choosing treatments - sdu/media/520a6be5587b4a...1 choosing treatments a ph.d. thesis on preference...

102
1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions on treatment of low back pain Mirja Elisabeth Kløjgaard University of Southern Denmark Faculty of Health Sciences 2014

Upload: others

Post on 25-Feb-2021

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

1

Choosing Treatments

A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with

applications to health care decisions on treatment of low back pain

Mirja Elisabeth Kløjgaard

University of Southern Denmark

Faculty of Health Sciences 2014

Page 2: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

2

Foreword

You’re a difficult equation with a knack for heart evasion

Will you listen to my proof or will you add another page in

It appears to me the graph has come and stolen all the laughs

It appears to me the pen has over analysed again

- Sia “Academia”

I’ve never really struggled deciphering agendas or motivations - but academia is something else.

It is a world of ancient cultural behavioural patterns, which for an outsider - or beginner insider - can

seem irrational and unpredictable. These towers of knowledge that we call universities, leaves its

occupants so dedicated, driven and focused that they forget to look out of the window unto the world

outside. And as the air gets thinner towards the top of the tower, some even forget why they started

climbing its stairs to begin with, and stops remembering the steepness of the lower floors and the many

desperate climbers.

My navigation in academia has been disturbed by trials and detours along the way, but I’ve managed to

find some invaluable guides on my sometimes vertical climb. My helpers have pointed me in the right

direction at the right time and I would have never made it this far without them.

Thanks have to be directed to my changing team of supervisors from whom I’ve learned different

aspects of how to navigate the academic world. To Rikke Søgaard for never hesitating to push me

forward onto the narrow path of good academic behaviour, always in the quest of nothing but results

and never weary in attempts to get them. To Jan Sørensen for teaching me valuable lessons on the ever-

changing faces of academia. And last, but so definitely not least, to Mickael Bech for staying until the

bitter end and for abilities in putting out fires without starting too many new ones.

Thanks should also be directed to Professors Benny Dahl from Rigshospitalet and Tom Bendix from

Glostrup Sygehus for letting me do fieldwork in their hospital wards. To Jens Olsen who was my boss for

Page 3: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

3

the majority of my employment as a PhD student, for supporting my ideas and especially for co-

financing the Danish Choice Modelling Days.

A special thank you goes out to Professor Claus Manniche and staff at Middelfart Spine Center for

helping me collect my quantitative data and be thorough and professional while doing so.

I’ve also been blessed with help and advice from some major external capacities and thanks should be

directed to them too.

To my superhero, Professor John Rose. For letting patience and teaching abilities to the test, while

helping me understand and achieve my potential. For helping me remember what’s important in life –

and to breathe, when I didn’t. For following me - for a change - coming all the way to Denmark to shine

with glory upon the Danish Choice Modelling Day(s). And for being the most childish, but best Aussie

“rock” star ever. I’m proud to be an appointment groupie.

To Professor Stephane Hess for not judging me on that first email to “Stephanie”. For helping to make

the first ever Danish Choice Modelling Day into more than a silly idea, and for knocking at least some

econometric knowledge into my utterly imperfect head. For listening to my worries and insecurities and

make them seem less important – or at least more normal. For caring. And for taking time; to slow down

to enable me to (try to) follow, to comment on my work, answer my emails, run models, write papers,

correct my calculations - and to have fun!

To Rob Sheldon for sharing his taxi and offering me a stay at his company Accent Marketing Research

following that. To everyone else at Accent MR for making that stay a success and educational and fun!

Finally, a heartfelt thank you goes out to my colleague – and partner in crime - Line Bjørnskov Pedersen.

Had we not been sitting on that quay in Venice, suffered through the British summer, shared those

magic days and fractured bones at Disney World, eaten strawberries like supermodels, lived in style

under the Harbour Bridge or simply spent all those hours discussing, I would have never been able to be

where I am now. Thank you for never doubting me and always reminding me of what I’m good at. For

picking me up from the deep black holes and bringing me back down to earth when I flew too high.

Thank you for climbing the stairs with me, overcoming the biggest obstacles, yet still stand beside me.

Page 4: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

4

The sprint to the finish line was made so much easier with the support of all my cheerleaders of family

and friends, led on by a very special man, who always seems to surprise me with his capabilities. My

journey must have seemed beyond imaginable, but I appreciate the honest attempts of trying to

understand and all the help which has been given me. Whatever form it took - free meals, hour-long

phone conversations, bottles of wine or hugs when needed – the support is remembered and cherished.

Having the ability and possibility to aspire for a high-level of education and knowledge is a more rare

combination that what we might think having already achieved it. I often think of my luck having been

supported while taking my skills to the highest possible level. I don’t owe my accomplishments to rich

parents or extraordinary talent. I owe them to a system in which I deeply belief. I hope to someday

somehow return my debt.

All those thank you’s and deep thoughts aside, all there is left is to offer happy reading.

Page 5: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

5

Index

Foreword ....................................................................................................................................................... 2

1 Introduction ............................................................................................................................................... 7

1.1 Motivations ......................................................................................................................................... 7

1.2 Research questions ............................................................................................................................. 8

1.3 About back pain ................................................................................................................................ 10

1.3.1 Definitions .................................................................................................................................. 10

1.3.2 Low Back Pain (LBP) Patients ..................................................................................................... 11

1.3.3 Pathogenesis and treatment ...................................................................................................... 11

1.3.4 Danish Guidelines and policy-movements ................................................................................. 12

1.3.5 Patient pathways and decision making in a Danish context ...................................................... 13

1.4 About preference elicitation in health economics............................................................................ 13

1.5 Contributions of this thesis ............................................................................................................... 15

1.5.1 Thesis outline, detailed contributions and relation between background, empirical work and

discussions .......................................................................................................................................... 16

2 Methods and existing knowledge of preferences and LBP ...................................................................... 18

2.1 Stated Preferences ............................................................................................................................ 19

2.2 Preferences for treatment of Low Back Pain .................................................................................... 21

2.2.1 Patients’ preferences for treatment of LBP ............................................................................... 24

2.2.2 Patients’ preferences for shared decision making and HCPs characteristics or service delivery

............................................................................................................................................................ 25

2.2.3 Patients’ preferences’ effects on treatment outcome .............................................................. 26

2.2.4 Conclusions from literature ....................................................................................................... 27

2.3 Methodology ..................................................................................................................................... 28

2.3.1 Theoretical base for choice modelling ....................................................................................... 28

2.3.2 Designing a stated preference choice experiment .................................................................... 30

2.3.3 Statistical analysis ...................................................................................................................... 37

3 Empirical work.......................................................................................................................................... 41

3.1 Formulating the research question ................................................................................................... 41

3.2. Selecting attributes and levels ......................................................................................................... 41

3.3 Construction of tasks ........................................................................................................................ 43

3.3 Experimental Design ......................................................................................................................... 45

Page 6: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

6

3.3.1 Final experimental design .......................................................................................................... 45

3.4 Preference elicitation and instrument design .................................................................................. 46

3.5 Collection of data .............................................................................................................................. 46

3.6 Statistical Analysis ............................................................................................................................. 47

4 Designing a stated choice experiment. The value of a qualitative process ............................................. 48

5 Patients’ preference for treatment of low back pain – a discrete choice experiment .......................... 101

6 Understanding the formation and influence of attitudes in patients’ treatment choices for lower back

pain: testing the benefits of a hybrid choice model approach ................................................................. 101

7 Discussion and future perspectives ....................................................................................................... 102

7.1 Results and existing knowledge ...................................................................................................... 103

7.1.1 Limitations ................................................................................................................................ 103

7.2 Policy implications .......................................................................................................................... 108

7.2.1 Putting preferences to practice ............................................................................................... 108

7.3 The patient and the medical experts .............................................................................................. 109

7.3.1 Who is sharing decision-making? ............................................................................................. 111

7.4 Methodological issues .................................................................................................................... 113

7.4.1 Are we utility-maximising?....................................................................................................... 113

7.4.2 Heterogeneity .......................................................................................................................... 114

7.5 Concluding remarks ........................................................................................................................ 115

8 Conclusion .............................................................................................................................................. 116

9 Summary ................................................................................................................................................ 118

10 Dansk Sammenfatning ......................................................................................................................... 124

11 References for sections 1, 2, 3 and 7 ................................................................................................... 128

Page 7: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

7

1 Introduction

This thesis is a collection of journal manuscripts and an independent overview that adds to the

clarification of the issues dealt with throughout the PhD studies and sets a frame for the used methods.

The independent sections surrounding the included papers, includes a literature review of existing

knowledge on the topic of preferences for treatment of back pain as well as a critical discussion of the

gained results put into a context of novel research approaches in the field of modelling choices,

including suggestions of future research areas, naturally following the presented results.

This section begins with underlining the initial motivations for the field of study. It further introduces the

context of the health issue of back pain and clarifies objectives of the thesis and of the papers presented

in later sections.

1.1 Motivations

Back pain is major problem in most of the western world. According to the Danish National Health

Interview Survey, more than 35 % of the adult population suffer from back pain of different severity and

length 1. It is the third most reported deficit and is one of the most common reasons for absenteeism or

early retirement. Not only is back pain a major issue as it is now, there’s a tendency of increasing

prevalence and impact. Although most patients are treated outside the health care system a substantial

amount of patients are referred from general practitioners (GPs) to the hospital sector. This picture is

not only seen in Denmark, but is common throughout the western world.

A series of studies in- and outside Denmark has shown substantial costs associated with back pain for

patients, health care systems and society 2,3. Studies also indicate that treatment of back pain is

influenced by a range of challenges. Firstly, conflicting evidence with no clear indication of optimal

treatments has left health care practitioners (HCPs) with difficult choices. This has also been a factor in

observed variation in treatment strategies throughout the (western) world. Possibly as a result of the

lack of clear evidence-based guidance, perceptions of and attitudes toward patients and their disease as

well as the feasibility of treatments, has been shown to impact HCPs’ decision-making 4–14. The typically

long patient pathways involving numerous health practitioners, possibly affects decision making and

agency-relationships by adding to patients’ perceptions of optimal treatments and potential outcomes.

Secondly, it has been argued that patients’ perceptions and preferences can impact treatment outcomes

and should be incorporated systematically into decision making in the case of back pain – and possibly in

Page 8: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

8

similar cases – as preferences can help guide shared decision making ensuring better results. But to be

able to do so, knowledge on patients’ preferences is vital 15,16.

This Ph.D. study provides knowledge on preferences and drivers of preferences by using state-of-the-art

methods and by investigating the area of interest thoroughly and with novel approaches. It focuses on

eliciting not only patients’ preferences for treatment of back pain with help of both qualitative and

quantitative methods, but also looks into how perceptions and attitudes affect treatment choices. The

studies presented in the thesis give basis for both methodological developments as well as practice-near

development and help to fill gaps of knowledge.

Firstly, a series of introductory sections and the research questions and the scope of contribution are

presented. The remainder of this chapter is organised as follows: Section 1.2 clarifies and describes the

objectives of the Ph.D. study and hence this thesis. Section 1.3 further motivate and elaborate on the

field of study in an international and Danish context and provides a range of definitions. Section 1.4

presents how preference elicitations and in particular stated preferences (SP) are being and have been

used in health economic, providing the background for section 1.5 dealing with the contributions of this

thesis, the links between the empirical contributions (sections 4,5 and 6) and how the empirical work fit

into the current practices of SP in health economics and gives an overview of the sections and outline of

the thesis.

1.2 Research questions

The objective of the PhD study has been two-fold. Firstly, it was the aim to explore the preferences of

patients with back pain when facing a choice of trade-offs between treatment options, outcomes and

risk. Secondly, it was an objective to add to the methodological work in the field of preference elicitation

by aiming at contributing with added focus on qualitative work prior to gathering quantitative data on

preferences and by focusing on the complex formation of preferences for patients.

More specifically the project aimed at:

1. Investigating patients’ preferences for treatment of low back pain (LBP), and

2. Contribute to the field of preference elicitation in health care in two distinct ways.

a. By exploring how qualitative work prior to designing stated preference experiments has an

impact on each step of the design process, and

Page 9: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

9

b. by exploring the importance of and the process by which attitudes and perceptions are

influencing the choices made by patients

The objectives of the PhD study are met in different ways. A study was performed to gain knowledge on

preferences to be able to include these in the decision-making processes about optimal treatment.

Doing so, is becoming more acceptable among doctors because knowledge about the patient’s general

expectations and preferences can guide the choice of treatment and, in some cases, even improve the

outcome of treatment as is described in section 2.2.3. Hence, quantifying preferences and exploring

trade-offs may be very helpful for patients and HCPs. The first research question is answered by a review

of literature presented in section 2.2 and empirically in section 4 and 5. The results are obtained using

stated preferences methods, which makes respondents state preferences by investigating their choices

of hypothetical, but realistic scenarios. The method is presented in section 2.1 and elaborated on in

section 2.3. Making investigations using stated choices requires solid work prior to quantitative data

gathering. This is agreed upon by researchers in the field, yet overlooked in literature 17. The work not

only helps ensuring a good and valid design, but also informs the experimental design by producing prior

knowledge on preferences, an approach widely used in the field, especially if sample sizes can be

expected to be low. The research question 2a. dealing with informing designs by qualitative work is

dealt with in sections 3 and 4. Existing knowledge on LBP patients and in stated preferences research

shows a great need for investigating differences in preferences in patients. These differences should not

only be acknowledged, but also investigated in regards to what drives them. This helps enlighten how

different individuals can be helped to receive the best possible and individualised treatment based on

the most informed prospect from both the patient and HCP. Modelling decisions in light of attitudinal

driving factors has yet to break through on the health economic scene, and the research question not

only provides interesting empirical knowledge on preferences, differences in preferences and factors

driving these, but also provides a highly usable and novel methodological contribution. The research

question 2b. is mainly dealt with in section 6, but also gave rise to a range of topics of discussion dealt

with in section 7.

Page 10: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

10

1.3 About back pain

Before introducing a detailed outline of the thesis, this section presents a range of definitions of some of

the terms used in this thesis and provides insight into the health issue of low back pain. This section also

shortly introduces the area of back pain in a Danish context. It introduces the group of patients,

international evidence for treatment and the Danish guidelines in the area. It also touches upon patient

pathways in Denmark and in presents a framework in which decisions on treatments can be seen.

1.3.1 Definitions

Back pain is an undefined term covering a range of different disorders. Suffering from back pain is not

always diagnosable which makes it hard to establish accurate measures of prevalence which is

completely dependent on the definition used. Hence a number of terms are used to characterize back

pain. With aging the spine changes and discs degenerates, which for the majority of people does not

cause any pain or problems. The changes in the spine are highly individual and degenerated discs can be

observed in young people and be without symptoms, while the same degenerations might cause severe

symptoms for others. One term used for back pain is thus degenerative disc disease, which is the term

used in the paper in section 4. However, as disc degeneration is not always present in patients with

lower back pain, non-specific low back pain is a broader term used to characterize all patients with pain

in the lower back that cannot be attributed to any recognisable known specific pathology i.e. infection,

tumour, osteoporosis, fractures, deformities etc. Hence this term more precisely characterise the

patients studied in this thesis, as a specific pathology other than normal degeneration was not present

in the patients. The change of term from section 4 to the following sections is simply a mirror of being

more precise in the spine-specific terminology.

A wide range of HCPs are dealing with the patients with lower back pain. These include doctors, like

neuro- and orthopaedic surgeons and rheumathologists as well as x-ray/MRI scan technicians etc. but it

also includes physiotherapists, masseuses, chiropractors, pain-coaches etc. Hence the term used to

characterize the group of therapists is HCPs.

When dealing with low back pain, which suffers from the imprecise terminology extrapolation of

research is also made difficult. This along with similar factors of challenges in defining patient groups,

exact content of (especially non-surgical) treatment, referral and care-taking differences and similar area

variation, makes comparison of studies very difficult 15. Hence the results presented in this thesis

primarily represent the setting in which they were collected and the patients who answered the

Page 11: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

11

questionnaires. Although this is a limitation of scope, it is no different from all other research performed

in this field.

1.3.2 Low Back Pain (LBP) Patients

Most people will experience low back pain in their lifetime. Although a lot of resources are spent within

the health care system on patients with back pain, most people suffering from back pains do not seek

assistance from doctors or other HCPs. Some evidence point to perceived disability as the main driver

for seeking care 18. Back pain patients come in all age groups with similar prevalence in children and

young adults as later on in life 19. But number of back pain related consultations rise with age 20,21.

Negative effects on ability to work and pursue everyday activities as well as limits to quality of life is

more commonly reported in adults and elderly people 22,23 and although many patients experience

recovery or periods of no symptoms, LBP is characterised by its recurring nature 24–26. Some literature

point to LBP severity being connected to socio-economic factors as higher ratings of pain severity and

disability is somewhat associated with employment status, self-rated health status and use of health

care services 27.

In Denmark, about 20 % of the adult population answer they suffer from back pain while 11 % in this

group claim to have actual spine disease 1. There’s a clear tendency of lower socio-economic status and

LBP 1 and a strong connection between low self-assessed health and LBP 28. People suffering from LBP

are more likely to be smoking and women 28. People who experience LBP have a substantial overuse of

health care resources compared to the normal population, especially concerning GP visits and hospital

treatments. An extensive use of out-of-pocket treatments from HCPs like chiropractors or

physiotherapists and prescriptive medicine is also detected. Further a production loss due to LBP-related

sick-leaves is estimated to be between 1.8-2.6 billion DKR/year – not including costs of pensions due to

LBP.

1.3.3 Pathogenesis and treatment

The exact factors causing acute low back pain remain unknown. Some patients have modifications or

transformations of their spine which could explain symptoms and literature has found an association

between degeneration of lumbar discs and pain 29,30, but as similar changes to the spine can be observed

in symptom-free persons it cannot be the full explanation 31. Some patients do not have apparent spinal

changes, and clinical examination often does not explain reported pains and effects 32.

Page 12: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

12

Some literature point to different genetics factors as possible explanations for back pain and disc

degeneration 33–39 but heredity is still widely discussed 40,41. Similarly lifestyle factors like smoking and

obesity have shown some connection to low back pain 42,43 while lifestyle factors related to working

position, lifting or carrying or other physical activities have not been proven to be causal to LBP 44–51.

Patients’ attitudes and beliefs toward LBP 52 or lack of appropriate coping 53 has however been

associated with chronicity and severity of impact of symptoms. Similarly studies have shown that HCPs

are also directed by beliefs and personal opinions when advising patients, which is also indicated as a

driver for surgery rates and the dominant variations of rates 9,54–56.

When deciding on what treatment to provide, patients with LBP is often scanned using MRI 57 despite

decisions not being taken based on scan results 58. Treatment is often commenced with some sort of

patient education on staying active etc. 59,60 along with pain-killers 59. This is followed up by cross-

disciplinary care, patient education and rehabilitation or sometimes surgery 15. The use of surgery in the

treatment of LBP has been widely discussed and rates are generally considered to be too high in most

settings 10,61. The critique of use of surgery in the treatment of LBP considers the lack of evidence

showing superiority of surgery compared to non-surgical interventions 62,63 but studies comparing the

two treatment modalities are often biased due to unclear selection criteria and substantial cross-over of

patients, which is in some cases caused by overrated perceptions of advantages of surgery among

patients 62,64,65.

1.3.4 Danish Guidelines and policy-movements

In Denmark, the Ministry of Health and the Danish Regions, who are in charge of hospital care, revised

the guidelines for treatment of back pain and spine disease in December 2010. The revision was

motivated by the increasing surgery rates and underlined a need for prioritization and improved referral

practices 66. The guidelines underlines that the unclear evidence and equal results upon 1-year follow-up

of surgical and non-surgical treatment options, makes choosing the right candidates for surgery central.

Indication for surgery is defined as a combination of s described and observed symptoms, anamneses

and MRI-scans. Even if the indication is positive, the guidelines underlines that all non-acute and no red-

flags patients need to test non-surgical options in 6-12 months before surgery should be performed 66.

Following the guideline revisions, the Regions committed themselves to ensuring at least a 3 month

non-surgical treatment of patients with low back pain. The diffusion of this treatment option has yet to

reach all Regions but the Region of Southern Denmark, in which the empirical data presented in this

thesis has been collected, had already formulated guidelines complying with the Ministries’67. The

Page 13: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

13

revised guidelines does not include considerations of patient preferences, but it is evident that an

essential shift in treatment pathways and practices calls for an investigation on how the patients and

HCPs involved make decisions on treatments and what preferences in relation to the decision-making

are at stake.

1.3.5 Patient pathways and decision making in a Danish context

In Denmark, patients with back pain generally have long pathways and try a range of different

treatments, often incurring substantial (out of pocket) costs. Patients can be referred to specialists or

hospital via their GP. They can also pursue a range of therapists like chiropractors, physiotherapists etc.

mostly covered out of pocket. If a patient has an additional, supplementary private health insurance,

they can sometimes skip the specialists or therapists or even the gatekeeping GP and receive treatment

on a private hospital or with a private specialist, who again can refer to any hospital. The patients who

serve as respondents in the studies included in this thesis were captured at a large, public spine clinic.

The patients had had long treatment pathways, but had not yet been treated at hospital level and were

unaware of hospital experts’ judgment on their disease and diagnosis. Hence, the patients had not

engaged in decision-making regarding treatment at a hospital yet.

1.4 About preference elicitation in health economics

SP is often used when where market choices are severely constrained by regulatory and institutional

factors, such as in health care 68. But since SP methods are used in a range of research fields which have

all used different terminologies, often for the same method or feature. Terms like conjoint

measurement, stated preferences, discrete choice-modelling, discrete choice conjoint analysis, paired

comparisons, choice experiments, pairwise choice or discrete choice experiments are all used for the

preference eliciting method used in this thesis. Throughout the thesis the term “stated preferences“ is

mostly used but the literature used to enlighten the methodology stems a wide range of fields of

research.

Studies of SP using choice modelling methods have increasingly won impact in health economic

research. The SP approach has grown rapidly in health economics in the past years and its usage is more

widespread than ever before, both geographically and with regards to the health related questions it

tries to answer 17.

A review by de Bekker Grob et al. 17 shows that the development in the use of SP in health economics

from 1990-2000 (baseline) compared to from 2001-2008 has been mostly in terms of geographical

Page 14: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

14

spread of researchers applying the methods, use of more and more advanced software packages

creating choice sets and more use of more advanced models investigating choices. The fields of

application has also grown from a more specific focus on patient’s experiences to where applications

include the valuation of labour-market choices for HCPs, utility weights for QALYs and clinical decision

making and priority-setting. The review also shows that designs and models are still fairly simple and

mostly doesn’t allow for differences in taste and focus solely on main effects.

Updating the search from de Bekker Grob et al.17 using the same search terms, same database and

limitations shows that the tendency of more applications is still true. The search in de Bekker Grob et al.

17yielded 682 possible references with 114 meeting inclusion criteria, while the updated search,

presented in table 1, gives 992 results with 279 references meeting inclusion criteria. Thus it seems fair

to conclude that the number of applications of SP-methods to health care is still rising.

Table 1. Literature search on SP in health care.

Search string in Medline Limitations Results

(((((((((discrete choice experiment) OR discrete choice experiments) OR discrete choice modelling) OR discrete choice modeling) OR stated preferences) OR stated preference) OR conjoint analysis) OR conjoint choice experiment) OR conjoint studies)

2008-2014 Humans

992 Sorted on abstracts (using inclusion criteria: Health care and not eg. Food or nutrition or traffic safety AND attribute-based) = 279

The updated search also show that the applications topics are still manifold and the work on e.g.

estimating utility weights for QALYs is still growing 69–78 as is the work focusing on HCPs preferences for

jobs, especially in rural settings and in Africa 79–84. Also, studies on priority-setting is still increasing 85–89

as is the health topics covered including diabetes 90–100, cancer 101–105 and end-of-life care 106–108.

More advanced models taking heterogeneity into account, especially mixed logit models, are more

commonly used 83,91,104,109–124 as is Latent Class models 75,125–144. Other methodological advantages are

also being explored more often, e.g. looking into different compensatory rules 145, number of choice sets

146 and cognitive ability to engage in SP-studies e.g. among mental ill 147 etc.

A range of studies state the use of qualitative methods to inform design 70,78,106,118,123,148–158.However, no

studies in the updated search are concerned with exploring how the qualitative work actually changes or

Page 15: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

15

guide the design and although 3 studies deal with the topic of back pain 159–161, one deal with scoliosis 161

one with lumbar surgery 159 and only one directly with LBP 160.

The empirical work in this thesis thus builds on what has become a growing field of research in health

economics and adds in depth knowledge of qualitative methods and how they can guide designs of SP-

studies, more practical and empirical knowledge of patient preferences for the major public health issue

of LBP and by taking the methodological development already happening to the next level in terms of

more advanced modelling.

The contributions of the thesis are presented in more depth in the following section.

1.5 Contributions of this thesis

This thesis makes several contributions to the field of modelling health care choices in general and

preferences in regard to treatment for low back pain in particular. As seen in the previous chapter the

use of SP-methods in health is growing and more effort is put into not only providing knowledge of

preferences, but also on methodological issues. This thesis already provided an overview of updated SP-

work in health and further provides a thorough and systematic review of the literature and existing

knowledge of the context in which the empirical work presented in thesis is part. Thus, it becomes

apparent how the contributions are part of a broader context while providing useful insights and novel

techniques. It builds on existing knowledge by adding an important layer and contribution to the

literature on preference for treatment of LPB and it extends the modelling capacity of future health care

researchers wanting to pursue looking into preferences by introducing the Integrated choice and latent

variable/hybrid models in health care SP-research and by exploring the potential benefits of this model.

The thesis builds it empirical work on the generic step-wise process by which SP-projects are usually

done, and focuses on the importance of qualitative work and how this informs designs and guide initial

steps in the process with great advantages. This is done in a cohesive process, where one step leads to

another - as is the case for the empirical chapters, with the first paper presenting the qualitative work

that went ahead of the design used to elicit preferences in the second and third paper, and with the

second paper looking into patients’ preferences for LBP with some depths, further elaborated upon in

the third paper, which also presents and discusses the benefits of highly novel modelling approaches.

Lastly this thesis discusses some of the present and forthcoming issues important for future work of

preferences in regards to LBP and SP in general.

Page 16: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

16

The next section presents the outline of the thesis and details on the contributions

1.5.1 Thesis outline, detailed contributions and relation between background, empirical

work and discussions

The thesis contains 10 sections. The first 3 sections deal with the background and methods while section

4-6 presents the empirical results in paper-formats. Finally section 7-10 contains discussion, conclusion

and a Danish and English summary.

Section 1 has the purpose of presenting the research area of interest and provides the reader with

thorough and systematic background knowledge to facilitate an understanding of the challenges of

treatment of low back pain and the context in which decision are made. It highlights the present and

historical use of studies on preferences in health care and the contributions of the thesis and

interdependency of the three empirical contributions.

Section 2 provides a systematic and thorough description of existing knowledge of preferences in

relation to LBP and thus helps to answer the research question of eliciting preferences for treatment of

low back pain, based on existing knowledge. It further describes how preferences are hypothesized to

influence or shape decision-making or even outcomes of treatments. Section 2 further provides a

deeper description of the stated-preference-elicitation method of choice modelling based on a step-

wise process of generating SP-studies and including an introduction to qualitative methods used in that

process.

Section 3 introduces empirical details on each step of the process, highlighting the use of qualitative

work that went into the process, and thoroughly describing all design measures taken in the empirical

work from the formulation of a research question to statistical analysis.

Section 4 starts the empirical contributions with the first paper presented in the thesis. The paper is

about the design of a stated preference or discrete choice experiment, which involves a process of

developing, testing and optimizing the experiment questionnaire. This process is important for the

success of the experiment and the validity of the results, but it is often not reported thoroughly. The

paper demonstrates how qualitative work can significantly impact and guide a design, making it clear

that a less thorough qualitative process would have resulted in a less useable and valid design. It reports

on fieldwork in clinical departments in Danish hospitals supplemented by qualitative interviews with

patients and doctors to determine relevant attributes and levels for the design.

Page 17: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

17

The paper provides a methodological contribution to the field of designing stated preference

experiments by answering the research question on how qualitative work prior to designing stated

preference experiments have an impact on each step of the design process.

Section 5 presents the examination of patients’ preferences for treatment of LBP. The paper build on

the design developed by the process described in sections 2, 3 and 4. It argues that since treatment

decisions are distorted by conflicting evidence the inclusion of patient preferences in decision- and

policy making should be emphasized. It builds on results from the DCE conducted across consecutive

patients referred to a regional spine centre. Respondents (n=348) were invited to respond to 6 choice

tasks each presenting two hypothetical treatment options and an opt-out option. Treatment attributes

included treatment modality, risk of relapse, pain reduction and increases in ability to perform activities

of daily living. In addition, waiting time to treatment effect was used as a payment vehicle. Analysis was

performed using Mixed Logit models. A sub-group analysis further explored the willingness to wait by

dividing respondents into socio-demographic and disease related categories.

As expected, results show that respondents assign positive utilities to positive treatment outcomes and

disutility to higher risks and longer wait for effects of treatment. Respondents also showed a disutility

towards surgical intervention and were on average willing to wait two years for effect of treatment to

avoid surgery, while they were willing to wait almost three years for effect of treatment, if the

treatment made them free of pain. The mixed logit model captured significant heterogeneity within the

sample for outcomes regarding pain reduction and ability to pursue activities of daily living and in

relation to treatment modality. The sub-group analysis showed differences in willingness to wait,

especially in regards to treatment modality and particularly in groups of high/low pain at time of data

collection and respondents with an ex-ante preference for surgery/non-surgery. The results indicated

that pain relief is the most important factor in choice of treatment, followed by treatment modality and

ability to perform activities of daily living while risk of relapse was the least important factor. This is not

only valuable information for practitioners in their communication with patients and everyday decision-

making, but also important information for policy makers who seek to ensure a utility-maximizing mix of

treatment options for patients. The section empirically answers the first research question by

investigating patients’ preferences for treatment of low back pain.

Section 6 presents the paper exploring the formation and influence of attitudes in patients’ treatment

choices for lower back pain and testing the benefits of a hybrid choice model approach, building on the

data used in section 5, generated from the design described in sections 2, 3 and 4.

Page 18: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

18

The paper argues that, possibly particularly in scenarios where clinical evidence is limited or not clear

cut, perceptions that patient forms, either through past experience or through discussion with other

patients and/or medical experts, will play a major role in shaping decisions. The paper thus investigates

the potential benefits in capturing the role that perceptions and attitudes may have in explaining

treatment choices made by patients by including a latent attitude in a joint model of the formation of

perceptions and of the choices. The findings from this model are then contrasted to structures allowing

for simple random heterogeneity.

The results show that while the hybrid structures provide some further insights into the formation of

attitudes, and some gains in efficiency, the overall results remain largely unaffected. Interestingly, the

commonly used socio-demographic variables did not prove to be of significant influence, while

recommendations of surgery from GPs or friends and family had a positive impact in simpler models,

while recommendations from friends and family are of even greater importance when looking at the

hybrid model. Results thus suggest that patients are significantly influenced by peers and less so by

professionals.

Section 7 provides a discussion of the results presented in relation to existing knowledge. It adds to the

discussions of limitations presented in the papers in sections 4-6 and further provides a discussion of the

validity of the stated choice method. The section also gives ideas to and discusses how results can be

put into practice, by formulating guidelines or developing decision-aids. Further, a discussion of the

relationship between patients and HCPs and on influences by peers on individuals’ decisions is

presented, by introducing novel approaches to thinking about choices, derived from literature outside of

health economics. The section ends by discussing alternative decision rules to standard choice modelling

theory and gives further insight to the methodological issue of heterogeneity.

Throughout the section, ideas for future research are presented.

Section 8 contains concluding remarks and finally section 9 contains a summary of the thesis in English

and section 10 a summary of the thesis in Danish.

2 Methods and existing knowledge of preferences and LBP

This section present the methodology primarily used in this PhD study. In section 2.1 it introduces stated

preferences and methods used to elicit preferences and their use in health care. Further an overview of

existing knowledge regarding preferences and LBP is provided in section 2.2. This overview is based on a

Page 19: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

19

systematic study of literature. Section 2.3 offers a description of the theory behind and methodology

relating to the use of choice modelling, highlighting efforts to capture and explain heterogeneity and

what to consider when generating SP-designs. Section 2.3 further introduces the step-wise process of

generating and analysing SP-studies, which is elaborated upon with empirical details in section 3.

2.1 Stated Preferences

We all make choices every day. What job to take, what partner to be with, where to live, what to eat,

what clothes to wear, what car to drive - to walk, bike, take the train or just stay home. All choices have

a range of possible outcomes or options. The goal of modelling choices is often to understand the

behavioural process behind choosing. In other words, to figure out why one option is chosen over

another, or several others. Modelling choices can also be used to compute welfare estimations or values

of willingness to pay. Regardless of the aim, the basic idea is that there are deterministic factors that

lead to or cause a choice being made. Some of these factors can be observed, and some of them are

hidden or unobservable.

Making a choice is impacted by individuals’ perception of the utility that the various alternatives have,

which can be broken down into the key characteristics an alternative consists of. Each alternative has

similar, but also different dimensions or attributes and each of these attributes can take on a range of

levels, and it is upon these that an individual base their comparisons or evaluations. Individuals weight

the different key attributes relative to another by comparing the level of quality or utility that an

attribute possess in a given alternative. Not only do individuals weight the utility of the attributes within

an alternative, they also trade-off levels of one attribute against levels of other attributes, implicitly

weighing and valuing both the attributes and the alternatives. These trade-offs lead to a development of

a preference for one alternative over another and ultimately results in that most preferred alternative to

be chosen.

It is assumed that any given individual makes choices based on a rational “calculation” of what gives

them the most utility (or least disutility), in other words chooses their most preferred option under

whatever constrains they might face.

When goods are bought and sold on a well-functioning, competitive market we can observe most of the

key attributes and the levels of these, directly and people reveal their preferences by their behaviour in

the market. Hence individuals’ preferences can be observed by observing buying and selling behaviour.

In many cases markets are biased by externalities or monopolies or information asymmetries or – as is

Page 20: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

20

the case of most health care functions in Denmark, including (most) treatment for LBP, regulation by a

public authority. The regulation means that the market does not function in a way that allows real

competition as the e.g. the number of providers is artificially decided upon and prices are not paid out of

pocket and are also artificially set. In addition the asymmetry of information between patients and their

HCPs further complicates the market for delivery of treatment of LBP. In markets that does not allow for

unbiased observations of choices or direct revealing of preferences, these can be elicited by using

revealed or stated preference methods.

The hypothetical nature of stated preference (SP) techniques has given rise to criticism. This issue is

further discussed in section 6.1.1.1. Having respondents subjectively evaluate different treatment

options or the like, instead of observing behaviour on actual markets, potentially creates a risk of

respondents making random choices as they have no incentive to be considerate in their choice

behaviour. Hence there’s a risk of imprecise welfare measures or biased willingness-to-pay (WTP)

estimates in a both upwards or downwards direction 162,163. However, as explained, SP methods can be

used where preferences cannot be observed or where no market is in place, evaluating future possible

goods and it allows for the estimation of choices in the presence of alternatives that do not exist to the

total value of both use and non-use, of a good. Some researchers claim that valuing WTP either way is

problematic when dealing with publicly provided health care as it is provided free of charge to the

patient or user. Instead of price some researchers in health economics are using “time” as a “payment”

attribute, which has been established as a valid and sensible approach 160,164–168.

Further, a particular advantage of SP is that it allows the researcher to control the variation in

respondents and attributes valued ensuring an efficient design 169 170. In regards to the research question

in this thesis, it quickly became apparent that using SP would enable the inclusion of some key-

attributes as reductions in pain and ability to perform everyday activities that would have been

unobservable in e.g. registers. Taken all of the above advantages and disadvantages into consideration,

SP was deemed to be able to fulfil this projects aim of investigating patients’ preferences for treatment

of low back pain and hence a choice experiment evaluating hypothetical treatment options was

generated.

Before getting into detail with the SP method this section provides an overview of what is already

known with regards to preferences in relation to treatment of low back pain and how preferences

affects treatment choices.

Page 21: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

21

2.2 Preferences for treatment of Low Back Pain

This section is built on a systematic study of existing literature.

The literature study had multiple purposes. It was used to inform the experimental design by given the

first hints towards possible attributes for the SP-study. Further, the purpose was to produce an overview

of existing knowledge on patients’ preferences in regards to multiple issues relating to treatments,

building the basis for evaluating gaps of knowledge and contributions to the field. Also, it shed light to

the context in which the empirical work was performed and investigated the effects that patients’

preferences might have.

A “preference” in a traditional SP way is a utility-measure based on trade-offs of other characteristics of

a good. In back pain literature a preference is not as clearly defined. A quick search in Medline using the

same search terms as in the search presented in table 1 in section 1.4 and pairing all the terms with “low

back pain”, resulted in only 40 hits, illustrating the lack of use of SP-studies in the field of low back pain

and the need to broaden the definition of a preference. Thus a new search strategy was made including

a much broader notion of a “preference” allowing studies on beliefs or expectations to be included and

aiming at finding studies looking into preferences for low back pain in a broader sense. This search

provided 602 results. The broad notion of preferences resulted in some noise in results and a rather

elaborate sorting process.

Table 2. Search strings in Medline

Initial search Final search

("Search (low back pain) AND ((((((((((((((((paired comparisons) OR paired comparison) OR pairwise choices) OR pairwise choice) OR stated preferences) OR stated preference) OR conjoint choice experiments) OR conjoint choice experiment) OR conjoint analysis) OR conjoint analysis) OR discrete choice model) OR discrete choice models) OR discrete choice modelling) OR discrete choice modelling) OR discrete choice experiments) OR discrete choice experiment)", 40)

("Search (((((((patient preference) OR patient preferences) OR patient belief) OR patient beliefs) OR patient expectation) OR patient expectations)) AND ""Low Back Pain""[Mesh]",602)

Page 22: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

22

Inclusion criteria were divided in 3 categories based on the purpose of the search, including enlightening

existing knowledge on patients’ preferences for treatments and factors related to treatment, shared

decision making and the connection between preferences and treatment outcome.

Table 3. In- and exclusion criteria

Inclusion category Number of papers included in category

1. Preferences (in the broadest of senses) for

different treatment options including both

surgical and non-surgical and factors influencing

preferences

11

2. Studies looking at preferences for shared

decision making and HCP characteristics/delivery

etc.

10

3. Patients’ preferences effects on treatment

outcomes

37 (of which 23 deal with fear-avoidance)

Total 58

Exclusion criteria Guideline adherence/development (not including

patients’ preferences), investigations of

treatments (not regarding preferences thereof),

questionnaire validation, development or testing,

non-back pain, investigations of risk-factors,

practioners’ preferences unrelated to patient

outcomes, screening, language, quality or delivery

of care

Sorting was done in two rounds. First round was based on title and abstract and the second round was

based on full text. The process is described in figure 1.

Page 23: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

23

Figure 1. Sorting of results. Figure is inspired by PRISMA 171

Only one study uses traditional stated preferences methods 160 while the majority of studies uses a

range of questionnaires based upon authors’ specific research question and a few uses qualitative

methods.

It is possible that the significant variation in study design and focus of the included literature is affecting

the conclusions drawn. Not all studies clearly define the objectives or comparators in focus, nor do they

define preferences. Also the method of validation of the surveys varies. But even if it varies greatly how

Records identified through database searching

(n = 602)

Scre

en

ing

Elig

ibili

ty

Ide

nti

fica

tio

n

Records after duplicates removed (n = 602)

Records screened (n = 602)

Records excluded (n = 472)

Full-text articles assessed for eligibility

(n = 130)

Full-text articles excluded, No full text available

(n =10) Measuring effects of

media campaign, questionnaire/guideline

development or adherence)

(n = 16) Language

(n=3) HCP’s

preferences/beliefs/knowledge

(n=24) Studies on quality or

delivery of care (n=17)

Not LBP (n=2)

Studies included in qualitative synthesis

(n =58 )

Page 24: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

24

the knowledge is obtained, researchers agree on the importance of studies looking into preferences,

whatever the definition, in this field 172.

2.2.1 Patients’ preferences for treatment of LBP

Only a few studies touch upon patients’ preferences for treatment of low back pain using different

methods and uncovering different issues.

Yi et al. 160 useds a discrete choice experiment to investigate patients’ preferences for a pain

management program for LBP patients. The study concludes that smaller group sizes and low intensity

programs over longer time spans increased patients’ desire to participate. The exact content or provider

mattered less to patients while travel time to clinic was of importance. The study detects some

differences in patients’ preferences based on patients’ stated severity of symptoms. Yi et al. 160 suggests

that patients’ preferences for treatment are influenced by perceived severity of symptoms.

Two studies directly asked patients ex-ante what sort of treatment they prefer 173,174. In the study by

George and Robinson 175, patients are asked to choose which of the following 3 treatment options they

prefer; physical therapy only (18 out of 105), graded activity (39 out of 105) or graded exposure (7 out of

105). Patients can also state no preference (39 out of 105). The study investigates how the delivery of

the preferred treatment affected perceived satisfaction, finding that the patients with no preference

had lower expectations towards effects and reported larger improvements and satisfaction, suggesting

that strong ex-ante preferences can lead to less satisfaction with actual treatment. This finding is

supported by the study by Donaldson et al. 174 where patients are asked to state a preference for either

thrust or nonthrust manipulation interventions. Here, patients stating no preference also gained the

most in terms of pain relief at the 6-month follow-up. However the number of patients stating no

preference was low (8 out of 149). The study also investigates the potential differences in outcome for

patients receiving preferred option compared with those not receiving preferred option, finding no

statistical differences.

A range of studies look into preferences in relation what is important for patients when making choices

176–179. The qualitative study by Slade et al. 176 looked into patients’ preferences for exercise programs

using 3 focus groups of adult chronic LBP-sufferers. Results point to range of issues regarding

preferences for exercise treatment, highlighting that patients are driven by previous experience and

preferring helpful and empowering skills in HCPs in interactions with patients. If HCPs behave as

preferred and exercise programs are designed with consideration to patients past experiences with

Page 25: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

25

exercise, patients are likely to participate in and benefit from such programs. Shaw et al. 178reports

results from a focus group of 23 individuals with LBP, showing that return to work and ability to pursue

everyday activity is of great matter to the informants, while Dean et al 179interviewing 9 LBP-patients,

show that patients are interested in integrating physiotherapy in everyday life, if it can be done in a

routine manner with limited time use. Finally the study by Liddle et al. 177 reports from three focus-

groups of 6 participants, suggest that patients prefer individually tailored exercise programs with

supervision and follow-up support.

These few studies looking into preferences for treatment are diverse in approach as well as topic. But

point to the importance of including patient preferences when designing or choosing treatments and

when formulating guidelines as stressed by Owens 172.

2.2.1.1 Factors influencing preferences

Three studies suggest how different factors influence patients’ preferences 180–182. Two of these studies

suggest that extended information to patients affects patients’ preferences leaving them more prepared

for self-management 182or generating better outcomes 180. Finally the study by George et al. 181 suggests

that patient education enhances patients’ ability to cope LBP. The three studies suggest that increasing

the communication to patients with LBP potentially shapes preferences and increases positive effects.

2.2.2 Patients’ preferences for shared decision making and HCPs characteristics or service

delivery

No studies on patients’ preferences for shared decision making was found, but a range of studies look

into patients’ preference for characteristics and involvement of an HCP 183–194.

Some of the studies measure patient preferences for the personalities, characteristics or abilities of

HCPs. Among these is a questionnaire-based study with 657 participants, investigating patients’

preferences for the skills or characteristics of HCPs, done amongst LBP patients choosing chiropractors

and physiotherapist by Bishop et al. 183 The study conclude that patients are concerned with perceived

technical abilities of HCP and that patients listens to and are guided by reputations of HCP. Farin et al.

184 report from another questionnaire based assessment, concluding that patients want trust and good

communication in their relationships with physicians and that the quality of which affects outcomes.

This is supported by Dima et al. 185 who reports from 13 focus groups, showing a preference for empathy

and expertise in HCPs treating LBP.

Page 26: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

26

The studies by McCarthy et al. and McIntosh & Shaw and Verbeek et al. 186,187,194 all conclude that

patients prefer as much information as possible in regards to diagnosis and treatment options, but

McIntosh and Shaw suggest that barriers for taken time to provide this information exists, leaving

patients to access information from other sources 187. This results becomes even more interesting when

looking into conclusions from May et al. who based on 12 interviews, suggest that when HCPs exhibit

doubt, patients listen less to advice and prefer to stress and follow own expertise 191.

Two studies look into patients’ preferences for involvement in treatment, both suggesting that patients

prefer HCPs to take the lead both when it comes to physiotherapy suggesting potentially less successful

outcomes from treatment if patients re not actively involved and perform self-management 192,193.

Finally the study by George & Hirsh 190 suggest that while patients preferences can be met in regards to

how physiotherapy is delivered and by whom that does not necessarily mean that patients’ are content

with results of treatment. Thus, the authors stress the need for distinguishing between these factors in

studies measuring preferences and outcomes.

2.2.3 Patients’ preferences’ effects on treatment outcome

Theories on the possible connection of patients’ beliefs and expectations and effects on treatment

outcomes has flourished in health research and studies looking into patient preferences and effects on

outcomes or “preference-effects” where found in the search showing inconsistent evidence. Many of

these studies revolves around be beliefs and not “true” preferences. In fact, a large proportion of

literature is concerned with fear and how exhibiting fear limit the potential outcome of any treatment.

This has been confirmed in a range of studies 195–212. However a few studies contradict the statement 213–

216.The literature on fear-avoidance is interesting on its own showing the potential for enhanced

communication from HCPs regarding patients’ fear, however, the literature is not concerned with

preferences and how they affect outcomes and will not be further discussed in this thesis.

One study directly measures preferences and impact on outcome. In the study by Sherman et al. 217

patients receive acupuncture as treatment and are asked in advance if that is a preferred treatment.

Analysis look into differences in effectiveness in regards ti disability, finding no difference in outcome for

patients preferring acupuncture compared to patients not preferring acupuncture. Another study by

Moffet el al. 218 uses a randomized controlled trial of exercise as treatment for LBP, controlling for

patients ex-ante preferences measured by asking directly if a patient preferred exercise. The

intervention group showed better outcomes but this was not influenced by preferences.

Page 27: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

27

A series of studies measure how patients’ feelings toward a treatment affect outcomes. Albeit different

settings and sizes all studies conclude that in patients feel strongly about a treatment and believe it will

work – outcomes are affected positively 219–225. Supporting this is results showing that patients ability to

live with pain, affects outcomes while expectations towards recovery and expected recovery time can

predict actual recovery time 226–228. Further, patients perceived credibility of and confidence in

treatment also affects outcomes 229,230.

It could be hypothesized that research addressing these potential preference-effects could suffer from

measurement bias as patient preferences as well as treatment outcomes are measured very differently

in the various trials. Also, a study by Oliveira et al. 231, find that patients with LBP need to see great

improvements in order to think non-surgical treatment is worthwhile, suggesting that if outcomes are

measured subjectively results might vary greatly from more objective measures. Further, the effect of

preferences, could potentially set in earlier than what is captured in these studies, regulating treatment

pathways, keeping some patients from ever using the health care system or ever getting referred to

different treatment options 220.

2.2.4 Conclusions from literature

The literature review point to a field acknowledging the fact that somehow patients’ preferences,

expectations and beliefs matter and can affect treatment outcomes.

Only one study uses traditional preference elicitation methods while the majority simply asks patients to

state their preference. When choosing treatments patients are influenced by previous experience and

the level of information can shape preferences.

Patients seem to prefer skilled and experienced HCPs who take them seriously and communicate clearly.

If HCPs exhibit uncertainty this can make patients lose faith and find information elsewhere.

Finally, it seems clear that somehow patients’ abilities, beliefs, expectations, trust and preference can

indeed affect their treatment outcomes, bearing in mind the possible biases from these studies.

Albeit the existing literature pointing to some issues (e.g. ability to pursue everyday activities) to

concern in an stated choice experiment investigating treatment preferences, a clear understanding of

driving factors is lacking. Further, it is clear that a gap of knowledge exists in regards to trade-off based

preferences for different treatment options and factors driving these. As a result the literature study

cannot be deemed to be sufficient to generate a SP-design measuring treatment preferences. Although

Page 28: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

28

some of the uncovered issues were included in questionnaire. As stated in introduction research in LBP

is highly context specific and further work was therefore performed to inform and test the design used

in the empirical work. This is further discussed in sections 2.3.2 and 3 while the results of the empirical

work in regards to existing knowledge is discussed in section 7.1.

2.3 Methodology

As should now be clear, modelling choices or getting people to state preferences by observing

hypothetical choices is a valid and well-established way of obtaining useful knowledge about peoples’

motivations and wishes. Also, it has been made clear that in the field of LBP, preferences of patients’ can

have effects, not only with regards to decision-making or treatment choices but also with regards to

treatment outcomes.

This section describes the theories behind choice modelling in more detail and serves as the backdrop

for understanding the following sections which are concerned with issues in relation to designing a

choice experiment and modelling results.

2.3.1 Theoretical base for choice modelling

As touched upon in section 2.1 the theoretical base for modelling choices is based on theories of how

choices are made, what yields utility and how it can measured by combining observed components with

an unknown component and how a good or a choice can be described based on its characteristics or

components.

This section briefly introduces the basic theoretical concepts and introduces some basic notations which

will be elaborated on in section 2.3.3.

In a the simplest of choice experiments the respondent is asked to choose a single alternative from a set

of choices made from mutually exclusive alternatives 232. The choice set mirrors the complete or

exhaustive choices and the ordering of the alternatives has no effect on the respondents decision-

making. Each alternative is characterised by a sum of utility, specific to each respondent as the

respondents vary due to their characteristics.

The most vital assumption in choice experimentation is that of utility maximisation, deeming that a

respondent will only choose one alternative i over alternative j, if Ui>Uj.

Page 29: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

29

If the true utility of a choice is called U and the factors observable to the researcher are given by V while

the unobserved factors are labeled Ɛ, we can express the behavioural process of choosing an alternative

(i), by a given individual (n), by a function of;

𝑈𝑖𝑛 = 𝑉𝑖𝑛 + 𝜀𝑖𝑛

The above equation is based upon the theory of Random Utility (RUT) which forms the basis of several

models and theories of consumer judgment and decision making 233–235as well as and the Lancastrian

theory of consumerism 236 which states that which proposes that utilities for goods can be decomposed

into separable utilities for their characteristics or attributes.

The function expresses utility as a function of known and unknown parts. What is known or observable

is the attributes (V) of a given alternative as well as the covariates such as sociodemographic variables or

taste of respondents. Hence, the researcher observes the attributes of an alternative and often also a

range of characteristics of the respondent, but not the total utility.

The observable part is defined as Vin = f (βn, xi,n, zn), where xi,n is the vector of measurable attributes of

alternative i as shown to respondent n and βn is a vector of parameters representing the taste of a

respondent n, which can be estimated from data and zn is a vector of sociodemographic variables. The

quality of how well the observable or systematic component of utility is estimated or identified depends

on how well the researcher succeeds in including the right attributes that influence choice. It is

therefore of great importance that researchers devote time and resources in advance of data collection

and analysis to focus on designing the best possible choice experiment including the right attributes.

This is dealt with in sections 3 and 4.

The unobservable part of the utility function, represented by the random component (Ɛ), expresses

omitted variables or measurement errors etc. The inclusion of this unobservable utility means that a

respondent’s choice is not deterministic and cannot be predicted precisely. Thus, the choice process

becomes probabilistic, in such a way that the alternative with the highest observed utility has the

highest probability of being chosen, which can be expressed as;

𝑃𝑖𝑛 = 𝑃(𝑈𝑖𝑛 = max𝑗𝜖𝐶𝑛

𝑈𝑖𝑛 ) = 𝑃( 𝑈𝑖𝑛 > 𝑈𝑗𝑛 ) = 𝑃( 𝑉𝑖𝑛 − 𝑉𝑗𝑛 > 𝜀𝑗𝑛 − 𝜀𝑖𝑛 )

Page 30: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

30

Again, under the assumption that j≠i and Cn = choice setn

As seen from the equation above the probability of i being chosen over j is given by the probability of

the total utility of alternative i is bigger than the total utility from alternative j. Hence, the probability of i

being chosen over j is given by the probability of the difference between the random, unobserved

components Ɛjn,in being smaller than the difference between the systematic, observed components Vjn,in..

As it is irrelevant to a respondents’ choice, it is not the magnitude or absolute level of utility that is being

measured, but only differences in total utility, which allows for constants to be added to the utility of

each alternative without changing the decision rule.

The researcher also has to specify how the observed variables combine to drive systematic preferences.

Thus, the researcher must decide upon the formal relationship between the choices made by the

respondent and the included explanatory factors. The majority of the work in the area of choice

modelling uses a linear formulation, where 𝑉𝑖𝑛 = βn’xin. Often a constant is added to this specification to

capture the effect of any utility not included in the model so that 𝑉𝑖𝑛 = βn’xin + ci , where ci is specific for

alternative i. Since again only differences in the alternative specific constants matter, one constant is

often set to zero and all other constants are thus interpreted as the effect of any omitted factors relative

to the constant normalised to zero 169,232.

In the analysis of SP data a model is developed to explain the observed choices by estimating the

parameters and making assumptions about the unobserved, random parameters. This is explained and

discussed in more detail in section 2.3.3, whereas the following section will introduce and discuss the

process of designing a stated choice experiment.

2.3.2 Designing a stated preference choice experiment

Making a stated choice experiment can be described as a stepwise process from the formulation of a

research question to the presentation of the study results. This process is shown in figure 2 adapted

from ISPOR taskforce on Conjoint Analysis in Health Care 68.

This section will provide an overview of the design process from the start to the point where data has

been collected, including a thorough description and theoretical background of the overlooked

qualitative work that can help inform the first steps of the process and that are described in detail and

discussed in section 4, which is the first journal paper included in the thesis. This section is followed by a

description of the last steps in the process, namely the statistical analysis. This section provides the

theoretical basis for the stated choice experiments that have been designed and used in the journal

Page 31: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

31

papers included in this thesis. The detailed process of the empirical work is described in section 3 and

discussed in section 7.1.1.

As seen in figure 2 a range of steps is required before an actual collection of data can begin. This process

is best informed by a triangulation of qualitative methods and quantitative testing. The goal of the

process prior to collecting data is to identify how individuals think about the evaluative process of the

problem one wants to investigate, i.e. what characterizes the choice situation, what attributes the

choice consists of and what factors to include in the components of the utility function. In the stepwise

design process, the selection of the components that are to be included in the experiment is the core

task. As introduced in section 2.1 the choice situation can be described in terms of its attributes and

levels, and selecting these, obviously requires careful consideration and testing. This process is often not

reported in detail in papers concerning results from stated choice experiments 17, but has been

dedicated some focus in research.

Figure 2. Step-wise process of SP-studies

Page 32: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

32

The following sub-sections will provide an introduction to qualitative methods and their relevance for

stated preferences

2.3.2.1 Qualitative Methods in stated preference research

Qualitative research methods have a well-reported use in health services research and health research

aimed at patient or user experiences. However, it is not as common a practice in health economic

research 17. As qualitative methods works particularly well when the aim is to get an insight into people’s

experience or perceived value or understanding of e.g. a treatment choice, some researchers have

dedicated more focus to these methods in relation to designing meaningful and valid SP-experiments

237,238.

Qualitative methods include observations and field-work as well as interviews of groups or individuals.

In health research all of these methods are used. Generally qualitative methods are used in a more

inductive way and a not aimed at generalizability but at painting a fuller picture of the subject of

interest. Thus, the case selection is different from quantitative methods as representativity is less

central than choosing respondents with important stories or knowledge.

Observations are used to understand contexts or interactions. The observer blends into the background

of the situation he/she is observing and observes the situation unfold naturally. This can provide

important knowledge on eg. a doctor/patient relationship or shared decision making. Being part of a

situation and not disturbing or changing it, is an important issue to consider, when performing

observations239.

Interviews of groups or individuals are used to better understand peoples’ motivations, understandings,

thoughts and stories. Unlike observations the interviewer is a part of the conversation and has an active

role in the data collection process. Thus, the interviewer needs to guide the interview without letting

personal opinions disturbing the process and while insuring a feeling of trust and safety with the

respondent and insuring that the respondent answers all important questions. Interviews are of the

semi-structured meaning that the interviewer knows the subjects he/she wants to touch upon, but also

lets the respondents’ information and personality guide the interview. The topics are often formulated

in an interview guide. Interview guides and questions need to be formulated with the respondent in

mind. Patients can be in pain or otherwise distracted so interviews should be short and contain easy

language, while professionals are able to answer more specific and difficult questions.

Page 33: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

33

When an interview has been completed a short protocol is formulated with a description of the

interview. Taped interviews are transcribed and analysed either theory-based (does the respondent

confirm a certain theory) or more explorative, philosophical ways (how is a phenomena talked

about/understood, how is the “world” of the respondent) 239.

In SP it is generally agreed upon that attributes and levels are best determined through some sort of

qualitative work and that this work is of great importance to the validity and quality of design.

However, while almost everyone uses this kind of work to determine attributes, level selection and

pre-testing of whole questionnaires, the qualitative process is only reported in one third of the

applied literature 17. It remains unclear whether this affects the validity of the experiments. And very

little research has been done into how qualitative processes lead to SP-designs or help inform design

processes.

The specific uses of the presented methods in this section is further elaborated on in section 3,

presenting the empirical work used in the papers in this thesis, while qualitative methods and their use

in SP is described and discussed thoroughly in section 4.

The following sections describe the next step of the design process.

2.3.2.2 Construction of choice tasks

Aside from the process and importance of choosing the correct attributes and levels, the researcher

needs to decide upon a range of issues which could possibly affect the outcome of the study and the

potential of the data to answer research aims. These issues include factors concerning the

administration of the survey, what questions to include beside the choice experiment, how to frame the

questions and what experimental design to use etc.

The construction of choice tasks includes consideration in regard to is each choice task should include all

or only some of the chosen attributes and how the choices are made – e.g. ranking, rating or choosing

best/worst out of a number of choice tasks. Traditionally the full number of attributes is included in

health economic stated preference studies 68 and respondents are asked to choose the alternative they

prefer. However, if the ultimate goal of choice experiment is to mimic the real world as closely as

possible, it is often more realistic to include an option of choosing not to choose in the experiment.

Choosing not to choose any of the presented alternatives can either be presented as choosing noting or

as choosing the current state a respondent is in. The two options yield different interpretations, as

choosing a current state often mean that the respondents status quo differs and should be interpreted

Page 34: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

34

as such. Including an option to opt out however, can more easily be seen as a universal alternative,

equal for all respondents. However, the choice not to choose or to choose the current state is still

debated in literature and object to research 240. It has been established though that including a no-

choice option remarkably changes results 241.

In general, one should design stated choice experiments to allow for observation and modelling of no-

choice, whenever it is an obvious element of real market behaviour.

2.3.2.2.1 Label or no label? Adding labels to the choice scenarios means branding or naming the overall alternative. Labels are not

the same as attributes and does not vary or take on levels. A label captures a respondents feeling

towards a brand or name and hence avoids biasing the trade-offs between attribute levels. If labels

makes sense to the market situation in question and adds realism to the experiment it is advised to use

them. Using labels can require fixing some attributes or parameters to one labelled alternative if the

attribute is linked to the label 242. If all attributes are generic and the experiment evaluates different

configurations of a single alternative, labels might be biasing results as respondents might be unwilling

to make trade-offs and only focuses on the labels 243. As with the rest of the design process, it is

important that the researcher tests how labels/no labels are perceived by respondents.

2.3.2.3 The experimental design

The experimental design combines the attribute levels into hypothetical scenarios. This can be done in a

range of different ways, each with different advantages and disadvantages. The experimental design is

essentially the task of finding the best way to combine chosen attributes and levels in an efficient way,

ensuring a minimum number of choice sets and maximum information on preferences pr. choice made.

To date, orthogonal designs remain the most commonly used. Othogonality of an experimental design

refers to the correlation structure of the included attribute. A design is said to be orthogonal if all

attribute correlations is zero. Practically, this means that all attribute levels, are evenly spread over all

choice tasks and that combinations of levels does not show a correlated or positive/negative pattern.

Orthogonal designs has gained popularity within the all fields of research using stated choice

experiments, but lately the method has been challenged by the so-called efficient designs. Early on,

orthogonal designs were questioned as the best option for creating realistic choice scenarios 244 and

experimental designs began evolving, trying to suit the non-linear models often used for analysis in

stated choice experiments245–247. Since then, experimental design has evolved even further, especially in

Page 35: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

35

regards to D-optimality or D-efficiency, which minimizes the determinant of the asymptotic variance-

covariance matrix of models, which minimizes the standard errors and thus maximizes reliability of

parameters estimated in models. D-optimal designs tries to maximize the information on tastes or

preferences obtained from each choice made by a respondent over all observations 248–258. D-optimal

designs have been proposed in a number of different ways, each with different assumptions as to how

to estimate what the variance-covariance matrix might look like, without having collected data.

Assumptions about a priori information also differs, and can be zero, fixed or unfixed values and even

uniform or normal distributions (in principal priors can take all sorts of distributions, but the Ngene-

software in which efficient designs are generated only allows for the two – so far). If distributions

around the prior parameters are assumed, a Bayesian approach is taken and the experimental design is

evaluated over a number of different draws. This simulation process can use different draws, with

Halton draws being the most common 242,259.Some researchers have suggested optimizing other

measures derived from the variance-covariance matrix, generating A- or S-efficient designs, considering

the trace of the variance-covariance matrix or the maximum t-ratio246,260.

When an experimental design has been found, a number of choice sets are generated. Different

researchers have interested themselves in the number of choice sets a respondent can manage and how

the number of choices affect preferences 261–264. In general, somewhere between 6-18 choice sets pr.

respondent is acceptable146,164, but this is clearly dependent on the characteristics of the respondent.

The number of choice sets found in the chosen experimental design might not fit with the number

manageable to the respondents; hence blocking the choice sets intro groups can solve the issue. In

efficient designs this is done based on minimum correlation between the blocking column and the

attributes columns.

2.3.2.4 Preference elicitation

Another issue to consider is how to best present the choice experiment within the survey. This is

especially important as most research is taking place without an interviewer to help explain the choice

tasks. The information about alternatives can attributes can be both written and/or visual. In general,

the more realistic a choice scenario is the better. This not only holds true with the choice of attribute

and levels, but also with how a choice scenario is depicted. If only text is used, this can be done in many

ways; bullet points, long explanations, one worded-attributes etc. Describing choice scenarios and

attributes/levels is a trade-off between accuracy and the cognitive burden for the respondent. Recently,

the easy access to graphics and multimedia has created a trend of including these in the choice

Page 36: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

36

scenarios under the assumption that pictures speak louder than words and are less subject to

interpretation 265. Again, the feasibility of using graphics is dependent on the specific experiment, the

survey mode and costs. The use of graphics was not tested for the survey designed for the data

collection for the empirical parts, as none of the attributes in the design were easily translatable into

pictures.

Similar to how to present the choice scenario, is how to frame the actual context of choosing. Framing

refers to the way in which the choice scenario is presented and what information is given to the

respondent before he/she makes choices. Framing has been shown to affect how respondents make

choices and hence the parameters of the utility function. However it is much less clear if/how framing is

only influencing the random, error component, or if it has actual effects on tastes. Some research

advocates that the latter is not the case 266–268. However, impacting the error term variance does impact

outcome, as a scale impact on the random component, also affects choice probabilities. Thus, framing is

impacting outputs, but it has yet to be shown how of if this affects measures of welfare.

2.3.2.5 Instrument design and data collection

Choice experiments are almost always part of a longer survey. Thus, researchers should not only take

the cognitive burden of the choice experiment itself into account, but see this as a part of longer and

possibly challenging survey. Hence the questions in the survey should be considered with care and help

enlighten important issues about the respondent and at the same time gather information possibly

affecting choices, while describing and presenting the attributes and the choice experimental context.

Some researchers have been concerned with the effects on results, possibly caused by the method by

which the survey is administered. In general, it is acknowledged that the survey mode can have an effect

on e.g. response rate or response quality and gathering data is usually time consuming and expensive. It

has been shown that verbal representation of a choice experiment facilitates the choice process of the

respondents, but interviewer-based data gathering in choice experiments is not among the most

commonly used methods. Possibly due to expenses 17. The internet has gained popularity as a quick a

cheap way to contact a large number of respondents, but it has been questioned if the quality of data is

up to standard. When comparing the validity of data gathered on the internet, via the phone or by

interviewers, researchers have not found any substantial differences in validity on a range of parameters

269–271. It is generally considered to be most important to choose the survey mode, most suitable for the

target group of interest, and if the general population is not it, an internet survey might not be most

Page 37: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

37

feasible way. In health, it has been found that the most suitable, cheap way of surveying groups of

patients is by handing out paper questionnaires in hospital settings 272.

Besides survey mode and content, sampling strategies and ethical considerations are of importance

when designing the data collection. These issues and all of the issues discussed in the above section and

sub-sections, are further discussed in an empirical context in section 3.

The next sections deal with the statistical analysis of choice data.

2.3.3 Statistical analysis

When a design has been settled upon and data collected, it is time for analysis. A range of models are

used in analysing stated choice data. Recently a lot of work has gone into finding ways to model and

explain both scale and preference heterogeneity within respondents. This section will present the

simplest model and more advanced models taking taste heterogeneity into account and serves as a basis

for understanding the model used in section 4 and 5. Section 5 also provides a presentation of one of

the most advanced models, looking into heterogeneity.

The multinomial logit model (MNL) is the simplest model in the family of models used to analyse stated

choice data. Vital to the MNL model is the assumption of “independent and identically distributed

random utility”, as defined by Manski in 1977 235.This assumption of the distribution of the error-term

prevents taking correlation in errors across alternatives or observations, but for the same respondent,

into account – otherwise known as panel-effects. Also, the assumption of identically distributed errors

prevents a treatment of taste heterogeneity across respondents 169. Further, it has been showed that

the MNL model relies on the assumption of independence of irrelevant alternatives (IIA). This means

that in the MNL the probabilities of two different alternatives are independent of the existence of other

alternatives and of the attributes in the included alternatives. Thus, any change in probability of one

alternative being chosen, draw equally from probabilities of the collected included alternatives 232,273.

While the IIA assumption can proof to be realistic it has its clear limitations and it is important to test

the validity of the assumption, as suggested by Hausman & Wise 274, to avoid fitting a MNL model when

choice sets violate the IIA assumption, as this would lead to misleading results 275. However, most

researchers fit MNL models to their data as a point of departure for more advanced models to

investigate how the data fits under the most constrained options. Also MNL remain the most common

model for comparison when looking into how preferences can be explained by including allowing for e.g.

taste heterogeneity. But the limitations of MNL have led to the development of a range of models that

Page 38: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

38

loosens the IIA assumption and takes panel-effects into account. One such model is the mixed

multinomial logit model (MMNL), which has been highly successful in the stated choice literature in

health economics in the recent years 276.

The MMNL model overcomes the limitations of the MNL model by loosening the IIA assumption

allowing for panel-effects, heteroscedasticity and maybe most importantly taste heterogeneity. This is

obtained by calculating the choice probabilities as an integral of MNL choice probabilities over an

assumed distribution of random terms. The utility function of a MMNL model is thus given by;

𝑈𝑖𝑛 = 𝑉𝑖𝑛 + 𝑛𝑖𝑛 + 𝜀𝑖𝑛

Where an additional vector of error terms nin results in an integral without a closed form, in need of

estimation with simulation. The MMNL probability is a weighted average of the logit formula where β

parameters are collapsed into a function of parameters and their distribution. Hence the formula is

evaluated at different values of β, with the weights given by the density f(β). The crucial part of the

model formulation is the specification of which coefficients to be randomly distributed and the choice of

distribution to use. These choices obviously affect the model outcome. Section 4 provides an example of

a MMNL model.

The MMNL specification can not only be used to vary β parameters, but also to vary what error-

component enter the utility of an alternative, known as an error-components MMNL. This allows for the

induction of controlled heteroscedasticity for one alternative. Also some researchers have used the

MMNL to incorporate deterministic heterogeneity in the random terms, either in the mean or standard

deviation, to relate variation in the random coefficients to observed attributes of a respondent 277.

One shortcoming of commonly used MMNL model is the lack of explanation it offers for the observed

taste heterogeneity. The model can simply show whether heterogeneity is present and for what

parameters, but just concluding people differ, is often not enough for policy recommendations. Of

course there are ways to get more information, like incorporating interaction terms. Some researchers

find that another model allowing for heterogeneity, the Latent Class model is more useful than random

parameters MMNL, as it assumes discrete distributions of β parameters taking a number of possible

values. This is useful if there are segments with similar tastes in the respondent population, as the

Latent Class model treats the segments as classes, to which observed respondent attributes can be

linked. However, this is only a valid approach if heterogeneity is discretely distributed. Recently more

Page 39: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

39

advanced model structures, allowing for the inclusion of latent attitudes and characteristics of the

respondent to be included as a dependent variable in the utility function, hence providing estimations of

if/how these factors drive choices. This approach is described in detail in section 6, which also serves as

an application example.

2.3.3.1 Variance Scale Parameter and Marginal Rates of Substitution

All utility measures or coefficients derived from any random utility model are confounded by a scale

parameter proportional to the inverse of the variance of the error term in the utility function. The scale

factor affects the estimation of taste parameters or utilities, in such a way that the larger the scale, the

bigger the coefficient (and vice versa; the smaller the scale the smaller the coefficients). Because of this

effect, it isn’t correct to compare utility measures from two models. Observed differences or similarities

could be solely related to scale as the taste parameters are expressed in a scaled form. Coefficients

reflect the effect of each observed parameter relative to the standard deviation of the unobserved part.

As the scale parameter does not affect the ratio of two coefficients, researchers often estimate marginal

rates of substitution (MRS) to be able to compare tastes, models or compute willingness to pay-

estimates. MRS is simply the ratio between the marginal utility of two attributes, examining the relative

importance of attributes to one another or in other words looking at the change in an attribute required

to compensate for a change in another attribute, keeping the total utility constant. It is assumed that

some level of improvement in one good can compensate some level of worsening of another good,

leaving an individual on the same indifference curve. This compensatory decision making is the core of

consumer theory and is the theoretical basis for MRS.

Statistically the constant marginal utility implies a linear payment-attribute. Often price/costs is used as

a denominator in the ratio, to make the researcher able to estimate marginal willingness to pay for a

change in any other attribute. In health, a time attribute is sometimes used a payment-vehicle,

estimating willingness to wait for a change in any other attribute. The use of price/time as the

denominator in the ratio is generally motivated by the continuous linear treatment of the associated

attribute but it is rarely ever tested if this assumption holds. However, when tested it is often suggested

that linearity in the payment vehicle cannot be assumed. Hence, researchers should not simply assume

that the payment-vehicle behaves in a continuous and linear way, but check for possible violations of

assumption of constant marginal utility. If payment-attributes are inconsistent with conventional

assumption, it is of great importance to acknowledge the consequences of this on welfare estimates and

consider other ways to perform calculations, depending on the purpose of the task. Some suggest that

Page 40: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

40

cheap talking the cost-attribute is a valid solution while other suggests recoding costs, with the possible

downside of adding to any hypothetical bias 278–280. However, since the MRS, if it is not calculated to

estimate marginal willingness to pay/wait, is simply a ratio, and in principle any suitable attribute can be

used as a denominator, when bearing in mind the theoretical and statistically premises and the

difference in interpretation of the trade-offs as it has been done in the paper presented in section 6.

This section presented a systematic review of the existing literature concerning preferences in regards

to LBP. It showed how the field is characterised with difficult choices with confusing evidence to inform

the choices. It also highlighted the gaps of knowledge that the empirical work in this thesis helps to fill.

Further, this section gave insight into the theory and methods relating to choice modelling and

presented the step-wise process by which the empirical work, presented in the following section, was

done.

Page 41: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

41

3 Empirical work

As states the existing knowledge uncovered in literature and especially the lack of trade-off based

preferences studies within the field of LBP, set the ground for a thorough empirical work, providing the

background for the following three papers. The empirical work took place in different settings and using

a mix of methods. Each step corresponding to the stepwise process presented and described in section

2.3.2 This section describes the empirical process in detail and in accordance with the stepwise process

with special emphasis on step 2 – formulating attributes and levels as a lot of extra effort was put into

this step in particular. Much of the work described in this section is elaborated on in section 4. Thus,

this section should be seen as an additional insight into the empirical work, the results of which is

presented in sections 4,5 and 6.

3.1 Formulating the research question

Formulating the research questions was done on the basis of literature searches and studies identifying

gaps of knowledge as already touched upon in section 2.2.

As the research performed in this thesis was founded by a large research grant from the Danish strategic

research council, the research questions were presented and discussed at a bi-annual meeting with the

other partners under the grant. These partners included a range of HCPs and economists, all performing

research in lumbar disease. Discussions in that for a help shape and narrow the research question and

pointed towards low back pain as an interesting field, characterised by confusing evidence and

potentially highly influenced by preferences. Further, literature searches on SP literature helped uncover

methodological issues yet to be enlightened or discussed further.

3.2. Selecting attributes and levels

Observational studies were performed at a large hospital-based ambulatory clinic and during the

three- day observational stay a large number of patient consultations each lasting about 10-20

minutes, were observed. Consulting doctors, of whom there were 3 of different age and experience,

were informed that I was present to get a deeper knowledge of what type of worries or questions

their patients had. At the beginning of each consultation the patients were informed by the doctor

that I was present to look into how consultations worked for research purposes.

Page 42: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

42

The observations were based on a guide, formulated with inputs from experts. The guide is

depicted below in table 2, and was also used as a framework for writing down the observed

characteristics.

Table 4. Observation guide

Focus Points to observe

Physical frame

- Room

- Atmosphere

Doctor’s presentation

of treatment options

- Framing

- Choice of words

- Body language

- ”Translation” to common language

Patient reactions and

interaction with doctor

- Non-verbal Communications

- Comments and questions

- Use of prior experiences

- Choice of words

- Listening ability

- Mood and mood changes (insecure, relaxed,

joking etc)

Patient characteristics - Gender

- Age

- Appearance

Facts - Medical history (prior surgeries etc)

- Consultation time

The observed patients were being informed about their treatment indications or had already received

treatment. The patients’ questions and thoughts in relation to choosing treatment and their

motivational explanations were observed. Furthermore, the doctors’ part in the decision-making

process was observed and was subsequently discussed with the doctor in the small breaks between

patients and at the end of each day.

The purposes of the observations were to gain a deep insight into the decision-making context patients

with back pain are faced with. Observing the meeting between a doctor and a patient gives a unique

possibility of seeing what actually happens when ´patients are informed about their options and chooses

one with the help of their doctors. Even though being present in the room potentially could affect the

Page 43: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

43

patients or doctors behaviour, none of the doctors or patients seemed disturbed or distracted in any

way.

The observations were summed in a short narrative report and resulted in a draft list of attributes and

formed the basis for some of the topics in the interview-guide used to interview doctors.

In addition to observations, interviews with different HCPs treating back pain patients were performed.

The HCPs represented different approaches to treatment. The interviews were semi-structured, leaving

room for pursuing different topics of interest while ensuring that issues from the observational studies

were touched upon and knowledge to inform the tentative list of potential attributes was gained.

Further, the interviewed served as a way of gaining a deeper understanding of the medical issues in

regards to back pain. The interviews took place in the doctor’s offices and each lasted 1-2 hours. The

interview guide is depicted below in table 3.

Table 5. Interview guide

The interviews resulted in a reduced list of attributes with suggested levels, mirroring the medical

possibilities for each attribute.

3.3 Construction of tasks

On the basis of literature, observations and discussions with experts a list of potential attributes and

levels was formed. This list was then qualified formed into tentative choice profiles and this along with a

number of clarifying questions was the formation for a series of semi-structured interviews. The

interviews took part with patients suffering from low back pain. The patient interviews were performed

•Education

•Job Description

•Experience

•Typical patients

Background information

•Medical Perspective

•Evidence - or lack thereof

•Decision-makingLow Back Pain

•Plausibility of attributes

•Importanceof attributes

•Levels for each attribute

Design of SP-study: Attributes

Page 44: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

44

in a hospital ward with back pain patients awaiting HCPs decisions on treatment. Only a few hospitals

keep LBP-patients as inpatients during this period, but in order to get informants, interviews were

performed on one such clinic. The semi-structured interviews contained topics of disease history and

concerns/hopes as well as a test of the questionnaire made as a think-aloud exercise and followed by a

discussion of the different attributes and level and the length of the questionnaire. Each interview lasted

about 1 hour and were recorded and transcribed.

As it will be discussed in section 4, the interviews resulted in a range of changes to the design of and

especially to the framing of the choice experiment. The design was changed to include fewer choice

tasks and attributes while the opt-out option was kept.

Table 6. Final list of attributes and levels

Attribute Levels Hypothesis

Modality Non-surgical

Surgical

-/+

Pain level Same

Less

None

+

++

Problems with

ADL*

Same

Fewer

None

+

++

Risk of relapse 1 in 10

2 in 10

3 in 10

-

--

Time to treatment

effect

1, 3, 6, 12 months -

*ADL = activities of daily living

Different framing approaches were tested and discussed during the interviews, especially in regards to

how to present and describe the risk of relapse from any treatment and how to present the overall task

of making hypothetical choices. This again is further discussed in section 4.

Page 45: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

45

3.3 Experimental Design

The above list of attributes and levels were then made into an initial experimental design. The initial

design was an optimal orthogonal design using 18 rows blocked in three with each 6 choice-sets. This

design was then part of a pilot-testing along with the rest of the questionnaire, the development of

which is explained further in section 3.4.

The pilot test took place during 3 weeks in the early spring of 2012 at Spine Centre of Southern

Denmark. This centre was chosen as the site of inclusion as it is the only centre treating spinal diseases

and back pain in the region of southern Denmark and thus has a large uptake and many patients coming

through each day. It also has both a surgical and non-surgical department making the choice of modality

real to every patient entering the centre. Further the centre has a large research unit and is in the

forefront of especially non-surgical treatment in Denmark. The centre was visited numerous times at

different stages of the data collection and an instructive meeting involving all personnel was held at the

initiation of the pilot test.

The pilot test included 17 respondents and the questionnaire was handed to each patient personally

with an explanation of the purpose of the test. The pilot data was then analysed using a simple MNL

model.

3.3.1 Final experimental design

The final experimental design was a Bayesian efficient design using 600 Halton draws and assuming

distributions around the priors obtained from the pilot-study. Effects were assumed to be non-linear for

treatment modality, ADL, pain, and risk and dummy-coding was thus used for these attributes. The

design included 3 alternatives of which alternative 3 was the opt out, and alternative 1 included a

constant. As with the pilot, all attributes were generic in alternative 1 and 2. The design used 18 rows

and d-efficiency as the optimizer and was blocked in 3. The experimental design is not only chosen

based on its more novel approach than standard orthogonal designs, but in particular because of

concerns about sample-size. Rose and Bliemer 260 notes that efficient designs not only maximize the

information on preferences obtained from each choice by a respondent, but also can decrease standard

errors of parameters in such a way that finding a more optimal design can decrease standard errors

more than increases in sample size.

Page 46: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

46

3.4 Preference elicitation and instrument design

As stated the SP-experiment as part of a larger questionnaire that was development simultaneously with

the qualitative work aimed at informing the experimental design. The questionnaire thus used questions

from Dallas Pain Questionnaire (DPQ)281. DPQ is a 16-item instrument assessing four aspects of daily

living of chronic back pain patients; day-to-day activities, work and leisure activities, anxiety-depression

status, and social interest. Scale extremities are labelled with specific words (e.g. ‘never’/’all the time’).

The questionnaire also included a pain-scale and items from The Low Back Pain Rating Scale (LBPRS) 282,

which is a rating system designed to evaluate the clinical outcome of LBP patients. The pain component

of the scale is a six question scale divided into two groups of three questions about back pain and three

questions about leg pain. Each item is scored 0-10. The disability component of LBPRS consists of 15

questions evaluating the patient's ability to perform daily activities.

Further the questionnaire included a range of background questions and questions regarding experience

with and attitudes toward treatment and HCPs treating LBP. Many of these questions stemmed from the

literature review.

3.5 Collection of data

At the Spine Centre all patients referred by GPs are met by a nurse. The initial visit thus contains initial

anamnesis, a computer-based short form of the LBPRS and an MRI scan. Subsequent visits include a

multidisciplinary team consultation, in-depth anamneses and clinical examinations. Patients meeting the

inclusion criteria of suffered from neck or back pain for more than two months and not having neck-pain

or acute disc-disease was handed a questionnaire with a pre-paid envelop by the nurse. Thus, the

questionnaire was distributed before the patients had any knowledge about the diagnosis and

treatment path suggested by the experts at the centre. The results of their MRI scans were also

unknown. The questionnaire came with instructions on how to return it by mail and the purpose of the

study. The nurses noted the questionnaire number on their patient-lists that were then kept by the head

nurse and collected to be included in the database including all patients receiving a questionnaire.

Upon the end of the data collection background variables and LBPRS-data on the patients handed a

questionnaire, without returning it was retrieved from the hospital database, enabling a non-response

analysis looking into potential differences between responders and non-responders in regards to age,

gender and average pain.

Page 47: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

47

During the data collection the Spine Centre was visited every second week. The visits were used to

remind nurses of sampling strategy and inclusion criteria and to bring more questionnaires if needed. As

the data collection was ended an “evaluation” of the process took place at a staff meeting.

At the end of data-analysis a final presentation of results was held in the summer of 2013.

3.6 Statistical Analysis

The statistical analysis if the SP-data differed in relation to the purpose of analysis and includes both

MNL, MMNL and hybrid models, results of which are presented in the papers in sections 5 and 6. The

focus of analysis was modelling heterogeneity and trying to explain the observed differences in

numerous ways. The full data set was used making an MMNL model and sub-groups for the paper in

section 5, while only respondents answering the whole questionnaire was used in the paper in section 6,

which presents the hybrid model structure and investigates if and how additional data can help explain

heterogeneity with greater power than MMNL models. Analysis were primarily made using Stata12,

while all models presented in section 6 are made using Ox.

The following sections will present the empirical work from start to end in three different papers, which

are then followed by discussion.

Page 48: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

48

4 Designing a stated choice experiment. The value of a qualitative

process

Kløjgaard ME, Bech M, Søgaard R

Designing a Stated Choice Experiment: The Value of a Qualitative Process

Journal of Choice Modelling, Volume 5, Issue 2, 2012, Pages 1–18 DOI: 10.1016/S1755-5345(13)70050-2

(Due to journal’s copyright the paper is not presented in this version of the thesis)

Page 49: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

101

5 Patients’ preference for treatment of low back pain – a discrete choice

experiment

Kløjgaard ME, Manniche C, Pedersen LB, Bech M, Søgaard R

Patient preferences for treatment of low back pain-a discrete choice experiment.

Value Health. 2014 Jun;17(4):390-6. doi: 10.1016/j.jval.2014.01.005. Epub 2014 Apr 21.

(Due to journal’s copyright the paper is not presented in this version of the thesis)

Page 50: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

102

6 Understanding the formation and influence of attitudes in patients’

treatment choices for lower back pain: testing the benefits of a hybrid

choice model approach

Kløjgaard ME, Hess S

Understanding the formation and influence of attitudes in patients' treatment choices for lower back

pain: testing the benefits of a hybrid choice model approach.

Soc Sci Med. 2014 Aug;114:138-50. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.05.058. Epub 2014 Jun 2.

(Due to journal’s copyright the paper is not presented in this version of the thesis)

Page 51: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

103

7 Discussion and future perspectives

This section will discuss the results shown in sections 4-6 in relation to the existing knowledge and hence

the contributions of the present studies to the field. Further, a section on limitations – specific and in

general, is presented, which also serves as a discussion of validity and a suggestion of one topic of future

research, namely collecting revealed preference data in a health setting. This section also discusses the

possible practical use of the elicited patient preferences and how knowledge on preferences can be

brought more into medical decision making. Further, the patient and the relationship and influence

on/of the HCPs, is discussed. This is followed by a discussion on who actually shares decision making and

how it is influenced by peers, a discussion that follows up on results from section 6. Also, this section

discusses some more methodological issues that came across as especially important during the studies,

leading to this thesis. One such topic is the feasibility of assuming utility maximisation and another is the

importance of heterogeneity.

7.1 Results and existing knowledge

The results presented in chapters 4 and 5 and 6 feed into the existing literature presented in section 2.2

in a number of different ways.

Firstly, literature points to some connection between patients’ preferences and treatment outcomes.

This is very interesting in regards to the lack of use of sophisticated methods to elicit preferences. In

itself the results of the empirical work thus provide a useful insight into factors influencing actual

treatment outcomes for LBP patients. Not only by investigating preferences including how utility is

traded, but also by further exploring factors influencing preferences, adding the importance of attitudes

and perceptions to the factor influencing preferences uncovered by literature, i.e. information and

previous experience. The latter being confirmed by the study presented in section 6.

Literature also suggests that patients prefer being involved in decision making to a great degree. In

literature, decision-aid tools have been used as one way of facilitating the process. The issue of how

such decision-aids can be made to include patients’ preferences has been dedicated a separate

discussion in section 7.2.1.

7.1.1 Limitations

There are several limitations to the studies presented in this thesis, some of which are specific to each

empirical contribution and some more general to the field of SP.

Page 52: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

104

This and the following section thus deal with a range of limits surrounding the produced results, both

methodologically and in relation to the papers in sections 4-6.

As discussed in the introduction, an overall limitation is the lack of a clear definition of back pain related

disorders. A number of terms, diagnosis and categories are thus used to categorize LBP patients, making

literature searches challenging and potentially limited in terms of precision. Also, a rather extensive

range of HCPs deal with patients with LBP, which again could influence the systematic literature review.

Further, the lack of consensus on terms and imprecisions resulting from it, limits extrapolation of

research within the field. This along with similar factors like e.g. exact content of treatment and area

variation makes results presented in this thesis primarily representative of the setting in which they

were collected. This is a serious limitation of scope, characterizing not only this thesis but also research

performed in this field in general. Despite the limitation, this thesis does provide new insight into

literature and contributes to the field in numerous ways as is has been discussed in several sections.

The extrapolation issue is not only present in the description of existing knowledge, but also influence

the potential use of this knowledge as a basis for generating and designing new studies in the field. Thus

an extra effort was out into generating solid qualitative knowledge of the field in Denmark. As the

existing literature on both SP and LBP showed, albeit present, qualitative studies are most often

concerned with investigating specific treatments and not on how the qualitative methods can help

inform a design of a larger investigation. Thus the purpose of the qualitative study presented in section

4, was not only the qualitative work in itself. Had it been more data would have been collected.

Including both interviews, observations and tests of questionnaire. But rather, the purpose was both to

gain insights into important issues, behaviours and perceptions of patients and HCPs and to investigate

how much or to what degree the different qualitative methods help shape the design of the SP study.

However, it is a limitation that a relatively small number of interviews were performed with patients and

HCPs. This was due to the limited time frame in which the qualitative work took place and the ever

changing number of patients eligible for interviewing. However, as stated in the paper, including all the

described steps, leaves little doubt that the chance of not including an important attribute or add

inappropriate levels is obviously substantially smaller than if the process had been limited or based on

experts’ opinions or researchers’ opinions and literature 283. Also, the paper does provide an insight into

a much overlooked process of qualitative work prior to designing a stated choice experiment and deals

with several different methods to do so.

Page 53: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

105

The SP-based empirical contributions in section 5 and 6, contributes with important knowledge of

patients preferences of LBP. However, a couple of different limitations due to the data collection are

present in the studies.

Firstly, the survey mode itself might have been a limitation to as paper surveys demands the respondent

to be able to read and understand the survey and to post it after answering. The method was chosen

however, due to the target population and hence possible lack of internet and computer skills as well as

practical reasons, as the visits to the hospital was an open opportunity to distribute the surveys. Ideally

an interview based method or the like could have been used possibly affecting the response rate

positively. The respondents however, were given a phone number to call if needed, which some did.

Another factor influencing response is the matter of missing data or non-response. A matter that

becomes even more important in studies using latent variables as non-response to any question used to

form a latent variable affects results. Again, interviewer-assisted data collection might have helped as it

has shown to generate less missing data 284. However, interviewing the number of patients needed for

this research in the middle of a busy clinical setting would not only have been utterly expensive but also

potentially very difficult. In the paper in section 4 all respondents are kept in analysis, even if all

respondents did not answer all choice sets, however, in the paper in section 5, respondents with missing

data either in choice sets or in questions used to form the latent variable is removed. In general,

researchers do not like to remove data, and there are no golden rules as to how non-response needs

treating, but some researchers argue that respondents who exhibit this sort of behaviour can distort

analysis and significantly bias results and thus should be removed 285. How much non-response to a few

or more questions in a survey actually distorts or affects results is still an unresolved matter in literature.

Also, it has been discussed in literature the site of inclusion might influence on results. Data was

gathered at a hospital ward, which specializes in non-surgical treatment of back-pain with the possibility

of surgical referral. This could be a plausible explanatory reason for the preferences expressed by the

respondents. The tendency of patients preferring whatever the clinic in which they are surveyed offers,

suggests that patients in general might just prefer what they are given or expected to be given. As

stated, the timing of inclusion in this study was deliberately done so that patients were captured early in

hospital sector pathway and hence before having been influenced by hospital doctors. This was done to

accommodate for any effects of pre-existing notions of treatments paths and in that light the results

might better reflect the true preference of a back pain patient. However, most patients had seen other

HCPs and their GP numerous times before referral, and also patients had also received advice from

Page 54: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

106

friends and family. Thus patients are influenced by others by the time they were included in the SP

studies. However, this is most likely also the case for the patients surveyed in the existing literature.

In general another limitation of the thesis is the lack of uncovered knowledge in regards to patient-HCP

relationship and shared decision-making. Interesting future studies could further investigate how well

HCPs include, know and act upon patients preferences and uncover potential discrepancies in

perceptions and attitudes driving the preferences of the patients and HCPs. These issues are touched

upon in sections 7.2 and 7.3.

The limitations section continues with a discussion of validity of results from the SP-studies.

7.1.1.1 Are stated preferences valid?

Another limitation, in another kind of scope, feed into the discussion on the so-called hypothetical bias.

As mentioned, stated preference research has been criticized for its hypothetical nature. The predictive

validity of SP-experiments refers to the ability to predict actual market behaviour. In other words, SP-

experiments are often designed to mimic real market choices and a valid SP-study predicts what

respondents would actually choose in real life. Data from actual choices on markets is known as

revealed preferences (RP). Dealing with RP also suffers from a different issue, as one doesn’t have the

opportunity to choose attributes or their levels, but only the ability to observe the actual market. Some

researchers have tried to combine the two types of data in SP+RP models, to utilize the strengths of

both types of data 286.

Testing whether a SP-experiment is has a good predictive validity can be difficult. So far, researchers

have compared aggregate differences in model outcomes from SP-experiments and data gathered on

the real market 287–289. Although aggregate analysis gives a hint on whether hypothetical bias is present it

leaves researchers without knowledge on its prevalence, or why it occurs. This is still an issue to pursue

in research and could potentially be done in relation to the studies presented in this thesis, as proposed

in the end of this section. Whether or not a SP-experiment is able to predict true preferences, have

been described to be a matter of how much respondents care about or feel responsible for their

answers to the hypothetical choices. It has been argued that the lack of real consequence for the

hypothetical choices distort behaviour 290,291 and a range of studies have been looking into this lack of

consequence292,293. In one study respondents were informed that one choice would randomly be

selected as binding and this was hypothesized to be an incentive for being honest when choosing 290.

This approach is not feasible in some studies on health care, in particular when there is no market to

Page 55: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

107

observe, and other researchers have proposed other ways of trying to ensure external validity. One of

which is framing, making the respondent aware of the importance of truthful answers, another is

certainty scales, making the respondents state their certainty either after each choice set or after the

full number of choice sets. Most commonly the scale is a simple 1-10 numeric scale and it is used to

either remove or recode uncertain individuals to get a more precise measure of preferences and reduce

hypothetical bias. However, there is no consensus on when a respondent can be said to be uncertain

and different approaches or cut-offs will most probably result in different outcomes. Some studies

shows that a good cut-off on a 1-10 scale is 7 or 8 289,294.

In the survey used in this thesis a 1-10 certainty scale was included after the completion of the entire

choice tasks. This pose another problem, as it is uncertain whether respondents answer about certainty

of the final choice set or overall certainty – which again is probably different from choice set to choice

set. When removing respondents stated 6 or less on the scale no significant difference from the full

sample on MRS was found. However, the validity of using certainty scales to test for anything – and in

particular to lessen hypothetical bias is limited.

The predictive power of the studies presented in this thesis is unknown, but of interest, not only as a

methodological test or curiosity, but also to gain knowledge on the persistence of initial preferences and

how often these are changed. Further, following-up on actual treatment results, and testing whether or

not these were correlated with receiving a preferred treatment would be a valid contribution to the

range of studies presented in section 2.2.3, dealing with how patients’ preferences potentially affect

treatment outcomes.

Comparing SP to RP is rarely ever done in health economics (see 295 for a rare exemption), and it has

been suggested that finding imaginative solutions to test predictive value and external validity should be

an issue of research in health economics 296 possibly helping to yield more credibility to results obtained

from SP-studies in health 297.

The lack of a real market for treatment of LBP makes observations difficult and hence testing external

validity or predictive value of this study hard. An interesting future study following the presented work

from this thesis could be a register-based follow-up of treatments chosen for and by patient

respondents from section 5 and 6. This could be done by observing the actual treatment choices with

the preferences collected ex-ante giving inside into whether the cohort or the different segments did

receive their preferred treatment and providing further insight into any potential better clinical

Page 56: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

108

outcomes for those receiving their preferred treatment compared to those who did not. Obviously such

a study would need to take the clinical reference points for patients into account. The Danish social

security and health insurance system provides a unique opportunity of following patients and treatment

choices as each patient is use of health care resources is registered by the health authorities. This

unique source of data is already used extensively in health economic research in Denmark but has yet to

be used as a mean comparing SP-choices with real life decisions.

7.2 Policy implications

Results generated by preference studies, as those presented in sections 5 and 6, arguably have a role to

play in health policy, especially in making clinical guidelines 15,16. Formulating clinical guidelines has been

an important factor in modern medicine. Methods to do so, have evolved and is has been acknowledged

that a huge challenge in guideline formulation is to create widely applicable, but incorporates or respect

patient heterogeneity 16.

Lately, guidelines have tried to accomplish this, by becoming more of an active player in decision making

processes as tried with Map of Medicine or the American Guided Patient Support(GPS) and Medical

Decision Support (MDS) and by introducing guideline knowledge to patients as done in the literature

with videos 298–300.

However, it might be possible to bring preferences even closer to decisions, which is discussed in this

section.

7.2.1 Putting preferences to practice

As suggested, one way of incorporating preferences is to make preference-informed clinical guidelines

16. Acknowledging that differences in preferences might lead to differences in preferred therapy or

treatment and thus clinical guidelines in fields in which patients’ preferences has the possibility of

playing a prominent role should be preference-based. This approach has the benefit of easily bringing

preferences to the attention of the HCPs making treatment decisions. Guidelines are a common way of

bringing evidence into decisions and in most countries HCPs are bound to follow clinical guidelines.

However, even if health professionals all agree that informed choices and patient-centred care is

important and correct, and with the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and

Evaluation (GRADE) 301, being widely implemented, including in Denmark, this development is further

encouraged, it still proofs difficult to actually implement patient-centred choices in health care on a

more practical level 302–304. Greatly so, due to the great variation or heterogeneity within the target

Page 57: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

109

population. Hence, making preference part of a clinical guideline only offers a generic view of

preference for a target group, who not only differs, but also changes – sometimes faster than guidelines.

Therefore eliciting individual preference on a site of treatment has the potential of facilitation patient-

centred treatment and truly shared decision-making. Future research could be aimed at implementing

and evaluating a computer-based system. Such an approach would be more practical and patient-near,

individualizing the HCP-patient meeting, by surveying preferences at a clinic. So far, to my greatest

knowledge, this has not been implemented in any clinical settings in Denmark – or maybe even

anywhere, but the means to do so are available. SP-approaches are not only being used in research in

transport, environment and health, but to a great extent also in marketing. In marketing, eliciting

preferences needs to be quick and easy, especially if it happens, not as part of a survey, but as part of a

costumer profiling prior to buying a product. This has been used for a range of companies, who’ve

incorporated short SP-surveys on their websites to target products to a potential buyer. Letting patients

do something similar before a consultation might be beneficial for the communication between an HCP

and a patient and could help uncover any unrealistic preferences or preferences of which an HCP

doesn’t think of. Often, patients are put in front of a computer to enter their basic information and

incorporating some sort of preference-elicitation mechanism, could be easily done. Thus, a small task of

trading off potential treatment outcomes and a simple algorithm helping the HCP to see the importance

of different treatment or outcome characteristics could benefit the information

7.3 The patient and the medical experts

One important issue the literature is concerned with and that the empirical work touched upon is the

patient-HCP relationship. It is this relationship that determines treatment paths and has the potential of

taking preferences into account in decision making. Already the relationship is characterised by

asymmetry of information on both sides. Arguably, this gap can be broadened, when HCPs are not

guided by solid evidence and decisions but are influenced by attitudes and perceptions. It has been

shown that HCPs do not adhere to guidelines on LBP in their treatment suggestions or referral practice

305–309. Instead, they make decisions based on perceptions, feelings or attitudes. Decisions that are

sometimes to the detriment of patients or society, leading to e.g. inappropriate referral practices and

that definitely shape and influence patient pathways.

Further, literature point to a discrepancy between patients’ and HCPs’ preferences, not only in regards

to causation, management or communication, but also in relation to actual treatment 310–314. And even if

Page 58: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

110

it has been argued that mere congruence of preferences does not uncover whether HCPs are focused on

incorporating their patients’ preferences, this discrepancy is of some concern 315.

Different practice settings or differences between groups of professionals may explain some of the

observed trends 316. However, it seems certain that HCPs are highly driven by beliefs and perceptions in

their recommendations to patients 55,195,316 and that their different attitudes e.g. towards origin and

physiology of pain 317–319 or fear-avoidance beliefs 54,195,320 or even completely non-clinical aspects like

giving patients “peace-of-mind”309, are heavily affecting the decisions made by HCPs when treating

patients with LBP. It appears to be likely that HCPs preferences serves as barriers or in some instances

facilitators of good treatment outcomes for patients.

Another factor that is pointed out in both literature, my studies and by the clinicians, is that patients

most in favour of surgery are in some ways more desperate patients, with sick-leaves and low SES.

Media, also suggest that society puts pressure on these candidates as patients can lose sick-leave-

benefits if they don’t accept surgery 321 even though evidence does not suggest a (quicker) recovery or

any recovery for surgical candidates and further suggest that those benefitting most from surgery – if

any - are the well-functioning patients 322.

This suggests a very important task of communication not only from HCP to patient, but also from HCP

to societal institutions, as HCPs are not necessarily informed about a patient’s social situation.

Knowledge of these social impacts on preferences as presented in section 6, might be helpful in

overcoming asymmetry of information.

A final point is that of the lack of influence by the GP. It was expected from the results presented in

section 6 that patients would be guided by a surgical recommendation from their GP, but this was not

the case. This could suggest a changing role for the GP or maybe reflect a frustration from patients who

had all had numerous visits during their pathway to the expert-clinic. As noted, interestingly patients are

guided in their preferences by their friends and family, who even if they mostly hadn’t recommended

anything or had recommend non-surgical treatment, had a significant impact on patients’ preferences

had they recommended surgery. This influence by peers is acknowledges in decision making research

and work has been commenced to try to model decisions shared by more than one individual or made in

groups. This is the topic of the following section.

Page 59: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

111

7.3.1 Who is sharing decision-making?

One of the more interesting results found in the paper presented in section 6, was the influence of

friends and family on preferences. Even if the result is subject of the usual precautions of limited data

and statistical uncertainties, it seems very plausible that decisions on health are not necessarily taken by

the patient in question, but could be taken jointly with that patient’s family and/or friends trading

against one and another until an optimal “group solution” is reached. Also, this is the ideal process of

decision making between a patient and his/hers HCP. It has been recognized for some time that

decisions are not always simply made by one individual, but still, the literature on group based models

of choice is relatively underdeveloped. However, the subject of joint decision-making has started gaining

attention in the SP-literature in other fields than health economics, acknowledging that many decisions,

such as vacation choices are often made by a family/couple and not individually 323. This could also cause

the hypothetical bias already discussed, as results from analyses based on data obtained with the

traditional mode asking one individual as representative of an entity, could differ from real world joint

decisions.

How the interactions of individual members of a group or entity, influence the joint decision making and

preference formation, represents an important dimension of understanding behaviour. This has

resulted in attention within the choice modelling literature to examine the role that social interactions

play in terms of preference formation and how these interactions can be caught – and modelled 324,325.

Simply aggregating preferences of individual members of a group into a “group”-preference is unlikely

to result in a valid or efficient choice as a decision made by more than one individual is subject of

negotiation and interaction and differences in strength of preference an individual has for the

alternatives in question. The latter being some of what we try to capture on certainty scales.

Given that many choices are a function of group decisions, understanding the dynamic nature of group

preference formation is essential in gaining accurate valuations for attributes and alternatives. The

universal nature of group decision making means that choices under such circumstances are of

particular interest to social scientists in any discipline.

Three different methodologies for exploring shared or joint decision making has been proposed. One is

the interactive agency choice experiments (IACE), another the group negotiated choice experiments

(GNCE) and a third the minimum information group inference (MIGI).

Page 60: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

112

GCNE is designed to represent the true group decision process with members of a group discussing

alternatives and agreeing on one common choice. IACE tries to make that process more real by

examining changes in preference structures resulting from group interaction step for step. This method

does not require the actual participants to meet as the method uses a series of choice tasks for each

member of a group to complete, while receiving continuous feedback of other members’ choices until

the group reaches consensus. It is reasonable to assume that each individual has a varying amount of

knowledge of the others’ preferences and a view on the influence of each agent and the completion of

iterative rounds of interactive review, choice, review, revision, etc. progress the level of knowledge and

influence both of which may change for each individual. The MIGI approach is parallel to IACE, but

makes group members rank the various alternatives and state their acceptable outcomes, enabling the

researcher to project what the group’s outcome might be.

Attempting to reproduce decisions in groups in an SP-experimental setting is potentially difficult and

costly, but researchers have explored the methods and proposed ways. Within the IACE framework Rose

and Hensher326 made respondents interact at various stages in the decision making process and bring

different perspectives and influences to the formation of preferences and in the final group choice. It

has even been shown that estimates derived from this process does not suffer from hypothetical bias as

they are equivalent to RP data 327. The modelling techniques that Rose and Hensher326 employ for IACE

data are also transferable to the MIGI and GNCE framework.

To overcome the potential costs of performing IACE, MIGI was introduced by Hensher and others in

2007 328,329. Here, the researcher establishes the trade-offs that each agent is willing to make. This is

accomplished by presenting interdependent respondents with the fundamental elements of interactive

choice settings, coordinating their stated preference rankings into projected joint choice outcomes and

then examining these outcomes in reference to the prevailing characteristics of the relationship

between the decision makers.

Literature proposes a number of different methods to model the influence of peers on choices. All of

which could proof beneficial in health economic SP-research. Future research should use these novel

techniques to investigate the influence of friends and family on a patient’s preferences and maybe even

more interestingly, the joint decision making taking place between HCPs and patients. However, before

individual utilities can be compared and meaningful implications about influences be made, the impact

of scaling effects need to be acknowledged as presented in section 2.3.3.1 and further discussed in

section 7.4.2

Page 61: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

113

7.4 Methodological issues

A range of methodological issues were raised during the research undertaken in this thesis, most of

which has been already addressed. Two issues, however, came to stand out, one - heterogeneity, has

already been touched upon, another - the feasibility of assuming utility maximizing behaviour has yet to

be discussed. The following sections will discuss alternatives to utility maximisation and further

elucidate the topic of preference as well as scale heterogeneity, the latter picking up from the discussion

on group decisions in section 7.3.1.

7.4.1 Are we utility-maximising?

In the qualitative work, it became apparent that assuming utility maximizing behaviour might not be in

accordance with decision rules applied in real life. This notion was not pursued as standard SP-work

suggested otherwise and knowledge on alternatives to RUM had yet to be gained. However, research

fields outside health economics have increasingly looked into alternatives, focusing especially on

random regret minimization (RRM) 330. This alternative behavioural paradigm emerged in the travel

choice literature. RRM assumes that choices are motivated by the desire to avoid scenarios made up

from unwanted attribute levels. So the chosen alternative is outperformed by the non-chosen one. Or

simply put, regret is what you experience when a foregone alternative performs better than the chosen

one.

Choice theories and choice models build on regret based choice, hypothesize that individuals try to

minimize regret, and not maximize utility, when choosing. Thus RRM models assume that as

alternatives are characterized in terms of multiple attributes, which implies that trade-off to be made by

the respondent, there will be regret in the sense that there will generally be at least one non-chosen

alternative that outperforms a chosen one in terms of one or more attributes 330–332.

The RRM approach has only just been introduced in health economics 333 and has yet to be tested and

applied in a series of studies, investigating whether it outperforms the RUM framework. RUM still have

the advantage of being standard in most software and in choice situations in which only two alternatives

are present, RUM and RRM measures are the same and even if respondents qualitatively did talk of

regret as a basis for choosing, using RUM as the decision framework for the studies in this PhD was

perfectly adequate. But when designing SP-studies one needs to recognise the impact of RRM and how

different attributes are perceived differently and chosen between differently 331.

Page 62: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

114

Taking the notion of alternative decision rules even further, some authors argue that a single generic

framework is never appropriate no matter its premises. Respondents might use a range of decision

rules to answer different choice tasks. Recent studies have highlighted the importance of allowing for

heterogeneity in terms of decision-making strategies 334,335. In the context of RRM, this has led to

modelling approaches which allow for different segments of the population to choose in a way more in

line with RUM premises, while others may choose in a way more in line with RRM premises 336,337.

Regardless of what decision framework is chosen in probably will never adequately be able to explain

the process by which choices are made in real live. However, notwithstanding the obvious value of these

approaches being able to capture heterogeneity, they only capture one or two dimensions of

heterogeneity (across decision makers and/or decision contexts). Testing which decision rule or whether

a mix of rules performs better is somewhat just another way of looking into possible explanations for

responses. Investigating and capturing heterogeneity in decision rules and in tastes and scale continues

to be at the forefront of choice research. The next section further discusses the issue of taste and scale

heterogeneity.

7.4.2 Heterogeneity

From the studies presented in sections 5 and 6 it is apparent that heterogeneity alters results of choice

data. In the review by De Bekker Grob et al. 17 it is concluded that main-effects models continue to

dominate , and despite increasing popularity, models taken account of some heterogeneity like nested

logit, mixed logit and latent class models are still not as commonly used as simple MNL models. It is

further concluded that heterogeneity is the single most salient feature in micro level consumer demand.

Also the LC, MXL, NL models acquire assumptions about e.g. what parameters to be random with what

distribution or number of classes etc. which needs to be done with care and in the most informed way

possible.

More advanced models, linking taste heterogeneity to underlying values and attitudes have only just

begun to emerge into health economics, with the paper presented in section 6 as a first attempt.

As stated, taste heterogeneity - or decision rule heterogeneity – is not the only sorts of heterogeneity

one need to look out for. Researchers have increasingly also acknowledged that scale heterogeneity is

an issue too. This has been tried to have been solved by the so-called Generalised Multinomial Logit

model (GMNL) 338, which however does not separate taste and scale heterogeneity as intended as

demonstrated by Hess and Rose in 2012 339.

Page 63: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

115

Scale heterogeneity embodies differences in behaviour of each respondent over repeated choice

observations. Differences in scale can be influenced by the mere variation in respondents, i.e. different

levels of experience, knowledge or cognitive ability or even be feeling one way or another while

completing the survey. Scale is inversely linked to the error variances. Hence, large scale effects are

linked to small error variances and vice versa. As stated, attempts have been made to try to model scale

separately from taste heterogeneity, but to date, models can only allow for differences in scale between

subsets of respondents i.e. in the nested logit or error component models. Accounting for scale

heterogeneity between respondents has been yet to be studied further in the literature clever ways

around the difficulties in estimation still needs to be found. Hence, the models used to evaluate the data

gathered from patients and HCPs, accounting for taste heterogeneity and providing explanations for the

observed differences is not just state-of-the-practice but state-of-the-art.

7.5 Concluding remarks

From dealing with the field of SP and LBP a number of important lessons have been learned. Firstly in

regards to typology of both fields, which is reflected in the papers that changes their wording

concurrently with my reading into literature on back pain and especially literature from other fields of

research on choice modelling. It quickly became apparent that the wide use of SP methods makes it

necessary to read into fields of literature way outside health economics to avoid overlap and to be able

to contribute significantly to health economics. This task is often overlooked by researchers and

mistakes are repeated.

Another thing that became apparent was the dilemma of perfecting models while maintaining or

increasing use of SP results. Accounting for heterogeneity has made modelling choice data a highly

complex task. Even with “simpler” models allowing for only taste heterogeneity like latent class and

mixed logits, require a number of decisions to be made by researchers, i.e. number of classes, draws and

random/fixed parameters and their distributions. Incorporating latent variables, multiple decision

makers or group choices or accounting for scale heterogeneity, makes models very complicated, harder

to estimate and analyse and understand. The latter being especially important in regards to making

policy recommendations on the basis of such models. So while there’s a definite room to grow and

advance in choice modelling in health economics, this move might counteract another movement,

namely the challenge of incorporating SP in economic evaluations and political decision making.

Page 64: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

116

8 Conclusion

This thesis presented a collection of journal manuscripts and an independent overview adding to the

clarification and elaboration of the issues.

The objective of the PhD study has been two-fold. Firstly, it was the aim to explore the preferences of

patients with back pain when facing a choice of trade-offs between treatment options, outcomes and

risk. Secondly, it was an objective to add to the methodological work in the field of preference elicitation

by aiming at contributing with added focus on qualitative work prior to gathering quantitative data on

preferences and by focusing on the complex formation of preferences for patients including

perceptions’ and attitudes’ influence on choices.

The objectives of the PhD study were met in different ways.

First, an introduction presented not only the field of interest, but also a thorough description of the

challenges of LBP and SP-studies. This knowledge served as a background for the presentation of a

thorough and systematic description of existing knowledge of preferences in relation to LBP and thus

helps to answer the research question of eliciting preferences for treatment of low back pain, based on

existing knowledge. Also an introduction to stated-preference-elicitation method of choice modelling

was given, presenting a step-wise process of generating SP-studies and including an introduction to

qualitative methods used in that process. The latter was highlighted numerous times both by thoroughly

describing all design measures taken in the empirical work from the formulation of a research question

to statistical analysis and by the first empirical paper contribution The paper presented the design of the

stated preference or discrete choice experiment and demonstrated how qualitative work can

significantly impact and guide a design, making it clear that a less thorough qualitative process would

have resulted in a less useable and valid design.

The examination of patients’ preferences for treatment of LBP was also done empirically by eliciting

patients’ preferences in a clinical setting. As expected, results show that respondents assign positive

utilities to positive treatment outcomes and disutility to higher risks and longer wait for effects of

treatment. Respondents also showed a disutility towards surgical intervention and were on average

willing to wait two years for effect of treatment to avoid surgery, while they were willing to wait almost

three years for effect of treatment, if the treatment made them free of pain. The mixed logit model

captured significant heterogeneity within the sample for outcomes regarding pain reduction and ability

to pursue activities of daily living and in relation to treatment modality. The sub-group analysis showed

Page 65: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

117

differences in willingness to wait, especially in regards to treatment modality and particularly in groups

of high/low pain at time of data collection and respondents with an ex-ante preference for surgery/non-

surgery. The results indicated that pain relief is the most important factor in choice of treatment,

followed by treatment modality and ability to perform activities of daily living while risk of relapse was

the least important factor. This is not only valuable information for practitioners in their communication

with patients and everyday decision-making, but also important information for policy makers who seek

to ensure a utility-maximizing mix of treatment options for patients. The section empirically answers the

first research question by investigating patients’ preferences for treatment of low back pain.

The investigation of the influence of attitudes and perceptions in patients’ treatment choices for lower

back pain and testing the benefits of a hybrid choice model approach, was also done empirically. The

results show that while the hybrid structures provide some further insights into the formation of

attitudes, and some gains in efficiency, the overall results remain largely unaffected. Interestingly, the

commonly used socio-demographic variables did not prove to be of significant influence, while

recommendations of surgery from GPs or friends and family had a positive impact in simpler models,

while recommendations from friends and family are of even greater importance when looking at the

hybrid model. Results thus suggest that patients are significantly influenced by peers and less so by

professionals.

All results were discussed in relation to existing knowledge. And further discussions of limitations and of

the validity of the stated choice method was performed. Also, discussions on how results can be put into

practice and on the relationship between patients and HCPs were presented, by introducing novel

approaches to thinking about choices, derived from literature outside of health economics.

Conclusively this thesis makes several contributions to the field of modelling health care choices in

general and preferences in regard to treatment for low back pain in particular.

Page 66: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

118

9 Summary

This thesis is divided into 10 sections providing empirical, theoretical and methodological insight into the

field of preferences elicitation in health economics focusing on treatment of low back pain.

This first section presented the background for this thesis. It introduced the field of back pain, providing

insight into the challenges in decision making in regards to conflicting evidence and the presence of

other factors influencing decisions and outcomes. Literature dealing with back pain was presented and

in summary this pointed to a widely prevalent disorder with severe consequences for sufferers and

society. Persons suffering from back pain often belong to lower social groups and have an extensive use

of health care services. Back pain pathogenesis is multifaceted including both hereditary factors and

lifestyle and treatments are as multifaceted including pain reducing efforts as well as non-surgical cross-

disciplinary management and surgical management. The latter being subject to much discussion

including in a Danish context in which recent changes to guidelines have put focus on finding the right

candidates for surgery.

The research questions of both empirical and methodological matter were presented as a two-fold

effort investigating patients’ preferences for treatment of low back pain and contributing to the field of

preference elicitation in health care in two distinct ways; by focusing on the much overlooked

qualitative work prior to designing stated preference and by exploring the importance of and the

process by which attitudes and perceptions are influencing the choices made by patients.

The introduction also highlighted how the work in this thesis relates to current and historical trends in

investigating hypothetical choices in health care and it presented the contribution to the field while

highlighting the interdependency of the three empirical papers.

The second section provided an introduction to the methods used and the existing knowledge on

patients’ preferences in relation to LBP and how preferences affect outcomes. Observing choices, tell us

something about what people find important. By choosing something over something else it can be

hypothesized that the preferred option gives more utility. It is assume that individuals choose based on

rational thought processes and by trading the positives/negatives of each option available. Also, it is

assumed that some of the factors leading to a choice are observable why some are hidden from a

researcher. Making stated preferences studies enables researchers to include otherwise unobservable,

but key characteristics into a hypothetical, yet plausible choice scenario.

Page 67: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

119

Some research has already been done in relation to preferences for LBP and conclusively literature point

to a field acknowledging the fact that somehow patients’ preferences, expectations and beliefs matter

and can affect treatment outcomes. However, only one existing study uses traditional preference

elicitation methods while the majority simply asks patients to state their preference.

When choosing treatments patients are influenced by previous experience and the level of information

can shape preferences. Patients seem to prefer skilled and experienced HCPs who take them seriously

and communicate clearly. If HCPs exhibit uncertainty this can make patients lose faith and find

information elsewhere.

Getting people to state preferences by observing hypothetical choices is a valid and well-established

way of obtaining useful knowledge about peoples’ motivations and wishes and the method is widely

used within a range of research fields. Designing and analysing stated preference studies calls for a

number of methodological choices to be made by the researcher. As the choice of design and model for

analysis affects results these choices are crucial and should be based on insight into literature as well as

thorough qualitative work looking into the field of interest. Firstly choosing the right attributes and

levels to include in the choice sets is a core task but researcher also need to focus on the presentation of

the choice sets and how these are framed. Further an experimental design must be made combining the

attribute levels into hypothetical scenarios. This can be done in a range of different ways, each with

different advantages and disadvantages. Previously, orthogonal designs were accepted as the best

design, but experimental design has evolved into focusing on maximizing the information on

preferences obtained from each choice made by a respondent over all observations. Decisions on

whether the design should include an option to opt out of choosing and how this should be formulated

should rely on mimicking the real life choice as perfectly as possible. Also researchers should decide

upon whether the choices should be labelled or not depending on whether a label appropriately mimics

the market and does not make respondents disregard attributes and choose based on labels only.

Modelling results from SP-studies used to rely on simple models excluding preference heterogeneity,

but much research has put into modelling taste heterogeneity in a number of ways. Primarily by simply

allowing utility estimates to have random distributions but also by linking choice response to latent

attitudes and perceptions in hybrid structures. In all cases one needs to be observant of potential issues

Page 68: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

120

of scale influencing observed differences, which is often stated as a reason for looking into marginal

rates of substitution, not influenced by scale.

The third section presented the design and data collection for the choice experiment used to generate

data for the empirical contributions. The section introduces empirical details on each step of the

process, highlighting the use of qualitative work that went into the process, and thoroughly describing

all design measures taken in the empirical work from the formulation of a research question to

statistical analysis.

The fourth section presented the first empirical and methodological contribution in a journal paper

format. The section provides an important insight into the essential qualitative work needed to design a

SP-experiment involving the developing, testing and optimizing of the survey. This process is important

for the success of the experiment and the validity of the results, but it is often not reported thoroughly.

The paper demonstrates how qualitative work can significantly impact and guide a design, making it

clear that a less thorough qualitative process would have resulted in a less useable and valid design. A

range of methods were used to gain knowledge prior to making an experimental design. These included

a literature review, fieldwork in clinical departments in Danish hospitals and was supplemented by

qualitative interviews with patients and doctors. The process help select the attributes included in the

experiment and the appropriate levels and also allowed for testing the framing and presentation of

choice sets. It helped determine not to label the experiment as the labels guided choices too much and

made respondents disregard attributes and it helped explore the inclusion of an opt out option which

was kept to mimic real life although disregard by respondents in the think aloud exercises.

Each step of the qualitative process helped guide the final design and it is useful for researchers to

perform similar research prior to designing SP-experiments as validity is increased.

The fifth section presented the empirical investigation of patients’ preferences for treatment of back

pain based on the design developed in the qualitative process. The journal paper argues that since

treatment decisions are distorted by conflicting evidence the inclusion of patient preferences in

decision- and policy making should be emphasized. It builds on results from the SP experiment

conducted in patients referred to a regional spine centre. Analysis was performed using Mixed Logit

models and a sub-group analysis further explored the willingness to wait by dividing respondents into

socio-demographic and disease related categories.

Page 69: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

121

As expected, results show that respondents assign positive utilities to positive treatment outcomes and

disutility to higher risks and longer wait for effects of treatment. Respondents also showed a disutility

towards surgical intervention and were on average willing to wait two years for effect of treatment to

avoid surgery, while they were willing to wait almost three years for effect of treatment, if the

treatment made them free of pain. The mixed logit model captured significant heterogeneity within the

sample for outcomes regarding pain reduction and ability to pursue activities of daily living and in

relation to treatment modality. The sub-group analysis showed differences in willingness to wait,

especially in regards to treatment modality and particularly in groups of high/low pain at time of data

collection and respondents with an ex-ante preference for surgery/non-surgery. The results indicated

that pain relief is the most important factor in choice of treatment, followed by treatment modality and

ability to perform activities of daily living while risk of relapse was the least important factor. This is not

only valuable information for practitioners in their communication with patients and everyday decision-

making, but also important information for policy makers who seek to ensure a utility-maximizing mix of

treatment options for patients.

The sixth section investigated the role of patients’ perceptions in treatment choice. The section served

as the second methodological contribution of introducing an integration of choice and a latent variable

in a hybrid choice model approach.

The paper argues that, possibly particularly in scenarios where clinical evidence is limited or not clear

cut, perceptions that patient forms, either through past experience or through discussion with other

patients and/or medical experts, will play a major role in shaping decisions. The paper thus investigates

the potential benefits in capturing the role that perceptions and attitudes may have in explaining

treatment choices made by patients by including a latent attitude in a joint model of the formation of

perceptions and of the choices. The findings from this model are then contrasted to structures allowing

for simple random heterogeneity.

The results show that while the hybrid structures provide some further insights into the formation of

attitudes, and some gains in efficiency, the overall results remain largely unaffected. Interestingly, the

commonly used socio-demographic variables did not prove to be of significant influence, while

recommendations of surgery from GPs or friends and family had a positive impact in simpler models,

while recommendations from friends and family are of even greater importance when looking at the

hybrid model. Results thus suggest that patients are significantly influenced by peers and less so by

professionals.

Page 70: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

122

The seventh section presented a discussion on how the empirical knowledge links to literature. One

important result is the link between perceptions and attitudes as a driving force for treatment choices

which is observed in numerous studies and confirmed in the study presented in section 6. It was also

shown by that same study that patients were less influenced by recommendations from HCPs and GPs

than what could’ve been expected, but interestingly patients were influenced by advice from peers. The

fact that many choices are a function of group decisions has given rise to an effort to understand the

dynamic nature of group preference formation. Different methodologies for exploring shared or joint

decision making has been propose and are discussed.

Also, a discussion on limitations was given, focusing not only on limitations in the empirical data

collection, but also on a more general level, looking into the criticism of SP-surveys being hypothetical

and not reflecting real live decision making. Testing whether SP results does suffer from hypothetical

bias, by comparing to revealed preference data is rarely done in health economics.

As was stated in the first section, incorporating patients’ preferences into clinical guidelines has been

giving raising attention, but suffers from preferences differing between people, and by preferences

being a moving target. Another way of using knowledge on preferences is thus to elicit theses just prior

to consultations as an onset of discussions between patients and HCPs, thus incorporating individual

measures into decision making in a decision tool.

Finally the section provided discussions on novel methodological approaches to SP-studies. The arrival of

random regret minimisation theory, has led to a hype of activity across different topic areas. In practice,

the benefits of the model are often not clear to analysts, nor are its limitations. But it is acknowledges

that the commonly used random utility maximisation model does not necessarily mimic real decision

making. A mix of decision rules based upon the nature of study and attributes is thus potentially more

accurate.

Looking into heterogeneity in decision rules, in scale and in taste has led to the development of more

and more complex models, requiring more insights and more decisions on model specifications by

researchers. The movement has dramatically changed to flexibility and accuracy of models and the

possibility of providing explanatory factors for observed choices, but has maybe also led to moving

preferences even further away from everyday policy-making and –formation. This is a dilemma choice

modellers need to think about in the future.

Page 71: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

123

Finally, the last section provided a conclusion on the thesis. The two-fold objective of exploring patients’

preferences with back pain and achieving methodological contributions by focusing on qualitative

methods and by focusing on how perceptions influence choices, were met in different ways. A stated

preference study was performed investigating patients’ preferences and prior to designing the study a

range of qualitative methods were used to ensure validity of the design. Lastly preferences of patients

were explained by the inclusion of a latent variable in the modelling of choices.

Page 72: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

124

10 Dansk Sammenfatning

Denne afhandling er inddelt i 10 afsnit, der tilsammen udgør et teoretisk, metodisk og empirisk bidrag til

præferenceafdækning indenfor sundhedsøkonomi med fokus på behandling af lænderygsmerter.

Det første afsnit præsenterede afhandlingens baggrund. Afsnittet introducerede litteratur om

lænderygsmerter og gav indblik i udfordringerne omkring behandlingsvalg, grundet manglende eller

modsatrettet evidens og tilstedeværelsen af andre faktorer, der influerer beslutningstagning og effekter

af behandling. Litteraturen viser at lænderygsmerter har en høj prævalens og har store konsekvenser for

de lidende og for samfundet. Mennesker, der lider af lænderygsmerter tilhører ofte lavere sociale

grupper og har et øget forbrug af sundhedsydelser. Patogenesen for lænderygsmerter er multifaktoriel

og inkluderer både arvelige og livsstils elementer og behandling for lænderygsmerter inkluderer både

ikke-kirurgisk behandling og kirurgi. Sidstnævnte er genstand for stor debat, inklusiv i Danmark, hvor de

seneste kliniske retningslinjer, har fokus på at nedbringe andelen af operationer og udvælge de rette

kandidater til operation.

Forskningsspørgsmålene som forsøgtes besvaret i afhandlingen, var af bade empirisk og metodisk

karakter og blev præsenteret som en todelt indsats, der dels undersøgte patienters præferencer for

behandling af lænderygsmerter og dels gav et metodisk bidrag ved at fokusere på den oversete

kvalitative indsats, der er nødvendig for at designe et præference-studie og ved at undersøge processen

hvormed holdninger og opfattelser influerer patienters behandlingsvalg.

Det første afsnit introducerede også et indblik i afhandlingens indplacering i eksisterende og historiske

trends i udviklingen af præferenceafdækning med fokus på sundhedsrelaterede emner og viste de

empiriske bidrags sammenhæng, såvel som afhandlingens samlede bidrag til feltet.

Det andet afsnit introducerede den anvendte metode og den eksisterende viden om patienternes

præferencer vedrørende lænderygsmerter og hvordan disse påvirkes af opfattelser af sygdommen og

holdninger og erfaringer.

Observation af valg, afslører, hvad folk synes er vigtigt. Ved at vælge noget fremfor noget andet, kan det

antages, at den foretrukne løsning giver større nytte. Det antages, at individer vælger baseret på

rationelle tankeprocesser og ved at afveje fordele og ulemper ved hver mulighed. Det er også antaget,

at nogle af de faktorer, der fører til et valg er observerbare og nogle er skjult for en observatør. At

Page 73: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

125

anvende præferencestudier, gør det muligt for en forsker at inkludere observerbare, men centrale

elementer i et hypotetisk, men plausibelt valg-scenarie. Der findes allerede en række studier vedrørende

præferencer og lænderygsmerter og litteraturen peger på en bred accept af at patienter præferencer

har indflydelse på udfaldet af behandling. Der eksisterer dog kun et studie, der benytter traditionelle

metoder til at afsløre præferencer. Litteraturen viser også, at patienter er influeret af tidligere erfaring

og deres informationsniveau i formationen af præferencer og at patienter foretrækker erfarne

behandlere, der tager patienten alvorligt og udviser stor troværdighed.

Behandling af lænderygsmerter kræver individualiserede behandlingsmuligheder og - beslutninger, der

bygger på patientens individuelle præferencer.

Præferencer baseret på hypotetiske valg er en veletableret måde at opnå brugbar viden om folks

motivationer og ønsker, og metoden er meget udbredt inden for en række forskningsområder. Design

og analyse af præferenceundersøgelser kræver en række metodiske valg, der skal foretages af

forskeren. Valg af design og model til analyse påvirker resultater og da disse valg er afgørende, bør de

være baseret på indsigt i litteraturen samt grundigt kvalitativ arbejde. Valg af de karakteristika, der skal

indgå i præference-afdækningen er helt central, men der skal også fokuseres på hvordan

beslutningsscenarierne beskrives og vises. Ydermere skal et eksperimentelt design, der kombinerer

karakteristika i valgsæt, vælges. Tidligere var disse design baseret på ortogonalitet, men har siden

udviklet sig i retning af at forsøge at maksimere oplysninger om præferencer pr. foretaget valg. Det skal

endvidere besluttes om præference-afdækningseksperimentet skal indeholde en mulighed for at vælge

ikke at vælge, og dette skal forsøge at afspejle det rigtige marked så vidt muligt.

Der findes en række modeller til analyse af præferencestudier. Disse har udviklet i sig retning mod at

kunne håndtere præferenceheterogenitet i en befolkning. Primært ved at tillade fordelinger af

præferencer, men også ved at integrere latente variable med valg i hybrid-strukturer.

Det tredje afsnit præsenterede baggrunden for de empiriske bidrag. Her blev dataindsamling detaljeret

beskrevet. Indsamlingen inkluderede både observationsstudier og interviews, så vel som kvantitative

pilottests forud for det endelig design.

Det fjerde afsnit præsenterede det første empiriske og metodiske bidrag i et tidsskriftsartikelformat.

Afsnittet giver et vigtigt indblik i det væsentlige kvalitative arbejdet forud for at designe et præference-

eksperiment, som omfatter udvikling, test og optimering af undersøgelsen. Denne proces er vigtig for

eksperimentets validitet og gyldigheden af resultaterne, men er ofte ikke rapporteret. Artiklen viser

Page 74: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

126

hvordan kvalitativt arbejde i væsentlig grad kan påvirke og vejlede et design, og gør det klart, at en

mindre grundig kvalitativ proces ville have resulteret i et mindre brugbart design. En række metoder

blev brugt til at få viden om feltet. Disse omfattede en litteraturgennemgang, feltarbejde i kliniske

afdelinger på danske sygehuse og blev suppleret med kvalitative interviews med patienter og læger.

Hvert trin i den kvalitative processen vejledte det endelige design, og det er nyttigt for forskere at

udføre lignende kvalitative tiltag i forbindelse med at designe præference-studier.

Den femte sektion præsenterede den empiriske undersøgelse af patienters præferencer til behandling

af rygsmerter baseret på designet udviklet i den kvalitative proces. Artiklen beskriver at

beslutningsprocessen vedrørende behandling af lænderygsmerter, influereres af den modstridende

evidens og at inddragelse af patienters præferencer er centralt i både klinisk og politisk optik. Artiklen

bygger på data indsamlet på Middelfart Rygcenter og data blev analyseret ved brug af en mixed-logit

model, suppleret af en subgruppeanalyse, der inddelte patienter i socioøkonomiske grupper. Som

forventet viste resultaterne at patienterne tildelte positiv nytte til positive behandlingsresultater og

negativ nytte til længere ventetid, større risici og kirurgi. Patienterne var i gennemsnit villige til at vente

to år på effekt af behandling for at undgå kirurgi, mens de var villige til at vente næsten tre år for effekt

af behandlingen, hvis behandlingen gjorde dem fri for smerter. Modellen fangede betydelig

heterogenitet i forhold til smertereduktion og evnen til at udføre dagligdagsaktiviteter samt

behandlingstype. Resultaterne viste endvidere at smertelindring var den mest centrale faktor i valg af

behandling. Dette er værdigfuld information for behandlere i deres kommunikation med patienter.

Det sjette afsnit præsenterede et studie omhandlende hvordan patienters præferencer for behandling

er influeret af en række faktorer. Afsnittet fungerer samtidigt som det andet metodiske bidrag ved at

introducere hybridmodeller.

Artiklen argumenter at især når klinisk evidens mangler eller er uklar, er der et rum for, at præferencer

kan have en stor rolle i beslutningstagning. Artiklen undersøger fordelene ved at integrere holdninger og

erfaring i en samlet model, der undersøger både latente variable og hypotetiske valg samtidigt.

Resultaterne blev dernæst diskuteret i forhold til modeller, der håndterer forskelle i respondenters valg

på mere udbredt vis.

Resultaterne viser, at mens hybridmodellen giver et større indblik i hvordan holdninger og erfaring

påvirker adfærd og at modellen giver mere præcision. Dog er de overordnede resultater de samme som

i mere simple modeller. Mest interessant er det, at sociodemografiske variable og lægers anbefalinger

Page 75: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

127

ikke synes at have indflydelse på patienternes valg, mens venners og families anbefalinger har stor

indflydelse på valgene.

Det syvende afsnit var en diskussion af hvorledes de empiriske bidrag både bekræfter og tilfører nyt til

den eksisterende viden. Et vigtigt resultat er sammenhængen mellem holdninger som drivkraft for valg

af behandlingsmuligheder, der både er observeret i adskillige undersøgelser og bekræftet i

undersøgelsen præsenteresti afsnit 6. Det blev også vist ved den samme undersøgelse, at patienterne

var mindre påvirket af anbefalinger fra praktiserende læger og andre sundhedsprofessionelle end hvad

der kunne have været forventet, men patienter blev påvirket af rådgivning fra ligemænd. Det faktum, at

mange valg er en funktion af en gruppes beslutninger, har givet anledning til en indsats for at forstå

dynamikken bag gruppes præferencedannelse. Forskellige metoder til at udforske delt eller fælles

beslutningstagning bliver foreslået og diskuteres. Derudover diskuteres studiernes begrænsninger og

metodiske begrænsninger, hvor kritikken om den hypotetiske natur i præferencestudier debatteres. Det

foreslås at flere sundhedsøkonomer fokuserer på at teste om de hypotetiske valg afspejler virkelige valg.

Som anført i det første afsnit, har indarbejdelse af patienters præferencer i kliniske retningslinjer været

givet opmærksomhed, men lider under heterogenitet og præferencers ikke statiske anlæg. En anden

måde at inddrage præferencer er således at undersøge disse i forbindelse med mødet mellem behandler

og patient. Endelig diskuteres nye metodiske tiltag og strømninger indenfor præferencelitteraturen,

men fokus på minimering af ubehag, som beslutningsregel og håndtering af heterogenitet i form af

skalaforskelle, præferencer og beslutningsregler. Udviklingen af modeller, har betydet en dramatisk

forbedring i håndteringen af heterogenitet, men har også betydet en væsentlig øget kompleksitet, der

måske er med til at bringe præferencestudier endnu længere væk fra klinisk og især politiske

beslutningstagning.

Endelig giver det sidste afsnit en konklusion på afhandlingen. Det dobbelte formål med at udforske

patienternes præferencer for behandling af rygsmerter og opnå metodiske bidrag ved at fokusere på

kvalitative metoder og ved at fokusere på, hvordan opfattelser påvirker valg, blev opfyldt på forskellige

måder. Patienters præferencer blev studeret og beskrevet og forud derfor blev grundigt kvalitativt

arbejde udført. Endelig blev patienters beslutninger undersøgt i forhold til disses påvirkning af latente

variable og i forhold til mere almindelige modeller.

Page 76: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

128

11 References for sections 1, 2, 3 and 7

1. Ekholm O, Kjøller M, Davidsen M, et al. Sundhed og sygelighed i Danmark og udviklingen siden 1987.; 2005.

2. Croft PR, Macfarlane GJ, Papageorgiou AC, Thomas E, Silman AJ. Outcome of low back pain in general practice: a prospective study. BMJ. 1998;316(7141):1356-1359. doi:10.1136/bmj.316.7141.1356.

3. Maniadakis N, Gray A. The economic burden of back pain in the UK. Pain. 2000;84(1):95-103.

4. Chou R, Baisden J, Carragee EJ, Resnick DK, Shaffer WO, Loeser JD. Surgery for low back pain: a review of the evidence for an American Pain Society Clinical Practice Guideline. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009;34(10):1094-109. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181a105fc.

5. Deyo RA, Battie M, Beurskens AJ, et al. Outcome measures for low back pain research. A proposal for standardized use. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1998;23(18):2003-13.

6. Van Tulder M, Koes B, Bombardier C. Low back pain. Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol. 2002;16(5):761-75.

7. Allen T, Rihn JF, Glassman SD, Currier BL, Albert TJ PF. An Evidence-Based Approach to Spine Surgery. Am J Med Qual. 2009;24.

8. Gibson JNA, Waddell G. Surgery for degenerative lumbar spondylosis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2005;(4):CD001352. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD001352.pub3.

9. Bederman SS, Coyte PC, Kreder HJ, Mahomed NN, McIsaac WJ, Wright JG. Who’s in the driver's seat? the influence of patient and physician enthusiasm on regional variation in degenerative lumbar spinal surgery: A population-based study. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2011;36(6) :(pp 481-489), 2011. Date of Publication: 15 Mar 2011.):489.

10. Bederman S, Mahomed NN, Kreder HJ, McIsaac WJ, Coyte PC, Wright JG. In the eye of the beholder: preferences of patients, family physicians, and surgeons for lumbar spinal surgery. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2010;35(1):108-15. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181b77f2d.

11. Irwin ZN, Hilibrand A, Gustavel M, et al. Variation in surgical decision making for degenerative spinal disorders. Part II: cervical spine. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2005;30(19):2214-9.

12. Arega A, Birkmeyer NJO, Lurie JDN, et al. Racial variation in treatment preferences and willingness to randomize in the Spine Patient Outcomes Research Trial (SPORT). Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2006;31(19):2263-2269.

13. Battié MC, Cherkin DC, Dunn R, Ciol M a, Wheeler KJ. Managing low back pain: attitudes and treatment preferences of physical therapists. Phys Ther. 1994;74(3):219-26.

Page 77: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

129

14. Barrett PH, Beck A, Schmid K, Fireman B, Brown JB. Treatment decisions about lumbar herniated disk in a shared decision-making program. Jt Comm J Qual Improv. 2002;28(5):211-9.

15. Balagué F, Mannion AF, Pellisé F, Cedraschi C. Non-specific low back pain. Lancet. 2012;379(9814):482-91. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60610-7.

16. Owens D. Patient Preferences and the Development og Practice Guidelines. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1998;23(9).

17. De Bekker-Grob EW, Ryan M, Gerard K. Discrete choice experiments in health economics: a review of the literature. Health Econ. 2012;21(2):145-72. doi:10.1002/hec.1697.

18. Adamson J, Hunt K, Nazareth I. The influence of socio-demographic characteristics on consultation for back pain--a review of the literature. Fam Pract. 2011;28(2):163-71. doi:10.1093/fampra/cmq085.

19. Jeffries LJ, Milanese SF, Grimmer-Somers K a. Epidemiology of adolescent spinal pain: a systematic overview of the research literature. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2007;32(23):2630-7. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e318158d70b.

20. Jordan KP, Kadam UT, Hayward R, Porcheret M, Young C, Croft P. Annual consultation prevalence of regional musculoskeletal problems in primary care: an observational study. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2010;11:144. doi:10.1186/1471-2474-11-144.

21. Plénet a, Gourmelen J, Chastang J-F, Ozguler a, Lanoë J-L, Leclerc a. Seeking care for lower back pain in the French population aged from 30 to 69: the results of the 2002-2003 Décennale Santé survey. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2010;53(4):224-31, 231-8. doi:10.1016/j.rehab.2010.03.006.

22. Puts MTE, Deeg DJH, Hoeymans N, Nusselder WJ, Schellevis FG. Changes in the prevalence of chronic disease and the association with disability in the older Dutch population between 1987 and 2001. Age Ageing. 2008;37(2):187-93. doi:10.1093/ageing/afm185.

23. Urquhart DM, Shortreed S, Davis SR, Cicuttini FM, Bell RJ. Are low levels of low back pain intensity and disability associated with reduced well-being in community-based women? Climacteric. 2009;12(3):266-75. doi:10.1080/13697130802635645.

24. Costa L d. CM, Maher CG, McAuley JH, et al. Prognosis for patients with chronic low back pain: inception cohort study. BMJ. 2009;339(oct06 2):b3829-b3829. doi:10.1136/bmj.b3829.

25. Stanton TR, Henschke N, Maher CG, Refshauge KM, Latimer J, McAuley JH. After an episode of acute low back pain, recurrence is unpredictable and not as common as previously thought. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2008;33(26):2923-8. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e31818a3167.

26. Stanton TR, Latimer J, Maher CG, Hancock MJ. How do we define the condition “recurrent low back pain”? A systematic review. Eur Spine J. 2010;19(4):533-9. doi:10.1007/s00586-009-1214-3.

Page 78: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

130

27. Von Korff M, Ormel J, Keefe FJ, Dworkin SF. Grading the severity of chronic pain. Pain. 1992;50(2):133-49.

28. Koch MB, Davidsen M, Juel K. De samfundsmæssige omkostninger ved rygsygdomme og rygsmerter i Danmark.; 2011.

29. De Schepper EIT, Damen J, van Meurs JBJ, et al. The association between lumbar disc degeneration and low back pain: the influence of age, gender, and individual radiographic features. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2010;35(5):531-6. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181aa5b33.

30. Cheung KMC, Karppinen J, Chan D, et al. Prevalence and pattern of lumbar magnetic resonance imaging changes in a population study of one thousand forty-three individuals. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009;34(9):934-40. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181a01b3f.

31. Endean A, Palmer KT, Coggon D. Potential of magnetic resonance imaging findings to refine case definition for mechanical low back pain in epidemiological studies: a systematic review. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2011;36(2):160-9. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181cd9adb.

32. Kapellen PJ, Beall DP. Imaging evaluation of low back pain: important imaging features associated with clinical symptoms. Semin Roentgenol. 2010;45(3):218-25. doi:10.1053/j.ro.2009.12.001.

33. Battié MC, Videman T, Levalahti E, Gill K, Kaprio J. Heritability of low back pain and the role of disc degeneration. Pain. 2007;131(3):272-80. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.01.010.

34. Reimann F, Cox JJ, Belfer I, et al. Pain perception is altered by a nucleotide polymorphism in SCN9A. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(11):5148-53. doi:10.1073/pnas.0913181107.

35. Tegeder I, Lötsch J. Current evidence for a modulation of low back pain by human genetic variants. J Cell Mol Med. 2009;13(8B):1605-19. doi:10.1111/j.1582-4934.2009.00703.x.

36. Karppinen J, Solovieva S, Luoma K, Raininko R, Leino-Arjas P, Riihimäki H. Modic changes and interleukin 1 gene locus polymorphisms in occupational cohort of middle-aged men. Eur Spine J. 2009;18(12):1963-70. doi:10.1007/s00586-009-1139-x.

37. Dai F, Belfer I, Schwartz CE, et al. Association of catechol-O-methyltransferase genetic variants with outcome in patients undergoing surgical treatment for lumbar degenerative disc disease. Spine J. 2010;10(11):949-57. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2010.07.387.

38. Wang H, Schiltenwolf M, Buchner M. The role of TNF-alpha in patients with chronic low back pain-a prospective comparative longitudinal study. Clin J Pain. 2008;24(3):273-8. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e31816111d3.

39. Yamauchi K, Inoue G, Koshi T, et al. Nerve growth factor of cultured medium extracted from human degenerative nucleus pulposus promotes sensory nerve growth and induces substance p in vitro. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009;34(21):2263-9. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181a5521d.

Page 79: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

131

40. Kalichman L, Hunter DJ. The genetics of intervertebral disc degeneration. Associated genes. Joint Bone Spine. 2008;75(4):388-96. doi:10.1016/j.jbspin.2007.11.002.

41. Kalichman L, Hunter DJ. The genetics of intervertebral disc degeneration. Familial predisposition and heritability estimation. Joint Bone Spine. 2008;75(4):383-7. doi:10.1016/j.jbspin.2007.11.003.

42. Shiri R, Karppinen J, Leino-Arjas P, Solovieva S, Viikari-Juntura E. The association between smoking and low back pain: a meta-analysis. Am J Med. 2010;123(1):87.e7-35. doi:10.1016/j.amjmed.2009.05.028.

43. Shiri R, Karppinen J, Leino-Arjas P, Solovieva S, Viikari-Juntura E. The association between obesity and low back pain: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;171(2):135-54. doi:10.1093/aje/kwp356.

44. Roffey DM, Wai EK, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of occupational sitting and low back pain: results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(3):252-61. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2009.12.005.

45. Wai EK, Roffey DM, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of occupational bending or twisting and low back pain: results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(1):76-88. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2009.06.005.

46. Roffey DM, Wai EK, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of awkward occupational postures and low back pain: results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(1):89-99. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2009.09.003.

47. Roffey DM, Wai EK, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of occupational standing or walking and low back pain: results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(3):262-72. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2009.12.023.

48. Wai EK, Roffey DM, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of occupational carrying and low back pain: results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(7):628-38. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2010.03.027.

49. Roffey DM, Wai EK, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of workplace manual handling or assisting patients and low back pain: results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(7):639-51. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2010.04.028.

50. Wai EK, Roffey DM, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of occupational lifting and low back pain : results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(6):554-566. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2010.03.033.

51. Roffey DM, Wai EK, Bishop P, Kwon BK, Dagenais S. Causal assessment of occupational pushing or pulling and low back pain: results of a systematic review. Spine J. 2010;10(6):544-53. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2010.03.025.

Page 80: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

132

52. National Advisory Committee on Health and Disability. New Zealand. Guide to Assessing Psychosocial Yellow Flags in Acute Low Back Pain. 1997.

53. Chou R. Will This Patient Develop Persistent Disabling Low Back Pain ? JAMA. 2013;303(13):1295-1302.

54. Linton SJ, Vlaeyen J, Ostelo R. The back pain beliefs of health care providers: are we fear-avoidant? J Occup Rehabil. 2002;12(4):223-32.

55. Coudeyre E, Rannou F, Tubach F, et al. General practitioners’ fear-avoidance beliefs influence their management of patients with low back pain. Pain. 2006;124(3):330-7. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.05.003.

56. Kent P, Keating J. Do primary-care clinicians think that nonspecific low back pain is one condition? Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2004;29(9):1022-31.

57. Sheehan NJ. Magnetic resonance imaging for low back pain: indications and limitations. Postgrad Med J. 2010;86(1016):374-8. doi:10.1136/ard.2009.110973.

58. Maus T. Imaging the back pain patient. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am. 2010;21(4):725-66. doi:10.1016/j.pmr.2010.07.004.

59. Dagenais S, Tricco AC, Haldeman S. Synthesis of recommendations for the assessment and management of low back pain from recent clinical practice guidelines. Spine J. 2010;10(6):514-29. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2010.03.032.

60. Chou R. Pharmacological management of low back pain. Drugs. 2010;70(4):387-402. doi:10.2165/11318690-000000000-00000.

61. Deyo R a., Mirza SK, Turner J a., Martin BI. Overtreating Chronic Back Pain: Time to Back Off? J Am Board Fam Med. 2009;22(1):62-68. doi:10.3122/jabfm.2009.01.080102.

62. Brox JI, Nygaard ØP, Holm I, Keller A, Ingebrigtsen T, Reikerås O. Four-year follow-up of surgical versus non-surgical therapy for chronic low back pain. Ann Rheum Dis. 2010;69(9):1643-8. doi:10.1136/ard.2009.108902.

63. Fairbank J, Frost H, Wilson-MacDonald J, Yu L-M, Barker K, Collins R. Randomised controlled trial to compare surgical stabilisation of the lumbar spine with an intensive rehabilitation programme for patients with chronic low back pain: the MRC spine stabilisation trial. BMJ. 2005;330(7502):1233. doi:10.1136/bmj.38441.620417.8F.

64. Brox JI, Sørensen R, Friis A, et al. Randomized clinical trial of lumbar instrumented fusion and cognitive intervention and exercises in patients with chronic low back pain and disc degeneration. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2003;28(17):1913-21. doi:10.1097/01.BRS.0000083234.62751.7A.

Page 81: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

133

65. Brox JI, Reikerås O, Nygaard Ø, et al. Lumbar instrumented fusion compared with cognitive intervention and exercises in patients with chronic back pain after previous surgery for disc herniation: a prospective randomized controlled study. Pain. 2006;122(1-2):145-55. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.01.027.

66. Sundhedsministeriet. Retningslinjer for Rygområdet. 2010.

67. Region Syddanmark. Patientforløbsprogram for rygområdet i Region Syddanmark. 2010;(September):1-54.

68. Bridges JFP, Hauber a B, Marshall D, et al. Conjoint analysis applications in health--a checklist: a report of the ISPOR Good Research Practices for Conjoint Analysis Task Force. Value Health. 2011;14(4):403-13. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2010.11.013.

69. Lancsar E, Wildman J, Donaldson C, Ryan M, Baker R. Deriving distributional weights for QALYs through discrete choice experiments. J Heal Econ. 2011;30(2):466-478. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2011.01.003.

70. Baker R, Bateman I, Donaldson C, et al. Weighting and valuing quality-adjusted life-years using stated preference methods: preliminary results from the Social Value of a QALY Project. Heal Technol Assess. 2010;14(27):1-162. doi:10.3310/hta14270.

71. Robinson A, Gyrd-Hansen D, Bacon P, Baker R, Pennington M, Donaldson C. Estimating a WTP-based value of a QALY: the “chained” approach. Soc Sci Med. 2013;92:92-104. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.05.013.

72. Flynn TN. Using conjoint analysis and choice experiments to estimate QALY values: issues to consider. Pharmacoeconomics. 2010;28(9):711-722. doi:10.2165/11535660-000000000-00000.

73. Ratcliffe J, Couzner L, Flynn T, et al. Valuing Child Health Utility 9D health states with a young adolescent sample: a feasibility study to compare best-worst scaling discrete-choice experiment, standard gamble and time trade-off methods. Appl Heal Econ Heal Policy. 2011;9(1):15-27. doi:10.2165/11536960-000000000-00000.

74. Stolk EA, Oppe M, Scalone L, Krabbe PF. Discrete choice modeling for the quantification of health states: the case of the EQ-5D. Value Heal. 2010;13(8):1005-1013. doi:10.1111/j.1524-4733.2010.00783.x.

75. Flynn TN, Louviere JJ, Peters TJ, Coast J. Using discrete choice experiments to understand preferences for quality of life. Variance-scale heterogeneity matters. Soc Sci Med. 2010;70(12):1957-1965. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.03.008.

76. Ramos-Goni JM, Rivero-Arias O, Errea M, Stolk EA, Herdman M, Cabases JM. Dealing with the health state “dead” when using discrete choice experiments to obtain values for EQ-5D-5L heath states. Eur J Heal Econ. 2013;14 Suppl 1:S33-42. doi:10.1007/s10198-013-0511-2.

Page 82: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

134

77. Gu NY, Botteman MF, Gerber RA, et al. Eliciting health state utilities for Dupuytren’s contracture using a discrete choice experiment. Acta Orthop. 2013;84(6):571-578. doi:10.3109/17453674.2013.865097.

78. Singh J, Longworth L, Baine A, Lord J, Orr S, Buxton M. Exploring what lies behind public preferences for avoiding health losses caused by lapses in healthcare safety and patient lifestyle choices. BMC Heal Serv Res. 2013;13:249. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-13-249.

79. Kolstad JR. How to make rural jobs more attractive to health workers. Findings from a discrete choice experiment in Tanzania. Heal Econ. 2011;20(2):196-211. doi:10.1002/hec.1581.

80. Lagarde M, Smith Paintain L, Antwi G, et al. Evaluating health workers’ potential resistance to new interventions: a role for discrete choice experiments. PLoS One. 2011;6(8):e23588. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023588.

81. Huicho L, Miranda JJ, Diez-Canseco F, et al. Job preferences of nurses and midwives for taking up a rural job in Peru: a discrete choice experiment. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e50315. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0050315.

82. Robyn PJ, Barnighausen T, Souares A, et al. Health worker preferences for community-based health insurance payment mechanisms: a discrete choice experiment. BMC Heal Serv Res. 2012;12:159. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-12-159.

83. Rockers PC, Jaskiewicz W, Wurts L, et al. Preferences for working in rural clinics among trainee health professionals in Uganda: a discrete choice experiment. BMC Heal Serv Res. 2012;12:212. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-12-212.

84. Ageyi-Baffour P, Rominski S, Nakua E, Gyakobo M, Lori JR. Factors that influence midwifery students in Ghana when deciding where to practice: a discrete choice experiment. BMC Med Educ. 2013;13:64. doi:10.1186/1472-6920-13-64.

85. Bridges JF, Dong L, Gallego G, Blauvelt BM, Joy SM, Pawlik TM. Prioritizing strategies for comprehensive liver cancer control in Asia: a conjoint analysis. BMC Heal Serv Res. 2012;12:376. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-12-376.

86. Watson V, Carnon A, Ryan M, Cox D. Involving the public in priority setting: a case study using discrete choice experiments. J Public Heal. 2012;34(2):253-260. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdr102.

87. Youngkong S, Baltussen R, Tantivess S, Koolman X, Teerawattananon Y. Criteria for priority setting of HIV/AIDS interventions in Thailand: a discrete choice experiment. BMC Heal Serv Res. 2010;10:197. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-10-197.

88. Mirelman A, Mentzakis E, Kinter E, et al. Decision-making criteria among national policymakers in five countries: a discrete choice experiment eliciting relative preferences for equity and efficiency. Value Heal. 2012;15(3):534-539. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2012.04.001.

Page 83: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

135

89. Green C, Gerard K. Exploring the social value of health-care interventions: a stated preference discrete choice experiment. Heal Econ. 2009;18(8):951-976. doi:10.1002/hec.1414.

90. Pickard AS, Tawk R, Shaw JW. The effect of chronic conditions on stated preferences for health. Eur J Heal Econ. 2013;14(4):697-702. doi:10.1007/s10198-012-0421-8.

91. Veldwijk J, Lambooij MS, van Gils PF, Struijs JN, Smit HA, de Wit GA. Type 2 diabetes patients’ preferences and willingness to pay for lifestyle programs: a discrete choice experiment. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1099. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-13-1099.

92. Guimaraes C, Marra CA, Colley L, et al. Socioeconomic differences in preferences and willingness-to-pay for insulin delivery systems in type 1 and type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Technol Ther. 2009;11(9):567-573. doi:10.1089/dia.2009.0034.

93. Yeo ST, Edwards RT, Fargher EA, Luzio SD, Thomas RL, Owens DR. Preferences of people with diabetes for diabetic retinopathy screening: a discrete choice experiment. Diabet Med. 2012;29(7):869-877. doi:10.1111/j.1464-5491.2012.03636.x.

94. Bogelund M, Vilsboll T, Faber J, Henriksen JE, Gjesing RP, Lammert M. Patient preferences for diabetes management among people with type 2 diabetes in Denmark - a discrete choice experiment. Curr Med Res Opin. 2011;27(11):2175-2183. doi:10.1185/03007995.2011.625404.

95. Chen TT, Chung KP, Huang HC, Man LN, Lai MS. Using discrete choice experiment to elicit doctors’ preferences for the report card design of diabetes care in Taiwan - a pilot study. J Eval Clin Pr. 2010;16(1):14-20. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2753.2008.01105.x.

96. Porzsolt F, Clouth J, Deutschmann M, Hippler HJ. Preferences of diabetes patients and physicians: a feasibility study to identify the key indicators for appraisal of health care values. Heal Qual Life Outcomes. 2010;8:125. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-8-125.

97. Park H, Chon Y, Lee J, Choi Ie J, Yoon KH. Service design attributes affecting diabetic patient preferences of telemedicine in South Korea. Telemed J E Heal. 2011;17(6):442-451. doi:10.1089/tmj.2010.0201.

98. Joy SM, Little E, Maruthur NM, Purnell TS, Bridges JF. Patient preferences for the treatment of type 2 diabetes: a scoping review. Pharmacoeconomics. 2013;31(10):877-892. doi:10.1007/s40273-013-0089-7.

99. Gelhorn HL, Stringer SM, Brooks A, et al. Preferences for medication attributes among patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus in the UK. Diabetes Obes Metab. 2013;15(9):802-809. doi:10.1111/dom.12091.

100. Kobayashi M, Mano T, Yamauchi K. Patients’ preference on selecting a medical institution. Int J Heal Care Qual Assur. 2013;26(4):341-352.

Page 84: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

136

101. Poulos C, Yang JC, Levin C, Van Minh H, Giang KB, Nguyen D. Mothers’ preferences and willingness to pay for HPV vaccines in Vinh Long Province, Vietnam. Soc Sci Med. 2011;73(2):226-234. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.05.029.

102. Essers BA, Dirksen CD, Prins MH, Neumann HA. Assessing the public’s preference for surgical treatment of primary basal cell carcinoma: a discrete-choice experiment in the south of the Netherlands. Dermatol Surg. 2010;36(12):1950-1955. doi:10.1111/j.1524-4725.2010.01805.x.

103. Park MH, Jo C, Bae EY, Lee EK. A comparison of preferences of targeted therapy for metastatic renal cell carcinoma between the patient group and health care professional group in South Korea. Value Heal. 2012;15(6):933-939. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2012.05.008.

104. Tinelli M, Ozolins M, Bath-Hextall F, Williams HC. What determines patient preferences for treating low risk basal cell carcinoma when comparing surgery vs imiquimod? A discrete choice experiment survey from the SINS trial. BMC Dermatol. 2012;12:19. doi:10.1186/1471-5945-12-19.

105. Taguchi R, Yamazaki Y, Nakayama K. [Eliciting preferences for mammography: using a discrete choice experiment approach]. Nihon Koshu Eisei Zasshi. 2010;57(2):83-94.

106. Coast J, Flynn T, Sutton E, et al. Investigating Choice Experiments for Preferences of Older People (ICEPOP): evaluative spaces in health economics. J Heal Serv Res Policy. 2008;13 Suppl 3:31-37. doi:10.1258/jhsrp.2008.008024.

107. Holdsworth L, King A. Preferences for end of life: views of hospice patients, family carers, and community nurse specialists. Int J Palliat Nurs. 2011;17(5):251-255.

108. Davison SN, Kromm SK, Currie GR. Patient and health professional preferences for organ allocation and procurement, end-of-life care and organization of care for patients with chronic kidney disease using a discrete choice experiment. Nephrol Dial Transpl. 2010;25(7):2334-2341. doi:10.1093/ndt/gfq072.

109. Eberth B, Watson V, Ryan M, Hughes J, Barnett G. Does one size fit all? Investigating heterogeneity in men’s preferences for benign prostatic hyperplasia treatment using mixed logit analysis. Med Decis Mak. 2009;29(6):707-715. doi:10.1177/0272989x09341754.

110. Johnson FR, Hauber AB, Ozdemir S, Lynd L. Quantifying women’s stated benefit-risk trade-off preferences for IBS treatment outcomes. Value Heal. 2010;13(4):418-423. doi:10.1111/j.1524-4733.2010.00694.x.

111. Shingler SL, Swinburn P, Ali S, Perard R, Lloyd AJ. A discrete choice experiment to determine patient preferences for injection devices in multiple sclerosis. J Med Econ. 2013;16(8):1036-1042. doi:10.3111/13696998.2013.811079.

112. Van Helvoort-Postulart D, Dellaert BG, van der Weijden T, von Meyenfeldt MF, Dirksen CD. Discrete choice experiments for complex health-care decisions: does hierarchical information integration offer a solution? Heal Econ. 2009;18(8):903-920. doi:10.1002/hec.1411.

Page 85: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

137

113. Howard K, Salkeld G, Pignone M, et al. Preferences for CT colonography and colonoscopy as diagnostic tests for colorectal cancer: a discrete choice experiment. Value Heal. 2011;14(8):1146-1152. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2011.07.012.

114. Scuffham PA, Whitty JA, Taylor M, Saxby RC. Health system choice: a pilot discrete-choice experiment eliciting the preferences of British and Australian citizens. Appl Heal Econ Heal Policy. 2010;8(2):89-97. doi:10.2165/11531170-000000000-00000.

115. Blaauw D, Erasmus E, Pagaiya N, et al. Policy interventions that attract nurses to rural areas: a multicountry discrete choice experiment. Bull World Heal Organ. 2010;88(5):350-356. doi:10.2471/blt.09.072918.

116. Howard K, Salkeld G. Does attribute framing in discrete choice experiments influence willingness to pay? Results from a discrete choice experiment in screening for colorectal cancer. Value Heal. 2009;12(2):354-363. doi:10.1111/j.1524-4733.2008.00417.x.

117. Kruk ME, Johnson JC, Gyakobo M, et al. Rural practice preferences among medical students in Ghana: a discrete choice experiment. Bull World Heal Organ. 2010;88(5):333-341. doi:10.2471/blt.09.072892.

118. Fegert JM, Slawik L, Wermelskirchen D, Nubling M, Muhlbacher A. Assessment of parents’ preferences for the treatment of school-age children with ADHD: a discrete choice experiment. Expert Rev Pharmacoecon Outcomes Res. 2011;11(3):245-252. doi:10.1586/erp.11.22.

119. Bridges JF, Mohamed AF, Finnern HW, Woehl A, Hauber AB. Patients’ preferences for treatment outcomes for advanced non-small cell lung cancer: a conjoint analysis. Lung Cancer. 2012;77(1):224-231. doi:10.1016/j.lungcan.2012.01.016.

120. Kjaer T, Gyrd-Hansen D. Preference heterogeneity and choice of cardiac rehabilitation program: results from a discrete choice experiment. Health Policy. 2008;85(1):124-32. doi:10.1016/j.healthpol.2007.07.002.

121. Parkinson B, Goodall S, Norman R. Measuring the loss of consumer choice in mandatory health programmes using discrete choice experiments. Appl Heal Econ Heal Policy. 2013;11(2):139-150. doi:10.1007/s40258-013-0017-1.

122. Benning TM, Kimman ML, Dirksen CD, Boersma LJ, Dellaert BG. Combining individual-level discrete choice experiment estimates and costs to inform health care management decisions about customized care: the case of follow-up strategies after breast cancer treatment. Value Heal. 2012;15(5):680-689. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2012.04.007.

123. Laba TL, Brien JA, Fransen M, Jan S. Patient preferences for adherence to treatment for osteoarthritis: the MEdication Decisions in Osteoarthritis Study (MEDOS). BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2013;14:160. doi:10.1186/1471-2474-14-160.

Page 86: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

138

124. Sweeting KR, Whitty JA, Scuffham PA, Yelland MJ. Patient preferences for treatment of achilles tendon pain: results from a discrete-choice experiment. Patient. 2011;4(1):45-54. doi:10.2165/11532830-000000000-00000.

125. Chan-Halbrendt C, Lin T, Yang F, Sisior G. Hawaiian residents’ preferences for Miconia control program attributes using conjoint choice experiment and latent class analysis. Env Manag. 2010;45(2):250-260. doi:10.1007/s00267-009-9415-4.

126. Lagarde M. Investigating attribute non-attendance and its consequences in choice experiments with latent class models. Heal Econ. 2013;22(5):554-567. doi:10.1002/hec.2824.

127. Shen J, Saijo T. Does an energy efficiency label alter consumers’ purchasing decisions? A latent class approach based on a stated choice experiment in Shanghai. J Env Manag. 2009;90(11):3561-3573. doi:10.1016/j.jenvman.2009.06.010.

128. Guo N, Marra CA, FitzGerald JM, Elwood RK, Anis AH, Marra F. Patient preference for latent tuberculosis infection preventive treatment: a discrete choice experiment. Value Heal. 2011;14(6):937-943. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2011.05.003.

129. Cunningham CE, Henderson J, Niccols A, et al. Preferences for evidence-based practice dissemination in addiction agencies serving women: a discrete-choice conjoint experiment. Addiction. 2012;107(8):1512-1524. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03832.x.

130. Carroll FE, Al-Janabi H, Flynn T, Montgomery AA. Women and their partners’ preferences for Down's syndrome screening tests: a discrete choice experiment. Prenat Diagn. 2013;33(5):449-456. doi:10.1002/pd.4086.

131. Cunningham CE, Deal K, Rimas H, et al. Modeling the information preferences of parents of children with mental health problems: a discrete choice conjoint experiment. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008;36(7):1123-1138. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9238-4.

132. Hole AR. Modelling heterogeneity in patients’ preferences for the attributes of a general practitioner appointment. J Heal Econ. 2008;27(4):1078-1094. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.11.006.

133. De Bekker-Grob EW, Rose JM, Donkers B, Essink-Bot ML, Bangma CH, Steyerberg EW. Men’s preferences for prostate cancer screening: a discrete choice experiment. Br J Cancer. 2013;108(3):533-541. doi:10.1038/bjc.2013.5.

134. Cunningham CE, Deal K, Rimas H, Chen Y, Buchanan DH, Sdao-Jarvie K. Providing information to parents of children with mental health problems: a discrete choice conjoint analysis of professional preferences. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(8):1089-1102. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9338-9.

135. Cunningham CE, Chen Y, Deal K, et al. The interim service preferences of parents waiting for children’s mental health treatment: a discrete choice conjoint experiment. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41(6):865-877. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9728-x.

Page 87: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

139

136. Mentzakis E, Ryan M, McNamee P. Using discrete choice experiments to value informal care tasks: exploring preference heterogeneity. Heal Econ. 2011;20(8):930-944. doi:10.1002/hec.1656.

137. Cunningham CE, Vaillancourt T, Rimas H, et al. Modeling the bullying prevention program preferences of educators: a discrete choice conjoint experiment. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):929-943. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9324-2.

138. Cunningham CE, Kostrzewa L, Rimas H, et al. Modeling organizational justice improvements in a pediatric health service : a discrete-choice conjoint experiment. Patient. 2013;6(1):45-59. doi:10.1007/s40271-013-0002-7.

139. Naik-Panvelkar P, Armour C, Rose JM, Saini B. Patient preferences for community pharmacy asthma services: a discrete choice experiment. Pharmacoeconomics. 2012;30(10):961-976. doi:10.2165/11594350-000000000-00000.

140. Whitty JA, Stewart S, Carrington MJ, et al. Patient preferences and willingness-to-pay for a home or clinic based program of chronic heart failure management: findings from the Which? trial. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e58347. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058347.

141. Cunningham CE, Vaillancourt T, Cunningham LJ, Chen Y, Ratcliffe J. Modeling the bullying prevention program design recommendations of students from grades five to eight: a discrete choice conjoint experiment. Aggress Behav. 2011;37(6):521-537. doi:10.1002/ab.20408.

142. Waschbusch DA, Cunningham CE, Pelham WE, et al. A discrete choice conjoint experiment to evaluate parent preferences for treatment of young, medication naive children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40(4):546-561. doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.581617.

143. Cunningham CE, Bruce BS, Snowdon AW, et al. Modeling improvements in booster seat use: a discrete choice conjoint experiment. Accid Anal Prev. 2011;43(6):1999-2009. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2011.05.018.

144. Grindrod KA, Marra CA, Colley L, Tsuyuki RT, Lynd LD. Pharmacists’ preferences for providing patient-centered services: a discrete choice experiment to guide health policy. Ann Pharmacother. 2010;44(10):1554-1564. doi:10.1345/aph.1P228.

145. Araña JE, León CJ, Hanemann MW. Emotions and decision rules in discrete choice experiments for valuing health care programmes for the elderly. J Health Econ. 2008;27(3):753-69. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.10.003.

146. Bech M, Kjaer T, Lauridsen J. Does the number of choice sets matter? Results from a web survey applying a discrete choice experiment. Health Econ. 2011;20(3):273-86. doi:10.1002/hec.1587.

147. Bridges JF, Kinter ET, Schmeding A, Rudolph I, Muhlbacher A. Can patients diagnosed with schizophrenia complete choice-based conjoint analysis tasks? Patient. 2011;4(4):267-275. doi:10.2165/11589190-000000000-00000.

Page 88: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

140

148. Bridges JF, Lataille AT, Buttorff C, White S, Niparko JK. Consumer preferences for hearing aid attributes: a comparison of rating and conjoint analysis methods. Trends Amplif. 2012;16(1):40-48. doi:10.1177/1084713811434617.

149. Muhlbacher AC, Rudolph I, Lincke HJ, Nubling M. Preferences for treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): a discrete choice experiment. BMC Heal Serv Res. 2009;9:149. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-9-149.

150. Muhlbacher AC, Nubling M. Analysis of physicians’ perspectives versus patients' preferences: direct assessment and discrete choice experiments in the therapy of multiple myeloma. Eur J Heal Econ. 2011;12(3):193-203. doi:10.1007/s10198-010-0218-6.

151. Gunther OH, Kurstein B, Riedel-Heller SG, Konig HH. The role of monetary and nonmonetary incentives on the choice of practice establishment: a stated preference study of young physicians in Germany. Heal Serv Res. 2010;45(1):212-229. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6773.2009.01045.x.

152. Silverman S, Calderon A, Kaw K, et al. Patient weighting of osteoporosis medication attributes across racial and ethnic groups: a study of osteoporosis medication preferences using conjoint analysis. Osteoporos Int. 2013;24(7):2067-2077. doi:10.1007/s00198-012-2241-1.

153. Pisa G, Freytag S, Schandry R. Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) patients’ disease-related preferences : a study using conjoint analysis. Patient. 2013;6(2):93-101. doi:10.1007/s40271-013-0007-2.

154. Shah A, Efstathiou JA, Paly JJ, et al. Prospective preference assessment of patients’ willingness to participate in a randomized controlled trial of intensity-modulated radiotherapy versus proton therapy for localized prostate cancer. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2012;83(1):e13-9. doi:10.1016/j.ijrobp.2011.11.072.

155. Lim MK, Bae EY, Choi SE, Lee EK, Lee TJ. Eliciting public preference for health-care resource allocation in South Korea. Value Heal. 2012;15(1 Suppl):S91-4. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2011.11.014.

156. Kinter ET, Prior TJ, Carswell CI, Bridges JF. A comparison of two experimental design approaches in applying conjoint analysis in patient-centered outcomes research: a randomized trial. Patient. 2012;5(4):279-294. doi:10.2165/11641100-000000000-00000.

157. Gerard K, Salisbury C, Street D, Pope C, Baxter H. Is fast access to general practice all that should matter? A discrete choice experiment of patients’ preferences. J Heal Serv Res Policy. 2008;13 Suppl 2:3-10. doi:10.1258/jhsrp.2007.007087.

158. Farley K, Thompson C, Hanbury A, Chambers D. Exploring the feasibility of Conjoint Analysis as a tool for prioritizing innovations for implementation. Implement Sci. 2013;8:56. doi:10.1186/1748-5908-8-56.

159. Bederman SS, Mahomed NN, Kreder HJ, McIsaac WJ, Coyte PC, Wright JG. In the eye of the beholder: preferences of patients, family physicians, and surgeons for lumbar spinal surgery. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2010;35(1):108-115. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181b77f2d.

Page 89: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

141

160. Yi D, Ryan M, Campbell S, et al. Using discrete choice experiments to inform randomised controlled trials: an application to chronic low back pain management in primary care. Eur J Pain. 2011;15(5):531.e1-10. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.10.008.

161. Bunge EM, de Bekker-Grob EW, van Biezen FC, Essink-Bot ML, de Koning HJ. Patients’ preferences for scoliosis brace treatment: a discrete choice experiment. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2010;35(1):57-63. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181bdeaa6.

162. Miller K, Hofstetter R, Krohmer H, Zhang Z. How Should We Measure Consumers’ Willingness to Pay? An Empirical Comparison of State-of-the-Art Approaches. J Mark Res. 2010;48(1):172-184.

163. Cameron TA, Poe GL, Ethier RG, Schulze WD. Alternative Non-market Value-Elicitation Methods: Are the Underlying Preferences the Same? J Environ Econ Manage. 2002;44(3):391-425.

164. Ryan M, Bate a, Eastmond CJ, Ludbrook a. Use of discrete choice experiments to elicit preferences. Qual Health Care. 2001;10 Suppl 1(Suppl I):i55-60.

165. Ratcliffe J. The use of conjoint analysis to elicit willingness-to-pay values. Proceed with caution? Int J Technol Assess Health Care. 2000;16(1):270-5.

166. Gerard K, Lattimer V. Preferences of patients for emergency services available during usual GP surgery hours: a discrete choice experiment. Fam Pract. 2005;22(1):28-36. doi:10.1093/fampra/cmh623.

167. Bethge M. [Patient preferences and willingness to wait for a work-related orthopaedic rehabilitation: a discrete choice experiment]. Gesundheitswesen. 2009;71(3):152-60. doi:10.1055/s-0028-1124109.

168. Gerard K, Lattimer V, Turnbull J, et al. Reviewing emergency care systems 2: measuring patient preferences using a discrete choice experiment. Emerg Med J. 2004;21(6):692-7. doi:10.1136/emj.2002.003707.

169. Louviere, Jordan J.; Hensher, David.; Swait J. Stated Choice Methods. Analysis and Application. Cambridge University Press; 2000.

170. Hanley N, Wright R, Adamowicz V. Using Choice Experiments to Value the Environment. Environ Resour Econ. 1998;11(3):413-428.

171. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097.

172. Owens DK. Spine update. Patient preferences and the development of practice guidelines. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1998;23(9):1073-9.

Page 90: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

142

173. George SZ, Robinson ME. Preference, expectation, and satisfaction in a clinical trial of behavioral interventions for acute and sub-acute low back pain. J Pain. 2010;11(11):1074-82. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2010.02.016.

174. Donaldson M, Learman K, O’Halloran B, Showalter C, Cook C. The role of patients’ expectation of appropriate initial manual therapy treatment in outcomes for patients with low back pain. J Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2013;36(5):276-83. doi:10.1016/j.jmpt.2013.05.016.

175. George SZ, Wittmer VT, Fillingim RB, Robinson ME. Comparison of graded exercise and graded exposure clinical outcomes for patients with chronic low back pain. J Orthop Sport Phys Ther. 2010;40(11):694-704. doi:10.2519/jospt.2010.3396.

176. Slade SC, Molloy E, Keating JL. People with non-specific chronic low back pain who have participated in exercise programs have preferences about exercise: a qualitative study. Aust J Physiother. 2009;55(2):115-21.

177. Liddle SD, Baxter GD, Gracey JH. Chronic low back pain: patients’ experiences, opinions and expectations for clinical management. Disabil Rehabil. 2007;29(24):1899-909. doi:10.1080/09638280701189895.

178. Shaw WS, Huang Y-H. Concerns and expectations about returning to work with low back pain: identifying themes from focus groups and semi-structured interviews. Disabil Rehabil. 2005;27(21):1269-81. doi:10.1080/09638280500076269.

179. Dean SG, Smith JA, Payne S, Weinman J. Managing time: an interpretative phenomenological analysis of patients’ and physiotherapists' perceptions of adherence to therapeutic exercise for low back pain. Disabil Rehabil. 2005;27(11):625-36. doi:10.1080/0963820500030449.

180. Burton AK, Waddell G, Tillotson KM, Summerton N. Information and advice to patients with back pain can have a positive effect. A randomized controlled trial of a novel educational booklet in primary care. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1999;24(23):2484-91.

181. George SZ, Bialosky JE, Fritz JM. Physical therapist management of a patient with acute low back pain and elevated fear-avoidance beliefs. Phys Ther. 2004;84(6):538-49.

182. Glattacker M, Heyduck K, Meffert C. Illness beliefs and treatment beliefs as predictors of short-term and medium-term outcome in chronic back pain. J Rehabil Med. 2013;45(3):268-76. doi:10.2340/16501977-1104.

183. Bishop FL, Smith R, Lewith GT. Patient preferences for technical skills versus interpersonal skills in chiropractors and physiotherapists treating low back pain. Fam Pract. 2013;30(2):197-203. doi:10.1093/fampra/cms066.

184. Farin E, Gramm L, Schmidt E. The patient-physician relationship in patients with chronic low back pain as a predictor of outcomes after rehabilitation. J Behav Med. 2013;36(3):246-58. doi:10.1007/s10865-012-9419-z.

Page 91: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

143

185. Dima A, Lewith GT, Little P, Moss-Morris R, Foster NE, Bishop FL. Identifying patients’ beliefs about treatments for chronic low back pain in primary care: a focus group study. Br J Gen Pract. 2013;63(612):e490-8. doi:10.3399/bjgp13X669211.

186. McCarthy CJ, Oldham JA, Sephton R. Expectations and satisfaction of patients with low back pain attending a multidisciplinary rehabilitation service. Physiother Res Int. 2005;10(1):23-31.

187. McIntosh A, Shaw CFM. Barriers to patient information provision in primary care: patients’ and general practitioners' experiences and expectations of information for low back pain. Health Expect. 2003;6(1):19-29.

188. Verbeek J, Sengers M-J, Riemens L, Haafkens J. Patient Expectations of Treatment for Back Pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2004;29(20):2309-2318. doi:10.1097/01.brs.0000142007.38256.7f.

189. Sigrell H. Expectations of chiropractic patients: the construction of a questionnaire. J Manip Physiol Ther. 2001;24(7):440-444.

190. George SZ, Hirsh AT. Distinguishing patient satisfaction with treatment delivery from treatment effect: a preliminary investigation of patient satisfaction with symptoms after physical therapy treatment of low back pain. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005;86(7):1338-44.

191. May CR, Rose MJ, Johnstone FC. Dealing with doubt. How patients account for non-specific chronic low back pain. J Psychosom Res. 2000;49(4):223-5.

192. Cooper K, Smith BH, Hancock E. Patients’ perceptions of self-management of chronic low back pain: evidence for enhancing patient education and support. Physiotherapy. 2009;95(1):43-50. doi:10.1016/j.physio.2008.08.005.

193. Oliveira TH, Oliveira VC, Melo RC, Melo RM, Freitas AE, Ferreira PH. Patients in treatment for chronic low back pain have higher externalised beliefs: a cross-sectional study. Rev Bras Fisioter. 2012;16(1):35-9.

194. Verbeek J, Sengers M-J, Riemens L, Haafkens J. Patient expectations of treatment for back pain: a systematic review of qualitative and quantitative studies. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2004;29(20):2309-18.

195. Poiraudeau S, Rannou F, Le Henanff A, et al. Outcome of subacute low back pain: influence of patients’ and rheumatologists' characteristics. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2006;45(6):718-23. doi:10.1093/rheumatology/kei231.

196. Fritz JM, George SZ. Identifying psychosocial variables in patients with acute work-related low back pain: the importance of fear-avoidance beliefs. Phys Ther. 2002;82(10):973-83.

197. Werneke MW, Hart DL, George SZ, Stratford PW, Matheson JW, Reyes A. Clinical outcomes for patients classified by fear-avoidance beliefs and centralization phenomenon. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90(5):768-77. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2008.11.008.

Page 92: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

144

198. Woby SR, Watson PJ, Roach NK, Urmston M. Adjustment to chronic low back pain--the relative influence of fear-avoidance beliefs, catastrophizing, and appraisals of control. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(7):761-74. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00195-5.

199. Fritz JM, George SZ, Delitto A. The role of fear-avoidance beliefs in acute low back pain: relationships with current and future disability and work status. Pain. 2001;94(1):7-15.

200. Poiraudeau S, Rannou F, Baron G, et al. Fear-avoidance beliefs about back pain in patients with subacute low back pain. Pain. 2006;124(3):305-11. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.019.

201. Grotle M, Vøllestad NK, Veierød MB, Brox JI. Fear-avoidance beliefs and distress in relation to disability in acute and chronic low back pain. Pain. 2004;112(3):343-52. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2004.09.020.

202. Al-Obaidi SM, Nelson RM, Al-Awadhi S, Al-Shuwaie N. The role of anticipation and fear of pain in the persistence of avoidance behavior in patients with chronic low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2000;25(9):1126-31.

203. Al-Obaidi SM, Beattie P, Al-Zoabi B, Al-Wekeel S. The relationship of anticipated pain and fear avoidance beliefs to outcome in patients with chronic low back pain who are not receiving workers’ compensation. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2005;30(9):1051-7.

204. George SZ, Wittmer VT, Fillingim RB, Robinson ME. Fear-avoidance beliefs and temporal summation of evoked thermal pain influence self-report of disability in patients with chronic low back pain. J Occup Rehabil. 2006;16(1):95-108. doi:10.1007/s10926-005-9007-y.

205. Grotle M, Vøllestad NK, Brox JI. Clinical course and impact of fear-avoidance beliefs in low back pain: prospective cohort study of acute and chronic low back pain: II. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2006;31(9):1038-46. doi:10.1097/01.brs.0000214878.01709.0e.

206. Al-Obaidi SM, Al-Zoabi B, Al-Shuwaie N, Al-Zaabie N, Nelson RM. The influence of pain and pain-related fear and disability beliefs on walking velocity in chronic low back pain. Int J Rehabil Res. 2003;26(2):101-8. doi:10.1097/01.mrr.0000070750.63544.06.

207. Cai C, Pua YH, Lim KC. Correlates of self-reported disability in patients with low back pain: the role of fear-avoidance beliefs. Ann Acad Med Singapore. 2007;36(12):1013-20.

208. George SZ, Fritz JM, Childs JD. Investigation of elevated fear-avoidance beliefs for patients with low back pain: a secondary analysis involving patients enrolled in physical therapy clinical trials. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2008;38(2):50-8. doi:10.2519/jospt.2008.2647.

209. Woby SR, Watson PJ, Roach NK, Urmston M. Are changes in fear-avoidance beliefs, catastrophizing, and appraisals of control, predictive of changes in chronic low back pain and disability? Eur J Pain. 2004;8(3):201-10. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2003.08.002.

Page 93: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

145

210. Beneciuk JM, Robinson ME, George SZ. Low back pain subgroups using fear-avoidance model measures: results of a cluster analysis. Clin J Pain. 2012;28(8):658-66. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e31824306ed.

211. Lamé IE, Peters ML, Vlaeyen JWS, Kleef M V, Patijn J. Quality of life in chronic pain is more associated with beliefs about pain, than with pain intensity. Eur J pain. 2005;9(1):15-24. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.02.006.

212. Newcomer KL, Shelerud RA, Vickers Douglas KS, Larson DR, Crawford BJ. Anxiety levels, fear-avoidance beliefs, and disability levels at baseline and at 1 year among subjects with acute and chronic low back pain. PM R. 2010;2(6):514-20. doi:10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.03.034.

213. Kovacs F, Abraira V, Cano A, et al. Fear avoidance beliefs do not influence disability and quality of life in Spanish elderly subjects with low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2007;32(19):2133-8. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e318145a74b.

214. Kovacs FM, Muriel A, Abriaira V, Medina JM, Castillo Sanchez MD, Olabe J. The influence of fear avoidance beliefs on disability and quality of life is sparse in Spanish low back pain patients. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2005;30(22):E676-82.

215. Reneman MF, Schiphorts Preuper HR, Kleen M, Geertzen JHB, Dijkstra PU. Are pain intensity and pain related fear related to functional capacity evaluation performances of patients with chronic low back pain? J Occup Rehabil. 2007;17(2):247-58. doi:10.1007/s10926-007-9078-z.

216. Kalauokalani D, Cherkin DC, Sherman KJ, Koepsell TD, Deyo RA. Lessons from a trial of acupuncture and massage for low back pain: patient expectations and treatment effects. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2001;26(13):1418-24.

217. Sherman KJ, Cherkin DC, Ichikawa L, et al. Treatment Expectations and Preferences as Predictors of Outcome of Acupuncture for Chronic Back Pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2010;35(15):1471-1477. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181c2a8d3.Treatment.

218. Moffett JK, Torgerson D, Bell-Syer S, et al. Randomised controlled trial of exercise for low back pain: clinical outcomes, costs, and preferences. BMJ. 1999;319(7205):279-283. doi:10.1136/bmj.319.7205.279.

219. Myers SS, Phillips RS, Davis RB, et al. Patient expectations as predictors of outcome in patients with acute low back pain. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(2):148-53. doi:10.1007/s11606-007-0460-5.

220. Main CJ, Foster N, Buchbinder R. How important are back pain beliefs and expectations for satisfactory recovery from back pain? Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol. 2010;24(2):205-17. doi:10.1016/j.berh.2009.12.012.

221. Lackner JM, Carosella AM. The relative influence of perceived pain control, anxiety, and functional self efficacy on spinal function among patients with chronic low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1999;24(21):2254-60; discussion 2260-1.

Page 94: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

146

222. Mannion AF, Junge A, Elfering A, Dvorak J, Porchet F, Grob D. Great expectations: really the novel predictor of outcome after spinal surgery? Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009;34(15):1590-9. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e31819fcd52.

223. Walsh DA, Radcliffe JC. Pain beliefs and perceived physical disability of patients with chronic low back pain. Pain. 2002;97(1-2):23-31.

224. Sloan TJ, Gupta R, Zhang W, Walsh DA. Beliefs about the causes and consequences of pain in patients with chronic inflammatory or noninflammatory low back pain and in pain-free individuals. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2008;33(9):966-72. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e31816c8ab4.

225. Foster NE, Bishop A, Thomas E, et al. Illness perceptions of low back pain patients in primary care: what are they, do they change and are they associated with outcome? Pain. 2008;136(1-2):177-87. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.12.007.

226. Lacker JM, Carosella a M, Feuerstein M. Pain expectancies, pain, and functional self-efficacy expectancies as determinants of disability in patients with chronic low back disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996;64(1):212-20.

227. Hallegraeff JM, Krijnen WP, van der Schans CP, de Greef MHG. Expectations about recovery from acute non-specific low back pain predict absence from usual work due to chronic low back pain: a systematic review. J Physiother. 2012;58(3):165-72. doi:10.1016/S1836-9553(12)70107-8.

228. Perrot S, Allaert FA, Concas V, Laroche F. “When will I recover?” A national survey on patients’ and physicians' expectations concerning the recovery time for acute back pain. Eur Spine J. 2009;18(3):419-429. doi:10.1007/s00586-008-0868-6.

229. Smeets RJEM, Beelen S, Goossens MEJB, Schouten EGW, Knottnerus JA, Vlaeyen JWS. Treatment expectancy and credibility are associated with the outcome of both physical and cognitive-behavioral treatment in chronic low back pain. Clin J Pain. 2008;24(4):305-15. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e318164aa75.

230. Goldstein MS, Morgenstern H, Hurwitz EL, Yu F. The impact of treatment confidence on pain and related disability among patients with low-back pain: results from the University of California, Los Angeles, low-back pain study. Spine J. 2002;2(6):391-9; discussion 399-401.

231. Oliveira VC, Ferreira PH, Ferreira ML, et al. People with low back pain who have externalised beliefs need to see greater improvements in symptoms to consider exercises worthwhile: an observational study. Aust J Physiother. 2009;55(4):271-5.

232. Train K. Discrete Choice Methods with Simulation. second edi. Cambridge University Press; 2009.

233. Thurstone L. A law of comparative judgement. Psychol Rev. 1927;34:273–286.

234. McFadden D. Conditional logit analysis of qualitative choice behaviour. In: Zarembka P, ed. Frontiers of Econometrics. New York: Academic Press; 1974:05–142.

Page 95: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

147

235. Manski CF. The structure of random utility models. Theory Decis. 1977;8:229-254.

236. Lancaster K. A new approach to consumer theory. J Polit Econ. 1966;74(2).

237. Coast J, Horrocks S. Developing attributes and levels for discrete choice experiments using qualitative methods. J Health Serv Res Policy. 2007;12(1):25-30. doi:10.1258/135581907779497602.

238. Coast J, Al-Janabi H, Sutton E, et al. Using qualitative methods for attribute development for discrete choice experiments: Issues and recommendations. Health Econ. 2012;21(6):730-741.

239. Silverman D (ed. . Qualitative Research. 3rd ed. SAGE; 2011.

240. Pedersen LB, Gyrd-Hansen D. The use of status quo and opt out options in choice experiments. Implications of researchers dubious use of the “neither” option. In: International Choice Modelling Conference.; 2013.

241. Dhar R, Simonson I. The Effect of Forced Choice on Choice. J Mark Res. 2003;40(2):146-160. doi:10.1509/jmkr.40.2.146.19229.

242. Jaeger SR, Rose JM. Stated choice experimentation, contextual influences and food choice: A case study. Food Qual Prefer. 2008;19(6):539-564. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2008.02.005.

243. Blamey RK, Bennett JW, Louviere JJ, Morrison MD, Rolfe J. A test of policy labels in environmental choice modelling studies. Ecol Econ. 2000;32(2):269-286.

244. Fowkes T, Wardman M. The Design of Stated Preference Travel Choice Experiments: With Special Reference to Interpersonal Taste Variations. J Transp Econ Policy. 1988;22(1).

245. Fowkes A, Wardman M, Holden DG. Non-orthogonal stated preference design. In: Proceedings of the PTRC Summer Annual Meeting.; 1993.

246. Huber JF, Zwerina K. The Importance of Utility Balance in Efficient Choice Designs. J Mark Res. 1996;33(3).

247. Bunch D, Louviere JJ, Anderson D. A Comparison of Experimental Design Strategies for Multinomial Logit Models : The Case of Generic Attributes.; 1996.

248. Toner JP, Clark SD, Grant-Nuller SM, Fowkes AS. ANYTHING YOU CAN DO, WE CAN DO BETTER: A PROVOCATIVE INTRODUCTION TO A NEW APPROACH TO STATED PREFERENCE DESIGN. In: World Transport Research: Selected Proceedings of the 8th World Conference on Transport Research.; 1999.

249. Sándor Z, Wedel M. Heterogeneous Conjoint Choice Designs. 2005;XLII(May):210-218.

250. Sandor Z, Wedel M. Profile Construction in Experimental Choice Designs for Mixed Logit Models. Mark Sci. 2002;21(4):455-475. doi:10.1287/mksc.21.4.455.131.

Page 96: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

148

251. Kanninen B. Optimal Design for Multinomial Choice Experiments. J Mark Res. 2002;39(2):214-227.

252. Burgess L, Street DJ. Optimal designs for choice experiments with asymmetric attributes. J Stat Plan Inference. 2005;134(1):288-301.

253. Street DJ, Burgess L. Optimal and near-optimal pairs for the estimation of effects in 2-level choice experiments. J Stat Plan Inference. 2004;118(1):185-199.

254. Kessels R, Goos P, Vandebroek M. A Comparison of Criteria to Design Efficient Choice Experiments. J Mark Res. 2006;43(3):409-419. doi:10.1509/jmkr.43.3.409.

255. Ferrini S, Scarpa R. Designs with a priori information for nonmarket valuation with choice experiments: A Monte Carlo study. J Environ Econ Manage. 2007;53(3):342-363. doi:10.1016/j.jeem.2006.10.007.

256. Scarpa R, Rose JM. Design efficiency for non-market valuation with choice modelling: how to measure it, what to report and why*. Aust J Agric Resour Econ. 2008;52(3):253-282. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8489.2007.00436.x.

257. Bliemer MCJ, Rose JM. Serial choice conjoint analysis for estimating discrete choice models. In: Hess S, Daly A, eds. Choice Modelling: The State-of-the-art and State-of-the-Practice.; 2010.

258. Bliemer MCJ, Rose JM. Construction of experimental designs for mixed logit models allowing for correlation across choice observations. Transp Res Part B Methodol. 2010;44(6):720-734. doi:10.1016/j.trb.2009.12.004.

259. Choice Metrics. Ngene. 2012.

260. Rose JM, Bliemer MCJ. Constructing Efficient Stated Choice Experimental Designs. Transp Rev. 2009;29(5):587-617. doi:10.1080/01441640902827623.

261. Rose JM, Hensher DA, Caussade S, Ortúzar J de D, Jou R-C. Identifying differences in willingness to pay due to dimensionality in stated choice experiments: a cross country analysis. J Transp Geogr. 2009;17(1):21-29.

262. Caussade S, Ortúzar J de D, Rizzi LI, Hensher DA. Assessing the influence of design dimensions on stated choice experiment estimates. Transp Res Part B Methodol. 2005;39(7):621-640.

263. Hensher DA. Identifying the Influence of Stated Choice Design Dimensionality on Willingness to Pay for Travel Time Savings. J Transp Econ Policy. 2004;38(3):425-446.

264. Hanley N, Mourato S, Wright RE. Choice Modelling Approaches: A Superior Alternative for Environmental Valuatioin? J Econ Surv. 2002;15(3):435-462. doi:10.1111/1467-6419.00145.

Page 97: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

149

265. Arentze T, Borgers A, Timmermans H, DelMistro R. Transport stated choice responses: effects of task complexity, presentation format and literacy. Transp Res Part E Logist Transp Rev. 2003;39(3):229-244.

266. Olsen GD, Swait J, Johnson R, Louviere JJ. Response Mode Influences on Attribute Weight and Predictive Ability When Linear Models are Not Certain to be Robust.; 1995.

267. Louviere JJ, Swait J. Searching For Regularities In Choice Processes, Or The Little Constant That Could.; 1996.

268. Louviere JJ, Fox MF, Moore WL. Cross-task validity comparisons of stated preference choice models. Mark Lett. 1993;4(3):205-213. doi:10.1007/BF00999227.

269. Olsen S. Choosing Between Internet and Mail Survey Modes for Choice Experiment Surveys Considering Non-Market Goods. Environ Resour Econ. 2009;44(4):591-610.

270. Börjesson M, Algers S. Properties of Internet and Telephone Data Collection Methods in a Stated Choice Value of Time Study Context. J Choice Model. 2011;4(2).

271. Klein A, Nihalani K, Krishnan K. A comparison of the validity of interviewer-based and online conjoint analyses. J Manag Mark Res. 2010;4.

272. Ryan M, Gerard K. Using discrete choice experiments to value health care programmes: current practice and future research reflections. Appl Health Econ Health Policy. 2003;2(1):55-64.

273. Debreu G. Review of R.D. Luce, Individual choice behavior: A theoretical analysis. Am Econ Rev. 1960;50:186 - 188.

274. Hausman JA, Wise DA. A Conditional Probit Model for Qualitative Choice: Discrete Decisions Recognizing Interdependence and Heterogeneous Preferences. Econometrica. 1978;46(2):403-26.

275. McFadden D. Modelling the Choice of Residential Location. Cowles Found Discuss Pap. 1977.

276. Hole AR. Modelling heterogeneity in patients’ preferences for the attributes of a general practitioner appointment. J Health Econ. 2008;27(4):1078-94. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.11.006.

277. Greene WH, Hensher DA, Rose J. Accounting for heterogeneity in the variance of unobserved effects in mixed logit models. Transp Res Part B Methodol. 2006;40(1):75-92.

278. Taylor LO, Cummings RG. Unbiased Value Estimates for Environmental Goods: A Cheap Talk Design for the Contingent Valuation Method. Am Econ Rev. 1999;89(3):649-665.

279. List J. Do explicit warnings eliminate the hypothetical bias in elicitation procedures? Evidence from field auctions for sportscards. Fram F Exp. 2001.

Page 98: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

150

280. Aadland D, Caplan AJ. Cheap talk reconsidered: New evidence from CVM. J Econ Behav Organ. 2006;60(4):562-578.

281. Lawlis GF, Cuencas R, Selby D, McCoy CE. The development of the Dallas Pain Questionnaire. An assessment of the impact of spinal pain on behavior. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1989;14(5):511-6.

282. Manniche C, Asmussen K, Lauritsen B. Low back pain rating scale: validation of a tool for assessment of low back pain. Pain. 1994;57:317-326.

283. Lancsar E, Louviere J. Experiments to Inform Healthcare Decision Making A User ’ s Guide. 2008;26(8):661-677.

284. Leeuw ED de. Reducing Missing Data in Surveys: An Overview of Methods. Qual Quant. 2001;35(2):147-160. doi:10.1023/A:1010395805406.

285. Hess S, Rose JM, Polak J. Non-trading, lexicographic and inconsistent behaviour in stated choice data. Transp Res Part D Transp Environ. 2010;15(7):405-417.

286. Hensher DA, Rose JM, Greene WH. Combining RP and SP data: biases in using the nested logit “trick” – contrasts with flexible mixed logit incorporating panel and scale effects. J Transp Geogr. 2008;16(2):126-133.

287. Carlsson F, Martinsson P. Do Hypothetical and Actual Marginal Willingness to Pay Differ in Choice Experiments? J Environ Econ Manage. 2001;41(2):179-192. doi:10.1006/jeem.2000.1138.

288. Moser R, Raffaelli R, Notaro S. Testing hypothetical bias with a real choice experiment using respondents’ own money. Eur Rev Agric Econ. 2013:jbt016-. doi:10.1093/erae/jbt016.

289. Ready RC, Champ PA, Lawton JL. Using Respondent Uncertainty to Mitigate Hypothetical Bias in a Stated Choice Experiment. Land Econ. 2010;86(2):363-381.

290. Harrison G. Making choice studies incentive compatible. In: Valuing Environmental Amenities Using Stated Choice Studies. Springer-Verlag; 2007:67-110.

291. Carson R, Groves T. Incentive and informational properties of preference questions. Environ Resour Econ. 2007;37(1):181-210.

292. Herriges J, Kling C, Liu C-C, Tobias J. What are the consequences of consequentiality? J Environ Econ Manage. 2010;59(1):67-81.

293. Vossler CA, Evans MF. Bridging the gap between the field and the lab: Environmental goods, policy maker input, and consequentiality. J Environ Econ Manage. 2009;58(3):338-345.

294. Norwood FB. Can Calibration Reconcile Stated and Observed Preferences? J Agric Appl Econ. 2005;37(01).

Page 99: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

151

295. Ryan M, Watson V. Comparing welfare estimates from payment card contingent valuation and discrete choice experiments. Health Econ. 2009;18(4):389-401. doi:10.1002/hec.1364.

296. Ryan, M.; Gerard K. Uisng Discrete Choice Experiments to Value Health Care Programs: Current Practice and Future Research Reflections. Appl Heal Econ Heal Policy. 2003;2(1).

297. Louviere JJ, Lancsar E. Choice experiments in health: the good, the bad, the ugly and toward a brighter future. Health Econ Policy Law. 2009;4(Pt 4):527-46. doi:10.1017/S1744133109990193.

298. Deyo R a, Cherkin DC, Weinstein J, Howe J, Ciol M, Mulley a G. Involving patients in clinical decisions: impact of an interactive video program on use of back surgery. Med Care. 2000;38(9):959-69.

299. Phelan E a, Deyo R a, Cherkin DC, et al. Helping patients decide about back surgery: a randomized trial of an interactive video program. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2001;26(2):206-11;discussion 212.

300. Nelson CW. Helping patients decide: from Hippocrates to videodiscs--an application for patients with low back pain. J Med Syst. 1988;12(1):1-10.

301. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, et al. GRADE guidelines: 2. Framing the question and deciding on important outcomes. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(4):395-400. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.09.012.

302. Mulley A, Trimble C, Elwyn G. Patients’ Preferences Matter: Stop the Silent Misdiagnosis. London; 2012.

303. Say RE, Thomson R. The importance of patient preferences in treatment decisions--challenges for doctors. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):542-5. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.542.

304. Lin GA, Trujillo L, Frosch DL. Consequences of not respecting patient preferences for cancer screening: opportunity lost. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(5):393-4. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2012.58.

305. Bishop A, Thomas E, Foster NE. Health care practitioners’ attitudes and beliefs about low back pain: a systematic search and critical review of available measurement tools. Pain. 2007;132(1-2):91-101. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.01.028.

306. Buchbinder R, Staples M, Jolley D. Doctors with a special interest in back pain have poorer knowledge about how to treat back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009;34(11):1218-26; discussion 1227. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e318195d688.

307. Willems P, de Bie R, Oner C, Castelein R, de Kleuver M. Clinical decision making in spinal fusion for chronic low back pain. Results of a nationwide survey among spine surgeons. BMJ Open. 2011;1(2):e000391. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000391.

308. Gurden M, Morelli M, Sharp G, Baker K, Betts N, Bolton J. Evaluation of a general practitioner referral service for manual treatment of back and neck pain. Prim Health Care Res Dev. 2012;13(3):204-10. doi:10.1017/S1463423611000648.

Page 100: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

152

309. Corbett M, Foster N, Ong BN. GP attitudes and self-reported behaviour in primary care consultations for low back pain. Fam Pract. 2009;26(5):359-64. doi:10.1093/fampra/cmp042.

310. Nagl M, Farin E. Congruence or discrepancy? Comparing patients’ health valuations and physicians' treatment goals for rehabilitation for patients with chronic conditions. Int J Rehabil Res. 2012;35(1):26-35. doi:10.1097/MRR.0b013e32834d4be3.

311. Chancellor J, Martin M, Liedgens H, Baker MG, Müller-Schwefe GHH. Stated preferences of physicians and chronic pain sufferers in the use of classic strong opioids. Value Health. 2012;15(1):106-17. doi:10.1016/j.jval.2011.07.002.

312. Montgomery a a, Fahey T. How do patients’ treatment preferences compare with those of clinicians? Qual Health Care. 2001;10 Suppl 1(Suppl I):i39-43.

313. Perrot S, Allaert FA, Concas V, Laroche F. “When will I recover?” A national survey on patients’ and physicians' expectations concerning the recovery time for acute back pain. Eur Spine J. 2009;18(3):419-29. doi:10.1007/s00586-008-0868-6.

314. Parsons S, Harding G, Breen A, et al. The influence of patients’ and primary care practitioners' beliefs and expectations about chronic musculoskeletal pain on the process of care: a systematic review of qualitative studies. Clin J Pain. 2007;23(1):91-8. doi:10.1097/01.ajp.0000210947.34676.34.

315. Kiesler DJ, Auerbach SM. Optimal matches of patient preferences for information, decision-making and interpersonal behavior: evidence, models and interventions. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;61(3):319-41. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2005.08.002.

316. Pincus T, Foster NE, Vogel S, Santos R, Breen A, Underwood M. Attitudes to back pain amongst musculoskeletal practitioners: a comparison of professional groups and practice settings using the ABS-mp. Man Ther. 2007;12(2):167-75. doi:10.1016/j.math.2006.06.005.

317. Bishop A, Foster NE, Thomas E, Hay EM. How does the self-reported clinical management of patients with low back pain relate to the attitudes and beliefs of health care practitioners? A survey of UK general practitioners and physiotherapists. Pain. 2008;135(1-2):187-95. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.11.010.

318. Houben RMA, Vlaeyen JWS, Peters M, Ostelo RWJG, Wolters PMJC, Stomp-van den Berg SGM. Health care providers’ attitudes and beliefs towards common low back pain: factor structure and psychometric properties of the HC-PAIRS. Clin J Pain. 2004;20(1):37-44.

319. Houben RMA, Ostelo RWJG, Vlaeyen JWS, Wolters PMJC, Peters M, Stomp-van den Berg SGM. Health care providers’ orientations towards common low back pain predict perceived harmfulness of physical activities and recommendations regarding return to normal activity. Eur J Pain. 2005;9(2):173-83. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.05.002.

Page 101: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

153

320. Ostelo RWJ., Stomp-van den Berg SG., Vlaeyen JW., Wolters PMJ., de Vet HC. Health care provider’s attitudes and beliefs towards chronic low back pain: the development of a questionnaire. Man Ther. 2003;8(4):214-222. doi:10.1016/S1356-689X(03)00013-4.

321. DR. Nej til operation koster sygedagpenge. 2013. Available at: http://www.dr.dk/Nyheder/Indland/2013/09/13/0913154041.htm.

322. Deyo RA, Nachemson A, Mirza SK. Spinal-fusion surgery - the case for restraint. N Engl J Med. 2004;350(7):722-6. doi:10.1056/NEJMsb031771.

323. Thiene M, Rungie C, Scarpa R. The influences of individuals in joint decisions: recreation in the Alps in Italy. In: International Choice Modelling Conference.; 2013.

324. McFadden D. Economic Choices. Am Econ Rev. 2001;91(3):351-378.

325. Manski CF. Economic Analysis of Social Interactions. J Econ Perspect. 2000;14(3):115-136. doi:10.1257/jep.14.3.115.

326. Rose J, Hensher DA. Modelling agent interdependency in group decision making. Transp Res Part E Logist Transp Rev. 2004;40(1):63-79.

327. Hensher DA, Rose JM, Black I. Interactive Agency Choice in Automobile Purchase Decisions: The Role of Negotiation in Determining Equilibrium Choice Outcomes. J Transp Econ Policy. 2008;42(2):269-296.

328. Hensher DA, Puckett SM. Theoretical and conceptual frameworks for studying agent interaction and choice revelation in transportation studies. Int J Transp Econ IJTE - Pisa [u.a] Serra, ISSN 0391-8440, ZDB-ID 1652813 - Vol 342007, 1, p 17-47. 2007.

329. Hensher D, Figliozzi MA, Hensher DA, Puckett SM, Rose JM. Agency decision making in freight distribution chains: Establishing a parsimonious empirical framework from alternative behavioural structures. Transp Res Part B Methodol. 2007;41(9):924-949.

330. Chorus CG. A new model of Random Regret Minimization. Eur J Transp Infrastruct Res. 2010;10(2):181-196.

331. Chorus CG, Rose JM, Hensher D a. Regret minimization or utility maximization: it depends on the attribute. Environ Plan B Plan Des. 2013;40(1):154-169. doi:10.1068/b38092.

332. Chorus CG, Arentze TA, Timmermans HJP. A Random Regret-Minimization model of travel choice. Transp Res Part B Methodol. 2008;42(1):1-18.

333. De Bekker-Grob EW, Chorus CG. Random regret-based discrete-choice modelling: an application to healthcare. Pharmacoeconomics. 2013;31(7):623-34. doi:10.1007/s40273-013-0059-0.

334. Hensher DA. Joint Estimation of Process and Outcome in Choice Experiments and Implications for Willingness to Pay. J Transp Econ Policy. 2008;42(2):297-322.

Page 102: Choosing Treatments - SDU/media/520A6BE5587B4A...1 Choosing Treatments A Ph.D. thesis on preference elicitation from idea to implementation with applications to health care decisions

154

335. Zhu W, Timmermans HJP. Cognitive process model of individual choice behaviour incorporating principles of bounded rationality and heterogeneous decision-rules. Environ Plan B Plan Des. 2010;37:59-74.

336. Chorus CG, Rose JM. Selecting a date: a matter of regret and compromises. In: Hess S, Daly A, eds. Choice Modelling: The State of the Art and the State of Practice. Edward Elgar Publishing; 2012.

337. Hess S, Stathopoulos A, Daly A. Allowing for heterogeneous decision rules in discrete choice models: an approach and four case studies. Transportation (Amst). 2012;39(3):565-591.

338. Fiebig DG, Keane MP, Louviere J, Wasi N. The Generalized Multinomial Logit Model: Accounting for Scale and Coefficient Heterogeneity. Mark Sci. 2009;29(3):393-421. doi:10.1287/mksc.1090.0508.

339. Hess S, Rose JM. Can scale and coefficient heterogeneity be separated in random coefficients models? Transportation (Amst). 2012;39(6):1225-1239. doi:10.1007/s11116-012-9394-9.