extending the notion of pragmatic completion: the case of ... · pdf fileextending the notion...

18
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/251586527 Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive compound action unit ARTICLE in FUEL AND ENERGY ABSTRACTS · DECEMBER 2011 DOI: 10.1016/j.pragma.2011.10.003 CITATIONS 2 READS 33 3 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: Heidi Kevoe-Feldman Northeastern University 13 PUBLICATIONS 25 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Jeffrey D Robinson Portland State University 44 PUBLICATIONS 1,130 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Available from: Heidi Kevoe-Feldman Retrieved on: 22 October 2015

Upload: doandang

Post on 19-Feb-2018

227 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Seediscussions,stats,andauthorprofilesforthispublicationat:http://www.researchgate.net/publication/251586527

Extendingthenotionofpragmaticcompletion:Thecaseoftheresponsivecompoundactionunit

ARTICLEinFUELANDENERGYABSTRACTS·DECEMBER2011

DOI:10.1016/j.pragma.2011.10.003

CITATIONS

2

READS

33

3AUTHORS,INCLUDING:

HeidiKevoe-Feldman

NortheasternUniversity

13PUBLICATIONS25CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

JeffreyDRobinson

PortlandStateUniversity

44PUBLICATIONS1,130CITATIONS

SEEPROFILE

Availablefrom:HeidiKevoe-Feldman

Retrievedon:22October2015

Page 2: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attachedcopy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial researchand education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

and sharing with colleagues.

Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling orlicensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party

websites are prohibited.

In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of thearticle (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website orinstitutional repository. Authors requiring further information

regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies areencouraged to visit:

http://www.elsevier.com/copyright

Page 3: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsivecompound action unit

Heidi Kevoe-Feldman a,*, Jeffrey D. Robinson b, Jenny Mandelbaumc

aDepartment of Communication Studies, Northeastern University, 204 Lake Hall, 360 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA 02115, United StatesbDepartment of Communication, Portland State University, United StatescDepartment of Communication, Rutgers University, United States

1. Introduction

Our understanding of the rules for conversational turn taking (Sacks et al., 1974) have significantly contributed to ourunderstanding of social organization. One of these rules is that, once a person gains the right to speak, they are normallyentitled to produce a single unit of talk (e.g., a word, phrase, clause and sentence), and that their turn comes to a place ofpossible completion after that of the unit.1 However, conversation analysis (CA) has long recognized that, and attempted todescribe how, this normal entitlement can be modified by a variety of exigencies associated with grammar (Betz, 2008),prosody (Ford and Thompson, 1996; Local and Walker, 2004; Schegloff, 1998), and, relevant to the present article,pragmatics (Ford and Thompson, 1996; Jefferson, 1991; Levinson, 1983, 2000; Robinson, in press). Within CA, pragmaticelements are commonly tied to the structure of social action, per se (for review, see Ford and Thompson, 1996). The presentarticle is concerned with how a sequence-initiating action can uniquely structure the character of a responsive action suchthat it is normally constituted bymultiple, particular, ordered sentential units, and thus this article is concerned with how asequence-initiating action can uniquely structure participants’ understandings ofwhat constitutes a possibly complete ‘unit’of talk (see Selting, 2000). Specifically,we demonstrate that a particular type of initiating actionmakes conditionally relevant

Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859

A R T I C L E I N F O

Article history:

Received 17 May 2011

Received in revised form 25 September 2011

Accepted 3 October 2011

Available online 1 November 2011

Keywords:

Compound action

Conversation analysis

Customer service

Sequence

Turn taking

Initiating actions

A B S T R A C T

An important rule of turn taking is that, once a person gains the right to speak they are

normally entitled to produce a single unit of talk, such as a single word, phrase, clause, or

sentence. Conversation analysis has long recognized that, and attempted to describe how,

this normal entitlement can be modified by pragmatic exigencies. Along these lines, this

article demonstrates that a particular type of initiating action (referred to as a status

inquiry) makes conditionally relevant a particular type of compound action unit (Lerner,

1991) that minimally contains two ordered pieces of information, each of which occupies

at least one sentential unit. Data are audiotapes of 193 calls between one of five customer-

service representatives and customers calling an electronics organization to check on the

status of equipment that they have previously sent in for repair. This article contributes to

our understanding of how pragmatic concerns can uniquely structure participants’

understandings of what constitutes a possibly complete ‘unit’ of talk, as well as ‘allowable’

places for speakership.

� 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 973 223 5918; fax: +1 617 373 8533.

E-mail address: [email protected] (H. Kevoe-Feldman).1 For various elaborations on the notion of turn constructional unit, see Ford (2001a), Lerner (1991), Selting (2000), and Schegloff (2006).

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Journal of Pragmatics

journal homepage: www.elsev ier .com/ locate /pragma

0378-2166/$ – see front matter � 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2011.10.003

Page 4: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

a particular type of compound action unit (Lerner, 1991). In the following introduction, we review prior work dealing withcompound action units and highlight potential contributions of the present article.

The concept of a compound action unit was originally formulated by Lerner (1991), whose article was primarily focusedon a different concept, that of the compound turn-constructional unit. However, toward the end of his article, Lernerobserved: ‘‘Not only are some turn-constructional units available as compound units, but larger, internally segmented actions

are as well’’ (p. 453, emphasis added). Lerner went on to argue that, ‘‘the sequencing of actions in conversational interactioncan also supply features that betoken a compound ‘action unit’’’ (pp. 454–455, emphasis added). Recognizing that there can bedifferent types of compound action units, one of them involves responsive actions (vs. turns, per se) that are projectable, byvirtue of the conditional relevance rules associatedwith their initiating actions, as having two ormore components that eachare constituted by at least one lexical, phrasal, clausal, or sentential unit.2 For example, in a subsequent publication, Lerner(1992) offered the example of the story prompt as an initiating action (e.g., ‘‘Oh you haftuh tell’m about yer typewriterhoney,’’ p. 251) thatmakes conditionally relevant a story (or some type of pre-structured extended telling), which is a type ofcompound action unit (in this case, a responsive one). For another example, see Extract 1. Referring to a play, Nancy promptsHyla to produce an extended telling: ‘‘Can yuh tell me what it’s abou:t?’’ (line 2).

Extract 1:

01 Nan: Kinda looking forward to it. What u:m, (1.0)

02 –> Can yuh tell me what it’s abou:t?=

03 Hyl: =.hhhhhhh [Yeah. It take-]

04 Nan: [O:r would it- uh-]

05 (.)

07 Hyl: [No. It takes] pla:ce, i:n, .t (0.2) u-=ni:neteen thirties

06 Nan: [( )]

08 HYL: in Oklahom[a,

09 Nan: [Uh hu:h,=

10 Hyl: =.hh A::n’, .hhh .t.hhh (0.2) It’s just like the

11 psychological backgroun’ behind all these different people

12 in this:f [am’ly.]=

13 Nan: [Mm hm: ]=

14 Hyl: =.hh Li:ke, the husban:d. . . ((Hyla continues))

Hyla projects a multi-unit responsive turn, which she ultimately produces across lines 3–14, with a long and pronouncedin breath (symbolized in the transcript by h’s preceded by a period; line 3; Schegloff, 1996). Nancy’s continuers (at line 9, ‘‘Uhhu:h,’’ and at line 13, ‘‘Mm hm:’’) display her orientation to Hyla as producing an extended telling (Schegloff, 1982).

Initiating actions, like the story prompt, that make conditionally relevant compound action units are virtually unstudiedwithin conversation analysis (but see Lerner, 1991, 1992). The uniqueness of such a practice can be appreciated in light of avariety ofways inwhich initiating actions are implicated in providing for the relevance ofmulti-unit responsive turns that donot represent compound actions (although a responsive compound action unit is, by definition, amulti-unit responsive turn,the reverse is not always the case).What follows are three examples.3 A first way inwhich an initiating action can provide fora multi-unit response involves the conditional-relevance rule, which normatively obligates a particular type of response(Schegloff and Sacks, 1973; Schegloff, 2007). If an initial, responsive unit of talk (e.g., aword, phrase, clause, or sentence) doesnot constitute a relevant response, then its possible completion does not constitute a place where the responsive turn ispossibly complete because a relevant response is still ‘due.’ As such, these responsive turns typically end up beingconstituted by multiple units. This is the case in Extract 2 (which was analyzed by Heritage, 1984b:266).

Extract 2:

01 B: Uh if you’d care to come over and visit a little while

02 this morning I’ll give you a cup of coffee.

03 –> A: Hehh Well that’s awfully sweet of you,

04 I don’t think I can make it this morning

05 .hh uhm I’m running an ad in the paper and- and uh

06 I have to stay near the phone

2 Houtkoop and Mazeland (1985) might have called this a closed (vs. open) discourse unit.3 Although there are avarietyof explanations forhowresponsive turns (or second-pair parts) come tobe composedofmultiple turnconstructionalunits – for

example, via the practice of the rush through (Schegloff, 1987) or the abrupt-join (Local and Walker, 2004) – here we are only concerned with explanations

involving social structures associated with initiating actions (or first-pair parts), such as rules associated with conditional relevance and preference

organization.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3845

Page 5: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

A’s initial responsive unit, an expression of appreciation, ‘‘that’s awfully sweet of you,’’ (line 3), is a projectably incompleteresponse because it does not constitute a conditionally relevant response type (e.g., acceptance or rejection of the offer),which gets produced in the second unit: ‘‘I don’t think I can make it this morning’’ (line 4). In this case, it is not that theinitiating action, per se, makes conditionally relevant a compound action (as did, for example, the story prompts discussedabove). Rather, the initiating action makes conditionally relevant a responsive action that can be accomplished in a single(i.e., non-compound) unit (e.g., ‘‘I don’t think I can make it this morning’’), but the responsive turn (vs. action) ends upconsisting of at least two units (i.e., the first and the second) by virtue of the conditional-relevance rule.

A second way in which an initiating action can provide for a multi-unit response involves preference organization, suchthatmany initiating actions ‘prefer’ particular answers over others (e.g., an invitation ‘prefers’ an acceptance over a rejection;re. ‘preference,’ see Pomerantz, 1984a; Schegloff, 2007). As such, dispreferred (albeit conditionally relevant) responsesfrequently ‘require,’ and thus project, additional actions, such as an account for the dispreferred response (this observationhas been extensively supported; Ford, 2001b; Robinson, 2009). For example, return to Extract 2 (above). As noted earlier, A’sinitial expression of appreciation, ‘‘that’s awfully sweet of you,’’ (line 3), while perhaps foreshadowing a dispreferredresponse, is not itself a conditionally relevant response type (e.g., acceptance or rejection of the offer). A’s rejection, ‘‘I don’tthink I can make it this morning’’ (line 4), is a dispreferred response type, and as such projects an account for the rejection,which is produced next: ‘‘I’m running an ad in the paper and- and uh I have to stay near the phone’’ (lines 5–6). Again, in thiscase, it is not that the initiating action, per se, makes conditionally relevant a compound action. Rather, the initiating actionmakes conditionally relevant a responsive action that can be accomplished in a single (i.e., non-compound) unit (i.e., ‘‘I don’tthink I canmake it thismorning’’), but the responsive turn ends up consisting of a third unit (i.e., the account) by virtue of theconditionally relevant response being dispreferred.

A third way in which an initiating action can provide for a multi-unit response involves the fact that some initiatingactions can be ‘double barreled’ (Schegloff, 2007). In these cases, an initiating action (e.g., an offer), which makesconditionally relevant a responsive action (e.g., acceptance/declination), is implemented through a grammatical vehicle(e.g., a Yes/No-interrogative; Raymond, 2003) that itself makes relevant a particular type of responsive action (e.g., Yes/No)(see also Lindstrom, 1997). In these cases, such as Extract 3 (whichwas analyzed by Schegloff, 2007:76), responsive turns arefrequently composed of multiple responsive actions, which are implemented through separate units of talk.

Extract 3:

01 Ali: You wan’ me bring you anything?

02 (0.4)

03 –> Bet: No: no: nothing.

04 Ali: AW:kay.

Beth’s multi-unit responsive turn (line 3) addresses the constraintsmade relevant by Ali’s Yes/No-interrogative (e.g., ‘‘No:no:’’) as well as the offer it implements (‘‘nothing.’’). However, in the case of double-barreled actions, each of the action‘barrels’ (e.g., the offer and the Yes/No-interrogative) makes relevant a responsive action that can be accomplished in a single(i.e., non-compound) unit, and the responsive turn ends up consisting of multiple units by virtue of the existence of multipleinitiating actions (vs. a single initiation action, as in Extract 2, above).

In sum, although prior research has described a number ofways inwhich initiating actions are implicated in providing forthe relevance of multi-unit responsive turns, we know very little about singular (vs. double-barreled) initiating actions thatmake conditionally relevant compound action units.4 Furthermore, although literature suggests an abundance of compoundaction units in ordinary (vs. institutional) contexts, such as giving driving directions and food recipes (Goldberg, 1975), weknow extremely little about the organization of compound action units other than stories (and other pre-structuredextended tellings; Jefferson, 1978; Mandelbaum, 2012). Even within institutional contexts, we know much more aboutactivities, which tend to be defined as encompassingmore than one sequence of action (for review, see Robinson, 2012), thanwe know about compound action units. The concept of a compound action unit continues to stretch our understanding ofthat of a turn-constructional unit (Sacks et al., 1974) beyond syntactic and prosodic parameters to include pragmatic ones(Selting, 2000).

In an attempt to fill some of these gaps, the present article examines a previously unstudied initiating action that issimilar to the story prompt in that it makes conditionally relevant a compound action unit. This initiating action involves acustomer, who is calling an electronics-repair organization, soliciting an update regarding the status of their equipment thatis currently in some stage of the process of being repaired (for convenience, this action will hereafter be referred to as a statusinquiry). The present article furthers our understanding of how social action – specifically, responsive compound action units –can be organized (Schegloff, 2007) and, relatedly, how pragmatic concerns affect the social organization of turn taking(Ford and Thompson, 1996).

4 Responsive compound action units can also be prefigured as such by the organization of non-ordinary (e.g., institutional) speech-exchange systems. For

example, in the context of broadcast news interviews, interviewees are ‘‘expected to produce elaborated answers’’ to interviewers’ questions, that is,

elaboration beyond mere Yes/confirmation or No/disconfirmation (Clayman and Heritage, 2002:252).

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–38593846

Page 6: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

In order to preview our argument, we present one of these calls in its entirety, which is Extract 4 (in transcripts, ‘‘REP’’stands for customer-service representative, and ‘‘CUS’’ stands for customer). The customer solicits a report of the status oftheir equipment with, ‘‘I need tuh check thuh status of my:: repair laptop,’’ (lines 4–5), which is responded to by therepresentative at lines 17–20.

Extract 4 [Call 26]:

01 ((Ring))

02 REP: Jack Camera. Kendra speakin’,=>how may I< help you.

03 (0.4)

04 –> CUS: Hi. .hhhh uh:: (.) I need tuh check thuh status of my::

05 –> repair laptop,

06 (0.5)

07 REP: What’s thuh repair number?

08 (21.0) ((CUS locates number))

09 CUS: 8Sorry for that.8 two three two, eight

10 zero (0.2) nine. sorry for that.

11 REP: Two three two, eight zero #nine. #

12 CUS: Y:eah.

13 (3.2)

14 REP: Rashad Oni?

15 CUS: >Yeh=right.<

16 (.)

17 ->> REP: Okay. (0.2) That is in li:ne with thuh techni:cian

18 ->> waiting to be worked o:n? .hhh repair times usually run

19 ->> approximately fiftee:n .hh to thirty business days

20 ->> from thuh date #that it was lo:gged into our system. #

21 (0.7)

22 CUS: Oh:. okay. So: (0.5) e-=this is your policy fer: aroun’

23 one mo:nth,

24 (0.4)

25 REP: .tch Ye:s.

26 (0.4)

27 CUS: O:ka:y.

28 (.)

29 REP: Oka:y?

30 CUS: >Okay.< >thanks.<

31 REP: A’right. your we [lco ]me.

32 CUS: [Bye. ]

33 REP: Bye.

Although the customer explicitly solicits the ‘‘status’’ (line 4) of his ‘‘repair laptop,’’ (line 5), there is no verbalizedindication (at least from lines 1–15) of what constitutes ‘repair status’ for participants. We argue that status inquiries makeconditionally relevant a compound action unit that minimally contains two, ordered pieces of information, each of whichoccupy at least one sentential unit of talk. The first piece of information is what we are calling current-location status, or thecurrent location of the equipment within the repair process, which can be seen in Extract 4 (above) as: ‘‘That is in li:ne withthuh techni:cian waiting to be worked o:n?’’ (lines 17–18). The second piece of information is what we are calling repair-completion status, or an estimation of when the repair will be completed, and can be seen in Extract 4 (above) as: ‘‘repairtimes usually run approximately fiftee:n .hh to thirty business days from thuh date that it was lo:gged into our system.’’(lines 18–20). The present article is concerned with how participants understand and navigate the possible completion ofrepresentatives’ responsive turns (i.e., lines 17–20 in Extract 4). We argue that the pragmatic requirements of customers’status inquiries structure interaction such that the possible grammatical completion of the initial status component, current-location status, does not constitute a place where representatives’ turns are possibly complete; Rather, it constitutes a placeof possible local pragmatic completion (Ford and Thompson, 1996), which, in Lerner’s (1991) terms, constitutes a place of

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3847

Page 7: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

conditional entry for next speakers. In contrast, the possible grammatical completion of the final component, repair-completion status, does constitute a place of possible global pragmatic completion (Ford and Thompson, 1996), and thus ofturn completion.

2. Data, method, and background

2.1. Data and method

Data were collected from the in-house repair facility of a mid-sized organization located in the Northeastern UnitedStates that sells and repairs cameras and other electronic goods.We refer to this organization by the pseudonym Jack Camera.Data are audiotapes of 193 calls between one of five customer-service representatives and customers calling to check on thestatus of equipment that they have previously sent in for repair. Our analyticmethod is conversation analysis (for review, seeHeritage, 1984b; Drew and Heritage, 1992). All calls were transcribed in their entirety using Jefferson’s transcription system(Atkinson and Heritage, 1984).

2.2. Background

Datawerecollectedaspart of afive-monthfieldobservationof JackCamera. Ethnographically,weknowthat the two typesofstatus thatmakesup representatives’ responses– that is, current-location status and repair-completion status – are thoroughlyinstitutionalized. As such, the compound action unit being described is likely not ‘generic,’ but rather specific to the presentinstitution. Current-location status involves information about the current location of customers’ equipment in the repairprocess. Prior to responding to customers’ status inquiries, representatives locate customers’ repair orders on computers (seeExtract 4, above, lines 7–15, and particularly line 8, where the representative accesses the computer). Computerized repairorders reveal current-location status, which involves one of seven organizationally technical categories, including: ‘logged in’(i.e., into the organization’s computer system), ‘in line’ (i.e., assigned to a technician), ‘on bench’ (i.e., on the technician’sworkbench/shelf), ‘onhold’ (for parts), ‘quality control,’ ‘shipping ready,’ andoptionally ‘manufacturer’ (i.e., the equipmentwassent to itsmanufacturer tobe repaired) (seeFig. 1 inAppendixA,which isa computerscreenshot). Representatives’ deliveriesofcurrent-location status lexically reflect these categories, such as ‘‘in li:ne’’ (Extract 4, line 17).5

Repair-completion status involves an estimated timeframe for when repairs will be completed or returned. This type ofstatus is institutionalized via a large whiteboard, which is prominently displayed to, and utilized by, all representativesduring calls. This whiteboard lists estimated repair-completion times for nine types of equipment (see Fig. 2 in Appendix A).Representatives’ report these timeframes during the delivery of repair-completion status, such as ‘‘fiftee:n .hh to thirtybusiness days’’ (Extract 4, above, line 19).6

3. Analysis

Our central argument is that customers’ status inquiries make conditionally relevant a compound action unit thatminimally contains two, ordered types of status: (1) current-location status; and (2) repair-completion status. The followinganalysis section provides two types of evidence for this argument. First, we provide a variety of ways in whichrepresentatives’ orient to our argument, and second we provide customers’ orientations. Note that, although this articlecentrally attempts to demonstrate that customers’ status inquiries make conditionally relevant a responsive compoundaction unit (and attempts to describe this unit’s organization), in section 4 (Conclusion) we provide an explanation for whythis might be so in this particular institutional context.

3.1. Representatives’ orientations

In this subsection, we provide two types of ways in which representatives orient to their own responses as involving bothcurrent-location status and repair-completion status. First, we demonstrate that representatives orient to current-locationstatus as not completing their responsive turns. Second, we demonstrate that representatives orient to repair-completionstatus as completing their responsive turns.

3.1.1. Representatives’ orientations to current-location status as being incomplete

In a variety of ways, representatives design their talk to show that the completion of current-location status does notconstitute a relevant place for turn transition.7 First, although representatives sometimes pause in between their production

5 For other examples, see Extract 5: ‘‘on the technician’s shelf’’ (line 13); Extract 6: ‘‘assigned to a technician’’ (lines 11–12); Extract 7: ‘‘in li:ne’’ (line 20);

Extract 8: ‘‘sent o:ut . . . fer thuh repa:ir?’’ (lines 10–11); Extract 9: ‘‘in line’’ (line 36); Extract 10: ‘‘returned from thuhmanufacturer’’ (lines 26–27); Extract 11:

‘‘shipping rea:dy?’’ (line 10); and Extract 12: ‘‘in li:ne’’ (line 34).6 For other examples, see Extract 5: ‘‘ten tuh twen’y business days’’ (line 15); Extract 6: ‘‘ten tuh uh: twen’y business days’’ (lines 13–14); Extract 7:

‘‘fifteen tuh thirty business days’’ (line 21); Extract 8: ‘‘ten tuh- fifteen business da:ys’’ (line 14); and Extract 9: ‘‘ten tuh twen’y business days’’ (line 38).7 Here and throughout we refer to ‘turn’ transition, versus ‘speaker’ transition, because the completion of current-location status does constitute a place of

conditional entry (Lerner, 1991) for next speakers (see Extracts 11–12, below).

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–38593848

Page 8: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

of current-location status and repair-completion status (with silence and or breathing),8 in many cases representativesproduce current-location status and repair-completion status in immediate succession, as in Extract 5.

Extract 5 [Call 20]:

02 CUS: Uh:m I’m calling tuh=f- get a little bit mo:re

03 information o:n thuh repa:ir of a digital camera

04 I sent in about a week ago,

((8 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

13 ->> REP: Mkay. .hh Uh:m that’s on thuh technician’s shelf

14 ->> waiting to be worked o:n, >thuh repair< ti:me (0.2)

15 ->> runs approximately ten tuh twen’y business days from

16 ->> thuh date that it’s logged into our system.

17 (1.1)

18 CUS: So::: fro:m:. (.) we’[re about- ]

19 REP: [>Twen’y< nin ]th.

Here, the representative moves directly from current-location status, ‘‘that’s on thuh technician’s shelf waiting to beworked o:n,’’ (lines 13–14) to repair-completion status, ‘‘>thuh repair< ti:me. . .’’ (lines 14–16), separated only by a normaltransition space (Jefferson, 1984; Sacks et al., 1974), which is symbolized in the transcript by a single, empty space between‘‘. . .o:n,’’ and ‘‘>thuh. . .’’. This observation is highlighted by a negative observation, which is that the representative does notutilize special practices for securing additional units of talk, such as the rush through (which occurs in Extract 7, below;Schegloff, 1987) or the abrupt-join (Local andWalker, 2004). In the present context of an ordinary speech exchange system inwhich turn transition would normally become relevant after a single, possibly complete sentential unit (Sacks et al., 1974) –for example, after current-location status – this first observation provides some evidence that representatives orient to theirproduction of current-location status followed by repair-completion status as constituting parts of a single responsiveaction.9

For another type of evidence, representatives frequently end current-location statuswith rising intonation that, at least inthe sequential context of a response to an initiating action, is not typical of turn-final pitch movement. Doing so can be apractice for projecting more talk, and thus for projecting that the completion of current-location status does not constitute arelevant place for turn transition (Szczepek-Reed, 2004). We saw an example of this in Extracts 4 and 5 (above). In Extract 4,the representative ends the first component, ‘‘That is in li:newith thuh techni:cianwaiting to beworked o:n?’’ (lines 17–18),with strong rising intonation at the end of ‘‘o:n?’’ (symbolized in the transcript by the question mark). As projected, therepresentative immediately begins to draw breath in preparation for producing repair-completion status, ‘‘.hhh’’ (line 18),which she then delivers: ‘‘repair times. . .’’ (lines 18–20). A similar pattern occurs in Extract 5 (above). The representativeends current-location status, ‘‘that’s on thuh technician’s shelf waiting to be worked o:n,’’ (lines 13–14) with slight risingintonation at the end of ‘‘o:n,’’ (symbolized in the transcript by the comma), and immediately proceeds to deliver repair-completion status: ‘‘>thuh repair< ti:me. . .’’ (lines 14–16). Extract 6 provides a third example:

Extract 6 [Call 13]:

05 CUS: Check on a repai:r plea:se.

((5 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

11 REP: Okay sir that has been: assigned to

12 –> a technician tuh work o:n, .hh repairs

13 usually run approximately ten tuh uh:

14 twen’y business days.

15 (1.3)

16 CUS: Ten tuh twen’y da:ys?

The representative ends current-location status, ‘‘that has been: assigned to a technician tuh work o:n,’’ (lines 11–12),with slight rising intonation at the end of ‘‘o:n,’’ (symbolized in the transcript by the comma). As projected, the

8 For example, see Extract 4: ‘‘. . .o:n? .hhh repair. . .’’ (line 18); Extract 6: ‘‘. . .o:n, .hh repairs. . .’’ (line 12); and Extract 9: ‘‘. . .o:n, .hh repair. . .’’ (line 37).9 For another example, see Extract 10, below: ‘‘. . .t’day. so:. . .’’ (line 27). Given our argument that the possible completion of repair-completion status is a

place of possible turn completion, the 1.1-s silence at line 17 in Extract 5 can be accounted for by the fact that the customer subsequently initiates repair

(line 18), and that other-initiation of repair is normatively delayed (Schegloff et al., 1979). A similar observation can be made about the 1.3-s silence at line

15 in Extract 6. However, there is no such delay in Extract 7 at line 22, or in Extract 10 at line 31. See also section 3.2.2.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3849

Page 9: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

representative immediately begins to drawbreath in preparation for producing the second component, ‘‘.hh’’ (line 12),whichshe then delivers: ‘‘repairs usually run. . .’’ (lines 12–14).10

Yet another type of evidence that representatives orient to their responses as consisting of both current-location status andrepair-completion status can be found in very rare (perhaps deviant) cases where representatives end current-location statuswith falling intonation that, in the sequential context of a response to an initiating action, is typical of turn-final pitchmovement. Doing so canbeapractice formarking turn completion (Szczepek-Reed, 2004) and is at oddswith (i.e., it constitutesa ‘mixed signal’ regarding) the compound nature of representatives’ responses. When this occurs, as it does in Extract 7,representatives employ practices for staving off turn transition, therebyworking to secure the compound nature of responses.

Extract 7 [Call 4]:

04 CUS: Yea::h=I wanted duh check on ay:=uh: a repair status.

((13 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

18 REP: We:ll it looks like it was entered into our

19 system on thuh ten:th, (0.4) a:nd- (0.4) currently

20 –> been in li:ne since thee eleventh.<these repairs are

21 taking fifteen tuh thirty business days.88Right now.88

22 CUS: Sh::ew. Go:d.

The representative begins the delivery of current-location status by reporting a past-tense location status: ‘‘We:ll it lookslike it was entered into our system on thuh ten:th,’’ (lines 18–19). As found in other service-industry contexts, the practice ofbeginning to respond to first-topic solicitations with events framed in the simple past tense is a practice for projecting amulti-unit response that will be complete upon the production of present-tense events (Robinson and Heritage, 2005). Bybeginning with a past-tense report of location status – as happens in Extract 7 (above), when the representative says ‘‘it wasentered. . .’’ (line 18) – representatives projectmore talk involving current-location status, bringing the report up to date. Thisclaim is supported in Extract 7 (above) by the fact that: (1) the representative ends the past-tense report of location statuswith rising intonation at the end of ‘‘ten:th,’’ (line 19), which can indicate a lack of turn-transition relevance (Szczepek-Reed,2004); (2) the customerdoesnot intercede throughout the representative’s 0.4-s inbreath (at line19); and (3) the representativecontinues to produce current-location status: ‘‘a:nd- (0.4) currently been in li:ne since thee eleventh.’’ (lines 19–21).11

In contrast to Extracts 5 and 6 (above), in Extract 7 (above) the representative ends current-location status with fallingintonation (symbolized in the transcript with a period) at the end of ‘‘eleventh.’’ (line 20), which can be a practice formarkingturn completion, and thus for projecting the relevance of turn transition (Sacks et al., 1974). The representative orients to thispossibility, as well as to the relevance of producing repair-completion status, by rushing through the transition space(symbolized in the transcript by the ‘less-than’ sign; Schegloff, 1987) to produce the second component: ‘‘these repairs aretaking. . .’’ (lines 20–21).

A final type of evidence is that, when customers produce tokens, such as Okay, that index their ‘acceptance’ of current-location status and might otherwise propose sequence closure (Beach, 1993; Schegloff, 2007), representatives do not treatthem as doing so, and instead proceed to produce repair-completion status. For example, in Extract 8 the representativetreats the customer’s Okay as a continuer (Guthrie, 1997):

Extract 8 [Call 34]:

04 CUS: ‘Eeah I was wondering if there’s any way of getting

05 a mo:re detailed update on thuh status of my: uh camera,

((4 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

10 REP: Okay? on thuh twen’y ni:nth it was sent o:ut

11 to Casio manufacturer fer thuh repa:ir?

12 –> CUS: Oka:y,

13 REP: A::nd (.) right now Ca:sio their turn around time

14 is approximately about ten tuh- fifteen business da:ys,

Across lines 10–11, the representative completes current-location status: ‘‘on thuh twen’y ni:nth it was sent o:ut to Casiomanufacturer fer thuh repa:ir?’’ Although the customer ‘lexically’ claims to accept current-location status with ‘‘Oka:y,’’(line 12; Beach, 1993; Schegloff, 2007), he designs the Okaywith slight rising intonation (symbolized in the transcript by thecomma) so as to treat the representative’s turn as ongoing (Guthrie, 1997). The representative proceeds to deliver repair-

10 For additional examples of representatives ending current-location statuswith rising intonation, see Extract 8: ‘‘. . .fer thuh repa:ir?’’ (line 11); Extract 9:

‘‘. . .worked o:n,’’ (line 37); and Extract 11: ‘‘. . .shipping department?’’ (line 11).11 This claim is also supported by the fact that the representative prefaces her initial responsive unit, which involves the past-tense report of location

status, withWell: ‘‘We:ll it looks like it was entered. . .’’ (line 18). As Schegloff and Lerner (2009) argued, this is a practice for alerting the customer that the

forthcoming response will not be straight forward.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–38593850

Page 10: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

completion status: ‘‘A::nd (.) right now Ca:sio their turn around time is approximately about ten tuh- fifteen business da:ys,’’.Extract 8 provides evidence that participants orient to the possible completion of current-location status as a place of possiblelocal (vs. global) pragmatic completion (Ford and Thompson, 1996), which constitutes a place of conditional entry for nextspeakers (Lerner, 1991). The possible completion of current-location status does not constitute a place of possible sequenceclosure, and thus customers’ Okay’s (at least ones that intonationally embody an orientation to possible continuation) areneither produced nor understood as proposing sequence closure. In these places of conditional entry, aside from claiming toaccept current-location status, customers can also be found claiming registration of current-location status with continuers(Schegloff, 1982)12 and, in rare cases, initiating repair (Schegloff et al., 1979).

3.1.2. Representatives’ orientations to repair-completion status as completing the responsive action

Whereas representatives design their talk to show that the completion of current-location status does not constitute arelevant place for turn transition (see section 3.1.1), representatives orient to the completion of repair-completion status asconstituting the completion of a response to customers’ status inquiries, and thus as constituting a relevant place for turntransition. A first type of evidence is that, while representatives routinely end current-location status with rising intonation(see above), which can be a practice for projecting further talk and thus that the current responsive turn is not yet complete,representatives routinely end repair-completion statuswith falling intonation (symbolized in transcripts by a period), whichis a practice for marking turn completion (Szczepek-Reed, 2004). For example, in Extract 4 (above), repair-completion statusis possibly completed with ‘‘. . .lo:gged into our system.’’ (lines 18–20), in Extract 5 (above) it is possibly completed with ‘‘. . .logged into our system.’’ (line 16), in Extract 6 (above) it is possibly completed with ‘‘. . .ten tuh uh: twen’y business days.’’(lines 13–14), and in Extract 7 (above) it is possibly completed with ‘‘. . .fifteen tuh thirty business days.’’ (line 21).13

Another type of evidence is that, after representatives bring repair-completion status to a place of possible completion, theytreat their response as complete by remaining silent. For example, in Extract 4 (above), the representative brings repair-completion status to possible completion after ‘‘system.’’ (line 19). The representative remains silent for 0.7 s (i.e., the silence atline 20) until the customer eventually claims to be informed by (Heritage, 1984a), and then to accept (Beach, 1993; Schegloff,2007), repair-completion status: ‘‘Oh:. okay.’’ (line 21). In Extract 5 (above), the representative brings repair-completion statusto completion after ‘‘system.’’ (line 16). The representative then remains silent for an extremely long 1.1-s (at line 17; Jefferson,1984). InExtract6 (above), the representativebrings repair-completion status topossible completionafter ‘‘days.’’ (line14). Therepresentative then remains silent for an extremely long1.3-s until the customer eventually indexes his surprise at 10–20 daysas an amount of equipment-turn-around time by producing a repeat-based repair initiator: ‘‘Ten tuh twen’y da:ys?’’ (line 16;Jefferson, 1972). By remaining silent, representatives embody ‘waiting’ for customers to make a next move.

Our argument that representatives ‘wait’ for customers to respond is reinforced by yet another type of evidence, which isthat, in cases such as Extract 9 where marked silences follow the possible completion of repair-completion status,representatives sometimes recomplete it, which can be a practice for re-establishing the relevance turn completion/transition (Pomerantz, 1984b).

Extract 9 [Call 24]:

03 Cus: .h Uh yes.=I don’t have (a/uh) computer: uh access

04 right now=>an’ I was tryin’ tuh< find out about

05 a repair.

((30 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

36 Rep: ‘Ka:y, that’s in line with thuh technician,

37 waiting to be worked o:n, .hh repair times

38 run ra- approximately ten tuh twen’y business days.

39 (0.5)

40 –> Rep: [From ] thuh time that it’s logged into our system.

41 Cus: [Wo:w.]

42 (.)

43 Cus: Oh. (.) Okay.

Upon the possible completion of repair-completion status (i.e., after ‘‘days.’’; line 38), there occurs a 0.5-s silence (line 39),which is structurally similar to those found in Extracts 4–6 (above, at lines 20, 17, and 15, respectively). Prior to the customerbeginning to speak – but simultaneous with the customer’s negative assessment of the repair-completion status, ‘‘Wo:w.’’(line 42) – the representative produces a grammatical increment (Ford et al., 2002; Schegloff, 2000) to her repair-completionstatus: ‘‘From thuh time that it’s logged into our system.’’ (line 41; This increment clarifies the start date for the 10–20 daysturn-around time). The production of this type of increment in this sequential location, especially one that is ended with

12 For example, see Extract 11: ‘‘Mm=hm,’’ (line 13); and Extract 12: ‘‘Uh hu:h’’ (line 36).13 Formore examples, see Extract 9 (below): ‘‘. . .business days.’’ (line 38); Extract 10 (below): ‘‘. . .business days.’’ (line 30); and Extract 11 (below): ‘‘. . .this

week.’’ (line 15).

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3851

Page 11: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

falling intonation (i.e., ‘‘system.’’, line 40), is a practice for re-establishing the relevance of turn completion/transition byrecompleting the turn (Schegloff, 2000), and displays the representative’s orientation to the relevance of the customerspeaking next. Along these lines, the customer proceeds to propose closure of the status-solicitation course of action byproducing ‘‘Oh.’’ (line 43), which claims a change in his state from unknowing to knowing (Heritage, 1984a), and ‘‘Okay.’’(line 43), which claims to accept the repair-completion status (Beach, 1993; Schegloff, 2007).

A final type of evidence is that, in instances such as Extract 10, when customers claim to accept repair-completion status,most commonly with the token Okay (Beach, 1993; Schegloff, 2007), representatives treat such claims as proposing closureof the status-solicitation course of action.

Extract 10 [Call 94]:

04 CUS: My- uhm laptop was se:nt onta thuh: Toshiba

05 factory from- uh:m other r’pairs.=How

06 do I check thuh status of that.

((19 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

26 REP: Okay. Yeah. It’s returned from

27 thuh manufacturer as of t’day. so:

28 it actually should be going back

29 to you: .h uhm within thuh next

30 two business days.

31 –> CUS: Oh. okay.

32 REP: Okay?

33 CUS: Thank you.

34 REP: No problem. [Bye:]

35 CUS: [Bye.]

The representative produces a possibly complete response to the customer’s status inquiry at lines 26–30. After a normaltransition space following ‘‘days.’’ (line 30; Jefferson, 1984), the customer immediately produces, ‘‘Oh. okay.’’ (line 31), where‘‘Oh.’’ claims a change in state from uninformed to informed (Heritage, 1984a), and ‘‘okay.’’ (line 31) claims to accept thestatus (Beach, 1993; Schegloff, 2007). The representative immediately orients to the relevance of possibly closing the call byinitiating a pre-closing sequence: ‘‘Okay?’’ (line 32; Schegloff and Sacks, 1973). In doing this, the representative treats thecustomer’s claim to have accepted the status as indicating possible completion of the status-solicitation course of action.

3.2. Customers’ orientations

In this subsection, we examine ways in which customers orient to the compound character of responses to statusinquiries. As in the previous section, we demonstrate that customers orient to current-location status as not completingrepresentatives’ responsive turns, and that customers orient to repair-completion status as completing such turns.

3.2.1. Customers’ orientations to current-location status as being incomplete

Customers also orient to responses to their status inquiries as being incomplete if they consist of only current-locationstatus. A first type of evidence for this is that, after representatives possibly complete their report of current-location status,customers regularly withhold talk and ‘wait’ for representatives to produce more talk, which in each case involves repair-completion status.We have already seen versions of such ‘waiting’ in Extracts 4, 6, and 9 (above). There, customers withheldtalk through representatives’ inbreaths that lasted three-tenths of a second (Extract 4, line 17) and two-tenths of a second(Extract 6, line 12; Extract 9, line 37) (In transcripts, a single ‘‘.h’’ represents one-tenth of a second). Although theseinteractions are ‘institutional’ rather than ‘ordinary,’ they are nonetheless organized by an ordinary speech exchange system(Sacks et al., 1974). As such, although the grammatical possible completion of current-location statusmight otherwisemakerelevant turn transition, this is offset by the social organization of customers’ status inquiries, which make relevantresponsive compound action units.

Extracts 4, 6, and 9 (above) are not the most convincing cases of ‘waiting’ because representatives’ inbreaths can projectmore talk (Schegloff, 1996), which can locally explain customers’ withholding of talk (e.g., in anticipation of representativescontinuing). However, we also find examples such as Extract 11 where customers withhold talk even in the midst of silence.

Extract 11 [Call 77]:

05 CUS: Ye:as. (.) I wanna follow up o:n a repa:ir?

((4 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

10 REP: Ma’m that’s shipping rea:dy? it’s in thuh

11 shipping department?

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–38593852

Page 12: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

12 –> (0.2)

13 CUS: Mm=hm,=

14 REP: =Uh:m: that’ll probably go out by thee end of

15 this week.

16 (0.2)

17 CUS: O:kay. Thank yo [u. ]

18 REP: [Ok ]ay.

19 (0.2)

20 CUS: Bye b [ye. ]

21 REP: [Buh ] bye.

At lines 10–11, the representativefirst produces a technical formulationof current-location status, ‘‘that’s shipping rea:dy?’’(line 10), which she then reformulates less technically: ‘‘it’s in thuh shipping department?’’ (lines 10–11). The representativeendsher reformulationwith rising intonation,which canbeapractice forprojectingmore talk (seeabove).However, in contrastto Extracts 4, 6, and 9 (above), rather than immediately proceeding to draw breath, the representative stops speaking. Thereensues an 0.2-s silence at line 12, which we argue is hearable as ‘belonging’ to the representative (i.e., the silence is a pause;Sacks et al., 1974), and thus is a placewhere the customer iswaiting for the representative to continue. This is supported by thefact that the customer breaks the silence by producing a continuer, ‘‘Mm=hm,’’ (line 13), which treats (and thus orients to) therepresentative’s responsive turnasnotyetcomplete (Schegloff, 1982). In linewith thisanalysis, the representative immediatelycontinues speaking and produces repair-completion status (i.e., component 2): ‘‘Uh:m: that’ll probably go out by thee end ofthis week.’’ (lines 14–15). A second case that illustrates this more convincingly is Extract 12:

Extract 12 [Call 178]:

05 REP: How can I help you.

06 CUS: Yes. I sent in: about a week er- (.) more

07 ago, .hh Uhm: something to repai:r.=If

08 I give my customer number would .hh you

09 be able s- check on thuh status.

((24 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

34 REP: Okay. It is in li:ne with our technician.

35 –> (0.2)

36 CUS: Uh hu:h.

37 (.)

38 REP: [So] it does take uh few weeks

39 CUS: [>It’ll-< ]

40 REP: befo:re [thuh repai-]

41 CUS: [Few wee: ]ks,

At line 34, the representative produces current-location status: ‘‘It is in li:newith our technician.’’ Unlike Extract 11 (above),the representativeends this componentwith falling intonation,whichcanbeapractice for indicating turncompletion, and thusformarking the relevance of turn transition (Szczepek-Reed, 2004). As in Extract 11 (above), the representative stops speaking,which results inan0.2-s silence, and the customerorients to this silence as the representative’spausebyproducinga continuer:‘‘Uh hu:h.’’ (line 36). As in Extract 11 (above), the representative proceeds to produce repair-completion status: ‘‘So it does takeuh fewweeks. . .’’ (lines 38–40). It is important to register that the prolonged ‘spaces’ (in the formof inbreaths and/or silence) inbetween theproductionof current-locationstatus and repair-completion status (aswesaw inExtracts4, 6, 9, 11, and12, above)are likely collaborative achievements, insofar as they are products of the fact that the possible completion of current-locationstatus is a place of possible local (vs. global) pragmatic completion (Ford and Thompson, 1996), and thus only a place ofconditional entry for next speakers (Lerner, 1991). In otherwords, these prolonged ‘spaces’ are likely the interactive product ofrepresentatives allowing for the possibility of customers’ conditional entry, and customers declining to enter.

A second type, and perhaps the strongest form, of evidence that customers orient to current-location status asconstituting an incomplete response to their status inquiries involves cases where: (1) representatives bring current-location status to a place of possible grammatical completion; (2) representatives then employ practices that somehowmarkpossible turn completion; and (3) customers explicitly pursue repair-completion status. These deviant cases forcefullydemonstrate that customers orient to repair-completion status as a next and required part of a complete response to a statusinquiry, and thus that customers orient to representatives’ responsive actions as normatively involving two particularordered components. For the first of two examples, see Extract 13.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3853

Page 13: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

Extract 13 [Call 32]:

06 CUS: I called a couple weeks ago:

07 tuh check on the status a r’pairs:

08 they told me: that it would

09 be comin’ back tuh me in

10 seven d’ ten days:=it’s been

11 a while. .hhh

((34 lines omitted; REP locates the customer’s order))

46 REP: Okay yeah. they’re still waiting

47 on a part tuh come in.<They said.

48 (0.2)

49 –> CUS: [Any: time f ]rame at a:ll,

50 REP: [So: uh:m ]

51 (0.2)

52 REP: Uh:m actually no. actually th- .hh

53 they have up until thirty days fer

54 thuh pa:rt? (.) eh- so:: (0.2) if it doesn’t

55 come in within thuh next week they’re

56 gunna send it o:ut to S:ony, (.) an’ I’m

57 not sure whether they gu:n’ tuh do:

58 like a buy back or somethin’.

59 CUS: .hhh

60 REP: But they will contact you.

61 (0.2)

62 REP: You’ll be (.) hearin’ somethin’ from

63 Mitch if they do something like

64 that.

65 CUS: (Oh) okay,

The representative brings current-location status to a place of possible grammatical completion with: ‘‘they’re stillwaiting on a part tuh come in.’’ (lines 46–47). The representative ends this unit of talk with falling intonation (i.e., ‘‘in.’’, line47), which can indicate possible turn completion. The representative rushes through (Schegloff, 1987) to produce agrammatical increment, ‘‘They said.’’ (line 47), which marks possible turn completion in two additional ways. First, it re-completes a responsive unit of talk (Schegloff, 2000), and second, it is ended with falling intonation (i.e., ‘‘said.’’). Finally, therepresentative stops talking, and a 0.2-s silence follows (line 48).

After the representative has produced a number of indicators of turn completion, the customer solicits repair-completionstatus: ‘‘Any: time frame at a:ll,’’ (line 49). By using the negative polarity item ‘‘Any’’, the customer builds in a grammaticalpreference for a No-response (Sacks, 1987) and thus builds in a presupposition that the representative may not be able toprovide repair-completion status. This is important because, in doing so, the customer orients to the accountability of therepresentative not providing repair-completion status. That is, if representatives do not provide repair completion status,perhaps they cannot provide it, and thus the customer orients to the normative, two-component organization of statusresponses. The customer’s presupposition turns out to be correct, insofar as the representative does not yet have theresources to provide a single time estimate (the is articulated by the representative in lines 52–58). Note that therepresentative prefaces her No-response, ‘‘actually no.’’ (line 52), with actually, which orients to the lack of repair-completion status as running counter to expectations (Clift, 2001), and thus orients to repair-completion status as being anaccountable part of a normative, two-component organization of status responses.

For a second example, see Extract 14.

Extract 14 [Call 38]:

03 CUS: I’d like tuh check on thuh sta:tus of camera

04 repair please.

((10 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

15 REP: Okay. I just checked up on thuh status uh:m

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–38593854

Page 14: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

16 a:t (0.2) thee: manufacturer, >well it went to a<

17 repair facility? .hh uhm that does thuh- (.) repairs

18 for Minoltas? .hh they started on thuh

19 repair <as of> yesterday?

20 (0.2)

21 REP: It was in repai:r at their repair facility.

22 –> CUS: ‘Kay. ‘ave=yuh got any expected time tha’=it’s

23 –> gunna be ou:t?

24 (.)

25 REP: As long as they have all thuh parts in

26 sto:ck? .hh it should be about ten tuh-

27 uh:m (.) it should be about ten business

28 days before we receive it ba:ck, .hh

29 however if they do not have thuh parts

30 in stock it can be up to thirty tuh forty

31 business days.

32 (.)

33 CUS: ‘Kay. Thuh reason I a:sk it’s already been there

34 ‘bout thirty days.

The representative produces current-location status with: ‘‘they started on thuh repair<as of> yesterday?’’ (lines 18–19).She ends this unit of talk with rising intonation at the end of ‘‘yesterday?’’ (line 19), which can indicatemore talk. After a 0.2-ssilence, in which the customer ‘waits’ for the representative to continue speaking, the representative re-completes her priorunit, and thus recompletes her presentation of current-location status; she does this through the use of a syntactic pivotconstruction (Betz, 2008) involving ‘‘<as of> yesterday. . .’’, which ended the previous unit (i.e., they started on the repair as ofyesterday), andwhich can be heard as beginning the next unit: ‘‘It was in repai:r at their repair facility.’’ (i.e., as of yesterday, itwas in repair at their repair facility). Insofar as recompleting a responsive unit of talk can be a practice for re-establishing therelevance of turn completion (Pomerantz, 1984b), and insofar as the representative ends her re-completion unit with fallingintonation (i.e., ‘‘facility.’’), which can also be a practice formarking turn completion, the representative strongly indicates thatshe has completed her response to the customer’s status inquiry.

The customer orients to the representative’s possible response completion by proposing to accept the current-locationstatus with: ‘‘‘Kay.’’ (line 22; Beach, 1993; Schegloff, 2007). However, the customer immediately proceeds to solicit repair-completion status: ‘‘‘ave=yuhgot any expected time tha’=it’s gunnabeou:t?’’ (lines 22–23).Note that, aswe found inExtract 13(above, and as we argued there), the customer uses the negative polarity item Any, which orients to the possibility that therepresentative isnotable toproviderepair-completionstatus,andthusto its lackofprovisionasbeingaccountable. Bysolicitingrepair-completion status, the customer orients to its relevance as part of a complete response to his status inquiry. Therepresentative responds by providing repair-completion status (lines 25–31).

A final type of evidence is found in the single deviant case in our data where a customer actually treats current-locationstatus as constituting a complete (vs. incomplete) response to their status inquiry (Extract 15, below). However, thecustomer orients to doing so as being accountable, and thus non-normative.

Extract 15 [Call 6]:

4 CUS: .hh Yes I’d like tuh check up on an order.

((12 lines omitted; REP locates customer’s order))

17 REP: Thee estimate was appro:ved on thuh twenty four:th,

18 (0.2)

19 REP: .hh A:n’ it’s currently in repair with thuh technician,

20 (0.2)

21 CUS: Okay excellent.

22 (0.2)

23 –> CUS: >That’s all I need tuh know.<

24 REP: Okay?

25 CUS: Thank=you=bye.

26 REP: No problem. bye:.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3855

Page 15: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

The representative possibly completes current-location status with: ‘‘. . .it’s currently in repair with thuh technician,’’(line 19). After an 0.2-s silence (line 20), the customer both claims to accept and positively assesses current-locationstatus with ‘‘Okay excellent.’’ (line 21). These two lexical units (Okay and Excellent) are prosodically produced as a singleunit of talk (Helasvuo, 2001). The customer orients to this turn as being designed to treat current-location status asconstituting a complete response to his status inquiry. Specifically, the customer proceeds to provide an account fordoing so: ‘‘>That’s all I need tuh know.<’’ (line 23). With his account, the customer orients to the fact that moreinformation would normally be provided, and thus that current-location status does not normally constitute a completeresponse to a status inquiry.

3.2.2. Customers’ orientations to repair-completion status as completing the response

There is also evidence that customers orient to the completion of repair-completion status as constituting the completionof a response to their status inquiries, and thus as constituting not only a relevant place for turn transition, but for beginninga new course of action, such as closing (in these largely mono-topical calls; Schegloff and Sacks, 1973). We already saw anindication of this in Extract 10 (above), where, after the delivery of repair-completion status (at line 30), the customer andrepresentative jointly collaborate in closing both the status-solicitation course of action and the call. In Extract 11 (above),after the representative possibly completes the delivery of repair-completion status, ‘‘Uh:m: that’ll probably go out by theeend of this week.’’ (lines 14–15), the customer claims to accept it with: ‘‘O:kay.’’ (line 17). The customer then proceeds tothank the representative, ‘‘Thank you.’’ (line 17), which is a practice, in customer-service contexts, for initiating a call pre-closing sequence (Zimmerman, 2006; Wakin and Zimmerman, 1999; Raymond and Zimmerman, 2007). Thus, in Extract 11,the customer’s ‘‘Thank you.’’ (line 17) orients to his ‘‘O:kay’’ (line 17) – which is a claim to accept repair-completion status –as projecting possible completion of the status-inquiry course of action.

One might wonder if customers’ status inquiries more strongly obligate repair-completion status (i.e., the project’send goal) relative to current-location status, which might be relevant-but-optional. However, in every case in our data,representatives respond to customers’ status inquiries with current-location status, and do so prior to repair-completionstatus. Stated negatively, although the data contain deviant cases in which customers hold representatives accountablefor ‘failing’ to provide repair-completion status (see Extracts 13 and 14, above), representatives never fail to providecurrent-location status. These facts suggest that customers’ status inquiries make both types of statuses (i.e., current-location status and repair-completion status) conditionally relevant (Schegloff and Sacks, 1973), rather than merelyrelevant.

4. Conclusion

This article identified a previously unstudied initiating action that is similar to the story prompt (Lerner, 1992) in that itmakes conditionally relevant a compound action unit (Lerner, 1991). Specifically, we identified the status inquiry, whichinvolved a customer, who was calling an electronics-repair organization (referred to as Jack Camera), soliciting an updateregarding the status of their equipment that was currently in some stage of the process of being repaired. We demonstratedthat status inquiries make conditionally relevant a compound action unit that minimally contains two, ordered pieces ofinformation (i.e., equipment-location status and repair-completion status), each of which occupy at least one sentential unitof talk. Along these lines, we demonstrated that the possible completion of equipment-location status is a place of possiblelocal (vs. global) pragmatic completion (Ford and Thompson, 1996), and thus only a place of conditional entry for nextspeakers (Lerner, 1991), whereas the possible completion of repair-completion status is a place of possible global pragmaticcompletion that constitutes a place of possible sequence closure. Although this compound action unit is likely specific to thepresent institutional context (vs. generic to conversation), our findings raise the possibility that, in addition to sententialunits being ‘semi-permeable’ (ala. Lerner, 1991, 1996), so are larger units, such as entire turns. In the case of sentential units,it is (largely) grammatical organization that provides for recognizable places of conditional entry (ala. Lerner, 1991, 1996),whereas in the case of compound action units, it is (largely) pragmatic organization (of course, the organization ofpragmatics is simultaneously conditioned by that of syntax and prosody).

It is worth attempting to explain how or why customers’ status inquiries make conditionally relevant this particular typeof compound action unit. Ethnographically, we know that, when customers send equipment in for repair, they are informedabout the organization’s website and encouraged to check the progress of their repair online. This ethnographic claim issupported by the fact that customers sometimes orient to the website in their status-inquiry turns. For example, in Extract 5(above), the customer solicits ‘‘a little bit mo:re information’’ (lines 2–3) on his camera, that is, more than what is currentlyprovided on the website; In Extract 9 (above), the customer prefaces his solicitation by claiming to not have ‘‘computer: uhaccess right now’’ (lines 3–4). The website provides customers with equipment-location status.

Thus, upon calling, customers may (but only may) know about current-location status. Customers sometimes orient toknowing current-location status in their status-inquiry turns. For example, in Extract 10 (above), as background to hisrequest, the customer says: ‘‘My- uhm laptop was se:nt onta thuh: Toshiba factory from- uh:m other r’pairs.’’ (lines 4–5).Much more frequently, though, status-inquiry turns do not reveal whether or not customers know about current-locationstatus. For example, customers frequently solicit status with ‘‘I need tuh check thuh status of my:: repair laptop,’’ (Extract 4,above, lines 4–5), ‘‘Check on a repai:r plea:se’’ (Extract 6, above, line 5), and ‘‘I wanna follow up o:n a repa:ir?’’ (Extract 11,above, line 5).

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–38593856

Page 16: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

There is an interactional rule that one should not ask for information one already has (Heritage and Robinson, 2006;Levinson, 2000).14 In the present context, customers are likely to know about current-location status from the website, andthus are likely to be soliciting some type of additional information. Due to this interactional rule, customers’ status inquiriesare produced and understood as making conditionally relevant (Schegloff and Sacks, 1973) another (or at least a different)type of status. As is made evident by how participants conduct themselves during responses, this additional/different statusturns out to be repair-completion status.

In sum, the action of sending electronic equipment in to Jack Camera to be repaired sets in motion what Lerner (1998)might call a completable project, which has a projectable, goal-oriented ending of the equipment being repaired.15 Thus,when customers call, prior to the completion of the repair, to check on the ‘status’ of their equipment, customers’ statusinquiries are produced and understood as ones regarding at least the end-goal of the project, which is repair-completionstatus. That said, customers are calling prior to the realization of the project’s end goal – that is, prior to having received theirequipment back – and thus current-location status is also relevant.

As reviewed by Selting (2000), within conversation analysis, the term turn constructional unit is frequently definedsyntactically (e.g., a word, phrase, clause, or sentence) and, more recently, prosodically as well. However, Schegloff (1996)clarified: ‘‘By ‘turn-constructional unit,’ it may be recalled,wemeant to register that these units can constitute possibly complete

turns; on their possible completion, transition to a next speaker becomes relevant (although not necessarily accomplished)’’(p. 55, emphasis added). The present article contributes to literature demonstrating that, in addition to syntactic andprosodic schemata, participants rely on pragmatic schemata – including what Selting (2000) referred to as activity-type-specific schemata – for understanding what constitutes a turn constructional unit (Selting, 2000, p. 512).

The concept of a compound action unit, which can be achieved within the confines of a single sequence of action, can bedifferentiated from the concept of activity, which literature tends to define as being achieved across more than one sequenceof action (for review, see Robinson, 2012). However, like activities, compound action units are associatedwith particular setsof inferential schemata that inform the nature and organization of their sub-parts (Levinson, 1992). Literature suggeststhe presence of other, more ordinary (vs. institutional) compound action units, such as giving driving directions and foodrecipes (Goldberg, 1975). If Sacks (1995) was correct that ‘‘[a] culture is an apparatus for generating recognizable actions’’(p. 226, emphasis original), then compound action units are (like other types of overall structural organization) a criticalcomponent of culture in need of further analytic attention.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Galina Bolden, John Heritage, Craig R. Scott, and the anonymous reviewers for comments on previousdrafts.

Appendix A[(Fig._1)TD$FIG]

Fig. 1. Computer screen shot of repair status.

14 The corollary to this rule is ‘do not tell people information they already know’ (Heritage and Robinson, 2006; Maynard, 2003; Terasaki, 2004).15 Another projectable ending is that the equipment cannot be repaired, but this is arguably not customers’ (or the institution’s) primary goal.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3857

Page 17: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

References

Atkinson, J.M., Heritage, J., 1984. Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Beach, W.A., 1993. Transitional regularities for ‘casual’ ‘‘Okay’’ usages. Journal of Pragmatics 19, 325–352.Betz, E., 2008. Grammar and Interaction: Pivots in German Conversation. John Benjamins, Amsterdam.Clayman, S., Heritage, J., 2002. The News Interview: Journalisms and Public Figures on the Air. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Clift, R., 2001. Meaning in interaction: the case of ‘actually’. Language 77, 245–291.Drew, P., Heritage, J., 1992. Analyzing talk at work: an introduction. In: Drew, P., Heritage, J. (Eds.), Talk at Work. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,

pp. 3–65.Ford, C.E., 2001a. Contingency and units in interaction. Discourse Studies 6, 27–52.Ford, C.E., 2001b. At the intersection of turn and sequence: negation and what comes next. In: Selting, M., Couper-Kuhlen, E. (Eds.), Studies in Interactional

Linguistics. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 51–79.Ford, C., Thompson, S., 1996. Interactional units in conversation: syntactic, intonational and pragmatic resources for themanagement of turns. In: Ochs,

E., Schegloff, E.A., Thompson, S.A. (Eds.), Interaction and Grammar. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 134–184.Ford, C., Fox, B., Thompson, S., 2002. Constituency and turn increments. In: Ford, C., Fox, B., Thompson, S. (Eds.), The Language of Turns and Sequences.

Oxford University Press, London.Goldberg, J., 1975. A system for the transfer of instructions in natural settings. Semiotica 14, 269–296.Guthrie, A.M., 1997. On the systematic deployment of Okay and Mmhmm in academic advising sessions. Pragmatics 7, 397–415.Helasvuo, M.L., 2001. Emerging syntax for interaction. In: Selting, M., Couper-Kuhlen, E. (Eds.), Studies in Interactional Linguistics. John Benjamins,

Amsterdam, pp. 25–50.Heritage, J., 1984a. A change-of-state token and aspects of its sequential placement. In: Atkinson, J.M., Heritage, J. (Eds.), Structures of Social Action.

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 299–345.Heritage, J., 1984b. Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Heritage, J., Robinson, J.D., 2006. The structure of patients’ presenting concerns: physicians’ opening questions. Health Communication 19, 89–102.Houtkoop, H., Mazeland, H., 1985. Turns and discourse units in everyday conversation. Journal of Pragmatics 9, 595–619.Jefferson, G., 1972. Side sequences. In: Sudnow, D. (Ed.), Studies in Social Interaction. Free Press, New York, pp. 294–338.Jefferson, G., 1978. Sequential aspects of storytelling in conversation. In: Schenkein, J. (Ed.), Studies in the Organization of Conversational Interaction.

Academic Press, New York, pp. 219–248.Jefferson, G., 1984. Notes on some orderlinesses of overlap onset. In: D’Urso, V., Leonardi, P. (Eds.), Discourse Analysis and Natural Rhetorics. Cleup Editore,

Padua, pp. 11–38.Jefferson, Gail, 1991. List construction as a task and resource. In: Psathas, G. (Ed.), Interactional Competence. Irvington Publishers, New York, NY, pp. 63–92.Lerner, G.H., 1991. On the syntax of sentences in progress. Language in Society 20, 441–458.Lerner, G.H., 1992. Assisted storytelling: deploying shared knowledge as a practical matter. Qualitative Sociology 15 (3), 247.Lerner, G.H., 1996. On the ‘semi-permeable’ character of grammatical units in conversation: conditional entry into the turn space of another speaker. In:

Schegloff, E.A., Ochs, E., Thompson, S. (Eds.), Interaction and Grammar. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 238–276, 133.Lerner, G.H., 1998. Completable projects and winnable games: notes on the organization of activity. Paper presented to the Center for Language, Interaction

and Culture, UCLA.Levinson, S., 1983. Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Levinson, S.C., 1992. Activity types and language. In: Drew, P., Heritage, J. (Eds.), Talk at work: Interaction in institutional settings. Cambridge University

Press, pp. 66–100.Levinson, S., 2000. Presumptive Meanings: The Theory of Generalized Conversational Implicature. MIT Press, Cambridge.Lindstrom, A., 1997. Designing social actions: grammar, prosody, and interaction in Swedish conversation. Dissertation. University of California, Los

Angeles.Local, J., Walker, G., 2004. Abrupt-joins as a resource for the production of multi-unit, multi-action turns. Journal of Pragmatics 36, 1375–1403.Mandelbaum, J., 2012. Storytelling in ordinary conversation. In: Sidnell, J., Stivers, T. (Eds.), Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Cambridge University

Press, Cambridge.Maynard, D., 2003. Bad News, Good News: Conversational Order in Everyday Talk and Clinical Settings. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Pomerantz, A., 1984a. Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In: Atkinson, J.M., Heritage, J.

(Eds.), Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 57–101.Pomerantz, A., 1984b. Pursuing a response. In: Atkinson, J.M., Heritage, J. (Eds.), Structures of Social Action. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp.

152–164.Raymond, G., 2003. Grammar and social organization: Yes/No interrogatives and the structure of responding. American Sociological Review 68, 939–967.

[(Fig._2)TD$FIG]

Fig. 2. Whiteboard listing repair completion time.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–38593858

Page 18: Extending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of ... · PDF fileExtending the notion of pragmatic completion: The case of the responsive ... pragmatics (Ford and ... elements

Author's personal copy

Raymond, G., Zimmerman, D., 2007. Rights and responsibilities in calls for help: the case of the Painted Cave fire. Research on Language and SocialInteraction 40, 31–61.

Robinson, J.D., 2009. Managing counterinformings: an interactional practice for soliciting information that facilitates reconciliation of speakers’incompatible positions. Human Communication Research 35, 561–587.

Robinson, J.D., 2012. Overall structural organization. In: Sidnell, J., Stivers, T. (Eds.), Handbook of Conversation Analysis. Cambridge University Press,Cambridge.

Robinson, J.D., Heritage, J., 2005. The structure of patients’ presenting concerns: the completion relevance of current symptoms. Social Science & Medicine61, 481–493.

Sacks, H., 1987. On the preferences for agreement and contiguity in sequences in conversation. In: Lee, B. (Ed.), Talk and Social Organization. MultilingualMatters, Cleveland, pp. 54–69.

Sacks, H., 1995. In: Jefferson, G. (Ed.), Lectures on conversation Volumes I and II. Blackwell, Oxford.Sacks, H., Schegloff, E., Jefferson, G., 1974. A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 50, 696–735.Schegloff, E., 1982. Discourse as an interactional achievement: some uses of ‘‘uh huh’’ and other things that come between sentences. In: Tannen, D.

(Ed.), Analyzing Discourse: Text and Talk. Georgetown University Press, Georgetown, pp. 71–93.Schegloff, E., 1987. Analyzing single episodes of interaction: an exercise in conversation analysis. Social Psychology Quarterly 50, 101–114.Schegloff, E., 1996. Turn organization: one intersection of grammar and interaction. In: Schegloff, E.A., Ochs, E., Thompson, S. (Eds.), Interaction and

Grammar. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 52–133.Schegloff, E.A., 1998. Reflections on studying prosody in talk-in-interaction. Language and Speech 41, 235–263.Schegloff, E.A., 2000. On turns’ possible completion, more or less: increments and trail-offs. Paper presented at the National Communication Association,

Seattle, Washington, November 2000.Schegloff, E.A., 2006. Words, words, words . . . phrases, phrases, phrases. Paper presented at the National Communication Association, San Antonio, Texas,

November 2006.Schegloff, E., 2007. Sequence Organization in Interaction. Cambridge University Press, New York.Schegloff, E.A., Lerner, G.H., 2009. Beginning to respond:well-prefaced responses toWh-questions. Research on Language and Social Interaction 42, 91–115.Schegloff, E.A., Sacks, H., 1973. Opening up closings. Semiotica 8, 289–327.Schegloff, E.A., Jefferson, G., Sacks, H., 1979. The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language 53, 361–382.Selting, M., 2000. The construction of units in conversational talk. Language in Society 29, 477–516.Szczepek-Reed, B., 2004. Turn-final intonation in English. In: Couper-Kuhlen, E., Ford, C.E. (Eds.), Sound Patterns in Interaction. John Benjamins, Amsterdam,

The Netherlands, pp. 97–117.Terasaki, A.K., 2004. Pre-announcement sequences in conversation. In: Lerner, G.H. (Ed.), Conversation Analysis: Studies from the First Generation.

Benjamins, Philadelphia, pp. 171–223.Wakin, M., Zimmerman, D., 1999. Reduction and specialization in emergency and directory assistance calls. Research on Language and Social Interaction 32,

409–437.Zimmerman, D., 2006. How closingmatters in emergency calls. Paper presented at the American Sociological Association,Montreal, Quebec, Canada, August

2006.

Heidi Kevoe Feldman (Ph.D. Communication, Rutgers University) is an assistant professor in the Department of Communication Studies at NortheasternUniversity. Her research focuses on the use of language and the organization of action in both ordinary and organizational settings.

Jeffrey D. Robinson (Ph.D., Sociology, UCLA;MA, Communication, USC; BA, Communication, UCSB) is an associate professor in the Department of Communicationat Portland State University. His research focuses on language and social interaction in both ordinary and institutional contexts.

Jenny Mandelbaum (Ph.D. in Communication Studies, University of Texas at Austin; BA, French and Philosophy, Oxford University) is associate professor ofCommunication at Rutgers University. Her research addresses the organization of everyday interaction, with particular emphasis on storytelling and othercommunication practices through which relationships and identities are constructed and managed.

H. Kevoe-Feldman et al. / Journal of Pragmatics 43 (2011) 3844–3859 3859