development of social-moral reasoning among kibbutz ... · pdf filedevelopment of social-moral...

16
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232600877 Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A Longitudinal Cross-Cultural Study ARTICLE in DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY · DECEMBER 1984 Impact Factor: 3.21 · DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.1.3 CITATIONS 50 READS 350 3 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: John Snarey Emory University 105 PUBLICATIONS 1,206 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Available from: John Snarey Retrieved on: 15 March 2016

Upload: lamanh

Post on 18-Mar-2018

226 views

Category:

Documents


5 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

Seediscussions,stats,andauthorprofilesforthispublicationat:https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232600877

DevelopmentofSocial-MoralReasoningAmongKibbutzAdolescents.ALongitudinalCross-CulturalStudy

ARTICLEinDEVELOPMENTALPSYCHOLOGY·DECEMBER1984

ImpactFactor:3.21·DOI:10.1037/0012-1649.21.1.3

CITATIONS

50

READS

350

3AUTHORS,INCLUDING:

JohnSnarey

EmoryUniversity

105PUBLICATIONS1,206CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

Availablefrom:JohnSnarey

Retrievedon:15March2016

Page 2: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

Developmental Psychology In the public domain 1985, VoL 21, NO. 1, 3-17

Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents: A Longitudinal Cross-Cultural Study

J o h n R. Snarey, Joseph Reimer, and Lawrence Kohlbc rg Harvard University

The development of social-moral judgment among Israeli kibbutz adolescents was studied from the perspective of Kohlberg's theory of moral judgment development. The sample included 92 adolescents, 64 of whom were interviewed longitudinally over a two-to-nine year period. The study's purpose was to evaluate the validity of Kohlberg's model and measure in a cross-cultural context and to assess the cultural uniqueness of social-moral reasoning among kibbutzniks. The developmental findings strongly supported the validity of Kohlberg's structural- developmental understanding of moral judgment. Stage change was found to be upward, gradual, and without significant regressions. Analyses also supported the internal consistency of the stages as operationally defined in the standardized scoring manual. There were no significant sex differences in moral development and fewer cultural differences than expected. Overall, the distribution of stage scores among the kibbutz subjects was unusually high when compared to the results of parallel studies in the United States and Turkey, the two previous longitudinal studies of moral judgment development that have used the standardized scoring system. The most important cultural variation involved the use of Stages 4/5 and 5. Whereas all of Kohlberg's stages were present among kibbutz members, not all elements of kibbutz postconventional reasoning were present in Kohlberg's model or scoring manual. In particular, the communal emphasis and collective moral principles of the kibbutz subjects were partially missed or misunderstood.

This article presents the results of a longi- tudinal study of social-moral reasoning among Israeli adolescents. The research oh-

This research was supported in part by a 3-year National Research Service Award (MH-14088) to the senior author from the National Institute of Mental Health.

A large constellation of people have provided crucial assistance or support during the research project. We must begin by thanking the educators and youth of Kibbutz Ramat Yedidim. They were unusually gracious and cooperative "subjects," and our lives are richer for having known them. Miriam Bar-Yam assisted Lawrence Kohlberg in collecting the first phase interviews, Mark Braverman assisted Joseph Reimer in collecting the middle phase interviews, and Anat Abrahami assisted John Snarey in collecting the final phase interviews. John Snarey conducted the data analysis and wrote this lon- gitudinal report. Joseph Blasi, Harry Lasker, Robert LeVine, Clark Power, Mcnachcm Rosner, Sandra Scarr, Carol Snarey, Terry Tivnan, and Philip Zodhiates offered many helpful suggestions on an initial draft of the article. Janet Mullaney provided careful editorial assistance.

Requests for reprints should be sent to John R. Snarey, Harvard University, Laboratory of Human Development, Larsen Hall #316, Appian Way, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138.

jectives were to evaluate Kohlberg's theory of social-moral development from the cross- cultural perspective of an Israeli kibbutz and to gain an understanding of the cultural uniqueness of kibbutz social-moral reasoning from the perspective of Kohlberg's theory.

The data presented, based on a sample of 92 kibbutz-born and Middle Eastern Israeli adolescents, are able to address these objec- tives for three reasons. First, it is a longitudinal study of moral reasoning, in contrast to most previous cross-cultural studies of moral de- velopment, which have been cross-sectional. These cross-cultural studies were reviewed by Edwards (1981, 1982) and Snarey (1982, 1984). Second, the sample includes males and females in contrast to Kohlberg's original longitudinal study in the United States (Colby, Kohlbcrg, Gibbs, & Lieberman, 1983; Kohl- berg, 1958, 1981) and Nisan and Kohlberg's longitudinal study in Turkey, both of which included only males (Turiel, Edwards, & Kohlberg, 1978; Nisan & Kohlberg, 1982). Finally, the present study also attempts to replicate many of the same analyses that were

Page 3: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

4 J. SNARE~ J. REIMER, AND L. KOHLBERG

performed in the United States and in Turkey, the two previous longitudinal studies that have used the standardized scoring system (Colby, Kohlberg, Gibbs, Candee, et al., 1983). This will enable us to make specific compar- isons among the samples and to begin to clarify the cross-cultural validity of Kohlberg's model and method.

Research Questions

This study considers six specific research questions. The first three apply conventional psychological indicators of reliability and va- lidity to Kohlberg's theory. To what degree does the development of moral reasoning in a kibbutz population, as measured by Kohl- berg's instrument, follow the same patterns as shown by other populations who have been studied? The second three apply anthropolog- ical methods to Kohlberg's theory. To what degree is the moral reasoning of kibbutzniks different from that of people from other cultures?

Developmental Questions

1. Stage sequence. The invariant-secluence assumption of Kohlberg's moral development theory should be supported by the combined results of the blind-scored longitudinal data. Stage change should be upward and sequential and stage regressions should not be found beyond the level explainable by scoring error (cf. Broughton, 1978; Kohlberg, 1984; Kur- tines & Grief, 1974).

2. Structural wholeness. Structural wholeness is a critical empirical criterion of construct validity. The internal consistency of the model and measure was previously established for subjects in the United States, using the standard scoring manual (Colby, Kohlberg, Gibbs, Candee, et al., 1983; Kohl- berg, 1981). A replication of their analyses on the kibbutz longitudinal data should yield similar findings.

3. Age norms and stages. Moral judgment can be expected to be positively correlated with age, as developmental theory suggests and previous research has found. The age norms among kibbutz subjects will also be compared with the findings from previous research.

Cultural Questions

4. Culturally defined sex differences. Moral development among kibbutzniks should not yield sex differences. Structural-developmental theory's claim to universality is not consistent with inherent sex differences in the structure of moral reasoning. From an anthropological perspective, however, the general equality of sex roles in the community's socialization process also suggests that sex differences in the level of moral development should not be expected among kibbutz subjects. That is, although the issue of sex difference in moral judgment is a topic of current debate (Gilligan, 1977, 1982), we would argue that sex differ- ences will not be found unless culturally de- fined sex roles assign different educational and social rights to males and females. Because kibbutz ideology stresses equality of sex roles, at least as an ideal, we would anticipate that there would not be sex differences in the development of moral reasoning among kib- butzniks.

5. Culturally defined moral issues. There may be culturally based differences in choices that subjects make regarding what issue or content within a dilemma that they will consider the most important. Each of Kohl- berg's dilemmas requires the subject to make a content choice between two moral issues (e.g., to steal or not to steal the drug). From an ethnographic perspective, one might expect Middle Eastern Youth Aliyah students to more commonly favor particular kinds of action (such as upholding the law and au- thority) that reflect the content of moral reasoning stressed in their culture and fami- lies, whereas kibbutz-born subjects might be expected to more commonly select other choices (e.g., upholding conscience, life, con- tract). This difference between the two groups, if it exists, would also be expected to decrease as Youth Aliyah students spend more time on ,the kibbutz.

6. Culturally defined moral structures. Another aspect of the cultural uniqueness question, aside from content differences, is the possibility that the scoring manual simply misses or misunderstands particular structures of moral reasoning because of cultural differ- ences between the subject and theoretician (cf. Bloom, 1977; Simpson, 1974). We as-

Page 4: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

DEVELOPMENT OF KIBBUTZ ADOLESCENTS 5

Table 1 Sample Description

Cohort number Cohort name

Place of Place of birth residence

Background data at first interview

Age Grade Males Females. N

I A Cyclamen Youth City Aliyah

I B Federation Kibbutz Kibbutz F Sabras

I C Middle class City academic students

1 D Lower class City vocational students

II A Sparrow Kibbutz Kibbutz RY Sabras

II B Sparrow Youth City Aliyah

I1 C Gazelle Youth Aliyah City III A Crane Kibbutz Kibbutz RY

Sabras III B Crane Youth Aliyah City

IV A Turtle Dove Kibbutz Kibbutz RY Sabras

IV B Turtle Dove Youth City Aliyah

Total

Kibbutz RY 15-17 10 10 0 10

Kibbutz F 15-17 10 9 0 9

City 15-17 10 9 0 9

City 15-17 10 10 0 10

Kibbutz RY 13-14 8 4 3 7

Kibbutz RY 13-14 8 4 4 8

Kibbutz M 13-14 8 6 5 11 Kibbutz RY 12-13 7 6 2 8

Kibbutz RY i 2-13 7 8 3 11

Kibbutz RY 13-15 8 2 3 5

Kibbutz RY 13-15 8 1 3 4

69 23 92

sessed the cultural uniqueness of stage for- mulations by examining interview material that the scorers indicated was difficult to score. This material was analyzed for patterns that relate to kibbutz norms and values.

Method

Samples Kibbutzim are intentionally created collective com-

munities in Israel characterized by communal childrear- in~ collective economic production, and direct partici- patory democracy. The kibbutz under study, which we will call Ramat Yedidim, was founded in 1949 in the northern Galilean hills by a group of young Jewish men and women who had grown up in the Young Guard (Hashomer Hatzair) Youth Movement.

Kibbutz Ramat Yedidim's educational system, in gen- eral, is typical of the approach of other kibbutzim within the National Kibbutz Federation (Kibbutz Artzi), the Federation most loyal to the traditional approach to structuring a kibbutz living environment. Small age- graded peer groups (kvutzot) live together in their own houses from infancy until 18, when they enter the army. Each of these cohorts is given a name that serves some of the same identification functions as a family surname in the United States, although they also have family surnames. Each cohort of children has one or more full- time teachers who guide their formal education and also

a house-parent or caretaker (metapeiet), who is responsible for directing and socializing the children in the communal living and work activities of the children's house. Because, to a limited degree, their peers are experienced as their family and their educators as parental figures, there is an unusual unity to their educational, work, and social experience. Kibbutz Ramat Yedidim has, however, mod- ified an element of the traditional kibbutz movement's approach to educating adolescents in that they fully integrate city-born youth into the kibbutz educational system. Most of these youth are so-called Middle Eastern Jews (i.e., their parents immigrated to Israel from the Arab countries), and they also come from economically disadvantaged backgrounds. A group of these 12 to 13- ycar-olds are brought to the kibbutz, through the services of the Youth Aliyah organization, and after a year are fully integrated with a parallel cohort of kibbutz-born youth. This new cohort of kibbutz-born and city-born are educated and live together until early adulthood (cf. Kohlberg & Bar-Yam, 1971; Reimer, 1977; Snarey, 1982).

The 92 adolescents in this study are divided into four subsamples, each of which corresponds to a kibbutz cohort. (See Table 1.) Each subsample of kibbutz-educated students includes both kibbutz-born and city-born ado- lescents. The subjects in all four subsamples were residing at Kibbutz Ramat Yedidim, with the following two exceptions: Subsample 1 was supplemented with kibbutz- born subjects from Kibbutz F and with two groups of city-born and city-educated youth; Subsample 2 was supplemented with a group of city-born kibbutz-educated youth from Kibbutz M.

Page 5: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

6 J. SNAREY, J. REIMER, AND L. KOHLBERG

Research Instrument

Kohlberg's moral judgment interview, Form A, was used to collect the longitudinal data on moral development. The three dilemmas in Form A are as follows: III, the classic Heinz dilemma, involving a husband's conflict between the issues of life versus law; III', the Brown dilemma, involving a court judge's conflict between conscience versus punishment; and dilemma I, the Joe dilemma, involving a father-son conflict between contract versus authority. Each dilemma is followed by 9 to 12 standardized probe questions designed to clarify the reasons "why" a subject has made a particular moral judgment.

The dilemmas and probe questions were translated into Hebrew and were also modified slightly for use with kibbutz subjects, for example, U.S. dollars became Israeli pounds, Heinz became Moshe, and so forth.

Data-Collection Procedure

The subjects were interviewed individually in Hebrew. Each interview was conducted privately, tape-recorded,

and later transcribed. Moral judgment interviews were collected from the entire cohort shortly a l~r a group of Youth Aliyah students arrived at Kibbutz Ramat Yedidim or shortly after the Youth Aliyah group was integrated with the parallel group of kibbutz-born subjects. All subjects in a particular subsarnple were then reintcrvicwed 1 or 2 years later and again 5 years later. For some kibbutz cohorts a city-residing comparison group was also interviewed. The interview schedule, according to both sample subgroups and frequency of interview, is presented in Table 2. The frequency of the longitudinal follow-up interviews may be summarized as follows: 32 subjects were interviewed 3 times each for a total of 96 interviews, 32 subjects were interviewed 2 times for a total of 64 interviews, and 28 subjects were interviewed only once. Thus, taken together, there was a total of 188 interviews from 92 subjects.

Scoring and Analysis

Moral judgment interviews. Moral development in- terviews were scored ufing the new Standard Issue Scoring Manual (Colby, Kohlberg, Gibbs, Candee, et ai., 1983).

Table 2 Interview Schedule

Time 1 Time 2 Cohort number Cohort name N Ages N Ages

Time 3 Time 4 Time 5 Total interviews

N Ages N Ages N Ages per cohort

I A Cyclamen 10 Youth Aliyah

I B Federation 9 Kibbutz Sabras

I C Middle-class 9 academic students

I D Working-class 10 vocational students

II A Sparrow Kibbutz Sabras

I IB Sparrow Youth Aliyah

lI C Gazelle Youth Aliyah

IIIA Crane Kibbutz Sabras

lII B Crane Youth Aliyah

IV A Turtle Dove Kibbutz Sabras

IV B Turtle Dove Youth Aliyah

Total interviews per 38 year

(15-17)

(15-17)

(15-17)

(15-17)

6 (17-19)

7 (13-14)

8 (13-14)

II .(13-14)

8 (12=13)

11 (12-13)

7 (15-17)

8 (15-17)

11 (15-17)

11

5

(13-14)

(13-15)

6 (24-26) 22

4 (24-26) 13

10

32 19 46 53 188

4 (13-15) 4 (18-20) 8

8 (20-22) 24

22

8 (18-19) 16

11 (18-19) 33

5 (18-20) 10

7 (20-22) 21

Page 6: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

DEVELOPMENT OF KIBBUTZ ADOLESCENTS 7

This standardized scoring method yielded two indexes of moral development: a global stage score and a moral maturity score. The global stage score is a categorical stage assessment that was calculated on a 9-point scale (1, 1/2, 2 . . . . 5). The moral maturity score is a continuous stage assessment that can range from 100 to 500.

To assign each of the 188 interviews in this study a global stage score and a moral maturity score, they were divided and distributed randomly to three expert scorers, one of whom was a co-author of the scoring manual. All interviews were scored blind from English transcripts, that is, without knowing the subject's name, age, sex, cohort membership, time of testing, or scores assigned to other interviews. To assess interrater reliability, 20 interviews were selected randomly and scored indepen- dently by all three scorers. Comparing the level of agreement between the global stage scores, we found that in 65% to 70% of the cases an interview received the exact same score from any two scorers. In 95% of the cases, the interview received the exact same or within one-half of a stage score from all three scorers. The mean reliability between all three scorers was estimated to be .89 for the categorical global stage scores and .91 for the continuous moral maturity scores. To assess the translation reliability, seven interviews were also randomly selected for independent scoring in Hebrew by a bilingual scorer. The interrater correlation was .84 for the global stage scores, and .93 for continuous scores. Translating the interviews, therefore, did not significantly alter the scoring reliability.

F ind ings and Analys is

Developmental Findings

The findings f rom the four samples will be c o m b i n e d in o rder to evaluate the degree to which they suppor t Hypotheses 1 to 3 re- garding the basic theoret ica l a s sumpt ions o f Kohlberg ' s deve lopmenta l m o d e l and the re- l iabi l i ty o f the new scoring method . The results will also be c o m p a r e d with findings f rom the two previous longi tudina l mora l deve lopment s tudies- - -one in the Uni ted States and one in T u r k e y - - t h a t have used the s t andard ized scor ing manua l .

Developmental sequence and regressions. Accord ing to Kohlberg , deve lopmenta l se- quence or stage change should be consecutive and upward , tha t is, wi thou t regressions or omissions. Table 3 s u m m a r i z e s the types o f stage change tha t occu r red in the in terval f rom t ime n to t ime n + 1 a m o n g the 64 longi tudinal subjects.

As an e x a m i n a t i o n o f Table 3 indicates, small a m o u n t s o f regression occu r red in 6 out o f 96 longi tudina l in terviews (6.3%) using the cus tomary 9 -po in t scale, and regressions

Table 3 Summary of Longitudinal Stage Sequence

Type of stage change Frequency %

Stage progression 2 to 2/3 1 2 to3 1 2/3 to 3 13 2/3 to 3/4 12 2/3 to 4 1 3 to 3/4 23 3 to4 8 3/4 to 4 1 3/4 to 4/5 3

No stage change 2/3 to 2/3 5 3 to 3 10 3/4 to 3/4 12

Stage regression 3 to 2/3 3 3/4 to 3 3

Total 96

65.6

28.1

6.3

100.

Note. The actual raw scores for each subject may be ex- amined in Snarey (1982).

occur red in 7 out o f 96 cases using the mos t different iated 13-point scale. To evaluate this finding, previous tes t - re tes t re l iabi l i ty da ta can be used as an es t imate o f the n u m b e r o f such devia t ions tha t can be a t t r ibu ted to m e a s u r e m e n t error. Colby and Kohlbe rg have repor ted tes t - re tes t e r ror for the F o r m A interview to be 19%. They also r epor t tha t the longi tudinal regressions in the Un i t ed States were 5% for the F o r m A in terview using the 9-poin t scale and 7% using the 13- po in t "scale and thus conc lude tha t because the tes t - re tes t reversals are well over twice as great as the longi tudinal reversals, it s eemed reasonable to a t t r ibu te the v io la t ions o f lon- gi tudinal sequence to m e a s u r e m e n t e r ror 0 9 8 3 ) . One might add, o f course, tha t some o f the nonreversals migh t also be due to m e a s u r e m e n t error, b u t it still seems reason- able to conclude tha t the a s sumpt ions regard- ing deve lopmenta l sequence and regressions are suppor t ed by the k ibbu tz f indings because the percentage of violat ions o f the longi tudinal sequence in this k ibbu tz longi tudina l s tudy was near ly ident ical to the f inding in the Un i t ed States longi tud ina l study. This conclu- sion is also suppo r t ed by the fact tha t in no case d id a longi tudina l subject comple te ly skip a stage. Each subject reached his or her

Page 7: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

8 J. SNAREY, J. REIMER, AND L. KOHLBERG

highest stage at his last interview time by going through each of the preceding inter- mediate stages between his first and last in- terview stage scores.

Structural wholeness. Kohlberg has sug- gested that, in addition to invariant sequence, the most critical empirical criterion of con- struct validity is "structural wholeness" or internal consistency. This refers to the gen- erality of stage usage across moral issues and dilemmas within the interview.

One indication that a stage forms a struc- tured whole would be the degree to which any particular individual reasoned at the same stage at any one interview time. Each subject's stage usage in percentage is presented elsewhere (Snarey, 1982). The 188 interview profiles indicate that in 156 (83%) of the cases all reasoning was at one major stage or in transition between two adjacent stages, and 32 (17%) of the cases included reasoning at three adjacent stages. In no case was a subject reasoning at two nonadjacent stages. To analyze such interviews with three stages represented, Colby and Kohlberg have pre- viously established a conservative error boundary of 10% with entries of .10 and below treated as error and entries above .10 treated as real (Colby, Kohlberg, Gibbs, Can- dee, et al., 1983). Applying this formula, 16 or 8.5% of the 188 interviews exhibit reason- ing at three stages. This is comparable to the U.S. longitudinal study in which 9% of the interviews exhibited reasoning at three adja- cent stages, though the stage range is narrower in the current study.

Another procedure for evaluating the structural-wholeness claim is to examine the correlations among the stage scores for each of the six issues that make up the global or overall interview score. A correlation matrix for the entire sample is presented in Table 4. Consistent with Kohlberg's assumptions, the correlations were all positive, significant, and moderately high (.745 to .422). If the struc- tural-wholeness hypothesis is correct, one would also expect to find one major factor, not several factors, accounting for the major portion of the variability. The factor loadings and principal components analysis for three age groupings (12 to 15, 16 to !9, 20 to 26) on all six moral issues were thus examined. The first factor accounts for 79% to 100% of

the variance, with eigenvalues of 3.49, 2.88, and 2.74. The second factor's eigenvalue is always less than 1 for each age group and for the sample as a whole. Nevertheless, to con- sider the possibility ofa multifactoral solution, the factors were rotated. Again, using a var- imax (orthagonal) rotated factor matrix, no underlying consistent pattern is interpretable across the factors. Thus, in summary, the eigenvalue and proportion of variance were predominately accounted for by only one general factor, and rotation of multiple factors did not yield any consistent pattern across the factors. The structural-wholeness hypoth- esis, therefore, cannot be rejected; the findings suggest that there is a general dimension of moral reasoning that is not issue-specific. These findings are also dearer than those from the U.S. study, in which the second eigenvalue was greater than 1 0.05 to 2.05) for similar age groups.

Age norms and moral stage. Moral devel- opment would be expected to show a clear relationship with age. The actual relationship between age and moral stage can be sum- marized by observing the mean and range of stages for each age. Stage 2/3 was the lowest commonly assigned stage, 61% of the subjects at age 12 were assigned this stage. No kibbutz subject aged 18 or over scored at Stage 2/3. Stage 3 was the model stage for ages 13 to 14 and 15 to 17. No one in the 13 to 17 group scored higher than Stage 3/4. Stage 3/ 4 was assigned to 62 (33%) of the interviews and was the model stage for the 18 to 26 age groups. Stage 4 was assigned to l0 (5%) of the interviews; it did not appear until the 18 to 19 age group, at which time 14.3% of this cohort was assigned Stage 4. Stage 4/5 also appears for the first time in the 18 to 19 age group, but it does not become common until the 24 to 26 age group. Stages 4/5 and 5 thus appear to be confirmed on the kibbutz as stages of adulthood and not of adolescence.

In sum, the mean moral maturity scores gradually and consistently increased from 278 at age 12 to 377 at ages 24 to 26. A regression analysis indicated that age accounts for 40% of the variance in moral maturity scores.

As Table 5 indicates, the Israeli sample's age norms compare favorably with the find- ings from the two previous longitudinal stud-

Page 8: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

DEVELOPMENT OF KIBBUTZ ADOLESCENTS

Table 4 Correlations Between Six Moral Issues in Form A Interview for Total Sample

Issue Law Conscience Punishment Contract Authority

Life 0.719 0.650 0.475 0.497 0.422 Law 0.745 0.702 0.515 0.457 Conscience 0.631 0.468 0.525 Punishment 0.468 0.480 Contract 0.494

Note. All ,o's < .001.

ies that have used the standardized scoring system. Colby and Kohlberg found that the scores on the Form A interview in their American sample ranged from Stage 1 to Stage 4/5 between ages 10 to 26, and Nisan and Kohlberg also found that in their Turkish sample the scores ranged from Stage 1 to 4/ 5 between ages 10 to 28. The Israeli scores are somewhat similar in that they ranged from Stage 2 to Stage 4/5 between ages 12 to 26, but there were no scores at Stages 1 or 1/2. The kibbutz sample's mean stage scores at all ages were consistently higher than the mean stage scores in samples from the United States and Turkey. Stage 2/3, for instance, disappeared on the kibbutz by age 16 and continued to be present until age 24 in the United States and until age 26 in Turkey. On the other hand, Global Stage 4/5 was first exhibited in the kibbutz sample at age 18, but in the United States sample Stage 4/5 was first present at age 24. The one subject in the Turkish sample who scored at Stage 4/5 was age 23.

Another dimension of the relationship be- tween age and stage is the stability of individ- ual differences, that is, the relationship be- tween a subject's score at one age with the same subject's score at a later age. The cor- relations among moral maturity scores for the different ages were thus examined. The correlations between moral maturity scores at one age with scores at a later age were positive but not always high or significant. The highest correlation within our sample was between scores at ages 15 to 17 and scores at the adjoining 18 to 19 age range (.75, p < .05), but scores at ages 13 to 14 were the best general predictors of scores in later adolescence and early adulthood (.48 with scores at ages 15 to 17, .49 with scores

at ages 18 to 19, and .56 with scores at ages 20 to 23). Kohlberg and Colby's longitudinal study in the United States also found that scores at ages 13 to 14 were better predictors of later stages, presumably because this age represents a period of stabilization after en- tering adolescence. The correlations reported by Colby and Kohlberg between scores at age 13 to 14 with later ages were generally higher than was found in the present study (.70 with scores at ages 16 to 18, .46 with ages 20 to 22, .70 at ages 24 to 26, and .67 at ages 28 to 30).

Cultural Findings

The findings reported in the previous sec- tion supported the developmental assumptions underlying Kohlberg's theory and have lent support to arguments for the cross-cultural universality of Kohlberg's model as well as his method of studying moral judgment. They have not spoken, however, to the cross-cultural particularity of moral reasoning among kib- butzniks. This section will focus on the cul- tural content of moral reasoning and the ways in which the cultural characteristics of kibbutz moral reasoning may be different.

Culturally defined sex differences. One assumption of structural developmental the- ory is that there will not be sex differences in the structure of moral reasoning. Yet some researchers have argued that there are sex differences in moral judgment (Gilligan, 1977, 1982).

The cross-tabulation of sex by global stage for all cohorts that included both male and female subjects (i.e., samples 2, 3, and 4) indicated that the association between sex and moral stage scores is weak and not significant [X 2 (3, N = 134) = 4.65, p = ns].

Page 9: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

10 J. SNAREY, J. REIMER, A N D L. KOHLBERG

Table 5 Percentage of Subjects in Each Age Group at Each Stage by Cultural Setting

Global moral stages

Ages Country N 1 1/2 2 2/3 3 3/4 4 4/5 M

12 Israel 18 6 I. I 33.3 5.6 278 10 U.S. 21 5.3 26.3 42.1 15.8 10.5 204 10-12 Turkey 28 3.5 57.1 17.9 17.9 3.5 183

13-14 Israel 40 2.5 32.5 55.0 10.0 288 13-14 U.S. 37 11.1 8.3 58.3 16.7 2.8 249 13-15 Turkey 23 26.1 13.0 60.2 219

15-17 Israel 70 1.4 17.1 57.1 24.3 303 16-18 U.S. 46 2.3 4.5 22.7 31.8 36.4 299 16-18 Turkey 22 9.1 9.1 72.7 4.5 4.5 241

! 8-23 Israel 50 14.0 68.0 16.0 2.0 348 20-22 U.S. 33 6.5 25.8 54.8 9.7 3.2 335 19-28 Turkey 36 25.0 44.4 27.8 0.0 2.8 298

24-26 Israel 10- 50.0 30.0 20.0 377 24-26 U.S. 25 4.3 17.4 43.5 21.7 13.0 365

28-30 U.S. 38 21.6 48.6 16.2 13.5 362

32-33 U.S. 23 4.5 68.2 18.2 9.1 366

36 U.S. 9 77.8 11.I 11.1 374

Note. The three samples are equivalent in that each includes an approximately equal number of lower and middle- class subjects.

(See Table 6.) The relationship between sex and specific moral stages can be further ex- amined by considering the percentage of rea- soning at each stage for males and females. This information is presented in Table 7. Sex and stage usage are cross-tabulated for each stage. The strength of the association between sex and percentage using a particular stage is always weak (V ranged from .16 to .25), and the sexes do not differ significantly in the percentages to which they use any of the stages.

Possible sex differences in stage of moral judgment were also considered separately for kibbutz-born males and females and for Mid- dle Eastern aliyah youth males and females because of the variation in their cultural

backgrounds in terms of the distribution of sex roles. The relationship between stage and sex for the kibbutz-born youth at their first and last interview times was examined as was the relationship between stage and sex for the aliyah youth at their first and last interview times. The analyses for the kibbutz-born

youth did not reveal any significant sex dif- ference at their first interview, X 2 (2, N = 20) = 4.45, p = ns, or at their last interview [Fisher's Exact Test, p (N = 20) = ns]. The aliyah youth also showed no significant dif- ferences at either their first interview time, x 2 (2, N = 23) = 3.62, p = ns, or at their last interview time, X 2 (2, N --- 23) = 3.05, p = ns. (See Table 8.)

Table 6 Cross-Tabulation of Sex by Global Stage Scores

Global stage scores

Sex 2,2/3 3 3/4 4,4/5 N

Males n 21 25 25 7 78 % 26.9 32.0 32.0 8.9

Females n 9 25 20 2 56 % 16.0 44.6 35.7 3.5

Note. x 2 (3, N = 4,65), p = ns, V = .19.

Page 10: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

DEVELOPMENT OF KIBBUTZ ADOLESCENTS 1 1

In sum, there were no signif icant sex dif- ferences in mora l j u d g m e n t for the age groups under study, cont ro l l ing for cu l tu ra l back- ground, stage usage, and interview t ime.

Culturally defined moral issues. For each o f the mora l d i l e m m a s inc luded in each interview, the subject is first asked to make a choice between two al ternat ive mora l ac t ions tha t should be taken in the d i l e m m a . These al ternat ive ac t ions are referred to as issue choices because each is based on one o f two different mora l issues tha t are in conflict. The classic He inz d i l emma , for instance,

requires a choice between life versus law. The Officer Brown d i l e m m a represents a confl ict be tween mora l i ty and conscience versus pun- i shment , and the Joe d i l e m m a presents a conflict be tween con t rac t versus authori ty .

The possibi l i ty tha t these issues are re la ted to the cul tura l differences was considered. For instance, one might expect k ibbu tz sub- jects to more c o m m o n l y choose the life, conscience, or con t rac t issues, whereas the more t rad i t iona l Midd le Eastern a l iyah you th might be expected to more c o m m o n l y choose the law, pun i shmen t , and au thor i ty issues.

Table 7 Cross-Tabulation of Sex with Percentage of Stage Usage at Each Stage

Stage usage in percentages Stage and

sex 0 4-24 25-49 50-74 75-100 p value

Stage 1 Males

n 76 2 % 97.4 2.5

Females n 54 2 % 96.4 3.5

Stage 2 Males

n 47 11 15 % 60.2 14.1 19.2

Females n 40 7 5 % 71.4 12.5 8.9

Stage 3 Males

n 2 6 11 % 2.5 7.6 14.1

Females n 1 2 8 % 1.7 3.5 14.2

Stage 4 Males

n 31 15 17 % 39.7 19.2 21.7

Females n 21 14 8 % 37.5 25.0 14.2

Stage 5 Males

n 74 4 % 94.8 5.1

Females n 52 3 1 % 92.8 5.3 i.7

4 5.1

4 7.1

34 43.5

19 33.9

9 11.5

13 23.2

1 1.2

0

25 32.0

26 46.4

6 7.6

r/s

r/s

/Is

~is

ms

Note. N = 134, male n = 78, female n = 56. Percentages refer to rows.

Page 11: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

12 J. SNAREY, J. REIMER, AND L. KOHLBERG

Table 8 Cross-Tabulation o f Stage and Sex, Controlling for Time and Culture

Global stage scores

Culture and interview time 2,2/3 3 3/4 4,4/5 p value

Kibbutz-born youth First interview time

Males n 3 8 I % 25.0 66.6 8.3

Females n 1 3 4 % 12.5 37.5 50.0

Last interview time Males

n 8 % 66.6

Females n 6 % 75.0

Middle Eastern Youth Aliyah First interview time

Males n 10 2 1 % 76.9 15.3 7.6

Females n 5 5 % 50% 50%

Last interview time Males

n 1 9 % 7.6 69.2

Females n 2 8 % 20.0 80.0

4 33.3

2 25.0

3 23.0

tLv

ns

nS

/'/S

This possible cultural difference in issue choice was examined for both cultural groups at different ages in order to compare their issue choices when the Middle Eastern aliyah youth first came to the kibbutz and again 5 to 7 years later after being kibbutz-educated.

With regard to the issue choices for the Heinz dilemma, the kibbutz-born and aliyah youth do not differ at ages 12 to 15, at ages 15 to 17, or at their final interview at ages 18 to 26. On average, kibbutz-born (96%) and Middle Eastern youth (94%) nearly always choose the life issue and advocate that the drug be stolen to save the woman's life. For the Officer Brown dilemma, again the groups do not differ at any of the age intervals; on average the conscience issue is the slight favorite for both groups (55% and 63%). Looking at the father-son dilemma, however,

the Middle Eastern aliyah youth are less likely to choose the contract issue than are the kibbutz-born subjects. At ages 12 to 15 the trend is not significant at the .05 level, but the trend becomes stronger and reaches significance among the 15- to 17-year-olds, X: (1, N = 35) = 3.98, p < .05, Phi = .40. The Middle Eastern youth, both on the kib- butz and in the city, often choose the authority issue (i.e., give the father the money) in spite of the unfairness of the father's request. The kibbutz-born youth always choose the con- tract issue (i.e., the father must keep his promise). In hindsight, this particular di- lemma seems the most likely to capture the different cultural origins of the groups. Kib- butz children tend to find the fact that the father has broken his promise and asked for his son's money to be amazing within the

Page 12: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

DEVELOPMENT OF KIBBUTZ ADOLESCENTS 13

context of the kibbutz childrearing and eco- nomic system. Middle Eastern youth, on the other hand, could be expected to find it more reasonable to give in to the father's request within the context of the Middle Eastern patriarchal family system, which emphasizes parental respect and authority. By the time of the final interview, after 5 to 9 years of kibbutz residence, the difference between kib- butz-born and aliyah youth has completely disappeared and both nearly always choose the contract issue (i.e., don't give the father the money).

Because all of the subjects in the age range for which differences in issue choices were found were also reasoning at the conventional stages, it appears that their choices were guided by the different conventional norms of their subcultures. This is further suggested by observing the frequency with which the Israeli subjects drew upon specific culturally defined values and norms from the social institutions to which they belonged in order to justify or support their moral judgments. That is, ref- erence to kibbutz democratic and family norms, Israeli legal and military protocols, Jewish religious and ethnic values parallel the above issue choice patterns. The number of cultural references made by the 12- to 14- year-old subjects in both the kibbutz-born and Youth Aliyah cohorts was quite low (M = 6%), and these subjects also had the highest use of preconventional reasoning. Among the older subjects, however, the num- ber of references to social institutions rose dramatically, with an average of 50% of the 15- to 17-year-old and 40% of the 18- to 26- year-old subjects supporting their moral rea- soning by referring to specific social institu- tions.

In sum, no significant differences between kibbutz-born and non-kibbutz-born youth were found on the Heinz dilemma or on the Officer Brown dilemma, but a culturally comprehensible difference was found on the father-son dilemma among the 15- to 17- year-olds. These findings contrast somewhat with the results of Colby and Kohlberg's United States study (1983), because in their sample 67% of the Americans argued that Heinz should steal the drug, compared to 95% of the kibbutz subjects; 45% of their

American sample argued for leniency in the Officer Brown dilemma, compared to 55% of the kibbutz-born youth; and 61% of their sample argued that the father must keep his promise, compared to 93% in the kibbutz- born sample. In each case, the kibbutz sub- jects are more likely to choose what Kohlberg has termed the principled response, and the differences remain essentially the same when age and stage of development are controlled. The kibbutz findings are also in contrast with the results of Nisan and Kohlberg's Turkish study (1982), which found the opposite pat- tern: In their sample Turkish village subjects were significantly more likely than Turkish city subjects to choose the law issue in the Heinz dilemma and the punishment issue in the Officer Brown dilemma, but there were no significant differences on the father-son dilemma. Nisan and Kohlberg's discussion, however, similarly accounts for these differ- ences in terms of subcultural differences be- tween the two groups. The next section will consider cultural differences among kibbutz- niks as they relate to the higher stages of moral development.

Culturally defined postconventional moral- ity. Another aspect of the cultural-uniqueness question can be assessed by examining the interview material that the scorers considered to be difficult to evaluate in terms of stage structure. Occasionally the reason a subject gives for prescribing a particular moral action cannot be matched with a structural example in the scoring manual. Under these circum- stances, the scorer assigns a "guess" score to the judgment, and it is included, but weighted less, in the scoring algorithm. Such difficult material, especially within cross-cultural in- terviews, may indicate aspects of moral rea- soning that the stage model and scoring man- ual miss or misunderstand because they were created from interviews collected in the United States (cf. Price-Williams, 1975; Cole et al., 1971).

Systematic examination of the guess-scored material at the conventional stages reveals that the items are usually unclear because of the briefness or incompleteness of that par- ticular judgment by the subject. The higher stage judgments that are difficult to score, however, are not only usually complete judg-

Page 13: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

14 J. SNAREY, J. REIMER, AND L. KOHLBERG

ments but also appear to be culturally pat- terned around the cooperative and collective working-class values of the kibbutz (cf. Snarey, 1982; Snarey & Blasi, 1980). Young adult kibbutzniks bring much more of a collective communal emphasis to solving the dilemmas than do middle class North Americans, and they express a much greater investment in the preservation and maintenance of social solidarity. Thus, while all of Kohlberg's five stages are present among kibbutz members, not all elements of kibbutz postconventional reasoning are present in Kohlberg's scoring manual. In particular, some judgments that were guess-scored as Stage 4 or 4/5 could be understood as full postconventional Stage 5 judgments if one took a kibbutz perspective rather than a middle class American perspec- tive to the data. The following two examples illustrate this thesis.

Excerpt 1 (Kibbutz female): Q. It is against the law for Moshe to steal the drug.

Does that make it morally wrong? A. It will be illegal or against the formal law, but not

against the law which is the moral law. Again, if we were in a utopian society, my hierarchy of values, and the hierarchy of others through consensus, would be realized.

Q. What are those values? A. Socialism! But (laughter) don't ask me to explain it. Q. What is wrong with a nonsocialistic society that

makes it unjust? A. In a utopia there will be all the things I believe in.

There would not be murder, robbery, and everyone will be equal. In this society, [he greatest value, the value of life, is perfectly held. Disvaluing life is forbidden. It is like our dream, our ideal. In one way it is ridiculous since this utopia will never be achieved, of course. You can even observe children in the kindergarten; they can be very nasty and cruel to each other.

Q. Should people still do everything they can to obey the law in an imperfect world?

A. Yes, unless it will endanger or hurt another important value . . . . But generally speaking, people should obey the law. The law was created in order to p r o t e c t . . , from killing, robbery, and other unjust uses of power . . . . I believe everyone has the right to self-growth and the right to reach happiness . . . . People are not born equally genetically, and it is not fair that one who is stronger physically should reach his happiness by whatever means at the expense of one who is weaker, because the right to happiness is a basic human right of every person and equal to all. A nonkibbutz society that is based on power negates the right and possibility of those who are weaker to get their happiness.

Excerpt 2 (kibbutz male): Q. Should Moshe steal the drug? Why or why not? A. Yes . . . . I think that the community should be

responsible for controlling this kind of situation. The medicine should be made available to all in need; the druggist should not have the right to decide on his o w n . . , the whole community or society should have the control of the drug.

Q. ls it important for people to do everything they can to save another's life? Why or why not?

A. If I want to create a better community, a nice and beautiful one, an ideal world, the only way we can do it is by cooperation between people . . . . We need this cooperation among ourselves in order to achieve this better world . . . . The h a p p i n e s s . . . principle underlies this cooperation--the greatest happiness for the greatest number of people in the society.

Q. Should people try to do everything they can to obey the law?

A. In principle, yes. It is impossible to have any kind of state, country, society without laws. [Otherwise], it will be complete anarchy and those who have the power will dominate the weaker.

Q. Why is that wrong? A. I am [not] strong, [Laughter]. But really, you can

see in the totalitarian countries today in contrast to, for example, the kibbutz. You damage the principle of democracy and, most importantly, you destroy the principle of equality. Which is why I [have chosen to] live on a kibbutz.

In scoring these excerpts as guess Stage 4 or 4/5, the scorer made two judgments: (a) there was not a clear match example in the scoring manual by which to evaluate these judgments, and (b) clinically, these statements seem to be at least Stage 4, but they do not seem to be fully postconventional Stage 5 judgments. Because the subjects do appear to be arguing for the maintenance of the social system, it seems reasonable, in the absence of material in the scoring manual, that these excerpts were so scored. Yet, we would argue that there is a clear sense in which the collective equality and happiness perspective common to these interviews is more mature than Kohlberg's definition of Stage 4 and should be included as a type of Stage 5 reasoning. One notes that the kibbutz func- tions for the subjects as an imperfect embod- iment of a more utopian ideal. Furthermore, in commonly making the assertion that the whole community or kibbutz should control the drug, they are making a solid Stage 5 judgment to the extent that they view com- munity membership as based on a commit- ment to collective equality and the aim of

Page 14: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

DEVELOPMENT OF KIBBUTZ ADOLESCENTS 15

cooperative happiness. Allusions in some in- terviews to the social system becoming dys- functional are also not necessarily conven- tional judgments if the reason they are pro- tecting the social system is because they see it as ideally embodying universal moral prin- ciples. The clear recognition by the subjects that the kibbutz does not fully meet these ideals also supports our assumption of the autonomous use of moral principles that are prior to a social perspective. A more culturally sensitive scoring of the excerpts presented above would thus have assigned a score of Stage 5 to these interviews. Such a culturally sensitive scoring would not, however, have drastically increased the number of Stage 5 interviews in the sample. Although the current stage distribution included three young adults who scored at Stage 4/5, the above consider- ations would have increased the number to five interviews scored as fully Stage 5.

Summary Conclusion

The moral development of 92 adolescents in Israel was studied with the aim of evalu- ating the cross-cultural validity of Kohlberg's theory of the development of moral judgment. The research questions both focused on the degree to which the moral development of kibbutzniks is like that of all other people and on possible culturally defined variations in moral development between kibbutz and other populations.

Developmental Universality of Moral Judgment

The longitudinal findings indicated that stage change was consecutive, gradual, and upward. The number of stage regressions was not higher than one would expect due to scoring error, and in no case did a subject skip a stage. Colby and Kohlberg's analyses regarding structural wholeness and internal consistency were also replicated. The 188 interview profiles indicated that in 83% of the cases all reasoning was at one major stage or in transition between two adjacent stages. The correlations among the stage scores for each of the six moral issues within each interview were all positive, significant, and

moderately high. Finally, the eigenvalue and proportion of variance were predominately accounted for by only one general factor.

Age showed a clear relationship with stage and accounted for 40% of the variance in moral maturity score. The age norms com- pare favorably with the findings from the two previous longitudinal studies that have used the standardized scoring system. The range of stages is from 2 to 4/5, compared to a range of 1 to 4/5 in the United States and Turkey. Furthermore, the kibbutz mean stage scores at all ages are consistently higher than the mean stage scores in the United States and Turkey. This distribution of the kibbutz stage scores is also impressive when one considers that previous research has found that children in rural communities generally progress more slowly than city children (Ed- wards, 1975; Turiel, 1969, p. 125). Yet in all studies to date, Stages 4/5 and 5 have been relatively rare: 8 subjects in the United States (Colby, Kohlberg, Gibbs, & Lieberman, 1983), 18 kibbutz founders in Israel (Snarey, 1982), 1 subject in Turkey (Nisan & Kohlberg, 1982), and 3 kibbutz youth in the present study.

Regarding the stability of individual differ- ences, Colby and Kohlberg had found that scores at ages 13 to 14 were the best predictors of later stages. The finding from the United States was replicated in Israel, although the correlations between scores at age 13 to 14 with later scores were generally lower in this present study than had been the case in the United States.

Cultural Variation in Moral Judgment

There were no significant differences in development between males and females in stage scores. Controlling for cultural back- ground, interview time and age also did not reveal any significant sex differences.

Regarding the content of moral reasoning, there were no significant differences in moral- issue choice between kibbutz and nonkibbutz subjects in moral-issue choice on the Heinz dilemma or the Officer Brown dilemma, but there was a culturally comprehensible differ- enee on the father-son dilemma among the 15- to 17-year-olds. The kibbutz-born youth nearly always made the contract choice and

Page 15: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

16 J. SNAREY, J. REIMER, AND L. KOHLBERG

argued that the father should keep his promise while the Middle Eastern youth often chose the authority issue and argued that the son should give his father the money. The 15- to 17-year-old subjects were more likely to draw on culturally defined values and norms from the social institutions to which they belonged in order to justify their moral judgments. In essence, the youngest age cohorts, whose members were primarily at the preconven- tional stages, seldom reflected cultural con- ventions in their reasoning; the middle 15 to 17 age cohorts, whose members were primar- ily at the conventional stages, were signifi- candy more likely to make moral choices that reflected the conventions of their society; finally, for the oldest cohorts, whose members had begun to make greater use of the less convention-bound stages 4 and 4/5, there was again a drop in the use of or conformity to conventions.

Culturally defined structural variations in kibbutz postconventional reasoning were sug- gested by an analysis of interview material that the scorers had labeled as difficult to score. The kibbutzniks' communal emphasis, greater investment in the preservation of social solidarity, and greater emphasis upon collective happiness seem to have been missed or misunderstood when evaluated strictly by the standardized scoring manual. Five inter- views that were rated as Stage 4 or 4/5 under the standardized scoring procedure were eval- uated as Stage 5 under a culturally sensitive second scoring. This suggests that the scoring manual needs to be revised in two ways. First, culturally indigenous examples of rea- soning at the higher stages need to be added to avoid bias when considering the content of reasoning by subjects from a cultural background other than the one of the original subjects on which the manual was based. Since it is not possible for one scoring manual to contain the universe of cultural variation, this first point also underscores the impor- tance of the researcher being thoroughly im- mersed in the culture of the population under study. Second, and more interestingly, there appear to be postconventional moral princi- ples other than those commonly held by mature middle class Westerners. There is thus a need for a more pluralistic understand-

ing of Stage 5 if scorers are to validly estimate the development of moral reasoning in diverse cultural settings.

In conclusion, Kohlberg's model and method have fared well: the kibbutz findings are remarkably consistent with a structural understanding of the development of moral reasoning. The data, however, also revealed some degree of cultural uniqueness in the moral judgments of kibbutzniks. These find- ings should be seen, of course, as only one part of a broader investigation of the validity of Kohlberg's theory within diverse cultural settings.

References

Bloom, A. H. (1977). Two dimensions of moral reasoning,: Social principledness and social humanism in cross- cultural perspective. Journal of Social Psychology, 101, 29-44.

Broughton, J. (1978). The cognitive-developmental ap- proach to morality: A reply to Kurtines and Grief. Journal of Moral Education, 7, 81-96.

Colby, A., Kohlberg, L., Gibbs, J., Candee, D., Speicher- Dubin, B., Hewer, A., & Power, C. (1983). The mea- surement of moral development: Standard issue scoring manual. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Colby, A., Kohlberg, L., Gibbs, J., & Lieberman, M. (1983). A longitudinal study of moral judgment. Monographs of the Social for Research in Child De- velopment. 48, 1-124.

Cole, M., et at. (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic Books.

Edwards, C. (1975). Societal complexity and moral de- velopment. Ethos, 3, 505-527.

Edwards, C. (1981). The comparative study of the devel- opment of moral judgment and reasoning. In R. Munroe, R. Munroe, & B. Whiting (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural human development (pp. 501-527). New York: Garland Press.

Edwards, C. (1982). Moral development in comparative cross-cultural perspective. In D. Wagner & W. Stevenson (Eds.), Cultural perspectives on child development (pp. 248-274). San Francisco: Freeman.

Giiligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women's con- ception of the self and of morality. Harvard Educational Review, 47 481-517.

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kohlberg, L. (I 958). The development of modes of moral thinking and choices in years 10 to 16. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.

Kohlberg, L. (1981). The meaning and measurement of moral development. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.

Koblberg, L. (I 981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice. San Francisco: Harper & Row.

Page 16: Development of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz ... · PDF fileDevelopment of Social-Moral Reasoning Among Kibbutz Adolescents. A ... Development of Social-Moral Reasoning

DEVELOPMENT OF KIBBUTZ ADOLESCENTS 17

Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: Moral stages and the life cycle. San Francisco: Harper & Row.

Kohlberg, L., with Bar-Yam, M. (1971). Cognitive-devel- opmental theory and the practice of collective moral education. In M. Wolins & M. Gottesman (Eds.), Group care: An Israeli approach (pp. 342-371). New York: Gordon & Breach.

Kurtines, W., & Grief, E. C. 0974). The development of moral thought: Review and evaluation of Kohlberg's approach. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 453-470.

Nisan, M., & Kohlberg, L. (1982). Universality and cross- cultural variation in moral development: A longitudinal and cross-sectional study in Turkey. Child Development, 53, 865-876.

Price-Williams, D. R. (1975). Explorations in cross- cultural psychology. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.

Reimer, J. (1977). A study in the moral development of kibbutz adolescents. Unpublished doctoral disserlation, Harvard University.

Simpson, E. L. (1974). Moral development research: A case study of scientific cultural bias. Human Develop- ment. 17, 81-106.

Snarey, J. (1982). The social and moral development of kibbutz founders and sabras: A cross-sectional and

longitudinal cross-cultural study. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University. Dissertation Abstracts Interna- tional 1983, 43(10), 3416b. (University Microfilms No. 83--02-435).

Snarey, J. (1984). The cross-cultural universality of social- moral development: A critical review of Kohlbergian research. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association, Denver, CO.

Snarey, J., & Blasi, J. (1980). Ego development among adult kibbutzniks: A eross-cuitural application of Loe- vinger's theory. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 102, 117-157.

Turiel, E. (1969). Developmental processes in the child's moral thinking. In P. Mussen (Eds.), Issues and trends in developmental psychology. New YorK: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Turiel, E., Edwards, C., and Kohlberg, L. (1978). Moral development in Turkish children, adolescents and young adults. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 9, 75- 85.

Received March 18, 1983 Revision received October 19, 1983 •

Berscheid A p p o i n t e d Editor, 1986-1991

The Publ ica t ions and C o m m u n i c a t i o n s Board o f the A m e r i c a n Psychological Asso- c ia t ion announces the a p p o i n t m e n t o f Ellen S. Berscheid, Univers i ty o f Minneso ta , as Edi tor o f Contemporary Psychology for a 6-year t e rm beg inn ing in 1986.

Publishers should note tha t books should not be sent to Berscheid. Beginning January 1, 1985, publishers should send two copies of books to be considered for review plus notices of publication to:

Contemporary Psychology A m e r i c a n Psychological Associa t ion c /o P s y c l N F O 1400 N o r t h Uhle Street Ar l ington, Virginia 22201

Please note tha t all reviews are wri t ten by invi ta t ion.