourage the child’s exploration of the environment, these amplifying

interactions may ultimately reach a limit; the over-stimulation of a more fearful or

distress-prone infant/child may lead to a situation where the child’s control abilities do

not quite meet the challenge. Some children may also find it more difficult to calm

down the emotional aftermath of the situation (Paquette, 2004; Parke, 1994; Rorthbart

et al., 2001). Further, the traditional role of fathers may, in some families, mean that the

father usually interacts with the infant/child after his day at work, i.e., in the evening

time, when the child’s mood and control abilities, as well as energy level may vary as a

function of fatigue.

Further, in the combined model of 109 fathers and 109 mothers, the predictive

association between high activity and later extraversion (c.f. Eaton, 1994) was similar,

but it did not reach statistical significance. However, in the models involving only

mothers (n = 231) or only fathers (n = 115), the paths from infant activity to later

extraversion were statistically significant. This predictive path from early activity to

later extraversion was also shown in the recent study of Putnam and his colleagues

(2008), where infant’s higher activity was associated with later characteristics belonging

under the construct of extraversion (activity, high-intensity pleasure, impulsivity).

Page 64: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

64

Enriching and complementing the current study, the person-centered approach

(Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Bergman & Magnusson, 2001) to continuity of mother-

rated temperament types further showed that temperament characteristics in infancy and

in middle childhood clustered to form profiles indicative of three theoretically-based

temperament types: relatively high levels of activity and positive characteristics in

infancy and a lack of negative affectivity in infancy and middle childhood clustered in a

type labeled as “resilient”. High activity in infancy and high negative affectivity in

infancy and middle childhood, combined with relatively weak child self-regulative

capacities, clustered in a type labeled “undercontrolled”. Furthermore, characteristics of

infant activity and positive affectivity that did not rise to the average, accompanied with

an average level of infant negative affectivity and with low childhood extraversion and

relatively high levels of effortful control and negative affectivity clustered in a type

labeled “overcontrolled”. These three profiles parallel the three widely established types

depicting personality in childhood and in adulthood, namely the resilient,

undercontrolled, and overcontrolled profiles, respectively (Asendorpf et al., 2001;

Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Caspi & Silva, 1995; Robins et al., 1996).

The current results thus provide further support for the continuity of childhood

personality types by offering a theoretically fruitful way of illustrating how the infant

and childhood temperament characteristics cluster to form stable temperament types that

resemble personality types shown in personality research. The distinction between the

internalizing (“overcontrolled”) and the externalizing (“undercontrolled”) negative

affectivity in the profiles is likely to be related to the level of the infant’s initial activity,

and to the activity of approach and self-regulation systems. The less intensive, relatively

passive and fearful children may, for instance, begin to regulate their fear through

inhibition (Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; van Brakel, & Muris, 2006) which, along with

the originally low activity level, serves to further restrict approach behaviors and overt

expression and sharing of affect. Inhibition may also begin to attenuate the child’s

positive affect, resulting in internalizing emotions (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997).

It has also been suggested that extraversion (high activity and approach tendency)

gives direction to manifestations of negative emotions in frustrating situations: while a

high level of extraversion may lead the child to increase his or her external force, a

lower level of extraversion may lead to more internalized reactions (Putnam et al., 2001,

Page 65: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

65

2008). The different types of negative affectivity are thus assumed to result from

combined initial reactivity and developmental interactions between reactivity and self-

regulation (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997, 2001).

4.2 Developmental transaction between parent and child

characteristics 4.2.1 Transactional development of parental personality traits and child temperament The continuity estimates of infant/child temperament reported in the first two studies

still leave room for contextual factors. Thereby, the third study aimed at complementing

our knowledge of reciprocal child-to-parent and parent-to-child effects in the

development of temperament and parental personality traits. More specifically, I

examined stability and change in parental self-rated personality traits of extraversion

and neuroticism in relation to the parents’ views of their child’s temperamental

affectivity sumscores and activity level from the infant’s age of six months to the

child’s age of five and a half years.

The concurrent and over-time correlations between parental personality and child

temperament were essentially similar in both parents’ ratings, but also showed some

differences in the salience of associations. Both parents’ higher extraversion at both

time points associated with the infant’s higher positive affectivity and the child’s higher

effortful control, and fathers’ higher extraversion at follow-up also associated with the

child’s higher extraversion, while mothers’ higher extraversion at follow-up associated

with the infant’s/child’s lower negative affectivity. This may be suggested to reflect the

fathers’ more playful and exploration-encouraging interaction with the child (Paquette,

2004), that may elicit similar behavior from the child; for mothers, positive interaction

with the child may attenuate the risk of overstimulation-related negative affect. Further,

the over-time associations between infant’s higher negative affectivity and parental

lower extraversion and higher neuroticism at follow-up were more salient for fathers:

the infant’s higher negative affect may have reduced the opportunities for paternal

positive interaction with the child, resulting in lower paternal extraversion and higher

neuroticism five years later. However, when the differences in the mother- and father-

rated cross-paths between parental personality and the infant’s/child’s temperament

Page 66: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

66

were tested, no statistically significant differences between maternal and paternal

ratings were shown.

In the final multilevel model, parents’ self-rated personality traits extraversion and

neuroticism exhibited the expected high stability over five years, but also changed over

time in relation to parents’ views of their child’s temperament. As hypothesized,

perceiving the infant as higher in positive affectivity predicted an increase in parental

extraversion over five years. Furthermore, parental views of the infant’s higher activity

level predicted a decrease in parental neuroticism over five years. Higher infant

negative affectivity was not, however, significantly associated with an increase in

parental neuroticism or a decrease in extraversion.

Johnson et al. (2005) proposed that stability and intensification of a personality trait

may result from a combination of genetic and continuing environmental influences.

This goes well with the current results showing an increase in parental extraversion in

response to perceived higher infant positive affectivity. Trait extraversion in itself

associates with attention to positive interpersonal information and feedback (Uziel,

2006). Living with a temperamentally positive child may have acted as an

environmental constancy factor for the extraverted parent (Fraley & Roberts, 2005;

Sameroff, 1995), thus contributing to the stability of and increase in parental

extraversion. The more extraverted parent may also have engaged in a parent–child

interaction that fits and strengthens his/her own extraversion-related characteristics

(Scarr & McCartney, 1983).

To understand the predictive association between the infant’s higher temperamental

activity and a decrease in parental neuroticism, the potential significance of the infant’s

activity for the parent–child relationship should be considered. First, children’s activity

level has been shown to be relatively stable from infancy onwards in the current data as

in other studies (Putnam et al., 2006), and it may therefore be considered an important

and enduring component at the core of the parent–child relationship. This characteristic

of the relationship, stemming from the infant’s constitutional characteristics, is also

beyond the control and selection of the parent (Johnson et al., 2005). Second, active

infants have also been shown to be in less need of adult stimulation, with respect to the

development of their cognitive abilities and mastery motivation (Wachs, 1987, 1992) or

exploratory behavior (Gandour, 1989). Thereby, with an initially active infant, the

Page 67: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

67

effects that parental neuroticism might have on the interaction with the child may not be

able to reduce the infant’s lively and initiative behavior towards the parent.

In personality research, the closest parallel to temperamental activity is perhaps in

the energy or activity dimension within the extraversion factor (McCrae & John, 1992).

Because high temperamental activity has thus been shown to underpin later extraversion

(Blatny, Jelinek, & Osecka, 2007; Eaton, 1994; Hagekull & Bohlin, 1998), the child’s

initially higher activity may also be accompanied by higher levels in other extraversion-

related characteristics like positive affect, approach anticipation, lack of shyness and

sociability (Blatny et al., 2007; Rothbart et al., 2001). In the current data, the infant’s

activity was, likewise, more strongly associated with the infant’s positive than negative

affectivity. These extraversion-related characteristics of temperament have also been

shown to be highly stable over the early years (Berdan, Keane, Calkins, 2008). Like a

more active child, a child high in positive reactivity may also become activated at lower

levels of parental stimulation (Rothbart, 1986).

On the other hand, as Neyer and Lehnart (2007) propose, higher neuroticism may in

itself include a high motivation for change. It is also important to acknowledge that

higher parental neuroticism and related characteristics do not necessarily or

automatically decrease the positive qualities of parenting, such as parental

responsiveness, tracking, or compliance to the children (Kochanska et al., 2004). It may

be suggested that an infant, seen by the parent as high in activity, may promote positive

qualities in a parent, or offer a chance for change by continuously challenging the parent

to be more active and responsive. This process between the parent and the child could,

over time, facilitate a change for the better in the parent’s neuroticism-related

characteristics and feelings.

One explanation for the lack of effect of infant negative affectivity in increasing

parental neuroticism could be that the negative affectivity factor comprises the separate

components of distress to novelty (fear) and distress to limitations. These

characteristics, which at the age of six months are not differentiated enough to show any

independent effects, may still differ somewhat in their overt expression, resulting in a

somewhat less consistent effect on feelings and behaviors related to parental

neuroticism (see e.g., Rubin et al., 1999). The existing literature does not report entirely

Page 68: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

68

consistent associations between infant’s negative temperamental characteristics and

parenting experience either (Boivin, Pérusse, & Dionne, 2005; Rohtbart, 1986).

The current results further suggest that extraversion and neuroticism in adulthood

may differ in terms of their susceptibility to environmental influence (see Fleeson,

2007). In other words, experiences may differentially influence an individual’s

development, because the impact of the environment is mediated by individual

differences in the organism; even though Belsky’s differential susceptibility hypothesis

originally refers to differences in the susceptibility of individuals high or low in specific

temperament- or personality characteristics (Belsky, 1997), it could also be suggested

that temperament and personality characteristics per se, for example positive or negative

affectivity, activity, neuroticism or extraversion, are differentially susceptible to

environmental influence. Parental extraversion and neuroticism may thus differ in terms

of the degree to which parenting experiences in general may affect, i.e. increase or

decrease, the level of that trait. Or, developmental changes in these parental traits may

also differ according to specific environmental characteristics, like the child’s unique

temperament.

In terms of social investment theory, the parenting role comes with its own set of

expectations and contingencies that calls for behaving in a certain fashion (Roberts et

al., 2006a). In this study, the roles that the parents were committed to may have differed

according to their perceptions of temperamental differences in their children (see e.g.,

Paris & Helson, 2002; Sameroff & Fiese, 2000; Sirignano & Lachman, 1985), and these

roles may have also differently contributed to the patterns of continuity and change in

extraversion and neuroticism. It can also be suggested that parents may change simply

through learning generalization: while in the middle of the everyday interaction with the

child, parents observe themselves acting in a certain way, and gradually come to a

different opinion about themselves that eventually generalizes across domains (Roberts

et al., 2006a).

In Costa and McCrae’s personality model (McCrae et al., 2000), an individual’s self-

concept is considered largely as a function of basic temperamental tendencies. The

dynamic interaction of basic tendencies with the environment produces characteristic

adaptations that comprise, for example, social relationships, skills, and habits.

Successful coping with parenting situations with temperamentally unique children can

Page 69: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

69

be assumed to require a permanent reorganization of one’s priorities and characteristic

adaptations to the environment: one could thus ask whether these characteristic

adaptations can, over time, moderate parents’ own feelings and behaviors, and thereby

also gradually modify their more ‘stable’ or basic self-concept, reflected in measures of

extraversion and neuroticism. Or, in other words, characteristics of extraversion and

neuroticism may shift over time along a continuous dimension extending from the

trait’s core to its surface, and the core trait may become more unstable due to significant

relationship experiences (Asendorpf & Van Aken, 2003).

In addition to showing evidence of change in parental personality, our findings

suggest that parents’ personality traits may change their views of their child’s

temperament over time. We found that parent’s own higher-rated extraversion promoted

higher ratings in the child’s effortful control, while higher parental neuroticism

promoted ratings of the child’s higher negative affectivity. These predictive associations

are well in line with the voluminous literature showing that the more positive parental

characteristics contribute positively to the child’s development, while the less favorable

parental characteristics may increase the risk for adverse development of the child (e.g.,

Gartstein & Bateman, 2008; Propper & Moore, 2006; Spinrad et al., 2007; Stright et al.,

2008). For example, parents’ extraversion-related positive affect during play may

stimulate children’s interest in a task or refocus waning attention, as well as create a

positive environment that increases enjoyment of such efforts (Gaertner et al., 2008).

In this transactional setting, parents’ ratings of positive and negative affectivity in

infants also showed theoretically relevant heterotypic continuity over five years (see

also Putnam et al., 2008). The predictive paths between temperament sumscores, i.e.,

the paths from higher positive affectivity in infancy to higher effortful control in middle

childhood, and the paths from higher negative affectivity to lower effortful control and

higher negative affectivity in middle childhood were as hypothesized, and similar to

those shown between latent temperament sumscores in Studies I and II. In addition, in

the multilevel analysis, the infant’s higher negative affectivity sumscore also predicted

lower extraversion in middle childhood.

In recent literature, the construct positive affectivity has been shown to be a

heterogenic composite, associated concurrently and longitudinally with both

extraversion- and effortful control-related characteristics (Kochanska, Aksan, Penney,

Page 70: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

70

& Doobay, 2007). The infant’s own basic predisposition to positive affect, along with

early signs of self-control, may explain the association between positive affect and

effortful control especially in the parent–child interaction (see Kochanska et al., 2007);

as suggested by Putnam and his colleagues (2008), early emerging tendencies to focus

attention and benefit from parental soothing may contribute to later regulation of

emotions, while positive affect (tendency to smile and laugh) may also lead the child to

experiences that promote the development of willful control and attention, or elicit

guidance from parents that facilitates the development of attentional skills underpinning

effortful control.

The stability of parents’ views of the child’s negative affectivity over five years may

reflect developmental continuity in the child’s negatively tuned reactions and behavior

within the family context, due to the child’s own constitutional characteristics, as well

as environmental characteristics (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). The path from the infant’s

perceived higher negative affect sumscore to lower extraversion and effortful control in

middle childhood may thus reflect initial difficulties in controlling fear and frustration

within everyday situations, or a developmental process where negative emotion and

attentional processes continually and mutually influence one another across time,

leading to a somewhat compromised development of attentional and inhibitory

mechanisms in the more negatively affective infant (Gaertner et al., 2008; Stifter &

Spinrad, 2002). At least, the challenge to control him- or herself and to approach new

situations may be harder for the easily frustrated or fearful infant.

In this transactional model, the child’s extraversion-sumscore did not contain aspects

of anger-frustration or self-control which, in the other three studies, loaded significantly

on the latent extraversion-superconstruct, presumably explaining the association

between higher infant negative affect and higher child extraversion in those three

studies. In this third study, the association between higher infant negative affectivity

and lower child extraversion-sumscore may be explained by predictive associations

between infant’s higher fearfulness and subsequent higher shyness and lack of

extraverted characteristics like approach anticipation and high-intensity pleasure.

I also examined whether parental personality and their views of the child’s

temperament showed parallel change over time (i.e., simultaneous change in parent and

child that was not explained by the predictive associations between infant and parent

Page 71: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

71

characteristics at baseline and at follow-up). Obviously, I cannot determine which, that

is the parent or the child, or some other environmental or individual aspect of the parent

or the child promoted the change. However, I found that an increase in the ratings of the

child’s negative affectivity and a decrease in positive-affectivity- and attention-related

characteristics from infancy to middle childhood were associated with an increase in

parental neuroticism, while parental extraversion and infant/child temperament did not

show any parallel change. These associations are well in line with what is reported in

the literature (e.g., Belsky et al., 1991; Kochanska et al., 2004; Propper & Moore,

2006), and complement the picture of transactional development between parental

personality and infant/child temperament.

4.2.2 Transactional development of maternal stress and child temperament

In the fourth study, maternal perceived overall stress was considered as an important

situational factor that may, besides the more constitutional personality characteristics,

influence mothers’ behavior and feelings at home. Maternal overall stress also belongs

to the likely significant contextual elements in the early development of temperament,

even though the child’s temperamental characteristics may, in turn, also act as

significant contributors of stability or change in the level of mothers’ perceived amount

of stress. Therefore, the fourth study aimed to explore the longitudinal relations between

maternal perceptions of stress and mother-rated temperamental development of the child

from infancy to middle childhood.

Support for the hypotheses that mothers’ experience of overall stress would predict

an increase in mothers’ ratings of temperamental maladjustment over time, and at the

same time, mother-rated temperament traits in infancy would predict either an increase

or a decrease in maternal stress was found. However, these effects were not shown in

the same model, i.e., temperament did not influence and was not influenced by maternal

stress in the same model. These results essentially complement prior, mainly cross-

sectional, findings on the relations between parental stress and child behavior, by

extending the period in question to cover the most important developmental phases in

early childhood.

Page 72: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

72

Maternal experience of stress proved to be a rather stable mental state from the

infant’s age of six months to five and a half years. In terms of changes in maternal stress

over time due to child temperament, we found only one significant path from infant

activity level to decreased maternal stress in the childhood period, controlling for the

level of initial maternal stress. This finding that a decrease in maternal stress was

associated with the parents’ views of their infant as high in temperamental activity goes

well in line with the finding that higher infant activity also reduced parental neuroticism

over this five-years period, and justifies the suggestions of the potential significance and

favorable effects of the infant’s activity for the parent–child relationship.

The more energetic and active the child is, the more likely he/she will also be to

continue interaction-eliciting behavior with the mother, regardless of the mother’s level

of stress. Thereby, as in the case of parental neuroticism, the effects that maternal

overall stress might have on her behavior towards the child, for example a tendency to

withdraw from interaction (Repetti & Wood, 1997), may not be able to reduce the

infant’s lively and initiative behavior. For the mother, focusing on the active child may

serve as an everyday means of forgetting the other stresses of life: these less stressed,

frequently occurring and intensive moments between mother and child may thus

eventually attenuate maternal perceptions of her overall stress. Further, the more active

child may not raise as much worry in a parent as a child that seems more vulnerable,

timid or fearful does.

It seemed that the changes in temperamental development from infancy to middle

childhood originating in maternal stress during the infancy period were more prominent

than changes in maternal stress. Changes in temperament included an increase in anger

and a decrease in attentional focusing, as well as a decrease in soothability. At the level

of latent temperament superconstructs of positive and negative affectivity, defined as in

the first two studies, maternal stress in the infancy period predicted a higher level of

child negative affectivity over time when infant negative affectivity was controlled for.

These results are in line with prior knowledge of parent-effects on child development

(Crnic et al., 2005). Maternal stress is thus likely to affect temperamental development

in terms of both reactive (e.g., anger) and self-regulative functioning (attentional

focusing and soothability). Similarly, the increase found in temperamental negative

affectivity encompasses both internalizing aspects of child behavior (e.g., fearfulness,

Page 73: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

73

shyness, sadness, and discomfort) as well as indicators of low self-regulation (low

attentional focusing, low soothability, and low inhibitory control).

Earlier research has shown how parental stress experiences are related to parent–

child interactions (Patterson, 1983; Snyder, 1991). Repetti and Wood’s (1997) notion of

maternal withdrawal from the interaction due to stress is of particular interest with

reference to the current findings. It could be hypothesized that parental stress-originated

withdrawal reduces the moments of shared attention between the dyad, moments that

are essential for the development of attention and self-regulation (Morales, Mundy,

Crowson, Neal, & Delgado, 2005). In the current study, the predictive associations

between higher maternal stress and the child’s reduced attentional and self-soothing

capacities and ability to control anger/frustration complement these suggestions.

4.3 General conclusions The current longitudinal study provided evidence for the theoretically relevant

continuity of mother- and father-rated temperament from infancy to middle childhood,

within the developmental psychobiological framework of Rothbart and her colleagues

(Rothbart & Bates, 2006). It was also shown that the types characterizing childhood

and adulthood personality may share common ground with temperament types from

infancy onwards. Furthermore, the view that the development of children’s

temperament is also sensitive and differentially susceptible to variability in contextual

factors, represented in this study by parental personality traits and perceived overall

stress (Belsky, 1997; Van den Boom, 1994; Wachs, 2006), was also supported.

For the adult personality traits extraversion and neuroticism, high stability over five

years was shown. However, it was also shown that the unique environmental forces

within a family that individually affect temperamental development in a child may not

disappear in adulthood: the development of parents’ personality traits seemed to be

susceptible to their perceptions of the child’s temperament. Similarly, parents’

experience of more situational factors, represented in this study by maternal perceptions

of overall stress, also seemed to be susceptible to experiences with the child.

The continuities and associations between parents’ and infants’/children’s

characteristics found in this study were independent of child gender. The cross-lagged

paths between temperament and personality were also similar for mothers and fathers,

Page 74: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

74

fitting with evidence showing that the rank-order stability of adult personality (Caspi et

al., 2005), and the extent to which various environmental factors affect individual

differences in men’s and women’s personalities and associated characteristics (e.g.,

Ansell & Pincus, 2004; Costa & McCrae, 1988; Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001) are

unaffected by gender. In terms of stress, the associations between maternal stress and

child temperament were, as in previous studies (Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996), also

independent of child gender. While the current study investigated only maternal stress,

similar indications and associations can be assumed for both parents’ stress (Deater-

Deckard, & Scarr, 1996; Deater-Deckard et al., 1994).

4.4 Weaknesses and strengths of the study There are limitations to this study that that should be borne in mind in the interpretation

of the findings. First, there were no intermediate assessments of temperament between

infancy and five-and-a-half years of age available: intermediate assessments certainly

would have specified more exactly the developmental paths of temperament. For the

third and fourth study, examining the developmental transaction between parental

personality and stress and the child’s temperament, one limitation is related to the lack

of micro-level interactional patterns between parent–child dyads that would illuminate

the paths between the predictor and outcome variables in a more detailed fashion.

In temperament research, the question of objectivity in measuring child behavior

according to parental reports is also still open to debate (Kagan, 1994, 1998; Seifer et

al., 2004). Kagan (1998), for example, noted that parents may be prone to form an

unintended but consistent disposition towards the child, although the prior experiences

of the child may not have been that consistent. However, sources of observational error

exist also in artificial laboratory settings, for example errors related to the characteristics

of the rater, or effects of the situation or measure on child behaviors, or interactions

between rater characteristics and child behavior (Rothbart & Goldsmith, 1985). Further,

in the more objective assessment settings, only a limited set of behaviors can be evoked.

Especially in the case of positive affectivity, some children could be less likely to

express positive affect in the laboratory, relative to other more familiar settings where

the parents are able to see this behavior (Gartstein & Marmion, 2008; Rothbart &

Goldsmith, 1985). The unfamiliar laboratory setting may also make the child exhibit

Page 75: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

75

more fearful reactions than what is representative of his/her general level of fearfulness

in everyday life (Rothbart & Goldsmith, 1985). Accordingly, recent evidence points to

greater predictive validity and greater situational consistency of parent-ratings in

comparison to these more objective perceptions (Majdandzic & van den Boom, 2007;

Pauli-Pott et al., 2003). Because temperament exerts its effect on the child’s everyday

life, Rothbart’s measures also allow the empirical investigation of temperament in

various situations and social contexts. The possibility of parental inaccuracy and bias is

further minimized by asking for frequencies of recent, specific, and concrete behaviors

in response to common everyday stimuli (Rothbart, 1981; Rothbart et al., 2001). The

items in the infant and child behavior questionnaires are also worded in a simple and

straightforward manner, in order to minimize misinterpretation (Putnam et al., 2001).

The relatively small sample sizes and response rates in this study also restrict the

external validity of the findings. Especially, due to the inclusion criteria for family units

in Study II and III, i.e., both parents had to provide complete data on all of the study

variables at the baseline and at the follow-up, the drop-out rate was relatively high.

Even though the study sample was representative of the initial population,

generalizations should still be drawn with caution.

Further, using the same informant for parents’ as well as infant’s/child’s

characteristics evokes the question of shared method variance. However, the important

advantage of path analysis and the analysis of correlated change is that it reduces shared

method variance: because the path coefficients control for the indirect paths, which

contain the full bias, the error is, at least partly eliminated (Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001).

Beyond that, it should also be acknowledged that the causality between parent and child

cannot be considered straightforward enough to enable the isolation of any single factor

as the sole origin of a developmental outcome. In other words, the analyses of cross-

paths and correlated change in the present study are unable to provide conclusive

evidence of causal effects, because the study designs did not allow us to rule out

alternative explanations, such as the coaction of other, for example genetic- or

community-level -factors, that push the development forward.

Page 76: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

76

4.5 Theoretical and clinical implications Characteristics of personality and temperament are theoretically and empirically related

(Caspi et al., 2005; Rothbart et al., 2000a), and closely associated with individuals’ well

being and psychosocial adjustment (e.g., Auerbach et al., 2008; Costa & McCrae, 1980;

Eisenberg, et al., 2007; Hampson, 2008; Löckenhoff, Sutin, Ferrucci, & Costa, 2008;

Nigg, 2006; Ramos, Guerin, Gottfried, Bathurst, & Oliver, 2005; Roberts et al., 2007;

Steel, Schmidt, & Shultz, 2008). While no clear consensus yet exists concerning the

exact nature of temperament or personality, an integrated understanding of the

constructs, their development and associations with environmental influences and

various clinically relevant outcomes is of increasing interest for both researchers and

clinicians. Therefore, evidence of continuity, as well as every association between

changes in the child’s temperament and parental personality is both theoretically and

clinically meaningful.

In the current study, temperament and personality were seen as broadly overlapping

domains, with temperament providing the primarily biological basis for the developing

personality (Caspi et al., 2005; Costa & McCrae, 2000; Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart

et al., 2000a). Besides the genetic association between temperament and personality

(e.g., Goldsmith et al., 1987), it was also acknowledged that temperament develops and

includes also later appearing elements, which further relates temperament to the

construct of personality. Temperament and personality were also considered most

meaningful in social contexts.

Thereby, the results of the current study were in line with the definition of

temperament as early appearing individual differences that; first, are relatively stable

across situations; second, are not all visible at birth, but become visible during time; and

third, are shaped by both hereditary and environmental factors. Interaction between

different temperament dimensions during development was also acknowledged. Further,

it was acknowledged that the same environment may have a unique influence on the

level of more than one dimension of temperament or personality at the same time.

For temperament, the results of the current study were in line with Rothbart’s

theoretical framework, highlighting especially the developmental coherence of positive

and negative affectivity, as well as the interplay between reactivity and self-regulation

in growth from infancy to middle childhood (Rothbart & Bates, 2006), from the

Page 77: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

77

perspectives of both of the child’s parents. The predictive validity of both parents’

ratings was also supplemented, which further emphasizes the importance of both

parents in developmental research. The confirmatory factor solution, shown to fit the

data well among six to seven years olds in Rothbart et al. (2001), was also replicated in

this Finnish sample. Thereby, as an additional contribution, the current study further

validated the measures and constructs of Rothbart and her colleagues (Rothbart et al.,

2001).

The individual level temperament profiles further illustrated how the common basis

of temperament and personality can be profiled in terms of infant/child temperament

dimensions. In a more practical or clinical sense, the resilient, under- and overcontrolled

temperament types showed how the early temperament variables may assume their

actual meaning in relation to other variables in an individual’s profile (Janson &

Mathiesen, 2008).

The current study also highlighted the relevance of temperament as a relational and

interactional construct (e.g., Rothbart & Bates, 2006), in which individual differences in

ways of regulating experiences are particularly susceptible to social influences. Instead

of conceptualizing temperament only as a set of traits inherent in the child, temperament

was, thus, also seen as a set of individual differences in the way children regulate

experience (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). In terms of theory-based change or lawful

discontinuity (Caspi & Roberts, 1999), the results of the present study were in line with

literature suggesting that the development of infant temperament, and especially the

characteristics related to the development of negative affectivity and attentional and

self-regulative capacities may be affected, besides genetic influence, by the unique

environment of the infant (Belsky, 1997; Belsky, Goldsmith et al., 1999; Hsieh, &

Crnic, 1998; Gilissen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & van der Veer, 2008;

Kochanska, 1995, 1997), here conceptualized as parental personality and experienced

overall stress.

For adult personality traits extraversion and neuroticism, theory-based high rank-

order stability over five years was shown (Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000; Terracciano et

al., 2006). However, despite high consistency, evidence of the possibility of

theoretically relevant non-normative change in these temperament-based personality

traits, due to important and enduring interpersonal relations (Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001;

Page 78: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

78

Robins et al., 2002) was also gained. More specifically, the current study suggests that

individuals seem to be able to maintain their level of extraversion, perhaps even in less

favorable environments, and be able to utilize a chance, at least in the form of the

child’s positive affectivity, to become more extraverted. In the case of neuroticism, and

also in the more situational experience of life as stressful, this study suggests that

individuals may be prone to detect reasons for decreasing the levels of these less

favorable behaviors and feelings (see Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001), while also the risk for

cumulative effects of the more negative parent and child characteristics seem to be true

for higher parental neuroticism and negatively tuned situational experiences like higher

parental overall stress.

The similarities found in the temperament-associations for neuroticism and stress are

understandable, as individuals who score high on neuroticism are also known to be

easily overwhelmed by stressful experiences (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1992). However,

whereas self-ratings of neuroticism rise from a range of individual temperament-based

characteristics, parents’ perceptions of stress may also result from situational factors,

like objectively higher amounts of stressful phenomena in their life. Parental experience

of life as stressful, caused by either environmental or individual-level causes, may still

have a detrimental effect on the proximal processes between parent and child, evidenced

here as increases in negative aspects of child temperament.

In a transactional sense, the predictive associations and simultaneous change between

parent and child characteristics detected in this study may have involved both evocative

and reactive person–environment transactions that, within a family, may be suggested to

lead to discontinuities as well as continuities in temperament and personality, causing

simultaneous change (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Or it may have been that parents and

children proactively created environments that fitted and strengthened their

characteristics, and these environments further promoted their genetically concordant

characteristics (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). The current study also provided evidence of

how these within-family experiences may differentially influence an individual’s

development, in terms of temperament- or personality-related differences in

susceptibility of unique environments in childhood as in adulthood. In terms of Belsky’s

(1984) ecological model, the idea of intertwined developmental influence between the

Page 79: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

79

three main classes of factors within a family, namely the parents’ own personality traits,

perceived overall stress and the child’s temperamental characteristics, was supported.

Considering the developmental relevance of parental views, it may be suggested that

the over-time continuity of parental views of the child’s temperament, and the

bidirectional influences between parent’s views of their own personality and the child’s

temperament may be those that matter the most, because this is the intimate and

enduring environment that is probably the most influential for both parent and child.

Whether parents’ individual characteristics shape their subjective interpretations of their

child or not, these interpretations are those that come to life between parent and child.

Similarly, parental perceptions of situational characteristics like overall stress can be

viewed as a combination of objective stressful events, coping processes and personality

factors that together form the outcome variable of perceived stress, which may then put

a strain on the everyday interaction of the parent and the child.

Further, considering the clinical relevance of early temperament, a child’s

temperament as it interacts with the environment, here conceptualized as the interaction

between parent and child characteristics may be a better predictor of developmental

outcomes than temperament alone (Rothbart & Putnam, 2002). Given the increasing

evidence of the influence of contextual characteristics upon central nervous system

development (e.g., Nelson & Bloom, 1997; Schore, 2001), it can be assumed that the

proximal processes between parent and child could also have an influence on the

constitutional underpinnings of temperamental characteristics. Within personality trait

theory, it is also acknowledged that an increased understanding of the mechanisms or

correlates of rank-order change in the temperament-based personality traits may further

allow the development of effective therapeutic interventions even during adulthood

(McCrae, 2001).

Furthermore, the predictive associations, along with the parallel changes in parents’

self-rated neuroticism and overall stress and the child’s temperament highlight the

importance of paying attention to the cumulative effects of parents’ self-representations

and the child’s temperament, at least when evaluating the developmental risks or

planning any therapeutic endeavors for a specific family (see e.g. Gartstein & Sheeber,

2004; Martorell & Bugental, 2006). Additional clinical relevance for recognizing the

negative cumulative effects of parent and child characteristics comes from the existing

Page 80: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

80

research showing that especially the more negative or challenging temperamental

characteristics in a young child are more susceptible to the harmful influence of less

optimal parental characteristics (Belsky, 1997).

However, the present study also suggests that, if offered a strong and enduring

enough environmental chance for change, parents’ and children’s reasonably stable

psychological characteristics may also be changed for the better. It may be proposed

that extraversion and individual temperament-dimensions linked with that trait can be

considered as buffering or protective factors in the child’s as well as in the adult’s life,

especially within their intertwined development during the child’s early years.

In sum, the current study showed that parent-rated temperament shows moderate to

high levels of continuity from infancy to middle childhood, and that the continuity of

mother- and father-rated temperament is quantitatively and qualitatively similar.

Theoretically relevant links between childhood temperament and personality types were

also shown. Furthermore, this study offered evidence of concurrent and predictive

bidirectional associations between child temperament and parental personality and

perceived stress. Thereby, this study also emphasizes the importance of paying attention

to the transactional effects of parents’ personality- and stress-related characteristics and

views of the child’s temperament, especially when evaluating the risks and protective

factors for a specific family.

Page 81: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

81

REFERENCES Abe, J. A. & Izard, C. E. (1999). A longitudinal study of emotion expression and personality relations in early development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 566-577. Abidin, R. R. (1990). Parenting stress index (PSI) manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc. Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children's temperament in the US and China: Similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7, 359-377. Aksan, N., Goldsmith, H. H., Smider, N. A., Essex, M. J., Clark, R., Hyde, J. S., Klein, M. H., & Vandell, D. L. (1999). Derivation and prediction of temperamental types among preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 35, 958–971. Amato, P. R. (1994). Father-child relations, mother-child relations, and offspring psychological well-being in early adulthood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 1031–1042. Amato, P. R., & Gilbreth, J. G. (1999). Nonresident fathers and children’s well-being: A meta- analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 557–573. Ansell, E. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2004). Interpersonal perceptions of the five-factor model of personality: An examination using the structural summary method for circumplex data. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 167–201. Anthony, L. G., Anthony, B. J., Glanville, D. N., Naiman, D. Q., Waanders, C., & Shaffer, S. (2005). The Relationships between parenting stress, parenting behavior and preschoolers' social competence and behavior problems in the classroom. Infant and Child Development,14, 133-154. Ardelt, M. (2000). Still stable after all these years? Personality stability theory revisited. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 392-405. Asendorpf, J. B., Borkenau, P., Ostendorf, F., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2001). Carving personality description at its joints: Confirmation of three replicable personality prototypes for both children

and adults. European Journal of Personality, 15, 169-198. Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (1999). Resilient, overcontrolled, and undercontrolled

personality prototypes in childhood: Replicability, predictive power, and the trait-type issue. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 815–832.

Asendorpf, J. B., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Personality-relationship transaction in adolescence: Core versus surface personality characteristics. Journal of Personality, 71, 629–666.

Auerbach, J. G., Berger, A., Atzaba-Poria, N., Arbelle, S., Cypin, N., Friedman, A., & Landau, R. (2008).Temperament at 7, 12, and 25 months in children at familial risk for ADHD. Infant and Child development, 17, 321-338.

Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 83-96. Belsky, J. (1996). Parent, infant, and social-contextual antecedents of father-son attachment security.

Developmental Psychology, 32, 905–913. Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to environmental influences: An evolutionary argument.

Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182–186. Belsky, J., & Barends, N. (2002). Personality and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.). Handbook of

parenting: Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (2nd ed., pp. 415-438). Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum. Belsky, J., Crnic, K., & Woodworth, S. (1995). Personality and parenting: Exploring the mediating role of

transient mood and daily hassles. Journal of Personality, 63, 905-929. Belsky, J., Fish, M., & Isabella, R. A. (1991). Continuity and discontinuity in infant negative and positive

emotionality: Family antecedents and attachment consequences. Developmental Psychology, 27, 421–431.

Belsky, J., Hsieh, K.-H., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301 – 319

Belsky, J., Rha, J.-H., & Park, S.-Y. (2000). Exploring reciprocal parent and child effects in the case of child inhibition in US and Korean samples. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 338–347. Belsky, J., Woodworth, S., & Crnic, K. (1996). Trouble in the second year: Three questions about family

interaction. Child Development, 67, 556–578. Berdan, L. E., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2008). Temperament and externalizing behavior: Social

preference and perceived acceptance as protective factors. Developmental Psychology, 44, 957-968.

Page 82: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

82

Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (2001). Person-centered research. In T. Cook & C. Ragin (Eds.), The international encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences: Logic of inquiry and research design (pp. 11333–11339). Oxford: Elsevier.

Bergman, K., P. Sarkar, P., Glover V., & O'Connor T. G. (2008). Quality of child–parent attachment moderates the impact of antenatal stress on child fearfulness. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1089-1098

Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibition? Child Development, 74, 1899–1917.

Blatny, M., Jelinek, M., & Osecka, T. (2007). Assertive toddler, self-efficacious adult: Child temperament predicts personality over forty years. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 2127-2136.

Boivin, M., Pérusse, D., & Dionne, G. (2005). The genetic-environmental etiology of parents’ perceptions and self-assessed behaviors toward their 5-month-old infants in a large twin and singleton sample.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 612–630. Bouchard, T. J., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality. Behavior Genetics, 31, 243-

273. Breckenridge, J. N. (2000). Validating cluster analysis: Consistent replication and symmetry. Multivariate

Behavioral Research, 35, 261–285. Bridges, L. J., Palmer, S. A., Morales, M., Hurtado, M. (1993). Agreement between affectively based

observational and parent-report measures of temperament at infant age 6 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 501-506.

Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Calkins, S. D., Blandon, A.Y., Williford, A. P., & Keane, S. P. (2007). Biological, behavioral, and

relational levels of resilience in the context of risk for early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 675-700.

Calkins, S. D., Hungerford, A., & Dedmon, S. E. (2004). Mothers’ interactions with temperamentally frustrated infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 219–239.

Carnicero, J. A. C., Pérez-López, J., Salinas, M. C. G., & Martinéz-Fuentes, M. T. (2000). A longitudinal study of temperament in infancy: Stability and convergence of measures. European Journal of Personality, 14, 21–37.

Carson, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions and children's peer competency. Child Development, 67, 2217-2226.

Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741-751.

Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg, (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 311-388). New York: Wiley.

Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality continuities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 158-172.

Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age 3 predict personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Child Development, 66, 486–498.

Caspi, A., & Roberts, B. W. (1999). Personality continuity and change across the life course. In Pervin, L. A. & John O. P. (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 300–326). New

York: Guilford Press. Caspi, A., & Roberts, B. W. (2001). Target article: Personality development across the life course: The

argument for change and continuity. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 49-66. Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453–484. Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1986). Temperament in clinical practice. New York: Guilford. Clark, L.A., Kochanska, G., & Ready, R. (2000). Mothers' personality and its interaction with child

temperament as predictors of parenting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 274-285.

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1999). Temperament: A new paradigm for trait psychology. In L. A. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 399–423). New York: Guilford Press.

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.

Page 83: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

83

Cole, P. M., Teti, L. O., & Sahn-Waxler, C. (2003). Mutual emotion regulation and the stability of conduct problems between preschool and early school age. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 1-18.

Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55,

218–232. Conger, R. D., McCarty, J. A., Yang, R. K., Lahey, B. B., & Kropp, J. P. (1984). Perceptions of child, child-rearing values and emotional distress as mediating links between environmental stressors and

observed maternal behavior. Child Development, 55, 2234–2247. Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic

pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 179-193.

Coplan, R. J., Bowker, A., & Cooper, S. M. (2003). Parenting daily hassles, child temperament, and social adjustment in preschool. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18, 376–395. Costa, P. T., Jr., Herbst, J. H., McCrae, R. R., & Siegler, I. C. (2000). Personality at midlife: Stability,

intrinsic maturation, and response to life events. Assessment, 7, 365–378. Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of Extraversion and Neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678. Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO personality inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO personality inventory. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 54, 853–863. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO personality inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4, 5–13. Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1997). Stability and change in personality assessment: The Revised

NEO Personality Inventory in the Year 2000. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 86-94. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2000). A theoretical context for adult development. In T. D. Wachs &

G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context (pp. 1–21). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cowan, C. P., Cowan, P. A., Heming, G., & Miller, N. B. (1991). Becoming a family: Marriage, parenting, and child development. In Cowan, P. A. & Hetherington E. M. (Eds.), Family transitions

(pp. 79–109). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Creasey, G., & Reese, M. (1996). Mothers’ and fathers’ perception of parenting hassles: Associations

with psychological symptoms, nonparenting hassles, and child behavior problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 393–406.

Crnic, K. A., Gaze, C., & Hoffman, C. (2005). Cumulative parenting stress across the preschool period: Relations to maternal parenting and child behavior at age 5. Infant and Child Development, 14, 117–132.

Crnic, K. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1990). Parenting stresses with young children. Child Development, 61, 1628–1637. Crnic, K., & Low, C. (2002). Everyday stresses and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.). Handbook of

parenting, Vol. 5: Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp. 243-267). Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum. Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2006). Infant and maternal behavior moderate reactivity to novelty

to predict anxious behavior at 2, 5 years. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 17–34. Crockenberg, S. B., & Smith, P. (2002). Antecedents of mother-infant interaction and infant irritability in

the first 3 months of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 2-15. De Fruyt, F., & Vollrath, M. (2003). Inter-parent agreement on higher and lower level traits in two

countries: Effects of parent and child gender. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 289–301. Deater-Deckard, K., & Scarr, S. (1996). Parenting stress among dual-earner mothers and fathers: Are

there gender differences? Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 45–59. Deater-Deckard, K., Scarr, S., McCartney, K., & Eisenberg, M. (1994). Paternal separation anxiety:

Relationships with parenting stress, child-rearing attitudes, and maternal anxieties. Psychological Science, 5, 341-346.

Denham, S. A., Lehman, E. B., Moser, M. H., & Reeves, S. (1995). Continuity and change in emotional components of infant temperament. Child Study Journal, 25, 289–308.

Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633–652.

Page 84: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

84

Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Early temperament and emotional development. In A. F. Kalverboer & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), Handbook of brain and behavior in human development (pp. 967–987). GB: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Digman, J. M. (1994). Child personality and temperament: Does the five-factor model embrace both domains. In: C. F. Jr Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 323-338). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Eaton, W. Q. (1994). Temperament, development, and the five-factor model: Lessons from activity level. In C. F. Jr. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of

temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 173–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eisenberg, N., Ma, Y., Chang, L., Zhou, Q., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. (2007). Relations of effortful

control, reactive undercontrol, and anger to Chinese children's adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 385-409.

Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., 6 Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children's effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 5, 1055-1071.

Elder, G. H. Jr., (1994). Time, human aging, and social change: Perspectives on the life course. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 4–15.

Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223-231.

Fleeson, W. (2007). Situation-based contingencies underlying trait-content manifestation in behavior. Journal of Personality, 75, 825–861.

Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1-21.

Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B.W. (2005). Patterns of continuity: A dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological

Review, 112, 60–74. Gaertner, B. M., Spinrad, T. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2008). Focused attention in toddlers: measurement,

stability, and relations to negative emotion and parenting. Infant and Child Development, 17, 339-363.

Gandour,M. J. (1989). Activity level as a dimension of temperament in toddlers: Its relevance for the organismic specificity hypothesis. Child Development, 60, 1092–1098.

Gartstein, M. A., & Bateman, A. E. (2008). Early manifestations of childhood depression: Influences of infant temperament and parental depressive symptoms. Infant and Child Development, 17, 223–248.

Gartstein, M. A., Gonzalez, C., Carranza, J.A., Ahadi, S. A., Ye, R., Rothbart, M. K., & Yang, S. W. (2006). Studying cross-cultural differences in the development of infant temperament: People's Republic of China, the United States of America, and Spain. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 37, 145-161.

Gartstein, M. A., & Marmion, J. (2008). Fear and positive affectivity in infancy: Convergence/discrepancy between parent-report and laboratory-based indicators. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 227-238.

Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 64–86.

Gartstein, M. A., & Sheeber, L. (2004). Child behavior problems and maternal symptoms of depression: A mediational model. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 17, 141-150.

Gartstein, M. A., Slobodskaya, H. R., & Kinsht, I. A. (2003). Cross-cultural differences in temperament in the first year of life: United States of America (US) and Russia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 316–328.

Ge, X., Conger, R. D., Cadoret, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Yates, W., Troughton, E., & Stewart, M. A. (1996). The developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual influence model of child antisocial behavior and parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 574-589.

Gelfand, D. T., Teti, D. M., & Fox, C. R. (1992). Sources of parenting stress for depressed and nondepressed mothers of infants. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 262–272. Gilissen, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H., & van der Veer, R. (2008). Parent

child relationship, temperament, and physiological reactions to fear-inducing film clips: Further evidence for differential susceptibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99,182-195.

Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying temperament via construction of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire. Child Development, 67, 218–235.

Page 85: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

85

Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., & Rothbart, M. K. (1987). What is temperament? Four approaches. Child Development, 58, 505-529.

Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Buss, K. A., & Campos, J. J. (1999). Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 35, 972–985. Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1990). The structure of temperamental fear and pleasure in infants: A

psychometric perspective. Child Development, 61, 1944–1964. Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Contemporary instruments for assessing early temperament

by questionnaire and in the laboratory. In J. Strelau & A. Angeleitner (Eds.), Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement (pp. 249–272). New York: Plenum.

Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Laboratory temperament assessment battery (LAB-TAB). Pre- and locomotor versions. University of Oregon.

Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Sullivan-Logan, G. M. (1998). Temperament, activity, and expectations for later personality development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1266-1277.

Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo hippocampal system. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Gross, J. J., Sutton, S. K., & Ketelaar, T. (1998). Relations between affect and personality: Support for

the affect-level and affective reactivity views. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 279-288.

Guerin, D. W., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994). Developmental stability and change in parent reports of temperament: A ten-year longitudinal investigation from infancy through preadolescence. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 334-355.

Gutteling, B. M., de Weerth, C., Willemsen-Swinkels, S. H. N., Huizink, A. C., Mulder, E. J. H., Visser, G. H. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). The effects of prenatal stress on temperament and problem behavior of 27-month-old toddlers. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 14, 41-51.

Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (1998). Preschool temperament and environmental factors related to the Five Factor Model of personality in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 194–215.

Hampson, S. E. (2008). Mechanisms by which childhood personality traits influence adult well being. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 264-268.

Helson, R., Kwan, V. S. Y., & Jones, O. P. (2002). The growing evidence for personality change in adulthood: Findings from research with personality inventories. Journal of Research in Personality,

36, 287–306. Heinonen, K., Räikkönen, K., Scheier, M. F., Pesonen, A.-K., Järvenpää, A.-L., & Strandberg, T. E.

(2006). Parents’ optimism is related to their ratings of their children’s behavior. European Journal of Personality, 20, 421–445.

Honjo, S., Mizuno, R., Ajiki, M., Suzuki, A., Nagata, M., Goto, Y., & Nishide, T. (1999). Infant temperament and child-rearing stress: Birth order influences. Early Human Development, 51, 123– 135. Huizink, A. C., Robles de Medina, P. G., Mulder, E. J. H., Visser, G. H. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2002). Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1078–1085. Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet

level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1556-1565.

Janson, H., & Mathiesen, K. S. (2008). Temperament profiles from infancy to middle childhood: Development and associations with behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1314-1328.

Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Krueger, R. F. (2005). Personality stability in late adulthood: A behavioral genetic analysis. Journal of Personality, 73, 523–551. Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A., Korten, A. E., & Rodgers, B. (1999).Using

the BIS/BAS scales to measure behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation: Factor structure, validity and norms in a large community sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 49-58.

Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy. New York: Basic. Kagan, J. (1998). Biology and the child. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child

psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp.177–235). New York: Wiley.

Kagan, J. (1971). Change and continuity in infancy. New York: Wiley.

Page 86: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

86

Kliewer, W., & Kung, E. (1998). Family moderators of the relations between hassles and behavior problems in inner city youth. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 278–292.

Kochanska, G. (1995). Children’s temperament, mothers’ discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597 – 615.

Kochanska G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.

Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Penney, S. J., & Doobay, A. F. (2007). Early positive emotionality as a heterogenous trait: Implications for children’s self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1054–1066.

Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., Tjebkes, T. L., & Husarek, S. J. (1998). Individual differences in emotionality in infancy. Child Development, 64, 375–390.

Kochanska, G., Friesenborg, A. E., Lange, L. A., & Martel, M. M. (2004). Parents’ personality and infants’ temperament as contributors to their emerging relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 744–759.

Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.

Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.

Kurdek, L. A. (2003). Correlates of parents’ perceptions of behavioral problems in their young children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 457–473. Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional

states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132-140. Lehnart, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2006). Should I stay or should I go? Attachment and personality in stable and

instable romantic relationships. European Journal of Personality, 20, 475-495. Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of

infant and childhood temperament. Developmental Psychology, 35, 189–204. Lengua, L. J. (2006). Growth in temperament and parenting as predictors of adjustment during children's

transition to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 42, 819-832. Lengua, L. J., & Kovacs, E. A. (2005). Bidirectional associations between temperament and parenting and the prediction of adjustment problems in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental

Psychology, 26, 21–38. Leve, L. D., Scaramella, L. V., & Fagot, B. I. (2001). Infant temperament, pleasure in parenting, and

marital happiness in adoptive families. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 545–558. Losoya, S. H., Callor, S., Rowe, D. C., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1997). Origins of familial similarity in

parenting: A study of twins and adoptive siblings. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1012-1023. Löckenhoff, C. E., Sutin, A. R., Ferrucci, L., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2008). Personality traits and subjective

health in the later years: The association between NEO-PI-R and SF-36 in advanced age is influenced by health status. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1334-1346.

Majdandzic, M., & van den Boom, D. C. (2007). Multimethod longitudinal assessment of temperament in early childhood. Journal of Personality, 75, 121–167.

Mandara, J. (2003). The typological approach in child and family psychology: A review of theory, methods, and research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6, 129–146.

Martorell, G. A., & Bugental, D. B. (2006). Maternal variations in stress reactivity: Implications for harsh parenting practices with very young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 641-647.

McBride, B. A., Schoppe, S. J., & Rane, T. R. (2002). Child characteristics, parenting stress, and parental involvement: Fathers versus mothers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 998–1011.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of a five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P.T. Jr. (1994). The stability of personality: Observation and evaluations.

Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 173-175. McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., H ebí ková M., Avia, M. D., Sanz, J.,

Sánchez-Bernardos, M. L., Kusdil, M. E., Woodfield, R., Saunders, P. R., & Smith, P. B. (2000). Nature over nurture: Temperament, personality, and life span development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 173–186. McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215.

Page 87: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

87

Mervielde, I., De Clercq, B., De Fruyt, F., & Van Leeuwen, K. (2005). Temperament, personality, and developmental psychopathology as childhood antecedents of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 171-201.

Metsäpelto, R-L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Personality traits and parenting: Neuroticism, extraversion, and openness to experience as discriminative factors European Journal of Personality, 17, 59-78.

Morales, M. Mundy, P., Crowson, M. M., Neal, A.R., & Delgado, C. E. F. (2005). Individual differences in infant attention skills, joint attention, and emotion regulation behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 259-263.

Mulsow, M., Caldera, Y. M., Pursley, M., Reifman, A., & Huston, A. C. (2002). Multilevel factors influencing maternal stress during the first three years. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 944-956.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2006). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén. Nelson, C. A., & Bloom, F. E. (1997). Child development and neuroscience. Child Development, 68,

970–987. Neyer, F. J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2001). Personality-relationship transaction in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1190–1204. Neyer, F. J., & Lehnart, J. (2007). Relationships matter in personality development: Evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study across young adulthood. Journal of Personality, 75, 535–568. Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395-422. Paris, R., & Helson, R. (2002). Early mothering experience and personality change. Journal of Family

Psychology, 16, 172–185. Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the Father-Child Relationship: Mechanisms and Developmental

Outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193-219. Parke, R. D. (2004). Fathers, families, and the future: A plethora of plausible predictions. Merrill-Palmer

Quarterly, 50, 456–470. Patterson, G. R. (1983). Stress: A change agent for family process. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds.),

Stress, coping and development in children (pp. 235–264). New York: McGraw-Hill. Patterson, G. R., & Fisher, P. A. (2002). Recent developments in our understanding of parenting:

Bidirectional effects, causal models, and the search for parsimony. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 5: Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp. 59-88). Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum.

Pauli-Pott, U., Mertesacker, B., Bade, U., Havercock, A., & Beckmann, D. (2003). Parental perceptions and infant temperament development. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 27–48.

Pesonen, A.-K., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Komsi, N., Strandberg, T., & Järvenpää, A.-L. (2005). Continuity of maternal stress from the pre- to the postnatal period: Associations with infant’s

positive, negative and overall reactivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 28, 36–45. Peterson, J., & Hawley, D. R. (1998). Effects of stressors on parenting attitudes and family functioning in

a primary prevention program. Family Relations, 47, 221. Pedlow, R., Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). Stability of maternally reported temperament

from infancy to 8 years. Developmental Psychology, 29, 998-1007. Piedmont, R. L. (2001). Cracking the plaster cast: Big five personality change during intensive outpatient

counseling. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 500-520. Prior, M. (1992). Childhood temperament. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, (33), 249–297. Propper, C., & Moore, G. A. (2006). The influence of parenting on infant emotionality: A multi-level

psychobiological perspective. Developmental Review, 26, 427–460. Pulver, A., Allik, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Hämäläinen, M. (1995). A big five personality inventory in two

non-Indo-European languages. European Journal of Personality, 9, 109–124. Putnam, S. P., Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence. In A. Eliasz, & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Advances in research on temperament

(pp. 165–182). Germany: Pabst Science. Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 386–401. Putnam, S. P., Rothbart, M. K., & Gartstein, M. A. (2008). Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fine-grained temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early childhood. Infant and Child

Development, 17, 387-405.

Page 88: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

88

Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child temperament and parenting. In M. H.Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 1 Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 255-277). Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum.

Putnam, S. P., Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach–inhibition in toddlers: prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76, 212-226.

Ramchandani, P., Stein, A., & Evans, J. (2005). Paternal depression in the postnatal period and child development: A prospective population study. Lancet, 365, 2201–2205.

Ramos, M. C., Guerin, D. W., Gottfried, A. W., Bathurst, K., & Oliver, P. H. (2005). Family Conflict and Children's Behavior Problems: The Moderating Role of Child Temperament. Structural Equation Modeling, 12, 278-298.

Rantanen, J., Kinnunen, U., Feldt, T., & Pulkkinen, L. (2008). Work-family conflict and psychological well-being: Stability and cross-lagged relations within one- and six-year follow-ups. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 73, 37-51.

Repetti, R. L., & Wood, J. (1997). Effects of daily stress at work on mothers’ interaction with preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 90–108. Roberts, B. W. (1997). Plaster or plasticity: Are work experiences associated with personality change in women? Journal of Personality, 65, 205–232. Roberts, B.W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003).Work experiences and personality development in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 582–593. Roberts, B.W., & Chapman, C. N. (2000). Change in dispositional well-being and its relation to role quality: A 30-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 26–41 Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 3-25. Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality:

The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 313-345.

Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 31-35.

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., Bogg, T., & Caspi, A. (2006a). De-investment in work and non-normative personality trait change in young adulthood. European Journal of Personality, 20, 461-474.

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006b). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 1–25.

Roberts, B.W., Wood, D., & Smith, J. L. (2005). Evaluating five factor theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 166–184.

Robins, R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2002). It’s not just who you’re with, it’s who you are: Personality and relationship experiences across multiple relationships. Journal of Personality, 70,

925–964. Robins, R. W., John, O. P., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Resilient,

overcontrolled, and undercontrolled boys: Three replicable personality types. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 157-171.

Robins, R. W., & Tracy, J. L. (2003). Setting an agenda for a person-centered approach to personality development. Monographs of the Society for Research in child Development, 68, 110–122.

Robinson, M., Oddy, W. H., Li, J., Kendall, G., de Klerk, N. H., Silburn, S. R., Zubrick, S. R., Newnham, J. P., Stanley, F. J., & Mattes, E. (2008). Pre- and postnatal influences on preschool mental health: a large-scale cohort study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 1118–1128.

Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Temperament and development. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp.187–248). New York: Wiley.

Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 22, 356–365.

Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578. Rothbart, M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1994). Temperament and the development of personality. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 103, 55-66. Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000a). Temperament and personality: Origins and

outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 122–135.

Page 89: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

89

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394–1408.

Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed), Handbook of child psychology: Social emotional and personality development (Vol. 3, 6th ed., pp.

99–166). New York: Wiley. Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In M.

E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (pp. 37–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000b). Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at seven years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span (pp. 85–119). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobiological approach to the development of temperament. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior (pp. 83–116). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Rothbart, M. K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1985). Three approaches to the study of infant temperament. Developmental Review, 5, 237-260.

Rothbart, M. K., & Putnam, S. P. (2002). Temperament and socialization. In L. Pulkkinen & A. Caspi (Eds.), Paths to successful development: Personality in the life course (pp. 19–45). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). The transaction between parents’ perceptions of their children’s shyness and their parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 937–958.

Räikkönen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Heinonen, K., Komsi, N., Järvenpää, A.-L., & Strandberg, T. E. (2006). Stressed parents: A dyadic perspective on perceived infant temperament. Infant and Child Development, 15, 75–87. Sameroff, A. J. (1995). General systems theories and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 659–695). New York: Wiley. Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Models of development and developmental risk. In C. H. Jr. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (2nd ed., pp. 3–19). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Saudino, K. J., Wertz, A. E., & Gagne, J. R. (2004). Night and day: Are siblings as different in

temperament as parents say they are? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 698–706. Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19. Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype to

environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435. Schore, A. N. (2001). The effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 7–66. Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2006). Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1152–1165. Seifer, R., Sameroff, A., Dickstein, S., Schiller, M., & Hayden, L. C. (2004). Your own children are

special: Clues to the sources of reporting bias in temperament assessments. Infant Behavior & Development, 27, 323–341.

Shannon, J. D., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Cabrera, N. J. (2006). Fathering in infancy: Mutuality and stability between 8 and 16 months. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6, 167–188.

Shipley, B. A., Weiss, A., Der, G., Taylor, M. D., & Deary, I. J. (2007). Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-Year prospective cohort study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 923-931.

Sirignano, S. W., & Lachman, M. E. (1985). Personality change during the transition to parenthood: The role of perceived infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 21, 558–567.

Smits, D. J. M., & Boeck, P. D. (2006). From BIS/BAS to the Big Five. European Journal of Personality, 20, 255-270.

Snyder, J. (1991). Discipline as a mediator of the impact of maternal stress and mood on child conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 263–276.

Page 90: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

90

Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Fabes, R. A., Valiente, C., Shepard, S. A., Reiser, M., Losoya, S. H., & Guthrie, I. K. (2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to children's social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 6, 498-510.

Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin,134, 138-161.

Stifter, C. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2002). The effect of excessive crying on the development of emotion regulation. Infancy, 3, 133–152.

Stifter, C. A., Willoughby, M. T., & Towe-Goodman, N. (2008). Agree or agree to disagree? Assessing the convergence between parents and observers on infant temperament. Infant and Child Development, 17, 407-426.

Stranberg, T. E., Järvenpää, A.-L., Vanhanen, H., & McKeigue, P. M. (2001). Birth outcome in relation to licorice consumption during pregnancy. American Journal of Epidemiology, 153, 1085–1088.

Stright, A. D., Gallagher, K. C. Kelley, K. (2008). Infant temperament moderates relations between maternal parenting in early childhood and children's adjustment in first grade. Child Development, 79, 186-200.

Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & Armeli, S. (2005). Personality and daily experience revisited. Journal of Personality, 73, 1–19. Terracciano, A., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2006). Personality plasticity after age 30. Personality

and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 999–1009. Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Brant, L. J., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2005). Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Psychology and Aging,

20, 493–506. Uziel, L. (2006). The extraverted and the neurotic glasses are of different colors. Personality and

Individual Differences, 41, 745–754. Vaidya, J. G., Gray, E. K., & Haig, J. (2002). On the temporal stability of personality: Evidence for differential stability and the role of life experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1469–1484. Van Aken, M. A. G., Denissen, J. J. A., Branje, S. J. T., Dubas, J. S., & Goossens, L. (2006). Midlife

concerns and short-term personality change in middle adulthood. European Journal of Personality, 20, 497–513

van Brakel, A. M. L., & Muris, P. (2006). A brief scale for measuring 'Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar’ in children. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28, 79-84

Van Egeren, L. A. (2004). The Development of the coparenting relationship over the transition to parenthood. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 453–477.

Van den Boom, D. C. (1989). Neonatal irritability and the development of attachment. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 299–318). Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.

Van den Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers

with irritable infants. Child Development, 65, 1457–1477. Verhoeven, M., Junger, M., Van Aken, C., Dekovic, M., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2007). Parenting during

toddlerhood: Contributions of parental, contextual, and child characteristics. Journal of Family Issues, 28, 1663-1691.

Wachs, T. D. (1992). The nature of nurture. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Wachs, T. D. (1987). Specificity of environmental action as manifest in environmental correlates of infant’s mastery motivation. Developmental psychology, 23, 782–790. Wachs, T. D. (2006). The nature, etiology, and consequences of individual differences in

temperament. In L. Balter, & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (2nd ed., pp. 27–52). New York: Psychology Press.

Watson, D., Suls, J., & Haig, J. (2002). Global self-esteem in relation to structural models of personality and affectivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 185–197.

Widiger, T. A., Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A proposal for Axis II: Diagnosing personality disorders using the five-factor model. In: P. T. Jr.; Costa, & T. A. Widiger (Eds), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 431-456). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.

Worobey, J., & Blajda, V. M. (1989). Temperament ratings at 2 weeks, 2 months, and 1 year: Differential stability of activity and emotionality. Developmental Psychology, 25, 257–263.

Page 91: Child temperament and parental personality - Helda -

91

Yik, M. S. M., & Russell, J.A. (2001). Predicting the Big Two of affect from the Big Five of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 247-277.

Östberg, M., Hagekull, B., & Hagelin, E. (2007). Stability and prediction of parenting stress. Infant and Child Development, 16, 207-223.