editorial making sense of social reality

Upload: laura-pop

Post on 03-Jun-2018

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    1/9

    Decembrie 2005 Cogniie, Creier, Comportament 599

    EDITORIAL MAKING SENSE OF SOCIAL REALITY

    Petru Lucian CUREU

    Department of Organisation Studies, Tilburg University, The Netherlands

    Department of Psychology, Babe-Bolyai University Cluj-Napoca, Romania

    Humans are social beings that spend most of their time interacting withothers in various contexts from family to organizational or even larger societal

    settings. The way in which they make sense of these interactions, and the way in

    which they understand the social reality inspired a considerable amount of research

    in the field of Social Psychology. One of the first attempts made by a psychologist

    to explain how people make sense of social reality was Carl Gustav Jungs

    collective unconscious theory. Using his background in Freudian theory and his

    extensive knowledge in mythology, religion and philosophy, he developed the first

    psychological theory that tried to explain how people make sense of their social

    world. Central in his theory is the concept of collective unconscious: if individual

    unconscious reflects the individual experience, the collective unconscious reflects

    the experience of the whole species and its contents are the archetypes (primordial

    images or tendencies to experience events in a particular way) (Jung, 1981). SinceJungs collective unconscious theory, several theoretical perspectives emerged in

    the field of Social Psychology trying to explain how people make sense of their

    social world. This special issue tries to illustrate the diversity of approaches that

    "hang around" in the academic and research communities, with a special interest in

    social cognition theories and constructionist approaches. It is not the aim of this

    special issue to integrate or reconcile different theoretical perspectives, but rather

    to find the value in the diversity of approaches. The special issue includes

    empirical and theoretical studies illustrative for the modern developments in both

    social cognition and constructionist approaches.

    Social cognition

    Based on its prominence in Social Psychology, social cognition is the first

    major trend to be discussed. After a quick scan of the Annual Review of

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    2/9

    Editorial: Making sense of social reality600

    Psychology, more than 732 papers matches the keyword social cognition. Rooted

    in Experimental Social Psychology (Whetherel, McGhee & Stevens, 2003)research in social cognition was initially dominated by the study of attitudes

    (Allport, 1935). Based on the assumption that inspired it, namely that social

    behavior is objectively describable and measurable (Whetherel, McGhee &

    Stevens, 2003), sophisticated quantitative methods have been developed to

    evaluate attitudes. Later on, based on the theoretical developments in Cognitive

    Psychology, several explanatory models have been put forward to explain their

    origins, functions, as well as their relation with behavior and models of attitudes

    change (Ajzen, 2001). Currently, the field of attitude research is more or less

    divided between a trend focusing on attitude change, persuasion and social

    influence (Wood, 2000, Crano & Prislin, 2006) and a trend focusing on attitude

    structure, function and origins (Ajzen, 2001). In a general sense, attitudes are

    cognitive structures, which guide information processing, and subsequently

    behaviors; in this sense they were considered the crown jewel of social

    psychology (Crano & Prislin, 2006, p. 360). Due to their relations with behaviors,

    attitudes and especially attitude change programs have been proven to have diverse

    applications in practical settings from marketing to health education trainings.

    A topic in social cognition that received considerable attention is the study

    of stereotypes. Initially brought to the attention of psychologists by a journalist

    Walter Lippmann (1992) and viewed as images in the brain about social groups,

    the stereotypes were then incorporated into the social categorization theory (Hilton

    & Hippel, 1996) and fueled by the large antidiscrimination movement in the 60s

    and 70s (Hamilton & Sherman, 1995, Ashkanasy, Hrtel & Daus, 2002), they

    became soon one of the hottest topics in social cognition. The researchenthusiasm generated around stereotypes, made them emblematic for the social

    cognition research (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000), determining some critics of the

    social cognition perspective (e.g. Moscovici, 1997) to identify it altogether with the

    study of stereotypes (even though I find this critique rather extreme and

    reductionist). A general view on stereotypes formation and use would suggest that

    in their interaction with the social world, people make use of categories to simplify

    it and make the best out of their limited representational and computational

    (cognitive) resources. Stereotypes are nothing else but common attributes

    associated with a particular social category or group (Hilton & Hippel, 1996). As it

    is the case with attitudes, stereotypes activation induces biases in the processing of

    social information and guides subsequent behavior. Because of their link with

    prejudice and discrimination, the study of (negative) stereotypes proved to haveimportant practical applications in designing diversity trainings, aiming at reducing

    the negative effects of (negative) stereotypes (Robertson, Kulik and Pepper, 2001).

    Most of our knowledge about stereotypes comes from laboratory studies, or studies

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    3/9

    Decembrie 2005 Cogniie, Creier, Comportament 601

    carried out on (psychology) students, while the larger social and historical context

    is being almost completely ignored.In this special issue, Sandra Schruijer addresses, among others, the use of

    historical data in the study of stereotypes formation and stereotypes change,

    advocating for a dialog between history and social psychology. The author offers

    an insight into the content and valence of stereotypes held by Protestants toward

    Roman Catholics in The Netherlands between 1795 and 1830. The incursion is

    particularly relevant since it illustrates the way in which the content and especially

    the valence of stereotypes changes under the impact of social and historical

    contextual factors. According to Judd and Park (1993), these contextual factors are

    especially important in understanding the true origins and change of stereotypes

    and the proposed dialog between history and psychology could add substance to

    this particular aspect of social cognition research. Further on, well-crafted

    arguments from psychoanalysis, social constructionismand from social cognition

    are combined to advocate the benefits of multidisciplinary approaches in

    understanding the way people make sense of their social world.

    Modern social cognition theories evolved more or less guided by the

    developments in cognitive sciences. First, inspired by the developments on

    unconscious information processing developments in cognitive psychology,

    modern social cognition theories take into account the implicit nature of attitudes

    and stereotypes (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995, Greenwald, et al., 2002). The implicit

    social cognition theory argues that attitudes and stereotypes are (partly)

    unconscious contents (unavailable to introspection), developed as a result of past

    experience; once activated they automatically influence behavior (they are

    unconscious traces of previous experience that influence the way in which peoplemake sense of social reality and the way they behave in social situations)

    (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995, p. 5). Second, the developments in cognitive

    neurosciences encouraged researchers to find brain substrates for attitudes and

    stereotypes (Adolphs, 2003), for the Self concept (Klein, Rozendal & Cosmides,

    2002) as well as for the recollection of social information (Schacter, Norman &

    Koutstaal, 1998). The interest in finding brain correlates for social information

    processing was so intense, especially in the last two decades, that according to

    Cacioppo et al (2003) a distinct discipline emerged, namely social neuroscience.

    Third, the developments in artificial intelligence generated yet another direction of

    development for social cognition theories, namely the use of artificial simulations

    to test and refine theoretical predictions. Connectionist models were developed to

    test and refine cognitive dissonance theory (van Overwalle & Jordens, 2002), aswell as to test proposition of intergroup biases and stereotypes use in group

    judgments (van Rooy, van Overwalle, Labiouse & French, 2003). Finally, in

    addition to the developments in cognitive sciences, modern social cognition

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    4/9

    Editorial: Making sense of social reality602

    theories were also fueled by the developments in social group research. The shared

    cognition theories extended the concept of social cognition from the individuallevel to the group level and discuss the implications of shared cognition on group

    performance and effectiveness (Cannon-Bowers & Salas, 2001, Mohammed &

    Dumville, 2001). The shared cognition approaches argue that the social group

    itself is a socio-cognitive system, which as an entity processes information and

    tries to make sense of the social world.

    The present special issue illustrates some of these modern developments in

    social cognition theories, by emphasizing their link with the neurosciences (Oana

    Benga) and by commenting on the developments of shared cognition theories

    (Petru Cureu & Diana Rus, and Smaranda Boro). Oana Benga reviews the most

    relevant findings in social neurosciences with an approach based on stages of

    (social) information processing, which offers more transparency in connecting

    cognitive aspects with brain correlates. The analysis starts with a first stage of

    perception and recognition, where brain correlates are reviewed for (emotional)

    face processing, emotional prosody as well as for the perception of biological

    movement and intentionality. At a higher level of analysis, the correlates of social

    judgments, stereotypes, belief attribution and the theory of mind are discussed. The

    author continues with the cognitive appraisal and social interaction levels of

    information processing, discussing the brain correlates for social interactions based

    on cooperation and competition. Finally, future research directions are put forward,

    based on possible connections between social constructivist theoretical

    underpinnings (e.g. social reality is not universal, it emerges from social

    interactions and is permanently re-construed) and the neurosciences framework.

    In a paper that extends the concept of cognitive complexity from theindividual to the group level, Petru Cureu and Diana Rus review the developments

    in shared cognition (particularly team cognition) research. The paper acknowledges

    the role of social interactions in the development of team cognition and it

    advocates the use of holistic elicitation and representations techniques in order to

    capture the impact of social interaction in the development of team cognition

    (group particularly cognitive complexity). Several ways to operationalize group

    cognitive complexity are discussed and possible research directions are identified

    based on the developments in social diversity and group composition literature.

    Smaranda Borodiscusses the implications of cognitive diversity in groups, using

    the concept of organizational identity as a form of shared cognition. In a quasi-

    experimental study, the author manipulates the cognitive diversity in groups and

    identifies the effects on group conflict and emergent states. The validity of the Q-sort approach to elicit and represent shared cognitions is also discussed and the Q-

    sort approach is contrasted to a conceptual mapping technique as elicitation and

    representation technique for shared cognitions.

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    5/9

    Decembrie 2005 Cogniie, Creier, Comportament 603

    Constructionist approaches andsocial representations theory

    First used by the sociologist Emile Durkheim to define products or results

    of collective actions, the concept of social representations was consecrated by

    Serge Moscovici (1961). He defined social representations as a special category of

    knowledge and beliefs (situated in between perception and concepts) that emerge

    from communication and used to make sense of reality and guide social behavior

    (Moscovici, 1984). Even though the theory of social representations was criticized

    as being vague and ill defined (Potter & Edwards, 1999), it is presently one of the

    most important developments in European Social Psychology (Huguet & Latane,

    1996). In his initial theory, Moscovici (1961) described two functions that social

    representations have: first, they represent conventions for social actors and social

    events and second, they prescribe individual behavior in social settings. Further

    research investigated the structure of social representations distinguishing between

    a central core of a social representation which is stable, coherent, consensual and

    historically marked and the peripheral system which is flexible, sensitive to

    immediate social context and has as principal function: the concretization of the

    central social representation core (Abric, 1993, p. 76). In addition to the functions

    described by Moscovici (representational and prescriptive), Abric (1993) describes

    separate functions for the central core (to generate the significance and determines

    the organization of social representation) and the peripheral system (to allow the

    social representation to adapt to the changing social context, to allow its content

    related differentiation and to protect the central core of the social representation)

    (Abric, 1993).

    As Moscovici (1997, 1998) repeatedly stated, social representations theoryshould be clearly distinguished both from the social cognition, and from discursive

    psychology. The main difference from social cognition would be that social

    representations theory does not emphasize a clear subject-object distinction, while

    the later stresses this distinction (an illustration of the social representations as a

    source and product of social context is presented in Figure 1). In social cognition

    representation is only an epiphenomenon (belongs to an individual), while in social

    representations theory the representation is the result of a collective sense-making

    effort (and in this sense is shared within a community).

    The essential difference from the constructionist approaches (e.g.

    discursive psychology) will be that social representations stress the role of

    representations in making sense of social reality and social constructionist

    approaches focuses on language as a fundamental way of making sense of and inthe same time constructing the social context. However, to some extent it is agreed

    that both approaches are constructionist in nature (Potter & Edwards, 1999) and

    to ask the question if language or representation is the most important element in

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    6/9

    Editorial: Making sense of social reality604

    the way people make sense of their social reality can be misleading (Moscovici,

    1998, Potter & Edwards, 1999).

    Figure 1.A scheme of social representations in relation with the social context

    From a constructionist perspective, reality does not exist, but it is

    construed through social interactions by individuals and groups. Language is anessential tool in making sense of and creating social reality. An individual (or

    group) does not exist independently of the social context, which instead does not

    make sense in the absence of a storyteller (a person to describe it through

    narratives). As opposed to social cognition (i.e. cognitivism), which de-

    contextualizes the social actors in order to study their internal cognitive states,

    social constructionist perspectives study the social actors in their natural social

    context. In a metaphoric sense, for social cognition, the social actor is a nave

    scientist or economizer of cognitive resources trying to make sense of the social

    reality, while for social constructionist approaches, the social actor is an un-

    separable part of the social context.

    It is not one of the aims of this special issue to address these theoretical

    disputes, but rather to illustrate the added value of each of these approaches to thescientific understanding of how people make sense of social reality. In an

    intellectually challenging paper, Dian Marie Hosking addresses the distinction

    between constructivism and critical relational constructionism based on the

    SOCIAL CONTEXT

    SOCIAL STIMULI(social objects, people, groups,

    social events)

    SOCIAL REPRESENTATIONS(symbolic categories)

    Person 1 Person 2

    Behavior 2Behavior 1

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    7/9

    Decembrie 2005 Cogniie, Creier, Comportament 605

    subject-object relation, emphasizing the dialogical conception of Self as relational

    (re)construction. The author starts with a discussion of entitative and post entitativethinking emphasizing the fact that both assume or even prescribes subject-object

    relations. The role of language in providing a nave reflection of the social world is

    central in this discussion and it is argued that in a third discourse of critical

    relational constructionism discourse, action and language are interwove and any

    subject-object relations are set aside, opening a radically changed agenda for

    inquiry.

    Further on, Adrian Neculau addresses the way in which the political

    context influences the development of social representations, illustrating their

    prescriptive role in specific social contexts. First of all, the author discusses the

    role of social context in the development of social representations and emphasizes

    the fact that social behavior is guided by the social representations developed in a

    particular context. Two aspects of social context are especially important in the

    development of social representations: first the global social context (a set of

    norms, beliefs or ideologies shared in a particular social group) and then the

    immediate social context (defined by the direct interaction of the individual with

    the social world). Further on, the author argues that once formed, social

    representations prescribe social behavior in specific social context; in this sense

    they are mechanisms of social conditioning. In order to support the above-

    mentioned theoretical arguments Adrian Neculau discusses in a meticulously

    designed case study the effects of the global social context (communist ideologies)

    on social representations and social practices.

    Finally, in the last paper of this special issue, Maurice Scheepens reports

    the results of an empirical study, investigating the way in which managers perceiveand create representations about an institutional change. The author uses a

    cognitive mapping technique to elicit and represent managerial cognitions relative

    to the phasing-out of the Multifiber Agreement (a situation of institutional change

    that affects the Indian textile industry). The study identifies the most relevant

    factors related to managerial cognitive complexity, and it provides insight into the

    possible outcomes of this concept (e.g. managerial decision-making effectiveness),

    drawing from the way in which it was conceptualized by Calori, Johnson and

    Sarnin (1994).

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    8/9

    Editorial: Making sense of social reality606

    REFERENCES

    Abric, J. C. (1993). Central system, peripheral system: Their functions and roles in the

    dynamics of social representations. Papers on Social Representations, 2, 75-78.

    Adolphs, R. (2003). Investigating the cognitive neuroscience of social behavior.

    Neuropsychologia, 41, 119-126.

    Ajzen, I. (2001). Nature and operation of attitudes.Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 27-

    58.

    Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed)Handbook of Social Psychology(p.

    798-884). Worchester, MA: Clark University Press.

    Ashkanasy, N.M., Hrtel, C.E.J. & Daus, C.S. (2002). Diversity and emotion: The new

    frontiers in organizational behavior research.Journal of Management, 28, 307-338.

    Cacioppo, J. T. Bernston, G. G., Lorig, T. S., Norris, C. T., Rickett, E. & Nusbaum, H.

    (2003). Just because youre imaging the brain, doesn t mean you can stop using

    your head: A primer and set of first principles. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 85, 650-661.

    Calori, R., Johnson, G., & Sarnin, P. (1994). CEOs cognitive maps and the scope of the

    organization. Strategic Management Journal. 15, 6, 437-457.

    Cannon-Bowers, J.A., & Salas, E. (2001). Reflections on shared cognition. Journal of

    Organizational Behaviour, 22, 195-202.

    Crano, W. D. & Prislin, R. (2006). Attitudes and persuasion. Annual Review of Psychology,

    57, 345-374.

    Fiske, S. T. (2000). Stereotyping, prejudice and discrimination at the seam between the

    centuries: Evolution, culture, mind and brain. European Journal of Social

    Psychology, 30, 299-322.Greenwald, A. G. & Banaji. M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem

    and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4-27.

    Greenwald, G. A., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., Mellott, D.

    S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-

    concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3-25.

    Hamilton, D. L. & Sherman, J. W. (1994). Stereotypes. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. K. Srull

    (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd

    ed, vol 2, pp. 1-68). Hilsdale, NJ:

    Elrbaum.

    Hilton, J. & Hippel, W. (1996). Stereotypes.Annual Review of Psychology, 47,237-271.

    Huguet, P. & Latane, B. (1996). Social representations as dynamic social impact. Journal

    of Communication, 46, 57-65.

    Judd, C. M. & Park, B. (1993). Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes.Psychological Review, 100, 109-128.

    Jung, C. G. (1981). The archetypes and the collective unconscious (Collected works of C.

    G. Jung, Vol. 9). Bollingen Series XX, Princeton, NJ.

  • 8/12/2019 Editorial Making Sense of Social Reality

    9/9

    Decembrie 2005 Cogniie, Creier, Comportament 607

    Klein, S. B., Rozendal, K. & Cosmides, L. (2002). A social-cognitive neuroscience analysis

    of the Self. Social Cognition, 20, 105-135.Macrae, C. N. & Bodehausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking categorically about

    others.Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93-120.

    Mohammed, S., & Dumville, B.C. (2001). Team mental models in a team knowledge

    framework: Expanding theory and measurement across disciplinary boundaries.

    Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 22,89-106.

    Moscovici, S. (1961). La psychoanalyse, son image et son public. Paris: Presses

    Universitaires de France.

    Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. In R. Farr and S.

    Moscovici (Eds.), Social representations (pp. 3-69). Paris: Cambridge University,

    Editions de la Maison des Sciences de lHomme.

    Moscovici, S. (1997). Social representations theory and social constructionism. Social

    representations mailing list (web document posted on 28 April 1997).

    http://psyberlink.flogiston.ru/internet/bits/mosc1.htm.

    Moscovici, S. (1998). The history and actuality of social representations. In U. Flick (Ed),

    The psychology of the social (pp. 209-247). Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press.

    Overwalle, van, F. & Jordens, K. (2002). An adaptive connectionist model of cognitive

    dissonance. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 204-231.

    Potter, J. & Edwards, D. (1999). Social representations and discursive psychology: From

    cognition to action. Culture and Psychology, 5, 447-458.

    Robertson, L., Kulik, C. T. & Pepper, M. B. (2001). Designing effective diversity training:

    influence of group composition and trainee experience. Journal of Organizational

    Behavior, 22,871-885.Rooy, van, D., Overwalle, van F., Labiouse, C & French, R. (2003). A reccurent

    connectionist model of group biases. Psychological Review, 110, 536-563.

    Schacter, D. L., Norman, K. A. & Koutstaal, W. (1998). The cognitive neuroscience of

    constructive memory.Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 289-318.

    Wetherell, M., McGhee, P. & Stevens, R. (2003). Defining Social Psychology. In R.

    Sapsford, A. Still, M. Wetherell, D. Miell and R. Stevens (Eds) Theory and Social

    Psychology. London: Sage Publications.

    Wood, W. (2000). Attitudes change: Persuasion and Social Influence. Annual Review of

    Psychology, 51, 539-570.