different strokes for different folks experimental ... · different strokes for different folks:...

58
DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING Prithwiraj Choudhury Harvard Business School [email protected] Evan Starr Robert H. Smith School of Business University of Maryland [email protected] Rajshree Agarwal Robert H. Smith School of Business University of Maryland [email protected] January 2018 Abstract The advent of artificial intelligence in the form of machine learning technologies ushers new questions regarding the pace at which it may substitute both older technology vintages and human capital. Rather than assuming superior productivity of one vintage over another, we examine contingency effects on productivity by heterogeneity in prior specialized knowledge bases in complementary human capital, and provision of concurrent expert advice. Within the research context of patent examination by the US Patent and Trademark Office, which has developed both machine learning and Boolean search technologies, we hypothesize that absorptive capacity from skill-technology matches will result in higher productivity, and these effects will be stronger in the presence of expert advice constituting technology and task specific information. We test and find support for our hypotheses using experimental methods that permit causal inferences and examination of underlying mechanisms. Our study contributes to literature streams on artificial intelligence, endogenous technological change, and strategic management of the pace of technological substitution by providing insights on complementarities between technologies and horizontally differentiated human capital.

Upload: others

Post on 26-Jun-2020

11 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON

COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

Prithwiraj Choudhury Harvard Business School

[email protected]

Evan Starr Robert H. Smith School of Business

University of Maryland [email protected]

Rajshree Agarwal

Robert H. Smith School of Business University of Maryland

[email protected]

January 2018 Abstract

The advent of artificial intelligence in the form of machine learning technologies ushers new questions regarding the pace at which it may substitute both older technology vintages and human capital. Rather than assuming superior productivity of one vintage over another, we examine contingency effects on productivity by heterogeneity in prior specialized knowledge bases in complementary human capital, and provision of concurrent expert advice. Within the research context of patent examination by the US Patent and Trademark Office, which has developed both machine learning and Boolean search technologies, we hypothesize that absorptive capacity from skill-technology matches will result in higher productivity, and these effects will be stronger in the presence of expert advice constituting technology and task specific information. We test and find support for our hypotheses using experimental methods that permit causal inferences and examination of underlying mechanisms. Our study contributes to literature streams on artificial intelligence, endogenous technological change, and strategic management of the pace of technological substitution by providing insights on complementarities between technologies and horizontally differentiated human capital.

Page 2: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

1

Artificial intelligence (AI) may well transform future production and work. In its current

development, AI relates to machine learning, where programmed computers learn from existing data to

conduct mental tasks and facilitate decisions (Agrawal, Gans and Goldfarb, 2017; Brynjolfsson and McAfee

2014). Bughin et al., (2017) predict machine learning, when combined with robotics, may increase global

productivity growth by 0.8-1.4%, while automating nearly 50% of current work performed by humans. AI has

also ushered debate on the substitution of humans by intelligent machines, and on who may benefit or lose

(Autor 2015; Benzell, Kotlikoff, LaGarda, and Sachs, 2015; Bessen 2016; Brynjolffson and McAfee 2014).

Budding empirical research compares decision making and productivity of machine learning vs. humans, and

wage and employment effects on vertically differentiated (skilled vs. unskilled) labor (Acemoglu and Restrepo

2017; Aghion, Bergeaud, Blundell and Griffith, 2017; Cowgil, 2017; Hoffman, Kahn, and Li 2017). At least in

the foreseeable future though, workplaces will require humans to interface with machines, so left unexamined

are factors at play for the pace of technological substitution (Adner and Kapoor, 2016; Adner and Snow,

2010; Christensen, 1997) of machine learning to older technology vintages. In particular, we lack an

understanding of how horizontal differentiation in skilled labor (caused by specialized knowledge domains)

may be differentially productive when matched interact with different technology vintages for productivity

differentials. We seek to fill this gap by comparing the productivity of a machine learning technology being

developed by the United States Patent and Trademark Office (USPTO) to the existing technology, based on

its use by individuals with different skills and experience. Using experimental methods, we address the

following research questions: As organizations introduce machine learning in the presence of old vintages,

how does heterogeneity in prior specialized knowledge bases of complementary human capital impact

productivity? What effect does concurrent provision of expert advice constituting technology and task-specific

knowledge have on productivity?

Answers to these questions are important to strategic renewal of firms, as managers must choose

whether and when to adopt new technologies, and how to create positive rather than negative

complementarities with existing resources and capabilities (Bloom, Sadun and Van Reenan, 2012; Christensen

1997). Misalignment of potentially productive new technologies with existing firm capabilities can lead to

failure (Henderson 2006). Chief among existing capabilities is the firm’s workforce (Hatch and Dyer 2004),

Page 3: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

2

given gains from productivity depend crucially on the interface of workers and technology (Autor, Levy, and

Murnane 2003; Bapna et al., 2013; Chari and Hopenhayn 1991; Teodoridis, 2017). The answers are also

important at social and individual levels, inasmuch as economic progress and social welfare requires

concomitant attention to effects of new technologies on individuals’ skill obsolescence and refurbishment,

and resultant unemployment and inequality (Bekman, Bound and Machin, 1998; Fernandez, 2001).

Our theory builds on the vintage-specific human capital literature (Chari and Hopenhayn, 1991) to

acknowledge its importance for unlocking productivity effects of any technology. We further integrate the

insight that highly skilled workers invest in developing expertise in knowledge domains (Schultz, 1961; Chase

and Simon, 1973) leading to horizontal differentiation in their prior knowledge base due to specialized skills

and experience. Using absorptive capacity logic, we hypothesize a moderating effect of prior knowledge base

on the productivity of a particular technology vintage. Specifically, we argue machine learning has higher

complementarities with computer science and engineering (CS&E) based human capital, while the older

technology vintage has higher complementarities with non-CS&E based human capital. We denote these

combinations of skills and technologies as skill-technology match. Further, we theorize that expert advice—

the provision of technology and task-specific tacit information—will enhance rather than attenuate the skill-

technology match effects on productivity of the technology.

Our research context and experimental design leverages two technologies developed by USPTO to

expedite and improve the quality of patent application examinations—the older vintage of Boolean search,

and the newer vintage of machine learning. Patent examiners, and innovators seeking patents at large, draw

from highly specialized knowledge domains representing biomedical, chemical, computer, mechanical and

natural sciences and engineering. Moreover, the patenting process also engages other specialized knowledge

domains (e.g. law, business). Accordingly, and with the help of USPTO personnel, we test our hypotheses by

conducting an experiment with 221 MBA subjects at a top-tier business school. Participants received training

to simulate the role of a patent examiner and adjudicate a patent application based on searches for prior art

using either Boolean or machine learning technology. We examine the efficacy of the two technologies, when

used by randomly assigned participants who differ in their prior CS&E human capital. We also randomized

Page 4: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

3

the provision of expert advice to participants, in the form of communication from a USPTO patent examiner

with tips regarding the usage of the specific search technology for the task at hand.

Previewing our findings, there is strong evidence for a skill-technology match: prior CS&E

knowledge is essential for success using machine learning technology, and prior non CS&E knowledge

complements more strongly with the older Boolean logic technology vintage. Moreover, these effects are

only unlocked in the presence of expert advice, underscoring the importance of concurrent task and

technology specific information. Indeed, the only instance in which we see success is when expert advice is

provided to those whose skills are pre-aligned with the technology. Subsequent analysis reveals one

mechanism underlying these results is the inability of non CS&E examiners to integrate expert advice within

the user interface underlying the machine learning technology.

The paper contributes to a number of related literature streams at the intersection of innovation and

human capital. First, while debate abounds for the future replacement of humans at work in the era ushered

by artificial intelligence and machine learning (Autor 2015; Benzell et al., 2015; Bessen 2016; Brynjolffson and

McAfee 2014), there is scant attention to the immediate implications for productivity of machine learning

substitutions for older technology vintages, particularly as this pace of technology substitution may be

conditioned by complementary human capital knowledge bases. Our study highlights that even within the

high-skilled labor force, horizontal differentiation due to specialization of human capital accounts for

significant productivity differentials when interfacing with different technology vintages. These differentials

have not been incorporated in theories for either artificial intelligence/machine learning’s effect on

productivity and workplace practices (Aghion et al., 2017; Agrawal et al. 2016, Autor 2015; Benzell et al.

2015), or in theories of technology diffusion (Chari and Hopenhayn, 1991; Jovanovic, 2009). Both theoretical

streams assume away horizontal differentiation in prior knowledge bases, even as they distinguish between

either vertical human capital differences (i.e. lousy or lovely jobs), or vintage specific human capital (i.e.

technology specific learning by doing). In contrast, our study theorizes and shows within the high-skilled

labor force, and within same technology vintages, individuals exhibit heterogeneous productivity based on

whether their prior knowledge base are matched with the technology with which they interface. Given

specialized knowledge bases distant from CS&E will be critical to productivity because of judgment, even if

Page 5: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

4

not prediction (Agrawal et al., 2017), these horizontal differences in specialized knowledge cannot be assumed

away as being rendered obsolete.

To the strategic management of innovation literature stream (Adner and Snow, 2010; Adner and

Kapoor, 2016; Agarwal and Prasad, 1999; Christensen, 1997), we contribute by noting new technologies may

fail to diffuse rapidly in spite of potential relative superiority because of their interactions with complementary

human capital. Finally, our study also contributes to the human capital and career specialization literature

(Becker and Murphy, 1992; Rosen, 1983; Zuckerman et al., 2003)—while scholars have noted the beneficial

effects of human capital investments in developing expertise (e.g. Castanias and Helfat, 1991), our study

highlights the need to develop a basic level of generalized skills for interfacing with a focal technology, to the

extent newer technology vintages shift the knowledge base requirements for productivity gains.

These finding are replete with important managerial implications, given the firm’s pre-existing

workforce is its most important capability, and also the most difficult to change. We suggest one reason firms

may fail following adoption of a seemingly profitable technology, despite training to update skill sets of their

workers, is that the new technology places differential emphasis on some specialized skill sets over others.

While a firm may invest in significant technology-specific training to help workers adapt to a new technology,

our results suggest such training will not help all firms, or all workers, equally: the prior knowledge bases of

the workforce will interact differently with new technologies, requiring firms and workers to re-think existing

complementarities between specialized knowledge domains, even as they consider the complementarities

between workers and technologies. Our study offers a cautionary note against a “one size fits all” training

regimen that is technology specific, and calls for more nuanced programs where the training is specific to the

technology-skill match. Similarly, to the extent that hiring, retention, innovation management, and

compensation are ultimately tied to productivity differentials based on available technologies, the implications

of our study extend to a careful re-thinking of all these practices.

BRIEF LITERATURE REVIEW

Artificial Intelligence and Implications for Productivity and Workplace Practices

Artificial intelligence (AI) is defined as “the capability of a machine to imitate intelligent behavior”

(Merriam-Webster, 2017), and in its current technological state takes the form of machine learning, where

Page 6: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

5

computers improve their learning over time autonomously, through programs which utilize additional

observational data and information from real-world interactions (Faggella, 2017). AI represents the latest

technological discontinuity within the larger context of automation dating back into time, for sure to the

industrial revolution (Autor, Levy and Murnane, 2003; Hounshell, 1985; Mokyr, 1990). From a socio-

economic perspective, AI is bringing back to fore age-old issues regarding implications of technological

breakthroughs for productivity and management practices, as well as changing human capital and skills

composition for individuals at work (Brynjolffson and McAfee 2014, Autor 2015, Bessen 2016). While similar

to earlier technologies in resurrecting the debate on whether AI substitutes or complements labor, the

difference from automation which mainly affected labor employed in physical, routine or low-skilled tasks is

in AI’s potential to substitute humans in non-routine, cognitive tasks performed by high skilled workers

(Jones, Aghion, and Jones 2017; Zeira, 1998). Depending on whether scholars take the perspective where

humans share significant complementarities with machines (Agrawal et al. 2016, Autor 2015), or one where

machines are superior substitutes to humans (Benzell et al. 2015), the debate is not just about “lousy and

lovely jobs” (Autor et al., 2003; Goos and Manning, 2007), but whether AI represents a technological

singularity where people are completely replaced by machines (Jones et al., 2017), to the point where they

“immiserate humanity” (Benzell et al. 2015).

Within this debate, some scholars have provided reasons why, at least in the foreseeable future, AI’s

substitution for humans at work is overstated (Agrawal, et al. 2017; Autor, 2014; Jones et al., 2017). These

reasons rely on both theories regarding whether AI is capital or labor augmenting, and on the comparative

advantage of humans vs. machines in different types of tasks. Agrawal et al. (2017) break down the anatomy

of a task into data, prediction, judgement and action. Noting that machine learning, the current state of AI

technology, is applicable largely to prediction, they highlight the importance of humans for judgment—the

ability to make considered decisions by accounting for the impact actions have on outcomes in light of

prediction (Agarwal et al., 2017). In a similar vein, Autor (2014) builds on codified vs. tacit knowledge to

highlight complementarities between machines’ comparative advantage in routine, codifiable tasks, and

humans’ comparative advantage on tasks requiring tacit knowledge, flexibility, judgment and creativity.

Page 7: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

6

Two limitations arise in the current discourse on AI, where theoretical models make different

assumptions about machines and humans in the underlying production function, task structure, and even

rates at which past investments by “dead high-tech workers” can replace effort by current period high-tech

workers (Aghion et al., 2017; Autor, 2015; Benzell et al., 2015). One, notwithstanding a budding set of

empirical papers (Acemoglu and Restrepo, 2017; Cowgil, 2017; Graetz & Michaels, 2015), there is a dearth of

empirical evidence on the effects of machine learning (the current state of artificial intelligence) on

productivity, relative to current alternative technologies. Two, the models ignore the literature on technology

diffusion, which notes new technologies may take prolonged periods of time for large scale adoption, and

firms/individuals continue to invest in old technologies even in the presence of newer regimes (Adner and

Kapoor, 2016; Chari and Hopenhayn, 1991; Mansfield, 1968), which we turn to next.

Vintages of Technology and Human Capital

Endogenous models of technological change highlight a new technology’s diffusion relative to

existing vintages depends not only to its “objective” superiority, but also on the technology’s interaction with

complementary resources and capabilities. Strategy scholars note both emergence challenges faced by new

technologies, and extension opportunities available for old technologies (Adner and Kapoor, 2016; Adner and

Snow, 2010). Emergence challenges to new technologies relate to bottlenecks posed in existing ecosystems in

the form of complementary products, resources and capabilities (Adner and Kapoor, 2016). Also, incumbents

may accelerate investments in old technologies to stave off competition from new technology entrants, or

exploit demand heterogeneity and consumer preferences for the old technology (Adner and Snow, 2010).

Similarly, models of endogenous technological change provide mechanisms underpinning prolonged periods

of technology diffusion, relating the time lag to its relative superiority to existing technology vintages and the

distribution of human capital familiar with each technology vintage (Chari and Hopenhayn, 1991; Jovanovic,

2009). Empirically, scholars examining technology diffusion have implicated differences in individuals and

workplace practices as critical to the technology’s acceptance and productivity (Agarwal and Prasad, 1999;

Black and Lynch, 2001; Greenwood, Agarwal, Agarwal and Gopal, 2017).

A salient difference of diffusion models, then, from models in the preceding section where AI and

machine learning may (exogenously) complement or substitute for human capital, is the focus not only the

Page 8: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

7

vintage of the technology (new vs. old), but also the vintage of the human capital interfacing with the

technology. Given each technology ushers with it different tasks, and also changes the composition of tasks

performed by machines or humans (Autor, et al., 2003), Chari and Hopenhayn (1991) define vintage human

capital as technology-specific skills which relate to both task-specific human capital (Gibbons and Waldman,

2004) accumulated through learning by doing, and the evolution of these tasks across technology vintages.

Thus, the literature on technology diffusion acknowledges both how strategies impact the pace of

technology substitution, and the importance of developing vintage specific human capital. However, it

abstracts away from within technology vintage heterogeneity in human capital: productivity of different vintages of

technology may not only entail complementarities between higher vs. lower levels of human capital and skills

(Jovanovic, 2009), but also complementarities with heterogeneous human capital that is qualitatively distinct

(rather than vertically differentiated), given specialization and division of labor (Becker and Murphy, 1992;

Schultz, 1961). Thus, even within a high skilled labor force, differences in prior knowledge bases may create

differences in productivity as they interact with successive technology vintages.

Taken together, the above literature streams along with their limitations sets the stage for our study’s

research context and design, where we examine productivity differentials between two technology vintages, as

contingent on the heterogeneity of prior knowledge embodied in the individuals who are interacting with

both technology vintages, and the provision of expert advice which facilitates the development of vintage

specific human capital through transmission of technology and task specific information.

RESEARCH CONTEXT: USPTO PATENT EXAMINATION PROCESS

Our research design exploits the use of two technology vintages for use by the USPTO examiners

when reviewing patent applications.1 The older vintage relies on Boolean keyword searches, while the newer

vintage utilizes machine learning. We provide brief descriptions of the patent examination process and each

of these techniques below, with more detailed information in Appendix A and B.

1 Patents protect inventors’ intellectual property rights by “exclud[ing] others from making, using, offering for sale, or selling the invention throughout the United States or importing the invention into the United States.” (https://www.uspto.gov/learning-and-resources/electronic-data-products/issued-patents-patent-grants-data-products, accessed July 2017). Anyone who wishes to secure the rights to an invention in the U.S. must apply for a patent from the USPTO. Patent applications include an abstract, which describes the patent filing, and a list of specific claims, which specify the precise components of the invention that the applicant wishes to patent.

Page 9: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

8

The Patent Examination Process

To meet the legal standards for a patent, claims must be both novel (i.e., a new, unique invention)

and non-obvious (i.e., sufficiently inventive), and the USPTO examiners review patent applications, compare

it to prior art (i.e., all existing patents, patent applications, and published materials), and determine whether an

inventor’s claims meet these two criteria. In maintaining a balance between expediency and patent

examination quality, the USPTO aims to provide at least a first office action within 10 months of receipt

(Brinkerhoff, 2014). However, increases in the number of patent applications received by the USPTO (e.g.

an 18 % increase in the five year period between 2010 and 2014)2 have created significant delays—over half a

million backlogged applications, and current processing of first office action at more than sixteen months3—

in spite of USPTO investing significant resources to increase the number of examiners by 28% from 2010 to

2014 (Crouch, 2014; Choudhury, Khanna and Mehta, 2017). The Government Accountability Office report

on intellectual property (GAO, 2016) notes these challenges are further exacerbated by recent increases in

disputes over patent validity, raising concerns “the USPTO examiners do not always identify the most

relevant prior art, which has resulted in granting some patents that may not meet the statutory requirements

for patentable inventions” (p. 2) To understand the reasons behind the above challenges and the potential

machine learning technology solution, we provide a brief description of the patent examination process (See

Appendix B, Exhibit 1, and Figure C1 for more details), and human capital and technologies utilized in the

examination process.

Upon application receipt, the USPTO classifies it based on technical subject matter to one or more

of classifications among thousands of different technologies. This classification routes the application to one

of eleven technology centers which collectively cover electrical, chemical and mechanical technology fields.

The eleven technology centers are further subdivided into smaller “art units” consisting of patent examiners

who are clustered based on expertise on related technologies within the unit. The technical competence of

2 U.S. Patent and Trademark Office, “U.S. Patent Statistics Chart Calendar Years 1963-2015,” June 15, 2016, https://www.uspto.gov/web/offices/ac/ido/oeip/taf/us_stat.htm, accessed July 2017. 3 U.S. Patent and Trademark Office, “First Office Action Pendency,” June 2017, https://www.uspto.gov/corda/dashboards/patents/main.dashxml?CTNAVID=1004, accessed July 2017 and U.S. Patent and Trademark Office, “June 2017 Patents Data, At a Glance,” https://www.uspto.gov/dashboards/patents/main.dashxml, accessed July 2017.

Page 10: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

9

patent examiners is key for identifying potential prior art and to make judgments regarding claims. All patent

examiners represent highly skilled workers, but have great diversity in their knowledge specialties. Patent

examiners specialties include biomedical, chemical, computer, electrical, industrial and mechanical science and

engineering. Additionally, the General Schedule (GS) levels of patent examiners represent different levels of

seniority/experience, productivity, and management responsibilities. Extensive training programs provided to

the patent examiners, both as part of their onboarding experience and for continuous learning, include

Quality Enhancement Meetings (QEM), Patent Examiner Technical Training Program (PETTP), Scientific

and Technical Information Center (STIC) lunch and learn/individual sessions, technology fairs, as well as

knowledge management and collaboration systems and internal shared databases for search techniques

developed by other examiners.4

The assignment to patent examiner within an art unit is based on examiner workload rather than

complexity of patent. The patent examiner adjudicates on novelty and non-obviousness of the application by

comparing it to prior art, and also determines the appropriate scope (intellectual property boundaries) of the

patent based on novelty of claims. To review prior art, examiners search within various databases, including

the Patent Full-Text and Image Database (PatFT) and Patent Applications Full Text (AppFT) owned by the

USPTO, but open to the public (see Appendix B, Exhibit 2 for the PatFT interface). Examiners also rely on

searches in other databases to locate prior art stored elsewhere (e.g., published articles, trade journals). This

step in the process is the most important and also the most laborious (GAO, 2016). In part, this is because

of the characteristics of prior art and patent applications: patent examiners often deal with non-uniform

language, intentionally vague or broad patent claims, and missing relevant references, which increase prior art

search costs. However, some of the challenges relate to the processes and tools utilized when conducting

prior-art search, which we turn to next.

Technology Vintages Currently Used in Prior Art Search

The current mainstream technology for prior art search tools available to patent examiners (and

accessible to others in public versions) are named EAST (Examiners’ Automated Search Tool) and WEST

4 U.S. Patent and Trademark Office, “Prior Art Frequently Asked Questions,” July 28 2014, https://www.uspto.gov/sites/default/files/patents/init_events/prior_art_faq20140728.pdf accessed November 2017

Page 11: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

10

(Web-based Examiners Search Tool), and rely on Boolean logic.5 Patent examiners use Boolean operators to

conduct structured searches using keywords and strings with the available databases to yield potential prior art

citations. As Alcacer and Gittelman (2006) document, two-thirds of prior art citations on the average patent

are inserted by examiners. When evaluating the current search tools and capabilities, GAO (2016) noted 48%

of the patent examiners interviewed/surveyed found these search tools made identification of prior art easier,

but 17% found them to make it more difficult. More than half of the patent examiners thought the tools

available to them in EAST/WEST as “less advanced and more reliant on keyword searching than other

available tools” (p 24). The report stated the desire expressed by the majority for search engines for increased

automation: both in search for concepts and synonyms related to keywords entered by the examiners, and in

identification of prior art based on information already included in the patent application (claims and

specification) without any reliance on patent examiner entered keywords.

In 2016, the USPTO invested resources in developing a machine learning tool called Sigma (Krishna

et al., 2016). Sigma increases automation in the search for existing patents (see Appendix B Exhibit 3).

Examiners manipulate the search algorithm by selecting patent application components (e.g., the abstract,

title, or description) they believe are most relevant, and also enter the most important search terms into a

“word cloud”.6 Sigma, in turn, adjusts the algorithm accordingly and retrieves a more refined set of search

results. Over time, Sigma learns the examiner’s habits to suggest searches based on the examiner’s prior

behavior. Sigma is currently being tested and refined for adoption. Importantly, while the first and critical

stakeholder group are the patent examiners whose productivity depends on the search technologies, as with

the earlier technology vintages, the USPTO intends to make these tools available to the general public, to

benefit the population of potential inventors and the personnel they employ and facilitate their patent

application process. Accordingly, the larger goal when investing in machine learning technologies relates to

their ease of use for all relevant stakeholders.

THEORETICAL PREDICTIONS

5 U.S. Patent and Trademark Office, “Prior Art Frequently Asked Questions,” July 28 2014, https://www.uspto.gov/sites/default/files/patents/init_events/prior_art_faq20140728.pdf accessed November 2017 6 Word clouds visualize text data by increasing the size of more important search terms and reducing the size of those deemed less important.

Page 12: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

11

To begin with, we note our focus is on workers not as developers of machine learning technologies, but

as users. As would be expected, the development and integration of machine learning and artificial intelligence

into commercially viable technologies and products is performed by those who possess computer science and

engineering (CS&E) knowledge. However, once developed, the technologies are used to increase predictive

power by a heterogeneous workforce specializing in different knowledge contexts, and these other knowledge

bases retain their importance for other task components such as decision making and judgment (Agrawal et

al., 2017). For example, recent evidence in human resource management suggest managers can improve

recruitment and retention by incorporating machine learning methods into their decision making (Cowgil

2017; Hoffman, Kahn, and Li 2017). Thus the question of how the adoption of machine learning

technologies affects productivity for both CS&E and non-CS&E workers, relative to existing vintages, is of

great relevance. In our research context, the machine learning tool Sigma is intended for use not only by

patent examiners who vary in their specialized knowledge domains within STEM (Science, Technology,

Engineering and Math) fields, but ultimately by all interested stakeholders. Within the more general

population, individuals interested in intellectual property largely comprise of a high skilled workforce with

both STEM and non-STEM (e.g. patent attorneys, management personnel) backgrounds. Accordingly, our

theoretical predictions below relate to the moderating effects of heterogeneity of knowledge bases in the

users of a technology vintage on the technology’s productivity. We examine in particular the role of prior

specialized knowledge bases embodied in the user’s human capital, and the concurrent provision of expert

advice constituting technology and task-specific information.

Production Complementarities between Technology and Prior Specialized Knowledge

In Figure 1, we define the four cells of the matrix mapping prior knowledge base of individuals (non

CS&E and CS&E) against the two technology vintages (Boolean and Machine Learning). We start by

recognizing that each technology requires specific skills with complementary human capital (Chari and

Hopenhayn, 1991). Within our research context, and more generally, the skillsets required for interacting with

machine learning and Boolean logic technology are quite different. The use of the machine learning tools

such as Sigma, requires interfacing with the technology for choice of inputs into the word cloud, assignment

of weights for the different patent application components, followed by reading/interpreting/modifying the

Page 13: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

12

visual cues to enhance prediction and facilitate judgment (Krishna et al. 2016). On the other hand, the use of

Boolean technology requires composing search strings, followed by reading/interpreting the resulting

documents and subsequently modifying search strings.

[Insert Figure 1 here]

When exposed to identical technologies, differentiation in prior knowledge bases will create differences

in absorptive capacity (Cohen and Levinthal, 1990), thus affecting the rate at which individuals learn and

develop technology-specific skills. Accordingly, productivity differentials will arise due to higher investment

costs incurred by workers whose prior knowledge base is more distant to the new technology than for those

whose prior knowledge base is more similar. As a result, the match of skills based on prior specialized

knowledge to the technology is an important conditioning factor for a technology vintage’s relative productivity.

We posit workers with CS&E knowledge bases (Cell 1) will be more productive with machine

learning technologies relative to those with non-CS&E knowledge bases (Cell 3). This is because the user-

interface of machine learning technologies will be more familiar to workers with prior CS&E knowledge

bases, than for those with non CS&E knowledge. In their seminal article on absorptive capacity, Cohen and

Levinthal (1990) note an individual’s prior related knowledge is key to their ability to absorb new knowledge.

They remark, “students who … mastered the principles of algebra find it easier to grasp advanced work in mathematics such as

calculus; …Pascal (computer programming language) students learned LISP (another programming language) much better

because they better appreciated the semantics of various programming concepts” (p. 130). Similarly, prior computer

programming knowledge will provide CS&E workers a better skill-set needed to interface with machine

learning tools. Prior knowledge gained from working on computer software tools that involve manipulating

user interfaces and interpreting visual cues should enhance their productivity when interacting with the tool,

relative to non-CS&E workers who may lack these skills and expertise.

In contrast, non-CS&E workers’ skill sets will be a better match for the Boolean technology (Cell 4),

relative to machine learning technology (Cell 3) to enhance prediction and facilitate judgment. Within highly

skilled workers specializing in non-CS&E domains, the logic of the Boolean search algorithms is largely

understood, given widespread availability of Boolean search engines. So, non-CS&E workers possess these

more generalized skills necessary for conducting and modifying Boolean string searches. Within our research

Page 14: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

13

context specifically, the older technology vintage for identifying prior art references requires core skills not so

much in the technology’s user interface, but in reading documents generated through Boolean search.

Turning to productivity differentials of the diagonal terms with the other off-diagonal term, Cell 2,

representing CS&E workers on Boolean, we expect all high skilled workers to possess the generalized skills

for interfacing with the older vintage. Nonetheless, while perhaps not an absolute advantage, workers with

prior CS&E knowledge should have a comparative advantage in machine learning technology (Cell 1) relative

to Boolean logic technology (Cell 2), inasmuch as their computer skills are better matched to modification of

programs, relative to speedy reading and interpretation of resulting documents. The same comparative

advantage applies to non-CS&E workers on Boolean technology (Cell 4), relative to CS&E workers on

Boolean (Cell 2). Altogether, our logic implies that the diagonal Cells 1 and 4, which we denote as a skill-

technology match, should have higher productivity relative to the off-diagonal Cell 3, and at least as much

productivity as the off-diagonal Cell 2. Accordingly, we hypothesize:

H1: Those with a high skill-technology match will have higher productivity than those with low skill-technology match.

Provision of Concurrent Expert Advice and Productivity from Skill-Technology Match

While the above hypothesis focused on the prior knowledge base of workers interfacing with different

technology vintages, productivity will also be affected by whether workers receive concurrent knowledge as they

perform tasks using each technology. When working with a given technology vintage, individuals have to

learn-by-doing, and make costly, technology-specific investments (Chari and Hopenhayn, 1991). The

diffusion of a new technology vintage, relative to the older vintage, is then a function of how quickly workers

navigate the learning curve (Argote and Epple 1990). In addition to their own learning efforts when

navigating new technologies and workplace practices, workers also benefit through vicarious learning,

particularly from experts in the domain (Coleman, Katz and Menzel, 1957; Dasgupta and David, 1994;

Greenwood et al., 2017; Thornton and Thompson, 2001). For example, Dasgupta and David (1994) highlight

the importance of “context which makes focused perception possible, understandable and productive” (p.

493), and note the important role of expertise acquired through experience and transferred by demonstration,

personal instruction and by provision of expert services such as advice, consultation, etc. Empirical research

Page 15: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

14

substantiates the increases in productivity of a new technology when workplace practices include the

provision of technology and task specific information (Bapna et al., 2013; Bartel, 1994; Black and Lynch,

2001), because the codification and transmission of tacit know-how and know-what increases the speed at

which followers learn from pioneers’ learning by doing and trial and error processes with the new technology

(Edmondson, Winslow, Bohmer and Pisano, 2003). Consistent with this large literature, we expect provision

of expert advice—which we define as transfer of information that is both task and technology specific from

experts to workers—will increase the productivity of all workers when interfacing with either technology.

H2: Workers will be more productive with a given technology when they are provided with expert advice.

While the above is a baseline expectation, what is unclear from the received literature is whether the

provision of expert advice will attenuate or exacerbate the productivity differentials we predicted as arising

from a skill-technology match. Provision of expert advice may reduce the gap created by prior knowledge

base. This will likely occur because the expert advice helps workers with more distant knowledge bases

overcome confusion regarding interfacing with the technology. However, the skill-technology match will also

impact the rate at which workers absorb and translate expert advice into productivity. We believe the latter

effect to dominate: while expert advice will benefit all workers, we expect the differential effect to be

stronger in the presence of a skill-technology match. Here too, the fundamental logic relates to absorptive

capacity; higher skill-technology matches will facilitate speedier absorption and transformation of advice into

new learning. Harking back to the old adage, “I can teach it to you, but I cannot learn it for you,” our

reasoning builds on the notion that expert advice interacts with the knowledge base for greater improvements

in productivity, because the worker’s skill-technology match enables them to incorporate the full value of the

information provided. Summarizing these arguments yields the following hypothesis:

H3: Those with a high skill-technology match will better integrate expert advice into productivity than those with low skill-technology match.

RESEARCH DESIGN AND EMPIRICAL METHODOLOGY

Description of Experiment

The ideal design to test our hypotheses is one in which (1) subjects do not self-select into

experiment, (2) technology vintage type and expert advice is randomly assigned, (3) there are ways to assess

Page 16: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

15

potential contamination between treatment and control group and (4) it is possible to not only causally

identify the relationships of interest, but also shed light on the underlying mechanisms. We briefly describe

the experiment before describing how our research design allows us to address each of these concerns.

The participants in this experiment were 221 graduate students enrolled in three sections of a course

at a top tier business school, who were asked to “examine” a patent with five claims over a period of five

days. We chose experimental subjects, i.e. MBA students rather than patent examiners for two reasons. The

use of patent examiners would translate into an inability to control for unobservable differences across

subjects in prior task and technology specific learning by doing, which could bias results and create concerns

regarding our cross-technology comparisons. In contrast, MBA students have no prior experience in either

patent examination or the use of either technology, so they better simulate new workers (with differences in

prior knowledge bases) being assigned to different technologies. Secondly, our field interviews indicate the

USPTO intends both technologies to be available for general public use. The heterogeneity in prior

backgrounds of MBA students may be more representative of the skilled labor force at large, thus our results

would inform likely productivity differences for the larger stakeholder groups.

The research team simulated conditions encountered by patent examiners in designing the patent

examination exercise with invaluable help from USPTO officials. First, we determined the time period to be

allotted for examination of the patent claims, and also ensured task manageability given expectations of time

investments by the subjects participating in the experiment. In consultation with the USPTO officials, we

determined 0.6 hours per claim to be reasonable; the additional 33% of time allotment per claim provided a

cushion given the newness of the task to the participants.7 Second, the USPTO officials helped identify a past

patent application of average complexity, to be used for the experiment. The specific patent application had

been examined in 2010, and was rejected based on a prior art citation that invalidated its novelty claims (see

Appendix B Exhibits 4 and 5). Given the applicant had abandoned the process after the claims were

rejected, no record of the patent examination results were available to our experimental participants (e.g. as

could be identified through a Google search). To pare down the time investments to manageable levels, the

7 As Lemley and Sampat (2012) document, while the average patent prosecution duration at the USPTO is three to four years, the examiner spends on average 18 hours examining the claims. Also, Alison and Lemley (2000) document an average patent has around 14.9 claims, indicating a USPTO examiner takes around 0.4 hours to examine a single claim.

Page 17: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

16

USPTO officials identified 5 claims from the 39 claims made in the original application which were deemed

most relevant for the outcome of the examination. Third, the information technology department at the

business school worked with USPTO officials to ensure smooth functioning of both technologies—Boolean

and Machine Learning, and capture the work-logs of participants as they interfaced with the technology.

The actual experiment proceeded over two weeks, as follows. In the first week, participants were

trained on details of the patent examination task by officials from USPTO, and provided information similar

to new patent examiner hires. Here, they were introduced to both search technologies, but at this point,

participants were unaware of which technology would be assigned to them. At the end of week 1, participants

were randomly assigned to a technology—Boolean or Machine learning (see details in Appendix A) using a

random number generator. Each participant received additional online documentation and general training

materials to ensure familiarity with the technology and task and were given five days to complete the exercise.

The participants then initiated the exercise, searching for prior patent references. Half of the participants

(randomly chosen again) on each technology received expert advice in the midst of this exercise, in the form

of an email from an experienced USPTO examiner who worked in the art-unit where the patent was

originally examined in 2010. The e-mail included useful tips for conducting prior art search for the specific

patent application, using the specific technology. Thus, the expert advice was both task and technology

specific (see Appendix B Exhibits 6a and 6b), and recommended explicit search strategies on how to

manipulate the user interface (for the machine learning group) and how to compose the search string (for the

Boolean search group). Correctly inputting these strategies resulted in a narrower list of prior art to review;

however, it was not sufficient for task completion. The subject still had to employ judgement in reading and

selecting prior art references relevant to adjudicating the claims. At the end of the exercise, we assessed

performance, measured by both search accuracy (referencing the relevant prior are citation) and speed.

As it relates to the four criteria for an “ideal experiment,” first, participants did not self-select into this

experiment. All participants were students enrolled in a semester-long, second-year elective MBA course, and

they were unaware at time of enrollment of their potential participation in the experiment. While

participation in the experiment was voluntary, two of the three sections of the course had a 100%

Page 18: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

17

participation rate for students physically on campus that week, and the third had a participation rate of 81%.8

We ensure robustness of results for potential self-selection into the experiment for the sub-sample of sections

with 100% participation rate. In addition, participants signed consent forms prior to knowing which

technology they were being assigned, alleviating the concern that participation in the experiment might be

endogenous to being assigned to a technology. Second, while it was not possible to randomly assign CS&E

and non-CS&E backgrounds to participants as these related to their own prior experiences before enrolling in

the MBA program, we ensured random assignment conditional on possessing a prior skill to the technology,

ensuring the skill-technology match was randomly determined. Similarly, expert advice was also randomly

assigned among the participants within each technology. Third, to prevent across group contamination,

participants were asked to sign an academic declaration of honesty, stating they would work on the exercise

on their own and would not collaborate with anyone during the exercise. While such contamination would

only attenuate our results, this alleviates some concern that individuals in one technology group accessed the

search tool assigned to the other group. Moreover, access to the electronic activity logs enables us to test how

many of the individuals in the group that did not receive expert advice input the advice as suggested by the e-

mail: we find that this occurred in only 3 cases, and the results are robust to these cases being excluded from

the analysis. Finally, the activity logs for each participant also help shed light on the underlying mechanisms

driving the causal relations of interest, including whether or not participants integrated the expert advice into

their search activities.

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the sample. Overall, the average participant spent 22.6

minutes on the tool; the time spent ranged from a minimum of 0 minutes, to a maximum of 93 minutes.

While 23 participants spent no time working on the tool, these seem to be randomly distributed across the

groups. Further, neither technology assignment, nor CS&E background, nor expert advice are predictive of

the time spent on the time tool.

[Insert Table 1 here]

Variable Definitions

8 For these two sections, the participation rate was 97% and 94% respectively, counting all students, including students who were not physically on campus that week. The students who were not on campus were traveling out of town to attend job interviews.

Page 19: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

18

Dependent Variables: Success in the task is based on whether the patent application being reviewed was rejected

for all five claims due to participants identifying the single relevant prior art citation. To measure an examiner's

success, we count the number of times they cited the prior art citation in their evaluation of each claim. To

measure an examiner's productivity, we compute the ratio of prior art citations to time spent on the platform.

Independent Variables: We manipulated two variables in this study: First, 𝑀𝐿𝑖 is a dummy variable set to 1 if the

examiner 𝑖 was assigned to use the machine-learning technology, Sigma (and 0 otherwise). Second,

𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑡 𝐴𝑑𝑣𝑖𝑐𝑒𝑖 , is a dummy variable set to 1 if the examiner 𝑖 received the expert advice email (and 0

otherwise). The prior knowledge base of examiners is captured by the indicator variable 𝐶𝑆&𝐸𝑖 , set to 1 if

examiner 𝑖 has a degree in computer science and engineering (and 0 otherwise). Finally, we define 𝑆𝑘𝑖𝑙𝑙 −

𝑇𝑒𝑐ℎ𝑛𝑜𝑙𝑜𝑔𝑦 𝑀𝑎𝑡𝑐ℎ𝑖 , as an indicator which takes a value of 1 (and 0 otherwise) if a) an examiner with a

computer science or engineering degree is working with the machine learning technology, or b) an examiner

without a computer science or engineering degree is working with the Boolean search technology.

Controls: Although randomization ensures unbiased estimates, we include several pre-treatment control

variables in our model to reduce residual variation and increase the precision of treatment effect estimates.

Controls include indicators for section, gender, whether the individual has a partner, and is a US citizen.

Balance Tests and Empirical Approach

As noted above, each examiner was randomly assigned to one of four treatment groups (Boolean vs.

Machine Learning; Received Expert Advice or not), stratified by examiner background (whether the examiner

had a CS&E degree) and examiner section. The sampling method comprised three steps. First, we sorted

student IDs by section and CS&E background. Second, we matched subjects in the four treatment groups to

the six strata (3 sections* CS&E background dummy). This ensured each of the six strata have an

approximately equal number of students assigned to each treatment group. In the final step, within each

stratum, we shuffled the treatment assignments using a random number generator. We verified our

randomized treatment assignment generated roughly equal numbers of each treatment within each stratum.

Table 2 presents means and standard errors of various pre-treatment variables by the four treatment groups.

The stratification and matching seems to have worked as designed, because the distribution of CS&E and

Page 20: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

19

section are evenly distributed between the four treatment groups. Other pre-treatment variables are also

balanced across the groups.

[Insert Table 2 here]

To examine our hypotheses, we estimate the following set of equations using OLS:9

𝑌𝑖 = 𝛾0 + 𝛾1𝑆𝑘𝑖𝑙𝑙 − 𝑇𝑒𝑐ℎ𝑛𝑜𝑙𝑜𝑔𝑦 𝑀𝑎𝑡𝑐ℎ𝑖 + 𝐵𝑿 + 𝜀𝑖 (1)

𝑌𝑖 = 𝛼0 + 𝛼1𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑡 𝐴𝑑𝑣𝑖𝑐𝑒𝑖 + 𝐵𝑿 + 𝜀𝑖 (2)

𝑌𝑖 = 𝛽0 + 𝛽1𝑆𝑇𝑀𝑖 + 𝛽2𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑡 𝐴𝑑𝑣𝑖𝑐𝑒𝑖 + 𝛽3𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑡 𝐴𝑑𝑣𝑖𝑐𝑒𝑖 ∗ 𝑆𝑇𝑀𝑖 + 𝐵𝑿 + 𝜀𝑖 (3)

where 𝑿 includes the controls noted above and 𝑌𝑖 is either the number of times the relevant prior art citation

is referenced or the examiner’s productivity (relevant prior art citations per minute). Equation (1) estimates

the main effect of skill-technology match, while equation (2) examines the main effect of the provision of

expert advice, and equation (3) examines the interaction between expert advice and skill-technology match. In

subsequent analyses, we break out the Skill-Technology Match variable into its constituent subcomponents.

Table 3 reports the results from our tests of Hypothesis 1. Columns (1) and (2) of Table 4 indicate

that skill-technology match is associated with a 0.2 increase in the number of times the relevant prior art is

cited, a 333% increase relative to the mean of 0.06. Similarly, skill-technology match is associated with a 0.007

increase in productivity, a 200% increase. Both estimates are very precise, with p-values less than 0.01, thus

supporting Hypothesis 1. These average effects are displayed in a bar graph in Figure 2.

[Insert Table 3-5, and Figures 2-3 here]

Table 4 reports the results from the test of Hypothesis 2 regarding the effects of concurrent, expert

advice. In accordance with our expectations, we find the random provision of expert advice is associated with

a 0.2 increase in the number of prior art citations, and an increase of 0.007 prior art citations per minute, with

both estimates being highly statistically significant. Given the mean number of prior art citations cited is 0.06

and that average productivity is 0.0037 prior art citations per minute, the impact of expert advice increases the

number of prior art citations by 333%, while productivity increases by 200%.10

9 We use OLS as opposed to count models such as Poisson because many of the observations are perfectly predicted, as is readily apparent in Figures 1 through 4. We also perform robustness checks using linear probability models and find results that are quantitatively consistent, both in terms of magnitude and statistical significance. 10 Interestingly, these numbers are nearly identical to those in Table 3. This nuance is explained by the fact that the effects of expert advice are driven entirely by those with skill-technology match, as shown in Table 5.

Page 21: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

20

Hypothesis 3 is tested in Table 5 using an interaction approach. In particular, we are interested in

whether those with skill-technology match are better able to translate expert advice into productivity.

Surprisingly, the results suggest the provision of expert advice is entirely ineffective in increasing productivity

for those whose skills are not matched with their technology. In addition, those whose skills match their

technology are not more effective than those with a poor skill-technology match as long as both do not receive

expert advice. Indeed, the only instance in which we see productivity increases is when expert advice is

provided to those whose skills are aligned with their technology, as shown in Figure 3. In this case, those who

receive expert advice and have a skill-technology alignment have a 685% increase in the number of relevant

citations (relative to the mean of 0.06) and a productivity increase of 405%.

In Table 6, we disaggregate the skill-match variable into its subcomponents, to examine the

interactions between prior background, technology vintage, and expert advice. Figures 4 and 5 also report

these results for the unconditional models. Both Table 6 (the odd columns) and Figure 4 indicate that the

only combinations of skills and technologies able to identify the relevant prior citation were non-CS&E

examiners working with the Boolean technology and CS&E examiners working with machine learning

technology. Figure 5 and the even columns of Table 6 further highlight relevant prior art citations are derived

from those who received the expert advice and were either non-CS&E working Boolean technology or CS&E

working with machine learning technology. Furthermore, we find that there is no statistically significant

difference, between CS&E working with machine learning and non-CS&E working with Boolean, as is

apparent in Figures 4 and 5.

[Insert Table 6, and Figures 4-5 here]

Mechanisms and Robustness Checks

Our central argument for why we observed these patterns is based on absorptive capacity. One

indicator of this mechanism is an individual’s inability to translate expert advice into productivity. By tracking

individual queries, we are able to assess precisely how well individuals integrated the expert advice into their

queries on the user interface, which would have given them a narrower list of prior art to review. In

particular, we create a dummy variable equal to one if the individual entered the expert advice into the user-

interface correctly, or zero if they did not. Thus, among those who have received such advice, we can assess

Page 22: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

21

whether skill-technology matches are better able to integrate the information into the user interface. The

results of these analyses are shown in Table 7 in which we estimate the following equation:

𝑌𝑖 = 𝜑0 + 𝜑1𝑆𝑘𝑖𝑙𝑙 − 𝑇𝑒𝑐ℎ𝑛𝑜𝑙𝑜𝑔𝑦 𝑀𝑎𝑡𝑐ℎ𝑖 + 𝐹𝑿 + 𝜀𝑖 (4)

where 𝑌𝑖 is an indicator equal to one if the individual enters the proper query (as detailed in the expert advice

the individual received) in the user interface detailed.

Column (1) of Table 7 indicates that 27.8% of those who received the e-mail and did not have skill-

technology alignment were able to incorporate the information correctly (the constant term), while those

whose skill and technology matched were 24 percentage points more likely to properly incorporate the

information to the user interface, nearly double the baseline rate. Column (2) confirms these results hold with

controls. Columns (3) and (4) breakout the skill-technology match variable into its subcomponents, and

reveal that non-CS&E working with Machine Learning are roughly 30 percentage points less likely to

incorporate the information properly relative to if they were working on Boolean. CS&E examiners are, in

contrast, relatively better at integrating the information with Machine Learning technology, though the point

estimates are not statistically significantly different from zero.

Given the randomization of technology vintage and expert advice, there are relatively few empirical

concerns. One potential concern is with the count measure as the dependent variable. We re-ran all our

results with simply an indicator for whether an individual found the relevant prior art citation at all, and the

results (available from authors upon request) are qualitatively and quantitatively very similar. Another

potential concern is that voluntary participation in the study created a nonrandom selection into the study,

thus creating generalizability issues due to sample selection. As noted above, two of the three sections had a

100% participation rate, and Table 8 provides a replication of all our main analyses where we included

observations from these sections only. Our results are also robust to only considering the 198 observations

for individuals who had participated in the exercise. The results are quantitatively and qualitatively similar. A

third concern is that potential contamination of treatment and control are attenuating our results. As we

noted above, we found that only 3 individuals in the group that did not receive expert advice input the

relevant information, though these individuals did not go on to cite the relevant prior art. Thus, we conclude

that such contamination is minimal.

Page 23: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

22

[Insert Tables 7 and 8 here]

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION

Our study is motivated by technological advances in machine learning—the current state of

development in AI—and the quest to examine production complementarities between technology vintage,

prior knowledge domains, and the concurrent provision of expert advice. These issues are theoretically and

practically important: complementarities between existing human capital and the technologies with which

they interface affect not only the pace of technological substitution, but also workplace practices and socio-

economic outcomes for individuals at work. We combined insights from models of technology diffusion and

specialization of knowledge domains to theorize about the importance of absorptive capacity in unlocking

production complementarities: both the match of prior skills and technology, and expert advice with task and

technology specific information should enhance productivity. Our experimental design simulated the context

of patent examiners interfacing with two technology vintages to determine patent claim novelty, based on

searches among the vast prior art.

The results confirm our theoretical predictions, in some ways even more strongly than we

anticipated. Specifically, a lack of technology-skill match resulted in zero success (no productivity), even

though we had expected only lower productivity gains. Moreover, even technology-skill matches resulted in

positive productivity only in the presence of expert advice. Without such technology and task specific

information, the potential productive benefits from prior familiarity with the technology remained unrealized.

The mirroring of these results for both technology vintages within our experimental setting is also illustrative

of the fact that these effects are not chiefly driven by a specific technology itself. Rather our results are more

indicative of the larger issue that specialized knowledge domains and vintage specific knowledge (both task

and technology specific) are critical for productivity differentials. Accordingly, objective superiority of any

technology aside, the realized gains in productivity depend on the absorptive capacity of individuals

interacting with the technology.

Of course, the generalizability of our study is subject to important limitations, some of which require

additional research to address. First, our focus in this study is on the window where workers first interface

with technologies, rather than after a prolonged association. This boundary condition is both a feature and a

Page 24: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

23

limitation. Initial conditions are important to study, as they have path dependent outcomes for the future,

both in terms of adoption decisions, and the strategies which will enable better diffusion of adopted

technologies. However, it is a limitation inasmuch as we are unable to examine performance improvements

over longer periods of time. While technology-skill matches and provision of expert advice increase the initial

productivity, what is unclear is whether constant exposure and learning by doing by workers would cause the

relative differences between the groups to grow or shrink over time.

Second, our reliance on an experimental design and choice of MBA students as subjects was

motivated by the need for causal inferences in a sample of highly skilled but heterogeneously specialized labor

force. Further, as noted above, this choice of experimental subjects permitted us to control for prior

familiarity with both task and technology, so we could isolate the effects of prior knowledge domains, and the

provision of expert task and technology specific information to randomly distributed subjects. However, as is

true for all laboratory experiments, generalizability of the results are limited by the abstractions from reality,

and applications to other relevant sub-populations of the labor force. While we expect our sample

population of MBA students at a top tier business school to be similar to the target sector of highly skilled

individuals interested in intellectual property rights, the results of our experiments need to be replicated in

similar contexts for confirmation. Also, we deliberately abstracted away from vertical differentiation (high vs.

low skilled labor), but widespread use of new technologies may well require an inquiry expanding to this

dimension as well.

Finally, our research context and technology vintages are very specific—applicable to USPTO’s

development of the machine learning tool Sigma relative to Boolean search. Machine learning technologies

are themselves heterogeneous, and their development for tasks may render user interfaces more or less

complex, which itself has implications for complementarities with human capital. Accordingly, while our

results should be interpreted with caution, we urge scholars to add to the budding empirical research

examining productivity of all machine learning technologies, and their contingencies.

Limitations notwithstanding, our study contributes to several literature streams. Recent theoretical

models and empirical findings related to artificial intelligence and machine learning have stressed substitution

effects of technologies on human capital, particularly in the domain of prediction (Agrawal et al. 2016; Autor

Page 25: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

24

2015; Benzell et al. 2015; Jones et al., 2017). Human capital in this literature stream is largely incorporated

either in the form of vertical differentiation (high vs. low skilled workers), or because of comparative

advantage in complementary tasks related to judgment, new discoveries, or tacit knowledge (Aghion et al.,

2017; Agrawal et al., 2017 Autor, 2015). We extend this literature in two ways—first, we integrate insights

from the related literatures on technology diffusion (Chari and Hopenhayn, 1991; Jovanovic, 2009) and pace

of technology substitution (Adner and Kapoor, 2016; Adner and Snow, 2010; Christensen, 1997) to explicitly

compare productivity of machine learning technologies with earlier vintages. Second, and contributing to all

three literature streams above, we highlight the effects on productivity of technologies of horizontal

differentiation caused by prior specialized knowledge domains, and the provision of expert advice containing

technology and task specific knowledge.

The strategic management literature on strategic renewal within the context of disruptive

technologies acknowledges both imperatives and challenges of change (Agarwal and Helfat, 2009;

Christensen, 1997; Tushman and Anderson, 1990). Moreover, scholars have noted the rate at which new

technologies substitute old technologies relate to complementarities with existing resources and capabilities,

in addition to endogenous strategies for extending the relevance of older vintages (Adner and Snow, 2010;

Agarwal and Prasad, 1999; Bloom et al., 2012; Bapna et al., 2013; Hatch and Dyer 2004; Henderson 2006).

Inasmuch as specialized knowledge domains embodied in heterogeneous human capital will continue to

retain value for judgment, and given that humans will be relevant at work in the foreseeable future, our study

highlights the importance of incorporating these contingencies in theories and empirical work examining

productivity of new (machine learning) technologies, rather than conceptualizing main effects alone. Doing so

enables the identification of real productivity costs incurred because of within-vintage differences in human

capital caused by differential rates of absorptive capacity (Cohen and Levinthal, 1990).

Finally, our study also has implications for related literature streams on human capital and career

specialization (Becker and Murphy, 1992; Lazear, 2004; Rosen, 1983; Teodoridis, 2017; Zuckerman et al.,

2003). While this literature has focused on life cycle effects of human capital investments, and generally found

beneficial effects of developing specialized knowledge (Becker and Murphy, 1992; Castanias and Helfat, 1991;

Greenwood et al., 2017), but also that benefits of specialization are contingent on market or institutional

Page 26: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

25

factors (Castanias and Helfat, 1991; Merluzzi and Phillips, 2016). We extend this literature by highlighting the

differential effects of specialized domains for absorptive capacity to interface with entirely new technologies.

New technological advances such as AI exhibit skill-bias not only in the vertical realm (i.e. high vs. low skill),

but also to the extent they privilege computer science and engineering knowledge relative to other specialized

domains. Accordingly, benefits and costs of career specialization must also be evaluated in the context of

what skills and knowledge are considered foundational for complementarities with the latest technology

vintages.

The study has several implications for practice. First, and immediately applicable to USPTO, our

study unearths technology-skill matches as critical to productivity of alternative technologies. Accordingly,

one immediate insight may be a sequential rather than simultaneous roll-out of machine learning technology.

While GAO (2016) has highlighted the hurdles posed by existing search technologies for patent examiners’

productivity—given significant public concern about backlogs in the patent approval process—our study

shows that these may nonetheless retain value for examiners who have non-CS&E knowledge bases. Overall

productivity, both in the short and long run, may be enhanced through CS&E examiners’ immediate use of

machine learning, and development of vintage specific knowledge by these experts. Such learning-by-doing

insights can then be transformed into a codified library of task and technology specific knowledge for a larger

scale diffusion among examiners and stakeholders for intellectual property rights in the general population.

Using these tools, potential innovators may be better able to assess the likely viability of a patent application,

which is an important element in the choice to start a new firm (Gambardella, Ganco and Honore, 2015).11

Second, and at a broader level, our study highlights the need for technology-skill match specific training,

rather than technology-specific alone. Given that new technologies require re-composition of tasks relative

to old technologies, training programs often focus on the provision of technology specific skills. Our study

calls for a more nuanced approach—one where firms need to account for prior knowledge base alignment.

Third, the implications of our study extend beyond training of the existing workforce to other human

11 Our field interviews indicated that the USPTO has plans to release search tools based on machine learning algorithms, for use by the general population. An example of a related prior initiative is the “peer-to-patent” initiative whereby the USPTO, New York Law School, and IBM are collaborating to provide patent examiners structured information on prior art, based on third-party submission of prior art.

Page 27: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

26

resource management practices, including recruitment, retention, and innovation management, especially for

firms that operate in dynamic industries. For example, the literature on recruitment outlines conditions for

when firms should hire externally vs. internally, including “acqui-hiring” (Chatterji and Patro, 2014) to gain

simultaneous access to technologies and complementary employees, or for positions with strong firm-specific

or industry-specific components (Bidwell and Keller, 2014; Bidwell and Mollick, 2015; Williams, Chen and

Agarwal, 2017). Our results indicate firms investing in new technologies need to incorporate skill-technology

matches for the specific knowledge domains as well, when making decisions on whether to hire externally, or

promote internal talent. Similarly, in the context of employee retention, in addition to factors such as

organizational culture, employee voice and work practices (Sheridan 1992; Spencer 1986; Guthrie 2001), our

study highlights job satisfaction and incentive alignment will also depend on the attention firms implementing

new technologies pay to whether employees have greater or lower skill-technology matches. Perceptions of

fairness and inequality may be exacerbated by underlying productivity differentials from working with

different technologies, so firms need to think through compensation mechanisms carefully.

Finally, and at the larger levels of innovation and education policy, our study highlights the

importance of computer programming training to increase familiarity and develop these as generalized skills

for the knowledge economy, even as subsequent human capital investments continue to focus on specialized

knowledge domains. Recently, Apple CEO, Tim Cook remarked to primary school students at a French

school that learning to code may be more important than learning English as a second language (Clifford,

2017). Elaborating on the comment further, Cook discussed the benefits of computer coding not just in

terms of higher salaries, but as a critical “backseat” complement to creativity in the “front seat,” because “the

blend with both of these, you can do such powerful things now.”

Page 28: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

27

References "Artificial Intelligence." Merriam-Webster.com. Accessed January 3, 2018. https://www.merriam-

webster.com/dictionary/artificial intelligence. Acemoglu, Daron, and Pascual Restrepo. "Robots and Jobs: Evidence from US labor markets." (2017). Adner, Ron, and Daniel Snow. "Old technology responses to new technology threats: demand heterogeneity

and technology retreats." Industrial and Corporate Change 19, no. 5 (2010): 1655-1675.

Adner, Ron, and Rahul Kapoor. "Innovation ecosystems and the pace of substitution: Re‐examining

technology S‐curves." Strategic Management Journal 37, no. 4 (2016): 625-648. Agarwal, R., & Prasad, J. (1997). The role of innovation characteristics and perceived voluntariness in the

acceptance of information technologies. Decision Sciences, 28(3), 557-582 Agarwal, Rajshree, and Constance E. Helfat. "Strategic renewal of organizations." Organization Science 20, no. 2

(2009): 281-293. Agarwal, Ritu, and Jayesh Prasad. "Are individual differences germane to the acceptance of new information

technologies?." Decision Sciences 30, no. 2 (1999): 361-391. Aghion, Philippe, Antonin Bergeaud, Richard Blundell, and Rachel Griffith. "Innovation, Firms and Wage

Inequality." (2017). Aghion, Philippe, Benjamin F. Jones, and Charles I. Jones. Artificial Intelligence and Economic Growth. No.

w23928. National Bureau of Economic Research, 2017. Agrawal, A., J. Gans, and A. Goldfarb. "How AI will change the way we make decisions." Harvard Business

Review (2017). Alcacer, Juan, and Michelle Gittelman. "Patent citations as a measure of knowledge flows: The influence of

examiner citations." The Review of Economics and Statistics 88, no. 4 (2006): 774-779. Allison, John R., and Mark A. Lemley. "Who's Patenting What-An Empirical Exploration of Patent

Prosecution." Vand. L. Rev.53 (2000): 2099. Anderson, Philip, and Michael L. Tushman. "Technological discontinuities and dominant designs: A cyclical

model of technological change." Administrative Science Quarterly (1990): 604-633. Argote, Linda, and Dennis N. Epple. Learning curves in manufacturing. Carnegie Mellon University,

Graduate School of Industrial Administration, 1990. Autor, David H. “Why Are There Still So Many Jobs? The History and Future of Workplace

Automation." Journal of Economic Perspectives, 29(3): 3-30. (2015) Autor, David H., Frank Levy, and Richard J. Murnane. "The skill content of recent technological change: An

empirical exploration." The Quarterly Journal of Economics 118, no. 4 (2003): 1279-1333. Autor, David. Polanyi's paradox and the shape of employment growth. Vol. 20485. Cambridge, MA: National

Bureau of Economic Research, 2014. Bapna, Ravi, Nishtha Langer, Amit Mehra, Ram Gopal, and Alok Gupta. "Human capital investments and

employee performance: an analysis of IT services industry." Management Science 59, no. 3 (2013): 641-658. Bartel, A. P. (1994). Productivity gains from the implementation of employee training programs. Industrial

relations: a journal of economy and society, 33(4), 411-425. Becker, G. S., & Murphy, K. M. (1992). The division of labor, coordination costs, and knowledge. The

Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(4), 1137-1160. Bekman, Eli, John Bound, and Stephen Machin. "Implications of skill-biased technological change:

international evidence." The Quarterly Journal of Economics 113, no. 4 (1998): 1245-1279. Benzell, Seth G., Laurence J. Kotlikoff, Guillermo LaGarda, and Jeffrey D. Sachs. Robots are us: Some

economics of human replacement. No. w20941. National Bureau of Economic Research, 2015. Bessen, James E. "How computer automation affects occupations: Technology, jobs, and skills." (2016). Bidwell, Matthew, and Ethan Mollick. "Shifts and ladders: Comparing the role of internal and external

mobility in managerial careers." Organization Science 26.6 (2015): 1629-1645. Bidwell, Matthew, and J. R. Keller. "Within or without? How firms combine internal and external labor

markets to fill jobs." Academy of Management Journal 57.4 (2014): 1035-1055. Black, S. E., & Lynch, L. M. (2001). How to compete: the impact of workplace practices and information

technology on productivity. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 83(3), 434-445.

Page 29: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

28

Bloom, N., Sadun, R., & Van Reenen, J. (2012). Americans do IT better: US multinationals and the productivity miracle. The American Economic Review, 102(1), 167-201.

Brinckerhoff, Courtenay C. 2014. How Long Is Too Long (Or Too Short) For Patent Application Pendency? July 18. https://www.pharmapatentsblog.com/2014/07/18/how-long-is-too-long-or-too-short-for-patent-application-pendency/. Accessed July 2017.

Brynjolfsson, Erik, and Andrew McAfee. The second machine age: Work, progress, and prosperity in a time of brilliant technologies. WW Norton & Company, 2014.

Bughin, J., E. Hazan, S. Ramaswamy, M. Chui, T. Allas, P. Dahlström, N. Henke, and M. Trench. "Artificial intelligence–the next digital frontier." McKinsey Glob Institute URL https://www. mckinsey. de/files/170620_studie_ai. pdf (2017).

Castanias, Richard P., and Constance E. Helfat. "Managerial resources and rents." Journal of Management 17, no. 1 (1991): 155-171.

Chari, Varadarajan V., and Hugo Hopenhayn. "Vintage human capital, growth, and the diffusion of new technology." Journal of Political Economy 99, no. 6 (1991): 1142-1165.

Chase, W.G., Simon, H.A. 1973. Perception in Chess. Cognitive Psychology. 4(1) 55-81. Chase, William G., and Herbert A. Simon. "The mind's eye in chess." (1973). Chatterji, Aaron, and Arun Patro. "Dynamic capabilities and managing human capital." The Academy of

Management Perspectives 28, no. 4 (2014): 395-408. Choudhury, P., Khanna, T., and Mehta, S. “Future of Patent Examination at the USPTO”, Harvard Business

School Case Study. 2017. Source: https://hbr.org/product/The-Future-of-Patent-Exam/an/617027-PDF-ENG

Christensen, Clayton. "Patterns in the evolution of product competition." European Management Journal 15, no. 2 (1997): 117-127.

Clifford, Catherine. 2017. Source: https://www.cnbc.com/2017/10/12/apple-ceo-tim-cook-learning-to-code-is-so-important.html. Accessed December 2017.

Cohen, Wesley M., and Daniel A. Levinthal. "Absorptive capacity: A new perspective on learning and innovation." Administrative Science Quarterly (1990): 128-152.

Coleman, J., Katz, E., Menzel, H. 1957. The Diffusion of an Innovation among Physicians. Sociometry. 20(4) 253-270.

Cowgill, Bo. “The Labor Market Effects of Hiring through Machine Learning”, Working Paper. 2017 Crouch, Dennis. 2014. USPTO's Swelling Examiner Rolls. November 30.

https://patentlyo.com/patent/2014/11/usptos-swelling-examiner.html. Accessed July 2017. Edmondson, A. C., Winslow, A. B., Bohmer, R. M., & Pisano, G. P. (2003). Learning how and learning what:

Effects of tacit and codified knowledge on performance improvement following technology adoption. Decision Sciences, 34(2), 197-224

Faggella, Daniel. “What is Machine Learning”. September 2, 2017. Source: https://www.techemergence.com/what-is-machine-learning/. Accessed November 2017.

Fernandez, R. M. (2001). Skill-biased technological change and wage inequality: Evidence from a plant retooling. American Journal of Sociology, 107(2), 273-320.

Gambardella, A. & Ganco, M. & Honore, F. (2015). Using What You Know: Patented Knowledge in Incumbent Firms and Employee Entrepreneurship. Organization Science (26), 456-474.

Government Accountability Office (2016). Intellectual Property: Patent Office Should Strengthen Search Capabilities and Better Monitor Examiner’s Work. Report to the Chairman, Committee on the Judiciary, House of Representatives.

Gibbons, R., & Waldman, M. (2004). Task-specific human capital. The American Economic Review, 94(2), 203-207.

Goos, Maarten, and Alan Manning. "Lousy and lovely jobs: The rising polarization of work in Britain." The Review of Economics and Statistics 89, no. 1 (2007): 118-133

Graetz, Georg, and Guy Michaels. "Robots at work." (2015). Greenwood, B., Agarwal R., Agarwal R., and Gopal A. 2017 The Role of Individual and Organizational

Expertise in the Adoption of New Practices

Page 30: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

29

Guthrie, James P. "High-involvement work practices, turnover, and productivity: Evidence from New Zealand." Academy of Management Journal 44.1 (2001): 180-190.

Hatch, Nile W., and Jeffrey H. Dyer. "Human capital and learning as a source of sustainable competitive advantage." Strategic Management Journal 25, no. 12 (2004): 1155-1178.

Henderson, Rebecca. "The innovator's dilemma as a problem of organizational competence." Journal of Product Innovation Management 23, no. 1 (2006): 5-11.

Hoffman, Mitchell, Lisa B. Kahn, and Danielle Li. Discretion in hiring. No. w21709. National Bureau of Economic Research, 2015.

Hounshell, David. From the American system to mass production, 1800-1932: The development of manufacturing technology in the United States. No. 4. JHU Press, 1985.

Jovanovic, Boyan. "The technology cycle and inequality." The Review of Economic Studies 76, no. 2 (2009): 707-729.

Krishna, Arthi M., Brian Feldman, Joseph Wolf, Greg Gabel, Scott Beliveau, and Thomas Beach. "User Interface for Customizing Patents Search: An Exploratory Study." In International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, pp. 264-269. Springer International Publishing, 2016.

Lazear, Edward P. "Balanced skills and entrepreneurship." The American Economic Review 94, no. 2 (2004): 208-211.

Lemley, Mark A., and Bhaven Sampat. "Examiner characteristics and patent office outcomes." Review of Economics and Statistics 94.3 (2012): 817-827.

Mansfield, E. (1968). Industrial research and technological innovation; an econometric analysis. London: Longmans.

Merluzzi, Jennifer, and Damon J. Phillips. "The specialist discount: Negative returns for MBAs with focused profiles in investment banking." Administrative Science Quarterly 61, no. 1 (2016): 87-124.

Mokyr, Joel. Twenty five centuries of technological change: an historical survey. Vol. 35. Taylor & Francis, 1990.

Partha, Dasgupta, and Paul A. David. "Toward a new economics of science." Research Policy 23, no. 5 (1994): 487-521.

Rosen S. 1983. Specialization and human capital. Journal of Labor Economics 1(1): 43–49. Schultz, T.W. 1961. Investment in Human Capital. The American Economic Review 1-17 Sheridan, John E. "Organizational culture and employee retention." Academy of Management Journal 35.5 (1992):

1036-1056. Spencer, Daniel G. "Employee voice and employee retention." Academy of Management Journal 29.3 (1986): 488-

502. Teodoridis, Florenta. "Understanding Team Knowledge Production: The Interrelated Roles of Technology

and Expertise." Management Science (2017). Thornton, R. A., & Thompson, P. (2001). Learning from experience and learning from others: An

exploration of learning and spillovers in wartime shipbuilding. American Economic Review, 1350-1368. Williams, C., Chen, P. L., & Agarwal, R. (2017). Rookies and seasoned recruits: How experience in different

levels, firms, and industries shapes strategic renewal in top management. Strategic Management Journal, 38(7), 1391-1415.

Zeira, Joseph. "Workers, machines, and economic growth." The Quarterly Journal of Economics 113, no. 4 (1998): 1091-1117.

Zuckerman EW, Kim T, Ukanwa K, von Rittmann J. 2003. Robust identities or nonentities? Typecasting in

the feature‐film labor market. American Journal of Sociology 108(5): 1018–1074.

Page 31: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

30

Figures

Figure 1. Skill-Technology Match

Figure 2. Prior Art Citations by Skill-Technology Match

Page 32: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

31

Figure 3. Prior Art Citations by Skill-Technology Match and Expert Advice

Figure 4. Prior Art Citations by Technology and Prior Knowledge Domain

Page 33: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

32

Figure 5. Prior Art Citations by the Receipt of Expert Advice

Page 34: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

33

Tables

Page 35: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

34

Page 36: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

35

Page 37: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

36

Page 38: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

37

Page 39: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

38

Page 40: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

39

Page 41: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

40

APPENDIX

Appendix A. Experiment Details

Research Context

The sample population for this study are second-year MBA students enrolled in a top tier

business school in the US, across three sections of the same course with approximately 70 students

each. The course explored strategies for maintaining and enhancing competitive advantage over time

During the final class of the spring semester, instructors taught “The Future of Patent

Examination at the U.S. Patent and Trademark Office (USPTO)” (HBS case no. 617-027). This case

introduces students to the patent examination process and describes the machine learning strategies

the USPTO is currently piloting to expedite patent examination and improve patent quality. To

enhance students’ learning experience, one of the researchers—working closely with the business

school information technology (IT) department and key USPTO stakeholders—created an online

exercise that allowed students to experiment with the USPTO’s machine learning tools (See Exhibit

7 in Appendix B for a complete tool development timeline.). The university IT department

partnered with key stakeholders at the USPTO to create an online tool—the Patent Examiner

Review Tool (PERT)—to simulate the process of examining a patent application. See Exhibit 8 in

Appendix B for a diagram of the PERT exercise workflow.

Designing the PERT Interface

The IT department decided to use a “reverse proxy” approach to build the PERT tool. In

essence, the IT department embedded the USPTO sites (either PatFT or Sigma) into the university

system and then built a “shell” site around them (see Exhibits 9 and 10 in Appendix B). This shell

site (i.e., the PERT tool) hid certain buttons from view to prevent students from navigating away

from the PatFT or Sigma sites. For instance, the PatFT site has an “advanced search” button (refer

Page 42: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

41

to Exhibit 2 in Appendix B) that directs workers to a page with several filters and settings. To avoid

confusion, the PERT tool hid this option from students’ view.

PERT also featured a side panel with information about each of the five patent claims under

review. Students could click to review Claim 1, for example, and then collapse the side panel to

search either the embedded PatFT or Sigma sites (depending on their assigned group) for prior art

relevant to that particular claim. If they located a similar existing patent, they could save it to the

reference panel within PERT (see Exhibit 11 in Appendix B). Because the patent under review was

submitted in January 2010, students could only reference prior art that predated this point.

Once students believed that they had exhausted the prior art search for Claim 1, they then

indicated their decision to approve or reject the claim within the PERT tool (see Exhibit 12 in

Appendix B). If students decided to disallow the claim, they were required to cite at least one prior

patent reference to justify this decision. Students followed the same process to adjudicate the

remaining four patent claims. The university server tracked how long it took students to answer each

question.12

Challenges in Developing the PERT Tool

Several challenges had to be successfully overcome to develop the PERT tool under time

constraints. First, Sigma was a proprietary USPTO tool and it took two months for the University to

receive clearance to use Sigma. Second, the PERT exercise was the first time that the IT department

had worked with an external organization to create a simulation exercise. For all other projects up to

that point, the IT department had owned the tools and databases used during these exercises, which

gave it complete control over the students’ experience. The PERT simulation, by contrast, required

students to use tools and databases that were owned and managed by the USPTO.

12 If a student was inactive for longer than five minutes, a pop-up box appeared and asked whether the student was still working. If the student did not reply, the server stopped tracking time.

Page 43: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

42

Reliance on these USPTO sites presented a significant amount of project risk and

uncertainty for the IT department. For instance, if students could not access the PatFT site during

the exercise or if existing database glitches suddenly emerged, our administrator would have to

contact the USPTO to correct the issue. Because PERT would be open to the students over the

course of a weekend, our administrator doubted the availability of a USPTO representative to

resolve problems in real-time. Moreover, because the PatFT database was a public site, a number of

external databases, such as Lexus Nexis, routinely trawled it for data. When site traffic was heavy,

load times could be exceedingly slow (e.g., it could take as long as five minutes for search results to

load). Delays of this magnitude during the student exercise would be untenable.

To mitigate risks, the IT department held a series of routine meetings with the USPTO team

in March and April to identify and resolve “bugs” in the system. As an example, in the days leading

up to the PERT exercise, the administrator realized that PatFT did not recognize curly quotation

marks (“…”) in a search string; the tool only worked with straight quotation marks ("..."). The IT

department contacted the USPTO team and resolved the problem.

To reduce the likelihood of system issues during the student exercise, the IT department

requested that the Patent Office avoid installing system updates or interfering with the PatFT and

Sigma interfaces in the days leading up to the exercise. The USPTO also agreed to distribute website

traffic across additional servers while the PERT exercise was open, thus improving load times.

Experimental Design

The students were randomly assigned to four groups, with each group receiving a slightly

different set of information and tools. Two groups were assigned Sigma while the other two were

assigned the Boolean technology. Each group was then asked to review a patent application that had

been submitted to the USPTO in 2010; students were asked to work alone to complete the exercise.

The application in question requested a patent for a specific method of delivering digital

Page 44: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

43

promotional content (see Exhibit 4 in Appendix B). As part of this exercise, five of the 39 claims

included in this application were extracted and students were asked to use the PERT tool to search

the prior art, decide whether each claim met the criteria for patentability, and justify their rationale

for allowing or rejecting the claim. Unbeknownst to students at the time of the exercise, the patent

examiner who adjudicated this particular application in 2010 rejected all five claims because they

were not novel. The patent examiner had used a single prior patent (the “prior art citation”) to reject

all of the claims.

To acclimate students to the PERT tool, the IT department developed a PERT user guide,

created a training video, and scheduled three in-person training sessions for interested students (on

April 12, 13, and 19) prior to the exercise. Approximately 10 students attended each of the in-person

trainings. However, the training video and the user guide was posted online and could be accessed

by all students at all times. The team posted separate technology specific videos and technology

specific online guides for the treatment group and the control group.

Students accessed the PERT tool by logging into the online course platform, at any point

between 3 p.m. EST on Wednesday, April 19 and 11:59 p.m. EST on Sunday, April 23. They were

instructed to use the Google Chrome browser since PERT is incompatible with Internet Explorer.

Participation in the exercise was voluntary. When students opened the PERT tool, a release form

appeared, which explained that the researchers intended to use the results of the PERT simulation

for research purposes. Thus, if students opted out of the exercise or stopped at any point, they

would not be penalized. A total of 198 students participated in the exercise.

Once logged into PERT, students were shown different dashboards, depending on their

group assignment. Two student groups could use the PatFT database only, while the other two only

had access to the Sigma tool. Students using the PatFT database searched the prior art by compiling

Page 45: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

44

lists of synonyms to describe each patent claim and conducting keyword searches, while those with

access to Sigma searched by weighting components of the application and creating word clouds.

To narrow the search results, the tool provided all students with the appropriate

“classification code” (i.e., the starting point for a search string) for this particular patent application.

The research team recognized that students would likely feel overwhelmed and unmotivated if their

search resulted in hundreds of thousands of hits. For instance, searching “digital communications”

within PatFT generates more than 18,000 results. Starting the search with the provided classification

code reduced students’ search hits down to the hundreds.

On the last day of the exercise, half of the students in each technology group were sent an e-

mail, depending on their randomly assigned technology, with expert advice from a USPTO

examiner. These messages are displayed in Appendix B Exhibit 6a and 6b. The expert advice in the

emails constituted both technology and task specific information. The e-mail sent to individuals

using the Boolean search tool provided helpful hints on keywords to use for the Boolean search

(advice related to task) and advice on using the filter option in the user interface to filter out certain

references (advice related to technology). For the email sent to individuals using the machine

learning search tool, the expert advice comprised keywords to consider in the search (advice related

to task), as well as advice on editing both the word cloud and the sliders that gave weights to the

patent abstract, claims, title and description during the search (advice related to technology). The

expert advice by itself was not sufficient for task completion. Individual students had to not only

integrate the expert advice into the search tool, but also subsequently use judgment in reading and

selecting prior art references. In other words, the task of the user was twofold: First, the task

required integrating the expert advice correctly on the user interface. Second, it required reading,

interpreting and selecting relevant prior art from the narrower set of prior art references that were

generated. Once the exercise was over and the results of the randomized trial were discussed in

Page 46: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

45

class, all participants were granted access to Sigma so that interested students assigned to the PatFT

group could experiment with the Sigma tool.

Page 47: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

46

Appendix B. Exhibits Related to the Experiment

Exhibit 1 Patent Examination Process

Source: PowerPoint Slides, PERT_Training

Page 48: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

47

Exhibit 2 Interface of USPTO Database, PatFT

Source: U.S. Patent and Trademark Office, “USPTO Patent Full-Text and Image Database,”

http://patft.uspto.gov/netahtml/PTO/search-bool.html, accessed July 2017.

Exhibit 3 Sigma Tool

Source: Arthi M. Krishna, Brian Feldman, Joseph Wolf, Greg Gabel, Scott Beliveau, and Thomas

Beach, “User Interface for Customizing Patents Search: An Exploratory Study, United States Patent

and Trademark Office, 2011.

Note: Workers enter important search terms in the word cloud and manipulate the sliders on the

left-hand side to assign weight to certain components of the patent filing.

Page 49: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

48

Exhibit 4 Patent Application (Front Page) Included in Classroom Simulation and Five Claims

Page 50: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

49

Patent Claims: 1) A method for delivering promotional content for presentation in a first interactive media guidance

application on a first user equipment device, comprising: receiving data corresponding to a plurality of user actions in a second interactive media guidance application; associating at least one of a plurality of weights with each of the plurality of user actions; based upon the plurality of user actions and the at least one of the plurality of weights associated with each user action, determining the likelihood of a user watching at least one of a certain program, a certain type of program, and a certain channel; selecting promotional content for presentation in the first interactive media guidance application based on the determination; and transmitting the promotional content selected to the first user equipment device with program content.

2) The method of claim 1, wherein the data specifies at least one of user actions taken, the time at which user actions are taken, and programs associated with the user actions taken.

3) The method of claim 1, wherein the plurality of user actions include at least one of selecting a program, watching a program, configuring to record a program, and configuring a reminder for a program.

4) The method of claim 1, wherein the plurality of user actions include at least one of selecting a channel, watching a channel, configuring to record a channel, and configuring a reminder for a channel.

5) The method of claim 1, wherein the second interactive media guidance application is presented as a Web page.

Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 51: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

50

Exhibit 5 The Relevant “Prior Art” Patent Reference

Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 52: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

51

Exhibit 6a Search Hints, Email Sent to the Group of Students with Access to the PatFT Database Dear all, I received some useful tips for solving the patent examination simulation from a senior patent examiner at the USPTO. I have copied the email I received below. I HAVE CHOSEN TO SHARE THESE HINTS WITH ONLY A RANDOMLY CHOSEN SELECTION OF THE CLASS, SO PLEASE DO NOT SHARE THEM WITH ANYONE ELSE. IF YOU HAVE ALREADY COMPLETED THE EXERCISE, YOU MIGHT WANT TO USE THE HINTS TO TAKE ANOTHER LOOK AT EXAMINING THE CLAIMS, IT MIGHT IMPROVE THE ACCURACY OF YOUR EXAMINATION. Have a great rest of the day Best, Professor XX I am a patent examiner for the USPTO, and I was informed that you were interested in some input on developing a search strategy for patent application 12/177,788 (PG Pub: US20100017814A1). My experience with these types of cases has taught me to use US Patent Classification code 725/46 when performing the search. Additionally, I suggest using the Issue Date code to filter out any references published after the US20100017814A1 pub date. After reading this application, my recommendation is to use the following search string in your query: CCL/725/46 AND (weight$ OR ranking) AND (tuning OR recording OR watching OR reminding) and ("web page" OR internet) AND (profile OR advert$) AND ISD/1/1/1990->1/20/2010 This should yield around 53 hits. Review these results carefully. Hope this helps. Good luck! Cheers, Mackinzie Simmons United States Patent and Trademark Office - an agency of the Department of Commerce Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 53: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

52

Exhibit 6b Search Hints, Email Sent to the Group of Students with Access to Sigma Dear student, I received some useful tips for solving the patent examination simulation from a senior patent examiner at the USPTO. I have copied the email I received below. I HAVE CHOSEN TO SHARE THESE HINTS WITH ONLY A RANDOMLY CHOSEN SELECTION OF THE CLASS, SO PLEASE DO NOT SHARE THEM WITH ANYONE ELSE. IF YOU HAVE ALREADY COMPLETED THE EXERCISE, YOU MIGHT WANT TO USE THE HINTS TO TAKE ANOTHER LOOK AT EXAMINING THE CLAIMS. IT MIGHT IMPROVE THE ACCURACY OF YOUR EXAMINATION. Have a great rest of the day Best, Professor XX I am a patent examiner for the USPTO, and I was informed that you were interested in some input on developing a search strategy for patent application 12/177,788 (PG Pub: US20100017814A1). My experience with these types of cases has taught me to leverage the weight boosters first and then edit the word cloud. After reading this application, here is my recommendation for how you can improve your search: Make sure PG Pub US20100017814A1 is loaded. If not, start a new search by entering “US20100017814A1” in the Search field at the top and click Search. Uncheck the “All Text” box and check the “Claims” box. Set the Claims weight to 10 and hit APPLY. Edit the word cloud by replacing the existing list with the following string of words (based on my experience, these words have yielded the best results in terms of this specific application):

advertis epg monitor schedul tune area grid pip screen tv button guid press select viewer can highlight profil televis watch channel inform program theme window data interact record tile displai mean region time

Close the Edit List window and click on the SEARCH button only. Review this page of these results carefully. Make sure any reference you cite is published before Jan 21, 2010 (the US20100017814A1 pub date). Hope this helps. Good luck! Cheers, Mackinzie Simmons United States Patent and Trademark Office - an agency of the Department of Commerce Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 54: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

53

Exhibit 7 Timeline of Preparatory Activities for the PERT Simulation

Date Action

11/03/2016 Professors submitted project concept statement

11/10/2016 Initial faculty consultation with IT

11/21/2016 First meeting with USPTO Lead

12/05/2016 First meeting with USPTO Sigma team: establish technical requirements

12/06/2016 Gather classroom experience requirements from Professors

12/15/2016 Project proposal submitted to Division of Research and Faculty Development

01/04/2017 PERT is approved for funding

01/05/2017 USPTO approves Sigma access for university use

02/07/2017 First draft of PERT interface completed

02/08/2017 Reviewed PERT interface draft

02/09/2017 Team meeting (IT – USPTO)

02/10/2017 Team meeting (IT – USPTO)

02/17/2017 Team meeting (IT – USPTO); USPTO team demonstrated Sigma tool to professors. Sigma behavior necessitated a change to the PERT interface

03/03/2017 Team meeting (IT – USPTO): IT demonstrated PERT interface prototype

03/10/2017 Team meeting (IT – USPTO)

03/17/2017 IT showed the PERT admin interface to Professors to get feedback

03/22/2017 PERT group test (IT only)

03/22/2017 USPTO delivers the patent and claims to be used for PERT exercise

03/23/2017 PERT Dry Run (IT – USPTO); USPTO helped to address the slow loading times on the PatFT site

03/29/2017 Team meeting (IT – USPTO)

04/04/2017 PERT group test (IT only)

04/06/2017 PERT group test (IT only) at 2pm PERT Final Dry Run (IT – USPTO) at 5pm

04/11/2017 Meet with Professors: PERT training prep

04/12/2017 PERT Student Training at 5pm

04/13/2017 PERT Student Training at 5pm

04/14/2017 Professors looped in Research Computing Services

04/17/2017 Meet with professors and Research Computing Services; PERT User Guide finalized; PERT Cohort assignment finalized

04/18/2017 IT created “How to Use PERT” video

04/19/2017 PERT exercise opened at 3pm for students; PERT assignment posted on Canvas (includes User Guide, How-to video, Training Slide Deck) PERT Student Training at 5pm

04/20/2017 Prepare ‘Search Hints’ email for PERT SIGMA and PERT PatFT cohorts

04/23/2017 Search Hints email sent at 11.29am; PERT exercised closed at 11:59pm

04/24/2017 Classroom debrief of USPTO case and PERT results; PERT reopened at 3pm (all SIGMA access only)

04/26/2017 Survey distributed on PERT exercise PERT closed permanently at 6pm

Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 55: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

54

Exhibit 8 Overall Student Workflow for PERT Exercise

Source: Researchers.

Exhibit 9 PERT Site with the US PatFT Database Embedded

Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 56: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

55

Exhibit 10 Pert Site with the Sigma Tool Embedded

Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 57: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

56

Exhibit 11 Saving Relevant Patent References within the PERT Tool

Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Exhibit 12 PERT Tool with Patent Claim 1 Expanded

Source: Class Exercise Materials.

Page 58: DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS EXPERIMENTAL ... · DIFFERENT STROKES FOR DIFFERENT FOLKS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON COMPLEMENTARITIES BETWEEN HUMAN CAPITAL AND MACHINE LEARNING

57

Appendix C

Figure C1. Patent Application Process (GAO 2016)