examining the effect of trust in leader

Upload: chika-yunindra

Post on 03-Jun-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    1/14

    Examining the effects of trust in leaders: A bases-and-foci approach

    Jixia Yang a,, Kevin W. Mossholder b,1

    a Department of Management, College of Business, City University of Hong Kong, Tat Chee Avenue, Kowloon, Hong Kongb Department of Management, College of Business, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama 36849, United States

    a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

    Little empirical research has explored the implications of different bases and foci of trust inleadership within the same study. To rectify this substantive void in the trust literature, we

    conceptualized variants of employee trust in organizational leadership in terms of two bases

    (cognitive and affective) and two foci (management and supervisor). Using this dual

    perspective, we examined the effects of trust in leaders on two work-related attitudes

    (organizational commitment, job satisfaction) and two work-related behaviors (in-role

    behavior, extra-role behavior). Using data collected from 210 employees and their

    supervisors, we found that the four variants of trust in organizational leadership were

    distinguishable, and had differential effects on employee outcomes. Affective trust in

    supervisor was a signicant predictor of in-role and extra-role behaviors. Affective trust in

    management and affective trust in supervisor explained variance in affective organizational

    commitment. Cognitive trust in management and affective trust in supervisor explained

    variance in job satisfaction.

    2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    Keywords:Trust in management

    Trust in supervisor

    Cognitive trust

    Affective trust

    1. Introduction

    Perceptions of trust are central to the process of effective organizational leadership (e.g., Pillai, Schriesheim, & Williams, 1999).

    In this process, leadership behaviors function to engender trust, which in turn, can greatly affect employees' work outcomes (Dirks

    & Skarlicki, 2004).Dened as a psychological state comprising a willingness to accept vulnerability based on positive expectations

    about the intentions or behavior of another (Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt, & Camerer, 1998), trust operates as a critical psychological

    mechanism in realizing leadership effectiveness. Perhaps because of complexities surrounding the concept of trust, the conditions

    under which the need for trust arises are agreed on only at a relatively high level of abstraction (Rousseau et al., 1998). This creates

    a research challenge, then, for a more precise understanding of how the enactment of trust contributes to leadership functioning.

    We believe that closer consideration of the basis for and effect of employees' willingness to become vulnerable to those occupying

    different positions in the leadership hierarchy is a critical step in meeting this challenge.

    That trust in leadership affects employee attitudes and behavior can be readily seen in the literature. What has been much less

    evidenced are the motives explaining why employees are willing to accept greater or lesser vulnerability to those in leadership

    positions. Theoretically, both cognitive and affective processes should be required for leadership effectiveness across a range of

    circumstances (Cremer, 2006; Ilies, Judge, & Wagner, 2006; Michie & Gooty, 2005). Trust has been considered an indicator of the

    psychological experiences underpinning employees' perceptions of leadership (Pillai et al., 1999), and has been argued to stem

    from both a cognitive base grounded on characteristics salient for task-related interactions, and an affective base grounded on

    The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

    The authors are grateful to Cheryl Adkins, Donald Ferrin, Kwok Leung, and Hettie Richardson for their constructive comments on the paper. An earlier version

    of the paper was presented at the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychologyannual conference, May 2006, Dallas.

    Corresponding author. Tel.: +852 3442 4750; fax: +852 3442 7220.

    E-mail addresses:[email protected](J. Yang),[email protected](K.W. Mossholder).1 Tel.: +1 334 844 6529; fax: +1 334 844 5159.

    1048-9843/$ see front matter 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.10.004

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    The Leadership Quarterly

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / l e a q u a

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.10.004http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/10489843http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/10489843http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.10.004mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    2/14

    socioemotional elements pertinent for interpersonal interactions (Dirks & Ferrin, 2002). Given that the two trust bases have rarely

    been examined together in the past, it is as yet unknown how subordinates' attitudes and behavior may be linked with cognitive

    versus affective trust. Relying on cognitive or affective trust represents divergent approaches used by leaders in building

    relationships with subordinates. It should be noted that some leadership perspectives, particularly leader member exchange

    theory (LMX), have considered the relational dynamics occurring between leaders and subordinates (Erdogan & Liden, 2002).

    However, within this literature, trust has been treated in an undifferentiated manner with respect to its cognitive and affective

    bases. Moreover, unlike LMX relationships, trust may not necessarily be mutual or reciprocal (Brower, Schoorman, & Tan, 2000;

    Schoorman, Mayer, & Davis, 2007). In sum, we believe that distinguishing between the two trust bases may provide a more

    nuanced examination of trust in organizational leaders.

    Given organizations' hierarchical nature, it is likely that employees will be subject to control and inuence from persons at more

    than oneorganizational level,andthe effects that leaders exert on employees may depend on the level from which inuenceoriginates

    (Shamir, 1995). Set within the framework of the present study, this notion suggests that employees may develop different degrees of

    cognitive or affective trust in leaders residing in different levels, and such trust could be associated with different employee outcomes.

    Thus, in addition to taking trust bases into account, we argue that considering in whom trust is placed (i.e., the focus of trust) could be

    instructive. An obvious means of differentiating between hierarchical leadership levels would be in terms of whether leaders are more

    proximal or distalwith regardto their subordinates. There is theoretical precedence forthis delineation.Researchersin theareasof both

    leadership and trust have noted that inuence exerted by immediate supervisors is qualitatively different from that exerted by higher

    level managers (e.g., House & Aditya, 1997; Schoorman et al., 2007). The former are involved in more task-related issues whereas the

    latter focus more on overall organizational concerns. Shamir (1995) has noted there are substantive differences between close versus

    distant charismatic leadership. One difference has to do with the magnitude of the importance of trust for the inuence of charismatic

    leadership to arise. In linewith Shamir's observations, we contend that the importance and effects of trust may vary with specied foci.

    Drawing on Lewin's (1943) eld theory, we argue that trust in the supervisor and trust in management may have differing importance

    depending on the types of employee-related outcomes (e.g., behavioral versus attitudinal) being considered.

    In short, two fundamental issuesthe impetus from which positive expectations are formed (i.e., trust's bases) and in whom

    positive expectations are placed (i.e., trust's foci)must be considered to more precisely understand processes and effectiveness

    associated with trust in organizational leaders. The need to consider trust foci and bases simultaneously when studying trust in

    leadership has been recognized only recently (Dirks & Ferrin, 2002; Dirks & Skarlicki, 2004). Addressing these issues is important

    because, as articulated in the following sections, we suggest that the four combination or variantsof trust in leaders formed by

    crossing affective and cognitive bases with supervisor and management foci may have divergent effects on salient employee

    outcomes. Uncertainties surrounding such effects need to be claried so that the signicance of trust can be better appreciated

    (Dirks & Skarlicki, 2004; Schoorman et al., 2007). Thus, we propose that explicitly recognizing and operationalizing these bases

    and foci together in one study may provide insights into trust's operational role in the context of organizational leadership.

    2. Bases and foci: Framing trust in organizational leaders

    Intention to accept vulnerability is rooted in the conceptualization of trust. When this acceptance is manifested as cognitive or

    affective trust in either the supervisor or management, differencesin employee attitudinal andbehavioral outcomes could accrue.As is

    explained below, we expect that high levels of trust should allow for more attentional resources to be devoted to the job, facilitating

    greater in-role (i.e., task) behavior and extra-role (i.e., helping) behavior (e.g., Colquitt, Scott, & LePine, 2007). With regard to work

    attitudes, individuals' organizational experiences of being willing to be vulnerable also has a directimpact on their jobsatisfaction and

    affective commitment to the organization (e.g.,Davy, Kinicki, & Scheck, 1997). Because it has been documented that trust in leaders

    may be generally associated withthese employee outcomes (Dirks & Ferrin, 2002; Dirks & Skarlicki, 2004), these work behaviors (i.e.,

    in-role task behavior and extra-role helping behavior) and attitudes (i.e., job satisfaction and affective organizational commitment)

    were chosen for purposes of investigation. Before offering the hypotheses and exploratory research questions, we provide a more

    complete explication of the bases-and-foci trust perspective.

    2.1. Bases of trust in organizational leaders

    Dueto theasymmetries of power andstatus inherentin hierarchicalrelationshipsbetweenemployeesand organizationalauthorities

    (Kramer, 1996), issues of vulnerability and dependency are particularly salient, which makes trust in leaders critical for enhancing

    positive employee behavior and attitudes at work. Trustworthiness attributions have a strong, widespread inuence upon people's

    reactions to leaders. Research shows that two distinctive psychological processes underlieemployees' trustworthiness attributions, one

    being instrumental in nature and the other being more relational (Tyler & Degoey, 1996). The former has been recognized as cognitive

    trust and focuses on another party's characteristics such as ability, dependability, and integrity (e.g., Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995),

    whereas the latter has been considered affective trust and derives more from personal bonds (McAllister, 1995). Leader behavior can

    help mitigate the risk of opportunism inherent in organizational contexts, thus making followers feel safe to be led by immediate

    supervisors and top managers (Whitener, Brodt, Korsgaard, & Werner, 1998). Specically, cognitive trust allows for comfortable task-

    related exchanges at work, such as work requests given and taken between supervisors and subordinates. Affective trust facilitates

    socioemotional communication including initiation and reciprocation of care and consideration (Dirks & Ferrin, 2002).

    Despite advances in conceptualizing trust and its relationships withother variables, there has been an imbalance in the amount ofempirical research devoted to the understanding of the two-base structure of trust ( Lewicki, Tomlinson, & Gillespie, 2006). To date,

    51J. Yang, K.W. Mossholde r / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    3/14

    less research has dealt with trust grounded in emotional bonds among interdependent individuals, despite evidence highlighting the

    importance of such bonds (McAllister, 1995; Webber & Klimoski, 2004). Considering the literature on trust in organizational

    leadership specically, a similar level of inattention on affective trust is also apparent. Although both bases of trust in leadership may

    be functional for engendering benecial outcomes in organizations, the literature has been skewed toward cognitive explanations

    (Schoormanet al.,2007). In the presentstudy, we attempt to address this research gapby directly examining the relative contribution

    of cognitive and affective trust in organizational leaders in predicting employee outcomes.

    2.2. Foci of trust in organizational leaders

    Trust is target-specic in that it varies according to the individual(s) with whom one is interacting. As it is not necessarily

    transferable across different targets, pinpointing the target of trust is critical for understanding its subsequent effects. In fact, Dirks

    and Parks (2003)argued that thoughtfully specifying the focus of trust is important, as trust in varying foci may be due to diverse

    factors and lead to different outcomes across conditions. Generally speaking, targets of trust can be a person, group, or rm.

    Likewise,Whitener (1997)suggested that employees can have trust in at least two different types of foci, one being specic

    individuals such as supervisors, and the other being generalized representatives, such as higher level management. Trust in one's

    supervisor is more immediate and circumscribed, whereas trust in management is more general.

    When management is the referent of interest in trust studies, it has been considered as an identiable upper-level collective whose

    actionshave a signicantimpacton nonmanagerialemployees (Mayer & Davis,1999). Examples of such collectives are: peoplewho make

    policies, rules, and major decisions in an organization (Scott, 1980); the group comprising an organization's strategic and operational

    executives, all of whom contribute directly to key business decisions (Albrecht & Travaglione, 2003); and those at the business division

    level (Spreitzer & Mishra, 2002). Because management is responsible for formulating strategies and allocating organizational resources,decisions and actions made by management have a broad impact on employees over time. In this regard, management's trustworthiness

    may affect whether employees buy into an organization's objectives and fully engage in management initiatives. On the other hand,

    decisions and actions by immediate supervisors tend to have greater inuence upon employees in daily operations.

    Both management and immediate supervisors create concerns for employees about being vulnerable in the hierarchical

    relationships. As such, these two foci are of prime importance in the discussion of trust in leaders. Leadership demonstrated either by

    immediate supervisors or by distant executives can have signicant implications for employee attitudes and behavior (e.g.,Avolio,

    Zhu, Koh, & Bhatia, 2004). Studies that examined both trust in management and immediate supervisors (e.g., Mayer & Gavin, 2005;

    Morgan & Zeffane, 2003; Tan & Tan, 2000) have offered evidence that employees distinguish between trustworthiness of immediate

    supervisors and management in general, and that each may affect employee outcomes.

    3. Predicting trust outcomes using bases and foci

    Field theory (Lewin, 1943) suggests that individuals react more strongly to psychologically proximate factors in the environmentthan to distal factors. Some researchers have suggested that more immediate behavioral demands are best addressed through timely

    responses appropriate to those creating the demands (e.g., Brandes, Dharwadkar, & Wheatley, 2004). Considering the two types of

    leaders in the present study, it is obvious that employees could respond more appropriately to ones with whom they work than ones

    with whom they have infrequent or indirect contact. The immediate supervisor is physically and interpersonally closer to employees

    than is top management. Thus, trust in the supervisor (whether cognitive or affective), rather than trust in management, should be

    associated with employees' in-role and extra-role behaviors. We further discuss below the potentially unique inuences of cognitive

    and affective trust in supervisor on such employee behaviors.

    3.1. Trust in the supervisor and work behavior

    In-role behavior consists of behaviors that are recognized by formal reward systems and xed by job descriptions (Williams &

    Anderson, 1991). Trust in supervisor has been shown to impact in-role behavior which directly benets the supervisor (Aryee,

    Budhwar, & Chen, 2002). Completing work goals entails engaging in task-related exchanges with the supervisor, which makes asubordinate vulnerable to the supervisor's decisions and actions for task accomplishment. Accordingly, it appears that having

    cognitive, as opposed to affective, trust could reduce the diversion of task-related attention and effort due to unnecessary worry and

    anxiety. Focused use of one's attentional resources is critical for engagement and subsequent task completion (Mayer & Gavin, 2005).

    When characteristics such as ability, reliability, and integrity are attributed to immediate supervisors, employees should be able to

    moreefcientlyfocustheir attentionalresources on behaviors fortask goal attainment.Conversely,employeeshavinglittle condence

    in the immediate supervisor's knowledge and skills in handling task-related problems would be unlikely to comply with the

    supervisor's directives and requests without some second-guessing.

    Hypothesis 1. Of the four variants of trust in leaders, cognitive trust in the supervisor will be most strongly related to in-role

    behavior.

    Extra-role behavior has been identied as an important form of citizenship behavior that contributes to the maintenance and

    enhancement of the social context that supports task performance (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Paine, & Bachrach, 2000), so it isimportant to organizations when work roles are interdependent and employees' cooperation can facilitate overall functioning

    52 J. Yang, K.W. Mossholder / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    4/14

    (Van Dyne & LePine, 1998). Like in-role behavior, task-oriented extra-role helping has been suggested to benet the supervisor

    (Aryee et al., 2002) and potentially the organization, even though it may be targeted at coworkers and other peers in the

    organization. Employees engage in this form of extra-role helping as they assist others in their jobs whenever the situation arises,

    such as when high workloads and unresolved problems are at hand (Settoon & Mossholder, 2002). It has been argued that

    supervisory trustworthiness can contribute to creating a safe social atmosphere at work. In particular, highly considerate

    leadership can facilitate coworkers' interactions and group cohesion (Burke et al., 2006). As such leader actions are likely to trigger

    affective trust as a response, we expect affective, as opposed to cognitive, trust in the supervisor to be effectual in encouraging

    employees feel comfortable and willing to assist one another.

    Hypothesis 2. Of the four variants of trust in leaders, affective trust in the supervisor will be most strongly related to extra-role

    behavior.

    3.2. Trust in leaders and work attitudes

    Persons occupying positions of organizational leadership are expected to inuence employee attitudes, as they shape any number

    of employee workplace experiences over time. Interactions with the supervisor are part of the more proximate job environment, and

    events arising in the broader work environment are likely to be connected with management. Arguably, the trust associated with

    either the immediate leader or distant leaders could inuence important work attitudes such as job satisfaction and organizational

    commitment. Job satisfaction and organizational commitment have been considered both supervisor- and organization-related

    outcomes (Spector, 1997). Accordingly, we believe both of these work attitudes could be affected by trusting relationships associated

    with the supervisor and management. Therefore, given the inadequate documentation with respect to the unique effects exerted by

    cognitive versus affective trust upon job satisfaction and organizational commitment, we decided to examine trust's relations with

    these attitudes in an exploratory manner.

    Job satisfaction has been increasingly portrayed as a combination of one's cognitive beliefs and affective work experiences (e.g.,

    Schleicher, Watt, & Greguras, 2004; Weiss, Nicholas, & Daus, 1999), meaning that both cognitive evaluation and affective experiences

    may affectit. Carr, Schmidt, Ford, and DeShon's (2003) meta-analysis reported that both affective and instrumental facets of the work

    environment are importantfor jobsatisfaction.Thisobservationsomewhat suggests that both affective trust andcognitive trust might

    incrementally explain job satisfaction. A recent meta-analysis (Dirks & Ferrin, 2002) showed that cognitive trust correlated with job

    satisfaction, but did not address the impact of affective trust on job satisfaction because a sufcient number of affective trust studies

    were not available. On the other hand, trust across each of the two foci should explain some variance in job satisfaction. Evidence has

    been found supporting the explanatory power of trust in both immediate supervisors and management when examined in separate

    studies (e.g., Aryee et al., 2002; Pillai et al., 1999). Because of its composite nature, overall job satisfaction is unlikely to be sufciently

    explained by any single source or referent (Fisher, 1980). Taken together, cognizant of the possibility that each base of trust may be

    associated with either the supervisor or management, we seek to answer the following question:

    Research question 1: Of the four variants of trust in leaders, which will be more strongly related to job satisfaction?

    Affective commitment reects the strength of an individual's identication with and involvement in an organization (Porter,

    Steers, Mowday, & Boulian, 1974). Leaders' trustworthiness in an organization makes employees believe that they intend to seek

    employees' best interest and act effectively on employees' behalf. With regard to our four trust variants, the literature provides an

    incomplete picture of their possible association with commitment. Meyer and Herscovitch (2001)have proposed that variables

    contributing to the likelihood that an individual desires being involved in actions relevant to an organization and derives his or her

    identity from organizational afliation, will contribute to the development of affective commitment to the organization. In view of

    their proposal, either cognitive or affective trust vested in both foci could be capable of enhancing such commitment. Dirks and

    Ferrin's meta-analysis (2002)showed that cognitive trust was related to affective organizational commitment, but their meta-

    analysis could not address the impact of affective trust on this form of commitment. Trust in management ( Konovsky &

    Cropanzano, 1991) and trust in immediate manager or supervisor (Simons, Friedman, Liu, & Parks, 2007) have been found to

    correlate with affective commitment. Unfortunately neither of these studies included separate measures of affective and cognitive

    trust. Because the affective commitment research stream contains too many uncertainties regarding relations of the four trust

    variants with the employee outcomes examined, we therefore pose the following question:

    Research question 2: Of the four variants of trust in leaders, which will be more strongly related to affective organizational

    commitment?

    4. Methods

    4.1. Organizational context

    Data were collected from a state-operated medical healthcare system in the southeastern United States. The state system had been

    increasingly facing the demands of shrinking budgets and cost containment. The two locations from which we collected data were

    within 70 miles of eachother.The human resources directors at each location had cooperated withone another to improve service by

    eliminating unnecessary redundancy across the hospitals. For example, some clinics in the communities served by the hospitals had

    been consolidated. The delivery of certain services was restructured in some cases. And

    nally, some employees were also asked tochange shifts or locations at which services were provided. Thus, it was likely thateither directly or indirectly, most study participants

    53J. Yang, K.W. Mossholde r / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    5/14

    experienced some uncertainty due to structural changes and retrenchment. These conditions made their vulnerability a salient

    factor and trust an important consideration in the work setting. Through meetings and organization newsletters, the top system

    administrator and human resource managers at the medical centers encouraged employee involvement in the survey. Aggregated

    survey results fed back to employees was offered as a potential benet to encourage greater overall participation.

    At both locations,the natureof work required individuals within departments to interact on a frequent basis and coordinate closely

    with the supervisor to complete requisite tasks. Also, we determined through interviews with the human resource managers that

    managementwas an identiable group of people (i.e., division directors) that employees distinguished from direct supervisors,

    suggesting no confusion as to who comprised this group. Departments from the medical centers that participated in the survey

    included nance and billing services, information technology, human resource services, facility services, and a variety of clinical units

    (e.g., radiology, intensive care, emergency, and nursing). Employees who participated in the study included registered and practical

    nurses, lab technicians, pharmacists, data entry personnel, food service workers, accounting specialists, and maintenance personnel.

    The variability of employee jobs allowed the survey to capture a range of individual perceptions and workplace behaviors.

    4.2. Sample and procedure

    Employees self-reported data on all study variables except for in-role behavior and extra-role behavior, which were evaluated

    by their immediate supervisors. An employee identication number was used to match subordinate responses with those of their

    supervisors. Supervisory surveys were dispersed at the managers' council meetings, and employee surveys were distributed by

    each supervisor after council meetings. Prior to the managers' council meetings, the survey project was publicized through email

    announcements and the medical center newsletter. A cover letter accompanying the survey explained the purpose of the survey

    and guaranteed condentiality. Respondents had the choice to return completed questionnaires at a designated collection box or

    mail them to the researchers using postage-paid envelopes. A majority of the completed questionnaires were returned via mail.

    In all, surveys were distributed to 864 employees and 96 supervisors. We received 353 completed employee surveys for a

    response rate of 41% and completed surveys from 47 supervisors for a response rate of 49%. On average, the survey respondents

    had been working at their locations for 8 years and were 43 years old. About 82% of the survey respondents were female. Based on

    human resource data from the healthcare system (average age= 43 years old, 80% were female; average organizational

    tenure=10 years), it was determined that respondents did not differ from the overall employee population in age, gender, and

    years of service.

    Of the 353 employee self-reported surveys, 210 had matched supervisory ratings. The average supervisory span of control among

    the matched cases was 5.12. Chi-square tests and independent samples t-tests were performed on respondent demographic variables

    and on behavioral and attitudinal outcomes betweenthe respondents who had matched supervisory ratings (n=210)and those who

    did not (n=143). No signicant differences were detected. Of the 210 surveyed employees, the average age was 42, and 84% were

    female. Seventy-three percent were Caucasian and 27% were African-American. In terms of education, 33% had high school degrees,

    22% had some college or vocational degrees, and 44% had college or graduate degrees. Subsequent analyses used the 210 employee

    responses having matched supervisory ratings.

    4.3. Measures

    A ve-point response format was used for the instruments in this study, ranging from 1 ( strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly

    agree). Employees responded to the survey questions including the four types of trust in leaders, affective organizational com-

    mitment, and job satisfaction. Their corresponding supervisors provided the ratings for in-role behavior and extra-role behavior.

    4.3.1. Trust variants

    The few studies distinguishing between the cognitive and affective trust have adapted McAllister's (1995)measure. However,

    more work needed to be done regarding a measure of trust using a bases-and-foci framework. The measure developed by McAllister

    (1995) was for trust between peer managers. In ourresearch, a measure thatcould reect trusting relationships withinorganizational

    hierarchy would be more suitable. To this end, we developed a trust instrument estimating the two substantive bases (cognitive andaffective)and two foci (management and immediate supervisor) in a pilot study. Therst step was to generate an initial pool of items

    measuring trust. Using denitional guidelines of cognitive and affective trust (Dirks & Ferrin, 2002; McAllister, 1995), we developed

    new items and adaptedotheritems fromthe literature for use in our study(Clark & Payne,1997; Cook & Wall, 1980; Johnson-George &

    Swap, 1982; Kim & Mauborgne, 1993; Luo, 2002; Mayer & Davis, 1999; McAllister, 1995; Mishra & Mishra, 1994; Rempel, Holmes, &

    Zanna, 1985; Rousseau & Tijoriwala, 1999; Scott, 1980). An initial pool of 44 items measuring trust was generated, with half of them

    being expected to tap into each base.

    Next, these items were tested for content validity. Seven organizational behavior faculty, ten graduate students, and eight

    nonacademics were asked to classify the items as tapping cognitive, affective, both, or neither base of trust, according to specied

    characteristics of cognitive and affective trust. To select items that appropriately sampled the content domain, we allowed only two

    dissenting votes among the evaluations.As a result,the itempool was reduced to a setof eight items capturing cognitive trust and a set

    of eight items assessing affective trust. Within bothsets, items were appropriately worded to refer to the two foci (the supervisor and

    management). This process resulted in a total of 32 items.

    The 32 trust items were used in a pilot survey involving a total of 178 participants. Of the participants, 19 were executive MBAstudents, 16 were employees at a local rm, and the rest were traditional MBA students from three universities located in the

    54 J. Yang, K.W. Mossholder / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    6/14

    southeastern U.S. An exploratory factor analysis was performed on the pilot data. Because of the moderate intercorrelations

    between trust variants, principle axis factoring with an oblimin rotation was performed (cf. Hair, Anderson, Tatham, & Black,

    1998). Factor loadings were examined to determine which items would be used in the nal survey. Using the criterion of an

    eigenvalue greater than one, four factors were extracted which explained 74% of the variance. Guidelines from Hair et al. (1998)

    were adopted for interpreting factor loadings: Factor loadings of .45 or above are considered signicant when sample size falls

    between 150 and 200. After examining these factors for low factor loadings or substantial cross-loadings, we retained a total of 20

    items (ve for each trust base/foci variation). A second principal axis factor analysis with oblimin rotation was then performed on

    these 20 items. Factor loadings ranged from .48 to .85, and the four-factor solution accounted for 77% of the variance. Overall, the

    factor structure improved with these 20 items, and they were retained for usein thenal survey. Complete results of pretesting are

    available from the rst author. The 20 items comprising the variants of trust in organizational leaders are shown in the Appendix.

    All ve-item measurescognitive trust in management (=.95), affective trust in management (=.94), cognitive trust in

    supervisor (=.94), and affective trust in supervisor (=.95)demonstrated acceptable reliability.

    4.3.2. In-role behavior

    A three-item measure (=.95) was utilized for supervisors to assess employees' in-role behavior. Two items from Williams

    and Anderson (1991)were usedFullls responsibilities specied in job description,and Performs tasks that are expected of

    him/her,along with a third item assessing broad in-role behavior Performs his/her job well.

    4.3.3. Extra-role helping behavior

    Supervisors rated their subordinates' task-oriented extra-role helping behavior. In comparison to an employee's coworkers,

    supervisors could better observe such behavior across a greater number of contexts, and should be more interested in such

    behavior because it can contribute directly to success in the overall task environment. The instrument estimating task-focused

    citizenship behavior directed toward coworkers developed bySettoon and Mossholder (2002)was used. Person-focused helping

    was not assessed for the study because it is more germane for the study of personal trust relationships among coworkers, a topic

    beyond the scope of the present study. Coefcient alpha was .96 for the ve-item measure of extra-role helping. Sample items

    included items such as Takes on extra responsibilities in order to help coworkers when things get demanding at work, and

    Helps coworkers with difcult assignments, even when assistance is not directly requested.

    4.3.4. Affective organizational commitment

    Affective commitment was measured by a ve-item scale (=.89) developedby Meyer and Allen (1984). A sample item was I

    would be very happy to spend the rest of my career with this organization.

    4.3.5. Job satisfaction

    A three-item measure of overall job satisfaction (=.83, Hackman & Oldham, 1975) was used. Items were All things

    considered, I am satised with my job, I like my job,and I am generally satised with the work I do in this job.

    4.3.6. Demographic variables

    People with different backgrounds may vary in their propensity to trust ( Mayer et al., 1995). As such, demographic variables,

    including age, gender, race, and education were collected. Gender was coded as 0=male and 1=female. Race was coded

    0=Caucasian and 1=African-American. Education was coded into four categories (1=High school degree, 2=Some college,

    3=College degree, 4=Graduate degree). In addition, it was reasonable to expect that trust relationships at work might also

    depend on the amount of time individuals have known and worked with one another ( Becerra & Gupta, 2003). Therefore, tenure

    (in years) with the current organization, supervisor, and work unit were also collected in the survey.

    4.3.7. Marker variable

    Self-reported data raise concerns about the potential effects of common method variance (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, &

    Podsakoff, 2003) on construct relationships under examination. Marker variable partial correlational analysis (Lindell & Whitney,2001) was performed to determine the presence of common method variance (CMV) in the current study. Creative self-efcacy,

    dened as the belief that one has the abilityto produce creative outcomes (Tierney & Farmer, 2002), servedas the marker variable.

    This three-item measure met criteria proposed by Lindell and Whitney, that is, being theoretically unrelated with other variables

    of interest, similar in format and number of items, novel in content, specic in denition, and having high reliability. A sample

    creative self-efcacy (=.81) item was I have condence in my ability to solve problems creatively.

    5. Results

    The means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations of all study variables are reported in Table 1. Within each of

    the trust foci, levels of cognitive trust were higher than levels of affective trust. According to the correlation matrix, none of

    the outcome variables were greatly inuenced by tenure-related differences. Thus, the tenure variables were excluded

    from subsequent analyses. A conrmatory factor analysis was used to assess the properties of a measurement model including the

    eight substantive study variables, speci

    cally four trust variants, in-role behavior, extra-role behavior, affective organizationalcommitment, and job satisfaction. All of the indicators loaded signicantly on their corresponding latent constructs. Overall, the

    55J. Yang, K.W. Mossholde r / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    7/14

    measurement model t the data well, as indicated by several t statistics (2=1086.01, df=566, pb .001, RMSEA=.068,

    NFI= .95, CFI= .97).

    5.1. Examining the four trust variants

    Conrmatory factor analysis (CFA) using LISREL8.53 (Jreskog & Srbom, 1996) was conducted rst to assess the t ofthefour-

    factor trust model as well as alternative models. Because items evoke a response toward both bases and foci of trust, it is difcult to

    know whether respondents reacted to foci, bases, or a combination of both. Five possible models by specifying trust foci and

    bases separately (cognitive and affective bases for supervisor and management foci, Model I), by collapsing affective and cognitive

    bases (Model II), by collapsing supervisor and management foci (Model III), by collapsing cognitive and affective bases of trust in

    management (Model IV), and by collapsing cognitive and affective bases of trust in supervisor (Model V) were assessed to

    determine the best tting model. In these ve nested models, the management set of items and the supervisor set of items only

    differed in the referent. As such, in recognition of the fact that each indicator might have indicator-specic variation as well as

    measurement error, we allowed the error terms of each pair of trust items re

    ecting either management or the supervisor referentto correlate.

    As shown inTable 2, the four-factor model had the lowest chi-square (2) value in relation to its degrees of freedom. Chi-

    square difference tests indicated that the four-factor model by specifying trust foci and bases was superior to the two-factor model

    by collapsing trust bases (2=338.05, df= 5,pb .01), to the two-factor model by collapsing trust foci (2=1950.60,df= 5,

    pb .01), to the three-factor model by collapsing trust bases for management (2=219.19, df= 3, pb .01), and to the three-

    factor model by collapsing trust bases for supervisor (2=123.02, df= 3,pb .01). In addition, values for the RMSEA, NFI, and

    CFI indicated that the model differentiating trust by two foci and two bases emerged as the best tting model.

    From the latent construct correlation (phi) matrix, the high correlations between the two bases of trust in management

    (=.81) and in supervisor (=.80) raised discriminant validity concerns between cognitive and affective bases of trust. The

    above nested model comparison results lent some support that the two bases of trust were distinguishable either with supervisor

    referent or with management referent. In addition, following the suggestions byFornell and Larcker (1981), we further examined

    the discriminant validity of the two pairs of latent constructs (cognitive trust and affective trust in management, cognitive trust

    and affective trust in supervisor). The variance extracted was computed to assess the amount of variance accountedin the items bythe underlying construct with measurement error taken into consideration. Firstly, a value of .50 or higher is commonly used as a

    Table 1

    Descriptive statistics and scale intercorrelations.

    Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

    1. Age 42.1 10.79

    2. Gender .84 .37 .003

    3. Race .27 .44 .06 .11

    4. Education 2.14 .93 .07 .07 .32

    5. Tenure with

    organization

    8.0 7.84 .44 .09 .10 .02

    6. Tenure with

    unit

    6.2 6.86 .33 .04 .03 .04 .65

    7. Tenure with

    supervisor

    3.4 4.44 .24 .03 .02 .01 .39 .63

    8. Cognitive

    trust in

    management

    3.22 .98 .07 .15 .13 .18 .11 .04 .02

    9. Affective

    trust in

    management

    2.91 1.03 .02 .10 .10 .08 .16 .05 .05 .79

    10. Cognitive

    trust in

    supervisor

    3.95 .93 .11 .01 .18 .10 .02 .05 .003 .23 .24

    11. Affective

    trust in

    supervisor

    3.79 1.06 .11 .05 .21 .10 .02 .003 .04 .26 .34 .85

    12. Affective

    commitment

    3.36 .98 .20 .04 .15 .07 .04 .10 .13 .33 .42 .39 .48

    13. In-role

    behavior

    4.19 .63 .13 .16 .13 .24 .01 .002 .02 .01 ..04 .10 .16 .04

    14. Extra-role

    behavior

    3.87 .82 .06 .06 .14 .21 .02 .02 .02 .01 .08 .28 .33 .12 .64

    15. Job

    satisfaction

    4.03 .80 .21 .04 .03 .05 .001 .04 .10 .44 .41 .39 .46 .69 .07 .01

    16. Creative

    self-efcacy

    4.02 .63 .03 .05 .06 .29 .002 .07 .06 .06 .03 .18 .23 .25 .22 .18 .18

    Note. Pairwise sample size ranged from 180 to 210. pb .05. pb .01.

    56 J. Yang, K.W. Mossholder / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    8/14

    threshold of acceptability (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). Secondly, if the squared correlation between the two latent constructs (2)

    was smaller than variance extracted estimates of the two constructs, evidence of discriminant validity would exist.

    Variance extracted estimates for cognitive trust in management (.75) and for affective trust in management (.72) were larger

    than .50, and both were larger than the squared correlation between cognitive and affective trust in management (2=.66).

    Likewise, variance extracted estimates for cognitive trust in supervisor (.76) and for affective trust in supervisor (.73) were larger

    than .50, and both were larger than the squared correlation between cognitive and affective trust in supervisor (2= .64). Thus,

    these results suggested sufcient differentiation between cognitive and affective trust constructs across the two foci examined in

    our study. In summary, trust bases and foci were found to be distinguishable.

    5.2. Testing hypotheses

    Multiple regression analysis was one means used to test the hypotheses. Given problems of interpreting regression coefcients

    when predictor variables are intercorrelated, we calculated relative importance indexes (LeBreton, Binning, Adorno, & Melcher,

    2004) to determine the unique contribution of each of the four trust variants in predicting outcome variables. The concept of

    relative importance refers to the contribution a variable makes to the prediction of a dependent variable considering its effect

    alone and in combination with other predictors (Johnson, 2004). The relative importance statistic epsilon (or relative importance

    weight) has proved effective in identifying important predictors and decomposing model R-square information among competing

    predictors (LeBreton et al., 2004). Relative weights are calculated by (a) transforming the original predictors to obtain the set of

    orthogonal variables that have the highest degree of one-to-one correspondence with the original predictors, (b) relating the

    orthogonal variables to the criterion, (c) relating the orthogonal variables back to the original predictors, and (d) combining the

    information to yield a set of weights reecting the relative contribution each predictor makes to the model when considered by

    itself and in the context of the other predictors (Johnson, 2004, p. 284). To compute the relative weights, we used the multiple

    regression procedure in SPSS 13.0.

    A series of multiple regression analyses were conducted with all the trust variants being entered as a set as independent

    variables. The results of these tests are reported inTables 3 and 4. Demographic variables that were signicantly correlated with

    the dependent variables under examination were enteredrst in the equation. The relative importance analyses were conducted

    to supplement the regression results. Relative importance weights for the control variables and the trust variables entered in the

    regression equation at step 2 are reported in the tables. Because relative importance weights sum to the model R-square, they

    were rescaled and expressed as percentage ofR-square. This allowed the relative importance weights to be interpreted as a

    proportion of the modelR-square attributed to each predictor.

    Hypothesis 1 wastestedby regressing in-role behavior onto the control variables (i.e.,genderand education)and thetrust variants.

    The regression model explained about 13% of the variance of in-role behavior (F=4.55,pb .001). Contrary to Hypothesis1, only

    affective trust in supervisor was a signicant predictor (=.29, pb .05, relative weights index=20%). Note the R-square change

    Table 2

    Fit indexes ofve trust measurement models.

    Measurement Models 2 df RMSEA NFI CFI

    Model I-4 Factor by specifying trust foci and trust bases 377.31 154 .08 .96 .98

    Model I I- 2 Factor by collapsing the two bases across tru st foci 715.34 159 .15 .93 .95

    Model III-2 Factor by collapsing the two foci across trust bases 2327.91 159 .37 .78 .79

    Model IV-3 Factor by collapsing the two bases of trust in management 596.50 157 .13 .94 .96

    Model V-3 Factor by collapsing the two bases of trust in supervisor 500.33 157 .11 .95 .97

    Note. pb .01.

    Table 3

    Multiple regression and relative importance analysis results for in-role behavior and extra-role behavior.

    Dependent variables

    In-role behavior Extra-role behavior

    Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2

    Control variables Control variables

    Gender .18 .20(25.3%) Education .21 .16(24.2%)

    Education .28 .28(46.6%)

    Cognitive trust in management .04 (0.8%) Cognitive trust in management .11 (2.1%)

    Affective trust in management .002 (1.0%) Affective trust in management .05 (3.1%)

    Cognitive trust in supervisor .20 (6.4%) Cognitive tru st in sup er visor .001 (26.0%)

    Affective trust in supervisor .29(19.9%) Affective trust in supervisor .33(44.6%)

    Fstatistic 10.93 4.55 Fstatistic 9.15 6.77

    R2 .11 .13 R2 .04 .15

    AdjustedR2 .096 .10 AdjustedR2 .04 .13

    R2 .11 .03 R2 .04 .10

    Note. Standardized regression coefcients and relative weights (in the parentheses) are reported. N=210. pb .05. pb .01. pb .001.

    57J. Yang, K.W. Mossholde r / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    9/14

    was not signicant, but one of the independent variables was (affective trust in supervisor). This type of result can occur when other

    independent variables included in the model have trivial effects on the criterion variable, thereby lowering the overall R-square

    (Cohen,Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). When extra-role behavior was the criterion variable, 15% of the total variance was explained by

    the regression model (F=6.77, pb .001). In support of Hypothesis2, affective trust in supervisor (=.33,pb .05, relative weights

    index=45%) was found to be signicantly related to extra-role behavior, after the effect of education was controlled.

    To investigate Research Question 1, affective organizational commitment was regressed onto the four trust variants. After

    controlling for the effects of age and race, affective organizational commitment was signicantly associated with affective trust in

    management (=.33, pb .01) and affective trust in supervisor (=.43, pb .01). The regression model accounted for a total

    amount of 35% of the variance in affective commitment (F=15.81, pb .001). Of this 35%, affective trust in management and

    affective trust in supervisor accounted for about 27% and 31%, respectively. In examining Research Question 2, the regression

    model accounted for a total of 34% of the variance in job satisfaction (F=20.41,pb .001). Of the four variants of trust in leadership,

    cognitive trust in management (=.27,p b .01, relative weights index=25%) and affective trust in supervisor (=.33,p b .01,

    relative weights index=29%) incrementally explained the variance in job satisfaction when the effect of age was controlled.

    5.3. Assessment of CMV

    Common method variance (CMV) was assessed using the marker variable technique and a latent variable approach (Podsakoff

    et al., 2003). According toLindell and Whitney (2001), the smallest observed correlation between the marker variable and any

    other substantive variable that is theoretically irrelevant is assumed to be due to CMV. A prescribed computational technique was

    used to adjust zero-order correlations by partialing out the correlation value reecting CMV from the correlations between any

    two substantive variables. The correlation value chosen to reect CMV was the one having smallest value (close to 0) between

    correlations of the marker variable with any substantive variable in this study. Two types of partial correlations were computed

    using formulas developed by Lindell and Whitney (2001): corrected correlations after partialing out CMV, and disattenuated

    partial correlations adjusted for scale reliability.

    Correlations between creative self-efcacy, the marker variable in this study (M=4.02, SD =.63), and other substantive

    variables were uniformly low. As perLindell and Whitney (2001), we chose the smallest positive correlation coefcient involving

    creative self-efcacy with affective trust in management (r=.03) for use in the partial correlation adjustment procedure. Partial

    correlation adjustments were then made on the intercorrelations of trust with affective organizational commitment and job

    satisfaction as well as on the intercorrelations between job satisfaction and commitment and between in-role behavior and extra-

    behavior.The original correlations, the corrected correlations after removing CMV, and the disattenuated partial correlations after

    adjusting for scale reliability are reported inTable 5. The corrected correlation coefcients were still statistically signicant after

    CMV was controlled.Table 5also shows that the disattenuated partial correlations were slightly higher than the corresponding

    partial correlations, a nding consistent with what has been observed by Lindell and Whitney (2001). Although the corrected

    correlation coefcients were slightly lower than the original ones, the differences were not substantive and suggested that CMV

    effects did not affect study results.

    We also used a latent variable approach to estimate the inuence of common method bias. In this approach, a single latent

    methodfactor is added to the measurement model, and all items are allowed to load on the substantive constructs as well as on the

    method factor. This approach does not require researchers to identify sources of method bias in a given study ( Podsakoff et al.,

    2003). Although model t was indeed improved by adding the method factor (2=158.16, df=36,pb .01), partitioning of the

    variance indicated that the method factor accounted for only 5.82% of the total variance. Additionally, the 90% condence internal

    for RMSEA of the model with the method factor (.055, .068) overlapped with that of the model without the method factor (.062,

    .074). Thus, the con

    rmatory factor analysis results were consistent with the marker variable technique result. Taken together,CMV was not a problem in the present study.

    Table 4

    Multiple regression and relative importance analysis results for affective organizational commitment and job satisfaction.

    Dependent variables

    Organizational commitment Job satisfaction

    Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2

    Control variable Control variable

    Age .17 .13(7.9%) Age .21 .15(8.9%)

    Race .13 .08 (4.9%)

    Cognitive trust in management .02 (12.2%) Cognitive trust in management .27(24.5%)

    Affective trust in management .33 (26.8%) Affective trust in management .09 (19.3%)

    Cognitive trust in supervisor .07 (17.0%) Cognitive trust in supervisor .004 (18.1%)

    Affective trust in supervisor .43(31.1%) Affective trust in supervisor .33(29.3%)

    Fstatistic 4.47 15.81 Fstatistic 9.29 20.41

    R2 .05 .35 R2 .04 .34

    AdjustedR2 .04 .32 AdjustedR2 .04 .32

    R2 .05 .30 R2 .04 .30

    Note. Standardized regression coefcients are reported. Relative weights are in the parentheses. N=210. *pb .05. **pb .01. ***pb .001.

    58 J. Yang, K.W. Mossholder / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    10/14

    5.4. Supplemental analysis using HLM

    In this study, the participating individuals were not entirely independent from one another, as they were nested within

    supervisors. As noted earlier, an average ratio of a supervisor to subordinates was ve. We re-examined the trust-outcome

    relationships using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM, Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), an analytic tool dealing with nested,

    interdependent samples like ours by decomposing variance of the dependent variable into individual level sources and higher

    level sources. We ran HLM four times at a lower level model, each time including one outcome variable and a set of four trust

    predictors. In such HLM models, the individual level relationships were considered after higher level sources variance was

    controlled. The relationship strength between each trust variant and outcome variables revealed from the four HLM model results

    was the same as from the multiple regression analyses. Hence, we are condent the study ndings were not affected by

    interdependencies in our sample.

    6. Discussion

    To more completely understand the potential consequences of trust in leaders within organizational contexts, we took a ner-

    grained approach to studying employee trust by explicitly recognizing two bases and two foci of trust in leadership. We framed

    trust bases as being cognitive and affective, and categorized trust foci into management and immediate supervisors. The study

    results demonstrated that the four variants of employee trust in leaders did not function equivalently.

    Consistent with proximal and distal tenets ofeld theory, the current study indicated that relations of the trust variants withoutcome variables varied somewhat when outcomes were segregated into behavioraland attitudinalcategories. Affective trust in

    supervisor appeared more important to the accomplishment of behavioral outcomes (i.e., in-role and extra-role behavior). This

    nding underscores the importance of interpersonal interactions with the supervisor for motivating and energizing positive work

    behavior on the part of subordinates. In contrast, results also revealed that trustworthiness attributed to both direct supervisors and

    upper management was important for predicting attitudinal outcomes. Specically, affective trust in management and affective trust

    in supervisor signicantly predicted affective organizational commitment, and a combination of cognitive trust in management and

    affective trust in supervisor was predictive of employees' overall job satisfaction. To our knowledge, this study is the rst to examine

    and discover that certain combinations of trust bases and foci may be more inuential than other combinations, depending on the

    outcomes of interest. This may provide guidance for organizations to better leverage the effects of trust within the constraints of

    organizational resources.

    The relative importance technique was used to supplement regular regression analysis results. Although research on relative

    importance methodology is still in its infancy, it has progressed in recent years and relative importance statistics are being

    reported more frequently (e.g.,Johnson, 2004). It should be noted that we can only make qualitative inferences from the relativeimportance technique results. Thus, we cannot conclude whether one trust variant would be more statistically signicant than

    another trust variant for predicting outcomes considered in the study.

    6.1. Implications for research and practice

    The utility of separating the bases and foci of trust in leaders when considering varying outcome variables is underscored by

    this study. First, empirical evidence was provided for a two-base structure of trust within organizational contexts. Research on

    trust has been dominated by cognition-oriented propositions, while affect-oriented propositions have been given less attention

    (Schoorman et al., 2007). The current study suggested that affective trust may be of considerable importance in a workplace where

    care, concern, and nurturance are part of the normal organizational culture. Not surprisingly, affective trust in management

    emerged as a signicant predictor for affective commitment to the organization. Moreover, affective, rather than cognitive, trust in

    supervisor proved to be important for predicting outcomes of affective commitment, job satisfaction, and in-role and extra-rolebehavior.

    Table 5

    Assessment of common method variance.

    Originalr Corrected r Disattenuated r

    Cognitive trust in managementaffective organi zational commitment .33 .31 .33

    Affective trust in managementaffective organi zational commitment .42 .40 .44

    Cognitive trust in supervisoraffective organizational commitment .39 .37 .40

    Affective trust in supervisoraffective organizational commitment .48 .46 .51

    Cognitive trust in managementjob satisfacti on .44 .42 .47

    Affective trust in managementjob satisfacti on .41 .39 .44

    Cognitive trust in supervisorjob sat isfaction .39 .37 .42

    Affective trust in supervisorjob sat isfaction .46 .44 .50

    Job sat isfactionaffective organizational commitment .69 .68 .80

    In-role behaviorextra-role behavior .64 .63 .66

    Note. PairwiseN=210;rdenotes the zero-order correlation coefcient. **pb .01.

    59J. Yang, K.W. Mossholde r / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    11/14

    The strong effects of affective trust in this study were not evident in Dirks and Ferrin's (2002) meta-analysis of trust in leaders and

    employee outcomes. Of course, this was expected in that they could not directly examine affective trust in their analysis because a

    sufcient number of affective trust studies were not available in the existing literature. The impact of affective trust in our study,

    however, does reect Tyler and Degoey's (1996) proposition that trust is more of a social than an economic commodity. The close ties

    that are encouraged by affective trust appear to be of considerable importance to employees. Lapidot, Kark, and Shamir (2006) noted

    that benevolence, a form of affective trust, was critical in trust building and associated with conditions of less vulnerability. They also

    suggested that in conditions of less vulnerability, a promotion self-regulatory focus (Higgins, 2000) is more likely to be adopted by

    individuals. A promotion focus differs from a prevention focus in that the former involve greater activation and eagerness in goal

    pursuits whereas the latter involves the use of greater caution and vigilance (Higgins, 2000). As such, people's motivation to expend

    their energy in pursuit of goals increases as situationally-induced promotion focus increases (Higgins, 1998). We argue that affective

    trust in supervisor could in part be responsible for activating a promotion focus in employees. Affective trust in supervisor should

    facilitate employees' embracing a promotion focus,and thiscould encourage higher performance and more positive attitudes. Beyond

    the issue of trust, transformational leadership, in particular, has also been proposed to be effective in activating followers' promotion

    focus (Ilies et al., 2006;Kark& Van Dijk, 2007). More research on the dynamics involved in transformational leadership, affective trust

    in supervisor, and promotion focus may advance our knowledge in this regard.

    It has been argued that specifying the referent is as important as identifying the base of trust (Dirks & Skarlicki, 2004). Explicitly

    recognizing referents for employee trust becomes particularly relevant when investigating the consequences of trust in leaders

    within organizations. InDirks and Ferrin's (2002)meta-analysis, the differences in the magnitude of trust-outcome relationships

    emerged for management and supervisor foci. However, they were unable to examine differences in the magnitude of found

    relationships within a single sample to determine the unique variance attributable to each of the trust foci. With this circumstance

    in mind, our study explored the relative impact of trust associated with different leadership foci in conjunction with trust bases.

    Given thatorganizational resources arealwayslimited, theresults of thisstudymay informdecision makers on setting prioritiesfor

    resource application. In this study, trust in different foci showed different relationships with workplace outcomes, thereby providing

    diagnostic guidance regarding the targets at which organizations should focus resources on building trust to achieve benecial effects.

    If an organization sees a need to boost employees' affective commitment to the organization, for example, then it should focus on its

    efforts in establishing employees' affective trust in management as well as in their supervisors.Considerationleadership manifestedby

    both upper managers and immediate supervisors may be needed to optimize trust's inuence upon employees' commitment to the

    organization.Of course, more research is required to advance insights concerning conditions under which thefour trust variants would

    be most efcacious. With such knowledge, organizations might be able to better leverage the effects of trust in achieving desired

    workplace outcomes.

    6.2. Limitations and future research

    There are caveats associated with the study design. The cross-sectional design utilized makes it difcult to truly discern causal

    relationships among study variables. Thus, any causal inferences should be made with caution. Another caveat concerns gen-

    eralizabilityof thestudy results. Thestudywas conductedin a healthcarecontext,andit is possible that some characteristics associated

    with the healthcare industry (e.g., cross-functional tasking across shifts and units, prominence of care-giving values) might make

    certain elements of trust more salient than others. Related to sample-specic concerns, individual differences other than demo-

    graphics (e.g., prior performance, career history) might also have inuenced study participants' behavior and work attitudes. These

    potential issues should be addressed in future studies of the impact of the four trust variants.

    A third limitation is that much of the data were collected by means of self-report. In-role and extra-role behavior data were

    collected from supervisors, lessening concerns involving the relationships of in- and extra-role behavior with the trust variants.

    The remaining measures were obtained from subordinates themselves. As several of the hypothesized relationships were subject

    to perceptpercept ination, marker variable and latent variable analyses were used to estimate this possibility. The results of

    these analyses results indicated that perceptpercept ination was not problematic in the study. Lastly, to address the issue of

    multicollinearity among the four trust variants in multiple regression analyses, we computed relative importance indexes ratherthan relying only on regression coefcients to determine the trust variants' explanatory contributions. Nevertheless, given that

    relative importance indexes cannot resolve inherent problems with correlated predictors (Baltes, Parker, Young, Huff, & Altmann,

    2004), caution should be exercised when interpreting relative importance results.

    The current study sought to examine the consequences of trust in leaders using a dual approach to bases and foci in

    conceptualizing trust. Future research should extend this line of inquiry by examining other work-related outcomes, such as

    turnover intentions, organization-directed citizenship, and withdrawal behavior. As this study showed that the trust variants do

    not function equivalently, it may be worthwhile to discover the relative strength of each in predicting such outcomes. Additional

    work should also explore the relationship of trust bases over time. The interplay of these two bases in an evolving work

    relationship would be an interesting avenue for future research. It has been argued that a trust relationship at early stages may be

    more cognitively-based, whereas at latter stages it may become more affectively-based. Young and Daniel's (2003) study revealed

    that at early stages of relationship building, trust tended to be more cognitively determined by levels of competence and goal

    congruence, whereas in later stages it depended more on personal feelings. Questions such as how trust develops with the passage

    of time and how it evolves from a cognitive to an affective base have not as yet been examined. Therefore, research addressing thisissue would prove to be fruitful.

    60 J. Yang, K.W. Mossholder / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    12/14

    We identied trust foci in the context of trust in leadership. Future research may benet by examining whether the two-base

    structure of trust can be expanded to other realms of work relationships. For example, it has been argued that employees can

    develop a generalized trust in coworkers (Hartog, 2003). Such generalized trust does not rest with knowledge of particular

    individuals but rather with implicit, normative behavior that occurs within the social unit as a whole. This kind of trust is relevant

    when employees' power and status are more similar rather than when power dependencies exist, such as between leaders and

    subordinates. Trust in coworkers/peers has been investigated as a group level variable in a few studies dealing with relations

    among intragroup conict, intragroup trust, and group performance (e.g., Jehn & Mannix, 2001). Considering the results of the

    present study, incorporating the cognitiveaffective trust distinction in examining of the effects of coworker trust in groups could

    prove enlightening. Additionally, it may be interesting to explore whether group peer trust would exert a contextual inuence on

    the trust effects observed in this study.

    In conclusion, the aim of this study was to highlight and demonstrate the importance of understanding workplace trust from a

    dual bases-and-foci perspective. Toward this end, the distinctiveness and importance of bases and foci of trust in leaders in

    predicting salient consequences were investigated. Our ndings point toward a greater need of understanding how to optimize

    employee trust in organizations. It is hoped that this study will serve as an impetus for more research in this direction.

    Appendix A

    Survey instruments for measuring trust in leaders by its foci and bases

    Cognitive trust in management

    1. I can depend on management to meet its responsibilities.2. I can rely on management to do what is best at work.

    3. Top managers follow through with commitments they make.

    4. Given management's track record, I see no reason to doubt its competence.

    5. I'm condent in management because it approaches work with professionalism.

    Affective trust in m anagement

    1. I'm condent that management will always care about my personal needs at work.

    2. If I shared my problems with management, I know they would respond with care.

    3. I'm condent that I could share my work difculties with management.

    4. I'm sure I could openly communicate my feelings to management.

    5. I feel secure with management because of its sincerity.

    Cognitive trust in supervisor

    1. I can depend on my supervisor to meet his/her responsibilities.

    2. I can rely on my supervisor to do what is best at work.

    3. My supervisor follows through with commitments s(he) makes.4. Given my supervisor's track record, I see no reason to doubt his/her competence.

    5. I'm condent in my supervisor because (s)he approaches work with professionalism.

    Affective trust in s upervisor

    1. I'm condent that my supervisor will always care about my personal needs at work.

    2. If I shared my problems with my supervisor, I know (s)he would respond with care.

    3. I'm condent that I could share my work difculties with my supervisor.

    4. I'm sure I could openly communicate my feelings to my supervisor.

    5. I feel secure with my supervisor because of his/her sincerity.

    References

    Albrecht, S., & Travaglione, A. (2003). Trust in public-sector senior management. International Journal of Human Resource Management,14, 7692.

    Aryee, S., Budhwar, P. S., & Chen, Z. X. (2002). Trust as a mediator of the relationship between organizational justice and work outcomes: Test of a social exchangemodel. Journal of Organizational Behavior,23, 267285.

    Avolio, B. J.,Zhu, W.,Koh, W., & Bhatia, P. (2004). Transformational leadershipand organizational commitment: Mediatingrole of psychological empowermentandmoderating role of structural distance. Journal of Organizational Behavior,25, 951968.

    Baltes, B. B., Parker, C. P., Young, L. M., Huff, J. W., & Altmann, R. (2004). The practical utility of importance measures in assessing the relative importance of work-related perceptions and organizational characteristics on work-related outcomes. Organizational Research Methods,7, 326340.

    Becerra, M., & Gupta, A. K. (2003). Perceived trustworthiness within the organization: The moderating impact of communication frequency on trustor and trusteeeffects.Organization Science,14, 3244.

    Brandes, P., Dharwadkar, R., & Wheatley, K. (2004). Social exchanges within organizations and work outcomes: The importance of local and global relationships.Group & Organization Management,29, 276301.

    Brower, H. H., Schoorman, F. D., & Tan, H. H. (2000). A model of relational leadership: The integration of trust and leader member exchange.Leadership Quarterly,11, 227250.

    Burke, C. S., Stagl, K. C., Klein, C., Goodwin, G. F., Salas, E., & Halpin, S. M. (2006). What types of leadership behaviors are functional in teams? A meta-analysis.Leadership Quarterly,17, 288307.

    Carr, J. Z., Schmidt, A. M., Ford, J. K., & DeShon, R. P. (2003). Climate perceptions matter: A meta-analysis relating molar climate, cognitive and affective states, andindividual level work outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology,88, 605619.

    Clark, M. C., & Payne, R. L. (1997). The nature and structure of workers' trust in management. Journal of Organizational Behavior,18, 205224.

    Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences, 3rd Ed. Mahwah, New Jersey:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    61J. Yang, K.W. Mossholde r / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    13/14

    Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., & LePine, J. A. (2007). Trust, trustworthiness, and trust propensity: A meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk taking andjob performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,92, 909927.

    Cook, J.,& Wall, T. (1980). Newwork attitude measures of trust, organizational commitment andpersonal need non-fulllment.Journal of Occupational Psychology,53, 3952.

    Cremer, D. D. (2006). Affective and motivational consequences of leader self-sacrice: The moderating effect of autocratic leadership. Leadership Quarterly,17,7993.

    Davy, J. A., Kinicki, A. J., & Scheck, C. L. (1997). A test of job security's direct and mediated effects on withdrawal cognitions. Journal of Organizational Behavior,18,323349.

    Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D. L. (2002). Trust in leadership: Meta-analytic ndings and implications for research and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87,611628.

    Dirks,K. T.,& Parks,J. M.(2003).Conictingstories:The state of thescienceof conict. In J. Greenberg(Ed.), Organizationalbehavior:The state of the science (pp.283324).2nd. Ed. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Dirks, K. T., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2004). Trust in leaders: Existing research and emerging issues. In R. M. Kramer, & K. S. Cook (Eds.), Trust and distrust in organizations:Dilemmas and approaches(pp. 2140). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Erdogan, B., & Liden, R. C. (2002). Social exchanges in the workplace: A review of recent developments and future developments in leader member exchangetheory. In L. L. Neider, & C. A. Schriesheim (Eds.),Leadership(pp. 65114). Greenwich, CN: Information Age Publishing.

    Fisher, C. D. (1980). On the dubious wisdom of expecting job satisfaction to correlate with performance. Academy of Management Review,5, 607612.Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research,18,

    3950.Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1975). Development of the job design diagnostic survey. Journal of Applied Psychology,60, 159170.Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Hartog, D. (2003). Trustingothers in organizations: Leaders,management,and co-workers. In B. Nooteboom & F. Six (Eds.),The trustprocess in organizations(pp.125146).

    Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing.Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology ,30, 146.Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from t.American Psychologist,55, 12171230.House, R. J., & Aditya, R. N. (1997). The social scientic study of leadership: Quo vadis? Journal of Management,23, 409473.Ilies, R., Judge, T., & Wagner, D. (2006). Making sense of motivational leadership: The trail from transformational leaders to motivated followers. Journal of

    Leadership and Organizational Studies,13, 122.Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2001). The dynamic nature of conict: A longitudinal study of intragroup conict and group performance.Academy of Management

    Journal,44, 238251.Johnson, J. W. (2004 ). Factors affecting relative weights: The inuence of sampling and measurement error.Organizational Research Methods,7, 283299.Johnson-George, C., & Swap, W. C. (1982). Measurement of specic interpersonal trust: Construction and validation of a scale to assess trust in a specic other.

    Journal of Personality a nd Social Psychology,43, 13061317.Jreskog, K., & Srbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8 user's reference guide. Chicago: Scientic Software International.Kark, R., & Van Dijk, D. (2007). Motivation to lead, motivation to follow: The role of the self-regulatory focus in leadership processes. Academy of Management

    Review,32, 500528.Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1993).Procedural justice, attitudes, and subsidiary top managementcompliancewith multinationals' corporatestrategic decisions.

    Academy of Management Journal,36, 502526.Konovsky, M. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1991). Perceived fairness of employee drug testing as a predictor of employee attitudes and job performance.Journal of Applied

    Psychology,76, 698707.Kramer, R. M. (1996). Divergent realities andconvergent disappointments in thehierarchic relation: Trust andthe intuitive auditor at work. In R. M. Kramer& T. R.

    Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 216245). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Lapidot, Y., Kark, R., & Shamir, B. (2006). The impact of situational vulnerability on the development and erosion of followers' trust in their leader. Leadership

    Quarterly,18, 1634.

    LeBreton, J. M., Binning, J. F., Adorno, A. J., & Melcher, K. M. (2004). Importance of personality and job-specic affect for predicting job attitudes and withdrawalbehavior.Organizational Research Methods,7, 300325.

    Lewin, K. (1943). Dening the eld at a given time.Psychological Review,50, 292310.Lewicki, R. J., Tomlinson, E. C., & Gillespie, N. (2006). Models of interpersonal trust development: Theoretical approaches, empirical evidence, and future directions.

    Journal of Management,32, 9911022.Lindell, M. K., & Whitney, D. J. (2001). Accounting for common method variance in cross-sectional research designs. Journal of Applied Psychology,86, 114121.Luo, Y. (2002). Building trust in cross-cultural collaborations: Toward a contingency perspective. Journal of Management,28, 669694.Mayer,R. C.,& Davis, J. H. (1999).The effects of theperformanceappraisalsystem on trust formanagement: A eld quasi-experiment.Journal of Applied Psychology,

    84, 123136.Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review,20, 709734.Mayer, R. C., & Gavin, M. B. (2005). Trust in management and performance: Who minds the shop while the employees watch the boss? Academy of Management

    Journal,48, 874888.McAllister, D. J. (1995). Affect- and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Journal,38, 2459.Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1984). Testing the side-bet theoryof organizational commitment: Some methodological considerations.Journal of Applied Psychology,

    69, 372378.Meyer, J. P., & Herscovitch, L. (2001). Commitment in the workplace toward a general model. Human Resource Management Review,11, 299326.Michie, S., & Gooty, J. (2005). Values, emotions, and authenticity: Will the real leader please stand up? Leadership Quarterly,16, 441457.

    Mishra, A. K., & Mishra, K. E. (1994). The role of mutual trust in effective downsizing strategies. Human Resource Management,33, 261279.Morgan, D. E., & Zeffane, R. (2003). Employee involvement, organizational change and trust in management. International Journal of Human Resource Management,

    14, 5575.Pillai, R., Schriesheim, C. A., & Williams, E. S. (1999). Fairness perceptions and trust as mediators for transformational and transactional leadership: A two-sample

    study. Journal of Management,25, 897933.Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and

    recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology,88, 879903.Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Paine, J. B., & Bachrach, D. G. (2000). Organizational citizenship behaviors: A critical review of the theoretical and empirical

    literature and suggestions for future research. Journal of Management,26, 513563.Porter, L. W., Steers, R. M., Mowday, R. T.,& Boulian, P. V. (1974). Organizational commitment, job satisfaction, andturnoveramongpsychiatric technicians.Journal

    of Applied Psychology,59, 603609.Raudenbush, S., & Bryk, A. (2002).Hierarchical linear models (2e). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Rempel, J. K., Holmes, J. G., & Zanna, M. P. (1985). Trust in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,49, 95112.Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: Across discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23 ,

    393404.Rousseau, D. M., & Tijoriwala, S. A. (1999). What's a good reason to change? Motivated reasoning and social accounts in promoting organizational change. Journal

    of Applied Psychology,84, 514528.

    Schleicher, D. J., Watt, J. D., & Greguras, G. J. (2004). Reexamining the job satisfaction

    performance relationship: The complexity of attitudes. Journal of AppliedPsychology,89, 165177.

    62 J. Yang, K.W. Mossholder / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063

  • 8/12/2019 Examining the Effect of Trust in Leader

    14/14

    Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C., & Davis, J. H. (2007). An integrative model of organizational trust: Past, present, and future. Academy of Management Review,32,344354.

    Scott, D. (1980). The causal relationship between trust and the assessed value of management by objectives. Journal of Management,6, 157175.Settoon, R. P., & Mossholder, K. W. (2002). Relationship quality and relationship context as antecedents of person- and task-focused interpersonal citizenship

    behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology,87, 255267.Shamir, B. (1995). Social distance and charisma: Theoretical notes and an exploratory study. Leadership Quarterly,6, 1947.Simons, T., Friedman, R.,Liu, L. A.,& Parks, J. M. (2007). Racial differences in sensitivityto behavior integrity: Attitudinal consequences, in-groupseffects, and trick

    downamong black and non-black employees. Journal of Applied Psychology,92, 650665.Spector, P. E. (1997). Job satisfaction: Application, assessment, causes and consequences. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publication.Spreitzer, G. M., & Mishra,A. K. (2002). To stay or to go:Voluntary survivor turnover following an organizational downsizing.Journal of Organizational Behavior,23,

    707729.Tan, H. H., & Tan, C. S. F. (2000). Toward the differentiation of trust in supervisor and trust in organization.Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs,126,

    241260.Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efcacy: Its potential antecedents and relationship to creative performance.Academy of Management Journal,45,

    11371148.Tyler, T., & Degoey, P. (1996). Trust in organizational authorities: The inuence of motive attributions on willingness to accept decisions. In R. Kramer & T. Tyler

    (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 331356). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Van Dyne, L., & LePine, J. A. (1998). Helping and voice extra-role behaviors: Evidence of construct and perceived validity. Academy of Management Journal, 41,

    108119.Webber, S. S., & Klimoski, R. J. (2004). Clientproject manager engagements, trust, and loyalty. Journal of Organizational Behavior,25, 9971013.Weiss, H. M., Nicholas, J. P., & Daus, C. S. (1999). An examination of the joint effects of affective experiences and job beliefs on job satisfaction and variations in

    affective experiences over time. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,78, 124.Whitener, E. M. (1997). The impact of human resource activities on employee trust. Human Resource Management Review,7, 389404.Whitener, E. M., Brodt, S. E., Korsgaard, M. A., & Werner, J. M. (1998). Managers as initiators of trust: An exchange relationship framework for understanding

    managerial trustworthy behavior.Academy of Management Review,23, 513530.Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and organizational commitment as predictors of organizational citizenship and in-role behaviors.Journal of

    Management,17, 601617.

    Young, L., & Daniel, K. (2003). Affectual trust in the workplace. International Journal of Human Resource Management,14, 139155.

    63J. Yang, K.W. Mossholde r / The Leadership Quarterly 21 (2010) 5063