general methodological issues - sage publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would...

27
PART I General Methodological Issues 9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 27

Upload: others

Post on 16-Apr-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

PART I

General Methodological Issues

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 27

Page 2: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 28

Page 3: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

Measures of the PersonalityFactors Found Recurrently in

Human Lexicons

Gerard Saucier

How can attributes of personality best beorganized and measured? Answers to thiscrucial scientific question provide the foun-dation not only for personality tests, but alsofor much research on personality and individ-ual differences. Studies of natural languagesprovide an important source of answers. Inthis chapter I review the approach used insuch lexical studies of personality attributes,as well as basic findings and major measuresassociated with these studies.

Lexical measures of personality factors areused primarily in research settings. Becausethe items themselves are terms from the lex-icon, they are easily embedded within lexical-study stimuli, where they provide the mostdirect representation of lexical factors. Theyhave also proven to be extremely useful tem-plates for the development of more sophisti-cated assessment instruments. Moreover,because lexical factors have a solid content-validity basis, they can be used in the valida-tion of other measures. This chapter presents anarray of measures for lexical personality fac-tors, concentrating on those measures based

most directly on lexical structures; that is,those designed to be markers of these struc-tures. Inquiries into the structure of attributeshinge strongly on how personality is defined.Therefore, the definition of personality is agood place to begin a discussion of structure.

DEFINING PERSONALITY

Definitions make one’s assumptions explicit.How one defines personality is consequential,affecting how one selects variables when study-ing personality. There is no single canonicaldefinition in current use. Personality is definedeither as (a) a set of attributes characterizingan individual, or as (b) the underlying systemthat generates the set of attributes. Funder(1997) provided a definition that includes both (a) and (b): Personality is ‘an individual’scharacteristic patterns of thought, emotion, and behavior, together with the psychologicalmechanisms – hidden or not – behind those pat-terns’ (1997: 1–2). Funder refers to a broad

1

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 29

Page 4: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

array of attributes that simultaneously are (i)ascribed to individuals, (ii) stable over time,and (iii) psychological in nature.

But there are other ways to define person-ality. In a classic early textbook, Allport (1937)catalogued 50 distinct meanings found indefinitions of personality. These meanings canbe arrayed in a continuum ranging from one’sexternally observable manner to one’s inter-nal self. Reacting against broad omnibus def-initions of personality (e.g. Prince, 1924),Allport’s definition – ‘personality is thedynamic organization within the individualof those psychophysical systems that deter-mine his unique adjustments to his environ-ment’ (1937: 48) – highlights attributes thatare seen as residing ‘within’ the individual.

However, other ways of defining personality,consistent with a ‘biosocial’ view that Allportdeprecated, emphasize attributes that are moreexternal or that involve the effect the indi-vidual tends to have on others. These include (a) attributes of external appearance (includ-ing qualities like physical size), (b) attributesassociated with the role one assumes or thestatus one has achieved in society (e.g. pro-fessional, motherly, famous), and (c) attrib-utes of an evocative type, that involve thepattern of reactions that the individual gener-ates in others given the kind of stimulus s/heis. (e.g. charming, intimidating, boring, believ-able, lovable, respected, offensive). Such socialeffects represent a person’s social stimulusvalue (Allport, 1937: 41; based on May, 1932).

Another class with controversial status aspersonality attributes is that containinghighly evaluative terms (e.g. stupid, evil,abnormal, good). Most personality conceptsare decidedly evaluative (clearly favorable orunfavorable; Goldberg, 1982), but these aredistinct in the high ratio of the evaluative tothe descriptive component. Highly evaluativeterms are not ‘pure evaluation’; one can finddescriptive dimensions from selections con-sisting purely of such terms (Benet-Martinezand Waller, 2002), so they do have somedescriptive component.

What about patterns of belief and attitudes?Allport (1937) generally regarded attitudes

as behavioral dispositions of a specific andexternal sort, being ‘bound to an object orvalue’ (1937: 294); that is, aroused in thepresence of a specifiable class of stimuli. If,however, an attitude is ‘chronic and tempera-mental’, expressed in almost any sphere ofthe person’s behavior’ (1937: 294), as in forexample radicalism or conservatism, then forAllport it differed little from a trait. Thus,generalized attitudes – those for which it isdifficult to specify the object – can be consid-ered personality traits. Factors derived fromthe correlations among large numbers of morespecific attitudes and beliefs define traits, inthat they represent consistent patterns acrossmany attitude objects.

Values can be seen as beliefs regarding‘how one ought or ought not to behave, orabout some end-state of existence worth ornot worth attaining’ (Rokeach, 1968: 124),and, echoing Allport’s distinctions, ‘not tiedto any specific attitude object or situation’(Rokeach, 1968: 124). Super (1995) charac-terized interests as related to values, beingpreferences for classes of activities in whichindividual expect to attain their values. Interestsinvolve assessing objects according to howliked or disliked they are (rather than theirfavorability or importance more generally).Career-interest measures show even higherstability than do personality measures (Lowet al., 2005). And there are dimensions ofvariation in career-interest items that are rel-atively independent of currently popular traitdimensions (Ackerman, 1996; Ackerman andHeggestad, 1997).

Including all such additional variables,one arrives at a broader definition of person-ality: all of the relatively stable attributes,qualities, or characteristics that distinguish thebehavior, thoughts, and feelings of individu-als. Such a broad definition is close to thatproposed by Roback (1931): ‘an integrativecombination of all our cognitive (knowledge),affective (feeling), conative (volitional) andeven physical qualities’ (1931: 31–32).

Such broad definitions are not unusual.However, since Allport and Odbert (1936),personality has often been defined broadly

30 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 30

readers
AU: Prince (1924): not in ref list. Please give details.
readers
(e.g. Prince, 1924), ‘personality is the
readers
AU: Super (1995): not in ref list. Please give details.
readers
Super (1995) related to values,
Page 5: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

but operationalized narrowly, so that manyclasses of relevant variables are excluded.These narrow variable selections have beenachieved by either (a) starting with a fullrange of attributes of persons and then purg-ing those judged to fall in categories consid-ered unsuitable using exclusion rules (e.g.Allport and Odbert, 1936; Ashton et al.,2004; Goldberg, 1990; Norman, 1967), or (b) relying on vaguely defined ‘personalityrelevance’ ratings of judges. In effect, a nar-rower definition is being used, that personal-ity is patterns of behavior (including stableaffective tendencies but generally not pat-terns of thinking) that are believed to residewithin the individual and that cannot be dis-qualified as attitudes, temporary states, socialeffects, or social roles, or because they areoverly evaluative. Such definition-by-exclusionmakes personality into a remarkably gerry-mandered construct.

Previous research indicates that the structureof personality attributes encoded in lexiconsdepends in major ways upon the upstreamselection of variables. This is unsurprising. Ifastronomers forbade themselves from inves-tigating regions of the sky beyond that narrowband of the firmament where the most obvi-ous objects of interest (the sun, the moon, theplanets) move across the sky, astronomy’sconclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that weignore important phenomena and miss majordiscoveries, we need a wider view. We shouldcouple our focus on the most prototypicalattributes of personality with a simultaneous‘bigger picture’ examination of a wider rangeof psychological attributes.

PARSIMONY IN PERSONALITYMODELS

How many important traits are there? Survey-ing the scales in current personality invento-ries, one finds a bewildering variety ofconstructs. And if one turns to single wordsin modern world languages, the situation

becomes overwhelming: Allport and Odbert(1936), for example, found nearly 18,000words in Webster’s Second InternationalDictionary referring to characteristics thatmight distinguish one human being fromanother. One needs a parsimonious summaryof this vast domain of concepts.

In the field of personality the search for ascientifically compelling classification of thehuge number of personality attributes excitesincreasing interest. A classification systemat-ically divides phenomena into ordered groupsor categories; it ‘chunks’ things. A scientificclassification helps organize and integrateknowledge and research findings, providinga standard scientific nomenclature that facil-itates communication and aids in the accu-mulation of empirical findings. Becausepersonality attributes describe continua andnot categories, such a classification will nat-urally be a ‘dimensional classification’ – morelike those used for classifying colors thanlike those for classifying species.

In constructing a classification a variety ofprocedures could be used to group the phe-nomena under study. The most useful is a classof statistical methods generically referred toas factor analysis. Factor analysis can be con-sidered a variable-reduction procedure, inwhich many variables are organized by a fewfactors that summarize the interrelations amongthe variables (Goldberg and Velicer, 2006).

THE BASIS FOR THE LEXICALAPPROACH

However, prior to conducting factor analysis,one must determine which variables toinclude in the analysis. Variable selection isvitally dependent on how personality isdefined. It is also guided to some degree bythe investigator’s beliefs about the criteria for the goodness of a structural model (seeSaucier and Simonds, 2006, for a listing ofsuch criteria).

As has long been recognized (e.g. Allportand Odbert, 1936; Cattell, 1943; Goldberg,

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 31

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 31

Page 6: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

1981; Norman, 1963), basic personalitydimensions might be discovered by studyingconceptions embedded in the natural lan-guage. The key premise of the lexicalapproach is this: The degree of representa-tion of an attribute in language has some cor-respondence with the general importance ofthe attribute in real-world transactions. Ifterms in a language are used as variables, anattribute that is represented by multiple termsin that language will likely appear as a factor.Moreover, if the factor includes terms thatare used with high frequency, the importanceof the factor is underscored.

Such factors are but a starting point forseveral reasons. The lexicon could omit orunderemphasize some scientifically impor-tant variables. Moreover, the meaning ofsingle natural-language terms can be vague,ambiguous, or context-dependent (John etal., 1988). Folk concepts of personality(Tellegen, 1993) provide basic but not exhaus-tive (necessary but not sufficient) componentsfor a science of personality attributes (Goldbergand Saucier, 1995). These components operateon the descriptive or phenotypic level, with-out implication as to what might be theunderlying biological or other causal basis.An established causal basis is an important cri-terion for the goodness of a structural model(Saucier and Simonds, 2006). Ultimately, astructural model of personality ought to alignthe descriptive level with the causal level,and there may turn out not to be perfecthomology between the two levels.

Nonetheless, lexicalized concepts – espe-cially those represented in very frequentlyused words – tend to have high social impor-tance. So variables and factors based on lex-icalized concepts have a virtual guarantee ofbeing important. Lexicalized concepts can befound in standard sources created by disinter-ested parties (e.g. linguists and lexicogra-phers), and basing variable selection on sucha source reduces the likelihood of investiga-tor bias in the selection process. And becauselexicalized concepts constitute a finite domain,one can sample them representatively and so establish content-validity benchmarks for

personality variables. For drawing conclu-sions regarding personality structure, theseconcepts thereby have a major advantageover statements and sentences: Drawing onthe generative capacity of a human language,a nearly infinite number of personality-descriptive sentences might be formed,meaning that establishing that any selectionof statements and sentences is representativewould be quite difficult.

The lexical-study paradigm gives specialimportance to one other demanding criterion.Cross-cultural generalizability can be used tojudge among competitor structures. Structuralmodels derived within one limited popula-tion, or a limited sample from that population,are prone to reflect the unique patterns foundwithin that population or sample. Althoughculture-specific patterns are certainly inter-esting, models that transfer well – acrosspopulations, languages, and socioculturalsettings – better satisfy scientific standards ofreplicability and generalizability.

We can apply this criterion in either alenient or a stringent way. The lenient way isto export a set of variables (e.g. those in asingle personality inventory) for use in otherpopulations, and then examine whether thesepreselected variables (after translation, ifnecessary) generate the same factor structurein each new language or culture (as inRolland et al., 1998; Rossier et al., 2005). Ifthe inventory’s scales generate similar factorsacross populations, one might argue (as inMcCrae and Costa, 1997) that the structure iswidely generalizable. A more stringent test isto identify the most salient and importantpersonality concepts within each linguistic/cul-tural context, derive an indigenous factorstructure from those variables, and then exam-ine how much this new structure corre-sponds to previously proposed structures. A structure that met this demanding test in anylanguage could be considered more truly ubiq-uitous and universal than a merely ‘translat-able’ structure.

The lexical approach involves such anindigenous research strategy. Analyses arecarried out separately within each language,

32 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 32

Page 7: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

using a representative set of native-languagedescriptors, rather than merely importingselections of variables from other languages(e.g. English).

The following review will detail the struc-tures that have emerged from lexical studiesof some 16 languages, and that appear mostreplicable. These structures involve alterna-tively one, two, three, five, six, and sevenfactors. In all cases, measures of lexicallyderived factors will be described in conjunc-tion with the structure.

What if we were constrained to only one factor?

Several lexical studies have reported evi-dence about factor solutions containing onlyone factor (Boies et al., 2001; Di Blas andForzi, 1999; Goldberg and Somer, 2000;Saucier, 1997, 2003b; Saucier et al., 2006).Findings have been quite consistent. Thesingle factor contrasts a heterogeneous mixof desirable attributes at one pole with a mixof undesirable attributes at the other pole.This unrotated factor can be labelled ‘evalu-ation’. A more specific interpretation, whichfits reported findings from lexical studies,would be ‘virtues’ versus ‘bad character’.

Evaluation is the first factor to emerge inthe cognitions of young children. Whereasolder children employ more differentiated traitconcepts, younger children typically rely onglobal, evaluative inference (Alvarez et al.,2001). One can refer also to a classic findingin cognitive psychology: In judgments aboutthe meanings of diverse objects in a widearray of cultural settings, a global evaluationfactor (good vs. bad) was found recurrentlyto be the first and largest factor (Osgood,1962; Osgood et al., 1975). Osgood hypoth-esized that the ubiquity of this evaluativefactor was related to basic evolutionary prin-ciples: Our forebears would not have sur-vived if they had not become adapted at a verybasic level to any signals of good versus badobjects or events – those to approach versusthose to avoid, those leading to pleasure versus

those leading to pain (e.g. ‘Can I eat it or willit eat me?’). This motivational dimension –what is liked and approached, as opposed towhat is disliked and avoided – provides onepossible theoretical account for the one-factor model. There is no widely used meas-ure of this ‘Big One’ factor. Indeed, the factorhas had relatively little attention in personal-ity studies. This contrasts strikingly with thesituation in the field of cognitive abilitieswhere a one-factor taxonomy has long beendominant (Carroll, 1993).

For measuring a general evaluation factor,several research measures are available.Saucier (1994b) developed an adjectivalmarker scale for the single ‘general evalua-tion’ (Ge) factor. This scale was intended to berelatively orthogonal to four non-evaluativedimensions derived in the same study. Thecontent at the favorable pole was character-ized as largely a combination of likeability,good judgment, and perceived maturity.Constituent terms and psychometric indicesare provided in Table 1.1, both for the longer24-adjective scale (Ge-24) and a briefer 12-item subset (Ge-12). An alternative meas-ure was developed specifically to minimizecorrelations with the octant scales for theNon-Evaluative Personality Circumplex(NEPC) (Saucier et al., 2001; described laterunder three-factor models). Terms andindices for this scale (NEPC-E) are also pre-sented in the table. As part of a study of thestructure of English type-nouns, Saucier(2003b) used an economical ten-adjectivemarker scale for the one broad factor(derived from the Big One factor in the lexi-cal study of Saucier, 1997) labeled ‘sociallydesirable qualities’. As another alternative,one could employ terms from the bipolarscales recommended by Osgood et al., 1975,table 4:18), among which good–bad, pleas-ant–unpleasant, nice–awful, and beautiful–uglyproved the most ubiquitously useful across awide range of cultural settings. Table 1.1 pres-ents such a set. A characteristic of Osgood’sitems is that they can be used to describeinanimate objects as well as animals orpeople, because they use terms (e.g. pleasant,

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 33

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 33

Page 8: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

34 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 34

readers
ED: Please provide the table.
Page 9: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

beautiful) without strong and specific moral/ethical connotations.

Several psychometric indices are includedin Table 1.1 and succeeding tables. Two ref-erence internal consistency: coefficient alphaand the mean inter-item correlation. Two are relevant to unidimensionality: (a) thestandard deviation of the inter-item correla-tions, which decreases as unidimensionalityincreases, and (b) the ratio of variance betweenthe first and second unrotated factors fromthe scale items, which becomes more lop-sided as unidimensionality increases. Finally,the table includes the scale mean, wherescores are the average response on a 1-to-7multipoint rating scale, as well as the scalestandard deviation and the skewness statistic(where values less than −1 or greater than +1indicate extreme negative or positive skew,respectively). Comparing the five alternativemarker scales with respect to these indices, it appears that the Ge-24 and Ge-12 scalesare superior, as they combine strong internalconsistency and unidimensionality withsomewhat less skewness than the other measures.

If dimensions of psychopathology are con-strained to be only one, that dimension wouldrepresent general maladjustment. Generalmaladjustment is probably strongly related tothe evaluation factor in personality. One dif-ference is that studies of psyhopathologyunderstandably pay little attention to favor-able qualities. Abnormal psychology tends tocontrast varieties of dysfunction with themere absence of dysfunction (i.e. normality).

The big two

Two-factor solutions from lexical studiesalso suggest a consistent pattern: One factorincludes attributes associated with positivelyvalued dynamic qualities and individual ascen-dancy, whereas the other factor includes attrib-utes associated with socialization, socialpropriety, solidarity, and community cohesion(Caprara et al., 1997; Di Blas and Forzi, 1999;Digman, 1997; Goldberg and Somer, 2000;

Hrebíckov· et al., 1999; Paulhus and John,1998; Saucier, 1997, 2003b; Saucier et al.,2005, 2006; Shweder, 1972; White, 1980).These two factors may be aligned with someof the other sets of dual personological con-structs reviewed by Digman (1997) and byPaulhus and John (1998), including Hogan’s(1983) distinction between ‘getting ahead’(dynamism) and ‘getting along’ (social propri-ety). They seem also to resemble higher-order factors of the Big Five (DeYoung,2006; Digman, 1997).

To date, this two-factor structure appearsto be as ubiquitous across languages and cul-tures as the one-factor structure. Moreover,like the one-factor structure and unlike struc-tures described later, it appears to be relativelyimpervious to variable-selection effects.These two factors seem to appear whetherthere is a relatively restricted or inclusiveselection of variables (Saucier, 1997), andwhether one studies adjectives or type-nouns(Saucier, 2003b) or even more diverse com-binations of variable types (De Raad andBarelds, 2006; Saucier et al., 2006). Not yetknown is the extent to which the two-factorswill be robust across even broader selectionsof variables (e.g. those that also include vari-ables representing beliefs, attitudes, values,and interests). If both the one- and two-factorstructures eventually turn out to be universal,the latter has a clear advantage, because twofactors provide more information than one.

No consensual theory is as yet associatedwith the Big Two, but Paulhus and John(1998) reviewed a number of theories associ-ated with two-factor structures of personality.De Young has specifically proposed that thetwo higher-order factors, which he labels‘stability’ and ‘plasticity’, are related respec-tively to individual differences in serotoninand in dopamine functioning. These two fac-tors might alternatively stem from the opera-tion of basic human motivations that operatein the observer: ‘social propriety’ might ref-erence the degree to which an observed personis safe versus dangerous or hazardous (i.e. pun-ishing) for others, whereas ‘dynamism’ mightreference the degree to which an observed

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 35

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 35

Page 10: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

person is stimulating versus boring (i.e. reward-ing) for others. Studies are needed to evaluatethese hypotheses.

There are as yet no widely used standardmeasures of the Big Two. Measures of theinterpersonal circumplex (e.g. Wiggins et al.,1988) will not serve, because its two dimen-sions are too narrow – omitting contribu-tions, for example, of openness/intellect,conscientiousness, and emotional stability.The same is true of Eysenck’s older ‘BigTwo’ – extraversion and neuroticism – whichobviously leave out contributions of a differ-ent combination of three Big Five factors.

As markers of the Big Two in lexical stud-ies in newly studied languages, Saucier hasused a relatively brief collection of Englishadjectives derived from the two-factor struc-ture in an English lexical analysis (Saucier,1997). Constituent terms and psychometricindices for these ‘initial approximation’scales are provided in Table 1.1.

There may be strong homology betweenstructures in the domains of personality andpsychopathology at the two-factor level. A favored two-dimensional model for psy-chopathology separates externalizing andinternalizing disorders, conceived as two cor-related factors (e.g. Krueger and Markon,2006). A reasonable hypothesis is that exter-nalizing disorders represent low social pro-priety (morality) whereas internalizingdisorders have a stronger relation to lowdynamism. More studies are needed to estab-lish homologies between domains at the two-factor level. Just as the single evaluativefactor is a higher-order combination of thefavorable poles of the Big Two, the singlepsychopathology factor (i.e. maladjustment)is a higher-order combination of the external-izing and internalizing dimensions.

Personality descriptors in three-dimensional space

In three-factor solutions, studies of most lan-guages of European origin (plus those inTurkish, Korean, and Chinese) have produced

factors corresponding to extraversion, agree-ableness, and conscientiousness. This structurewas not observed in Filipino, French, Greek,or Maasai studies. Still, this three-factor struc-ture does appear readily in a large subset oflanguages, and in more languages than theBig Five (De Raad and Peabody, 2005).

Peabody (1987; Peabody and Goldberg,1989) demonstrated that the unrotated-factorversions of this Big Three can be labeled asevaluation, assertive versus unassertive (oraggressive vs. accommodating), and tightversus loose (or impulse control vs. impulseexpression). The first two of these are themost ubiquitous, as they rotate into the socialpropriety (morality) and dynamism factorsthat make up the Big Two. The Big Threedoes not replicate in all lexical studies simplybecause a tight–loose factor does not neces-sarily appear third, but rather sometimesfourth or later, in the sequence of unrotatedfactors.

For the rotated versions of these threedimensions, scales for the first three of the BigFive – that is, for extraversion, agreeable-ness, and conscientiousness (see Table 1.3) –will function reasonably well. But the unro-tated versions are also of interest, becausethey concentrate social-desirability respond-ing in only one of the factors (i.e. evaluation)and thus remove it from the other two. Thiswas demonstrated by Saucier et al. (2001),who likewise showed that these unrotatedfactors are quite similar in English and inGerman. This set of factors includes oneevaluative factor and two non-evaluative fac-tors, and the latter were presented as a non-evaluative circumplex (cf. Di Blas et al.,2000). Saucier et al. (2001) provided psycho-metric indices for the octant scales taken sep-arately. These scales produce unusuallysymmetric (non-skewed) distributions buttend to be multidimensional and only moder-ately homogeneous.

An additional three-factor model is theaffective-meaning dimensions of Osgoodand colleagues, which have a quasi-lexicalbasis, being drawn from ratings of a widevariety of objects and entities. The most

36 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 36

Page 11: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

ubiquitous bipolar-scale markers for activityand potency across cultures (Osgood et al.,1975, table 4:18) appear to be strong–weak,big–little/small, and heavy–light (for potency),and fast–slow, young–old, active–passive, andalive–dead (for activity). Although the threeOsgood dimensions are known to apply wellacross a very broad range of target entities,activity and potency have not provided a par-ticularly good account of lexical factors.

Another three-factor alternative is evidentin the convergence between models ofEysenck (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1975), ofTellegen and colleagues (Tellegen, in press;Clark and Watson, 1999), and of Rothbart(Rothbart and Bates, 1998), all of whichshare an emphasis on affect and on biologicalbases of temperament. One factor is extraver-sion, approach, or positive emotionality. A second is neuroticism, negative affectivity,or negative emotionality. A third is psychoti-cism (which might be better labeled as somecombination of psychopathy and impulsivesensation seeking), constraint (labeled by theopposite pole), or effortful control (labeledby the opposite pole). Although this model isprominent in contemporary psychology, it isyet to be reported from a lexical study, per-haps because it tends to omit content fromagreeableness, a large and prominent con-stituent of the personality lexicon.

Saucier (1997) found that, for Englishadjectives, this structure was as robust acrossvariable selections as were the one- and two-factor structures described previously.However, that remains the only demonstra-tion of this sort. Saucier’s (2003b) study ofthe structure of English type-nouns failed toconfirm this three-factor structure, althoughit did confirm the Big One and Big Two. Andthe three factors did not appear in two recentlexical studies with more inclusive selectionsof variables (Saucier et al., 2005; Saucier etal., 2006). A conclusion is that they are notvery robust across variable selections.

The same variable-selection caveat per-tains to the next two structural models to be discussed. In the case of the Big Five and the Cross-Language Six, the structure

seems to be dependent on a narrow way ofoperationalizing personality (using exclusionrules). And all of these models may bedependent on the use of adjectives, to theexclusion of other word-forms. In order toincrease our understanding of the contingen-cies between variable selection and obtainedstructure, all lexical studies should ideallycompare results from a conventional, narrowvariable selection with that from a moreinclusive selection of variables (as in Saucier,1997; Goldberg and Somer, 2000; Saucier et al., 2006).

At this point the reader may be interestedin how the one-, two-, and three-factor levelsare related. Table 1.2 provides the correla-tions among all of the adjective marker scalesdescribed in this chapter; some of the highercorrelations are affected by item overlapbetween marker sets at different levels. The general evaluation factor, regardless ofthe scale for it, is related to both S (socialpropriety) and D (dynamism) but more to Sthan to D.

Regularities at the five-factor level

The Big Five factors are extraversion, agree-ableness, conscientiousness, emotional sta-bility, and intellect/imagination. Lexicalstudies in Germanic and Slavic languages(including English) have been supportive ofthe Big Five, and so has a study in Turkish(Goldberg and Somer, 2000). But studies inItalian (De Raad et al., 1997) and Hungarian(Szirmak and De Raad, 1994) found nocounterpart to the intellect factor in five-factor solutions. Extraction of additional fac-tors was necessary to find a factor related tointellect. In a study of modern Greek(Saucier et al., 2005), there was no intellector imagination factor (intellect terms weremore associated with a factor emphasizingcourage and self-confidence).

Several lexical studies have included a rel-atively broad selection of variables, eachincluding many terms that could be classifiedas referring to emotions and moods or as

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 37

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 37

readers
AU: De Raad et al (1997): ref list gives (1998). Which is correct?
readers
De Raad, 1994) the intellect factor
Page 12: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

38 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

Tabl

e 1.

2In

terc

orre

lati

ons

Am

ong

Adj

ecti

ve M

arke

r Sc

ales

For

Str

uctu

res

of O

ne t

o Se

ven

Fact

ors

(N=

533,

deci

mal

poi

nts

omit

ted)

1.G

e-24

2.G

e-12

953.

NEP

C-E

6260

4.SD

Q55

5452

5.O

sgoo

d E

5051

4568

6.S

4949

4775

587.

D36

3340

4341

048.

TL05

03−0

405

−07

00−2

29.

TALU

−04

−08

−06

−14

−20

−35

1043

10.A

U−0

1−0

3−0

3−0

8−0

7−4

543

1158

11.A

LUT

0404

0804

04−2

157

−50

0340

12.B

5MM

-I27

2531

3330

0084

−13

0944

5713

.B5M

M-II

5253

5171

6575

27−2

5−4

6−2

904

2114

.B5M

M-II

I39

3531

5840

3620

4811

10−1

917

3115

.B5M

M-IV

4644

4850

3852

1911

06−1

200

1937

2816

.B5M

M-V

2523

3927

1907

41−1

114

2220

2016

1016

17.C

L6-H

5758

4159

4965

1009

−26

−29

−16

0861

4037

0318

.CL6

-E−3

1−2

8−2

7−2

7−1

7−0

5−3

6−2

6−5

7−3

2−1

2−3

206

−27

−43

−25

−03

19.C

L6-X

3634

3745

4114

81−1

8−0

234

5592

3624

2517

22−2

720

.CL6

-A41

4344

6549

8102

−09

−36

−51

−15

−06

6521

5509

49−0

807

21.C

L6-C

4541

3255

4040

1550

0805

−26

1434

8423

0746

−20

2023

22.C

L6-O

2926

4027

2305

51−1

617

2632

2916

0920

8702

−31

2706

0423

.ML7

-gr

1716

1823

2201

68−3

3−1

528

6279

24−0

204

0505

−03

81−0

4−0

513

24.M

L7-s

a48

4741

4537

1853

0538

3426

4520

3347

3223

−−775

4618

3041

2025

.ML7

-et

3938

4454

3970

0702

−20

−36

−05

0948

1971

0637

−21

1679

1706

0423

26.M

L7-c

fo41

4038

5042

6206

−16

−47

−42

−06

0269

2822

−02

6620

1751

35−0

309

0632

27.M

L7-c

o24

2119

3726

1911

5209

10−2

711

1875

1002

30−0

915

0675

−04

−03

2003

2228

.ML7

-ov

3028

4426

2104

45−0

718

2719

2214

1216

8402

−29

1909

1084

0435

06−0

505

29.M

L7-n

v34

3624

5853

5610

24−0

6−1

1−2

113

4345

3500

48−1

622

3649

−03

0424

3129

34−0

41

23

45

67

89

1011

1213

1415

1617

1819

2021

2223

2425

2627

28

Not

e.Eu

gene

-Spr

ingf

ield

Com

mun

ity S

ampl

e,N

=53

3.De

cim

al p

oint

s om

itted

.Bet

wee

n-se

t cor

rela

tions

abo

ve.6

0 in

mag

nitu

de a

re p

rinte

d in

bol

dfac

e ty

pe.A

ll co

rrel

atio

ns w

ithin

set

(the

sam

e le

vel,

stru

ctur

e;an

d nu

mbe

r of f

acto

rs) a

re in

ital

ics.

For B

ig F

ive,

corr

elat

ions

are

bas

ed o

n ES

CS-1

995.

Som

e sc

ales

from

diff

eren

t set

s ha

ve o

verla

ppin

g ite

ms,

whi

ch m

ay in

flate

the

corr

elat

ions

am

ong

the

mor

e cl

osel

y re

late

d sc

ales

.*T

raits

may

be

mat

ched

aga

inst

jobs

,the

rapi

es tr

aits

may

be

mat

ched

aga

inst

jobs

,the

rapi

es

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 38

Page 13: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

being unusually highly evaluative, and two ofthese studies (Goldberg and Somer, 2000;Saucier, 1997) included terms referring tophysical appearance. In these studies, therehas been no difficulty in replicating the one-and two-factor structures reviewed earlier.But none of these analyses has found the BigFive in a five-factor solution.

Because of the long history of Big Fivemodels and because of its long salience inlexical studies, numerous measures of thelexical Big Five have been constructed.Saucier and Goldberg (2002) provide adetailed account of the some major adjectivalBig Five marker scales in English. A shortersummary is provided here.

Goldberg (1990) originally experimentedwith bipolar and cluster scoring methods formeasuring the Big Five as found in adjec-tives. Then he settled on a standard set of 100‘unipolar’ adjectives, 20 for each factor(Goldberg, 1992). Although this marker sethas been widely used, it is now judged overlylong for many purposes. This influentialmarker set became the starting point forreduced-length marker scale sets. The firstwas the Mini-Markers (Saucier, 1994a),which included only a 40-item subset of the100, those most univocally loading on eachof the five factors; there are indications thatvalidity is comparable with that for thelonger marker set (Dwight et al., 1998). Analternative subset is the Ortho-40 (Saucier,2002), differing from the Mini-Markers inhaving lower interscale correlations. Anotherproblem with the 100 unipolar adjectives,and to some degree these reduced-lengthdescendents, was the use of many negations(un- terms) (Graziano et al., 1998). Byincluding some adjectives not contained inthe 100 unipolar set, Saucier (2002) devisedan alternative 40-adjective set (the 3M40)that had fewer negations while retaininginterscale correlations as low as those fromthe Ortho-40.

Constituent terms and psychometricindices are provided in Table 1.3 for theMini-Markers for peer-ratings as well as self-ratings. The 569 peer-ratings are averaged

ratings from three well-acquainted peersnominated by each of the 569 persons whoprovided self-ratings, who were described bythe three peers. The scales scored from peerratings sometimes have higher internal con-sistency – specifically for agreeableness,conscientiousness, and emotional stability(the Big Five factors most highly associatedwith the broader social propriety/moralityfactor). When aggregated, peer ratings havethe potential for psychometric propertiessuperior to what self-ratings can provide(Hofstee, 1994). Correlations between selfand aggregated peer ratings were 0.66, 0.45,0.50, 0.41, and 0.49, respectively, for extra-version, agreeableness, conscientiousness,emotional stability, and intellect/imagination.

Big Five scales are also available from theitems of the International Personality ItemPool (Goldberg, 1999). Goldberg used the100 markers as orienting points for selectingitems for 20-item scales (in the IPIP-100)and 10-item scales (in the IPIP-50), with aneye to maximizing internal consistency whilebalancing the number of forward- andreverse-keyed items (Saucier and Goldberg,2002). Donnellan et al. (2006) recentlydeveloped a ‘mini-IPIP’ questionnaire byshortening the IPIP-50 to only 20 items.These IPIP scales can be expected to meas-ure factors similar to the lexical ones cap-tured by the 100 markers; however, they areone step removed from the lexical studies(Goldberg, 1990, 1992) that led to the 100markers, and they do not share method variance with adjective scales. Thus, they are not lexical-factor measures by a strict criterion.

The same can be said for the NEOPersonality Inventory (Costa and McCrae,1985, 1992b), described in another chapter inthis volume, as well as its short form, theNEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). It isworth noting, however, that the developmentof the agreeableness and conscientiousnessdomain scales for the NEO measures wasstrongly influenced by earlier lexical meas-ures of the corresponding Big Five factors(McCrae and Costa, 1985).

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 39

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 39

Page 14: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

40 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

Tabl

e 1.

3Ps

ycho

met

ric

Indi

ces

for

Mar

ker

Scal

es f

or t

he B

ig F

ive

No.

of

Coef

ficie

nt

% o

f Var

ianc

eM

arke

r sca

leite

ms

Sam

ple

rete

st r

2-ye

arAl

pha

rM

ean

rSD

of

r1st

– 2nd

Fact

or

Mea

nSD

Skew

Min

i-m

arke

rs (

self

-rat

ings

)I -

Extr

aver

sion

8ES

CS-1

993

0.83

0.83

0.38

0.13

47 –

13

4.10

0.56

−0.0

2II-

Agr

eeab

lene

ss8

ESCS

-199

30.

700.

790.

340.

0842

– 1

25.

740.

65−0

.91

III-C

onsc

ient

ious

ness

8ES

CS-1

993

0.78

0.83

0.38

0.11

46 –

12

5.30

0.84

−0.7

0IV

-Em

otio

nal s

tabi

lity

8ES

CS-1

993

0.73

0.76

0.28

0.12

39 –

14

4.59

0.88

−0.1

4V-

Inte

llect

/imag

inat

ion

8ES

CS-1

993

0.77

0.79

0.33

0.13

42 –

17

5.17

0.82

−0.3

8M

ini-

mar

kers

(se

lf-r

atin

gs)

I -Ex

trav

ersi

on8

ESCS

-199

8b0.

860.

420.

1350

– 1

23.

470.

80−0

.36

II- A

gree

able

ness

8ES

CS-1

998b

0.81

0.36

0.11

45 –

13

4.30

0.53

−0.7

6III

-Con

scie

ntio

usne

ss8

ESCS

-199

8b0.

860.

430.

1451

– 1

14.

080.

66−0

.86

IV-E

mot

iona

l sta

bilit

y8

ESCS

-199

8b0.

800.

330.

1342

– 1

63.

670.

70−0

.38

V-In

telle

ct/im

agin

atio

n8

ESCS

-199

8b0.

830.

380.

1446

– 1

83.

850.

64−0

.49

Min

i-m

arke

rs (

peer

-rat

ings

)I -

Extr

aver

sion

8ES

CS-1

998b

0.86

0.43

0.13

51 –

12

3.70

0.66

−0.5

4II-

Agr

eeab

lene

ss8

ESCS

-199

8b0.

870.

460.

0853

– 1

24.

280.

50−0

.95

III-C

onsc

ient

ious

ness

8ES

CS-1

998b

0.89

0.49

0.13

57 –

10

4.12

0.61

−0.9

6IV

-Em

otio

nal s

tabi

lity

8ES

CS-1

998b

0.84

0.40

0.15

48 –

17

3.57

0.63

−0.2

6V-

Inte

llect

/imag

inat

ion

8ES

CS-1

998b

0.81

0.36

0.17

44 –

20

3.98

0.49

−0.4

3

Not

eES

CS –

Eug

ene-

Sprin

gfie

ld C

omm

unity

Sam

ple

(199

3,N

=11

25;1

998b

and

199

8b s

ampl

es,N

=56

9).E

SCS-

1998

b us

ed a

1-5

ratin

g sc

ale

rath

er th

an 1

–7.%

of v

aria

nce

figur

es b

ased

on

a pr

inci

pal-a

xes

anal

ysis

of a

ll ite

ms

in th

e sc

ale.

See

sour

ce a

rtic

les

(Sau

cier

(199

4a,2

002)

or a

utho

r’s w

eb-p

ages

for l

ist o

f Min

i-mar

ker i

tem

s an

d fo

r ind

ices

bas

ed o

n ot

her s

ampl

es.S

ome

com

para

ble

indi

ces

for t

he 1

00 m

arke

rs,O

rtho

-40,

and

3M40

are

ava

ilabl

e in

pre

viou

s pu

blic

atio

ns (S

auci

er,2

002;

Sauc

ier a

nd G

oldb

erg,

2002

)

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 40

Page 15: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

Two other lexically influenced question-naires deserve mention. One is the Big FiveInventory (BFI) (Benet-Martinez and John,1998). This measure has 44 short phraseitems. The content and positions for the fivefactors on this instrument were clearly influ-enced by both the Big Five adjective scalesand by the NEO inventory. For example, inBig Five measures one factor is intellect orimagination, whereas the correspondingNEO domain is labeled openness to experi-ence. In the BFI, the corresponding scale haselements of all three kinds of content, and sooverall represents a sort of compromise.Hendriks and her colleagues developed theFive Factor Personality Inventory (FFPI)(Hendriks et al., 1999), a 100-item Big Fiveinventory using an IPIP-style item formatthat has been translated and used in numer-ous languages (Hendriks et al., 2003). TheFFPI was constructed based in large part onresults of Dutch lexical studies, especiallythe innovative study of Hofstee and De Raad(1991). The BFI and FFPI are useful Big Fivemeasures, although not lexical-factor meas-ures by a strict criterion.

Even shorter measures of the Big Fivehave begun to appear. Gosling et al. (2003)developed a ten-item Big Five measure thatshowed adequate retest reliability and ade-quate convergence both with other Big Fivemeasures and between self and observer rat-ings. Major sources for the items wereGoldberg’s marker sets and the BFI.

As these examples of Big Five measuresillustrate, measures of lexical personalityfactors tend to be provisional and are usedprimarily in research, but they have also pro-vided a useful template for the developmentof more sophisticated assessment instruments.

Lexical six-factor models

Ashton et al. (2004) have presented evidencethat many of the lexical studies conducted todate yield a consistent pattern in six-factor solu-tions. Although the structure was first detectedin studies of Korean (Hahn et al., 1999) and

French (Boies et al., 2001), it has also appearedto a recognizable degree in Dutch, German,Hungarian, Italian, and Polish. This structureseems less bound to the Germanic and Slaviclanguage families than the Big Five.

Empirically, the extraversion, conscien-tiousness, and intellect factors in this six-factor model differ relatively little fromcorresponding factors in the Big Five.Emotionality is more related to (low) emo-tional stability than to any other Big Fivefactor. The other two factors emerge largelyout of the interstitial areas between Big Fivefactors: agreeableness from big five agree-ableness and emotional stability, and hon-esty/humility from big five agreeablenessand conscientiousness. However, as Table 1.2indicates, emotionality and honesty in partic-ular tend to have relations to more than twoBig Five factors.

Evidence to date indicates that the replic-ability of the six-factor structure across lan-guages probably exceeds that for the BigFive. Moreover, this ‘Cross-Language Six’might be considered superior because it pro-vides more information than the Big Five.In the first reported ‘horse races’ betweenthe models, replication comparisons in lexi-cal study of modern Greek (Saucier et al.,2005) and of the language of the Maasai(Saucier et al., 2006), the six-factor modelseemed about equally as replicable as the BigFive. In neither study, however, were five-or six-factor models nearly as well repli-cated as were one- and two-factor models.

Other measures focused on in this chapterhave included adjectives as items, and onemight employ adjective markers to indexthese six factors. The best approach would beto utilize as many as possible of the adjec-tives that Ashton et al. present in their ‘sum-mary of the six-factor solutions’ (2004: 363)in various languages. Table 1.4 presents theconstituent terms and psychometric indicesfor a set of marker scales so constructed;large subsets of these terms (in translation)have been used as marker scales for theCross-Language Six in two previous lexicalstudies (Saucier et al., 2005, 2006).

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 41

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 41

Page 16: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

42 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

Tabl

e 1.

4Ps

ycho

met

ric

Indi

ces

for

Adj

ecti

ve M

arke

r Sc

ales

for

the

Cro

ss-L

angu

age

Six

rwith

HEX

ACO

-PI

Mar

ker S

cale

No.

of

Coef

f. %

of V

ar.

Item

sSa

mpl

eAl

pha

Mea

n r

SD o

f r1st

– 2nd

Fac.

Mea

n SD

Sk

ewH

EX

AC

OHo

nest

y/Hu

mili

ty (H

)10

ESCS

0.71

0.24

0.12

33 –

13

5.94

0.58

−0

.69

0.40

0.10

0.02

0.25

0.22

−0.0

8Em

otio

nalit

y (E

)11

ESCS

0.65

0.14

0.15

23 –

18

3.80

0.67

0.17

0.05

0.57

−0.1

7−0

.02

−0.1

5−0

.13

Extr

aver

sion

(X)

8ES

CS0.

750.

280.

1137

– 1

54.

890.

89−0

.21

−0.0

10.

080.

700.

000.

120.

08Ag

reea

blen

ess

(A)

9ES

CS0.

760.

290.

1138

– 1

25.

250.

79−0

.54

0.27

0.02

−0

.08

0.59

0.04

0.02

Cons

cien

tious

ness

(C)

9ES

CS0.

760.

260.

1135

– 1

35.

700.

72−0

.96

0.13

0.00

0.06

−0.0

10.

69−0

.16

Ope

nnes

s (O

)11

ESCS

0.76

0.23

0.14

31 –

13

5.18

0.70

−0.4

8−0

.15

−0.1

60.

33−0

.02

0.03

0.60

Not

eES

CS –

Eug

ene–

Sprin

gfie

ld c

omm

unity

sam

ple,

N=

533,

exce

pt N

=51

9 fo

r cor

rela

tions

with

HEX

ACO

-PI.

% o

f var

ianc

e fig

ures

bas

ed o

n a

prin

cipa

l-axe

s an

alys

is o

f all

item

s in

the

scal

e.Ad

ject

ives

sel

ecte

d as

mar

kers

for C

ross

-Lan

guag

e Si

x ar

e dr

awn

from

thos

e ci

ted

as m

ost r

ecur

rent

acr

oss

lang

uage

s by

Ash

ton

et a

l.(2

004)

,and

are

as

follo

ws:

(hon

esty

/hum

ility

)ho

nest

,sin

cere

,fai

r,lo

yal,

mod

est,

vs.d

ecei

tful,

hypo

criti

cal,

conc

eite

d,sl

y,gr

eedy

;(em

otio

nalit

y) a

nxio

us,f

earfu

l,vu

lner

able

,em

otio

nal,

sens

itive

,sen

timen

tal v

s.st

rong

,cou

rage

ous,

inde

-pe

nden

t,to

ugh,

inde

pend

ent,

self-

assu

red;

(ext

rave

rsio

n) ta

lkat

ive,

soci

able

,che

erfu

l,en

erge

tic v

s.qu

iet,

shy,

pass

ive,

with

draw

n;(a

gree

able

ness

) gen

tle,t

oler

ant,

peac

eful

,agr

eeab

le,g

ood-

natu

red

vs.i

rrita

ble,

argu

men

tativ

e,ag

gres

sive

,sho

rt-t

empe

red;

(con

scie

ntio

usne

ss) o

rder

ly,pr

ecis

e,ca

refu

l,se

lf-di

scip

lined

vs.

diso

rgan

ized

,laz

y,ne

glig

ent,

reck

less

,irr

espo

nsib

le;(

open

ness

)cr

eativ

e,in

telle

ctua

l,ph

iloso

phic

al,t

alen

ted,

educ

ated

,witt

y un

conv

entio

nal v

s.un

crea

tive,

unin

telle

ctua

l,un

educ

ated

,con

vent

iona

l.Ps

ycho

met

ric in

dice

s fo

r the

HEX

ACO

Per

sona

lity

Inve

ntor

y (H

EXAC

O-P

I) ca

n be

obt

aine

d fro

m L

ee a

nd A

shto

n (2

004)

.

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 42

Page 17: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

However, the standard way to measurethese six factors is with a questionnairecalled the HEXACO Personality Inventory(HEXACO-PI) (Lee and Ashton, 2004). Inthis inventory, each of the six factors has foursubscales measuring facets of the six factors.Psychometric indices for the six higher-order scales are presented elsewhere (Leeand Ashton, 2004). Correlations betweenHEXACO scales and adjective markers forthe Cross-Language Six are also included in Table 1.4. The correlations indicate goodlexically based content validity for theHEXACO scales, with one exception: theconvergence of lexical and questionnairehonesty/humility (r = 0.40) is rather weak.This is probably due to the H scale’s use offairly specific contextualized items. Generally,the questionnaire scale appears less suffusedwith evaluation and agreeableness than thelexical version.

This six-factor model may be found onlyin the adjective domain. Saucier (2003b)found that the structure of type-nouns inEnglish yielded six factors very similar tothose found in studies of Dutch (De Raad andHoskens, 1990) and German (Henss, 1998).However, these six factors – liveliness,antagonism, malignancy/cowardice, mas-culinity, intellect/openness, and attractive-ness – as a set do not correspond closely tothe Cross-Language Six described here.

Seven-factor models found with awider inclusion of lexical variables

Analyses leading to the five- or six-factorstructure have involved, in effect, removal ofthe most extremely evaluative terms at anearly stage of the variable-selection process.Indeed, Allport (Allport and Odbert, 1936)and Norman (1963) both favored removal ofhighly evaluative terms. Also removed havebeen (a) terms indicating relative eccentricity(e.g. average, strange, unusual); (b) termsthat can refer to both stable and temporaryattributes (e.g. happy, tired, bored); (c) ten-dencies to affect others in a consistent way

(e.g. likeable, annoying); (d) social statusindicators (e.g. wealthy, famous); and (e)attributes of physique and health (e.g. tall,fat, sickly). When investigators have usedwider variable selections (i.e. those includingmany or all of these excluded types of vari-ables), the Big Five has not appeared readilyin five-factor solutions. Studies in Englishand Turkish, however, did find Big-Five-like factors within a seven-factor solution(Goldberg and Somer, 2000; Saucier, 1997;Tellegen and Waller, 1987).

Of the two additional factors, one wasfound in all three studies: ‘negative valence’is a factor emphasizing attributes withextremely low desirability and endorsementrates and with descriptive content involvingmorality/depravity, dangerousness, worth-lessness, peculiarity, and stupidity (cf. Benet-Martinez and Waller, 2002). Its overallthemes – extreme social impropriety, failinga threshold for social acceptability, and notbeing worthy of trust or credence – involvenon-normativeness: Does one stand outsideof social norms to a high enough degree thatone becomes liable for exclusion from thegroup? The other factor varied more acrossthe three studies and involved descriptorsindicating some kind of power to impressothers, either in the form of a ‘positive valence’factor emphasizing positive attributes (possi-bly social effects) like ‘impressive’ and ‘out-standing’ (found by Tellegen and Waller,1987) or, where attractiveness terms wereincluded, an ‘attractiveness’ factor (found byGoldberg and Somer, 2000; Saucier, 1997;also Saucier, 2003b). Whether attractivenessor negative valence fall within the domain ofpersonality can be debated, but both factorsinvolve phenomena of great interest to socialpsychologists.

The structure labeled the Big Seven wasestablished in an unpublished lexical study ofEnglish descriptors that used the method ofsampling one descriptors from one in everyfour pages of a dictionary (Tellegen and Waller,1987). The structure includes five Big-Five-like factors, except that ‘intellect/ imagina-tion’ is reconceived as ‘unconventionality’.

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 43

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 43

Page 18: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

The two additional factors are labeled ‘posi-tive valence’ and ‘negative valence’. Scrutinyof empirical indices of replication indicatethat two attempts to replicate this structure,in studies of Spanish (Benet-Martinez andWaller, 1997) and Hebrew (Almagor et al.,1995) had only modest success, althougheach of these studies found structures of inter-est in their own right. A standard lexicalmeasure of the Big Seven in its originalEnglish-study structural form is the Inventoryof Personal Characteristics (IPC-7) (Tellegenet al., 1991).

Studies in some other languages withbroad variable-selection criteria indicate analternative seven-factor structure. The con-vergences between these studies occurred inspite of their many differences in methodol-ogy. Lexical studies in Filipino (Church et al., 1997, 1998) and Hebrew (Almagor et al., 1995) – languages from unrelated language-families and cultures – yielded ahighly convergent seven-factor structure,even though this similarity was obscured bydiscrepant labels. The English translations ofmarker adjectives for the Filipino andHebrew factors have been shown to corre-spond in a one-to-one way (Saucier, 2003a).

One of these new factors resembles thenegative valence factor just described. Twoof them resemble Big Five factors – consci-entiousness and intellect. The other three BigFive factors – extraversion, agreeableness,and emotional stability – correlate substan-tially with the remaining four factors, whichmap an affective-interpersonal domain (cf.Saucier, 1992). These four can be labeled‘gregariousness’ (or ‘liveliness’), ‘self-assur-ance’ (or ‘fortitude’), ‘even temper’ (‘toler-ant’ versus ‘temperamental’), and ‘concernfor others’ (versus ‘egotism’). Big Five extra-version is related to gregariousness and self-assurance, emotional stability to self-assuranceand even temper, and agreeableness to eventemper and concern for others.

The relation of the Multi-Language Seven(ML7) to the Cross-Language Six (CL6) isbest explained with the help of Figure 1.1. Thisfigure shows the relations between lexical

structures of one, two, five, six, and sevenfactors. It joins any factors at adjacent levelsthat have a substantial correlation (more than0.35 in magnitude) in Table 1.2. The SDQscale was used for the one-factor level. Pairsof factors correlated the most highly (above0.70) are joined by a thick and bold line. Thefigure depicts very strong relations betweenCL6 extraversion and ML7 gregariousness,between CL6 agreeableness and ML7 eventemper, between the conscientiousness fac-tors, and between CL6 openness and ML7originality/virtuosity. ML7 self-assurance isrelated to both CL6 emotionality (reverse-scored) and extraversion, whereas ML7 concern for others and negative valence(reverse-scored) are both related both to CL6 agreeableness and honesty/humility,negative valence being related also to CL6conscientiousness.

It is noteworthy that negative valence issubstantially correlated with CL6 honesty(H), conscientiousness (C), and agreeable-ness (A) (−0.36 to −0.49 with each), anotherindicator that this factor contains descriptivecontent. Unlike extraversion, emotionality,and openness, these three factors (H, C, andA) concern moral and prosocial behavior,and are clearly related to the broad socialpropriety and morality factor (and not todynamism). The aspect of social proprietyand morality uniquely captured by so-callednegative valence is normality violation; thatis, the tendency to behave in ways that areawry, askew, and violative of normal stan-dards for behavior, by way of undependabil-ity, recklessness, abusiveness, incompetence,or sheer eccentricity. The favorable pole ofthis dimension is characterized by ‘vanilla’descriptors like normal and trustworthy(Saucier, 2003a). The unfavorable pole isparticularly richly represented (in English)by type-nouns, like creep, idiot, fool, twit,crook, and deadbeat, terms whose useimplies that the target is being singled out forsocial exclusion (Saucier, 2003b). The con-tempt implied in these descriptors may not beunusual when we encounter others who vio-late the standards of what we consider normal.

44 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 44

Page 19: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

Saucier (2003a) developed a 60-adjectivemarker set for the seven factors. Constituentterms and psychometric indices are providedin Table 1.5.

An integrative framework forstructures of one to seven lexicalfactors

How are the structures (and measures) ofone, two, three, five, six, and seven lexicalfactors related to one another? Some answersmight be found by examining the intercorre-lations of the scales measuring their factors(Table 1.2). This table leads to a clear pictureof the relation of the one- and two-factorstructures with each other and with the five-,six-, and seven-factor structures. The generalevaluation factor bifurcates into S and D.Social propriety divides into agreeableness,conscientiousness, and emotional stability(Big Five), or into honesty, agreeableness,and conscientiousness (Cross-LanguageSix), or into even temper, concern for others,conscientiousness, and (reversed) normalityviolation (ML7). Dynamism divides intoextraversion and intellect/imagination (BigFive), into extraversion, openness, and (low)emotionality (CL6), or into gregariousness,self-assurance, and originality/virtuosity(ML7). Thus, the Big Two is a sensiblehigher-order organization for each of thesethree structures. However, it is far more diffi-cult to give a simple description of how thefive-, six-, and seven-factor structures relateto each other. Indicating complexity, inFigure 2.1 the lines joining levels of five, six, and seven factors have several crossinglines.

A hierarchical structural representationcombining both broader and narrower con-structs will provide the best compromisebetween parsimony and accuracy. The broadlevels, with wider bandwidth constructs,offers higher efficiency (i.e. parsimony). Thenarrower levels offer higher fidelity (i.e. pre-dictive accuracy). Given the differences incovariation structure between languages, it

seems appropriate to defer such studies oflexically derived facets until a consensualhierarchical structure at the broad levels isbetter defined.

Belief, value, and attitude factors asadditions to the dimensionalclassification

As the above review indicates, the Big Fiveand Cross-Language Six are structureswhose appearance seems contingent on a rel-atively narrow selection of variables, andthus on an operational definition of personal-ity that has many exclusion clauses. TheMulti-Language Seven may be contingent ona more inclusive variable selection and defi-nition of personality. Structures with one ortwo broad factors seem less dependent on thevariable selection on the definition of person-ality. However, none of the previous lexicalstudies of personality has included a substan-tial representation of belief, value, and attitudevariables. Would including such variables leadto additional factors?

Unfortunately, the research literature onthe structure of beliefs, values, and attitudeshas been poorly developed. To rectify this sit-uation, Saucier (2000) used a lexical ration-ale, extracting from a large dictionary allEnglish nouns ending in ‘-ism,’ such termspostulated to represent many of the mostimportant beliefs and attitudes. From the def-initions of these terms, Saucier developed389 questionnaire items, which he adminis-tered to a large sample of college students,who indicated their extent of agreement witheach item. Analyses revealed four broaddimensions of beliefs and attitudes, whichwere replicated in a follow-up sample, andlater in Romanian (Krauss, 2006). The studyyielded important increments to knowledgeabout belief/attitude dispositions: Two of thefour factors are little represented in previousmeasures.

Saucier’s (2000) four ‘isms’ factors arelabeled as traditional religiousness (α), sub-jective spirituality (δ), unmitigated self-interest

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 45

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 45

Page 20: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

46 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

Tabl

e 1.

5Ps

ycho

met

ric

Indi

ces

for

Adj

ecti

ve M

arke

r Sc

ales

for

the

Mul

ti-L

angu

age

Seve

nCo

effic

ient

%

of V

aria

nce

Mar

ker S

cale

No.

of I

tem

s Sa

mpl

eAl

pha

Mea

nr

SD o

fr1st

– 2nd

Fact

orM

ean

SDSk

ewG

rega

rious

ness

(GR)

8ES

CS0.

780.

300.

1340

– 1

44.

070.

960.

06Se

lf-As

sura

nce

(SA)

9ES

CS0.

740.

250.

1134

– 1

35.

210.

78−0

.63

Even

Tem

per (

ET)

8ES

CS0.

810.

350.

1545

– 1

24.

601.

06−0

.29

Conc

ern

for O

ther

s (C

FO)

8ES

CS0.

700.

250.

1235

– 1

75.

460.

71−0

.41

Cons

cien

tious

ness

(CO

)9

ESCS

0.72

0.23

0.16

35 –

13

4.84

0.81

−0.4

4O

rigin

ality

/Virt

uosi

ty (O

V)9

ESCS

0.73

0.25

0.12

34 –

14

4.98

0.79

−0.3

2N

egat

ive

Vale

nce

or

Nor

mal

ity V

iola

tion

(NV)

9ES

CS0.

760.

270.

1036

– 1

56.

390.

64−1

.72

Not

e.N

= 5

92.%

of v

aria

nce

figur

es b

ased

on

a pr

inci

pal-a

xes

anal

ysis

of a

ll ite

ms

in th

e sc

ale

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 46

Page 21: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

(β), and protection of civil institutions (γ).These four dimensions showed low correla-tions with markers for the Big Five (Saucier,2000), indicating that adding the stable dispositions underlying beliefs and attitudesin a dimensional classification of personal-ity will result in at least four additional factors. Contemporary personality invento-ries, however, include scales for constructslike self-transcendence, traditionalism, andopenness to experience, which are morehighly related to belief, value, and attitudedispositions.

Saucier (2006) has developed a brief set ofmarker items for these four factors, the itemsbeing based mostly on dictionary definitionsfrom the earlier study (Saucier, 2000). Table 1.6provides psychometric indices for thesescales. In addition to reasonable internal con-sistency (a from 0.69 to 0.79), the scalesshow impressive retest stability (r from 0.64to 0.85) across nearly four years, and reteststability for tradition-oriented religiousness(0.85) well exceeds that for the typical per-sonality measure. Table 1.7 provides the cor-relations between the four isms scales andlexical marker scales reviewed earlier in this chapter. The highest r is 0.31 (betweensubjective spirituality and loose vs. tight)and, consistent with these being factors addi-tional to those in the lexical marker scales,there are few correlations above 0.20 in magnitude.

As for values, Renner (2003a, 2003b)developed a questionnaire from a lexicalstudy of German, and found four factors incommon across adjective and noun variableselections. These factors were labeled salva-tion, profit, intellectualism, and balance; thefirst two may correspond to tradition-ori-ented religiousness and unmitigated self-interest from Saucier (2000). The sameauthor completed a similar project using theNorthern Sotho language from South Africa(Renner et al., 2003). Again, factors relatedto salvation and profit (though differentlylabeled), plus three additional factors, werefound. Renner’s program of studies makesclear that values can be studied by the lexical

approach, and that lexical value factors prob-ably have some relation to lexical isms fac-tors. These studies promise to lead eventuallyto lexically based measures of values.Analogous lexical studies of interests wouldbe very useful.

CONCLUSIONS

Lexical studies of personality attributes nar-rowly defined have now reached a stagemature enough that key aspects of their struc-ture are becoming evident in the recurrentfindings from these studies. However, the‘personality’ represented in most of thesestudies is a considerably narrower phenome-non than personality as it is typically defined,and the structure of personality attributesencoded in lexicons depends in major waysupon the upstream selection of variables.Therefore, personality psychology shouldcouple the focus that it already has, on themost prototypical attributes of personality,with a simultaneous ‘bigger picture’ exami-nation of all psychological attributes onwhich there are stable individual differences.

Recurrent aspects of the factors at the topof the personality-attribute hierarchy – theone- and two-factor levels – are already quiteclear. Beneath this top level, findings seemmore dependent on variable selection. Givenrelatively narrow variable selection proce-dures, the Big Five emerges readily from somelanguages (mainly those having origins innorthern Europe) while the Cross-LanguageSix emerges readily from an apparently evenwider range of languages. Given more inclu-sive procedures, studies to date are too few topermit firm conclusions. More studies areneeded, and the direct measures of lexicon-derived personality factors reviewed in thischapter are a vital tool for these studies.These measures help facilitate the search forwhat is recurrent and ubiquitous (and what is not) in the personality tendencies that dif-ferentiate humans, as sedimented in humanlexicons.

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 47

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 47

Page 22: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

48 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

Tabl

e 1.

6Ps

ycho

met

ric

Indi

ces

for

Dic

tion

ary-

Base

d M

arke

r Sc

ales

for

the

Fou

r Is

ms

Fact

ors

No.

of

4-ye

ar

Coef

ficie

nt

Mea

n In

ter-

SD o

f Int

er-

% o

f Var

ianc

eM

arke

r Sca

leIte

ms

Sam

ple

Rete

st r

Alph

aite

m r

item

r 1st

– 2nd

Fact

orM

ean

SDSk

ewTr

aditi

on-O

rient

ed R

elig

ious

ness

(TR;

a)6

ESCS

0.85

0.79

0.39

0.11

50 –

14

2.94

0.90

−0.1

0U

nmiti

gate

d Se

lf-In

tere

st (U

SI;fl

)8

ESCS

0.65

0.70

0.23

0.08

33 –

15

1.72

0.50

0.77

Prot

ectio

n of

Civ

il In

stitu

tions

(PCI

;?)

8ES

CS0.

640.

690.

220.

1033

– 1

64.

280.

46−1

.05

Subj

ectiv

e Sp

iritu

ality

(SS;

d)6

ESCS

0.77

0.75

0.33

0.08

45 –

15

2.94

0.81

−0.1

5

Not

eN

= 7

03,e

xcep

t ret

est r

,bas

ed o

n N

= 6

52.%

of v

aria

nce

figur

es b

ased

on

a pr

inci

pal-a

xes

anal

ysis

of a

ll ite

ms

in th

e sc

ale

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 48

Page 23: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Work on this article was supported by GrantMH-49227 from the National Institute ofMental Health, US Public Health Service. I am grateful to Lewis R. Goldberg for edit-ing suggestions. Correspondence regardingthis article may be addressed to: GerardSaucier, Department of Psychology, 1227University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403,USA (E-mail: [email protected]

REFERENCES

Ackerman, P.L. (1996) ‘A theory of adult intel-lectual development: Process, personality,

interests, and knowledge’, Intelligence, 22:227–57.

Ackerman, P.L. and Heggestad, E.D. (1997)‘Intelligence, personality, and interests:Evidence for overlapping traits’, PsychologicalBulletin, 121: 218–45.

Allport, G.W. (1937) Personality: A Psycholo-gical Interpretation. New York: Holt.

Allport, G.W. and Odbert, H.S. (1936) ‘Traitnames: A psycho-lexical study’, PsychologicalMonographs, 4(1, Whole No. 211):

Almagor, M., Tellegen, A. and Waller, N. (1995)‘The Big Seven model: A cross-cultural repli-cation and further exploration of the basicdimensions of natural language of traitdescriptions’, Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 69: 300–7.

Alvarez, J.M., Ruble, D.N. and Bolger, N. (2001)‘Trait understanding or evaluative reasoning?

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 49

Table 1.7 Correlations Between Isms Factor Scales and Adjective Marker Scales ForStructures of One to Seven Factors Isms Factor ScaleAdjective Scale TR (α) USI (β) PCI (γ ) SS (δ)Ge-24 −0.09 −0.11 0.19 0.07Ge-12 −0.08 −0.10 0.18 0.08NEPC-E −0.08 −0.17 0.18 0.10SDQ −0.01 −0.07 0.25* 0.00Osgood E 0.02 −0.09 0.18 0.09S 0.05 −0.12 0.22* 0.03D −0.10 −0.02 0.07 0.08TL 0.00 −0.03 0.15 −0.31*TALU −0.17 0.10 −0.04 −0.18AU −0.16 0.10 0.01 −0.07ALUT −0.09 0.06 −0.17 0.19B5MM-I −0.03 −0.03 0.08 0.04B5MM-II 0.07 −0.14 0.22* 0.12B5MM-III 0.04 −0.02 0.22* −0.13B5MM-IV −0.07 −0.07 0.20* −0.11B5MM-V −0.15 −0.11 −0.07 0.07CL6-H 0.14 −0.16 0.24* −0.03CL6-E 0.18 −0.02 −0.02 0.11CL6-X −0.02 −0.02 0.14 0.06CL6-A 0.02 −0.05 0.20* 0.06CL6-C 0.07 −0.08 0.27* −0.14CL6-O −0.20* −0.09 −0.08 0.10ML7-gr 0.00 −0.02 0.07 0.11ML7-sa −0.10 0.02 0.11 −0.03ML7-et 0.03 −0.06 0.13 −0.04ML7-cfo 0.23* −0.07 0.16 0.16ML7-co 0.24* −0.05 0.27* −0.14ML7-ov −0.21* −0.10 −0.03 0.09ML7-nv −0.02 0.08 −0.29* 0.22*

Note Eugene–Springfield Community Sample, N = 521. All correlations.09 and above are significant, p < .05. For Big Five,correlations are based on ESCS-1995, which is closer in time to the administration of the isms measures. * correlationsover.20 in magnitude. Highest correlation in each column is underlined. TR – tradition-oriented religiousness,USI – unmitigated self-interest, PCI – protection of civil Institutions, SS – subjective spirituality.

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 49

readers
REFERENCES
readers
AU: Please give issue nos for all journal refs
readers
lexical study’, Psychological Whole No. 211):
readers
AU: Allport and Odbert (1937): page nos?
Page 24: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

An analysis of children’s behavioral predic-tions’, Child Development, 72: 1409–25.

Ashton, M.C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P.De Vries, R.E., Di Blas, L., Boies, K. and DeRaad, B. (2004) ‘A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutionsfrom psycholexical studies in seven lan-guages’, Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 86: 356–66.

Benet-Martínez, V. and John, O.P. (1998) ‘LosCinco Grandes across cultures and ethnicgroups: Multitrait multimethod analyses ofthe Big Five in Spanish and English’, Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 75:729–50.

Benet-Martinez, V. and Waller, N.G. (1997)‘Further evidence for the cross-cultural gen-erality of the Big Seven factor model:Indigenous and imported Spanish personalityconstructs’, Journal of Personality, 65: 567–98.

Benet-Martinez, V. and Waller, N.G. (2002)‘From adorable to worthless: Implicit andself-report structure of highly evaluative per-sonality descriptors’, European Journal ofPersonality, 16: 1–41.

Boies, K., Lee, K., Ashton, M.C., Pascal, S. andNicol, A.A.M. (2001) ‘The structure of theFrench personality lexicon’, European Journalof Personality, 15: 277–95.

Bouchard, T.J. (1994) ‘Genes, environment,and personality’, Science, 264: 1700–1.

Caprara, G.V., Barbanelli, C. and Zimbardo,P.G. (1997) ‘Politicians’ uniquely simple per-sonalities’, Nature, 385: 493.

Carroll, J.B. (1993) Human Cognitive Abilities:A Survey of Factor-analytic Studies. NewYork: Cambridge University Press.

Cattell, R.B. (1943) ‘The description of person-ality: Basic traits resolved into clusters’,Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,38: 476–506.

Church, A.T., Katigbak, M.S. and Reyes, J.A.S.(1998) ‘Further exploration of Filipino per-sonality structure using the lexical approach:Do the Big Five or Big Seven dimensionsemerge?’, European Journal of Personality,12: 249–69.

Church, A.T., Reyes, J.A.S., Katigbak, M.S. andGrimm, S.D. (1997) ‘Filipino personalitystructure and the Big Five model: A lexicalapproach’, Journal of Personality, 65: 477–528.

Clark, L.A. and Watson, D. (1999) ‘Tempera-ment: A new paradigm for trait psychology’,

in L.A. Pervin and O.P. John (eds), Handbookof Personality: Theory and Research (2nd ed.,pp. 399-423). New York: Guilford Press.

Cloninger, C.R., Bayon, C. and Svrakic, D.M.(1998) ‘Measures of temperament and char-acter in mood disorders: A model of funda-mental states as personality types’, Journalof Affective Disorders, 51: 21–32.

Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1985) The NEOPersonality Inventory Manual. Odessa, FL:Psychological Assessment Resources.

Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1992a) ‘Fourways five factors are basic’, Personality andIndividual Differences, 13: 653–5.

Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1992b) RevisedNEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI)Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: PsychologicalAssessment Resources.

De Raad, B. and Barelds, D. (2006) ‘A new tax-onomy of Dutch personality traits based on acomprehensive and unrestricted list of descrip-tors’, unpublished manuscript, University ofGroningen.

De Raad, B., Di Blas, L. and Perugini, M. (1998)‘Two independent Italian trait taxonomies:Comparisons with Italian and betweenItalian Germanic languages’, EuropeanJournal of Personality, 12: 19–41.

De Raad, B. and Hoskens, M. (1990) ‘Personality-descriptive nouns’, European Journal ofPersonality, 4: 131–46.

De Raad, B. and Peabody, D. (2005) ‘Cross-cul-turally recurrent personality factors: Analysesof three factors’, European Journal ofPersonality, 19: 451–74.

DeYoung, C.G. (2006) ‘Higher-order factors ofthe Big Five in a multi-informant sample’,Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

Di Blas, L. and Forzi, M. (1998) ‘An alternativetaxonomic study of personality descriptors inthe Italian language’, European Journal ofPersonality, 12: 75–101.

Di Blas, L. and Forzi, M. (1999) ‘Refining a descriptive structure of personality attrib-utes in the Italian language: The abridgedBig Three circumplex structure’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76:451–81.

Di Blas, L., Forzi, M. and Peabody, D. (2000)‘Evaluative and descriptive dimensions fromItalian personality factors’, European Journalof Personality, 14: 279–90.

50 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 50

readers
of Personality, Bouchard, and personality’,
readers
AU: Bouchard (1994): not cited in text. Please add a citation or remove.
readers
pp. 399-423). Cloninger, C.R.,
readers
AU: Cloninger et al (1998): not cited in text. Please add a citation or remove.
readers
Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1992a) ways five factors are basic’, Personality
readers
AU: Costa and McCrae (1992a): not cited in text. Please add a citation or remove.
readers
AU: DeYoung (2006):volume/issue/page nos?
readers
DeYoung, C.G. (2006) the Big Five in a multi-
Page 25: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

Digman, J.M. (1997) ‘Higher order factors ofthe Big Five’, Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 73: 1246–56.

Donnellan, M.B., Oswald, F.L., Baird, B.M. andLucas, R.E. (2006) ‘The Mini-IPIP Scales: Tinyyet effective measures of the Big Five factorsof personality’, Psychological Assessment,18: 192–203.

Dwight, S.A., Cummings, K.M. and Glenar, J.L.(1998) ‘Comparison of criterion-relatedvalidity coefficients for the Mini-Markers andfor Goldberg’s markers of the Big Five per-sonality factors’, Journal of PersonalityAssessment, 70: 541–50.

Eysenck, H.J. (1991) ‘Dimensions of personal-ity: 16, 5, or 3? Criteria for a taxonomic par-adigm’, Personality and Individual Differences,12: 773–90.

Eysenck, H.J. and Eysenck, S.B.G. (1975)Manual of the Eysenck PersonalityQuestionnaire. London: Hodder & Stoughton.

Funder, D.C. (1997) The Personality Puzzle.New York: Norton.

Goldberg, L.R. (1981) ‘Language and individualdifferences: The search for universals in per-sonality lexicons’, in L.W. Wheeler (ed),Review of Personality and Social Psychology(Vol. 2) Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. pp. 141–65.

Goldberg, L.R. (1982) ‘From Ace to Zombie:Some explorations in the language of per-sonality’, in C.D. Spielberger and J.N. Butcher(eds), Advances in Personality Assessment(Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 203–34.

Goldberg, L.R. (1990) ‘An alternative “descrip-tion of personality”: The Big-Five factorstructure’, Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 59: 1216–29.

Goldberg, L.R. (1992) ‘The development ofmarkers for the Big-Five factor structure’,Psychological Assessment, 4: 26–42.

Goldberg, L.R. (1993) ‘The structure of pheno-typic personality traits’, AmericanPsychologist, 48: 26–34.

Goldberg, L.R. (1999) ‘A broad-bandwidth,public-domain, personality inventory meas-uring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models’, in I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. DeFruyt and F. Ostendorf (eds), PersonalityPsychology in Europe (Vol. 7). Tilburg, TheNetherlands: Tilburg University Press. pp. 7–28.

Goldberg, L.R. and Saucier, G. (1995) ‘So whatdo you propose we use instead? A reply toBlock’, Psychological Bulletin, 117: 221–5.

Goldberg, L.R. and Somer, O. (2000) ‘The hier-archical structure of common Turkish person-descriptive adjectives’, European Journal ofPersonality, 14: 497–531.

Goldberg, L.R. and Velicer, W.F. (2006)‘Principles of exploratory factor analysis’, inS. Strack (ed), Differentiating Normal andAbnormal Personality (second edition). NewYork: Springer. pp. 209–37.

Gosling, S.D., Rentfrow, P.J. and Swann, W.B.(2003) ‘A very brief measure of the Big Fivepersonality domains’, Journal of Research inPersonality, 37: 504–28.

Graziano, W.G., Jensen-Campbell, L.A., Steele,R.G. and Hair, E.C. (1998) ‘Unknown wordsin self-reported personality: Lethargic andprovincial in Texas’, Personality and SocialPsychology Bulletin, 24: 893–905.

Hahn D.W., Lee, K. and Ashton, M.C. (1999) ‘A factor analysis of the most fre-quently used Korean personality trait adjec-tives’, European Journal of Personality, 13:261–82.

Hendriks, A.A.J., Hofstee, W.K.B. and De Raad,B. (1999) ‘The Five-Factor PersonalityInventory (FFPI)’, Personality and IndividualDifferences, 27: 307–25.

Hendriks, A.A.J., Perugini, M., Angleitner, A.,Ostendorf, F., Johnson, J.A., De Fruyt, F.,Hrebícková, M., Kreitler, S., Murakami, T.,Bratko, D., Conner, M., Nagy, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A. and Ruisel, I. (2003) ‘The Five-Factor Personality Inventory: Cross-culturalgeneralizability across 13 countries’,European Journal of Personality, 17: 347–73.

Henss, R. (1998) ‘Type nouns and the fivefactor model of personality description’,European Journal of Personality, 12: 57–71.

Hofstee, W.K.B. (1994) ‘Who should own thedefinition of personality?’, European Journalof Personality, 8: 149–62.

Hofstee, W.K.B. and De Raad, B. (1991)‘Persoonlijkheidsstructuur: de AB-sub-5C-taxonomie van Nederlandse eigenschapster-men [Personality structure: The Abridged BigFive-Dimensional Circumplex (AB5C) struc-ture of Dutch trait adjectives’, NederlandsTijdschrift voor de Psychologie en haarGrensgebieden, 46: 262–74.

Hogan, R. (1983) ‘A socioanalytic theory ofpersonality’, in M.M. Page (ed), NebraskaSymposium on Motivation. Lincoln: Universityof Nebraska Press. pp. 336–55.

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 51

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 51

readers
AU: Eysenck (1991): not cited in text. Please add a citation or remove.
readers
Eysenck, H.J. (1991) personality: 16, 5, or 3? Criteria
readers
Psychological Assessment, Goldberg, L.R. (1993) phenotypic personality
readers
AU: Goldberg (1993): not cited in text. Please add a citation or remove.
Page 26: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

Hrebícková, M., Ostendorf, F., Osecká, L. andCermák, I. (1999) ‘Taxonomy and structure ofCzech personality-relevant verbs’, in I.Mervielde, I.J. Deary, F. De Fruyt and F.Ostendorf (eds), Personality Psychology inEurope (Vol. 7). Tilburg, The Netherlands:Tilburg University Press. pp. 51–65.

John, O.P. (1990) ‘The “Big Five” factor taxon-omy: Dimensions of personality in the natu-ral language and in questionnaires’ in L.A. Pervin (ed), Handbook of Personality:Theory and Research. New York: Guilford.pp. 66–100.

John, O.P., Angleitner, A. and Ostendorf, F.(1988) ‘The lexical approach to personality: Ahistorical review of trait taxonomic research’,European Journal of Personality, 2: 171–203.

Krauss, S. (2006) ‘Does ideology transcend cul-ture? A preliminary examination in Romania’,Journal of Personality, 74: 1219–56.

Krueger, R.F. and Markon, K.E. (2006)‘Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-basedapproach to understanding and classifyingpsychopathology’, Annual Review of ClinicalPsychology, 2: 111–33.

Lee, K. and Ashton, M.C. (2004) ‘Psychometricproperties of the HEXACO PersonalityInventory’, Multivariate Behavioral Research,39: 329–58.

Low, K.S.D., Yoon, M., Roberts, B.W., Rounds,J. (2005) ‘Stability of vocational interestsfrom early adolescence to middle adulthood:A quantitative review of longitudinal stud-ies’, Psychological Bulletin, 131: 713–37.

May, M.A. (1932) ‘The foundations of person-ality’, in P.S. Achilles (ed), Psychology atWork. New York: McGraw-Hill.

McCrae, R.R. and Costa, P.T. (1985) ‘UpdatingNorman’s “adequate taxonomy”: Intelligenceand personality dimensions in natural lan-guage and in questionnaires’, Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 49: 710–21.

McCrae, R.R. and Costa, P.T. (1997) ‘Personalitytrait structure as a human universal’,American Psychologist, 52: 509–16.

Norman, W.T. (1963) ‘Toward an adequate tax-onomy of personality attributes: Replicatedfactor structure in peer nomination personal-ity ratings’, Journal of Abnormal and SocialPsychology, 66: 574–83.

Norman, W.T. (1967) 2800 Personality TraitDescriptors: Normative Operating Charact-eristics for a University Population. Department

of Psychology, University of Michigan, AnnArbor.

Osgood, C.E. (1962) ‘Studies on the generalityof affective meaning systems’, AmericanPsychologist, 17: 10–28.

Osgood, C.E., May, W. and Miron, M. (1975)Cross-cultural Universals of AffectiveMeaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

Paulhus, D.L. and John, O.P. (1998) ‘Egoisticand moralistic biases in self-perception: Theinterplay of self-descriptive styles with basictraits and motives’, Journal of Personality,66: 1025–60.

Peabody, D. (1987) ‘Selecting representativetrait adjectives’, Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 52: 59–71.

Peabody, D. and Goldberg, L.R. (1989) ‘Somedeterminants of factor structures from personality-trait descriptors’, Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 57: 552–67.

Renner, W. (2003a) ‘A German value question-naire developed on a lexical basis:Construction and steps toward validation’,Review of Psychology, 10: 107–23.

Renner, W. (2003b) ‘Human values: A lexicalperspective’, Personality and IndividualDifferences, 34: 127–41.

Renner, W., Peltzer, K. and Phoswana, M.G.(2003) ‘The structure of values amongNorthern Sotho speaking people in SouthAfrica’, South African Journal of Psychology,33: 103–8.

Roback, A.A. (1931) Personality: The Crux ofSocial Intercourse. Cambridge, MA: Sci-Art.

Rokeach, M. (1968) Beliefs, Attitudes, andValues: A Theory of Organization andChange. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Rolland, J.P., Parker, W.D. and Stumpf, H.(1998) ‘A psychometric examination of theFrench translations of the NEO-PI-R andNEO-FFI’, Journal of Personality Assessment,71: 269–91.

Rossier, J., Dahouru, D. and McCrae, R.R.(2005) ‘Structural and mean-level analyses ofthe five-factor model and locus of control:Further evidence from Africa’, Journal ofCross-Cultural Psychology, 36: 227–46.

Rothbart, M.K. and Bates, J.E. (1998)‘Temperament’, in W. Damon (series ed.) andN. Eisenberg (vol. ed.), Handbook of ChildPsychology: Vol. 3, Social, Emotional andPersonality Development (5th edition). NewYork: Wiley. pp. 105–76.

52 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF PERSONALITY THEORY AND ASSESSMENT

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 52

readers
AU: John (1990): not cited in text. Please add a citation or remove.
readers
John, O.P. (1990) ‘ taxonomy: Dimensions
Page 27: General Methodological Issues - SAGE Publications€¦ · conclusions about the universe would cer-tainly be altered. To remove the risk that we ignore important phenomena and miss

Saucier, G. (1992) ‘Benchmarks: Integratingaffective and interpersonal circles with the Big-Five personality factors’, Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 62: 1025–35.

Saucier, G. (1994a) ‘Mini-markers: A brief ver-sion of Goldberg’s unipolar Big-Five mark-ers’, Journal of Personality Assessment, 63:506–16.

Saucier, G. (1994b) ‘Separating description andevaluation in the structure of personalityattributes’, Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 66: 141–54.

Saucier, G. (1997) ‘Effects of variable selectionon the factor structure of person descrip-tors’, Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 73: 1296–312.

Saucier, G. (2000) ‘Isms and the structure ofsocial attitudes’, Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 78: 366–85.

Saucier, G. (2002) ‘Orthogonal markers fororthogonal factors: The case of the Big Five’,Journal of Research in Personality, 36: 1–31.

Saucier, G. (2003a) ‘An alternative multi-lan-guage structure of personality attributes’,European Journal of Personality, 17:179–205.

Saucier, G. (2003b) ‘Factor structure of English-language personality type-nouns’, Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 85:695–708.

Saucier, G. (2005) ‘Framework for integratinglexical structures of five, six, and seven fac-tors’, ORI Technical Report, 45(3). Eugene:Oregon Research Institute.

Saucier, G. (2006) ‘A brief measure of the fourfactors in the Survey of Dictionary-basedIsms (SDI)’, unpublished manuscript,University of Oregon.

Saucier, G., Georgiades, S., Tsaousis, I. andGoldberg, L.R. (2005) ‘The factor structureof Greek personality adjectives’, Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 88: 856–75.

Saucier, G. and Goldberg, L.R. (2002)‘Assessing the Big Five: Applications of 10psychometric criteria to the development ofmarker scales’, in B. De Raad, and M.

Perugini (eds), Big Five Assessment. Seattle:Hogrefe & Huber. pp. 29–58.

Saucier, G., Ole-Kotikash, L. and Payne, D.L.(2006) ‘The structure of personality andcharacter attributes in the language of theMaasai’, unpublished report, University ofOregon.

Saucier, G., Ostendorf, F. and Peabody, D.(2001) ‘The non-evaluative circumplex ofpersonality adjectives’, Journal of Personality,69: 537–82.

Saucier, G. and Simonds, J. (2006) ‘The struc-ture of personality and temperament’, inD.K. Mroczek and T.D. Little (eds), Handbookof Personality Development. Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum. pp. 109–28.

Shweder, R.A. (1972) ‘Semantic structure andpersonality assessment’, unpublished doc-toral dissertation, Harvard University.

Szirmák, Z. and De Raad, B. (1994) ‘Taxonomyand structure of Hungarian personalitytraits’, European Journal of Personality, 8:95–118.

Tellegen, A. (1993) ‘Folk concepts and psycho-logical concepts of personality and personal-ity disorder’, Psychological Inquiry, 4: 122–30.

Tellegen, A. (in press) Manual for theMultidimensional Personality Questionnaire.Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Tellegen, A., Grove, W. and Waller, N.G. (1991)‘Inventory of personal characteristics # 7’,unpublished measure, University ofMinnesota.

Tellegen, A. and Waller, N.G. (1987) ‘Re-exam-ining basic dimensions of natural languagetrait descriptors’, aper presented at the 95thAnnual Convention of the AmericanPsychological Association.

White, G.M. (1980) ‘Conceptual universals ininterpersonal language’, American Anthro-pologist, 82: 759–81.

Wiggins, J.S., Trapnell, P. and Phillips, N. (1988)‘Psychometric and geometric characteristicsof the Revised Interpersonal Adjective Scales(IAS-R)’, Multivariate Behavioral Research,23: 517–30.

MEASURES OF THE PERSONALITY FACTORS FOUND RECURRENTLY IN HUMAN LEXICONS 53

9781412946513-Ch01 1/9/08 2:58 PM Page 53

readers
AU: Tellegen (in press): any update?
readers
personality disorder’, Psychological Tellegen, A. (in press) Multidimensional Personality