functional morphology and systematics: studying …

26
Annu. Rev. Ecol, Syst, I99<). 27.317-40 Copyright © 1990 by Annual Reviews tnv. All rights reserved FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying Functional Patterns in an Historical Context George V. Lauder Departtnent of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Irvine, California 92717 KEY WORDS: structure, funetion. evolution, systematics, homology INTRODUCTION The study of function is a neglected area of systematic and histotica! biology. Over the last 20 years, systematic biology has expanded to iticlude in its purview the study of many different kinds of patterns, and structural features of all kinds have been the subject of phylogenetic analyses. In addition to macroscopic structural features that have been the traditional source of characters reflecting patterns of ancestry and descent, systematists have in- creasingly relied on DNA hase and amino acid sequences, electrophoretic banding patterns, and ontogenetie sequences of character transformation to sort out genealogical patterns (e.g. 21, 51, 63, 72, 111). Morphological features of organisms have been used by systematists as the hasis for biogeographic (120, 157, 158), morphometric (17, 134), ontogenetie (1. 54, 55, 72), and quantitative genetic analyses (130) as well as for studying speciation patterns, and ecological and coevolutionary interactions (18-20, 27, 104), But data on organismal function have been both the least used and the least understood class of infomiation about organisms in systematic biology. There are three main reasons why the form-function relationship, long a central dichotomy in biology (124), has been so heavily weighted toward the 317 0066-4162/90/0410-0317$02.00

Upload: others

Post on 25-Feb-2022

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

Annu. Rev. Ecol, Syst, I99<). 27.317-40Copyright © 1990 by Annual Reviews tnv. All rights reserved

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY ANDSYSTEMATICS: Studying FunctionalPatterns in an Historical Context

George V. Lauder

Departtnent of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Irvine,California 92717

KEY WORDS: structure, funetion. evolution, systematics, homology

INTRODUCTION

The study of function is a neglected area of systematic and histotica! biology.Over the last 20 years, systematic biology has expanded to iticlude in itspurview the study of many different kinds of patterns, and structural featuresof all kinds have been the subject of phylogenetic analyses. In addition tomacroscopic structural features that have been the traditional source ofcharacters reflecting patterns of ancestry and descent, systematists have in-creasingly relied on DNA hase and amino acid sequences, electrophoreticbanding patterns, and ontogenetie sequences of character transformation tosort out genealogical patterns (e.g. 21, 51, 63, 72, 111). Morphologicalfeatures of organisms have been used by systematists as the hasis forbiogeographic (120, 157, 158), morphometric (17, 134), ontogenetie (1. 54,55, 72), and quantitative genetic analyses (130) as well as for studyingspeciation patterns, and ecological and coevolutionary interactions (18-20,27, 104), But data on organismal function have been both the least used andthe least understood class of infomiation about organisms in systematicbiology.

There are three main reasons why the form-function relationship, long acentral dichotomy in biology (124), has been so heavily weighted toward the

3170066-4162/90/0410-0317$02.00

Page 2: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

318 LAUDER

study of form in systematic and evolutionary biology. First, comparativephylogenetic analysis arose historically from a morphological tradition in thenineteenth century, while functional analysis became part of the researchtradition in physiology. Since the divergence of the morphological and physi-ological research programs near the lum of the century (5, 6. 25), littleinteraction has occurred between them, and functional analysis has played arelatively small role in phyiogenetic research. Second, functional analysis isoften extremely time consuming, and obtaining even limited functional datafor many species witbin a clade may take years. Thus, only rarely arefunctional data available for broad comparative investigations. Thirdly, theinference of function from morphology and the heuristic use of functionalconcepts have often been substituted for the direct experimental measurementof functional attributes in living organisms. While many general discussionshave recognized the importance of data on organismal function (13-15, 41,4 3 ^ 5 , 62, 74. 83, 97. 137. 149, 151), a large number of papers havepresented "tunctional analyses" based primarily on the study of structure (e.g.11, 12, 33, 34, 59, 98, 99, 129, 136, 137). Relatively few papers haveactually quitntified function in living animals and provided direct examples inwhich tneasured functional attributes of organisms are useful lor understand-ing problems in historical biology. Thus, despite many statements in theliterature supporting the importance of functional analysis, only a smallnumber of papers use both experimentally determined function andphylogenetic analysis. Many discussions of the utility of functional analysispresent purely morphological data and then infer rather than measure func-tion.

The central theme of this article is that the direct experimental measurementof organismal function in living animals provides insights, not obtainable byother means, into (a) the uses of structural characters, tb) how and whycharacters are distributed tbe way they are on a cladogram, fc) interactionsand correlations among characters, (d) the nature of organismal diversity, and(ei historical patterns to organismal design.

WHAT ARE FUNCTIONAL DATA?

The study of function is the study of bow structures are used, and functionaldata are those in which the use of structural features has been directlymeasured. Functions are the actions of phenotypic components (34, 47, 48,83, 84). For example, the sequence of amino acids in an enzyme constitutesstructural (morphological) data on that enzyme. The maximum rate of cataly-sis (Vn,a )̂ or the affinity of an enzyme for its substrate (K,,,) constitutesfunctional data on the enzyme. Similarly, structural data on a muscle mightinclude the morphology of the sarcoplasmic reticulum, the amino acid se-quence of the myosin molecules, changes in the electrophoretic banding

Page 3: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 319

pattem of muscle myosins during ontogeny, the amount of pinnation in themuscle fibers, the cross-sectional area of the muscle, and the length of themuscle. Functional (or physiological) data on muscle might include the timeto peak tension, maximum tetanic tension, myosin ATPase reaction rates,work done or oxygen consumption in a single contraction, patterns of electri-cal activity during animal movement, or how any muscular rate processchanges with temperature (Om). I do not include measurements of musclessuch as lever arms or other morphometric features as functional data, eventhough such information may be interpreted in a functional context and maybe useful in inferring functional characteristics.

Functional characteristics may also be measured at the whole animal level.As measurements are made that integrate increasing numbers of lower levelfunctions, functional data become difficult to distinguish from behavioraldata. Examples of functional attributes at tbe wbole organism level includeresting metabolic rate, maximal running speed, footfall patterns, and pre-ferred body temperature. Functional data may overlap measures of animalperformance. Performance measures the ability of an animal to execute abebavior or tbe effectiveness of an animal at accomplishing a particular task(7, 39, 83, 141), Typical performance measures include maximal swimmingspeed, the percent of successful feeding strikes, maximal jumping distance, orthe largest size of prey that can be crushed. In addition, functional data mayform part of a behavioral analysis. For example, behavioral analyses of frogor bird vocalizations often include the study of specific functional attributes ofthe call or song.

Use of the term "function" is subject to two confusions in the literature.First, ethologists commonly use the tenn function as a synonym of "selectivevalue" (10, 23, 80). As discussed elsewhere (80) the use of "function" in thisway is associated with many difficulties, not the least of which is the necessityof accurately understanding selective forces on a structure before one canspeak of its function.

Second, function is often confused with the role that a structure playsduring the life history of an organism. Bock & von Wahlert (16) emphasized auseful distinction between the terms "function" and "biological role." Func-tions are tbe uses of structures, while the biological rote of a structure refiectstbe task of a structure during a behavior such as mating or escape frompredation. The idea of a biological role is in many ways similar to the use ofthe term function by ethologists.

FUNCTION AND HISTORICAL BIOLOGY

There are five key areas in which the interplay between functional andhistorical analysis is likely to be particularly fruitful. These are treatedseriatim below, to focus attention on the possible mutual influences of

Page 4: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

320 LAUDER

functional and historical analysis. Mayr (105, 106) bas emphasized thedistinction between functional biology, concerned primarily with understand-ing how organisms work and proximate causation, and evolutionary biology.committed particularly to studying historical pathways and ultimate causa-tion. A major theme to emerge from a consideration of five areas of interplaydiscussed below is that clear areas of overlap exist between these twoapproaches that may provide new insights into organismal diversity and themechanisms that have produced it. Research into the evolution of functionincreasingly exemplifies aspects of both "types" of biology.

The Evolution of Fimction

Functional attributes are an important class of organismal characters. How arestructures used by organisms, and how has tbe use of structures changed inevolution? These questions may be addressed by an analysis of both form andfunction in an historical context. Analysis of structural features alone willprovide an incomplete picture of the nature of organismal design. Our un-derstanding of the evolution of gastrointestinal hormones in vertebrates (140).for example, is greatly enhanced if we can, in addition to describingphylogenetic patterns to amino acid sequences, describe how changes in thestructure of the active site affect function and reaction rates in each clade. Arecertain changes in amino acid sequence uncorrelated with changes in func-tion? Are particular functions retained as primitive characters despite changesin structure? Do certain structural changes at the active site permit newfunctions while retaining the primitive function?

Figure I illustrates a simple hypothetical example of a functional analysisconducted on seven species (A to G) in conjunction with a phylogeneticanalysis. The branching pattern of the cladogram (Figure IB) has beendetermined from a previous analysis, and structural features of a biomeehani-cal system of muscles and bones are diagrammed in Figure 1 A. Each speciesis studied to determine both the topology of the muscles and bones and theassociated electrical activity patterns (motor patterns) of the muscles. Theprocedure of mapping both structural and functional attributes of the terminalspecies onto a cladogram allows one to make several observations about tbeevolution of muscle function. First, the control of bone d (Figure I) evolvedby a two-stage process: first ligament 6 arose, followed by ligament 7.Pathway / arose first, and species in clades A to E have the capability ofmoving bone d by pathway /. Subsequently, pathway (7 arose by the additionof ligament 7: only clade A possesses pathway ii. Note that the activitypatterns of muscle 5 have diverged in two clades (D - E; A - B) while clade Cretains the primitive functional pattern for muscle 5. The origin of muscle 4 iscongruent with its functional pattem (muscle activity pattem D), while activ-ity patterns associated with muscle 3 show convergent evolution between

Page 5: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 321

clade A and clade G - F. Muscle 3 thus exhibits an incongruence between tbeevolution of structural and functional characteristics.

Three main benefits are to be gained from such an analysis. First, it enablesone to define precisely the historical sequence of both structural and function-al change that permits an understanding of how a particular biomeehanicalsystem was constructed. In any attempt to understand a mechanical systemand the causal factors involved in its construction, it is useful to know theorder in which a system was assembled, to help determine the in-terrelationships among the parts (45). In machines constructed by humans, theorder of assembly may often be reasonably inferred by the way in which theparts fit together. But for biological systems the interrelationship amongstructural components alone rarely provides this information. Second, histor-ical analysis of function provides the basis for an analysis of coevolutionary

F E D CMotor patttm F lor maOt 3

mat 7

ltet» piHwn C for imitcto 5

Mol« paltem E (or mutda 3

MalH patltrr D lor muscb 4

Misdat

'or musda S

Function^ character

Struciiral character

Figure I Schematic diagram of a complex biomeehanical system (A) and the historical se-quence in which it was constructed (B). Circles indicate bones, labelled with lower-case letters,while ligaments are indicated by dashed lines and muscles by black lines. There are twomechanical pathways for moving bone d: i and ii. Pathway i involves muscle 4 and ligament 6.Pathway ii involves muscle 3 and ligament 7. Note that based on ihe phylogenetic analysis of (B)in which Ihe characteristics of species A to G are mapped onto a cladogram derived from olhercharacters, ihc muscle activity pattem (motor pattem) for muscle 3 is convergent between clade Aand clade G. Note also that functional characters (open bars) and structural characters (solid bars)may exhibit incongruent distributions: e.g.. muscle 5 arises with one motor pattem (A) and onlythe motor pattem is transformed (into motor pattem B and C) during subsequent cladogenesis.

Page 6: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

322 LAUDER

patterns between structural and functional data sets: do historical changes tendto occur congruently in muscle morphology and function, for example? Third,this type of analysis allows an assessment of the conservatism (or lackthereof) in functional data as compared to structural information, and a test ofthe oft-stated conclusion that functional data tend to be evolutionariiy plastic.These last two points are considered in more detail below in sections 2 and 4.

While considerable progress has been made in attempting analyses such asthose presented in Figure I (22, 46, 67. 74, 80. 81. 92. 94-96. 107, 126, 148,150), cotnplete data sets of both structural and functional characters are notyet available for many groups (84). In part, this is due to the considerabledifficulty in obtaining functional data from a variety of species. Determiningmechanical properties of tissues, muscle activity patterns, rates of oxygenconsumption, bone movements or deformations, or fluid pressures from threeto five individuals in each of a dozen or so species is a formidable task. Manyspecies important in phylogenetic analysis are either rare or found in inacces-sible habitats, or they do not respond well to laboratory conditions, furthercomplicating the gathering of functional data. Despite such difficulties, thedevelopment of complete case studies of structural and functional evolutionwould be a significant step forward in attempts to understand the evolution oforganismal design.

One additional area where functional and phylogenetic analyses may in-teract in studies of the evolution of function lies in the framing of specificfunctional hypotheses. Phylogenetic analyses of morphology may suggestspecific functional questions (81, 94. 96. 98, 99, 126) which may then beexplored. One example was provided by an analysis of locomotion in ray-finned fishes (81). An analysis on a cladogram of the sequence of characterchange of muscles and bones in the tail of ray-finned fishes indicated thatcertain muscles arose prior to the origin of an externally symmetrical tail. Thissuggested that the action of these muscles might influence the way the tail wasused and in fact might be able to change the function of the tail between twoaltemative states. A functional analysis (measuring bone strain and muscleelectrical activity) confirmed this prediction and indicated that previous viewson the evolution of the tail in ray-finned fishes may be in need of revision.

Function and Homology

In addition to mapping functional and structural characters onto a phylogenyto conduct an historical analysis of a biomeehanical system, functionalcharacters may be used as elements of a phylogenetic analysis and maycontribute to tbe total evidence (71) available for phylogenetic reconstruction.As such, functional characters are not different from any other attribute thatmight contribute to our understanding of genealogy; functions as well as

Page 7: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 323

Structures may be synapomorphies. For example, two species of sunfishes{Lepomis gibbosus. the pumpkinseed, and Lepomis microlophus, the redear)share a synapomorpbic pattern of muscle activity (76, 78) not found primi-tively in the sunfish clade. Although it has previously been suggested thatfunctional characters are not suitable for systematic analysis (26, 137), recentwork has shown that functional characters (such as bone movements ormuscle electrical activity patterns) may be used as characters (77, 80, 128,144) to successfully delineate monophyletic clades.

If a particular functional character (such as: "double burst pattem ofelectrical activity in the rectus cervicis muscle") is treated like any othercharacter, then clearly functional characters may be homologous or con-vergent (just like morphological features) and may show complex patterns ofphylogenetic distribution independent of their underlying structural basis. Anexample of muscle activity pattems treated like characters is provided by thesunfishes (Centrarchidae) discussed by Lauder (80). A further implication oftreating functional characters just like structural features is that determiningthe homology or convergence of functional attributes is accomplished byusing the same procedures applied to structural characters. Both convergentfunctions and stmctures are recognized by their incongruent distribution on acladogram (42, 80, 109, 110), and the status of particular characters (whetherfunctional or structural) as homologous or convergent is dependent on thetopology of the cladogram. Character status may change as the cladogramchanges. Figure 2 illustrates this point. In Figure 2A structure 1 and function1 are homologous and are synapomorphies for clade A - B , based on thetopology of the cladogram given. If, however, a subsequent phylogeneticanalysis shows that the cladogram topology of Figure 2B is much bettersupported by available data (so that now clades A and B are unrelated to eachother), then both structure one and function one must be interpreted asconvergent between these two clades.

When functional attributes are treated like stmctural features, function isnot useful as an a priori guide for detemiining homology of stmctures:Function does not take primacy in homology decisions. However, a numberof authors have argued that functional analysis does provide an a priori guideto detemiining the homology of associated stmctural features (8, 13, 59, 60,65, 132, 137). Under such a view, it is "impossible for nonhomologousstmctures to have homologous functions" (8: p. 60), and "Functions . . .should not be spoken of as homologous" (60: p. 323). Tyler (137: pp. 334,344) has argued that "A complete determination of the probability of homolo-gy, however, is not possible on morphological grounds alone; we need afunctional analysis to gauge the probability of convergence," and that"Homology applies most appropriately to the stmctural features, not theirfunctions." In this view the role of functional analysis is primary, and.

Page 8: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

324

:. STRUCTURE 2m FUNCTION 2D STRUCTURE 1• FUNCTION 1

Figure 2 Inlcrpretalion of the homolopy of buth siruclurts and functions depends on Ihe[opology of the cludograni of the species studied. Panel .\\ Bolh sirueturc I and function 1 arehomologous in eludes A and B. Panel B: Given an new ctadogram topology with the same patlemof characlerdislrihulion. func'lion I and structure I arc considered to be convcrgeni between cladeA and cladc B.

independent of the distribution of characters in other taxa. functional analysisis able to assess homology or convergence of an individual character.

The idea of a priori functional analysis as a guide to homology in systemat-ics suffers from two primary difficulties. First, the "functional" analysesconducted to determine structural homology usually only involve morphologi-cal studies. Thus, Tyler's (137) method of "functional hierarchies" involvesthe heuristic use of functional ideas and hypothesized functions but not thedirect measurement of function or the experimental manipulation of stmc-tures. Similarly, the methods of Bock (13, 14),Gutmann(59). and Dullemeij-er (e.g.. 34: p. 227) are based almost entirely on a morphological analysis.Secondly, structural characters detennined in some way to be homologous bya functional analysis are not subject to refutation by the discovery of anotherbetter-corroborated cladogram. For example, if detailed a priori functional

Page 9: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 325

analysis is applied to two stmctural characters foUowing the procedures ofTyler (137). Gutmann (59), or Dullemeijer (34), and these two stmctures aredetermined to be homologous (e.g. structure one in Figure 2A), what wouldbe concluded if an analysis of DNA base sequences showed that cladogram B(Figure 2B) were in fact correct with a high degree of statistical probability?Would these authors conclude that stmcture one in clades A and B (Figure2B) is still homologous?

The Analysis of Correlated CharactersFunctional analyses help to address the question of how and why charactersare correlated with one another in phylogeny. Pattems of character distribu-tion on ciadograms have been the subject of increasing analysis in recent years(e.g. 31, 66. 117. 131. 150). and interest in explaining distributions ofcharacters and determining the significance of correlated character distribu-tions has increased also (18. 20. 24. 37, 38. 58. 66. 67. 130. 157).

One role that functional or biomeehanical analysis may play in the analysisof correlated characters is to place boundary conditions on hypotheses ofform-function-performance relationships (82). For example, a common goalof comparative functional analyses is to analyze morphology and function aswell as the performance of the organism at particular behavioral tasks (7, 39,101, 141). For example, the jumping performance (ability) of species in aclade of lizards, as measured by maximal jump distance, may be studied byanalyzing the stmcture of the limb bones and muscles in an attempt to explainthe evolution of interspecific differences in locomotor ability. One mighthypothesize that a particular morphology (such as a long femur) or function(such as coactivation of the rectus femoris and gastrocnemius muscles) in agroup of species is causally related to increased jumping performance. InFigure 3A. the clade of species A-B-C is characterized by both a long femurand increased jumping performance (solid circle and square). There is acongment (correlated) historical origin of these characters on the cladogram.If, after an analysis of femur length and jumping distance for species A to Fanincongment origin was found (Figure 3B) such that the origin of increasedbone length precedes the origin of increased jumping ability, then clearly acausal hypothesis of relationship among the two characters is not supported.Greene (58) discusses this pattem of character distribution and notes as aminimal requirement that features thought to be adaptations should have ahistorical origin concordant with the proposed behavioral advantage.

However, the criterion of historical concordance is not sufficient to es-tablish a causal link between a particular morphological or functional attri-bute and increased performance. Typically, many derived features willcharacterize a clade. If. as in Figure 3C. several derived morphologicalattributes characterize the clade A-B-C, then how are we to choose which

Page 10: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

326 LAUDER

Congtueni

PERFORMANCE

STRUCTURE

B •OF

n•E

n•D

D•C

Causal ink via abiomeeharicil modei

Figure S Diagram to indicate how functional analysis may play a role in analyzing the pattem ofcharacters on a cladogram. Although congruenl historical origins of performance, functional, orstructural characters tnay suggest a causal hypothesis of relalionship. a biomeehanical or func-tional analysis can assist in testing a causal hypothesis. Clades will often be characterized bymany novelties at each level (panel C) but how are we lo distinguish historically coirclaledcharacters from those sharing a causal relationship? A biomeehanical or funciional analysis (panelD) may assist by providing a causal model.

changes in body or limb proportions or which stmctura! characters (features2-6, solid circles) are causally related to the observed changes in performance(feature 1, solid square). Perhaps novelty 3 in Figure 3C is the morphologicalcharacter "increased lever arm of the biceps muscle." Without some extemalmodel or criterion to rely on, there is no way to determine which of the fivestmctural characters sharing a congruent (correlated) historical origin iscausally related to the performance change (Figure 3C). The fact that changesin the biceps muscle are not expected to affect jumping ability in any way isonly predictable from a causal biomecbanical model (37. 82. 160).

A critical role of functional morphology in historical analyses of form andfunction is to provide an experimental, theoretical, and mechanistic basis forchoosing which of several correlated characters are in fact causally related tohistorical changes in function or performance (Figure 3D). Functional analy-sis contributes to understanding at a mechanistic, proximate level whycharacters are historically correlated with one another (37, 82). Thus, onlycharacters 5 and 6 (Figure 3D) might be biomechanically related to increased

Page 11: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 327

jumping performance. Characters 2, 3. and 4 may have nothing to do withperformance character I. As analyses of large data sets in systematics becomemore common, as pattems of cbaracter evolution are increasingly examinedusing computer programs such as MacClade and PAUP (31, 135), and asinterest rises in explaining why characters have the distributions they do, itbecomes increasingly important to be able to separate characters that havecongment (correlated) distribution patterns by chance from those that arecausally related.

Three main types of functional analysis might be done to assist in theanalysis of pattems of character distribution such as those shown in Figure3C. First, descriptive functional studies such as quantifying pattems of move-ment, muscle activity, or pressure change in living animals (3. 4. 29. 30, 49,52, 61, 68, 70. 81, 93, 114, 121, 122, 147. 159) provide a baseline offunctional data that may provide a test of proposed causal links amongcharacters. For example, if two muscles attaching to bone A had a congmenthistorical origin with that bone (as in characters 2, 3. and 4 in Figure 3C),measurement of muscle activity might show that only muscle 1 is electricallyactive during the stance phase of locomotion. An hypothesis that the corre-lated historical origin of muscle 2 and bone A is related to acceleration abilityin species A, B, and C (Figure 3C) would be refuted as the muscle is notactive during the appropriate phase of the locomotor cycle. Thus, evenprimarily descriptive functional analyses have the ability to provide a decisivetest of historical explanations for character distribution.

A second type of functional analysis involves direct experimental modifica-tion of stmctures in animals to test proposed form—function—bebaviorrelationships (e.g. 50, 69. 79. 86, 91: also see Eaton & DiDomenico—36, fora theoretical discussion of manipulation experiments). By cutting a ligamentconnecting two bones, for example, and monitoring behavioral or kinematicpattems before and after surgery, proposed functional hypotheses may betested directly. If a novel muscle and increased jumping performance show acorrelated historical origin in a clade (Figure 3C), then functional experimentsinvolving cutting of the muscle tendon and the assessment of jumping per-formance before and after surgery provide a direct test of the causal linkbetween the presence of the muscle and jumping ability.

A third method of assessing the significance of correlated characters is thatof modelling (28, 113, 155. 160). By constmcting theoretical models ofmorphology and using such models to generate a range of possible outputs,given known morphological inputs, understanding is gained of the signifi-cance of morphological variation in a particular mechanical system. Suchmodels may involve mathematical descriptions of shape or direct measure-ments of lever arms, muscle masses, and lines of muscle action to estimate theeffect of changing one anatomical link in a complex linkage system. Me-

Page 12: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

328 LAUDER

chanlcal models of the feeding system in mammals (28, 57, 155), for ex-ample, have allowed understanding of the functional relationships among thebones and muscles and of the significance of changes in the jaw articulation atthe base of the mammalian clade.

In summary, one major role of functional analysis in systematic biology isto contribute to our understanding of how characters interact within theorganism. How do we explain pattems of character distribution? Why is it thaton a particular cladogram two characters appear to have evolved together?Functional morphology by no means provides all the answers to this questionas other explanations (not mutually exclusive) also exist: correlated charactersmay occur because of genetic linkages (40, 127, 130). But functional analysisis able to provide experimental tests, at the phenotypic level, of hypothesizedfunctional dependencies among characters.

Congruence Among Classes of CharactersFunctional analyses are important for examining the extent of congruence inchange among characters at different levels of biological organization. Tosome extent, the analysis of congmence among classes of characters is aspecial case of the analysis of correlated characters considered above. Howev-er, the question of the extent of congmence among different types of charac-ters (e.g. behavioral, morphological, ecological) is an issue that is receivingincreasing attention from comparative biologists (20, 24, 58, 66, 82. 101).Since the study of function provides the hierarchical link between morpholog-ical characters and performance, behavioral, and ecological characters, func-tional characters form an important class of information about organisms thatmerits detailed investigation. Table 1 summarizes one possible hierarchy oflevels of design. This hierarchy illustrates seven classes of characters thatcould be investigated if one wanted to obtain an understanding of organismaldesign from the level of the nervous system to ecological interactions. Acritical point is that functional information provides a link between levels 7and 5 and between levels 5 and 3. An understanding of the morphology alone(levels 7 and 5) is not sufficient to understand the interactions among thelevels of organismal design.

With a hierarchy of levels such as that depicted in Table 1 as a startingpoint, one can address questions about phylogenetic changes at these differentlevels, the extent of congment change among levels, and questions of con-servatism at any individual level. For example, within a clade are certainlevels of design more evolutionariiy conservative than others? Do changes incharacters at two adjacent levels tend to occur in concert while charactersfrom disparate levels show more divergent specializations? Does any onelevel exhibit a greater degree of homoplasy than other levels?

Most authors have viewed functional and physiological data as extremely

Page 13: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 329

Table 1 Levels of analysis that might be studied in attempts to analyze historical pallcms tocharacter transformation.

Level of analysis Example of pattems that might be studied

1. Ecological Iniraspecific and interspecific resource use2. Behavioral Sequence of behaviors used during mating3. Performance/effectiveness Distance moved per unit time: number of prey captured

per unit time4. Functional/physiological: at Physiological properties of muscles and the timitig of

the level of peripheral tissues activation; kinematics of movement; enzyme kinetics;biomechanicai properties of tissues

5. Structural: at the level of periph- Topology of the musculoskeletal system; tissueera! tissues histology

6. Functional/physiological: at the Neuronal spiking pattems: neurotransmitter modulationlevel of the nervous system

7. Structural: at the level of the ner- Neuronal morphology; pattems of neuronal circuitryvous system

plastic and subject to considerable homoplasy when comparisons are madeacross taxa. Level 4 in Table I, function and physiology at the organismallevel, is the class of characters that has been considered the most labile inevolution. Tyler (137: p. 344) asserts that functions vary extensively acrossclades and that therefore "functions do not constitute reliable systematiccharacters in their own right." Other authors (9, 64, 152) have advocated theview that physiological data are plastic and exhibit little phylogenetic coher-ence.

While there is no doubt that certain functional characters may vary con-siderably across taxa, there are no quantitative data indicating that function isany more or less variable than stmctural characters on average. When in-dividual functions (such as metabolic pathways) are compared across a widerange of taxa and found to vary considerably, tbe fact is often ignored thatmany stmctural features of these same taxa are also extremely variable. Nostatement about the historical lability of function has yet been based on aphylogenetic analysis of both stmctural and functional data.

In fact, growing evidence exists to support the opposite view, that function-al characters may often be extremely conservative: Functions may beplesiomorphic characters within a clade that are retained while associatedstmctural features undergo considerable specialization (53. 76, 77, 80, 83,90, 115, 125, 128, 131, 140, 144). Also, when changes do occur in function-al characters, the clades diagnosed by the individual functional characterstates often corroborate the monophyly of clades diagnosed by stmcturalfeatures.

For example, Mommsen & Walsh (107) show that urea synthesis in

Page 14: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

330 LAUDER

vertebrates has a coherent phylogenetic pattern in which changes in ureasynthetic pathways corroborate phylogenetic groupings based on stmcturalfeatures. Goslow et al (53) note that the locomotor system of vertebrates mayhave retained several basic functional pattems despite substantial changes inthe peripheral morphology of the limbs across vertebrates. Dumont & Robert-son (35) have argued that many features of neuronal circuit function arehighly conserved in evolution despite significant morphological changes inperipheral stmctures.

Over the last ten years, research on the evolution of function in the feedingand respiratory systems of ray-finned fishes and salamanders has also shownthat functional patterns may be conservative (73, 76. 77. 80, 94, 95, 100,123, 125, 142-145. 156). Figure 4 is a schematic summary of some of thefmdings from this research which has focused on the evolution of muscle andbone structure and function as a case study in the evolution of levels 2. 3, 4,and 5 in Table 1. Figure 4 illustrates the fact that functional attributes, whentreated phylogenetically, may be more conservative than morphological fea-tures. For example, sunfishes (Centrarchidae) differ considerably in morphol-ogy of the feeding system and are ecologically disparate in the food resourcesutilized (144). However, as shown in Figure 4A. the muscle activity pattem(or motor pattem) used by the jaw muscles is similar for all species that havebeen studied. Similarly, the pattem of muscle activity in the pharyngeal jawmuscles is conserved across sunfishes except in two species that share aderived motor pattern (Figure 4B: 76, 78). Many kinematic features associ-ated with prey capture in ray finned fishes have been found to be conservedacross phylogeny. as have basic pattems of pharyngeal jaw muscle activity(Figure 4C; 73. 90). Finally. Reilly & Lauder (115) have found that manyfeatures of the kinematics of prey transport in vertebrates are conserved acrossthe aquatic-terrestrial transition (Figure 4D): terrestrial tongue-based preytransport uses a kinematic pattem very similar to that used by fishes forhydraulic prey transport.

Functions may also be ontogenetically conservative as shown by an analy-sis of muscle activity pattems during salamander ontogeny (Figure 5; 88. 89).When tiger salamanders undergo metamorphosis (during which considerablemorphological change takes place in the head muscles and bones; 32. 87.116), the pattern of muscle activity used for feeding in the water remains tbesame. Thus, the larval motor pattern is retained across metamorphosis, andthe terrestrial motor pattern is added onto the previous functional pattem(Figure 5). Weeks & Tmman (153, 154) have shown that there is considerableconservatism in the motor pattems used during metamorphosis in the tobaccohomwom (Manduca): Even after the prolegs and their muscles are lost atpupation, the motoneurons continue to generate the larval motor pattern.

Thus, one major conclusion to emerge from research on the ontogeny and

Page 15: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 331

Ambkipltes Pomoxis Lepomis

1. Divefgeril morptioligy

2. Conservdive tnstor pattern 2. [)eriv«d molor pattern

Pol/pterus lepisosteus km leleostei

• • • •D D D D

Actinopterfgi Dipnoi Amphibia Amniola

D D • D •

D

1. Primiine aspecis ofthe kinemotic patternduring initiol prey copiure

!. Primitive ospwts ol Ihemotor pattern lo the

D

n

i primitivepottsrro during prey

troisport

2. l&Kmotics oltongue-basedprey Ironsporl

n

Figure 4 Four examples of studies that provide an historical analysis of both structure andfunction. Panel A: Results from the research of Wainwrighl & Lauder (143. 144) showing thatwithin the sunfish family (Centrarchidae) the pattem of muscle activity used during feeding isconserved while the feeding morphology has diverged considerably throughout the clade. PanelB: Results from Lauder (76. 78) showing that two species share a derived molor pattem in thepharyngeal jaw muscles. Panel C: Results from the work of Lauder (73, 77. 79. 90) to show thatmany functional attribtJtes (of both initial prey capture in fishes, solid squares, and pharyngealjaw muscle activity pattems. open squares) are conserved throughout the evolution of ray-finnedfishes. Panel D: Results from Reilly & Lauder (115) to show that many aspects of the function ofthe jaws and tongue during prey transport are retained across the aquatic—terrestrial transition invertebrate evolution (e.g.. hyoid retraction occurs during the fast opening phase). Experimentaldata for atnphibians show that they retain many primitive kinematic features and that they possesssome derived aspects of prey transport: thus this elade is indicated by both open and filledsquares.

phylogeny of muscle and bone function is that functional characters may infact be more conservative than many structural characteristics. This will notalways be the case, but it is certainly incorrect given present data to assert thatfunctional characters are evolutionariiy labile.

General Principles of Organismal DesignFunctional analyses contribute to defining general pattems and principles inthe evolution of design. Central to any attempt to define general principles oforganismal design is an understanding of how structural components of theorganism are built and function (74, 82, 146). Biomechanicai analysis serves

Page 16: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

332 LAUDER

AQUATIC

AQUATIC

TERRESTRIAL

tnetBmaphDSls

Larv^ motor pattern

Terrestrial motor pattern

^ Larval morphology

Post-metamotphicmorphology

Figure 5 Schematic diagram of ontogeny in metamorphosing tiger salamanders (Ambvsloma

tigrinum) shows that functions may be ontogenetically conserved (88. 89t, Al metamorphosis amuscle activity pattem used for terrestrial feeding is acquired (black box), but the larval pattem isretained and is used during aquatic feeding (open box). Retention of the larval muscle activitypattem occurs despite the many changes in morphology of the bones and muscles of the head atmetamorphosis (hatched boxes).

to define the physical constraints within which organisms must work (56, 75,!47), and a phylogenetic analysis of both form and function allows thehistorical pathways taken during transformations of organismal design to befollowed, and hypotheses about the evolution of design to be tested (74, 75,97).

Two central contentions of my previous discussions of historical pattems toorganismal design (74, 75, 82, 83. 85) are, (a) that there are general pattemsand regularities to how structure and function change and interact in evolutionand that hypotheses about these patterns may be generated and tested, and <b)that much of the regularity to historical pattems of form and function stemsfrom intrinsic design features of organisms. Some authors (e.g. 133: p. 128)have argued that historical events are unique and that in the unique history oflife there is no place for general pattems or "recurrent repeatable rela-tionships." This view ignores three key facts: (a) that many aspects ofhistorical change in organismal design are not unique and do occur over andover again in the history of life (e.g. segmentation of structures, helical fiberorganization), (b) that organisms exhibit constrained ontogenies that limit therange of possible organismai design solutions to environmental problems (2.56), and (c) that many characters, both stmctural and functional, are retainedthroughout speciation events (as plesiomorphies) and thus form part of thehistorical "burden" (118) of a clade.

Page 17: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 333

One example of a repeatable historical pattern is the repetition of individualstructural components of design (other examples are presented in 74, 75, 126,138, 139). Repetition of structural elements has occurred in many groups ofvertebrates, invertebrates, and plants. Vertebrae, limbs, btxly segments,genes, many aspects of plant structure, as well as numerous other componentelements of organisms are all repeated modifications on a common structuraltheme. Typically, specialization of some of the repeated elements has fol-lowed phylogenetically from primitive similarity among the individual com-ponents.

What is the significance of repeated design elements for the evolution ofform and function? Can we frame testable hypotheses about the evolution ofstructural diversity? As one example of the historical implications of repeatedelements of design, consider the evolution of gene structure and function. Ifone asks the question: "What is the causal explanation for the diversity ofproteins in the family of vertebrate hemoglobins?," it is clear that at least oneproximate historical cause is gene duplication early in the evolution ofhemoglobin. As has been discussed in detail elsewhere (85, 103, 108, 112,119), gene duplication is historically permissive in that a primitive geneduplication event allows subsequent specialization of both structure andfunction in the second gene copy during cladogenesis. While we may have noidea of the ultimate causes of the duplication event itself (or of the selectivefactors, if any, that lead to gene duplication), without the additional source ofstructural and functional variability provided by additional copies of func-tionally important genes, the diversity of functions supported by the multiplevariants of hemoglobin proteins would not exist: one copy of a functionallyimportant gene is constrained from change by the critical nature of its productor regulatory function. While gene duplication is not the only mechanismresponsible for protein family diversity, it is certainly a critical one for manyclasses of proteins. Minimally, gene duplication is sufficient for generatingprotein diversity; it is perhaps also necessary if other genetic mechanisms arenot active for any particular family of proteins (108).

Raff et al (112) have discussed another major consequence of proteindiversification through gene duplication: the ability to modulate the timing,location, and amount of protein synthesis to a greater extent than when asingle-gene copy is present. Gene duplication, then, allows both stmcturaland functional specialization in families of proteins.

Similar pattems of structural duplication are found at the organismal levelin many plants and animals, and similar historical consequences may beobserved. After the origin of repeated stmctural elements in a clade, subse-quent cladogenesis is typically marked by independent specialization (bothstructural and functional) of at least some of the repeated elements. Theevolution of the feeding system in ray-finned fishes has provided one examplein which the evolution of a second biomechanicai linkage system in the jaw

Page 18: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

334 LAUDER

has permitted subsequent historical divergence in the morphology and func-tion of the feeding mechanism (73, 74, 85).

It is also possible to define a set of steps by which historical hypothesesabout the evolution of morphological and functional diversity may be tested(85). Historical hypotheses are testable by quantitatively comparing ingroupand outgroup taxa with respect to a proposed novelty. As the example of geneduplication illustrates, there are common features to organismal design thatappear to have general (and predictable) consequences for subsequent di-versification of structure and function.

Emerson (38) has provided the best quantitative test to date of the historicaleffect of a morphological novelty. Emerson investigated frog pectoral girdleswith the aim of assessing the extent of repeatable historical transformation inshape. Specifically, she tested the decoupling hypothesis (74)—that an in-crease in morphological constraint (and therefore a decrease in morphologicaldiversity) should be associated with a reduction in the number of independentdesign elements. Eight phylogenetically independent cases of cartilage fusionin the frog pectoral girdle were analyzed. Emerson (38) showed that therewere repeated historical changes in shape in each clade following cartilagefusion. These data provide strong support for the idea that historically regularpatterns of morphological change do exist.

SUMMARY

Functional data have been both the least used and the least understood class ofdata in systematics. Compared to the use of morphological features fromDNA sequences to gross structural characters, pattems of distribution, andeven ecological and behavioral attributes of organisms, functional charactershave not been generally thought of as useful for resolving systematic andhistorical questions.

The current status of functional data in systematics is due to three primaryfactors, both historical and practical. First, current research in systematicsdeveloped primarily out of the nineteenth century morphological traditionwhile the analysis of organismal function was centered in physiological andexperimental embryological research. At the turn of the century these tworesearch traditions, initially complementary, diverged. While several authorshave debated the severity of the divergence in research between morpholo-gists (interested in the comparative analysis oi' structure, its development, andphylogeny) and experimental biologists (interested in function, physiology, oruses of structures) (5, 6, 102), there is little doubt that these two researchareas diverged in the early part of this century and have remained largelyseparate.

Second, functional data are hard to obtain on a diversity of taxa: Gathering

Page 19: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 335

a range of experimental data on even one small clade is time consuming, andconducting manipulative experiments to understand causal relationships onlyadds to the difficulty of comparative functional analyses. Third, many so-called "functional" analyses and discussions of the import of function forsystematics are in fact purely morphological. The adjective "functional" hasacquired a cachet: It sounds quantitative, technical, and sophisticated. As aconsequence, it is frequently used by papers in which there is no resemblanceof a true functional analysis, where organismal function is directly measuredand compared with measured function in other clades. The valid heuristic useof functional ideas should not be a substitute for direct measurement.

A key aim of this paper is to suggest that functional data are in fact criticalto understanding five important issues in systematics. Despite difficulties ingathering functional data, the development of even a small number of well-understood case studies would greatly enhance our appreciation of (a; histor-ical patterns to functional transformation, (b) the use of functional charactersto define monophyletic clades. (c) the causal basis of character distributionson ciadograms. (d) the extent to which changes in structural, functional, andbehavioral characters are historically congruent, and the extent of evolution-ary conservatism at any particular level of organismal design, and (e) generalpattems and principles in the evolution of form and function.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank Drs. Peter Wainwright, Steve Reilly, and Bruce Jayne for commentson the manuscript. Preparation of this article supported by grants NSF BSR8520305 and DCB 8710210.

Literature Cited

1. Alberch, P., Alberch. J. 1981.Heterochronic mechanisms of morpho-logical diversification and evolutionarychange in the neotropical salamander.Bolitoglossa occidentalis (Amphibia:Plethodontidae). J, Morphol, 167:249-64

2. Alberch. P., Gould. S. J., Oster. G. F..Wake. D. B. 1979, Size and shape inontogeny and phylogeny. Paleobiotoev5:296-317

3. .Alexander, R. McN. 1975. Biomechan-ics, London: Chapman & Hall

4. Alexander. R. McN. 1983. AnimalMechanics. Oxford: Biackwell. 2nd ed.

5. Allen. G. 1978. Life Science in theTwentieth Century. Cambridge: Cam-bridge Univ. Press

6. Allen. G. 1981. Morphology and twen-tieth cetitury biology: a response. J.Hist. Biol, 14:159-76

7. Arnold. S. J. 1983. Morphology, per-formance, and fitness. Am. Zool.23:347-61

8. Atz. J. 1970. The application of the ideaof homology to animal behavior. In De-velopment and Evolution of Behavior:Es,tays in Honor ofT. C. Schneirla. ed.L. Aronson, E. Tobach. D. S. Lehrman.J. S. Rosenblatt. San Francisco: W. H.Freeman

9. Barrington. E. J. W. 1975. Comparativephysiology and the challenge of design.J. Exp. Zool. 194:271-86

10. Bertram. B. 1976. Kin selection in lionsand in evolution. In Growing Points inEthology, ed. P. P. G. Batesoti. R. A.Hinde. pp 281-301. Cambridge: Cam-bridge Univ. Press

11. Bock, W. J. 1964. Kinetics of the avianskull. J, Morphol. 114:1-42

12. Bock, W. J. 1980. The definition and

Page 20: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

336 LAUDER

recognition of biological adaptation.Am. Zool. 20:217-27

13. Bock. W. J, 1981. Functional-adaptiveanalysis in evolutionary classiflcation.Am. Zool. 21:5-20

14. BockW.J. 1988. The nature of explana-tions in morphology. Am. Zool. 28:205-15

15. Bock. W. J. 1989. Organisms as func-tional machines: a connectivity explana-tion. Am. Zool. 29:1119-32

15. Bock, W.. von Wahlert. G. 1965.Adaptation and the form-function com-plex. Evol. 19:269-99

17. Bookstein, F., Chemoff. B.. Elder, R..Humphries, J.. Smith. G.. Strauss, R.1985. Morphometrics in EvolutionaryBiology. Philadelphia: Acad. Nat. Sci.

18. Brooks. D R . 1984. Whafs going on inevolution? A brief guide to some newideas in evolutionary theory. Can. J.Zool. 61:2637-45

19. Brooks. D. R. 1985. Historical ecology:a new approach to studying the evolutionof ecological assixriations. Ann. Miss,Bot, Card, 72:660-80

20. Brooks. D. R.. Wiley. E. O. 1988.Evolution as Entropy. Chicago: Univ.Chicago Press, 2nd ed.

21. Buth. D- 1984. The application ofelcctRiphoretic data in systematic stud-ies. Annu. Rev. Ecot, S\.U. 15:501-22

22. Carrier. D. R. 1987. The evolution oflocomotor stamina in tetrapods:circumventing a mechanical constraint.Paleohiologv I3:326--Ii

23. Clutton-Brock. T. H.. Harvey. P. 1976.Evolutionary rules and primate societies.In Growing Points in Ethology, ed. P. P.G. Bateson. R. A. Hinde. pp 195-237.Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press

24. Coddington. J. 1988. Cladistic tests ofadaptational hypotheses. Cladistics 4:i-22

25. Coleman, W. 1977. Biology in theNineteenth Century: Problems of Form,Function, and transformation, Cam-bridge: Cambridge Univ. Press

26. Cracraft. J. 1981. The use of functionaland adaptive criteria in phylogenetic sys-tematics. Am. Zool, 21:21-36

27. Cracraft. J. 1986. Origin and evolutionof continental biotas: speciation and his-torical congruence within the Australianavifauna. Evolution 40:977-96

28. Crompton, A. W.. Parker. P. 1978.Evolution of the mammalian masticatoryapparatus. Am. Sci. 66:192-201

29. Crompton, A. W.. Thcxton. A. J., Par-ker. P., Hiiemae. K. 1977. The activityof the jaw and hyoid musculature in iheVirginia opossum, Didelphis virginiana.In The Biology of Marsupials, ed.

B. Stonehouse, G. Gilmore, pp. 287-305. New York: MacMillan

30. Cundall.D. 1983. Activity of head mus-cles during feeding by snakes: a com-parative study. Am. Zool. 23:383-96

31. Donoghue.M.J 1989. Phylogenies andthe analysis of evolutionary sequences.with examples from seed plants. Evolu-tion Ay.\ Ml-'it

32. Duellman. W. E..Trucb. L. 1986. Biol-ogv of Amphibians. New York: McGrawHill

33. Dullemeijer, P 1972. Explanation inmorphology. Acta Bioiheor. 21:26O-73

34. Dullemeijer, P. 1980. Functionalmorphology and evolutionary biology.Acta Biotheor. 29:151-250

35. Dumont. J., Robemon, R. M. 1986.Neuronal circuits: an evolutionary per-spective. Science 233:849-53

36. Eaton. R. C , DiDomenico. R. 1985.Command and the neural causation ofbehavior: a theoretical analysis of thenecessity and sufficiency paradigm.Brain Behav. Evol. 27:132-64

37. Emerson. S. 1982. Frog postcranialmorphology: identification of a function-al complex. Copela 1982:603-13

38. Emerson. S. 1988. Testing for historicalpatterns of change: a case study withfrog pectoral girdles. Paleobiology14:174-86

39. Emerson. S.. Diehl. D. 1980. Toe padmorphology and mechanisms of stickingin frogs. Bioi. J. Linn. Soc. 13:199-216

40. Falconer. D. S. 1981. Introduction toQuantitative Genetics. London: Long-man

41. Feder. M. E.. Bennett, A. F.. Burggren.W.. Huey. R. B. 1987. New Directionsin Ecological Physiology. Cambridge:Cambridge Univ. Press

42. Fink, W. L. 1988. Phylogenetic analysisand the detection of ontogenetie pat-tems. In Heterochrony in Evolution, ed.M. L. McKinney. pp. 71-91. NewYork: Plenum

43. Fisher. D. C. 1981. The role of func-tional analysis in phylogenetic inference:examples from the history of the Xipho-sura. Am. Zool. 21:47-62

44. Fisher, D. C. 1985. Evolutionarymorphology: beyond the analogous, an-ecdotal, and the ad hoc. Paleobiology11:120-38

45. Frazzetta.T. H. 1975. Complex Adapta-tions in Evolving Populations. Sunder-land. Mass: Sinauer

46. Gans. C. 1970. Straiegy and sequence inthe evolution of the external gas ex-changers of ectothermal vertebrates.Forma et Functio 3:61-104

Page 21: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 337

47. Gans. C. 1974. Biomechanics. AnApproach To Vertebrate Biology. Phila-delphia: J. B. Lippincott

48. Gans. C. 1988. Adaptation and theform-function relation. Am. Zoot,28:681-97

49. Gans. C . Gomiak, G. C. 1982. Func-tional morphology of lingual protrusionin marine toads (Bufo marinus). Am J.Anat. 163:195-222

50. Gans, C . Gomiak. G. C. 1982. Howdoes the toad flip its tongue? Test of twohypotheses. Science 216:1335-37

51. Goodman. M. 1982. MacromalecularSequences in Systematic and Evolution-ary Biology. New York: Plenum

52. Gomiak, G. C . Rosenberg H. 1., Gans,C. 1982. Mastication in the Tuatara.Sphenodon punctaius (Reptilia: Rhyn-chocephalia): structure and activity ofthe motor system. J. Morphot. 171:321-53

53. Goslow, G. E.. Dial, K. P.. Jenkins, F.A. 1989. The avian shoulder: an ex-perimental approach. -4m. Zoot. 29:287-301

54. Gould, S, J. 1966. Allometry and size inontogeny and phyiogeny. Biol. Rev.41:587-640

55. Gould S. J. 1977. Ontogeny and Phy-logeny. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press

56. Gould. S. J. 1980. The evolutionary bi-ology of constraint. Daedalus 109:39-52

57. Greaves, W. S. 1978. The jaw leversystem in ungulates: a new model. J.Zoot,, Lond. 184:271-85

58. Greene. H. 1986. Diet and arboreality inthe Emerald Monitor. Varanusprasinus.with comments on the study of adap-tation. Fieidiana (Zool.. N.S.) 31:1-12

59. Gutmann. W. F. I98L Relationshipsbetween invenebrate phyla based onfunctional-mechanical analysis of thehydrostatic skeleton. Am. Zool. 21:63-81

60. Haas. O.. Simpson. G G. 1946. Analy-sis of some phyiogenetic terms, with at-tempts at redefinition. Proe. Am. Phil.Soc. 90:319-49

61. Herring, S. W. 1985. The ontogeny ofmammalian mastication. Am. Zool.25:339-49

62. Hickman. C. S. 1988. Analysis of formand function in fossils. Am. Zool, 28:775-93

63. Hillis, D. 1987. Molecular versus mor-phological approaches to systematics.Annu. Rev. Ecol. Svst, 18:23-42

64. Hoar, W. S. \9^i. General and Com-parative Physiology. New Jersey: Pren-tice Hall

65. Hodos. W. 1976. The concept of homol-ogy and the evolution of behavior. InEvolution, Brain, and Behavior: Per-sistent Problems, ed. R. B. Masterion.W. Hodos, H. Jerison, pp 153-67. Hill-sdale. NJ: Erlbaum

66. Huey. R. 1987. Phylogeny, history, andthe comparative method. In New Direc-tions in Ecological Physiology, ed. M.E. Feder, A. F. Bennett. W. W. Burg-gren, R. B. Huey, pp. 76-101. Cam-bridge: Cambridge Univ. Press

67. Huey, R. B.. Bennett. A. F. 1987.Phylogenetic studies of coadaptation:preferred temperatures versus optimalperformance temperatures of lizards.Evolution 41:1098-1115

68. Jayne, B. C. 1988. Muscular mech-anisms of snake locomotion: an eleciro-myographic study of the sidewindingand concertina modes of Croiatuscerastes, Nerodia fasciata, and Etapheobsoleta, J. Exp. Biol. 140:1-33

69. Jayne, B. C . Bennett, A. F. 1989. Theeffect of tail morphology on locomotorperfomiance in snakes: a comparison ofexperimental and correlative methods. J.E.xp. Zool, 252:126-33

70. Jenkins. F. A.. Weijs. W. A. 1979. Thefunctional anatomy of the shoulder ofthe Virginia opossum Didelphis virgi-niana. J. Zoot.. Lond, 188:379-410

71. Kluge, A. G. 1989. A concern for evi-dence and a phylogenetic hypothesis ofrelationships among Epicrates (Boidae.Serpentes). Sv.vr. Zool. 38:7-25

72. Kluge. A. G.. Strauss. R. E. 1985.Ontogeny and systematics. Annu. Rev.Ecol. Syst. 16:247-68

73. Lauder, G. V. 1980. Evolution of thefeeding mechanism in primitive actino-pterygian fishes: a functional anatomicalanalysis ni Polypietiis. Lepisosteus, andAmia. J. Morphol, 163:283-317

74. Lauder. G. V. 1981. Form and function:structural analysis in evolutionarymorphology, Paleohiotogy 7:430-42

75. Lauder, G. V. 1982. Historical biologyand the problem of design. J. Theor,Biol. 97:57-67

76. Lauder. G. V. 1983. Functional andmorphological bases of tmphic special-ization in sunfishes (Teieostei: Centrar-chidae). J. Morphol, 178:1-2!

77. Lauder, G. V. 1983. Functional designand evolution of the pharyngeal jawapparatus in eutcleostean fishes. Zool. J,Linn, Soc, 77:1-38

78. Lauder, G. V. 1983. Neuromuscularpatterns and the origin of trophicspecialization in fishes. Science 219:1235-37

79. Uuder, G. V. 1983. Prey capture hy-

Page 22: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

338 LAUDER

drodynamics in fishes: experimentaltests of two models. J. E.\p. Biol, 104:1-13

80. Lauder. G. V. 1986. Homology. analo-gy, and the evolution of behavior. In TheEvolution of Behavior, ed. M. Nitecki,J. Kitchell, pp. 9-40. Oxford: OxfordUniv. Press

81. Lauder.G.V. 1989. Caudal fin locomo-tion in ray-finned fishes: historical andfunctional analyses. Am. Zool. 29:85-102

82. Lauder, G. V. 1990. Bii)mechanics andevolution: integrating physical and his-torical biology in the study of complexsystems, In Biomechanics in Evolution.ed. J. M. V. Rayner. Cambridge: Cam-bridge Univ, Press. In press

83. Lauder. G. V. 1990. An evolutionaryperspective on the concept of efficiency:how does function evolve? In Conceptsof Efficiency in Biolofiiccd Systems, ed.R. W. Blake. Cambridge: CambridgeUniv. Press. In presss

84. Lauder, G. V.. Crompton, A. W..Gans, C . Hanken. J.. Liem, K. F.. etal. 1989. How are feeding systems inte-grated and how have evolutionary inno-vations been introduced? Group Report# 1 . in Complex Organismal Functions:Integration and Evolution in Ver-tebrates, ed. D. B. Wake. G. Roth, pp.97-115. New York: John Wiley

85. Lauder, G. V., Liem. K. F. 1989. Therole of historical factors in the evolutionof complex organismal functions. InComplex Organismal Functions: Inte-gration and Evolution in Vertebrates,ed. D, B. Wake. G. Roth. pp. 63-78.New York: John Wiley

86. Lauder. G. V.. Reilly. S. M. 1988.Functional design of the feeding mech-anism in salamanders: causal bases ofontogenetie changes in function. J. E.xp.Biol. 134:219-33

87. Lauder, G. V., Reilly. S. M. 1990.Metamorphosis of the feeding mech-anism in tiger salamanders (Amhysiomatigrinum): the ontogeny of cranial mus-cle mass. J. Zool.. Lond. In press

88. Lauder, G. V,. Shaffer. H. B. 1988.The ontogeny of functional design in ti-ger salamanders lAmbystoma tigrinum):are motor pattems conserved during ma-jor morphological transformations? J.'Morphol, 197:249-68

89. Lauder, G. V.. Shaffer. H. B. 1990.Design of the aquatic vertebrate skull:major pattems and their evolutionary in-terpretations. In The Vertebrate Skull,Vol. 3, ed. J. Hanken. B. Hall. Chicago:Univ. Chicago Press. In press

90. Lauder. G. V., Wainwright. P. C. 1990.

Function and history: the pharyngeal jawapparatus in primitive ray-finned fishes.In Systematics, Historical Ecology, andNorth American Freshwater Fi.'ihes. ed.R, W. Mayden. Stanford: StanfordUniv. Press. In press

91. Liem. K. F. 1970. Comparative func-tional anatomy of the Nandidae (Pisces:Teieostei). Eieldiana Zool. 56:1-166

92. Liem. K. F. 1973. Evolutionary strat-egies and morphological innovations:cichlid pharyngeal jaws. Syst. Zool,22:425-41

93. Liem. K. F. 1986. The pharyngea! jawapparatus of the Embiotocidae (Teie-ostei): a functional and evolutionary per-spective. Copeia 1986:311-23

94. Liem. K. F. 1988. Form and fuction oflungs: the evolution of air breathingmechanisms. Am. Zool, 28:739-59

95. Liem, K. F. 1989. Respiratory gasbladders in teleosts: functional con-servatism and morphological diversity.Am. Zool. 29:333-52

96. Liem. K. F . Greenwood. P. H. 1981. Afunctional approach to the phylogeny ofpharyngognath teleosts. Am, Zool.21:83-101

97. Liem. K. F.. Wake. D. B. 1985.Morphology: current approaches andconcepts. In Functional VertebrateMorphology, ed, M. Hildebrand. D. M.Bramble. K. F. Liem. and D. B. Wake.pp. 336-77. Cambridge: Harvard Univ.Press

98. Lombard. R. E., Wake. D. B. 1976.Tongue evolution in the lungtesssalamanders, family Plethodontidae. I.Introduction, theory and a general modelof dynamics. J. Morphol. 148:265-86

99. Lombard. R. E.. Wake. D. B. 1977.Tongue evolution in the lunglesssalamanders, family Plethodontidae. II.Function and evolutionary diversity. J.Morphol. 153:39-80

100. Lombard. R. E.. Wake. D. B. 1986.Tongue evolution in the lunglesssalamanders. Family Plethodontidae.IV. Phylogeny of plethodontidsalamanders and the evolution of feedingdynamics. Syst. Zool, 35:532-51

101. Losos. J. 1990. Concordant evolution ofliKomotor behavior, display rate andmorphology in Anolis lizards. Behavior.In press

102. Maienschein. J., Rainger, R.. Benson,K. R. 1981. Special section on Amer-ican morphology at the tum of the cen-tury. J. Hist. Biol. 14:83-191

103. Markert. C. L.. Shaklee, J. B.. Whitt.G. S. 1975. Evolution of agene. 5c7('nce189:102-14

104. Mayden. R. L. 1988. Vicariance bio-

Page 23: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS 339

geography, parsimony, and evolution inNorth American freshwater fishes. Syst,Zool. 37:329-55

105. Mayr. E. 1961. Cause and effect in biol-ogy. Science I34:15OI-W

106. Mayr, E. \9S2. The Growth of Biologic-al Thought. Cambridge: Harvard Univ.Press

107. Mommsen, T. P., Walsh, P. J. 1989.Evolution of urea synthesis in ver-tebrates: the piscine connection. Science243:72-5

108. Ohno. S. 1970. Evolution hy GeneDuplication, New York: Springer-Verlag

109. Patterson. C. 1980. Cladistics. Bio/wem27:234-40

110. Patterson. C. 1982. Morphologicalcharacters and homology. In Problemsof Phylogenetic Reconstruction, ed. K.A. Joysey. A. E. Friday. London: Aca-demic

111. Patterson. C. 1987. Molecules andMorphology in Evolution: Conflict orCompromise? London: CambridgeUniv. Press

112. Raff, E. C . Diaz. H. B., Hoyle. H. D..Hutchens. J. A.. Kimble. M. et al.1987. Origin of multiple gene families:are there both functional and regulatoryconstraints? In Developmeni as an Evo-lutionary Proces.s, ed. R. Raff, E. C.Raff. pp. 203-38. New York: Alan Liss

113. Raup. D. M. 1972. Approaches tomorphologic analysis. In Models inPaleobiology. ed. T. J. Schopf. pp 28-44. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman

114. Reilly. S. M.. Lauder, G. V. 1989.Kinetics of tongue projection in Amb\s-toma tigrinum: quantitative kinematics,muscle function and evolutionary hy-potheses. J. Morphol. 199:22.3-43

115. Reilly. S. M.. Lauder. G. V. 1990. Theevolution of tctrapod prey transport be-havior: kinematic homologies in feedingfunction. Evotution. In press

116. Reilly. S. M.. Lauder. G. V. 1990,Metamorphosis of cranial design in tigersalamanders {Ambystoma tigrinum): amorphomeiric analysis of ontogenetiechange. J. Morphol, 204:121-37

117. Ridley. M. 1983. The E.xpkmation ofOrganic Diversity. Oxford: Clarendon

118. Riedl. R, \91?.. Order in Living Organ-isms. New York: J. Wiley

119. Romero-Herreni, A. E.. Lehman. H..Joysey, K. A.. Friday. A. E. 1978. Onthe evolution of myoglobin. Phitos.Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 283:61-163

120. Rosen. D. E. 1978. Vicariant pattemsand historical explanation in biogeogra-phy. Syst. Zoot. 27:159-88

12L Roth G. 1976. Experimental analysis of

the prey catching behavior of Hvdro-mantes italicus Dunn (Amphibia.Piethodontidae). J. Comp. Phvxiol. tO9:47-58

122. Rotb. G. 1986. Neural mechanisms ofprey recognition: an example in amphib-ians. In Predator-Prey Relationships:Perspectives and Approaches From theStudy of Lower Vertebrates, ed. M. E.Feder. G. V. Lauder, pp. 42-68. Chica-go: Univ. Chicago Press

123. Roth, G.. Wake, D. B. 1989. Con-servatism and innovation in the evolu-tion of feeding in vertebrates. In Com-plex Organismal Functions: Integrationand Evotution in Vertebrates, ed. D. BWake, G. Roth. pp. 7-21. New York:John Wiley

124. Russell. E. S. 1916. Form and Func-tion: a contribution lo the hi.stor\ of an-imal morphology. (Reprinted 1982.Univ. Chicago Press.) Chicago: Univ.Chicago Press

125. Sanderson. S. L. 1988. Variation inneuromuscular activity during prey cap-lure by trophic specialists and generalists(Pisces: Labridae). Brain Behav. Evol.32:257-68

126. .Schaefer, S. A.. Lauder. G. V. 1986.Historical transformation of functionaldesign: evolutionary morphology offeeding mechanisms in loricarioid cat-fishes. Sy.st. Zool. 35:489-508

127. Schluter. D. 1989. Bridging populationand phylogenetic approaches to theevolution of complex traits. See Ref.151. pp. 79-95

128. Schwenk. K.. Throckmonon. G. S.1989. Functional and evolutionarymorphology of lingual feeding insquamate reptiles: phylogenetics andkinematics. J. Zool., Lond. 219:153-75

129. Seilacher. A. 1973. Fabricational noisein adaptive morphology, Svst. Zool.22:451-65

130. Shaffer. H. B. 1986. Utility of quantita-tive genetic parameters in characterweighting. S\st. Zool. 35:124-34

131. Sillen-Tullberg. B. 1988. Evolution ofgregariousness in aposematic butterflylarvae: a phylogenetic analysis. Evolu-tion 42:293-.305

132. Simpson, G, G. 1958. Behavior andevolution. In Behavior and Evolution.ed. A. Roe. G. G. Simpson, pp 507-35.New Haven: Yale Univ. Press.

133. Simpson, G. G. 1964. This ViewofLife.New York: Harcoun Brace & World

134. Strauss. R. E. 1985. Evolutionaryallometry and variation in body form inthe South American catfish genus Cory-doras (Callichthyidae). Syst, Zool. 32:381-96

Page 24: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …

340 LAUDER

135. Swofford, D. L. 1984. Phylogeneticanalysis using parsimony (PAUP). ver-sion 2.3. Illinois: III. Nat. Hist. Survey

136. Szalay, F. S. 1981. Functional analysisand the practice of the phylogeneticmethod as reflected by some mammalianstudies. Am. Zool. 21:37-45

137. Tyler. S. 1988. The role of function indetemiinalion of homology and con-vergence—examples from invertebrateadhesive organs. Forrsch. Zool. 36:331-47

138. Vermeij. G. 1973. Adaptation, versatil-ity and evolution. Syst. Zool. 22:466-77

139. Vermeij. G. 1973. Biological versatilityand earth history. Proe, Natl, Acad, Sci.USA 70:1936-38

140. Vigna. S. R. 1985. Functional evolutionof gastcrointestinal homiones. In Evolu-tionary Biology of Primitive Fishes, ed.R. B. Foreman, A. Gorbman, J. M.Dotld. R. Olsson. pp. 401-12. NewYork: Plenum

141. Wainwright. P. C. 1987. Biomechanicailimits to ecological pertomiance:mollusc-crushing by the Caribbean hog-fish, Lachnolaimus maximus {Labridae).J. Zoot., Lond. 213:283-97

142. Wainwhght. P. C. 1989. Prey process-ing in haemuiid fishes: pattems of varia-tion in pharyngeal jaw muscle activity./ . Exp. Biol. 141:359-76

143. Wainwright, P.. Lauder, G. V. 1986.Feeding biology of sunfishes: pattems ofvariation in prey capture. Zool. J. Linn.Soc. Lond. 88:217-28

144. Wainwright. P C , Lauder. G.V. 1990.The evolution of feeding biology in sun-fishes (Centrarebidae). In Systematics,Historicat Ecotogy, and North AmericanFreshwater Fishes, ed R. W. Mayden.Stanford: Stanford Univ. Press. In press

145. Wainwright. P. C Sanford. C. P.,Reiliy, S. M.. Lauder, G. V. 1989.Evolution of motor pattems: aquaticfeeding in salamanders and ray-fmnedfishes. Brain Behav, Evol. 34:329-41

146. Wainwrigbt, S, A. 1988. Axis and Cir-cumference: The Cylindrical Shape ofPlants and Animals. Cambridge: Har-vard Univ. Press

147. Wainwright, S. A., Biggs. W. D.. Cur-rey. J. D.. Gosline, J. M. 1976. Me-chanical Design in Organisms. NewYork: John Wiley

148. Wake. D. B. 1982. Functional and de-

velopmental constraints and opportuni-ties in the evolution of feeding systemsin urodeles. In Environmental Adapta-tion and Evolution, ed. D. Mossakow-ski. G. Roth. pp. 51-66. New York:Gustav Fisher

149. Wake. D. B. 1982. Functional and evo-lutionary morphology. Pers. Biol. Med.25:603-20

150. Wake, D. B., Larson. A. 1987. Multi-dimensional analysis on an evolvinglineage. Science 238:42-8

151. Wake, D, B.. Roth. G.. eds. 1989.Complex Organismal Functions: Inte-gration and Evolution in Vertebrates,New York: John Wiley

152. Waterman. T. H. 1975. Expectation andachievement in comparative physiology.J. Exp. Zool. 194:309-44

153. Weeks, J. C , Truman. J. W. 1984.Neural organization of peptide-activatedecdysis behaviors during the meta-morphosis of Manduca .se.xta I. Con-servation of the peristalsis motor pattemat tbe larval-pupal transformation. J.Comp. Phy.siot, A 155:407-22

154. Weeks. J. C.. Tmman. J. W. 1984.Neural organization of peptide-activatedecdysis behaviors during the meta-morphosis of Manduca se.xta II. Reten-tion of the proleg motor pattem despiteloss of the prolegs at pupation. J. Comp,Physiol. A 155:423-33

155. Weijs. W. A..Dantuma. R. 1981. Func-tional anatomy of the masticatory appa-ratus in the rabbit iOryctolagus cunicu-tus L,), Neth, J. Zoot. 31:99-147

156. Westneat. M. W., Wainwright, P. C.1989. Feeding mechanism of Epibutusinsidiator (Labridae: Teieostei): evolu-tion of a novel functional system. J.Morphot. 202:129-50

157. Wiley. E. O. 1981. Phytogenetics, NewYork: John Wiley

158. Wiley. E, O. 1988. Vicariancebiogeography. Annu. Rev. Ecot. Syst,19:513-54

159. Zweers. G. 1974. Structure, movement,and myography of the feeding apparatusof the mallard iAnas ptatyrhynchos L.).a study in functional anatomy. Neth. J.Zoot. 24:323-467

160. Zweers, G. 1979. Explanation of struc-ture by optimization and systemization.Neth. J, Zoot. 29:418-40

Page 25: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …
Page 26: FUNCTIONAL MORPHOLOGY AND SYSTEMATICS: Studying …