cq example 5

Upload: roxy-shira-adi

Post on 13-Oct-2015

47 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

CQq, cultural intelligence, various, training methid,

TRANSCRIPT

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    1/49

    Running Head: A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 1

    Embracing the Unexpected:

    An Experiential Education Pathway to Cultural Competence

    Brent MacNab

    University of Sydney

    Valerie Rosenblatt and Reginald Worthley

    University of Hawaii

    Author Note

    The authors would like to thank professors Linn Van Dyne, Soon Ang and Gilad Chen

    for their organization of a 2009 CQ research symposium. Their valuable review of our work

    was important for the development of this manuscript. Finally, we wish to sincerely thank the

    participants of this study for their valuable time and feedback.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dr. Brent MacNab,

    University of Sydney Business School, Room 448, H10 Storie Dixon Building, The University

    of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia. E-mail:[email protected]

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    2/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 2

    Abstract

    Social learning paradigm suggests that people learn from observation and experience. This study

    examines a process by which experiential contact with members of a different cultural group

    contributes to the development of an individuals ability to effectively function in culturally

    diverse contexts. The hypothesized model, grounded in contact theory and experiential learning

    research, proposes that contact type influences the development of cultural intelligence (CQ) by

    means of disconfirmation and an increase in stereotype awareness and alteration. The process

    was tested in a theoretically-based CQ education program with management students and

    professionals. The results, based on structural equation modeling, confirmed the important roles

    of disconfirmation, stereotype awareness, and stereotype alteration in the development of CQ.

    Given the experience of optimal contact with members of a different cultural group, participants

    reported experiencing disconfirmation about attitudes and beliefs. Disconfirmation was found

    to be related positively to both the increase in stereotype awareness and alteration. In turn,

    these were found to play complementary roles in the development of all examined aspects of

    CQ development indicators. Managers and educators understanding these processes are better

    equipped to craft the most effective CQ development, training and education efforts.

    Keywords:cultural intelligence, contact theory, disconfirmed expectancy, stereotypes,

    experiential education

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    3/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 3

    Embracing the Unexpected:

    An Experiential Education Pathway to Cultural Competence

    Increasing globalization and diversity compels researchers and practitioners to look

    for ways to increase the ability to adjust and function effectively in culturally-diverse contexts

    (Earley & Ang, 2003), also known as cultural intelligence (CQ). Earley and Ang (2003) argue

    that individuals with higher levels of CQ are likely to show greater cultural knowledge, engage

    in a greater amount of critical thinking, reflective observation, have greater motivation to learn

    about different cultures, and can exhibit culturally appropriate behaviors. These individuals

    are also proposed to undergo more effective adjustment in different cultures, achieve better

    performance, and tend to be more successful in culturally diverse settings (Ang & Van Dyne,

    2009).

    CQ is viewed as a vital competency in cross-cultural management (Earley & Ang, 2003)

    . Specifically, CQ has been connected to a wide range of positive management aspects, such as

    effective adjustment of expatriates (Berry & Ward, 2006; Ng, Van Dyne, & Ang, 2009), better

    performance (Ang, et al., 2007), lower burnout (Tay, Westman, & Chia, 2009), constructive

    team dynamics (Flaherty, 2009), suspension of judgment (Brislin, Worthley, & MacNab, 2006),

    and effective development of leadership (Ng, et al., 2009).

    Considering the above, cultural intelligence development is an important topic to

    researchers and practitioners (Ng, et al., 2009; Tan & Chua, 2003). Cultural Intelligence

    Education (MacNab, in press; MacNab, Brislin, & Worthley, in press) has been proposed as

    a process designed to enhance individual levels of CQ through guided contact with members

    of different cultural groups. Aiming to distinguish from cross-cultural training methods

    primarily focused on cross-cultural knowledge and skills, CQ education is a process-based

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    4/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 4

    approach emphasizing the development of the dynamic aspects of CQ, including metacognition,

    motivation and behavior; it also emphasizes CQ development as a longer-term commitment.

    Drawing from Banduras (1977) social learning theory, Kolbs (1976) experiential learning

    theory, Cushner and Brislins (1996) social contact learning, and Allports (1954) contact

    theory, CQ education uses the effectiveness of optimal contact, awareness, and reflection to

    build CQ capabilities; aspects which are also recommended by other experts in the area of CQ

    development (Kim & Van Dyne, 2011; Ng, et al., 2009).

    Research provides evidence that successful contact and experience with different cultures

    can contribute to the development of cultural intelligence and greater functioning in culturally

    diverse contexts (Church, 1982; Crowne, 2008; Ng, et al., 2009); however further process

    clarity would be beneficial and is needed. In addition, not all experiences may contribute to

    the development of cultural intelligence equally (Brislin & Macnab, 2004; Kim & Van Dyne,

    2011). Researchers have established that optimal contact conditions proposed by Allport (1954)

    , including equal status among participants, common goals, personalized connection, and support

    of authority, are necessary for achieving favorable results during the experiential CQ education

    (Kim & Van Dyne, 2011; MacNab, et al., in press).

    This work investigates the process by which optimal contact influences the development

    of individual cultural intelligence in the context of experiential CQ education. Integrating

    contact theory research (Allport, 1954; Brown & Hewstone, 2005; MacNab, et al., in press) and

    Adlers (2002) effective stereotyping framework with models of experiential and social learning

    (Bhawuk, 2009a, 2009b; Cushner & Brislin, 1996; Kolb, 1976), this work proposes a model

    whereby optimal contact with members of culturally different groups influences the development

    of CQ by providing an opportunity to experience disconfirmed expectancy and by increasing

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    5/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 5

    the awareness of, and ability to alter, stereotypes. Contact theory research shows that optimal

    contact with members of different groups may lead to disconfirmation of expectations and

    stereotypes, making individuals more aware of the stereotypes they use when interacting with

    others and ultimately resulting in adjustment of stereotypes (Brown & Hewstone, 2005). Adler

    (2002) argues that culturally effective international managers must be aware of their stereotypes

    and be able to adjust their stereotypes to fit different cultural contexts. Earley and Ang (2003)

    suggest that greater awareness and ability to modify ones stereotypes is related to higher levels

    of ones CQ. Figure 1 summarizes schematically the posited model.

    This work contributes toward research on experiential education and the related influence

    in the development of cultural intelligence. Further, we empirically investigate the role of

    disconfirmed expectancy in the development of an individuals ability to adjust and function

    effectively in culturally new settings. In addition, this research explores the role of stereotype

    awareness and alteration in the advancement of CQ. In exploring these relations process

    specificity can surface and process clarity then becomes attainable.

    Following we provide an overview of CQ and CQ education, an explanation of the

    hypothesized relations in the proposed process, a summary of the CQ experiential education

    method, proposed model analysis, and discussion with recommendations.

    CQ and Experiential CQ Education

    Following Sternberg and Dettermans (1986) framework of the multiple foci of

    intelligence, cultural intelligence is theorized as a multi-dimensional construct consisting

    of cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, and behavioral facets (Earley & Ang, 2003).

    Metacognitive CQcenters on higher-order cognitive processes incorporating conscious cultural

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    6/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 6

    awareness, reflection on experiences, active challenging of assumptions, and adjustment of

    cognitive structures (Ang & Van Dyne, 2009). Motivational CQincorporates drive, enthusiasm

    and perseverance to deal with challenges of intercultural encounters, and capability to direct

    focus in a culturally different context (Ang & Van Dyne, 2009). Behavioral CQrefers to the

    ability to exhibit verbal and non-verbal action appropriately in different cultural contexts (Ang &

    Van Dyne, 2009).

    As a competency, cultural intelligence may be developed through training (Earley &

    Ang, 2003; Ng, et al., 2009; Tan & Chua, 2003). Built on the premises of experiential learning

    (Bhawuk, 2001; Kolb, 1984), social learning processes (Bandura, 1977; Cushner & Brislin,

    1996), and contact theory (Allport, 1954), CQ educationis the cyclical process of developing the

    competencies and capacities for effective cultural awareness, knowledge, passion, interaction,

    and adjustment (MacNab, et al., in press; MacNab, Worthley, & Brislin, 2007). Experiential

    learning (Kolb, 1976) advocates the effectiveness of direct experience and reflection in the

    development of capabilities. Social learning processes (Bandura, 1977; Cushner & Brislin, 1996)

    accentuate the role of social sharing in synthesis, reflection, and further application of direct

    experiences. Contact theory (Allport, 1954) specifies optimal contact conditions necessary to

    achieve favorable results during experiential CQ education and developmental efforts (Kim &

    Van Dyne, in press; MacNab, et al., in press; MacNab & Worthley, 2010; MacNab, et al., 2007).

    The CQ education process we employ for this study is a six to eight-week, multi-

    stage process (see MacNab, in press, for discussion); we selected this approach because of its

    specificity, replicability in an advanced learning environment, and establishment in the literature.

    First, participants are provided with awareness-level knowledge, encompassing key concepts

    such as culture, stereotypes, and cultural intelligence. In addition, individuals are given an

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    7/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 7

    opportunity to reflect on the acquired information by engaging in hands-on group activities

    designed to apply the new concepts in simulated intercultural contexts. Then, individuals are

    guided through a quasi-controlled, real-life contact with members of a culturally different group,

    which allows them to learn from experience and test themselves in using key aspects of CQ.

    Participants are encouraged to seek experiences that meet optimal contact parameters (Allport,

    1954). After the contact, participants reflect and write about their real-life interactions as

    specifically related to the development and use of cultural intelligence. Finally, participants are

    provided with feedback from the trainer and the fellow trainees and a discussion is held.

    MacNab and colleagues (in press; 2010; 2007) provide empirical evidence in support

    of the positive impact of their experiential CQ education process on all examined areas of

    CQ, including metacognitive, motivational, and behavioral CQ. In addition, optimal contact

    parameters in the context of their CQ education process have been shown to positively relate to

    the enhancement of individual stereotype syntheses (MacNab, 2009). In this study, we aim to

    uncover a process by which optimal contact with members of different cultural groups influences

    the CQ development process.

    The Process of CQ Development via Contact

    Optimal Contact and the Development of CQ

    Research has shown that greater amounts of cross-cultural contact and experience

    contribute to the enhancement of all four facets of individual CQ (Crowne, 2008; Ng, et al.,

    2009; Shannon & Begley, 2009; Tarique & Takeuchi, 2009). Grounding their argument in social

    learning theory (Bandura, 1977), Tarique and Takeuchi (2009) explain that individuals with a

    greater amount of international experience are more likely to develop greater cognitive structures

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    8/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 8

    that govern social behavior, show greater motivation to learn about new cultures, and display

    more appropriate behavior guided by more comprehensive knowledge structures.

    Drawing from Dewey (1938) and Itin (1999), MacNab and Worthley (2010) point

    out that not all types of experience may contribute to the development of cultural intelligence

    equally. Unstructured approaches may become too overwhelming, preventing future efforts to

    experience and learn. Drawing on contact theory research (Allport, 1954; Brown & Hewstone,

    2005; Dovidio, Gaertner, & Kawakami, 2003; Pettigrew, 1998), MacNab, Brislin, and Worthley

    (2010) argue that establishment of the optimal contact conditions outlined by Allport (1954)

    are beneficial in cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, and behavioral transformations. These

    conditions include: (a) equal status between contact parties; (b) establishments of common goals/

    common ground; (c) meaningful personalized contact; and (d) support and endorsement of

    contact from recognized authorities or leaders (Allport, 1954). Cross-cultural contact literature

    tends to support Allports (1954) position as meaningful (Caligiuri, 2000; Church, 1982).

    Brislin (1981) explains that equal-status contact, when members of one cultural group are

    not perceived as having more power than another, promotes willingness to interact, to motivate

    mutual feedback, and to encourage learning from others. Common goals help overcome

    tensions inherent in cross-cultural encounters and encourage cooperation. Personalized contact

    provides an opportunity for individuals to break down barriers of communication, to challenge

    preconceived attitudes and stereotypes, and to develop a more accurate view of others. Finally,

    support of authorities may enhance motivation to engage in cross-cultural contact and to apply

    the acquired knowledge and experience to future contact opportunities. Based on previous

    theoretical and empirical evidence, we propose:

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    9/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 9

    Hypothesis 1. Optimal contact is positively related to the development of: (a)

    metacognitive CQ: (b) motivational CQ; and (c) behavioral CQ.

    Optimal Contact and Disconfirmed Expectancy

    During contact with members of a different cultural group, individuals often experience

    disconfirmed expectancy (Bhawuk, 2009b; Brislin, 1981; Cushner & Brislin, 1996). Cross-

    cultural disconfirmed expectancy involves situations in which a person expects a certain

    attitude, reaction or behavior from an individual of another culture, yet experiences something

    else. According to Snyder and Stukas (1999), disconfirmed expectancy may result from the

    use of stereotypes to predict behaviors of others during interactions. Stereotypes, described

    as cognitive categories depicting qualities for broad social categories of individuals (Fiske

    & Taylor, 1984), are frequently used to deal with complexity of stimuli and help individuals

    interpret others behavior (Brislin, 1981). For example, armed with a strong subconscious

    or rigid stereotype that all Germans are punctual (Cooper & Kirkcaldy, 1995), a person may

    experience a disconfirmation when a German business partner shows up to a meeting late

    without an agenda. Consistent with previous arguments, we posit:

    Hypothesis 2. Optimal contact is positively associated with experience of disconfirmed

    expectancy.

    Optimal Contact, Stereotype Awareness and Stereotype Alteration

    Drawing on Adlers (2002) effective stereotyping framework, MacNab (2009) argues that

    stereotype awarenessinvolves an individual ability to understand and articulate what stereotypes

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    10/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 10

    are and how stereotypes influence ones attitudes, judgments, and actions. Stereotype alteration

    refers to an individuals propensity to modify existing stereotypes based on actual interaction

    or evidence. Effective global managers and sojourns must be aware of their stereotypes and

    regulate them when faced with disconfirming experiences (Adler, 2002; Cushner & Brislin,

    1996).

    A sizable part of contact theory literature has examined the impact of optimal

    contact on stereotype awareness and stereotype alteration (Brown & Hewstone, 2005; Dovidio et

    al., 2003; Pettigrew, 1998). Brewer and Miller (1984) argue that optimal contact increases the

    awareness of individual characteristics, undermines the validity of outgroup stereotypes, and

    leads to dissolution of social categories. Gaertner and Dovidio (2000) suggest that optimal

    contact allows individuals to redraw social category boundaries through the formation of

    common identities and superordinate categorization, making interactants aware that members of

    another group are also members of ones own group on a different dimension (Gaertner, et al.,

    2000, p. 102). Hewstone and Brown (1986) propose that optimal contact increases the salience

    of each groups superiorities and inferiorities, such that members of both groups become aware

    of and appreciate the indispensible contribution of one another. Consistent with these views, a

    number of studies have demonstrated that optimal contact is associated with increased stereotype

    awareness and formation of more favorable attitudes toward members of other groups (Brown

    and Hewstone, 2005; Dovidio et al, 2003). Hewstone (1996) has argued that work on

    stereotype change via stereotype disconfirming information is simply a cognitive analysis of the

    contact hypothesis (Hill & Augoustinos, 2001, p. 244). Thus, we expect:

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    11/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 11

    Hypothesis 3. Optimal contact is positively related to the development of: (a) stereotype

    awareness and (b) stereotype alteration.

    Disconfirmed Expectancy, Stereotype Awareness and Stereotype Alteration

    Contact theory suggests that disconfirmation is crucial for successful outcomes of inter-

    group contact, including stereotype awareness and stereotype alteration (Cook, 1978; Pettigrew,

    1998). Research on cognitive processes involved in the revision of stereotypes has identified

    three possible mechanisms: Bookkeeping, conversion, and subtyping (Weber & Crocker, 1983)

    . All three mechanisms suggest that social categories change on the basis of encountering

    disconfirmations or discrepancies with ones current stereotypes. The bookkeeping modelposits

    that stereotypes change incrementally (Rothbart, 1981) as individuals become aware of minor

    stereotype inconsistencies and fine tune the stereotypes accordingly. The conversion model

    suggests that stereotype change is more radical (Rothbart, 1981), resulting from encounters with

    dramatic and salient stereotype inconsistencies. Thesubtyping modelcontends that stereotypes

    are hierarchical structures and advocates that awareness of discrepant information leads to the

    development of more differentiations and subtypes within a particular stereotype. Crocker,

    Fiske and Taylor (1984) argue that disconfirmations are most likely to contribute to stereotype

    awareness and change when the discrepancies are undeniable, unambiguous, memorable and

    stable. In line with the cognitive models of stereotype alteration, we argue:

    Hypothesis 4. Greater experience of disconfirmed expectancy is positively related to the

    development of: (a) stereotype awareness and (b) stereotype alteration.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    12/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 12

    Disconfirmed Expectancy and the Development of CQ

    Building on Kolbs (1976) experiential learning model, Bhawuk (2009a) proposed that

    disconfirmed expectancy resulting from concrete experiences with individuals from different

    cultures may provide an opportunity to develop intercultural competencies. When expectations

    are met during interactions, individuals often use automated, habitual responses. However,

    when expectations are not met, individuals attention is directed to the disconfirmed evidence,

    and motivated individuals, such as those in related educational processes, more readily engage

    in reflective observations (Bhawuk, 2009a, 2009b). Reflective observations, encompassing

    awareness and critical thinking, have been linked to the development of metacognitive CQ (Ang

    & Van Dyne, 2009).

    Disconfirmed expectancy may also positively relate to the development of motivational

    CQ. Theorists suggest that discrepancies between expectations and actual occurrences motivate

    discrepancy-reduction efforts (Adams, 1963; Bandura, 1986; Festinger, 1957; Rokeach, 1979).

    According to Bandura (1986), increased motivation to learn occurs when individuals experience

    imbalance between the schemata or scripts they already have and the actual perceived course of

    events. Such disconfirmations are abundant during intercultural experiences (Bhawuk, 2009b).

    Also, disconfirmed expectancy may connect positively to the development of behavioral

    CQ. According to Bhawuk (2009a), as a result of disconfirmed expectancy, individuals

    engaging in reflective observations gain culture-specific knowledge. Having culture-specific

    knowledge allows individuals to engage in culture-appropriate behavior in the future (Earley &

    Ang, 2003). Based on the previous evidence, we posit:

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    13/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 13

    Hypothesis 5. Experience of disconfirmed expectancy is positively associated with the

    development of: (a) metacognitive CQ; (b) motivational CQ; and (c) behavioral CQ.

    Stereotype Awareness and Stereotype Alteration

    Cross-cultural managers unaware of stereotypes, and how these influence perception or

    behavior, are in danger of making inefficient decisions (Adler, 2002; Osland, Bird, Delano, &

    Jacob, 2000). For example, discussingself-fulfilling stereotypes,Snyder (1982) points out that

    a lack of stereotype awareness may lead individuals to act in ways that conform to stereotypes

    of oneself and others, ignoring reality that does not fit the stereotypes. Adler (2002, p. 81)

    suggested that effective stereotypingstartswith the stereotype being consciously held, leading

    toward the possibility of stereotype alteration. Osland and colleagues (2000), in their work

    on sophisticated stereotyping, establish that awareness of ones stereotypes is a critical first

    step in gaining the possibility for adjustment of stereotypes. These positions help establish the

    following in our model:

    Hypothesis 6. The development of stereotype awareness is positively related to

    stereotype alteration.

    Stereotype Awareness and the Development of CQ

    Effective global managers must be aware of their cultural stereotypes (Adler, 2002;

    Osland, et al., 2000). Not being aware of stereotypes, individuals may inaccurately evaluate

    a person or a situation, basing their judgment on broad assumptions about a particular group

    instead of the actual individual attitudes and behavior (Berry, Poortinga, Segall, & Dasen, 2002).

    We posit that the development of stereotype awareness is connected to the advancement of CQ.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    14/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 14

    Greater awareness in general has been argued to contribute to the development of greater

    metacognition (Kuhn, 2000). Flavels (1979) seminal work on metacognition asserts that the

    most fundamental contribution to the advancement of individual metacognitive abilities occurs

    when individuals become aware that others behavior is guided by beliefs and values which may

    not be congruent with their own. As people become more aware of their beliefs or stereotypes,

    during contact with members of different cultural groups, they become more reflective and

    cognizant of their thought processes, leading to greater metacognitive CQ.

    Greater stereotype awareness is also likely to relate to the enhancement of motivational

    CQ. As suggested by Rokeach (1979), lack of congruence between categorical representations

    of stimuli may result in the experience of self-dissatisfaction due to moral, or competence-

    related, issues. Guided by the hedonic principle of pursuing pleasure and avoiding pain,

    individuals are motivated to minimize this self-dissatisfaction (Higgins, 1998). They become

    motivated to learn more about the new person and new cultural environment they interact with

    (Rokeach, 1979).

    The development of stereotype awareness may also be viewed as a prerequisite of

    culturally intelligent behavior (MacNab & Worthley, 2010; Thomas & Inkson, 2004). Culturally

    intelligent behavior is characterized by greater awareness of nuances (e.g. stereotypes, norms,

    scripts, etc.) of different cultural systems and the ability to incorporate and express these nuances

    in behaviors during cross-cultural contacts (Earley & Ang, 2003). Thus:

    Hypothesis 7. The development of stereotype awareness is positively related to the

    development of: (a) metacognitive CQ; (b) motivational CQ; and (c) behavioral CQ.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    15/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 15

    Stereotype Alteration and the Development of CQ

    In addition to having greater stereotype awareness, most internationally effective

    managers are able to alter their stereotypes (Adler, 2002). Thus, we believe the ability to alter

    stereotypes is also associated with the development of CQ. For example, Ang and Van Dyne

    (2009) state that individuals who actively question cultural assumptions and adjust their own

    cognitions are likely to show greater levels of metacognitive CQ.

    Individuals willing to change their stereotypes are also likely to exhibit higher levels of

    motivational CQ. The motivational facet of cultural intelligence is positively associated with

    the personality trait of openness to experience (Ang, Van Dyne, & Koh, 2006). Flynn (2005)

    reports that those who score higher on openness to experience are more predisposed to consider

    stereotype-disconfirming evidence and alter their stereotypes.

    According to Walsh (1995), cognitive structures that individuals develop and change as

    a result of experience influence interpretation of environmental stimuli and subsequent behavior.

    Snyder, Tanke, and Berscheid (1977) suggest that stereotypes may guide and influence peoples

    interactions and serve as a basis for predictions of others future behavior. Individuals, who are

    likely to adjust their stereotypes, are also likely to modify their behavior to fit the newly updated

    stereotypes. Therefore, we posit:

    Hypothesis 8. The development in the ability to alter stereotypes is positively related to

    the development of: (a) metacognitive CQ; (b) motivational CQ; and (c) behavioral CQ.

    Overall, the extant literature reveals that optimal contact, disconfirmed expectancy,

    and stereotype awareness and alteration influence the development of metacognitive,

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    16/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 16

    motivational, and behavioral facets of cultural intelligence. Given the relationships described

    above, it is reasonable to speculate that disconfirmed expectancy, stereotype awareness, and

    stereotype alteration mediate the relationship between optimal contact and the development

    of CQ. However, since currently there is insufficient theoretical groundwork to make a-priori

    predictions, this study examines the mediating role of disconfirmed expectancy, stereotype

    awareness, and stereotype alteration in an exploratory manner within a process:

    Research Question 1: Does disconfirmed expectancy and the development of stereotype

    awareness and alteration mediate the relationship between optimal contact and the

    development of cultural intelligence?

    Methods

    Participants and Procedure

    Participants in the study were 367 graduate university management students and

    professionals who partook in a multi-week experiential CQ education process as part of an

    international management course. Of the trainees, who were on average 25.20 years old, 64%

    were female, 71% were Asian, and 70% had work experience.

    An important part of the CQ education process (see MacNab, in press) involved

    preparation for and participation in direct contact with members of a culturally different group.

    Participants were instructed to engage in a new cultural experience conforming to the optimal

    contact conditions:(a) equal status positions among participants and target group members; (b)

    establishments of common goals/grounds; (c) meaningful, personalized, one-on-one contact with

    members of the target culture group; and (d) as supported by the instructors and the authorities

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    17/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 17

    of the target culture group. Among the different types of experiences that participants partook

    included interactions with culturally-different religious groups, participation in culture-specific

    sport/wellness activities not common in ones own culture, or a significant interaction with a

    family from a different culture. Prior to the experience, participants were provided with an

    introduction to the concepts of optimal contact, disconfirmed expectancy, stereotype awareness

    and alteration, culture, cultural intelligence and CQ development. After the contact, participants

    were given an opportunity to reflect on the experience individually, and in a group, and were

    provided with feedback from the instructors.

    Following the experiential CQ education process, participants completed questionnaires

    assessing their experience of optimal contact and disconfirmed expectancy during the

    engagement. They also reflected on their levels of cultural intelligence, stereotype awareness,

    and stereotype alteration before andafter the CQ education process. Because all participants

    were enrolled in programs which required high levels of English proficiency, the study was

    conducted in English. All participants remained anonymous during the data collection process.

    They were encouraged to provide honest assessments and were assured that the information

    provided in the survey would have no influence on their course performance. Participants were

    offered an alternative, non-survey, reflective option if they did now wish to participate in the

    survey. The response rate was over 90%.

    Measures

    The items for this study were available from a combination of published validated scales

    and established research on contact theory, disconfirmed expectancy, stereotype awareness

    and alteration, and cultural intelligence (Ang, et al., 2007; MacNab, 2009, 2010; MacNab &

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    18/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 18

    Worthley, 2010, in press; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). All responses were made on a 5-point scale

    ranging from 1 =strongly disagreeto 5 =strongly agree.

    Development of cultural intelligence. The development of the metacognitive,

    motivational, and behavioral facets of CQ were assessed using the before and after approach,

    sometimes referred to as a retrospective pretest-posttest design (Howard, Schmeck, & Bray,

    1979, p. 130), with fourteen items from the CQ scale (Ang et al., 2007). For each CQ item,

    participants evaluated themselves in two temporal settings before and after. Before

    represents participant self-assessment of their level of CQ prior to partaking in the CQ education

    process in the beginning of the course. After represents participant self-assessment of their CQ

    following the CQ education process. These temporal measuring anchors were clearly explained

    both verbally and in writing to all participants. This temporal approach is recommended

    as one effective avenue for measuring complex interpersonal human attributes over time

    (Golembiewski, Billingsley, & Yeager, 1976; Howard & Dailey, 1979; Terborg, Maxwell, &

    Howard, 1982); it is deemed especially effective in educative settings where participants may not

    fully understand the measured concepts (like CQ) until after the training intervention, as in our

    study.

    The development of the three facets of cultural intelligence was determined as a

    difference between the before and after scores. Four items measured the development of

    metacognitive CQ (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.76). An example item is I am conscious of the

    cultural knowledge I use when interacting with people with different cultural backgrounds:

    Now (currently following the new culture experience); Then (before this semester). Five

    items measured the development of motivational CQ (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.80). An example

    item is I enjoy interaction with people from different cultures: Now (currently following the

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    19/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 19

    new culture experience); Then (before this semester). Five items measured the development

    of behavioral CQ (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.76). An example item is I use different facial

    expressions when a cross-cultural situation requires it: Now (currently following the new culture

    experience); Then (before this semester).

    Development of stereotype awareness. Five items measured participants levels of

    awareness of stereotypes in social interactions (MacNab, 2009) before and after the CQ

    education process. A sample item is I am aware of my stereotype perceptions about other

    groups: Now (currently following the new culture experience); Then (before this semester).

    The development of stereotype awareness was determined as a difference between the before and

    after scores for all five items (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.77).

    Development of stereotype alteration. Five items assessed participants ability to alter

    stereotypes in social interactions (MacNab, 2009) before and after the CQ education process.

    An example item is I am capable of changing my stereotypes about people I interact with:

    Now (currently following the new culture experience); Then (before this semester). The

    development of stereotype alteration was determined as a difference between the before and after

    scores for the five items (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.81).

    Experience of disconfirmed expectancy.Four items allowed participants to reported

    their experience of disconfirmed expectancy (MacNab, 2009) during contact with members

    of a different cultural group (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.75). A sample item is, I am aware that I

    sometimes experience the unexpected during a new culture experience.

    Optimal contact. Twenty one items were used to assess the four conditions of optimal

    contact (MacNab, et al., in press). Five items measuring equal status (e.g., People did not

    attempt to dominate each other during the experience) were averaged to yield the equal

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    20/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 20

    status score (Cronbach's alpha = 0.75). Five items measuring common goals (e.g., For both

    myself and the group (new contact culture) I interacted with, there were common goals) were

    averaged to yield the common goals score (Cronbach's alpha = 0.77). Five items measuring

    personalized contact (e.g., I came to know some people in the other group (new contact culture)

    on a personal level) were averaged to yield the personalized contact score (Cronbach's alpha

    = 0.72). Three items measuring support of authorities (e.g., I felt the instructor encouraged

    me to participate in this cultural experience) were averaged to yield the support of authorities

    score (Cronbach's alpha = 0.77). Three items measuring support of authorities of the other

    group (e.g., Leaders of the other group (new contact culture) had a positive attitude toward this

    interaction) were averaged to yield the support of other authorities score (Cronbach's alpha =

    0.62). Cronbachs alpha of the scale with five averaged indicators was 0.70.

    Control variables. We controlled for age, gender (0 = male; 1 = female), ethnicity (0 =

    Asian, 1 = otherwise), and work experience (0 = no work experience; 1 = previous work

    experience) because these variables have been found to influence individual adjustment and CQ

    performance in cross-cultural environments (Chen, Liu, & Portnoy, in press).

    Analysis

    Structural equation modeling (SEM) procedures based on the analysis of covariance

    structures were used to test the proposed theoretical model. We have specified the model

    structure a-priori based on previous theoretical and empirical research and as such used a

    confirmatory approach to the data analysis. Analyses were conducted using the AMOS 17

    program. First, we tested the fit of the measurement model as recommended by Anderson and

    Gerbing (1988). Following the establishment of the measurement model, the data was matched

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    21/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 21

    to the hypothesized model and assessed for goodness-of-fit. The assessment of the model fit was

    based on multiple criteria consistent with the recommendations made by Hu and Bentler (1999):

    Normed chi-square (2 / df, values of 1 to 3 indicate good fit, Bollen, 1989); the comparative fit

    index (CFI, values close to 0.90, Bollen, 1989); the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI, values close to

    0.95, Bentler & Bonett, 1980); and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA,

    values below 0.05, Browne & Cudeck, 1993). The hypotheses were tested utilizing path

    analysis. Non-significant parameters were deleted, and the final best-fitting model was

    determined using the 2 difference test. In the final reduced model, the mediated effects were

    examined and tested for significance using the Bootstrap estimation procedure in AMOS with

    95% bootstrapping confidence intervals recommended by Cheung and Lau (2008). All analyses

    used covariance matrix and maximum likelihood estimation.

    Results

    Table 1 show descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations.

    Measurement Model

    In the model, optimal contact, experience of disconfirmed expectancy, development

    of stereotype awareness, development of stereotype alteration, and development of cultural

    intelligence were latent constructs. For the control variables, single indicators were used

    assuming that they were measured without error. The measurement model provided an

    acceptable fit to the data: 2/df = 1.482, CFI = .929; TLI = .917; and RMSEA = .036. Standard

    factor loadings ranged from .41 to .77.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    22/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 22

    Hypothesized model

    The hypothesized model provided an acceptable fit to the data: 2/df = 1.499; CFI = .925;

    TLI = .914; and RMSEA = .037. Overall, it is estimated that all predictors of the development

    of metacognitive CQ explain 46.8 percent of its variance; all predictors of the development of

    motivational CQ explain 34.1 percent of its variance; and all predictors of the development of

    behavioral CQ explain 52.7 percent of its variance. Figure 1 presents the results of the path

    analysis with standardized parameter estimates (for ease of presentation control variables are not

    depicted in the figure).

    Among the control variables, work experience was significantly and positively related to

    the experience of disconfirmed expectancy (standardized parameter estimate = 0.13, p= 0.03).

    Age was related positively to the development of stereotype awareness (standardized parameter

    estimate = 0.13,p= 0.04). Those who identified themselves as Asian demonstrated less

    profound experience of disconfirmed expectancy (standardized parameter estimate = 0.20,p=

    0.001) but more profound motivational CQ development (standardized parameter estimate = -

    0.18,p= 0.003).

    Directly, optimal contact was related to the development of only metacognitive CQ

    (Hypothesis 1a is supported), but not behavioral or motivational CQ (Hypothesis 1b and 1c

    are not supported). Optimal contact was also directly associated with participants experience

    of disconfirmed expectancy (Hypothesis 2 is supported). Optimal contact was only indirectly

    related to the development of stereotype awareness and alteration by the mediation of

    disconfirmed expectancy (Hypothesis 3 is not supported). While the experience of disconfirmed

    expectancy was directly linked to the development of stereotype awareness (Hypothesis 4a is

    supported), it was not so to stereotype alteration (Hypothesis 4b is not supported). Furthermore,

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    23/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 23

    the experience of disconfirmed expectancy was not related directly to the development of

    the three facets of cultural intelligence (Hypothesis 5 is not supported). As predicted, the

    development of stereotype awareness was positively linked to the increased capability to alter

    stereotypes (Hypothesis 6 is supported). In addition, greater stereotype awareness was also

    associated with the development of metacognitive CQ and behavioral CQ (Hypothesis 7a and

    7c are supported), but not motivational CQ (Hypothesis 7b is not supported). Greater ability to

    alter stereotypes was found to be directly related to the advancement of motivational CQ and

    behavioral CQ (Hypothesis 8b and 8c are supported), but not metacognitive CQ (Hypothesis 8a

    is not supported).

    We compared the fit of our proposed model to an alternative trimmed model, which

    dropped nonsignificant paths. This respecification resulted in no significant change in statistical

    fit according to the 2 difference test, albeit a more parsimonious model: 2/df = 1.465; CFI

    = .926; TLI=.920; RMSEA = 0.036. Figure 3 presents the reduced model with standardized

    parameter estimates. The exploratory mediation analyses of the reduced model show that, in

    addition to being directly related to the development of metacognitive CQ, optimal contact is

    also related to the development of metacognitive CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate =

    0.08,p= 0.002), motivational CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.05, p= 0.001), and

    behavioral CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.08,p= 0.002) indirectly by means of

    disconfirmed expectancy and stereotype awareness plus stereotype alteration. The experience of

    disconfirmed expectancy mediates the relationship between optimal contact and the development

    of stereotype awareness (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.12,p= 0.002). Both

    disconfirmed expectancy and the development of stereotype awareness support the link between

    optimal contact and the development of the ability to alter stereotypes (standardized indirect

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    24/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 24

    effect estimate = 0.10,p= 0.002). Stereotype awareness mediates the relationship of the

    experience of disconfirmed expectancy with the development of the ability to alter stereotypes

    (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.23,p= 0.002) and the development of metacognitive

    CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.18,p= 0.002). Together the development of

    stereotype awareness and stereotype alteration support the connection between the experience of

    disconfirmed expectancy and the development of motivational CQ (standardized indirect effect

    estimate = 0.12,p= 0.001) and behavioral CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.19,p=

    0.001).

    Common Method Variance.Common method variance (CMV) may be an issue for

    studies where data for the dependent and independent variables are collected from a single

    source using a single instrument. To provide a level of assurance that the statistical and practical

    significance of the results has not been influenced by CMV, we conducted two statistical

    procedures recommended by Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, and Podsakoff (2003). First, we

    conducted the widely used Harmans single factor test. In an exploratory factor analysis, a total

    of 33 items were loaded on a single latent factor. The model showed a poor fit (2/df = 3.751;

    CFI = .635; TLI=.608; RMSEA = .087), suggesting that a single common method factor does not

    account for the majority of the covariance among the measures. Second, we controlled for the

    effects of an unmeasured latent methods (CMV) factor. We allowed the items to load on their

    theoretical constructs as well as on a latent CMV factor. We then examined the significance of

    the structural paths in the model with and without the latent common method variance factor.

    The model encompassing the latent CMV factor showed a slightly better fit (2/df = 1.416; CFI

    = .941; TLI=.928; RMSEA = 0.034); however, the significance and the estimates associated with

    the structural paths remained practically unchanged.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    25/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 25

    Alternative models. A number of alternative models were tested where we

    changed the order of the variables in the model. The fit of the alternative models was compared

    using the descriptive model fit statistics test based on the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), in

    which the smaller AIC index value indicates better fit (Kline, 2011). Path analyses were also

    conducted. In one alternative model, the direction of the paths from the development of

    stereotype awareness and alteration to the three facets of cultural intelligence was reversed,

    suggesting that optimal contact and disconfirmed expectancy were first linked to the

    development of cultural intelligence which in turn was linked to the development of stereotype

    awareness and alteration. However, this model showed an inferior fit and a much larger AIC

    value (2/df = 1.579; CFI = .913; TLI = .900; RMSEA = .040; AIC = 1236.904) than the final

    model (2/df = 1.465; CFI = .926; TLI = .920; RMSEA = .036; AIC = 1152.353). In another

    model, all paths were reversed such that the development of cultural intelligence was associated

    with greater development of stereotype awareness and alteration, which in turn were associated

    with greater experience of disconfirmed expectancy and ultimately greater optimal contact

    experience. The fit of this model (2/df = 1.595; CFI = .911; TLI = .897; RMSEA = .040; AIC =

    1246.090) was also inferior to the final model. In addition, the paths from the development of

    stereotype awareness and alteration to the experience of disconfirmed expectancy and optimal

    contact were not significant.

    Supplementary Analyses

    To provide a greater level of assurance that our results have not been influenced by the

    CMV bias, which may have resulted from the fact that the participants reported their

    development of stereotype awareness, stereotype alteration, and cultural intelligence scores

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    26/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 26

    retrospectively in a single post-intervention survey, we conducted supplementary analyses. A

    subset of our sample (N= 229) reported their levels of stereotype awareness, stereotype

    alteration, and cultural intelligence prior to and after engaging in the CQ education program. So

    instead of calculating the development scores using the before and after, or retrospective

    pretest-posttest design approach as described previously, we used the pre and post approach.

    The pre represents the participants non-retrospective and real-time report of their level of CQ

    prior to the CQ education process. An example of a pre-intervention item from the development

    of metacognitive CQ scale is I am conscious of the cultural knowledge I use when interacting

    with people with different cultural backgrounds: Now (currently). The post represents the

    participants report of their CQ levels following the CQ education process. An example of a

    post-intervention item from the development of metacognitive CQ scale is I am conscious of

    the cultural knowledge I use when interacting with people with different cultural backgrounds:

    Now (currently, following the new culture experience). The development of the three facets of

    cultural intelligence, stereotype awareness, and stereotype alteration was determined as a

    difference between the pre and post scores.

    We tested the proposed theoretical model using the data from the smaller subsample

    containing the pre and post scores using the same analytical procedures described above.

    Optimal contact (5 items, Cronbachs alpha = 0.70), experience of disconfirmed expectancy (4

    items, Cronbachs alpha = 0.73), development of stereotype awareness (5 items, Cronbachs

    alpha = 0.66), development of stereotype alteration (5 items, Cronbachs alpha = 0.77),

    development of metacognitive CQ (4 items, Cronbachs alpha = 0.75), development of

    motivational CQ (5 items, Cronbachs alpha = 0.68), and development of behavioral CQ (5

    items, Cronbachs alpha = 0.65) were represented as latent constructs in the model. Single

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    27/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 27

    indicators were used for the control variables. Standard factor loadings ranged from .33 to .75.

    Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics and zero order correlations for the pre and post

    data sample. First, we estimated the recursive measurement model with confirmatory factor

    analysis in AMOS 17. The measurement model fit was acceptable: 2/df = 1.200; CFI = .934;

    TLI=.923; RMSEA = 0.030. Second, we assessed the hypothesized model using structural

    equation modeling in AMOS 17. The results showed an acceptable fit of the data to the

    hypothesized model: 2/df = 1.225; CFI = .925; TLI=.914; RMSEA = 0.031. Next, we dropped

    the nonsignificant paths. The trimmed model also showed an acceptable fit 2/df = 1.207;

    CFI = .927; TLI=.921; RMSEA = 0.030. The difference in chi-square between the full and

    the reduced model was not significant. Figure 4 presents the reduced model with standardized

    parameter estimates.

    The results of the analyses of this subsample based on the pre and post data for the

    development of stereotype awareness, alteration, and cultural intelligence (Figure 4) closely

    resemble the results based on the before and after data (Figure 3). Optimal contact was directly

    associated with the participants experience of disconfirmed expectancy. The experience of

    disconfirmed expectancy is positively associated with the development of stereotype awareness.

    The development of stereotype awareness was linked positively to the development of the ability

    to alter stereotypes and the development of metacognitive CQ. The development of stereotype

    alteration is positively associated with the development of motivational CQ and behavioral

    CQ. Unlike the results based on the before and after data, optimal contact was not directly

    related to the development of metacognitive CQ in the pre and post measure model. In addition,

    there was no significant link from the development of stereotype awareness to the development

    of behavioral CQ. However, the association between the development of the ability to alter

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    28/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 28

    stereotypes and the development of behavioral CQ was stronger in the model based on pre and

    post data. Like the before and after measure model, the pre and post measure model also shows

    the complimentary role of both stereotype awareness and alteration as influential (together) on

    all aspects of CQ development.

    Corroborating the results based on the before and after data, the results of the exploratory

    mediation analyses based on the pre and post data show that disconfirmed expectancy, stereotype

    awareness, and stereotype alteration mediate the relationship between optimal contact and the

    development of metacognitive CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.09, p= 0.001),

    motivational CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.07,p= 0.001), and behavioral CQ

    (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.07,p= 0.001). Disconfirmed expectancy mediates the

    relationship between optimal contact and the development of stereotype awareness (standardized

    indirect effect estimate = 0.14,p= 0.001). Both disconfirmed expectancy and the development

    of stereotype awareness support the connection between optimal contact and the development

    of the ability to alter stereotypes (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.12,p= 0.001).

    Stereotype awareness mediates the relationship of the experience of disconfirmed expectancy

    with the development of the ability to alter stereotypes (standardized indirect effect estimate =

    0.32,p= 0.002) and the development of metacognitive CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate

    = 0.23,p= 0.002). Together the development of stereotype awareness and stereotype alteration

    support the connection between the experience of disconfirmed expectancy and the development

    of behavioral CQ (standardized indirect effect estimate = 0.19,p= 0.001).

    Discussion

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    29/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 29

    In this study, we introduced and tested a model explicating how optimal contact

    with members of a different cultural group influences the development of metacognitive,

    motivational, and behavioral aspects of individual cultural intelligence development. We

    posited that optimal contact contributes to the advancement of cultural intelligence directly

    and mediationally by providing individuals with an opportunity to experience disconfirmed

    expectancy, by raising individual awareness of stereotypes, and by enhancing the ability to

    change stereotypes.

    What stands out the most from our results based on both before-and-after and pre-and-

    post data is that, mediationally, optimal contact was linked positively to all three examined

    aspects of cultural intelligence. This finding is consistent with the social cognitive perspective

    (Bandura, 1977; 1986) stating that experiences contribute to the development of self-awareness,

    self-control, and knowledge structures which later influence attitudes and behaviors. Our

    study also provides empirical evidence in support of experiential learning theory (Kolb, 1984)

    and experiential CQ education (MacNab, in press), showing the important role of awareness,

    reflection, thinking, and doing in the effective transformation of experience into learning.

    Another interesting finding is that the development of stereotype awareness is positively

    related to the development of metacognitive CQ, but not behavioral CQ. On the other hand, the

    ability to alter stereotypes is positively linked to the development of behavioral CQ, but not

    metacognitive CQ. To develop conscious cultural awareness during cross-cultural interactions,

    individuals need to become more aware of stereotypes they are holding about others and

    stereotypes others hold about them. To be able to exhibit appropriate behaviors when interacting

    with people from different cultures, an adjustment of cognitive structures, or stereotypes, appears

    to be significant. Thus, stereotype awareness and stereotype alteration perform complementary

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    30/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 30

    roles in the development of cultural intelligence and should likely be important considerations in

    CQ training, development, and education. Adler (2002) also underscored the importance of both

    stereotype awareness and alteration in the development of internationally effective individuals.

    Our study empirically links the development of both stereotype awareness and alteration to the

    enhancement of CQ and shows their complementary effects.

    Finally, our model emphasizes the important role of disconfirmed expectancy in the

    development of stereotype awareness, stereotype alteration, and all examined facets of cultural

    intelligence. Disconfirmed expectancy is shown to mediate the impact of optimal contact on

    the development of stereotype awareness, alteration, and CQ. The influence of disconfirmed

    expectancy on stereotype awareness and stereotype alteration has been conceptually and

    empirically illustrated by Monteith (1993). Theoretically, the role of disconfirmed expectancy in

    the development of cultural competencies has been stressed by Cushner and Brislin (1996) and

    Bhawuk (2009a). This study empirically shows the mediating role of disconfirmed expectancy

    on the development of CQ. Overall, guided understanding and experience of disconfirmed

    expectancy is a critical part of the experiential CQ education process.

    There are a number of limitations for this study. To fully understand the benefits

    of direct experience with the members of different cultural groups built around the optimal

    contact conditions as suggested by Allport (1954), experimental solutions should be developed

    encompassing a control group. Given the requirements encountered during our university

    human subjects review process (coupled with semester timeframe limitations), designing a true

    experimental version of this project, with a control group, was not immediately feasible. For

    this reason, future research could design experimental approaches as a progression of this current

    work. Future studies should also examine this process using different samples and measures.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    31/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 31

    Practical Implications

    The results of this work are essential for organizations looking for ways to increase their

    members ability to adjust, function, and perform effectively in culturally diverse contexts.

    First, although cognitive approaches to the development of CQ have their value, the inclusion

    ofguidedoptimal contact with members of a different cultural group may provide a richer

    environment to enhance cultural intelligence. Second, introduction of contact during cross-

    cultural training may allow trainers to target static as well as dynamic aspects of CQ. As

    suggested by Rockstuhl and Ng (2009), existing programs tend to focus too narrowly on

    increasing individuals knowledge, such as knowledge of cultural differences. Third, this

    research points out the benefits of the optimal contact characteristics for inter-group, experiential

    training programs; not all types of contact may positively influence the development of cultural

    intelligence. It was also suggested by Brislin and colleagues (2008) that cross-cultural training

    research needs to more carefully consider context in relation to training outcomes.

    Finally, our study demonstrates the importance of theprocessin the development of

    CQ. The advancement of all three facets of cultural intelligence was mediated by a series of

    other constructs. If training participants are involved in a cross-cultural encounter, but do not

    experience disconfirmed expectancy along with stereotype awareness and alteration, the benefit

    to the development of CQ might be lower or otherwise deficient. Thus, although a process-based

    approach to cultural intelligence requires more time, effort and expertise, it is likely to prepare

    individuals for cross-cultural encounters more effectively.

    In summary, based on our findings we recommend that effective experiential CQ

    education endeavors: (a) include a foundation that integrates optimal contact as related to contact

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    32/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 32

    theory; (b) provide conditions for the encounter of guided cultural disconfirmed expectancy; (c)

    steer the participants toward identification and awareness of held stereotypes; and (d) guide the

    participants toward challenging and altering those held stereotypes.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    33/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 33

    Bibliography

    Adams, J. S. (1963). Toward an understanding of inequity.Journal of Abnormal Psychology,

    67(5), 422-436.

    Adler, N. J. (2002).International dimensions of organizational behavior. Cincinnati, OH:

    Thompson Learning.

    Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice: Addison-Wesley.

    Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review

    and recommended two-step approach.Psychological Bulletin, 103(3), 411-423.

    Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. (2009). Conceptualization of cultural intelligence: Definition,

    distinctiveness, and nomological network. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (Eds.), Handbook of

    cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications(pp. 3-15). Armonk, NY:

    M.E. Sharpe.

    Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., & Koh, C. (2006). Personality correlates of the four-factor model of

    cultural intelligence. Group and Organizational Management, 31(1), 100-123.

    Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., Koh, C., Ng, K. Y., Templer, K. J., Tay, C., et al. (2007). Cultural

    intelligence: Its measurement and effects on cultural judgment and decision making,

    cultural adaptation and task performance.Management and Organization Review, 3(3),

    335-371.

    Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-

    Hall.

    Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of

    covariance structures.Psychological Bulletin, 88(3), 588-606.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    34/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 34

    Berry, J., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (2002). Acculturation and intercultural

    relations. In J. W. Berry, Y. H. Poortinga, M. H. Segall & P. R. Dasen (Eds.), Cross-

    cultural psychology: Research and applications(2 ed., pp. 345-383). Cambridge, UK:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Berry, J., & Ward, C. (2006). Commentary on redefining interactions across cultures and

    organizations. Group and Organization Management, 31(1), 64-77.

    Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2001). Evolution of culture assimilators: Toward theory-based assimilators.

    International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 25(2), 141-163.

    Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2009a). Competence development and triple-loop cultural training. In S. Ang

    & L. Van Dyne (Eds.),Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and

    applications(pp. 342-355). Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe.

    Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2009b). Intercultural training for the global workplace: Review, synthesis, and

    theoretical explorations. In R. S. Bhagat & R. M. Steers (Eds.), Culture, organization,

    and work(pp. 462-488). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York, NY: Wiley-

    Interscience Publication.

    Brislin, R. W. (1981). Cross-cultural encounters: Face-to-face interactions. New York, NY:

    Pergamon.

    Brislin, R. W., & Macnab, B. (2004). Inter-cultural communication and effective decision

    making: Contributions to successful international business ventures. In K. Leung & S.

    White (Eds.),Handbook of Asian Management(pp. 397-414). New York: Kluwar.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    35/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 35

    Brislin, R. W., MacNab, B., & Nayani, F. (2008). Cross-cultural training: Applications and

    research. In P. B. Smith, M. F. Peterson & D. Thomas (Eds.),Handbook of cross-cultural

    management research(pp. 397-410). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Brislin, R. W., Worthley, R., & MacNab, B. (2006). Cultural intelligence: Understanding

    behaviors that serve people's goals. Group & Organization Management, 31(1), 40-55.

    Brown, R., & Hewstone, M. (2005). An integrative theory of intergroup contact.Advances in

    Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 255-343.

    Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing fit. In K. A. Bollen & J.

    S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models(pp. 136162). Newbury Park, CA:

    Sage.

    Caligiuri, P. M. (2000). Selecting expatriates for personality characteristics: A moderating

    effect of personality on the relationship between host national contact and cross-cultural

    adjustment.Management International Review, 40(1), 61-81.

    Chen, X.-P., Liu, D., & Portnoy, R. (in press). A multilevel investigation of motivational cultural

    intelligence, organizational diversity climate, and cultural sales: Evidence from U.S. real

    estate firms.Journal of Applied Psychology.

    Cheung, G. W., & Lau, R. S. (2008). Testing mediation and suppression effects of latent

    variables: Boostrapping with structural equation models. Organizational Research

    Methods, 11(2), 296-325.

    Church, A. T. (1982). Sojourner adjustment.Psychological Bulletin, 91(3), 540-572.

    Cook, S. W. (1978). Interpersonal and attitudinal outcomes in cooperating interracial groups.

    Journal of Research and Development in Education, 12(1), 97-113.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    36/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 36

    Cooper, C. L., & Kirkcaldy, B. D. (1995). Executive stereotyping between cultures: The British

    vs. German manager.Journal of Managerial Psychology, 10(1), 3-6.

    Crocker, J., Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Schematic bases of belief change. In R. Eiser

    (Ed.),Attitudinal judgment(pp. 197-226). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

    Crowne, K. A. (2008). What leads to cultural intelligence?Business Horizons, 51(5), 391-399.

    Cushner, K., & Brislin, R. W. (1996).Intercultural interactions: A practical guide. Thousand

    Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Dewey, J. (1938).Experience and education. New York, NY: Dover.

    Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Kawakami, K. (2003). Intergroup contact: The past, present,

    and the future. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 6(1), 5-21.

    Earley, P. C., & Ang, S. (2003). Cultural intelligence: Individual interactions across cultures.

    Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Festinger, L. (1957).A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Flaherty, J. E. (2009). The effects of cultural intelligence on team member acceptance and

    integration in multinational teams. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (Eds.),Handbook of

    cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications(pp. 192-205). New York,

    NY: M.E. Sharpe, Inc.

    Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive inquiry.

    American Psychologist, 34(10), 906-911.

    Flynn, F. J. (2005). Having an open mind: The impact of openness to experience on interracial

    attitudes and impression formation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5),

    816-826.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    37/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 37

    Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000).Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity

    model. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

    Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., Banker, B. S., Houlette, M., Johnson, K. M., & McGlynn, E.

    A. (2000). Reducing intergroup conflict: From superordinate goals to decategorization,

    recategorization, and mutual differentiation. Group Dynamics, 4, 98-114.

    Golembiewski, R. T., Billingsley, K., & Yeager, S. (1976). Measuring change and persistence

    in human affairs: Types of change generated by OD designs. The Journal of Applied

    Behavioral Science, 12(2), 133-157.

    Hewstone, M. (1996). Contact and categorization: Social psychological interventions to change

    intergroup relations. In N. Macrae, C. Stangor & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Stereotypes and

    stereotyping(pp. 323-368). New York, NY: Guildford.

    Hewstone, M., & Brown, R. (1986). Contact is not enough: An intergroup perspective on

    the 'contact hypothesis'. In M. Hewstone & R. Brown (Eds.), Contact and conflict in

    integroup encounters(pp. 1-44). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle.

    Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 1-46.

    Hill, M. E., & Augoustinos, M. (2001). Stereotype change and prejudice reduction: Short- and

    long-term evaluation of a crosscultural awareness programme.Journal of Community &

    Applied Social Psychology, 11(4), 243-262.

    Howard, G. S., & Dailey, P. R. (1979). Response-shift bias: A source of contamination of self-

    report measures.Journal of Applied Psychology, 64(2), 144-150.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    38/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 38

    Howard, G. S., Schmeck, R. R., & Bray, J. H. (1979). Internal invalidity in studies employing

    self-report instruments: A suggested remedy.Journal of Educational Measurement,

    16(2), 129-135.

    Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:

    Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1-55.

    Itin, C. (1999). Reasserting the philosophy of experiential education as a vehicle for change in

    the 21st century. The Journal of Experiential Education, 22(2), 91-98.

    Kim, Y. J., & Van Dyne, L. (2011). Cultural intelligence and international leadership potential:

    The importance of contact for members of the majority.Applied psychology: An

    International Review.

    Kim, Y. J., & Van Dyne, L. (in press). Cultural intelligence and international leadership

    potential: The importance of contact for members of the majority.Applied psychology:

    An International Review.

    Kline, R. B. (2011).Principles and practice of structural equation modeling(3rd ed.). New

    York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Kolb, D. A. (1976). Management and the learning process. California Management Review,

    18(3), 21-31.

    Kolb, D. A. (1984).Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and

    development. New York, NY: Prentice Hall.

    Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science,

    9(5), 178-181.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    39/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 39

    MacNab, B. (2009).Experiential stereotype awareness training: The relevance of context and

    self-efficacy. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management,

    Chicago, IL.

    MacNab, B. (2010).An experiential approach to cultural intelligence education. Working paper.

    University of Sydney. Sydney, Australia.

    MacNab, B. (in press). An experiential approach to cultural intelligence education.Journal of

    Management Education.

    MacNab, B., Brislin, R., & Worthley, R. (in press). Experiential cultural intelligence

    development: Context and individual attributes.International Journal of Human

    Resource Management.

    MacNab, B., & Worthley, R. (2010).An experimental approach to teaching cultural

    intellingence: Individual and contextual influences.Paper presented at the annual

    meeting of the Academy of Management, Montreal, Canada.

    MacNab, B., & Worthley, R. (in press). Individual characteristics as predictors of cultural

    intelligence development: The relevance of self-efficacy.International Journal of

    Intercultural Relations, forthcoming.

    MacNab, B., Worthley, R., & Brislin, R. W. (2007).An experiential approach to teaching

    cultural intellingence: The relevance of context and intra-personal characteristics

    within the learning process. Paper presented at the the annual Applied Business and

    Entrepreneurship Association International Conference, Maui, HI.

    Monteith, M. J. (1993). Self-regulation of prejudiced responses: Implications for progress in

    prejudice-reduction efforts.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(3), 469-

    485.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    40/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 40

    Ng, K.-Y., Van Dyne, L., & Ang, S. (2009). From experience to experiential learning: Cultural

    intelligence as a learning capability for global leader development. Academy of

    Management Learning & Education, 8(4), 511-526.

    Osland, J. S., Bird, A., Delano, J., & Jacob, M. (2000). Beyond sophisticated stereotyping:

    Cultural sensemaking in context.Academy of Management Executive, 14(1), 65-79.

    Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory.Annual Review of Psychology, 49(1), 65-85.

    Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(5), 751-783.

    Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method

    biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended

    remedies.Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879-903.

    Rockstuhl, T., & Ng, K. Y. (2009). The effects of cultural intelligence on interpersonal trust in

    multicultural teams. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (Eds.),Handbook of cultural intelligence:

    Theory, measurement, and applications(pp. 206-220). New York, NY: M.E. Sharpe, Inc.

    Rokeach, M. (1979). Understanding human values: Individual and societal. New York, NY:

    Free Press.

    Rothbart, M. (1981). Memory processes and social beliefs. In D. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive

    processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior(pp. 145-181). Hillsdale, N.J.:

    Erlbaum.

    Shannon, L. M., & Begley, T. M. (2009). Antecedents of the four-factor model of cultural

    intelligence. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (Eds.),Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory,

    measurement, and applications(pp. 41-55). Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe.

    Snyder, M. (1982). Self-fulfilling stereotypes.Psychology Today, July, 60-68.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    41/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 41

    Snyder, M., & Stukas Jr, A. (1999). Interpersonal processes: The interplay of cognitive,

    motivational, and behavioral activities in social interaction.Annual Review of

    Psychology, 50(1), 273-303.

    Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior:

    On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes.Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 35(9), 656-666.

    Sternberg, R. J., & Detterman, D. (1986). What is intelligence?Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.

    Tan, J.-S., & Chua, R. Y.-J. (2003). Training and developing cultural intelligence. In P. C. Earley

    & S. Ang (Eds.), Cultural intelligence: Individual interactions across cultures(pp. 258-

    303). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Tarique, I., & Takeuchi, R. (2009). Developing cultural intelligence: The roles of international

    nonwork experience. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (Eds.),Handbook of cultural intelligence:

    Theory, measurement, and applications(pp. 56-70). New York, NY: M.E. Sharpe, Inc.

    Tay, C., Westman, M., & Chia, A. (2009). Antecedents and consequences of cultural intelligence

    among short-term business travellers. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (Eds.),Handbook of

    cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications(pp. 126-144). Armonk,

    NY: M.E. Sharpe.

    Terborg, J. R., Maxwell, S. E., & Howard, G. S. (1982). On the measurement and control of

    beta change: Problems with the Bedeian, Armenakis, and Gibson technique.Academy of

    Management Review, 7(2), 292-295.

    Thomas, D. C., & Inkson, K. (2004). Cultural intelligence: People skills for global business. San

    Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler Publishers.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    42/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 42

    Walsh, J. P. (1995). Managerial and organizational cognition: Notes from a trip down memory

    lane. Organization Science, 6(3), 280-321.

    Weber, R., & Crocker, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in the revision of stereotypic beliefs.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(5), 961-977.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    43/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 43

    Table 1

    Means, Standard Deviations, and Zero-Order Correlations

    Variable Mean S.D. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

    1. Optimal Contact 4.08 0.41 (0.70)

    2. Disconfirmed Expectancy 4.16 0.50 0.29* (0.75)

    3. Stereotype Awareness 1.05 0.68 0.07 0.21* (0.77)

    4. Stereotype Alteration 0.82 0.62 -0.03 0.10 0.65* (0.81)

    5. Metacognitive CQ 0.87 0.64 0.14* 0.20* 0.49* 0.43* (0.76)

    6. Motivational CQ 0.56 0.60 0.04 0.02 0.32* 0.45* 0.39* (0.80)

    7. Behavioral CQ 0.65 0.55 0.09 0.11* 0.50* 0.50* 0.38* 0.46* (0.76)

    8. Female 0.64 0.48 0.05 0.03 0.07 0.06 0.04 0.05 -0.02

    9. Age 25.20 3.99 -0.01 0.07 0.09 0.13* 0.06 0.02 0.05 -0.15*

    10. Work Experience a 0.70 0.46 0.01 0.17* 0.03 0.01 0.01 -0.05 -0.04 -0.05 0.28*

    11. Ethnicity b 0.29 0.46 0.04 0.23* -0.06 -0.09 -0.04 -0.21* -0.04 -0.17* 0.21* 0.32*

    Note: N= 367. Values in parentheses represent the reliability (Cronbachs Alpha) for the scale; signifies development, assessed as

    a difference between beforeand afterscores. Before represents participants retrospectiveself-assessment scores of their stereotype

    awareness, stereotype alteration, and CQ prior to partaking in the CQ education process reported following the intervention. After

    represents participants self-assessment scores of the same constructs following the CQ education process.

    a Binary variable (0 = no, 1 = yes);

    b Binary variable (0 = Asian, 1 = Non-Asian);

    * Significant at least atp< 0.05.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    44/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 44

    Table 2

    Means, Standard Deviations, and Zero-Order Correlations

    Variable Mean S.D. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

    1. Optimal Contact 4.08 0.43 (0.70)

    2. Disconfirmed Expectancy 4.18 0.47 0.27* (0.73)

    3. Stereotype Awareness 0.43 0.58 0.05 0.20* (0.66)

    4. Stereotype Alteration 0.33 0.68 0.03 0.12 0.55* (0.77)

    5. Metacognitive CQ 0.47 0.71 0.05 0.18* 0.38* 0.39* (0.75)

    6. Motivational CQ 0.09 0.62 0.05 0.12 0.35* 0.37* 0.29* (0.68)

    7. Behavioral CQ 0.25 0.66 -0.01 0.13 0.31* 0.41* 0.33* 0.24* (0.65)

    8. Female 0.62 0.49 0.05 0.03 0.08 0.08 0.01 0.07 0.04

    9. Age 25.39 4.23 -0.05 0.16* -0.03 0.05 0.09 -0.01 0.14* -0.16*

    11. Work Experience a 0.72 0.45 0.03 0.17* -0.06 0.12 0.11 -0.06 -0.10 0.01 0.29*

    12. Ethnicity b 0.35 0.48 0.10 0.29* -0.10 -0.12 -0.01 -0.09 -0.10 -0.18* 0.23* 0.42*

    Note: N=229. Values in parentheses represent the reliability (Cronbachs Alpha) for the scale; signifies development, assessed as

    a difference betweenpreandpostscores. Pre represents participants self-assessment scores of their stereotype awareness, stereotype

    alteration, and CQ prior to partaking in the CQ education process reported prior to the intervention. Post represents participants self-

    assessment scores of the same constructs following the CQ education process.

    a Binary variable (0 = no, 1 = yes);

    b Binary variable (0 = Asian, 1 = Non-Asian);

    * Significant at least atp< 0.05.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    45/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 45

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    46/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 46

    Figure 1:Hypothesized model of the relationship between optimal contact, experience of

    disconfirmed expectancy, development of stereotype awareness, development of the ability to

    alter stereotypes, and development of the three examined facets of cultural intelligence. For

    the ease of presentation control variables and paths from the four control variables to all other

    constructs are omitted.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    47/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 47

    Figure 2: Structural equation model results for the full hypothesized model.

    Note: signifies development, assessed as a difference between beforeand afterintervention

    scores. Before represents participants retrospectiveself-assessment scores of their stereotype

    awareness, stereotype alteration, and CQ prior to partaking in the CQ education process reported

    following the intervention. After represents participants self-assessment scores of the same

    constructs following the CQ education process. Although the model does not depict this for the

    ease of presentation, we controlled for the effects of age, gender, ethnicity, and work experience.

    Standardized path coefficients are reported. Bold lines signify significant paths, dashed lines

    signify nonsignificant paths.

    N= 367; *p< 0.05; **p< 0.01; ***p< 0.001.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    48/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 48

    Figure 3: Structural equation model results for the reduced model.

    Note: signifies development, assessed as a difference between beforeand afterintervention

    scores. Before represents participants retrospectiveself-assessment scores of their stereotype

    awareness, stereotype alteration, and CQ prior to partaking in the CQ education process reported

    following the intervention. After represents participants self-assessment scores of the same

    constructs following the CQ education process. Although the model does not depict this for the

    ease of presentation, we controlled for the effects of age, gender, ethnicity, and work experience.

    Standardized path coefficients are reported.

    N= 367; *p< 0.05; **p< 0.01; ***p< 0.001.

  • 5/24/2018 CQ Example 5

    49/49

    A PATHWAY TO CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE VIA CONTACT 49

    Figure 4: Structural equation model results for the reduced model based on pre and post

    development scores.

    Note: signifies development, assessed as a difference betweenpreandpostintervention

    scores. Pre represents participants self-assessment scores of their stereotype awareness,

    stereotype alteration, and CQ prior to partaking in the CQ education process reported prior to

    the intervention. Post represents participants self-assessment scores of the same constructs

    following the CQ education process. Although the model does not depict this for the ease

    of presentation, we controlled for the effects of age, gender, ethnicity, and work experience.

    Standardized path coefficients are reported.

    N= 229; *p< 0.05; **p< 0.01; ***p< 0.001.