fisher 1993 jcr

Upload: lucianaeu

Post on 01-Jun-2018

224 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    1/14

    ocial esirability

    Indirect Questioning

    and the Validity of

    ROBERT J. FISHER

    Indirect  i.e.,  structured projective) questioning has been employed frequently in

    marketing and other social sciences to reduce social desirability bias, that is, sys-

    tematic error in self-report measures resulting from the desire of respondents to

    avoid embarrassment and project a favorable image to others. Yet little is known

    about the validity of indirect questioning in reducing social desirability bias. This

    article reports on three studies that examine indirect questioning as a technique to

    reduce social desirability bias on self-report measures. The effects of asking indirect

     i.e.,  structured, projective) questions were compared with direct  i.e.,  structured,

    personal) questions. The pattern of results indicates that indirect questioning reduces

    social desirability bias on variables subject to social influence and has no significant

    effect on socially neutral variables. The social nature of the differences between

    direct and indirect questioning groups, and the attribution of an undesirable trait to

    an out-group but not an in-group target, supports the view that subjects projected

    their beliefs and evaluations in the indirect response situation. These results are

    consistent across several product categories and indirect question wordings.

    M

    uch of what we think we know about human

    behavior comes from self-report measures (Pe-

    terson and Kerin 1981). Unfortunately, the basic hu-

    man tendency to present oneself in the best possible

    light can significantly distort the information gained

    from self-reports. Respondents are often unwilling or

    unable to report accurately on sensitive topics for ego-

    defensive or impression management reasons. The re-

    sult is data that are systematically biased toward re-

    spond ents' perceptions of what is cor rec t or socially

    acceptable (Maecoby and Maccoby 1954). This phe-

    nomenon is called social desirability bias and has been

    found to occur in virtually all types of self-report mea-

    sures and across nearly all social sciences literatures

    (e.g.. Levy 1981; Peltier and Walsh 1990; Robinette

    1991;  Simon and Simon 1975; Zerbe and Paulhus

    1987).

    Not only is social desirability bias pervasive, but it

    can lead to the reporting of spurious or misleading re-

    search results. Prior studies have found that social de-

    sirability bias can attenuate, inflate, or moderate vari-

    able relationships (Zerbe and Paulhus 1987); increase

    measurement error (cf. Cote and Buckley 1988); and

    *Robert J. Fisher is assistant professor of marketing. School of

    Business Adm inistration. U niversity of Southern C alifornia. Los An-

    geles. CA. 90089-1421. The author would like to thank Valerie S.

    Folkes. Jill A. Grace. J. Jeffrey Inma n. Kent Naka moto. Linda L.

    Price. Dennis W. Rook, and David W. Stewart for comments on

    earlier versions of this article. The author also benefited from dis-

    cussions with Stephen J. Hoch and com ments by the editor and three

    reviewers.

    affect variable means (Peterson and Kerin 1981), Re-

    search that does not recognize and compensate for social

    desirability bias may lead to unwarranted theoretical

    or practical conclusions about consumers' psychological

    traits (e.g.. Cam pbell 1950; Peltier and Walsh 1990);

    purchase motivations (e.g.. Levy 1981); and attitudes,

    intentions, and behaviors (e.g., Mensch and Kandel

    1988).

    An important technique used by researchers to mit-

    igate the effects of social desirability bias is indirect (i.e.,

    structured, projective) questioning.' Indirect question-

    ing is a projective technique that asks subjects or re-

    spondents to answer structured questions from the per-

    spective of another person or group (Anderson 1978;

    Calder and Burnkrant 1977; Robertson and Joselyn

    1974). For example, marketing researchers have asked

    subjects to predict the types of people m ost likely to eat

    hot cereal (Westfall. Boyd, and Campbell 1957); to in-

    dicate the extent to which a con sum er would react

    to reference group influence (Bearden and Etzel 1982;

    Brinberg and Plim pton 1986; Park and Lessig 1977);

    to identify the emotional responses of the anonymous

    author of

     

    story describing a past consumption expe-

    rience (Havlena and Holbrook 1986); and to state the

    'In ma rketing research the emph asis has been on »/istructured pro-

    jective methods such as thematic apperception tests, sentence com-

    pletion , and word associa tion (e.g., Levy 1981; Rook 1985; for a

    review see Kassarjian [1974]). Unlike the structured approach iden-

    tified here, unstructured metJiods allow subjects m uch greater freedom

    in organizing and responding to the projective stimulus. Conse-

    quently, unstructured projective questioning is particularly valuable

    in helping subjects express latent m otivations.

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    2/14

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    3/14

    305

    hese outcomes. This component reflects the extent to

    which individuals are m otivated by the expectations of

    another person or group. The individual, by fulfilling

    (or acting contrary to) the expectations of others, an-

    ticipates socially mediated rewards (or punishments).

    The individual does what a referent wants him or her

    to do because it is instrumental in achieving a social

    purpose.

    Social desirability b ias is likely to affect self-rep orted

    normative outcomes because the pursuit of social ap-

    proval as a purchase motivation is inconsistent with

    social norms and expectations. For example, shopping

    list research by Haire (1950) and replications or ex-

    tension s by othe rs (e.g., Robertson and Joselyn 1974)

    suggest that consumers are unable or unwilling to ex-

    press the extent to which they are motivated by social

    factors. When asked directly, consumption motivations

    tend to focus on the functional aspects of a product

    such as taste or gas mileage (Haire 1950). Con sum ption

    motivations such as social positioning and the approval

    of others are revealed only with indirect questioning.

    Research on American values also indicates that the

    overt pursuit of social approval through consumption

    is undesirable. Am ericans consistently assign a low rank

    to social recognition (the respect and admiration of

    others) and a high rank to independence as cultural

    values (Rokeach 1979, p. 133: Rokeach and Ba ll-Ro-

    keach 1989, p. 778). Of 18 termina l v alues, social rec-

    ognition was ranked 17th in 1971 and 18th in 1974 and

    1981,

     while independe nce was ranked third in all three

    studies. Given that self-reported values represent cul-

    turally desirable ideals (Rokeach 1979), these rankings

    imply that expressing one's desire for social approval

    is inappropriate in American culture.

    Accordingly, socially desirable responding behavior

    should cause normative outcomes to be understated

    with direct questioning because subjects are inclined to

    present themselves in a way that

     is

     consistent with social

    expectations. Under indirect questioning, however,

    subjects are able to disengage themselves from the social

    implications of their responses, leading to larger esti-

    mates of normative beliefs and evaluations. Subjects

    should most actively engage in impression management

    when they believe their responses will be used as evi-

    dence about themselves and not as objective statements

    about the real wo rld.

    The anonymity of the research setting should interact

    with questioning method to have a differential effect on

    self-reported normative outcomes. Removal of ano-

    nymity should cause subjects answering direct questions

    to feel greater pressure to make their responses consis-

    tent with social expectations. These responses are most

    likely to have symbolic or comm unication al properties

    because they represent the personal thoughts and feel-

    ings of the subject on a socially sensitive topic and the

    responses are visible to others. However, eliminating

    anonymity is not expected to have the same effect on

    subjects answering indirect questions because they are

    distanced from their responses by the projective

    method. A summary of study hypotheses related to the

    effects of questioning method and anonymity on mean

    scores follows:

    Hla:

      Method of question ing has no effect on mean

    personal outcomes because of the social

    neutrality of this variable.

    ,  Hlb:  Indirect questioning reduces social desir-

    ability bias, resulting in higher mean nor-

    mative outcomes.

    H2a:  Anonymity has no effect on mean personal

    outcomes because of the social neutrality of

    this variable.

    H2b:  Removing anonymity increases social desir-

    ability bias, resulting in lower mean nor-

    mative outcomes under direct que stioning.

    M e t h o d

    A 2 (questioning method)

     X

      2 (anonymity) between-

    subjects experiment was performed to test the hy-

    potheses.  convenience sample of 184 male and female

    undergraduate students was selected and randomly as-

    signed to the con ditions (46 per cell). A student sample

    was selected because of the widespread study of this

    population in consumer research on normative influ-

    ence and other socially sensitive topics. Subjects were

    contacted within a normal classroom situation and told

    that the purpose of the study was to generate infor-

    mation to be used in the advertising campaign for a

    new produ ct targeted at college stud ents. A fictional

    new product was selected to avoid the influence of prior

    beliefs on subjects' responses. A pretest indicated the

    new product, an innovative stereo headphone designed

    for use with Walkman-type cassette or compact-disc

    players, was believable and of interest to those in the

    target market. Moreover, the pretest indicated that

    students perceived that adoption of the new product

    had significant social implications because of its visi-

    bility and innovative features. To enhance involvement,

    subjects were shown a mock-up of the new product, a

    professionally designed brochure layout, and entered

    in a drawing for three $50 prizes for participating in

    the study.

    The experimental manipulations took the form of

    instructions to subjects and question wording.-* In the

    direct questioning co ndition, subjects were asked to re-

    spond to a series of items in terms of their own beliefs

    and evaluations. In the indirect questioning condition,

    subjects were asked to predict the likely responses of

      a typical college stu de nt. Item wording differed be-

    ^The new headphone design was described as similar in size and

    weight lo those currently used with portable stereo systems, except

    that the new design does not require wires connecting the headp hone

    set to the player unit.

    'All manipulations and measures

     are available from the author

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    4/14

    306

    JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEAR

    Iween conditions only to the extent that third-person

    wording was substituted for first-person wording in the

    indirect cond ition. For example, the words the typical

    college student will . . . were substituted for I

    will . . . in the indirect condition . The items mea-

    suring the constructs of interest were randomly mixed

    with other items unrelated to this study.

    Anonymity was manipulated with information about

    the likelihood subjects would be asked to discuss their

    responses with a researcher after com pleting the instru-

    ment. Subjects in the unanonymous condition were

    told, You may be asked to discuss your responses with

    a researcher when you have finished. In addition, sub-

    jects in this condition were asked to include their stu-

    dent identification num ber on the first page of the in-

    strument. Subjects in the anonymous condition were

    told. Yo ur responses are completely ano nym ous, and

    they were not asked to include their student identifi-

    cation number.

    Measurement

    After a review of the relevant literatures, 10 items

    were generated to measure the personal and normative

    outcome constructs. The items, worded in the first per-

    son {i.e., directly), were administered as part of a larger

    data collection efi'ort to 90 gradua te and undergra duate

    students at another university. Two items were deleted

    by corrected item-to-total correlations and principal

    axis factor analysis with varimax rotation . The analyses

    resulted in eight items measuring the two constructs of

    interest.

    Measures of the con struc ts were designed specifically

    for the student sample and new headphone product.

    For personal outcom es, a pretest on an unrelated sample

    indicated that one of the new product's key advantages

    was the increased freedom of movement it afforded.

    This attribute was identified by pretest respondents as

    being intrinsically valued, that is. desirable regardless

    of the social aspects of cons um ption . C onseque ntly, be-

    liefs and evaluations of personal outcomes were mea-

    sured with items such as the new headpho nes provide

    more freedom of movement than other headphones.

    For normative outcomes, subjects were asked to indi-

    cate their beliefs and evaluations of statements such as

      students I know would have a favorable reaction if I

    bought one of the new prod ucts . The belief com po-

    nents of the outcome variables were measured with

    seven-point highly likely to not at all likely scales.

    The evaluation comp onents were measured with seven-

    point very imp ortan t to very unim porta nt scales.

    For hypothesis testing, the multi-item scales were sum-

    mated to form a single indicator of each construct.

    Results

    Measurement.  Confirmatory factor analyses were

    conducted for the individual two-factor models within

    TABLE

    SCALE CHARACTERISTICS STUDY

     

    Group measure

    Direct

     { = 92):

    Personal outcomes

    Beliefs only

    Evaluations only

    Normative outcomes

    Beliefs only

    Evaluations only

    Indirect {

    ^ 92):

    Personal outcomes

    Beliefs only

    Evaluations only

    Normative outcomes

    Beliefs orly

    Evaluations only

    No,

    of

    items

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    4

    Actual

    scale

    range

    47-144

    12-24

    15-24

    0-105

    0 -22

    0 -24

    42-144

    9 -24

    11-24

    0-132

    5-22

    0-24

    Scale

    mean

    109,4

    20,4

    21.3

    36,4

    12.7

    9 2

    111.8

    20.6

    21.4

    65.8

    15.9

    15.3

    SD

    23.2

    2.9

    2.6

    29.6

    5.8

    6.4

    27.1

    3.3

    2.9

    31.6

    4.0

    5.2

    A

    7

    .

    .

    .

    .

    .

    ,

    .

    .

    .

    .

     

    NOTE,—Personal

     outcomes

     and

     normative outcomes were measured

     as

    sum of the

      product

      of

      beliefs

      (i.e.. no t at all  likely to

      'highly likely )

     

    evaluations

     (i,e.,

      very unimportant

    to

      very important').

     A ll

     scales were sco

    from zero

      to six.

      Higher numbers indicate higher levels

     of

     each variable.

    the direct and indirect que stioning groups. Both mod

    have acceptable internal and external consistency giv

    an overall X of 34.25  df^  19, p = .017) with a go

    ness-of-fit index (GFI) ^ .92 for the direct group and

     of

     33 45

     {df=  19. p = .021) with a GFI = .92 for

    indirect group. The normed-fit index (Bentler and B

    nett 1980) indicates that 91 percent of the observe

    measure covariation is explained in each of the me

    surement models.

    The internal consistency of the m easures is support

    by three additiona l tests. First, the scales exhibited go

    internal consistency with coefficient alphas for person

    outcomes of .79 (direct) and .81 (indirect) and forno

    mative outcomes of  91 (direct) and .89 (indirec t). S

    ond, variance-extracted estimates reflect the amount

    variance captured by a measure relative to rando

    mea surem ent e rror (Fornell and L arcker 1981). A

    measures had variance-extracted estimates that a

    proached or exceeded the .50 level with values of .

    (direct) and .53 (indirect) for personal outcomes an

    .71 (direct) and .66 (indirect) for normative outcome

    Finally, all indicator /-values in the confirmatory fac

    analysis exceeded 5.0  p

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    5/14

    3 7

      the  unanonymous mean  X =  10.96).  No  unin-

     or  interaction effects were found. Second,

    y manipulation  may have caused subjects

      for  their responses than  stu-

      in the

      unanonymous condition, thus causing

      in  this group  to  respond more carefully  (cf.

     and Kim  1987). To evaluate this possible con-

      a summ ated four-item man ipulation check was

      to  measure subjects' perceptions about  the  care

      1

     w as care-

      I answered  the questions on this survey. No

      (/? > .10) were found  for  anonymity,

      or

      anonymity

      X

      questioning

      on response care.

    Tests of Hypotheses.  Hypothesis  la was supported

     no main effect of questioning method on personal

      ^ .43, NS),

     with similar means

     in

      the  direct

      {X =

      109.4)  and  indirect  (A =  111.8)

      lb was

     supported with

     a

      main

      of  questioning method  on  normative outcomes

    F{ 1.180) = 43.15, /J

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    6/14

    308

    J O U R N A L O F C O N S U M E R R E S E A R

    Measure and source

    Personal outcome:

    Beliefs X evaluations

    Anon

    Oues

    Anon X ques

    Beliefs only:

    Anon

    Ques

    Anon X ques

    Evaluations only:

    Anon

    Ques

    Anon X ques

    Normative outcomes:

    Beliefs X evaluations

    Anon

    Ques

    Anon X ques

    Beliefs only:

    Anon

    Ques

    Anon X ques

    Evaluations only:

    Anon

    Ques

    Anon X ques

    df

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    t

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    1

    TABLE 2

    ANQVA RESULTS: STUDY 1

    Sum of

    squares

    13.6

    272.7

    869.6

    .2

    2 2

    26.6

    1.4

    .2

    .2

    115.8

    39.619.6

    5 544 0

    1.6

    453.9

    35.7

    52 2

    1 704 4

    244.3

    F

    .02

    .43

    1.37

    .02

    .23

    2.81

    .18

    .03

    .03

    .13

    4 3 . 1 5

    6.04-

    .06

    18 .30

    1.44

    1.62

    5 2 . 7 7 -

    7 . 5 6

    18

     02

     09

     21

    .03

    Anonymous

    and direct

    110.3

    109.4

    20.5

    20,4

    21.3

    21.3

    51.9

    36.4

    14.2

    12.7

    12.8

    9 2

    Means

    Unanonym

    and indire

    110.8

    111.8

    20.4

    20.6

    21.4

    21.4

    ' •

    •  I

    50.3

    65,8

    14.4

    15.9

    11.7

    15.3

    NOTE.— n

      = 184 for Study 1. The independent variables are the djchotomous manipulations while the dependent variables are the continuous manipulatjon che

    Anon, manipulated anonymity: ques, manipulated questioning method. Although the hypotheses relate to the overall personal outcome and normative outco

    variables, the effects of the manipulations on the individual belief and evaluation components are included to provide additional detail.

    •p < .05.

    " p <

      .01.

      ' '

    to manage their responses when answering directly

    worded questions on a socially sensitive variable in an

    unanonymous situation.

    STUDY 2

    A second experim ent was designed to investigate the

    effects of indirect questioning on the relationships be-

    tween personal and norm ative outcomes and intentions.

    This study was undertaken for two reasons. First, many

    consumer researchers on socially sensitive topics are

    interested in structural orassociational evidence rather

    than mean scores {e.g., Alpert  1971; Fishbein and Ajzen

    1975). This investigation could not be carried out in

    study  because a Box's M-test revealed significant dif-

    ferences in the covariance matrices across anonymity

    conditions within the indirect group (X" ^ 13.67,  f

    = 3, /? < .01). As a consequence, it would be inappro-

    priate to pool the anonymous and unanonymous co-

    variance matrices within each questioning group. Sec-

    ond, it is desirable to establish the reliability of the key

    findings of study  through a partial replication.

    Consistent with study I, social desirability bias shou

    differentially affect variables that are neutral and se

    sitive to social influence. Again using the Miniard a

    Cohen (1983) model of behavioral intentions, the

    lationship between personal outcomes and intentio

    is expected to be unaffected by questioning me tho

    Subjects should not be motivated to "manage" the

    lationship between persona outcom es and intentio

    because their responses are independent of social e

    pectations. In the present context, there appear to

    no relevant norms governing beliefs and evaluations

    the freedom of movement offered by the new hea

    phones. No differences should exist in the relationsh

    between personal outcom es and intentions across qu

    tioning methods.

    The estimated effect of normative outcomes on

    tentions, however, should be attenuated by social d

    sirability bias when direct qu estioning is used. Subje

    may consciously or unconsciously engage in ima

    managem ent by distorting the association between th

    normative ou tcome and intention scores. For examp

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    7/14

    FIGURE 1

    PERSONAL AND NORMATIVE OUTCOME BY OUESTIONING METHOD AND ANONYM ITY

    Anonymity

    150-

    140-

    130-

     ersonai

    Outcomes n o -

    100-

    9 0 -

    BO-

    7 0 -

    6 0 -

     

    0

    Ye s

    Indirect

    Direct

    I

    No

    Anonymity

    ects may lower normative outcom e scores when in-

    H3a : T he association between persona outcom es

    and intentions is the same with direct and

    indirect questioning because the variance

    common to these variables is not subject to

    social desirability bias.

    H b Indirect questioning removes social desir-

    ability variance from normative outcomes,

    resulting in a stronger association between

    normative outcomes and intentions under

    this form of questioning.

    A two-group experiment was conduc ted in which the

      {u =

      170)

      n =

      182) groups. This sample was drawn

    easurement

    Measures of persona and normative ou tcomes were

    early were measured with one indicator composed of

    the summation of four self-report items. The four-item

    directly worded scale included items such as "I will

    probably purchase one ofthe new products soon after

    they are on the market," measured on a seven-point

    "strongly agree" to "strongly disagree" scale. The in-

    direct scale asked subjects to respond in terms of the

    typical stud ent. The coefficient alphas for this scale were

    .93 (direct) and .81 (indirect). The means were 13.5

    (direct) and 14.9 (indirect).

    Results

    Before testing the formal hypotheses, an analysis of

    personal and normative outcome mean scores reveals

    the same pattern of differences between groups found

    in study

      1.

     Specifically, the means for personal outcomes

    are not significantly different across the direct and in-

    direct questioning groups (A dircci = 96.7 , .findireci

     =

     101.2,

    t =

      1.5. NS), while the mean for normative outcomes

    is significantly higher in the indirect group

      Xa.tcci

    =

      39.4, X.̂ direa ^ 63.2 , / - 8.72. /J < .00 1, one-ta iled

    test).

      As in study I. these results suggest a systematic

    social desirability effect on normative outcome mean

    scores with direct questioning.

    Tests of differences in the estimated effects of personal

    and normative outcomes on intentions in study 2 were

    undertaken in two ways. First, given differences in the

    ratio of the standard deviations for the predictor and

    criterion variables across questioning groups, the ap-

    propriate test of differences in the relationsh ip between

    outcomes and intentions is a comparison of product-

    moment correlations across subgroups (Arnold 1982).

    On the basis of Fisher's Z-transform ation, Hypothesis

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    8/14

    310

    JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEAR

    3a is supported with no significant difference in the cor-

    relation between personal outcomes and intentions

    across groups

      rawca =

     -432, r.ndirect = -493, z = .77, NS).

    Hypothesis 3b is supported with a significantly higher

    correlation between norm ative outcomes and intentions

    in the indirect group (rdirea

      =

      -269. rindireci ^ -442, z

    = 1.81, ;? < .05. one-tailed test). Second, an ordinary

    least squares path analysis model was run for each

    questioning-method group in which intention to early

    adopt was regressed on personal and normative out-

    comes. This analysis was performed to compa re the pa-

    rameter estimates that might result from path-analytic

    studies und er different questioning me thods (see Table

    3).

      Congruent with the study's hypotheses, the stan-

    dardized beta weight for normative outcomes on inten-

    tions is higher in the indirect (/? = .332, / = 5.19,  p

    < .001) compared to the direct (^ = .205,  t  - 2.94,  p

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    9/14

    OCIAL DESIRABILITY BIAS

    311

     self

    and purchases

    nfluenced only by  othe rs ^  were selected for inclusion

    usic, hairstyle, and cologne/perfume.

    A convenience sample of 75 male and female un-

    ents were randomly assigned to one of

    nly in the source of influence identified in the ques-

    ions.

     Subjects in groups 1, 2, and 3 answered questions

    hat identified the source of influence as others, friends,

    nd their best friend, respectively. Subjects \yilhin each

    luations of the im portance of social

    pproval for themselves, their best friend, and the typ-

    ical student in the purchase of each of the five products

    idenlitied in the pretest. For example, the following

    hree question stems were used in group 1:

      It's very important

      TO ME

      that OTHERS approve

    f . . .

    It's very important  TO MY BEST FRIEND   that

    THERS approve of . . .

    It's very important  TO THE TYPICAL STUDENT

    hat OTHERS approve of . . .

    Questions were randomized within the groups to avoid

    order bias.

    Measurement

    Items for the five expressive products were summed

    to form a single indicator of the importance of social

    influence for expressive products to simplify the pre-

    sentation of the results and provide more reliable mea-

    sures. *

      Scale characteristics are presented in Table 4.

    As indicated, the internal consistency of the scales is

    acceptable given that all alphas exceed .70 with only

    one exception (.67).

    The tendency to respond in a socially desirable man-

    ner was measured with Reynolds's (1982) short form

    of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. The

    scale comprises 13 culturally approved behaviors (five

    worded positively and eight worded negatively) that

    have a low probability of occurrence. The scale is ad-

    ^This approach dratnatically reduces the number of significance

    tests but does not materially affect the overall results. Spiecifically,

    using summated scales reduced the number of Ntests from 30 to six

    and the number of associational tests from 45 to nine. Nevertheless,

    the mean and associational results using summated scales are con-

    sistent with the individual results. For the mean contrasts, 29 of 30

    individual tests were consistent with the results reported for the sum -

    mated scales. Of the nine associational tests, five results held for all

    five products, two for four of five products, and two for three of five

    products. Individual test results are available from the author.

    TABLE 4

    SCALE CHARACTERISTICS: STUDY 3

    Source of influence

    and measure

    Others

     {n

    25):

    SELF

    BEST

    TS

    Friends n = 26):

    SELF

    BEST

    TS

    Best friend n = 24):

    SELF

    BEST

    TS

    Overall n  75):

    S D R

    No.

    of

    items

    5

    5

    5

    5

    5

    S

    5

    5

    5

    13

    Actual

    scale

    range

    0-24

    2-25

    9-30

    1-22

    0-26

    6-30

    0-20

    0-20

    0-27

    0-11

    Scale

    mean

    13.0

    12.8

    21.7

    9.6

    10.7

    17.8

    10.8

    11.3

    17.5

    4.49

    SD

    6. 4

    6.1

    5.6

    5.9

    7.1

    6.9

    6 .5

    6.4

    6 .3

    2.78

    Alpha

    .67

    .72

    ,85

    .78

    .82

    .73

    .7 8

    .82

    .7 3

    .69

    NOTE.—Abbreviations   are as follows: SELF, predictions about  self;  BEST,

    predictions about biest

      friend: TS,

     predictions about typical student

    SDR, the

    tendency to  respond in a  soctally desirat>le manner as  measursd by the Marlowe-

    Crowne short  form.

    ministered in a true or false format, and a high score

    on the scale indicates the subject's tendency to present

    himself or herself in a socially favorable manner. Scale

    characteristics are consistent with prior studies (e.g.,

    Reyn olds 1982; see Table 4).

    Results

    An examination of mean scores for the importance

    of social approval in the purchase of expressive p roducts

    reveals the same pattern of results as studies I and 2.

    Specifically, subjects consistently indicated that the

    typical student is more m otivated by social approval in

    consumption behavior than they are themselves.

    Through dependent /-tests, this difference was found

    when the source of influence was others (A's^f = 13.0,

      t̂ypical studeni  = 21.7, / = 5.11, /? < .001); friends {A'^cif

    = 9.8, X,,pie,u,,den. =

      17._8,

      t =   5.47,  p < Mi):  an d

    best friend   (X^^K  =  10.8. -V,ypjeaismdcm = 17.5, / - 5.36 ,

    p< .001

    ). The results for th e three groups are also clear

    when the projective target was the subject's best friend.

    Predictions for the best friend were not significantly

    different from the means for the self when the source

    of influence was others  X^^ij =  13.0, Xbejifnend = 12 .8,

    t = - .26 , p >  . 10); friends  ,̂eif = 9-8, X ^ ,,

     u,.r.A_=

      10.7

    t = .86, p > .10); and best  (ncnd {X^^it = 10.8, Xbesi friend

    =

      11.3,/

      = .61,/?> .10).

    The effects of social desirability bias on self-reports

    and pre dictions abou t o thers were evaluated by regress-

    ing the direct and indirect scales on the tendency to

    respond in a socially desirable manner. All hypotheses

    were directional, and thus all relational tests are one-

    tailed. As summarized in Table 5, response tendency

    was significantly associated with direct questions on so-

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    10/14

    312

    JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEAR

    TABLE 5

    EFFECTS OF SOCIAL DESIRABILITY BIAS ON

    PREDICTIONS ABOUT SELF, BEST FRIEND,

    AND TYPICAL OTHER: STUDY 3

    Source of influence and dependent variable

    Independent

    variable

    (SDR)

    Others  {n = 25):

    SELF

    BEST

    TS

    Friends  {n = 26):

    SELF

    BEST

    TS

    Best friend

      n

     = 24):

    SELF

    BEST

    TS

    .37-

    .49 '

    .10

    .32

    .34

    .16

    .47

    .10

    .06

    NOTE.—Abbreviations

      are as follows: SELF,

      predictions about self;

      BEST,

    predictions about best

      friend; TS,

     predictions about typical student;

      SDR, the

    tendency

     to

     respond

     in a

     soaally desiraWe manner

     as

     measured

     by th Marlowe-

    Growne short  form.

      p < .05  one-tailed test).

     p <

      .01

      one-tailed

     test).

    cial approval tnotives when the source of influence was

    others(/3 = -. 3 7 , / =  - 1 . 9 1 , p < .05); friends  /3 - - . 32 ,

    t =

     -\

    .68,

    p

      .10); friends ()3 = - .1 6 , f

    = - . 80 , p >  .10); and best friend  {0 = --06 , / = - .27 ,

    p>  .10). The results are less straightforward when the

    predictive target was the subject's best friend, with a

    significant relationship when the source of influence was

    others (^  =  - .49 , / = -2 .74 ,  p   .10).

    Overall, the results support the hypotheses. For Hy-

    pothesis 4a .  the pattern of effects for direct and typical

    student indirect questions is the same as was found in

    the first wo studies for a functional innovation. Subjects

    in all three groups indicated that social approval for

    expressive prod ucts was more im portan t for the typical

    student than for themselves. Also, the tendency to re-

    spond in a socially desirable manner was significant for

    ail direct questions and nonsignificant for all indirect

    questions in which the typical student was the projective

    target. Hypothesis  4b  is supported because the mean

    and structural results supporting Hypothesis  4a  were

    consistent across all three sources of influence, namely,

    others, friends, and best friend. Hypothesis 5a is sup-

    ported with a significantly higher mean importan ce rat-

    ing for the out-group rating compared to the self-rating.

    Finally, Hypothesis 5b is supported given no significant

    difference in the mean importance of social appro

    between self and best friend reports. The combinat

    of the results of Hypothesis  a and  b suggests that

    jects engaged in classical projection when making p

    dictions about a typical other.

    Discussion

    Study 3 reveals a pattern of results similar to t

    found in studies I and 2 for mean scores on the i

    portance of social approval as a consumption moti

    Individuals consistently evaluated themselves as l

    motivated by social approval than typical others. C

    sidering all three studies, the mean results are com

    rable across influence sources (i.e., others, friends, a

    best friend), between- and within-subject designs, a

    product categories. The results suggest predictable me

    differences between direct and indirect questions

    socially sensitive variables and no differences for socia

    neutral variables.

    The associationai tests in study 3 support the abo

    interpretation. First, the tendency to respond in a

    cially desirable m ann er was significantly associated w

    direct measures of the influence of others, friends, a

    best friend. In each case the inclination to bias respon

    toward social norms resulted in lower self-reports

    social approval as a purchase motive. Second, respon

    tendency was not significantly associated with pred

    tions about the importance of social approval for

    typical student, regardless of the source of influence

    Analysis of the mean scores and social desirabil

    effects on indirect questions in which the predictive t

    get was the subject's best friend suggests that the

    sponses w ere affected by social desirab ility bias in

    same direction and degree as direct questions. First,

    differences were found in the mean scores between  s

    and best friend predictions across the three influen

    sources. Second, the tendency to respond in a socia

    desirable manner was found to have a significant n

    ative effect on best friend predictions when the sou

    of influence was others and friends. T hese findings su

    port the proposition that subjects engaged in classi

    projection by denying an undesirable trait in themsel

    and their best friend but attributing it to a socially d

    tant other.

    The one inconsistency in study 3 results

     is

     the find

    of no significant effect of response tendency on b

    friend predictions when the source of influence was

    best friend's best friend. This result may have occurr

    because of the unique nature of the prediction requ ir

    for this question. To simplify the indirect wording

    was assumed that the best friend's best friend was

    subject. On this basis, the question stem was It's ve

    important to  MY BEST FRIEND  that / appro

    of . . . The anoma lous finding may have occurr

    because the social norms governing the subject's inf

    ence over his or her best friend are different than t

    norms affecting other influence sources. Specifically

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    11/14

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    12/14

    31 4

    JOURN L OF CONSUMER RESE R

    sociations between variables. Fourth, the studies pro-

    vide insights into the generality of effects across product

    categories, influence sources, and experimental designs.

    Overall, the research contributes to our understanding

    of the validity of indirect questioning as a technique

    for reducing social desirability bias and insights into

    how consumers make predictions about others.

    [Received  August 1992. Revised February 1993 ]

    REFERENCES

    Alpert. Mark 1. (1971). Identification of Dete rmin ant At-

    tributes: A Comparison of Methods,

    Journal of

     Mar

    keting Re.search.  8 (May).

      184-191.

    Anderson. James

     C

    (1978), The V alidity of Haire's Shopping

    List Projective Technique.

    Journal of Marketing Re-

    search.

     15 (November). 64 4-64 9.

    Arnold. Hugh J. (1982), Mo derator Variables:  Clarification

    of Conceptual. Analytic, and Psychometric Issues,

    Or-

    ganizational Behavior and Human Performance.  29

    (April). 143-174.

    Bagozzi. Richard P. and Youjae Yi (1991), M ultitra it-M ul-

    timethod Matrices In Consumer Research. Journal of

    Consumer Re.search.

      17 (March). 426 -439 .

    Bearden. William O, and M ichael J. Etzel (1982). Reference

    Group Influence on Product and Brand Purchase Deci-

    sions. Journal of Consumer Research. 9

     (September).

    183-194.

    Bentler. P. M. and Douglas G. Bonett (1980), Significance

    Tests and Goodness of Fit in the Analysis of Covariance

    Structures,

    Psychological Bulletin.

      88 (November), 588 -

    606.

    Brinberg. David and Linda Plimpton (1986). Self-Mon itor-

    ing and Product Conspicuousness on Reference Group

    Influence. in

     Advances in Consutner Reseatch.

     Vol. 13,

    ed. Richard J. Lutz. Provo. UT: Association for Con-

    sumer Research. 297-300.

    Calder, Bobby J. and Robert E. Burn krant (1977 ). Inte r-

    personal Influence on Consumer Behavior: An Attribu-

    tion Theory Approach.

    Journal of Consumer Research.

    4 (June). 29-38.

    Camp bell, Donald T. (1950). Th e Indirect Assessment of

    Social Attitudes,

    Psychological

      Bulletin. 47 (January),

    15-38.

    Conger, Anthony J. (1974), A Revised Definition for Sup-

    pressor Variables: A Guid e to their Identification and

    Interpretation. Educational and Psychological Mea-

    suretnent.

     34 (Spring). 35-46 .

    Cote.

     Joseph A. and M. Ronald Buckley (1988). Me asure-

    ment Error and Theory Testing in Consumer Research:

    An Illustration of the Importance of Construct Valida-

    tion.

    Journat of Consumer Research.

     14 (March). 57 9-

    582.

    Davis, Harry L., Stephen J. Hoch. and E. K. Easton Ragsdale

    (1986),

      An Anchoring and Adjustment Model of

    Spousal Predictions. Journal of Consumer Research. 13

    (June).

     25-37.

    Fishbein, Martin and Icek Ajzen (1975),  Belief Attitude, In-

    tention, and

     Behavior.

      Reading. MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Fornell. Claes and David F. Larcker (1981), Evalu ating

    Structural Equ ation Models with Unobservable Variables

    and Measurement Error. Journal of Marketitig R

    search. 18 (February). 39-50.

    Haire, Mason (1950). Projective Techn iques in Market

    Research.

    Journal of M arketing.

     14 (April). 649-65

    Haviena . William J. and Mo rris B. Holbrook (1986). T

    Varieties of Consumption Experience: Comp aring T

    Typologies of Emotion in Consumer Behavior.

    Jour

    of Consumer Research. 13 (December). 394-404.

    Holmes. David S, (1968), Dimen sions of Projection.

    P

    chological Bulletin.

     69 (April), 248-268.

    Kassarjian, Harold H. (1974), Projective Meth od s.

    Ilatidhook of Marketing Research,

     ed. Rob ert Ferb

    New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 3-85 to 3-100.

    Kelman. Herbert C. (1961). Process of Opinion C hang

    Public Opinion Qttarterly.

      25 (Spring). 57-78.

    Levy. Sidney J. (1981). Interp reting Consu mer M ytholo

    A Structural Approach to Consumer Behavior, Jour

    of M arketing.

     45 (Summer).

      49-61.

    Maccoby, Eleanor E. and Nathan Maccoby (1954), Th e

    terview:  Tool of Social Science. in Handhook of So

    Psychology. Vol. I, ed. Gardiner Lindzey. Cambrid

    MA: Addison-Wesley. 4 49-4 87.

    Mensch. Barbara S. and Denisc B. Kandel (1988), Un der

    porting of Substance Use in a National Longitudi

    Youth Cohort.

    Public Opinion Q uarterly.

     52 (Sprin

    100-124.

    Min iard, Paul W. and Joel B. Cohen (198 3). Mo deling P

    sonal and Normative Influences on Behavior.

    Jour

    of Consumer Research.

     10 (September), 169-180.

    Park. C. Whan and V. Parker Lessig (1977), Stu den ts a

    Housewives: Differences in Susceptibility to Referen

    Group Influence.

    Journal of Consumer Re.warch

    (September). 102-110.

    Paulhus. Delroy L. (1984), Two-C ompo nent Models of S

    cially Desirable Respon ding.

    Journal of Personality 

    Social

      Psychology.

      46 (March). 598-609.

    Peltier, B. David and James A. Walsh (1990 ). An Inve

    gation of Response Bias in the Chapman Scales.

    Ed

    cational and Psychological Measurement.  50 (Wint

    803-815.

    Peterson. Robert A. and Roger A. Kerin (198 1). Th e Qua

    of Self-Report Data: Review and Synth esis, in

     Rev

    of M arketing,  ed. Ben M. Enis and Kenneth J. Roeri

    Chicago: American Marketing Association. 5-20.

    Reyn olds. William M. (1982). Dev elopm ent of Reliable a

    Valid Short Forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social D

    sirability

      Sca\c. Journal of Clinical Psychology.

     38 (

    uary),  l ' l9-125.

    Robertson. Dan H. and R obert W. Joselyn (1974), Project

    Techniques in Research.

    Journal of Advertising R

    .search.

      14 (Octob er).

      27-31.

    Robinette, Randy L. (1991). Th e Relationship between

    Marlowe-Crowne Form C and the Validity Scales of

    MMPl,

    Journal of Clinical Psychology.

     47 (May).

    399.

    Rokeaeh. Milton (1979),

     Understanding Human Values.

     

    York: Free Press.

    and Sandra J. Ball-Rokeach (1989), Stability a

    Change in American Value Priorities,

    American P

    chologist.  44 (May). 775-784.

    Rook. Dennis (1985). The Ritual Dimension of Consum

    Behavior, Joutnal of Consumer Research. 12 (Dec

    ber),

     251-264.

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    13/14

    SOCIAL DESIRABILITY BIAS 315

    Sherwood. Gregory G. ( 98 I). "Self-serving Biases in Person and Judg em ent Processes in a Persona lity Predictio n

    Perception: A Reexam ination of Projection as a Mech- i Ta sk," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.  52

    anism of Defense."  Psychological  Bulletin,  90 (Novem- (April), 700-7 09.

    her). 445-4 59. Westfall. Ralph L.. Harper W. Boyd. Jr.. and Donald T.

    imon. Julian and Rita Simon (1975). "Th e Effect of Money Campbell (1957), "T he Use of Structured Techniques in

    Incentives on Family Size: A Hypothetical-Question Motivation Rese arch,"  Journal of Marketing,  22 (Oc-

    Study." Public pinion  Quarterly.  38 (Winter). 585-595 . tober). 134-139.

    teele. Howard (1964), "On the Validity of Indirect Que s- Zerb e. Wilfred J, and Delroy L, Pau lhus (1987). "Socially

    uons.,' Journal of Mark eting Re.search, 2S (Augusi), 46-  Desirable Responding in Organizational Behavior: A

    49 .  Reconception."  Academy of Management Review,  12

    Ietlock, Phillip E, and Jae il Kim (1987), "Acco untability (April), 250-2 64.

  • 8/9/2019 Fisher 1993 Jcr

    14/14