drawing disorders in alzheimer's disease

Upload: icaro

Post on 13-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    1/23

    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301592114

    Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Diseaseand Other Forms of Dementia

    Article in Journal of Alzheimer's disease: JAD April 2016

    Impact Factor: 4.15 DOI: 10.3233/JAD-160009

    READS

    19

    2 authors:

    Luigi Trojano

    Second University of Naples

    226PUBLICATIONS 2,867CITATIONS

    SEE PROFILE

    Guido Gainotti

    Catholic University of the Sacred Heart

    337PUBLICATIONS 9,884CITATIONS

    SEE PROFILE

    Available from: Luigi Trojano

    Retrieved on: 26 April 2016

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301592114_Drawing_Disorders_in_Alzheimer%27s_Disease_and_Other_Forms_of_Dementia?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_2https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301592114_Drawing_Disorders_in_Alzheimer%27s_Disease_and_Other_Forms_of_Dementia?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_2https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301592114_Drawing_Disorders_in_Alzheimer%27s_Disease_and_Other_Forms_of_Dementia?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_3https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301592114_Drawing_Disorders_in_Alzheimer%27s_Disease_and_Other_Forms_of_Dementia?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_3https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Luigi_Trojano?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_5https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Second_University_of_Naples?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_6https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Luigi_Trojano?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_7https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Guido_Gainotti?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_5https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Catholic_University_of_the_Sacred_Heart?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_6https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Guido_Gainotti?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_7https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Guido_Gainotti?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_7https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Catholic_University_of_the_Sacred_Heart?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_6https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Guido_Gainotti?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_5https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Guido_Gainotti?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_4https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Luigi_Trojano?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_7https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Second_University_of_Naples?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_6https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Luigi_Trojano?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_5https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Luigi_Trojano?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_4https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_1https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301592114_Drawing_Disorders_in_Alzheimer%27s_Disease_and_Other_Forms_of_Dementia?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_3https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301592114_Drawing_Disorders_in_Alzheimer%27s_Disease_and_Other_Forms_of_Dementia?enrichId=rgreq-6f6baea8-4e64-4b77-a198-380e0368005a&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwMTU5MjExNDtBUzozNTUwODUzMDY2MTM3NjFAMTQ2MTY3MDM0MjU5OQ%3D%3D&el=1_x_2
  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    2/23

    Journal of Alzheimers Disease xx (20xx) xxxDOI 10.3233/JAD-160009IOS Press

    1

    Review1

    Drawing Disorders in Alzheimers Diseaseand Other Forms of Dementia

    2

    3

    Luigi Trojanoa,b and Guido Gainottic,d4aDepartment of Psychology, Second University of Naples, Italy5bS. Maugeri Foundation, Scientific Institute of Telese Terme (BN), Italy6cCenter for Neuropsychological Research, Institute of Neurology, Catholic University, Rome, Italy7dIRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Department of Clinical and Behavioral Neurology, Rome, Italy8

    Handling Associate Editor: Jason Brandt9

    Accepted 16 March 2016

    Abstract. Drawing is a multicomponential process that can be impaired by many kinds of brain lesions. Drawing disorders

    are very common in Alzheimers disease and other forms of dementia, and can provide clinical information for the distinction

    of the different dementing diseases. In our review we started from an overview of the neural and cognitive bases of drawing,

    and from a recollection of the drawing tasks more frequently used for assessing individuals with dementia. Then, we analyzed

    drawing disorders in dementia, paying special attention to those observed in Alzheimers disease, from the prodromal stages

    of the amnesic mild cognitive impairment to the stages of full-blown dementia, both in the sporadic forms with late onsetin the entorhino-hippocampal structures and in those with early onset in the posterior neocortical structures. We reviewed

    the drawing features that could differentiate Alzheimers disease from vascular dementia and from the most frequent forms

    of degenerative dementia, namely frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body disease. Finally, we examined some peculiar

    aspects of drawing disorders in dementia, such as perseverations, rotations, and closing-in. We argue that a careful analysis

    of drawing errors helps to differentiate the different forms of dementia more than overall accuracy in drawing.

    10

    11

    12

    13

    14

    15

    16

    17

    18

    19

    20

    Keywords: Alzheimers disease, constructional apraxia, drawing disorders, frontotemporal dementia, Lewy body disease,

    vascular dementia

    21

    22

    INTRODUCTION23

    Different kinds of drawing disorders can be found24

    in Alzheimers disease (AD) and other forms of25

    dementia. Investigations conducted in patients with26

    focal brain lesions andwith differentforms of demen-27

    tia have shown that many of these disorders can28

    be traced back to visual-spatial [15] or planning29

    disturbances [610]. These disorders are the main30

    determinants of constructional apraxia (CA) and are

    Correspondence to: Luigi Trojano, Dept. of Psychology, Sec-

    ond University of Naples, Viale Ellittico 31, 81100 Caserta, Italy.Tel./Fax: +39 0823 274784; E-mail:[email protected].

    subsumed by lesions affecting the parietal [1114] 31

    and frontal regions of the brain [15]. They are usu- 32

    ally observed in copying tasks, whereas other forms 33

    of drawing disorders, mainly due to disruption of (or 34

    impaired access to) the pictorial representations of 35

    objects, can be observed on tasks of drawing from 36

    memory [1619]. The relationships between clini- 37

    cal forms of dementia and the corresponding patterns 38

    of drawing disabilities stem, therefore, from the fact 39

    that (at least in the early stages of the disease) the 40

    brain pathology affects different brain networks pref- 41

    erentially. Furthermore, the need for simple tasks 42with rich informational content in the assessment of 43

    ISSN 1387-2877/16/$35.00 2016 IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    3/23

    2 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    dementia prompted the implementation of drawing44

    tasks, such as the clock drawing test (CDT) [2022],45

    in place of those classically used for assessing CA in46

    patients with focal brain lesions. The present review47

    will therefore include several sections. The first will48

    illustrate the neural and cognitive bases of draw-49

    ing. The second will focus on the drawing tasks50

    more frequently used in dementia. In the third sec-51

    tion we will pass to analyze drawing disorders in52

    AD, from the prodromal stages of the amnesic mild53

    cognitive impairment (aMCI) to the stages of full-54

    blown dementia, both in the sporadic forms with late55

    onset in the entorhino-hippocampal structures and in56

    those with early onset in the posterior neocortical57

    structures. In thenextsection we will consider thefea-58

    tures of drawing disorders that could differentiate AD59

    from vascular dementia and from the most frequent60forms of degenerative dementia, namely frontotem-61

    poral dementia (FTD) and the Lewy body disease62

    (LBD). In the last section we will take into account63

    some peculiar aspects of drawing disorders in demen-64

    tia, paying particular attention to the closing-in65

    phenomenon [2325].66

    Drawing disorders in dementia are a field that is67

    attracting an ever-growing interest. A 2005-2015 lit-68

    erature search limited to Pubmed database and to69

    papers in English language, with the terms [drawing70

    OR copying OR constructional] AND dementia71

    as keywords, identified 793 papers. Faced with this72

    huge amount of studies, we realized that our work73

    could not be exhaustive but wanted to provide a74

    comprehensive review including the most relevant75

    clinical aspects of drawing disorders.76

    NEURAL AND COGNITIVE BASES OF77

    DRAWING78

    Poppelreuter [26] observed that some brain dam-79

    aged patients may be impaired in a series of activities80

    requiring a careful control of vision on action, such81

    as drawing, but Kleist [27] was the first to propose the82

    term CA to designate a specific disturbance which83

    appears in formative activities (such as assembling,84

    building, or drawing) in which the spatial form of85

    the task is missed, although there is no apraxia of86

    the single movements. In subsequent years, drawing87

    became progressively more popular in neuropsycho-88

    logical assessment, and a lot of studies have been89

    aimed at comprehending its neural and cognitive90

    bases in brain-damaged patients and in healthy sub-91jects [22, 28].92

    Studies on focal brain-damaged patients have often 93

    provided contrasting evidence about the lateralization 94

    and the intra-hemispheric locus of lesions provoking 95

    drawing disabilities. Since 1962 Benton [29] under- 96

    lined that the subjective nature of the clinical method, 97

    and the large variability of the tasks used to study 98

    constructive disorders could account for the incon- 99

    sistencies found in previous literature. Benton thus 100

    suggested using gradedtests with precise scoring pro- 101

    cedures to improve the comparability of results [29]. 102

    The ensuing authors adopted this pivotal standpoint 103

    in trying to comprehend the neural basis of drawing 104

    through the analysis of performance in focal brain 105

    damaged patients. However, many studies did not 106

    detect differencesin prevalenceor severityof drawing 107

    disorders after right or left hemispheric lesions (e.g., 108

    [2, 6, 3032]). Then, prompted by a first systematic 109survey about the relationships between visuocon- 110

    structive disabilities and hemispheric locus of lesion 111

    [12], several authors attempted to ascertain whether 112

    qualitative differences existed between the drawing 113

    disorders resulting from right or left hemispheric 114

    lesions. On one hand, correlational studies demon- 115

    strated that drawing disabilities are tightly related 116

    with scores on visual-perceptual tasks in right but 117

    not in left brain-damaged patients, e.g., [3, 4, 18, 33]. 118

    On the other hand, drawing disorders in left-brain 119

    damaged patients have been ascribed to a planning 120

    disorder [6, 7], but this interpretation has not been 121

    supported by several empirical studies [8, 9, 17, 18, 122

    33]. In one of the most recent systematic attempts at 123

    identifying the neural structures involved in copy- 124

    ing a complex figure, in a wide sample of focal 125

    brain-damaged patients, voxel-based morphometry 126

    revealed that different lesions in the two hemispheres 127

    were significantly correlated with different aspects of 128

    the drawing production [34]. 129

    Rather inconclusive results on the neural bases of 130

    drawinginhealthyindividualshavealsobeenobtained 131

    by functional neuroimaging studies, because the lat- 132

    ter have been constrained by the relevant artifacts 133

    induced by the hand and arm movements, and have 134

    oftenassesseddrawing-relatedtasksratherthanactual 135

    drawing [14, 3538]. Only very recently the devel- 136

    opment of functional MRI-compatible graphic tablets 137

    [39] allowed the analysis of hand movements during 138

    drawing, and one recent study using such a device 139

    reported activation within a wide network extend- 140

    ing from the temporo-occipital to parietal and lateral 141

    frontal areas bilaterally in healthy participants while 142

    they were drawing faces or abstract patterns with dif- 143ferent levels of visual details [40]. 144

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    4/23

    L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia 3

    Neuropsychological and functional neuroimaging145

    data are thus consistent with the view that the process146

    of drawing involves different cognitive components.147

    Several authors tried to single out these components148

    within comprehensive models [4143]. All these149

    models share the idea that visuospatial processes,150

    dedicated planning abilities, and general control pro-151

    cesses are involved in drawing [44], although the152

    models differ from each other in terms of formal153

    characteristics, depth of analysis and sometheoretical154

    aspects. It is worth mentioning that Grossi [43], draw-155

    ing on the distinction between lexical and sub-lexical156

    components of language, proposed the existence of157

    two copying procedures: a lexical route and a line-158

    by-line procedure. The former would consist of159

    motor subroutines, which could be considered as part160

    of a constructional lexicon, could be used to draw161well-known figures (such as a square or a face), and162

    woulddevelop as a resultof formaleducation andper-163

    sonal aptitudes [4546]. The latter procedure would164

    not rely on previously acquired constructional repre-165

    sentations, could be used for copying novel stimuli,166

    and would be only based on a piecemeal spatial anal-167

    ysis. Both procedures could be adopted for copying168

    complex pictures, but some patients might be con-169

    strained to use either one or the other. For instance,170

    patients with visual agnosia tend to adopt a slavish171

    line-by-line copying procedure for familiar objects172

    they cannot recognize [4748]. On the contrary some173

    demented patients can draw simple figures success-174

    fully but fail at integrating correctly shaped simple175

    elements in a coherent whole, because of planning or176

    visuospatial defects.177

    DRAWING TASKS FREQUENTLY USED IN178

    DEMENTIA179

    Drawing tasks have been widely used in patients180

    with dementia, but drawing performance may vary181

    greatly, as a function of the task (copying and free182

    drawing cannot be considered equivalent), of the183

    stimuli used in the task (more complex stimuli pose184

    greater load on visuospatial and planning functions),185

    and even of the patients pre-morbid abilities (largely186

    dependent on age,educational level, and even cultural187

    background) [49].188

    Free drawing, in which the patient is required to189

    draw a named object (e.g., a clock, a face, an ani-190

    mal, or a tool), can reveal information about the191

    patients ability to draw complete shapes or a ten-192dency to omit parts and about their ability to organize193

    the figure as a whole, with its component elements in 194

    the correct spatial relationships. However, this task 195

    also relies on non-constructional cognitive abilities, 196

    particularly on lexical-semantic knowledge, pictorial 197

    representations, and mental imagery [16, 50, 51]. 198

    A clear instance of the complexity of free draw- 199

    ing tasks is CDT which is widely used in the first 200

    steps of detecting cognitive impairment and dementia 201

    (Fig. 1), as a stand-alone neuropsychological test or 202

    Fig. 1. Drawing a clock face on an empty circle, and putting thehands at 2 : 45. Spatial distortions and errors reflecting impairedsemantic knowledge in drawings by patients with Alzheimers

    disease (first row). Planning errors, perseverations, and stimulus-bound responses in drawings by patients with severe forms of

    vascular dementia (second row) and of behavioral vascular demen-tia (second row) or behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia(third row). Gross spatial distortions and perseverations in draw-

    ings by patients with Lewy body disease (bottom row). Oftenpatients with dementia are not able to draw the clock hands at

    the given time, and some write numbers instead of drawings thehands.

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    5/23

    4 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    included into composite screening batteries [20, 21,203

    52]. In its classical form [53], CDT requires patients204

    to draw the face of a clock with all the numbers205

    and set the two hands to 10 after 11. Many admin-206

    istration procedures and scoring systems have been207

    proposed [5457], but there is no consensus as to208

    which is the most useful for dementia screening [20,209

    54]. The scoring systems share commonalities, but210

    each may reflect different cognitive components and211

    be correlated with different areas of brain atrophy212

    [58]. However, independently from the scoring pro-213

    cedure, performance on CDT in demented patients214

    seems to be inversely correlated with lesion load in215

    medial temporal lobe, in subcortical structures and in216

    periventricular white matter [59, 60] and might pro-217

    vide prognostic information for cognitive decline [61,218

    62]. Some authors suggested that qualitative analysis219of errors in clock drawing might improve diagnos-220

    tic accuracy for dementia screening. For instance,221

    errors such as inaccurate time setting, missing hands222

    or numbers, and number substitutions or repetitions223

    might be particularly useful for dementia screening224

    [63]. Using appropriate error classification criteria,225

    error analysis in CDT might be useful to differen-226

    tiate different types of dementia, since conceptual227

    errors are quite frequent in AD [64], but in a recent228

    longitudinal study conceptual errors and persevera-229

    tions were more frequently observed in the advanced230

    stages of all dementing diseases, whereas spatial231

    and planning errors were more frequent in mild-to-232

    moderate dementia [65]. Errors in setting numbers233

    and clock hands seem to correlate with regional234

    hypometabolism in bilateral parietal and posterior235

    temporal areas and in the right middle frontal gyrus236

    [58]. By the same token, it has been recently observed237

    that different types of errors in demented patients238

    performance on CDT might correlate with atrophy239

    in different brain regions, within frontal, parietal and240

    temporal lobes [66].241

    One strategy for improving diagnostic accuracy of242

    CDT is to compare the drawing to command con-243

    dition with a copy condition, in which subjects are244

    required to copy a pre-drawn clock, since in the copy245

    condition performance would rely only on a subset of246

    cognitive functions centered on visuoperceptual and247

    constructional skills [16]. However, although copy-248

    ing seems to directly assess the patients ability to249

    reproduce a figure, even this task can imply prob-250

    lem solving and executive abilities and is affected by251

    age, educational level, and even cultural background252

    [49]. In assessing copying abilities, it is therefore253necessary to adopt standardized tasks, with solid254

    Fig. 2. Copying of geometrical drawings: cube (modelon thetop).

    Gross simplifications, spatial distortions, errors of perspective inpatients withAlzheimers disease (firstrow) and vascular dementia(second row). Simplifications, distortions and perseverations in

    patients with moderate to severe frontotemporal dementia (thirdrow). The bottom figures show instances of near closing-in (on the

    left) and of adherent closing-in (on the right).

    normative data, and to use different kinds of stim- 255

    uli, well-known or novel, of graded complexity from 256

    simpleshapes, such as circles andsquares, to complex 257

    figures, such as a cube (Fig. 2), two intersecting pen- 258

    tagons (Fig.3), or the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure 259

    (ROCF) [67, 68]. In particular, the copy of the ROCF 260

    (Fig. 4) is often used in the assessment of drawing 261

    abilities in individuals with dementia, and provides 262

    an opportunity to assess copying procedures together 263

    with copying accuracy [67]. The copy of the ROCF 264

    has high sensitivity for detecting brain damage, since 265it involves a wide network of brain areas, including 266

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    6/23

    L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia 5

    Fig. 3. Copying of geometrical drawings: interlocking pentagons (model on the top). Simplifications and distortions in drawings by patientswith Alzheimers disease (first row); perseverations, simplifications and spatial distortions in patients with vascular dementia (second row);

    simplification, distortions and planning errors in patients with moderate to severe frontotemporal dementia (third row). The bottom figuresshow instances of near closing-in (on the left) and of adherent closing-in (on the right).

    frontal, superior temporal, posterior parietal and mid-267

    dle occipital cortex, more extensively in the right268

    hemisphere [69]. Several neurofunctional studies in269

    dementia showed that accuracy in copying the ROCF270

    is related to metabolic rate in bilateral temporal-271

    parietal cortex and occipital lobe, and in right frontal272

    lobe, whereas the procedure used to copy, namely the273

    tendency to draw first the main organizational lines of274

    the model, correlated with metabolism in right lateral275

    temporal cortex [70]. It is worth mentioning, how-276

    ever, that neurofunctional studies comparing copying277

    of different kinds of stimuli in dementia demon-278

    strated that the neural correlates of copying tasks, 279

    mainly centered in the posterior brain regions, differ 280

    as a function of task complexity [71]. Moreover, it 281

    appears advisable to complement evaluation of accu- 282

    racy in copying tasks with a qualitative analysis of 283

    the patients production, which can provide further 284

    relevant information. This fact has been underlined 285

    above with respect to the CDT, and is also sug- 286

    gested by recent studies showing that the copy of 287

    two intersecting pentagons (Fig. 3), included in the 288

    Mini-Mental State Examination [72], may provide 289

    elements for differential diagnosis of degenerative 290

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    7/23

    6 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    Fig. 4. Copying of Rey complex figure (model on the top). Gross spatial distortions and simplifications in patients with Alzheimers disease(firstrow); planning errors, spatial distortions, and perseveration in patientswith vascular dementia(second row)and frontotemporaldementia(lowest row). In the left sided column are reported drawings of patients with mild forms and in the right-sided columns those of patients

    with severe forms of dementia. Note in the right top figure that the reproduction of the single subcomponents is relatively spared, but spatialrelationships among them are lost; in the right bottom figure an instance of rotation.

    dementias [73, 74]. Although error analysis in draw-291

    ing tasks may not reveal a straightforward procedure292

    [22, 28, 44], several clinical phenomena observed in293

    drawing have been sufficiently characterized in their294possible cognitive and neural correlates, as it will be295

    discussed in the next sections of this paper.296

    It is important to stress, however, that in the diag-297

    nostic work-up for dementia performance on drawing298

    tasks, and qualitative error analysis should be com-299

    plemented by assessment of the deficits that can300

    contribute to constructional apraxia. As it will be301

    discussed later, mechanisms inducing errors in spon-302

    taneous drawing or in copying may encompass basic303

    deficits in visuoperceptual abilities, spatial attention,304

    spatialworkingmemoryorspatialplanning,andexec-305

    utive function [75]. Particularly crucial in this respect306

    seem to be visuospatial perception whose different 307

    aspectscanbeimpairedindementia[44,7578].Visu- 308

    ospatial perception is often assessed by tests such as 309

    Bentons Judgement of Line Orientation, in which 310subjects are required to identify the lines that have 311

    the same angulation as those presented as stimuli 312

    [79], or by comprehensive test batteriesassessing sev- 313

    eral aspects of visuoperceptual processing, such as 314

    Warrington and James Visual Object and Space Per- 315

    ception Battery [80], or the Battery for Visuospatial 316

    Abilities[4,81].Themechanismsunderlyingdrawing 317

    disorders can also be clarified by means of plan- 318

    ning/executive tests, such as Trail Making test [82] or 319

    tests for figural fluency [83], and other tests that have 320

    been used to assess patients with focal lesions. Motor 321

    programming deficits have also to be considered, 322

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    8/23

    L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia 7

    because patients with dementia may be affected by323

    limb apraxia or optic ataxia with a strong impact on324

    drawing.Therefore,thesetypesoftestshavetobeused325

    to evaluate the different forms and the different stages326

    of dementia. To establish an algorithm for assessing327

    the different abilities involved in the drawing pro-328

    cess and the disorders than can cause constructional329

    apraxia might provide a powerful tool to understand330

    the genesis of drawing disorders and of the different331

    types of errors in dementia.332

    DRAWING DISORDERS IN ALZHEIMERS333

    DISEASE334

    The observation that many brain regions in both335

    hemispheres are involvedin different aspects of draw-336

    ing, and by implication of constructional tasks, can337

    provide an explanation of the classical findings sug-338

    gesting that CA is related to poor intellectual abilities339

    in focal brain damaged patients [30], and can repre-340

    sent an index for diffuse cognitive deterioration, after341

    lesions in either left [84] or right hemisphere [85].342

    According to old studies, in AD drawing disorders343

    are present since early stages of the disease, and their344

    severity increases as the illness progresses [22, 28].345

    However, more recent investigations have shown that346

    in the early stages of AD simple models can be eas-347

    ily copied and that drawing disabilities are observed348

    only with rather complex tasks [86, 87].349

    Several variables must therefore be taken into350

    account in the study of drawing disorders in AD.351

    Among these variables we will separately consider:352

    (1) the drawing tasks used in AD and the corre-353

    sponding kinds of errors; (2) the drawing disorders354

    in prodromic (aMCI) and clinical forms of AD; (3)355

    the drawing disabilities in early and late onset forms356

    of AD; (4) the drawing disorders in different stages357

    of evolution of AD; (5) the mechanisms giving rise358

    to drawing disorders in AD patients.359

    The drawing tasks used in AD and the360

    corresponding kinds of errors361

    An attempt at a systematic description of AD362

    patients errors in a free drawing task [9] has shown363

    frequent occurrence of simplifications, spatial alter-364

    ations, and lack of perspective. However, since free365

    drawing poses a heavy load on semantic memory,366

    errors on this task (e.g., simplifications in drawing a367

    house) may derive from impaired access to semantic368

    knowledge or to impaired visuoperceptual process-369ing [51]. For this reason, spontaneous drawing could370

    be impaired in the early stages of AD, while copy- 371

    ing may deteriorate later [88]. The reproduction of 372

    complex figures is particularly sensitive to the pro- 373

    gression of the disease [89]. For instance, the ROCF 374

    may be reproduced in a simplified way, with single 375

    constitutive elements put one after the other, even in 376

    early AD (see Fig. 4). In these cases, patients seem 377

    able to recognize and reproduce single well-known 378

    elements but are unable to reproduce complex spa- 379

    tial relationships correctly. Another simplification 380

    error may consist in the reproduction of more familiar 381

    or simpler figures instead of more complex ones (e.g., 382

    a square instead of a diamond). As the disease pro- 383

    gresses, patients usually become unable even to draw 384

    simple figures correctly, as they no longer had access 385

    to well-consolidated motor subroutines and can show 386

    peculiar patterns of behavior, such as the closing-in 387phenomenon [2325, 90, 91] (see Figs. 2 and 3). 388

    Drawing disorders in prodromic (aMCI) and 389

    clinical forms of AD 390

    Drawing disorders are not usually present in MCI, 391

    at least when copying of simple figures is employed 392

    [92]. However, a different pattern might emerge as a 393

    function of clinical characteristics of MCI patients, 394

    particularly if more complex tasks are used. In a 395

    study on brain morphometric correlates of CDT 396

    in patients with MCI or AD, Thomann et al. [86] 397

    observed that, even though MCI patients did not dif- 398

    fer from matched healthy controls in copying simple 399

    geometrical drawings, CDT could discriminate MCI 400

    from matched healthy controls and AD from MCI 401

    patients. Analogously, in a further brain morpho- 402

    metric study on MCI and AD patients, accuracy in 403

    copying the ROCF was significantly different in MCI 404

    patients, AD patients, and healthy controls [87]. It 405

    has also been observed that MCI patients make con- 406

    ceptual and graphic errors more often than matched 407

    healthy controls, and that AD patients score lower 408

    and make significantly more conceptual, graphic, and 409

    spatial-planning mistakes than MCI individuals, thus 410

    suggesting that a detailed scoring system is necessary 411

    to differentiate individuals with MCI from healthy 412

    adults [63, 93]. Scoring systems focusing on hand 413

    and number placement might better differentiate MCI 414

    individuals from healthy controls, but the solidity of 415

    CDT as a screening tool for MCI has been ques- 416

    tioned [94]. In this respect, it should be taken into 417

    account that cognitive characteristics of MCI individ- 418

    uals might correlate with performance on CDT. It is 419very important to acknowledge at this point that MCI 420

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    9/23

    8 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    patients are rather heterogeneous from the etiological421

    point of view andthat amnesic MCI canbe considered422

    as that most related to development of AD [95, 96].423

    In line with this caveat is the fact that, a recent study424

    contrasting MCI of the amnesic, dysexecutive, or425

    multi-domain type on CDT [97] confirmed that aMCI426

    did not differ from a matched sample of healthy con-427

    trols, whereas both dysexecutive and multi-domain428

    MCI individuals made a significantly larger number429

    of errors in hand and number placement.430

    Poor performance on CDT might predict future431

    development of AD in patients with MCI [98], but432

    not all qualitative scoring procedures might be suf-433

    ficiently sensitive to capture longitudinal changes434

    of CDT from MCI to AD [61]. A recent meta-435

    analysis [99] confirmed that the CDT might reveal436

    a useful measure of cognitive decline over time, and437that conceptual clock drawing errors (with particu-438

    lar mention of hand and number placement) would439

    be most informative about the cognitive decline.440

    This meta-analysis also showed that CDT might441

    differentiate individuals who are going to develop442

    dementia in subsequent years, thus being included443

    among the tests helpful for predicting conversion to444

    dementia.445

    Drawing disabilities in early and late onset446

    forms of AD447

    The structural or functional neuroimaging abnor-448

    malities correlate with the clinical features in AD449

    patients, because typical late-onset patients (LOAD)450

    present with memory disorders and medial tempo-451

    ral lobe atrophy, whereas focal neocortical forms452

    of AD prevail in early-onset patients (EOAD) and453

    are associated with executive-behavioral disorders,454

    logopenic aphasia, and (in posterior cortical atrophy)455

    with progressive visual-spatial disorders [100, 101].456

    Some authors have, therefore, examined the relations457

    between age at onset and drawing disorders in AD458

    patients, and reported a high prevalence of visuocon-459

    structional disabilities, assessed with the copy of the460

    ROCF, in EOAD patients [101103]. In some stud-461

    ies [102, 103], but not in others [101], these drawing462

    disorders were associated with visuospatial deficits.463

    Furthermore, some studies [104] have shown that464

    the presence and severity of drawing disorders cer-465

    tainly depends on the AD phenotype and not just on466

    age. Most frequent and severe drawing disorders are467

    found in posterior cortical atrophy (PCA), which is468

    associated with prominent visuospatial impairments469and relative preservation of memory, insight, and470

    judgment [105]. PCA is associated with atrophy in 471

    the occipital, parietal, and posterior temporal lobes 472

    [106], but recently a lesion of long white-matter 473

    tracts, including the superior and inferior longitudinal 474

    fasciculi and the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, 475

    has also been documented [107]. This early-onset 476

    AD-related pathologygives rise to complex visuospa- 477

    tial difficulties, such as visuoperceptual impairments 478

    [108], optic ataxia, simultanagnosia, gaze apraxia 479

    (often with the complete spectrum of Balint syn- 480

    drome), and even egocentric [109, 110] or allocentric 481

    [111] unilateral spatial neglect. All such visuospa- 482

    tial impairments have a strong impact on drawing 483

    abilities, affecting both spontaneous drawing and 484

    copying, and giving rise to severe displacements of 485

    drawing elements, omissions, gross spatial distor- 486

    tions (exploded drawings) [112]. 487

    Drawing disorders in different stages of 488

    evolution of AD 489

    A strictrelation between the progression of visuop- 490

    erceptual spatial impairment and the progressive 491

    deterioration of the performance on copying the 492

    ROCF has been reported [89] (see Fig. 4). Mild AD 493

    usually spares basic visual sensory processes and 494

    affects all high-order visual processes, along both the 495

    ventral (occipito-temporal) and the dorsal (occipito- 496

    parietal) visual streams [113]. Caine and Hodges 497

    [114] carried out two separated studies to investigate 498

    ventral and dorsal visual functions in a sample of 499

    AD patients at early stages of the disease. A small 500

    subsample of patients were impaired on visuospatial 501

    tasks, thus suggesting that this small group of patients 502

    with prominent visuospatial disorders might repre- 503

    sent one end of a continuous spectrum at the other 504

    end of which are patients affected by a focal degener- 505

    ative dementia involving occipito-parietal cortex, the 506

    so-called PCA [115118]. In the moderate stages of 507

    AD, visuospatial impairments would become more 508

    evident, and play a relevant role in the develop- 509

    ment of constructional disorders [89]. More recently, 510

    Guerin et al. [119] examined the cognitive mech- 511

    anisms underlying the constructional performance 512

    of AD patients at different stages of the disease by 513

    means of a copying task and of visuospatial tasks 514

    measuring spatial exploration (visual search tasks), 515

    judgment of spatial relations, and planning abilities. 516

    The results suggested that cognitive defects underly- 517

    ing constructional impairment in patients with AD 518

    involved the early phases of spatial-constructional 519processing, likely exploration and judgment of 520

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    10/23

    L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia 9

    spatial relationships, rather than the late stage of521

    planning.522

    Mechanisms giving rise to drawing disorders in523

    AD patients524

    The drawing impairment in AD patients may stem525

    from different cognitive mechanisms.526

    In recent years, the different mechanisms giving527

    rise to CA in AD patients have been tackled with by528

    means of morphometric and neurofunctional studies.529

    However, such studies did not provide strongly con-530

    vergent data. For instance, in a sample of AD and531

    MCI patients overall score on CDT was inversely532

    correlated with atrophy in the middle and superior533

    temporal gyri (BA 21 and 22) bilaterally, but more534

    strongly in the left hemisphere, and in the left entorhi-535nal area (BA 28) [86], whereas in a sample of AD536

    patients impaired performance on the ROCF copying537

    task was correlated with atrophy in several fronto-538

    temporo-parietal regionsin the right hemisphere [87].539

    In a further study [5], impaired performance on a540

    copying test was correlated with bilateral parietal541

    atrophy. The differences between such studies might542

    originate from different characteristic of the AD sam-543

    ples, and from the neuropsychological measure used544

    for assessing drawing. Severalrecent neurofunctional545

    studies are illustrative of these sources of variabil-546

    ity. In a large sample of AD patients, Shon et al.547

    [120] found that drawing from memory or copy-548

    ing a clock face correlated with regional glucose549

    metabolism in bilateral temporo-parietal regions, but550

    these correlations changed as a function of dementia551

    severity. Matsuoka et al. [58] reported another rep-552

    resentative instance of variability in neurofunctional553

    findings depending on the neuropsychological mea-554

    sure in a large sample of drug-nave AD patients,555

    in whom overall performance on CDT, the partial556

    score on locating numbers of the clock and the557

    partial score on setting the hands were correlated558

    with atrophy in distinct parietal, posterior tempo-559

    ral and frontal regions. A PET study using copying560

    of simple geometrical figure as a measure of con-561

    structional abilities showed, instead, an exclusive562

    correlation of copying performance with the left infe-563

    rior parietal lobule and the left inferior frontal gyrus564

    [121].565

    These data explain why the diffuse involvement of566

    several areas of the brain in AD can impair draw-567

    ing performance since the early clinical stages of568

    the diseases, particularly when drawing is assessed569by means of complex stimuli. Therefore, drawing is570

    characteristically affected in AD, as well as visuospa- 571

    tial cognition in general [76]. 572

    DRAWING DISORDERS IN THE 573

    DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS BETWEEN 574

    AD AND VASCULAR DEMENTIA OR 575

    OTHER DEGENERATIVE FORMS OF 576

    DEMENTIA 577

    The widespread involvement of several neural net- 578

    works in drawing can explain the clinical observation 579

    of frequent drawing disorders in AD, but also implies 580

    that drawing disorders are often found in other forms 581

    of dementia, bothof vascularand degenerative nature. 582

    In recentyearsa growingnumber of studies attempted 583

    to ascertain whether drawing could provide addi- 584

    tional information for refining clinical differentiation 585

    among dementias. Here we will provide an overview 586

    of such studies, from which it will emerge that it can 587

    be difficult to disentangle drawing disorders in AD 588

    from those shown by patients affected by other kinds 589

    of progressive dementias, and that some distinctive 590

    features might be captured by qualitative analysis 591

    of drawing productions (see Table 1). As it will be 592

    argued in the next section, it is possible, however, 593

    that if patients with different etiologies share some 594

    neural lesions or cognitive defect, they will produce 595

    similar error types in drawing. 596

    Drawing disorders in vascular dementia 597

    Since AD and vascular dementia (VaD) are the 598

    most common forms of dementia in old age, several 599

    investigations tried to evaluate if drawing disor- 600

    ders can be useful to differentiate the two forms 601

    of dementia. In particular, a few studies compared 602

    drawing abilities in AD and VaD patients on the 603

    CDT, without providing convergent evidence. Looi 604

    and Sachdev [122] reviewed 11 studies comparing 605

    the performance of VaD and AD patients in the area 606

    of constructional apraxia, using tests such as block 607

    design, CDT, or the ROCF copying task. Eight of 608

    the studies showed no difference between the two 609

    groups, while in three it was found that the AD 610

    were less impaired. Wiechmann et al. [123] did not 611

    find difference in accuracy on CDT between AD 612

    and VaD. Instead, in a study on a sample of outpa- 613

    tients matched for general cognitive impairment VaD 614

    scored significantly lower than AD on CDT, likely 615

    related to poorer executive abilities in VaD patients 616

    [124]. Kitabayashi et al. [125] stratified their patient 617sample by general cognitive impairment, and found 618

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    11/23

    10 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    Table 1

    Summary of studies comparing drawing disorders in Alzheimers Disease and in other forms of dementia

    First author Drawing test Findings

    Comparison AD-VaD

    Wiechmann [123] CDT No difference in accuracy

    Kitabayashi [125] CDT Conceptual and spatial/planning errors in AD in all disease stages;conceptual and graphic errors in moderate-severe VaDFukui [126] CDT, figure copying No difference at mild disease stages; sVaD worse

    than AD on both tasks at severe stagesGraham [127] ROCF No difference between AD and sVaDComparison AD-FTD

    Rascovsky [131] CDT, figure copying AD worse than FTD on overall CDT score; no differences in figure copyingBlair [132] CDT AD worse than FTD; less conceptual, spatial

    and planning errors in FTD than in ADRazani [133] CDT AD worse than right- and left-onset FTD

    Thompson [134] Figure copying AD worse than FTD; less spatial errors,but more perseverations in FTD than in AD

    Possin [135] Simplified ROCF AD worse than FTD

    Perri [136] ROCF AD worse than FTDGorno-Tempini [137] ROCF AD worse than non fluent PA

    Grossi [138] ROCF No difference between AD and bvFTDGasparini [139] ROCF No overall difference between AD and bvFTD;

    AD better than bvFTD in drawing detailsRascovsky [141] Figure copying Steeper decline of copying abilities in FTD with respect to ADMendez [142] Figure copying No 2-year decline in FTDComparison AD-LBD

    Nervi [152] Figure copying LBD worse than AD

    Crowell [153] Figure copying LBD worse than ADTiraboschi [154] Figure copying LBD worse than AD; no differences in pentagon copyingCahn-Wiener [156] CDT No differences in overall score, but more conceptual and planning errors in LBD

    Hamilton [157] Clock copying No differences, but lower scores predicted steeper decline in LBDCagnin [158] CDT LBD worse than AD

    Palmqvist [159] CDT, cube copying LBD worse than ADGnanalingham [162] CDT, clock copying No difference in CDT; LBD worse than AD in clock copying

    Ala [164] Pentagon copying LBD worse than ADCormack [165] Pentagon copying LBD worse than ADConnor [166] Pentagon copying No difference

    Caffarra [167] Figure copying, No difference in figure copying,pentagon copying worse scores in LBD on an analytic scoring system for pentagon copying

    AD, Alzheimers disease; VaD, vascular dementia; sVaD, subcortical VaD; FTD, frontotemporal dementia; bvFTD, behavioral variant of

    FTD; PA, progressive aphasia; LBD, Lewy body dementia; CDT, Clock drawing test; ROCF, copy of Reys complex figure. Reference

    numbering follows the text.

    that CDT can well discriminate patients with AD or619

    VaD from healthy controls, but the main differences620

    between AD and VaD patients were in the relative621

    percentage of error types, with AD patients showing622

    many conceptual and spatial/planning errors inde-623

    pendently from disease severity and VaD patients624

    showing conceptual errors and graphic difficulties625

    only in presence of moderate cognitive impairment626

    (see Fig. 1). Partially different findings have been627

    reported by Fukui et al. [126], who did not find dif-628

    ferences between AD and subcortical VaD patients629

    at mild disease stages on CDT and figure copying,630

    whereas at later stages VaD achieved significantly631

    lower scores than AD in both drawing tasks. Graham632

    et al. [127] also found that patients with subcortical633

    VaD were more impaired than AD patients on exec-634

    utive/attentional functioning, and visuospatial and635

    perceptual skills. In their recent review on CDT, Tan 636

    et al. [128] concluded that the overall accuracy on 637

    CDT in AD versus VaD patients did not provide 638

    consistent results, whereas qualitative analyses might 639

    reveal useful differences between the two groups of 640

    patients, since VaD patients tended to show more fre- 641

    quent executive/planning or perseveration errors than 642

    AD patients. 643

    Drawing disorders in frontotemporal dementia 644

    The relative preservation of visuospatial and 645

    constructional abilities is a feature capable of dis- 646

    tinguishing FTD from other degenerative dementias 647

    and, notably, AD [129, 130]. Consistent with this 648

    idea, a few studies observed better performance on 649CDT in FTD compared with AD. For instance, in 650

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    12/23

    L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia 11

    a sample of autopsy-verified FTD patients, Rascov-651

    sky et al. [131] observed similar scores in copying652

    geometrical figures, but higher overall CDT scores653

    in FTD compared to AD patients. In the same vein,654

    Blair et al. [132] reported higher overall scores, and655

    fewer conceptual, spatial, and planning errors in FTD656

    compared to the AD group (see Fig. 1). Compar-657

    ing the performance of AD patients with those of658

    patients with right and left FTD, Razani et al. [133]659

    also confirmed that the AD group displayed sig-660

    nificant impairments in visual-constructional ability661

    relative to the two FTD groups. On the other hand,662

    Thompson et al. [134] observed that FTD patients663

    outperformed AD patients in copying geometrical664

    figures of different complexity showing a lower num-665

    ber of spatial errors, but were also characterized by666

    a higher number of perseverative errors and of poor667organization of the copy than AD patients. Thus spa-668

    tial errors were highly predictive of AD, whereas669

    organizational errors, perseverations, or overelabo-670

    rated copies were significant predictors of FTD (see671

    Figs. 24). Data supporting the hypothesis of a preva-672

    lence of spatial errors in the drawings of AD patients673

    and of executive disorders in those of FTD patients674

    have also been obtained in a study by Possin et al.675

    [135], that used quantitative morphometric evalua-676

    tion of structural MRI. These authors reported that677

    a sample of patients with the behavioral variant of678

    FTDperformedbetter than a sample of AD patients in679

    copying drawings, but also observed that the drawing680

    impairments in the two patient groups might be based681

    on different cognitive and neuroanatomical corre-682

    lates. In AD patients poor figure copy was associated683

    with performance on spatial perception and attention684

    tasks, and correlated significantly with volumes in685

    the right parietal cortex. In FTD patients, instead,686

    performance on figure copy correlated significantly687

    with scores on spatial planning and working memory688

    tasks, and correlated with right dorsolateral pre-689

    frontal cortex volumes. These observations would fit690

    clinical data demonstrating that patients with promi-691

    nent behavioral disorders and frontal involvement692

    (behavioral variant FTD, bvFTD) outperformed AD693

    patients on ROCF immediate reproduction [136],694

    and that patients with prominent language deficits695

    (non-fluent progressive aphasia) did not differ from696

    healthy controls on cube copying and ROCF imme-697

    diate reproduction [137]. Therefore, several studies698

    confirmed the basic tenet that visuospatial skills and699

    drawing abilities are relatively preserved in FTD,700

    but at least two studies did not detect differences701in overall accuracy on ROCF immediate reproduc-702

    tion between bvFTD and AD patients [138, 139]. It 703

    is thus possible that during the disease course FTD 704

    patients might show relatively preserved construc- 705

    tional abilities in the earlystages, whereas differences 706

    between FTD and AD might blur in late stages of 707

    the disease. This hypothesis might find support in 708

    a meta-analysis showing that constructional abilities 709

    assessed on copying the ROCF quickly deteriorate 710

    as FTD progresses [140], and in a longitudinal study 711

    showing a trend toward a steeper decline of copy- 712

    ing abilities in FTD with respect to AD [141, but for 713

    contrasting findings see 142]. 714

    Divergences about drawing disorders in FTD 715

    will be reconciled by taking into account the het- 716

    erogeneous clinical features and neuropathological 717

    underpinnings of the disease [108, 143]. In this 718

    respect, it is important to underline a distinction 719between the drawing abilities of patients with bvFTD 720

    and those of patients with the temporal variant or 721

    semantic dementia (SD), because both are relatively 722

    spared on copying tasks, but the latter can be either 723

    enhanced or selectively impaired in drawing from 724

    memory the typical shape of common objects. The 725

    seemingly contradictory data about drawing abil- 726

    ities in SD are illustrated by results obtained by 727

    Bozeat et al. [19] and by Miller et al. [144146]. 728

    Bozeat et al. [19] showed that when SD patients 729

    are required to produce drawings of concrete objects 730

    from dictation of their names, their drawings are 731

    characterized by a loss of the distinctive features of 732

    the represented objects. On the other hand, Miller 733

    et al. [144146] showed that in the early stages of 734

    SD, patients with left anterior temporal lobe atrophy 735

    and severe anomia increase their drawing abilities, 736

    copying with great precision and sometimes become 737

    artists. In these cases, artworks show an enhanced 738

    tendency to realism. This improvement of visuocon- 739

    structive and artistic abilities was attributed by Miller 740

    et al. [144, 146] to the sparing of the parietal lobe 741

    and of the right hemisphere, considered as dominant 742

    for the three key features of visual artistry: visual 743

    constructive ability, spatial attention,and internal rep- 744

    resentation. The apparent contradiction between data 745

    reported by Bozeat et al. [19] and by Miller et al. 746

    [144146] is probably due to the different stage of 747

    evolution of SD in which observations were gath- 748

    ered (an early stage of SD, with unilateral left sided 749

    atrophy in the case of Miller et al. [144146] and 750

    a later stages of SD, with bilateral anterior tempo- 751

    ral lobe (ATL) atrophy in the case of Bozeat et al. 752

    [19] observations). Gainotti [147, 148] has, indeed, 753recently reviewed data showing that the format of 754

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    13/23

    12 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    conceptual representations is mainly verbal in the left755

    ATL and mainly sensorial/pictorial in the right ATL.756

    In the early stages of SD the atrophy of the left ATL757

    can release, with a mechanism labeled paradoxical758

    functional facilitation by Kapur [149] the pictorial759

    representations stored in the right ATL, leading to an760

    enhancement of pictorial abilities. On the other hand,761

    in later stages the bilateralspread of atrophy provokes762

    a complete disruption of verbal and non-verbal con-763

    ceptual representations of concrete entities, leading764

    to an inability to draw them from memory.765

    Drawing disorders in Lewy body dementia766

    In LBD, visuospatialdifficulties areoften early and767

    prominent [150]. Visuospatial tasks, such as object768

    size discrimination, formdiscrimination, overlapping769figure identification, and visual counting tasks, may770

    reveal more impaired performances in LBD than in771

    AD, and these defects likely contribute to the dispro-772

    portionate impairment in constructional tasks in LBD773

    patients [151153].774

    In line with these observations, case series of775

    pathologically verified LBD [154] as well as sys-776

    tematic reviews [155] confirmed that an impairment777

    in copying geometrical figures is very common in778

    LBD patients since early stages of the disease. How-779

    ever, several studies employing CDT did not report780

    significant differences in overall total score in LBD781

    and AD patients [156, 157], likely because of the782

    complexity of the task (see Fig. 1), although at least783

    two studies reportedsignificantly lower overallscores784

    in LBD versus AD patients [158, 159]. A SPECT785

    study showed that LBD patients with defective786

    performance on CDT presented more marked hypop-787

    erfusion in a frontal-subcortical network, involving788

    frontal eye fields and the thalamus, with respect to789

    LBD patients with normal CDT scores [160]. A790

    more recent neurofunctional investigation by PET791

    revealed, instead, that, after controlling for overall792

    cognitive impairment, there was a direct associa-793

    tion between frontoparietal dysfunction and impaired794

    CDT performance in LBD [161].795

    The early and prominent impairment of visuospa-796

    tial processing in LBD led some authors to suggest797

    that LBD patients may be characterized by parallel798

    impairments in free drawing and in figure copying799

    since early stages of the disease whereas AD patients800

    would show relative sparing of figure copying ([162],801

    but also see [163]). Likely because of its complexity,802

    copy of the ROCF has not been extensively used in803LBD, whereas some interesting observations came804

    from the copy of the intersecting pentagons included 805

    in the MMSE. Ala et al. [164] found that patients with 806

    LBD were more likely than those with AD to copy 807

    pentagons incorrectly, and suggested that this impair- 808

    ment might help identifying LBD patients. Cormack 809

    et al. [165] confirmed that LBD patients show signifi- 810

    cantly lower performance in copying pentagons with 811

    respect to AD; moreover, while in AD the impair- 812

    ment in copying figures was correlated with general 813

    cognitive deterioration, in LBD drawing was corre- 814

    lated with visuospatial tasks only, thus suggesting the 815

    existence of a specific defect in this disease. Differ- 816

    ent scoring criteria can likely explain the reason why 817

    other authors failed to detect differences between 818

    LBD and AD in pentagon copying [108, 159], but 819

    more extended copying tests might show signifi- 820

    cantly lower performance in LBD with respect to 821AD patients [108]. Recently, it has beendemonstrated 822

    that qualitative analysis of the pentagon copy test can 823

    provide additional information for distinguishing AD 824

    from LBD patients, since a lower number of angles 825

    is among distinctive features of LBD patients copies 826

    [73, 167], and can also predict development of LBD 827

    in MCI patients [74]. 828

    PECULIAR ASPECTS OF DRAWING 829

    DISORDERS IN DEMENTIA 830

    In the previous sections of this paper it has been 831

    repeatedly underlined that qualitative observations 832

    (i.e., errors analysis) might provide additional infor- 833

    mation about the cognitive and neural correlates of 834

    drawing disorders in patients with different kinds 835

    of dementia. Some qualitative phenomena observed 836

    in drawing tasks in demented patients deserve, 837

    therefore, some brief comments, because of their fre- 838

    quency and their potential clinical usefulness. 839

    Graphic perseveration 840

    Generally speaking, perseveration can be consid- 841

    ered among productive (or positive) pathological 842

    signs, since it consists in iterative behavioral 843

    responses, not adequate to the current stimulus (see 844

    Figs. 24) [168]. Patients may produce the same fig- 845

    ure repeatedly, in response to only one stimulus, or 846

    replicate stimulis elements (continuous persevera- 847

    tion, accordingto Sandson andAlbert) [169];on other 848

    occasions, patients may inappropriately draw figures 849

    already drawn in previous trials, instead of repro- 850

    ducing the current stimulus (recurrent perseveration) 851[169]. 852

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    14/23

    L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia 13

    AspecifickindofperseverationisobservedinCDT,853

    when patients produce repeatedly the same numbers,854

    or start numeration over and over (see Fig. 1). These855

    errorsinclockdrawingshavebeendescribedinasmall856

    proportionofADpatients[170],butwouldincreasein857

    moderate to severe stages of the disease [88]. Ryan et858

    al. [171] compared graphic performances on the Ben-859

    der visuomotor Gestalt test and onCDT in AD with or860

    without wandering, and observed a larger number of861

    both continuous and recurrent perseverative errors in862

    individualsshowingthe wandering phenomenon,thus863

    suggesting a relationship between perseveration and864

    behavioral control.865

    A first study specifically comparing graphic per-866

    severations in patients with AD or VaD showed that867

    perseverations were more frequent in VaD than in868

    AD [172]. However, Cosentino et al. [173] assessed869patients on CDT and on copying clocks and observed870

    that perseveration and closing-in (see below) in AD871

    and VaD were more frequent in patients with higher872

    numberof white matterlesions andwith more marked873

    impairment on executive frontal tasks. These findings874

    suggested that the executive impairment associated875

    withfrontal-subcortical dysfunctioncontributesto the876

    genesis of perseveration in clock drawing in demen-877

    tia. Recently, in a retrospectivestudyon a large patient878

    sample, De Lucia et al. [174] observed that frequency879

    of graphic perseverative errors was similar in AD880

    and VaD, and that patients with moderate-to-severe881

    dementia produced a significantly higher number of882

    perseverations than individuals with mild dementia.883

    In both groups graphic perseverations were related884

    with frontal and visuoconstructional impairments,885

    thussupportingtheviewthatfrontal-executivedefects886

    canhamperinhibitionof iterativegraphicproductions.887

    In FTD, patients perseveration in drawing would888

    be often present also in early stages of the disease889

    while specific disturbances in reproducing spatial890

    relationships would become evident later during the891

    course of the disease [175]. In a systematic analysis892

    of errors observed in a wide range of neuropsycho-893

    logical tests, Thompson et al. [134] reported that894

    FTD patients outperformed a group of AD patients895

    in drawing tasks, but the most distinctive feature896

    between the two groups were perseveration and poor897

    overall organization (see Figs. 24). It is worth898

    mentioning, however, that other studies focusing on899

    qualitative error analysis [138, 139] did not observe900

    a higher proportion of graphic perseverations in FTD901

    with respect to AD.902

    In LBD, several authors suggested that less903efficient executive control might induce higher fre-904

    quency of perseveration than in AD [176, 177], 905

    but perseverations in drawing tasks have not been 906

    assessed specifically. 907

    Rotation 908

    In copying stimuli, some patients may respect 909

    spatial relationships among constituent elements but 910

    reproduce a model with general orientation different 911

    from the stimulus, e.g., rotating the reproduction by 912

    90 or 180 degrees (see Fig. 4). This behavior has been 913

    already mentioned in early studies on constructional 914

    apraxia[27,178,179]andhasbeenreported,although 915

    rarely, in subsequent descriptions of patients with 916

    brain lesions. From a consecutive unselected series of 917

    240neurologicalpatients,Solmsetal.[180]identified 918

    16 patients who reproduced the ROCF with its major 919axis vertically rotated, independently from accuracy 920

    in reproducing spatial relationships among inner ele- 921

    ments. Seven of these patients had diffuse cerebral 922

    involvement, but all remaining cases showed a lesion 923

    involving frontal regions. The authors suggested that 924

    thisbehaviorcouldreflectthelackofplanningandver- 925

    ification abilities of frontal patients [180]. In another 926

    case series of patients with AD, rotation of one or 927

    both items of the MMSE interlocking pentagons was 928

    reported in about 5% of patients [181]. Frequency of 929

    rotation of MMSE pentagons would not differ in AD 930

    or LBD patients, but it could progressively increase 931

    during the disease course in LBD patients [73]. 932

    Recent findings suggest that complex visuopercep- 933

    tual and planning mechanisms might contribute to the 934

    genesis of rotations. In a retrospective study on a large 935

    sample of patients with MCI or degenerative demen- 936

    tia who underwent copy and recall of the ROCF, 937

    Isella et al. [182] observed rotation on the copy con- 938

    dition in 2.7% patients and on recall in 3.3% patients. 939

    In a subsequent prospective study on a mixed sam- 940

    ple of patients with degenerative dementias or focal 941

    brain lesions, rotation at the copy of the ROCF was 942

    associated with visuospatial and selective attention 943

    impairments, and with more severe temporo-parieto- 944

    occipital atrophy or hypometabolism, whereas no 945

    specific profile of cognitive impairment distinguished 946

    patients with rotation at recall of the ROCF, in whom 947

    frontal abnormalities were more frequent [183]. 948

    Closing-in 949

    In copying tasks, demented patients often show the 950

    tendency either to put the pencil directly over the 951model, producing a scrawl, or to overlap the lines 952

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    15/23

    14 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    of the model with those of the copy or to draw very953

    near to the models elements (see Figs. 2 and 3).954

    Such behavior, termed closing-in (CI), has beenfirst955

    described by Mayer-Gross [23], and later reported956

    more often in demented patients than after focal brain957

    lesions [24, 90]. Among dementing syndromes, CI is958

    predominant in AD [88, 184], suggesting the idea959

    that CI might represent a neuropsychological marker960

    for clinical diagnosis of AD [90, 185, 186]. One961

    more recent study [25] confirmed that CI is more fre-962

    quent in AD with respect to VaD patients, and that963

    the two patient groups also tended to show qualita-964

    tively different CI phenomena, as AD patients more965

    often overlapped at least one element of the copy onto966

    the model (adherent-CI), whereas VaD patients more967

    often drew close to the model without overlapping968

    onto it (near-CI). On the other hand, one retrospec-969tive study showed that CI is as frequent in AD as in970

    FTD patients, but did not provide qualitative obser-971

    vations [187]. Indeed, in a sample of patients with the972

    behavioral variant of FTD, near-CI has been observed973

    more frequently than adherent-CI, and was often (but974

    not obligatorily) associated with other imitation and975

    utilization behaviors [188]. Notably, more frequent976

    occurrence of near-CI than of adherent-CI has been977

    recently reported also in non-demented Parkinsons978

    disease patients [189], whereas the reverse pattern979

    has been observed in Parkinsons disease dementia980

    [190].981

    In his original description, Mayer-Gross [23]982

    ascribed CI to a disturbance in performing spatial983

    movements of hands and fingers correctly. In later984

    years, interpretative accounts of CI clustered within985

    two main streams. The first interpretative framework986

    might originate from the consideration that CI could987

    occur when patients who are unable to structure an988

    empty space look for a reference point to solve dif-989

    ficult constructional dilemmas [191]. Following this990

    suggestion, in recent years CI has been considered as991

    a compensatory behavior implemented to overcome992

    deficits of visuospatial skills or of visuospatial work-993

    ing memory [192]. In this compensation account,994

    patients with impaired ability to represent the model,995

    or to hold its representation for the need of repro-996

    ducing it, might tend to close-in to the model in the997

    attempt to reduce their difficulties. Such a compen-998

    sation hypothesis received support by the finding999

    that complex models could increase severity of CI1000

    in AD patients [192], and by the observation that AD1001

    patients with CI showed more severe impairments on1002

    several visuospatial tests, compared to AD patients1003without CI [193].1004

    The alternative account might be traced back to the 1005

    hypothesis that CI might represent a primitive reflex 1006

    leading patients to be strongly attracted by the model 1007

    and be unable to detach from it [24]. In this attrac- 1008

    tion framework, CI would represent a default 1009

    behavior released by defects of attentional-executive 1010

    abilities [194, 195]. In analogy with the compensa- 1011

    tion hypothesis, the attraction hypothesis predicts 1012

    that CI would be enhanced in dealing with complex 1013

    models because they likely imply high attentional 1014

    load, and reduce available resources for monitor- 1015

    ing graphic productions. However, this hypothesis 1016

    specifically envisages strong correlations between CI 1017

    and frontal/executive dysfunction, and foresees that 1018

    CI can be triggered by high attention-demanding task 1019

    conditions. Several converging pieces of evidence 1020

    supported this account, since a significant correla- 1021tion between CI and frontal/executive dysfunction 1022

    has been reported in MCI [196], AD [191], VaD [25], 1023

    and Parkinsons disease [189], and in such diseases 1024

    copying geometrical figures in dual-task conditions 1025

    enhanced CI [25, 184, 197]. A first attempt at iden- 1026

    tifying the neural correlates of CI in AD patients 1027

    would point to bilateral orbito-frontal cortex as the 1028

    area in which atrophy was significantly associated to 1029

    presence of CI [198]. 1030

    Most available evidence would thus suggest that, 1031

    independently from the diagnosis, patients sharing 1032

    similar cognitive impairment might produce CI, and 1033

    this attempt at identifying cognitive and neural corre- 1034

    lates of specific errors might provide further insights 1035

    on drawing disorders in dementia. 1036

    CONCLUDING REMARKS 1037

    It is interesting to note, at the end of this review, 1038

    that several patients reported by Kleist [27] and 1039

    Mayer Gross [23] in the earliest studies on construc- 1040

    tional apraxia and drawing disorders were affected by 1041

    dementia. It is also worth of note that, in one of the 1042

    first studies aiming to investigate with standardized 1043

    tests the patterns of neuropsychological impairment 1044

    shown by various diagnostic groups of dementia, 1045

    Gainotti et al. [199] showed that AD patients were 1046

    particularly impaired on memory and drawing tasks. 1047

    In another study, Villa et al. [200] showed that 1048

    the coexistence of mental deterioration was more 1049

    strongly associated with drawing disabilities than lat- 1050

    erality of lesion, or intrahemispheric locus of lesion 1051

    in focal brain-damaged patients. More recent investi- 1052gations have followed two complementary strategies 1053

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    16/23

    L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia 15

    to clarify incidence and qualitative aspects of draw-1054

    ing disabilities observed in AD and other forms of1055

    dementia. The first strategy consisted in adopting1056

    short drawing tasks that could be particularly appro-1057

    priate to investigate drawing disorders in demented1058

    patients. The second strategy consisted in taking into1059

    account the nature and the qualitative aspects of1060

    drawing disorders that could facilitate the differential1061

    diagnosis between AD and the other clinical forms of1062

    dementia.1063

    The most popular task adopted according to1064

    the first strategic approach is CDT, which allows1065

    obtaining different scores (e.g., graphic difficul-1066

    ties, related to elementary motor disorders; stimulus1067

    bound responses, resulting from inhibition difficul-1068

    ties; conceptual deficits, related to semantic memory1069

    disorders; perseverations resulting from frontal lobe1070dysfunctions and visuospatial disorders), and can be1071

    administered both under verbal command and in the1072

    copy of a pre-drawn clock (see Fig. 1). According1073

    to several authors [21, 88, 125, 126, 201, 202], the1074

    CDT is able to distinguish different severity levels1075

    and clinical forms of dementia, but other authors1076

    reported contrasting findings with respect to both1077

    issues [65, 123, 128, 131, 158]. These deceiving1078

    results are probably due to two main factors. The1079

    first is that the CTD is basically a screening test and as1080

    such cannot provide a large and diversified amount of1081

    clinical information. The second is that many admin-1082

    istration procedures and scoring systems have been1083

    proposed [20, 21, 5457], but there is no consensus1084

    as to which is the most useful for dementia screen-1085

    ing or for providing hints at a differential diagnosis1086

    among different forms of dementia. It is also possible1087

    that different diagnostic criteria and, above all, rate1088

    of diagnostic uncertainty might have biased some of1089

    the findings reported in the present review, and this1090

    concern might only be overcome by gathering fur-1091

    ther well-designed and accurate studies on drawing1092

    disorders in the different forms of dementia.1093

    More substantial results have been obtained fol-1094

    lowing the second research strategy, because several1095

    studieshaveshownthatthediscrepancybetweencom-1096

    mand and copy condition (in the CDT or in other1097

    drawing tasks) is greater in forms of dementia, such1098

    as the subcortical vascular dementia [127, 128] or the1099

    bvFTD [129, 130, 134136], which are characterized1100

    by a prominent deficit of the executive and control1101

    functions. Furthermore, the drawing impairments in1102

    AD andFTD patients could be based on differentcog-1103

    nitive andneuroanatomical mechanisms in agreement1104with the different prevalence of lesions in these two1105

    diseases [134, 135]: in AD patients poor figure copy 1106

    was associated with performance on spatial percep- 1107

    tion and attentional tasks, and correlated significantly 1108

    with volumes in the right parietal cortex; in FTD 1109

    patients, instead, performance on figure copy cor- 1110

    related significantly with scores on spatial planning 1111

    and working memory tasks, and was connected with 1112

    right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volumes.Very dif- 1113

    ferent from the drawing disorders of patients with 1114

    bvFTD are those of patients with semantic dementia, 1115

    because both obtain good performances on copying 1116

    tasks,butthelatterareselectivelyimpairedindrawing 1117

    from memory the typical shape of common objects. 1118

    In fact, when SD patients were given the names of 1119

    concrete objects and asked to produce the correspond- 1120

    ing designs, these drawings were characterized by 1121

    a loss of the distinctive features of the represented 1122objects, resulting from disruption of the correspond- 1123

    ing conceptual representations, stored in the right and 1124

    left anterior temporal lobes [19, 203]. Copying tasks 1125

    are, therefore, substantially spared in degenerative 1126

    diseases mainly involvingthe frontal (bvFTD)or tem- 1127

    poral(SD)corticesandmightbemildlyimpairedinthe 1128

    early stages of standard forms of AD, in which atro- 1129

    phy prevails in the medial temporal lobes, although 1130

    neuroimaging studies repeatedly showed a pattern 1131

    of reduced metabolism in the temporo-parietal areas 1132

    [204, 205]. A much more severe impairment in copy- 1133

    ing tasks is observed in early-onset AD patients and 1134

    in PCA variant, in which severe constructive defects 1135

    are usually associated with progressive visual-spatial 1136

    disorders, including Balint syndrome and unilateral 1137

    spatial neglect [100, 101]. A very similar pattern of 1138

    drawing impairment is found in LBD, in which visu- 1139

    ospatial difficulties are often early and prominent and 1140

    contribute to the disproportionate impairment in con- 1141

    structionaltasksobservedinthesepatients[151153]. 1142

    We can, therefore, conclude that the pattern of 1143

    drawing disorders observed in AD and in other forms 1144

    of dementia can be explained if we take into account 1145

    the main functions of the neural networks involved 1146

    in these forms of degenerative diseases. Among 1147

    the peculiar drawing patterns that are observed in 1148

    the moderate-to-severe forms of dementia the most 1149

    important is certainly the closing-in symptom (see 1150

    Figs. 2 and 3), which is significantly more frequent 1151

    and more severe in AD than in vascular forms of 1152

    dementia [24, 25], because AD patients tend to put 1153

    the pencil directly over the model, or to overlap the 1154

    lines of the model with those of the copy (adherent- 1155

    CI), whereas VaD patients more often draw in close 1156proximity to the model without overlapping onto it 1157

  • 7/26/2019 Drawing Disorders in Alzheimer's Disease

    17/23

    16 L. Trojano and G. Gainotti / Drawing Disorders in Dementia

    (near-CI). If the CI might improve the differential1158

    diagnosis between AD and VaD, it is less useful in1159

    the distinction between AD and FTD, because the1160

    role of the frontal lobes in its production is still under1161

    scrutiny. From this point of view, it is important1162

    that recent neuroanatomical data demonstrated the1163

    selective association of atrophy in the orbito-frontal1164

    areas, implied in inhibiting primitive reflexes, with1165

    the closing-in phenomenon [198]. Even if several1166

    neuropsychological and pathophysiological aspects1167

    of drawing disorders in ADandother formsof demen-1168

    tia require further clarification, the clinical interest1169

    of drawing tasks in the differential diagnosis among1170

    different forms of dementia can, therefore, be con-1171

    sidered as firmly established, particularly taking into1172

    account analysis of drawing errors more than overall1173

    accuracy in drawing.1174

    DISCLOSURE STATEMENT1175

    Authors disclosures available online (http://j-alz.1176

    com/manuscript-disclosures/16-0009r2).1177

    REFERENCES1178

    [1] Ettlinger G, Warrington E, Zangwill OL (1957) A further1179

    study of visual-spatial agnosia. Brain80, 335-361.1180[2] Arena R, Gainotti G (1978) Constructional apraxia and1181

    visuoperceptive disabilities in relation to laterality of cere-1182bral lesions.Cortex14, 463-473.1183

    [3] Gainotti G, Tiacci C (1970) Patterns of drawing disability1184in right and left hemispheric patients. Neuropsychologia11858, 379-384.1186

    [4] Trojano L, Fragassi NA, Chiacchio L, Izzo O, Izzo G,1187Di Cesare G, Cristinzio C, Grossi D (2004) Relationships1188between constructionaland visuospatial abilities in normal1189subjects and in focal brain-damaged patients. J Clin Exp1190

    Neuropsychol26, 1103-1112.1191[5] Serra L, Fadda L, Perri R, Spano B, Marra C, Castelli1192

    D, Torso M, Makovac E, Cercignani M, Caltagirone C,1193Bozzali M (2014) Constructional apraxia as a distinc-1194tive cognitive and structural brain feature of pre-senile1195Alzheimers disease.J Alzheimers Dis38, 391-402.1196

    [6] Warrington EK, James M, Kinsbourne M (1966) Drawing1197disability in relation to laterality of cerebral lesion.Brain119889, 53-82.1199

    [7] Hecaen H, Assal G (1970) A comparison of construc-1200tive deficits following right and left hemispheric lesions.1201

    Neuropsychologia8, 289-303.1202[8] Gainotti G, Miceli G, Caltagirone C (1977). Construc-1203

    tional apraxia in left brain-damaged patients: A planning1204disorder?Cortex13, 109-118.1205

    [9] Kirk A, Kertesz A (1991) On drawing impairment in1206Alzheimers disease.Arch Neurol 48, 73-77.1207

    [10] Pillon B (1981) Troubles visuo-constructifs et methodes1208

    de compensation. Resultats de 85 patients atteints de1209lesions cerebrales.Neuropsychologia19, 375-383.1210

    [11] Critchley M (1953) The ParietalLobes, HafnerPress, New 1211York. 1212

    [12] Piercy M,Hecaen H, Ajuriaguerra J (1960)Constructional 1213apraxia associated with unilateral cerebral lesions-left and 1214right sided cases compared.Brain83, 225-242. 1215

    [13] De Renzi E (1982) Disorders of Space Exploration and 1216

    Cognition, Wiley, Chichester. 1217

    [14] Makuuchi M, Kaminaga T, Sugishita M (2003) Both pari- 1218etal lobes are involved in drawing: A functional MRI 1219study and implications for constructional apraxia. Brain 1220

    Res Cogn Brain Res 16, 338-347. 1221

    [15] Roth HL, Bauer RM, Crucian GP, Heilman KM 1222(2014) Frontal-executive constructional apraxia: When 1223delayed recall is better than copying. Neurocase 20, 1224283-295. 1225

    [16] Price CC, Cunningham H, Coronado N, Freedland A, 1226Cosentino S, Penney DL, Penisi A, Bowers D, Okun MS, 1227Libon DJ (2011). Clock drawing in the montreal cognitive 1228assessment: Recommendations for dementia assessment. 1229

    Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord31, 179-187. 1230

    [17] Gainotti G, Silveri MC, Villa G, Caltagirone C (1983) 1231Drawing objects from memory in aphasia. Brain 106, 1232613-622. 1233

    [18] Kirk A, Kertesz A (1989) Hemispheric contributions to 1234drawing.Neuropsychologia27, 881-886. 1235

    [19] Bozeat S, Lambon Ralph MA, Graham KS, Patterson K, 1236Wilkin H, Rowland J, Hodges JR (2003) A duck with 1237four legs: Investigating the structure of conceptual knowl- 1238edge using picture drawing in semantic dementia. Cogn 1239

    Neuropsychol20, 27-47. 1240[20] Agrell B, Dehlin O (2012) The clock-drawing test. 1998. 1241

    Age Ageing 41(Suppl 3), 41-45. 1242[21] Freedman MI, Leach L, Kaplan E, Winocur G, Shulman 1243

    KJ, Delis DC (1994) Clock Drawing, Oxford University 1244

    Press, Oxford. 1245

    [22] Trojano L, Conson M (2008). Visuospatial and visuocon- 1246structive deficits. InHandbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 124788, Goldenberg G, Miller BL, eds. Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1248pp. 373-392. 1249

    [23] Mayer Gross W (1935)Some observation on apraxia. Proc 1250R Soc Med28, 1203-1212. 1251

    [24] Gainotti G (1972) A quantitative study of the closing- 1252in symptom in normal children and in brain-damaged 1253patients.Neuropsychologia10, 429-436. 1254

    [25] De Lucia N, Grossi D, Trojano L (2014) The genesis of 1255closing-in in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia: A 1256comparative clinical and experimental study. Neuropsy- 1257chology28, 312-318. 1258

    [26] Poppelreuter W (1917) Die Psychischen Schaedigungen 1259durch Kopfschuss im Kriege 1914-1916, Vol. 1, Voss, 1260Leipzig. 1261

    [27] Kleist K (1934)Gehirnpathologie, Barth, Leipzig. 1262[28] Gainotti G (1985) Constructional apraxia. InHandbook of 1263

    Clinical Neurology, Vol. 45, JAM Fredericks, ed.,Elsevier, 1264Amsterdam, pp. 491-506. 1265

    [29] Benton AL (1962) The visual retention test as a construc- 1266tional praxis task.Confin Neurol22, 141-155. 1267

    [30] Arrigoni C, De Renzi E (1964) Constructional apraxia and 1268hemispheric locus of lesion.