Transcript
Page 1: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

This article was downloaded by: [Central Michigan University]On: 05 November 2014, At: 10:47Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

Research on Language andSocial InteractionPublication details, including instructions for authorsand subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hrls20

Putting Prior Talk Into Context:Reported Speech and theReporting ContextRichard ButtnyPublished online: 14 Jun 2010.

To cite this article: Richard Buttny (1998) Putting Prior Talk Into Context: ReportedSpeech and the Reporting Context, Research on Language and Social Interaction, 31:1,45-58, DOI: 10.1207/s15327973rlsi3101_3

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327973rlsi3101_3

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all theinformation (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform.However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness,or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and viewsexpressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of theContent should not be relied upon and should be independently verified withprimary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for anylosses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly orindirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of theContent.

Page 2: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found athttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 3: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

Research on Language and Social h~teractinn, 31(1), 45-58 Copyright O 1998, Lawrence Er1bat.m Associates, Inc.

Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

Richard Buttmy Department of Speech Communication

Syracuse University

Within the disciplines interested in language and social interaction, there are multiple approaclhes to the notion of context (Goodwin & Duranti, 1992). Although there is no unanimity on how to treat context, there is wide agreement that rve need to attend to it. It is by now axiomatic: If context is ignored, then we cannot adequately interpret the text. In this article, I offer some general ruminations on context before turning to the specific problem of reported speech and context.

THE IDEA OF CONTEXT

Perhaps the very vocabulary we use to raise these issues, "text" and '"context," misleads us. The literary emphasis of the notion of "text," as opposed to "talk," or "conversation," or "talk-in-interaction" has been pointed out (Schegloff, 199'7). A related criticism could be made of the term context. Taken literally, context surrounds the text; it is that in which the text takes place. Thought of as surrounding the text, context conjures up spatial imagery+ontext as a container, context as the sociophysical

--

Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to Richard Buttny, Department of Speech Communication, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244. E-mail: [email protected]

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 4: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

46 Richard Ruttny 7-

setting, the specialized institution, the foreign culture. If text encompasses the words spoken during an exchange, then context involves the remaining aspects of the socid interactian. If text is the microlevel of social inter- action, context includes macrolevel dimensions in which to situate the text, such as histoi-ical background, the participants' power or economic position. Operationally, the text is that which can be written up in a transcript whereas the context is the background information supplied parenthetically at the beginning-the cast of characters, the social or physical setting, the participants' relationship, and so on.

Few, if any, theorists subscribe to the view of context as container, though in research practice it is not uncolnmonly treated in this way. A more reflective view takes context as cxisting both independently of the text and as jrelcreated in and through the text. On this dual-aspect conception, context is both external and internal to the talk. both exogenotkii and endogenous to communicative interaction (Heritage, 1984); context Is simultaneously brought dong and brought about through the talk (Auer. 1992).

A sensible way to begin is to get to know as much as you can about the context. This seemingly innocuous claim raises the problem of how do we know that this background knowledge about the context is relevant for the participants here and now (Schegloff, 1991). Given the indefinitely vast amount of contextual information, as analysts we need some way to show that our background knowledge of the context is relevant for the participants. 'To tell that contextual featlrres are relevant we need to see that participants orient to them in some way. If participants are orienting to the context, then context may be available in the text.

An approach to showing how participants orient to context comes from cmversation analysis, particularly studies focusing on institutional or specialized settings. for example, the courtroom, medical settings, broadcast news interviews, and the like (Drew & Heritage. 1992a; Drew & Sorjonen, f 991). The analytical goal is ti3 show how participants' social interaction reflects md constitutes their situated context. In such contexts partic'ipants enact and display institutional identities as seen in their talk. Judge-counsel, physician-patient, interviewer-interviewee exhibit char- acteristic ways of speaking that display these identities. These charac- teristic ways of speaking may include specialized forms of turn-taking, word choice, drawing inferences. and the like. The anaIyst searches the texts for the identifjeng details that mark &at talk as a particular context. So, contexts get talked into being through participants' particular ways of speaking and orienting to each other.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 5: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

Putting Prior Talk Into Context 47

We need to take context as that which can be empirically investigated rather than as some factor that a priori is assumed to be at work. The proposition that gender or race is always relevant as a context to a conversation is the kind of claim being cautioned against. Although these features may indeed be relevant, we need to show in what ways they are rdevant through persons' orientations, that is, their t.allr, noticeable si- lence, or other nonverbal dilsplays. In a recent study on "talking race" (Buttny, 1997) I found, not surprisingly, that race was relevant in discur- sive con,structions about race but did not make a difference in describing the conversational practice of reporting speech.

In some of my prior work I have concentrated OII the context of couple therapy (Buttny, 19980; Buttny & Cohen, 1991; B~uttny & Jensen, 1995; Buttny, 1996). Part of this project has been to specify characteristic ways that participants orient to and display the interactic~n as therapy. In orienting to the interaction a,s therapy, participants are partaking in varied speech acts, positionings, and footings that constitute the therapeutic context. However, these conversational practices, although characteristic of therapy, are not unique to therapy-they can be found in other contexts a~s well. As Heritage (1984) pointed out, the third turn of a question-an- swer-evaluation sequence k, seen in a variety of institutional settings as 21 slot for the institutional representative to evaluate the client's prior answer. Also, everyday talk (:an take on a therapy-like chxacter. Ordinary c:onversation can exhibit w ~ y s of speaking also found in therapy, such as cautiousness, qualifying stakements, expressing uncertainty, citing pub- licly available facts, or discussing possibilities (Perakyla & Silverman, 1991; Bergmann, 1992; Drew & Heritage, 1992b; Gaik, 1992). Granted, therapeutic discourse is not so unique or distinctive that it cannot be found in other contexts. Nonethe1e:ss the general claim still holds-these ways of speaking are characteristic of the practice of therapy. If we were to view a videowe or read the transcript without knowing what it was, I submit that we would be able to readily identify the context by the participants' discourse. It is through certain ways of speaking and conversational practices that persons display and orient to their situated identities and local knowledge of context.

An endogenous sense of context seems to make a stronger case than an exogenous context (cf. Mandelbaum, 1990191). My own approach follows a kind of methodological dictum: If the analyst can show how participants orient to context as situated communicative practices identified through characterj stic speech patterns, interactional sequences, or inference making, then do so.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 6: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

48 Richard Buttny

THE CONVERSATIQNAL ERACTICE OF PUTTING PRIOR TALK INTO CONTEXT

Ongoing personal relationships may be seen as lived through a series of conversations (Tamen, 1990). A prior bit of a conversation can serve as a context for what is happening now. To reference one's own or another's prior words, the current speaker needs to place such discourse in a context. In the remainder of this article, I examine bow previous talk is used and put into context in a present canversation-the practice of reported speech.

Reported speech may be simply characterized as quoting another perkon's, or one's own, prior words. This may take the form of direct quotation (purportedly quoting the actual words of the original speaker) or indirect quotation (a summary of what was said in the words of the teller) (Holt, 1996).' Reported speech can be characterized as a "double- voiced discourse" reflecting both the original speaker and the present speaker (Bakhtin, 1981)-"double voiced" in that the present speaker appropriates the quoted discourse for his or her own purposes in the present context. "double-voiced" character of reported speech pre- sents some intriguing problems for the study of context:

Between the reported speech and the reporting contexf litalics added], dynamic relations of high complexity and tension are in force. A failure to take these into account makes it impossible to understa~ld any farm of reported speech. (Volosinov, 1971, p. 153)

Volosinov's point here is that the "original" speech gets altered by being uttered by the reporting speaker in the reporting context. So reported speech, no matter how accurate its reproduction of the original text, cannot convey the same meaning as its use in its original context (Tannen, 1989).2

To develop Volosinov's point, three senses of context may be dis- tinguished: (a) the original words uttered in context, what might be called "the original context"; (b) the context offered by the reporting speaker to situate the reported speech, "the context of the storyworld"; and (c) the iresent interactional context of the reporting speaker conversing with interlocutor(s), "the reporting context."

Reported speech commonly, though not exclusively, occurs in nar- ratives. Many of our actions about which we tell stories are things done with words, so what I, or another, has said often becomes a resource we

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 7: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

Putting Prior Talk Into Context 49

draw on in narratives. Reported speech seems reserved for capturing the most crucial or interesting parts of the narrative. This ]nay be because r~zported speech, especially direct quotes, comes closest to presenting what was said, and thereby, done. The reported speech conveys the "what was said," but some context for the reported speech is necessary to indicate "what actions were done" along with their social significance.

In some approaches to narrative, context is taken in a commonsensical way as providing the background information for the story, an orientation to the tirne and place and characters involved (Labov & Waletzky, 1968). This background information, as it were, provides a context to understand the subsequent actions and events.

But context needs to be seen as involving more than mere background information in telling a narrative. Context is continually oriented to by participants. As Sacks noted in discussing reported talk in narratives:

Thers are some reasons why these kinds of context information designed for the listener are used, and that is, to keep them attentive to hotv to read wlmt they're being told. And there is reason for those things to be put directly before or afer [italics added]. (Sacks, 1992. p. 274)

So context works for recipient design and contextual framing purposes, for telling recipients "how to read what they're being told."

This contextual framing activity (or telling recipients "how to read what they're being told") is especially apparent in reporting speech. I first noticed this in a study of racial discourse; participants would quote their OMTI or others' voices and simultaneously evaluate them--over- whelmingly to criticize out-group members (Buttny, 1997). The evaluative component serves as a contextual frame for how to take the reported speech. To put this another way, context involves utterances designed to tell recipients how to hear the reported speech.

To further investigate how speakers contextually frame reported speech, we need to examine some articular cases. The. following tran- P script is taken from an initial couple therapy session. I want to draw attention to Larry's narrative (beginning at line 055) and reported speech (lines 066-068, 078-079) and the various layers of context.

((The three participants are the therapist, t, Lurry, L, and Jenny, J))

001 T : Ah you mentioning (0.6) That he is, (0.6) afrAa:id =

002 J: [Uh huh

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 8: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

= of (1.5) ah: is that a: (.) feeling that fits::

(1.3) your own: (0.5) perception? that when you don't

share your feelings it's because you are afrcid? >you

feel < (i .O) krightened?

No:. No (.) so that's a ell: (0.7) r- if I wodd be y;:

I would ah: jump: on:: tha:t and I would object to

the statement (1.1) thar you are afraid >not that you

doh't express feelings (1.1) because I believe that

you two agree with that< (.)but the question of ah expresssng (.j of- cf k i n g afraid (;.I) is something that I- makes me feel a bit uncod~rtable . =

= Yeah I- I don't agree with tb::at

= I- it's not fear (0.6) of expressing my feelings,

>I Eean I get < (0.8) I (0.9) zi other situations I get

a= and shaw my anger easi:iy .h (0.8) uhm I get

and 1- I think sho:w my happiness easiIy (0.8)

I'm not afra::id to show emorion I- f L3ut ,you< you

are "with me:, I think0 (0.4) you don't do it as much

with as you do it with other people? .hh

(4.3) Ri ht, so it's not fear:, it might '%be lack of tru:st f: (1.1) "but it's not fear."

Urn hum (.) .hhh ah lack of trust means ah:: (0.9) the the ways in: which: (0.5) you ah::: (2.4) ah:: worry

(0.8) she may be handling those feelings:

Ye:s: (.) very goo-d !Uh huh zuh huh<=

((skip 10 lines of joking))

Uhm (3.3) >where were we?< Uhm feelings sharin g (shared feelings) FE AR

[We were talking about that I Oh about that ( )

Ah that I- (0.4) may:be, the thing that I m i s E is

Would: you: mind describing that a bit?

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 9: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

Putting Prior Talk Into Context 5 1 -

uh[m . What is your experience of her handling lro:ur::

(0.5) feelings?

pt Well: in the ah:: (0.4) in the first number of

years of our (.) relationship (0.9) I would ah (0.9) we were in a stgge: I think that was fairly different

>we were younger< and ah (0.8) uhrn I think she was a

different person and I was a different person in the

sense that ah (0.8) uhrn our values: were different

and (.) our intern31 (1.4) whatever drives us I think

was slightly different (0.5) things have happened to us

since then 10.7) that have changed ah just like in -- life >things happen to everyone< .hhh but in tho:se

years: ah (0.6) I would express a: & of emotion

"you know0 are you going out with those other

men why don't you: stay with me an' (0.6) ah why don't you move up to Portland at the time she was in the.

state of Maryland (0.6) for a (0.9) in college and (.)

sh- she wouldn't move up (0.5) at that ti:me (0.5)

uhrn she kept the boyfriend (an-) a number of boyfriends

and a (0.8) and s3 there was this triangle most of

those ear:ly years:and I MEAN THERE WERE A LOT OF EMOTION THAT I- I ( ) I would w g r by the

phone (0.6; for her to call (0.4) and she would always be late or not call at all, (1.3) uhrn >things like that<

(0.4) and I and I would tell her >you know< how distress& I was "ya know0 why didn't ya ca:ll:? you

know I was wai:tQ by the- in the ah (.) I'd show all my emotions .hhh and AT THAT TIME (0.9) ah I don't-

you know they weren't handed (0.7) in a way that ah: made my thi? happiest >"let's put it that way0<

.h hhhhh but uhrn (1.7) pt but I think that she had =

[Uh hum

= her gwn problems at that time=I had my problems dealing with ah:: that kind of situation (0.5) "ya know0 I think I would handle that kind of sit- that particular situation diff:erently nAow: and I wouldn't be so unhappy (0.4) as I was then:, (0.7) uhrn (1.7)

SO I guess what I'm saying is that you can't just look

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 10: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

52 Richard Buttny

091 at what happened last week why:: (1.5) why do I fear 092 or mis- why don't

093 T: [ > ~ h well< ah (1.6) yo:u: can't but let me try 094 just the same . . .

Notice that when the therapist asks Larry to describe his feelings or experience (lines 051-054), he tells a story. Sarbin (1989) has noted that people commonly use narratives to account for their emotions. The story- the characters, circumstances, and what occurred-puts the narrator's feelings into context by making t b s e feelings and emotions under- standable, as a response to certain contirigacies. Affect, feelings, and emotions are discursive objects, not merely raw sensations. As such, they need to be situated in a nexus of social relations and actions to be presented as intelligible and justifiable. So Larry's narrative of his girlfriend having lived out of state to go to college, of having another boyfriend(s), and not telephoning are the sorts of circumstances that can make a person emotional and even mistrustful. These circumstances can make one re- spond emotionally as seen through Larry's affect display and reported speech. When we look at affect as a discursive construction, then narrative provides a context for undei.standing affect.

Turning to look at this narrative more closely, Larry begins by offering a background or orientation to their prior relational state and "stage" they were in at that time. Larry then avows his general disposition to be expressive of his emations (Lines 064066), which contextually frames his reported speech (lines 066-068) as instances of this expres- siveness. Returning to Volosinov's point, we can see how the reported speech gets struc~wed by the reporting context, by how the reporting speakcr wants recipients to take the reported speech. Without the imme- diately prior contexhdizing statement, ""Twuld express a: lot of emo- tion," it seems doubtful that listeners would hear the reportespeech of lines 066T68 in just that way.

Larry's reported speech here is farmed grammatically as a series of three why-questions: are you going out with those other men why don't you: stay with me an' (0.6) ah why don't you move up to Portland" (lines 066-068). fn the context of the storyworld these questions, ad- dressed to Jenny, can be heard in various ways as a blaming, a request, or a plea, These reparted why-questions in~plicate some prior troublesome actions dune by Jenny, for instance, that for which she is blamed. Also, these reported why-questions sequei~tially implicate responses from Jenny

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 11: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

htt ing Prior Talk Into Context 53 --

that Larry supplies in continuing with the narrative: "she wouldn't move up" (line 070) and "she kept the boyfriend (an-) a number of boyfriends" (line 071).

For the reporting context-that is, the present conversation-not only is the frame of expressing emotion relevant but also the implicit blame in Larry's questions and Jenny's reported responses. To understand the report- ing context it is crucial to bear in mind the participation framework. Larry's rtarrative is addressed to the. therapist with Jenny as a coparticipant. His rlarrative is not a disinterested report of events. In his story, Larry seems to discursively position himself as responding emotionally to Jenny's misdeeds.

This contextual framing of reported speech, and reported action, is also apparent as the narrative continues. Larry's emphatic reference to emotion he expressed (lines 073-074) provides a context for his reported action of waiting by the phone (lines 074-076) and his reported speech of telling her of his feelings (lines 078-079).

072 L: and a (0.8) and sla there was this triangle most of tllose 073 ear:ly years: and I MEAN THERE WERE A LOT 074 OF EMOTION THAT I- I ( ) I would w2:t by the 075 phone (0.6) for her to call (0.4) and she would always

076 be late or not call at all, (1.3) uhm >things like that< 077 (0.4) and I and I would tell her >you know< how

078 dist- I was Oya know0 why didn't ya ca:ll:? you 079 know I was wai:t& by the- in the ah (.) I'd show all

080 my emotions ((narrative continues))

Larry does not merely claim that he expressed emotion, he displays affect in recounting these events. This strong emotion gets sliown by the in- creased volume of his avovd (lines 073-074), the emphatic intonation in his reported imploring of Jenny, "why didn't ya ca:ll:?" - (line 078), and in his cut-off, exasperated reported complaint, ''you know I was 1vai:ting by the-" (lines 0'78-079). Direct reported spr~ech, of course, allows the speaker to use prosodic and paralinguistic features in recounting h o ~ ; something was said. These serve as contextualizatioc~ cues (Gumperz, 1982), which are consistent with and amplify the contextual framing of the reported speech.

Notice that Sacks's observation (quoted previously) is borne out in that the contextual framing components come immediately before or after the reported speech in both excerpts we have examined. This sequential

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 12: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

54 Richard Buttnv

contiguity (Sacks, 1987) between the contextual frames and reported speech has to do with recipient design considerations.

An exception to this sequential contiguixy thesis is that Larry's avowal of expressing emotion in these excerpts can be heard as a response to a dispute with Jenny some moments earlier aver whether Lany shows emotions to her (transcript lines 022-028 and also 045-049). Indeed this whole episode begins with the therapist asking Larry about Jenny's prior complaint that he is afraid to share his feelings (lines 001-007). This ongoing dispute over Larry's emotionaI expressiveness can be heard as a context for I . ' s avowals of emotion in his narrative.

,La-ry's insistence that he is expressive of emotion is uttered three times in his narrafive. The reported speech and reported action work as evidence or examples of his assertion. So Larry's framing statement of emotional expressiveness provides a context for how to take the reported speech and the reported speech provides instances in support of this framing statement,

In the literature on reported speech, Volosinov's (1971) caution on attending to the reporting context has net always been followed. In an otherwise excdlent study, HoIt (1996) explored the ways direct reported speech works as evidence but seemed to neglect the contextual framing of reported speech. For instance, Holt claimed.

Using [direct reported speech] enables the speaker to give an air of objectivity to the account. The reported speaker is, in a way, allowed to "speak for himself or herself." (p. 230)

What I take issue with is this assertion, aIbeit qudified, that direct reported speech dlows the reported speaker to "speak for himself or herself." Reported speech gets presented in some contextual frame. For instance, in the following transcript the man's reported speech (lines 15-17) is contexted by Lesley's immediately prior utterances (lines 10-1 1 and also lines 1-6). Also, Lesley's subsequent exasperated comment (line 26) works to contextually frame the reported speech.

(Hoit, 1996, pp. 229-230; Note: transcript is simplified)

[(Lesley is talking abo~tf visiting a sale at the vicarage with her husband.))

1 Lesley: ,And em: .p we (.) really didn't have a lot'v

2 ch2:nge that (.) day becuz we'd been to Bath 'n

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 13: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

Putting Prior Talk Into Context 55 --

3 we'd been: Chiistmas shopping (0.5) but we thought

4 we'd better go along t' th' sale 'n do what we could,

5 (0.2) we hadn't got a lot (.) of s:e- ready cash

6 t' spg:nd.

7 (0.3) 8 Lesley: .t hh

9 y e : rMh.=

10 Lesley: =In 9 case are thought th' things were very

11 expensive.

12 Joyce: Oh did you.

13 (0.9)

14 Lesley: uh we were looking rou-nd the stalls 'n

15 poking about 'n he came up t' me 'n said Oh:

16 hhello Lesley, (.) "stiJ trying to buy something

17 f'nothing,

18 ( ) .tch! 19 Joyce: .hh hahhhhhhl-I! 20 Leslie: -[A.hhohhh! - 21 (0.8)

22 Joyce: 00 :::: Lesley

23 Lesley: -["OO: -- ehh heh "heh

24 (0.2)

25 Joyce: I : sp7t A

26 Lesley: "Wha do y u "sa:y.

27 (0.3) 28 Joyce: Oh isn't hg drca:dful.

Holt's point was that the reported speech is presented (lines 15-17) by the reporting speaker. Lesley, to allow the recipient, Joyce, to evaluate It for herself (line 28). My position is that Lesley's prior and posterior statements contextually frame the reported speech to be heard in a certain way. One could imagine a different prior contextual framing such that the identical reported speech of lines 15-17 could be heard in a different way, say as a humorous teasing remark.

To conclude with a bit of reported speech on context that I want to contextually frame, Charles Antaki3 made the point that many social scientists treat context as a rather narrow range of factors (e.g., demo- graphic categories, personality traits, situational variables) considering the indefinitely vast number of things that people can do with each other

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 14: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

56 Richard Buttny

during social interaction. This observation is well taken; we need to consider the innumerable things we do with words and how we put those words in context. The conversational practice of reported speech takes a prior utterance situated in a particular context and unearths it and gives it a life again in the new soil of the reporting context. To fit into its new context, the reported speech often needs to be contextually framed so recipients understand it in the desired way. So Larry's contextual frame of eqressing emotions and the reporting context of Jenny's defending herself against the man's reported complaint. are the kinds of things that give context to talk.

NOTES

1 This familiar distinction between direct and indirect discourse is problematic, especially the notion of indirect speech, but it need not concern us here (see Sternberg, 1982, for a discussion and Payne, n.d., for a more adequate category scheme).

2 Tannen (1989) argued from this that the label reported speech is a misnomer; a better term is constructed dialogue. Although this point is well taken, nonetheless reported speech seems to be the entrenched term. More important. there seems to be a consensus in the literature that reported speech need not be a true reproduction of the original.

3 Comment made during the discussion at the Conference on Language and Social PsychoIogy, Ottawa, 1997 (see pp. 13-28, this issue).

REFERENCES

Auer, P. (1992). Introduction. In P. Auer & A. DiLuzio (Eds.), The co~ztextualimtion of language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic imagination (M. Holquist, Ed., C . Emerson & M. Holquist, Trans.). Austin: University of Texas Press.

Bergmann, J. R. (1992). Veiled morality: Notes on discretion in psychiatry. In P. Drew & J. Heritage (Eds.), Talk at work: Interaction in institutional settings (pp. 137-162). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Buttny, R. (1990). Blame-accounts sequences in therapy: The negotiation of relational meanings. Semiotics, 78, 21 9-247.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 15: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

Putting Prior Talk Into Context 57

Buttny, R. (1996). Clients' and therapist's joint construction of the clients' problems. Research on Language and sSocial Interaction, 29, 125-153.

Buttny, R. (1997). Reported speech in talking race on campus. Hunzan Communication Research, 23, 475-504.

Buttny, R., & Cohen, J. R. (1991). The uses of goals in therapy. In K. Tracy m.), Understanding face-to-face interaction: Issues linking goals and discourse (pp. 63-77), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Inc.

Bunny. R , & Jensen, A. D. (1995). Telling problems in an initial family therapy session: The hierarchical organization of problem-talk. In G. H. Morris & R. J. Chenail (Eds.), The talk ofthe clinic: Explorations in the aluzlysis ofrnec;'ical and therapeutic discourse (pp. 19-47). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Inc.

Drew, P., & Heritage, J. (Eds.). (1992a). Talk at work: Interaction in institutional settings. New York Cambridge Umversity Press.

Drew, P., 8: Heritage, J. (1992b). Analyzing talk at work: An introduction. In P. Drew & J. Heritage (Eds.), Talk at ~ w r k : Interaction in institutio~uzl settings (pp. 3-65). New York: Cambridge Uniwersity Press.

Drew, P., & Sorjonen, M. (1997). Institutional discourse. In T. A. van Dijk (Ed.), Discourse as social interaction (pp. 92-1 18). London: Sage.

Gaik, F. (1992). Radio talk-show therapy and the pragmatics of possible worlds. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (%is.), Rethinking context: Lanpuape as an inferactive phenonzenon (pp. 271-290). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Goodwin, C., & Duranti, A. (1992). Rethinking context: An introduction. In A. Duranti h C. Goodwin (Eds.), Reflzinking context (pp. 1-42). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gumperz, J. J. (1982). Discozrrse strategies. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Heritage, J. (1984). Garj5nkeE anrl ethnornethodology. Cambridge, England: Polity.

IHolt, E. (1996). Reporting on talk: The use of direct reported speech in conversation. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 29, 219-245.

ILabov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1963). Narrative analysis. In J. Helm @I.), Essays on verbal and visual arts (pp. 12-44). Seattle: University of Washington Press.

Mandelbaum, J. (1990191). Beyond mxndane reasoning: Conversation analysis and context. Research on Lunguage and Social Interaction, 24, 333-350.

Payne, &I. B. (n.d.). Spoken quotation: An analysis of quoted talk in conversation. Unpublished manuscript, State Uni~ersity of New York at Albany.

Perakyla, A., & Silverman, D. (1!291). Owning experience: Describing the experience of other persons. Text, 11, 441-480.

Sacks, H. (1987). On the preference for agreement and contiguity in sequences in conversation. In G. Button Ct J. R. E. Lee (Eds.), Talk and social organzsation (pp. 54-69). Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.

Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on co~zversation (Vol. 2, edited by G. Jefferson). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Sarbin, T. R. (1989). Presentation on narrative psychology. Joint Meeting of the Rocky Mountain and Western Psychological Associations, Reno, IVV.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014

Page 16: Putting Prior Talk Into Context: Reported Speech and the Reporting Context

53 Richard Buttny

Schegloff, E. A. (1991). Reflections on talk and social structure. In D. Boden & 13. H. Zirnmerman (Eds.j, Talk and social structme (pp. 44-70). Berkeley: University of California Pre$s.

Schegloff, E. A. (1947). Whose text? Whose context? Discourse & Society, 8, 165-187. Sternberg, M. (1982). Roteus in quotation-land: Mimesis and the forms of reported

discourse. Poetics Today, 3, 107-1 56.

Tannen. D. (19891. Ykiking voices. New York: Cambridge University Press. Tannen, D. ( I 990). Ygu just don't understand: Wornen and merz in conversation. New

York: Ballantine. Volosinov, V. N. (1971). Reported speech. In L. Matejka h K. Pomorska (Eds.), Readings

in Russian poetics (pp. 149-175). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Cen

tral

Mic

higa

n U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

47 0

5 N

ovem

ber

2014


Top Related