goal attainability and performance: do you run...

34
a Assistant Professor of Economics, College of Business, University of Central Oklahoma, 100 North University Dr., Edmond, OK, 73034. [email protected], (405)974-2829. b Corresponding Author, Assistant Professor of Economics, Fairfield University, 1073 North Benson Rd., Fairfield, CT, 06824. [email protected], (203)254-4000, ext. 2795. c Assistant Professor of Economics, College of Business, University of Central Oklahoma, 100 North University Dr., Edmond, OK, 73034. [email protected], (405)974-5326. Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower? Mariya Burdina a , R. Scott Hiller b , and Neil E. Metz c Abstract In this paper we test the importance of goal attainability by measuring whether performance improves when setting a more realistic goal. Goal-setting literature suggests that workers respond to challenging but achievable goals with increased performance. Empirical evidence supports the notion of goals increasing performance, however the evidence on how attainability of goals affects performance is mixed. This study uses publicly available marathon data from 1970-2015 to directly analyze the effect of a realistic goal on performance. For many non- professional runners the goal is to qualify for the Boston marathon, which involves running a certified marathon within a specified amount of time, where qualifying time depends on runner’s gender and age group. We test whether runners increase their effort, and consequently improve their performance if they enter a new age group, and as a result have a more attainable goal. We find that runners who enter a new age group and as a result have a more relaxed qualifying time, perform better than the runners whose qualifying time did not change. We also observe a non- linear relationship between the attainability of a goal and performance, as the goal becomes relatively easy, performance decreases. Keywords: goal setting, performance, effort JEL Classification: D84, J22, L83, Z20

Upload: others

Post on 12-Jul-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

a Assistant Professor of Economics, College of Business, University of Central Oklahoma, 100 North University Dr., Edmond, OK, 73034. [email protected], (405)974-2829.

b Corresponding Author, Assistant Professor of Economics, Fairfield University, 1073 North Benson Rd., Fairfield, CT, 06824. [email protected], (203)254-4000, ext. 2795.

c Assistant Professor of Economics, College of Business, University of Central Oklahoma, 100 North University Dr., Edmond, OK, 73034. [email protected], (405)974-5326.

Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

Mariya Burdinaa, R. Scott Hillerb, and Neil E. Metzc

Abstract

In this paper we test the importance of goal attainability by measuring whether performance

improves when setting a more realistic goal. Goal-setting literature suggests that workers

respond to challenging but achievable goals with increased performance. Empirical evidence

supports the notion of goals increasing performance, however the evidence on how attainability

of goals affects performance is mixed. This study uses publicly available marathon data from

1970-2015 to directly analyze the effect of a realistic goal on performance. For many non-

professional runners the goal is to qualify for the Boston marathon, which involves running a

certified marathon within a specified amount of time, where qualifying time depends on runner’s

gender and age group. We test whether runners increase their effort, and consequently improve

their performance if they enter a new age group, and as a result have a more attainable goal. We

find that runners who enter a new age group and as a result have a more relaxed qualifying time,

perform better than the runners whose qualifying time did not change. We also observe a non-

linear relationship between the attainability of a goal and performance, as the goal becomes

relatively easy, performance decreases.

Keywords: goal setting, performance, effort

JEL Classification: D84, J22, L83, Z20

Page 2: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

2

1. Introduction

Incentive literature in economics often focuses on the efficacy of monetary rewards.

Improving productivity and results can be a function of the financial incentives, but non-

pecuniary rewards are also important in many cases. These rewards are much more difficult to

measure, and their mechanisms harder to understand, but in many cases, may be as important to

personal incentives as monetary rewards. In a business environment, Corgnet, Gómez-

Miñambres, and Hernán-Gonzalez (2015) test the importance of goal-setting using an

experimental setting. Hsiaw (2013) provides a theory model of goals as internal motivation, with

realistic goals being important for achievement. In this paper we follow in the same line of

literature by empirically exploring the importance of setting challenging while realistic goals.

Specifically, we test the effect of attainable goals on results, using the qualification of

marathon runners for the Boston Marathon. Qualifying for the Boston Marathon could be the

ultimate goal for many runners, yet it is elusive to most. In order to qualify for Boston, one must

run a marathon on a certified course within a specific time limit determined by the Boston

Athletic Association.1 This qualifying time is based upon an individual runner’s gender and age

group. Runners in younger age groups have faster qualifying times than runners in older age

groups and males have faster qualifying times compared to females. This qualification structure

makes runners more capable of qualifying once they age into the next cohort.

Figure 1 shows the number of finishers by age for the Boston Marathon in years 2012-

14.2 There is a noticeable spike in the number of runners who finish the race at the start of their

1 For the full list of rules on qualifying for Boston Marathon please see http://www.baa.org/Races/Boston-Marathon/Participant-Information/Qualifying.aspx 2 We examine the number of finishers as information is not available on age and runners starting the race. While there will be slightly less finishers than starters, an examination of finishers still allows us to see patterns by age. Also, in 2013 the number of runners finishing is low due to the very unfortunate bombing near the finish line.

Page 3: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

3

age group. Ages 40, 45, and 50 have many more finishers than those just one year younger at

ages 39, 44, and 49. The increase in the number of finishers at the beginning of a new age group

could be explained by the qualifying standards becoming easier. However, data shows that the

number of finishers drops one year into each age group, which suggests that runners may be

willing to put effort into qualifying once they know their goals become more attainable.

[Insert Figure 1 here]

We suggest that runners entering a new age group have a more attainable goal because of

the slower qualifying time, and thus may put in more effort to improve their finish time.

Generally, as in Hsiaw (2013) the easier the goal the less inclined a runner may be to stop early,

maintaining a training schedule that allows for better results. This paper tests whether marathon

runners improve their performance as their goal of qualifying for the Boston Marathon becomes

more realistic when entering a new age group.

Running experts suggest setting specific, challenging, yet realistic goals in order to

improve running times (for examples on running advice from experts see Frazier 2012, Johnson

2013, Keflezighi and Douglas 2015). This advice is largely based on goal-setting theory

developed by Locke in (1968), which has been examined by researchers in the fields of

psychology, weight loss, athletic performance, as well as economics. Goal-setting literature

suggests that specific yet challenging and realistic goals improve performance.3 While much of

the research supports the notion of goals needing to be specific and challenging, very few studies

have examined whether or not attainability plays a key factor in improved performance.

Our paper attempts to further clarify the relationship between goal attainability and

performance. We investigate how adjusting the goals from unattainable to more realistic affects

3 See Locke and Latham (1990) and (2002) for a more complete review of the literature.

Page 4: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

4

performance. Using public data obtained from many well-known and sizeable marathons, we

investigate whether marathon runners improve their performance as their goal of qualifying for

the Boston Marathon becomes more realistic when entering a new age group or if their

performance suffers due to the changes that make qualifying more challenging. We are able to

collect data that allows us to determine how far from the BQ time the runner was in the past, and

whether their past performance combined with the transition into a new age group affects their

future performance.

For each marathon, runners may be motivated by three specific goals (Arbuckle 2014):

1. To finish

2. To improve upon the last finish time

3. To qualify for the Boston Marathon

Finishing a marathon is the goal for many first-time marathoners. The runners in our

study have already accomplished this goal, as only those who ran at least two marathons were

included in our dataset. Among these runners, improving the previous finish time is a recurring

goal regardless of whether or not they are trying to qualify for Boston. Qualifying for Boston

could be the most challenging goal for those who have not been able to do so in the past. For

some, this goal may not be realistic, but for those who finished previous marathons close enough

to the stated qualifying time, it could be attainable. Our dataset allows us to determine how close

a runner was to BQ during their previous marathon race and thus to determine if a runner is

likely to set the BQ time as a goal.

This paper is organized as follows. In Section 2, we present a brief review of literature

looking into goal-setting and performance. In Section 3, we discuss the data, Section 4 the

Page 5: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

5

research methodology, in Section 5 we present the results, and in Section 6 we provide the

conclusions and discuss avenues for further investigation.

2. Literature Review

The goal-setting theory developed by Locke (1968) states that setting challenging yet

achievable goals increases intrinsic motivation, and as a result improves performance. Latham,

Mitchel and Dossett (1978) examined Locke’s theory in workplace production and confirmed the

link between goal-setting and workplace performance. When describing the characteristics of

successfully set goals, Locke and Latham (1985) suggest that in order to boost performance,

goals must be specific, challenging and also attainable and/or realistic. They hypothesized that

unrealistic and unreasonable goals will result in failure, which will lead to a drop in motivation

and lower overall performance.

Substantial research supports the idea that goals need to be specific and challenging

(Mento, Steel, and Karen 1987; Smith, Hauenstein, and Buchanan 1996; Goerg and Kube 2012).

Easy and non-specific goals do not provide enough motivation and as a result, performance

suffers while challenging goals are a stronger motivator than “do your best” goals. Locke and

Latham (1990) and Locke and Latham (2002) reviewed an extensive amount of laboratory and

field studies involving over 40,000 people. In more than 90 percent of the reviewed articles, they

found either strong or mostly strong support for the idea that setting challenging goals improves

performance more than setting less challenging goals.

Contrary to the volume of studies supporting the notion of goals needing to be specific

and challenging, very few have examined whether or not attainability plays a key factor in

improved performance. To our knowledge, few studies have empirically examined the impact of

Page 6: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

6

realistic goal setting on performance. Among them, there is no consensus on whether attainable

goals have a greater effect on performance compared to unattainable goals.

A study conducted by Locke and Somers (1987) was among the first to examine the

characteristics of successful goals. The authors analyzed data from a natural experiment in which

emphasizing the importance of goals resulted in improved performance. In their setting, the

original goals were unrealistic and not enforced. Consequently, the goals were not met.

Intervention in the form of enforcing the importance of set goals and adjusting them to be more

realistic demonstrated improved performance. It is not clear which change had a greater effect on

the end result. However, the findings suggest that the potential for improved performance

increases when goals become more attainable.

Researchers examining the idea of realistic goal-setting often turn to the area of weight

loss. These studies have produced mixed results. In most cases, overweight people are advised to

set small, reasonable goals when losing weight.4 Yet there is no solid evidence to demonstrate

that this method is more successful. Some studies show that setting unrealistic weight-loss goals

increases the probability of participants quitting the weight loss treatment (Dalle Grave et. al.

2005), or could lead to a greater weight regain after the treatment is over (Byrne, Cooper, and

Fairburn 2004). Other studies found no significant relationship between attainability of weight

goals and weight loss or regain (Ames, Perri, and Fox 2005; Gorin, Pinto, and Tate 2007). In

other studies when people had more ambitious goals, more weight loss was achieved when the

participants set high and unrealistic goals (Fabricatore, Wadden, and Rohay 2008; Linde, Jeffery,

and Finch 2004). De Vet, Nelissen, and Zeelenberg (2012) also found that participants who set

4 The Practical Guide Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults, 2000.

National Institutes of Health. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. North American Association for the Study

of Obesity. Available at http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/files/docs/guidelines/prctgd_c.pdf

Page 7: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

7

unrealistically high goals put more effort into the weight-loss attempt. One reason for the mixed

results from these studies is their reliance on an experimental setting which lacks a substantial

sample size. Further, it is not clear what determines attainability of the goals discussed in these

studies.

Studies examining the relationship between goal attainability and performance in the

field of athletic performance also produce mixed results when testing the hypothesis of

unrealistic goals and its possible diminishing impact on performance (Weinberg, Fowler,

Jackson, Bagnall, and Bruya 1991; Garland, Weinberg, Bruya, and Jackson 1988; Bar-Eli, Levy-

Kolker, Tenenbaum and Weinberg 1993; Bar-Eli, Tenenbaum, Pie, Btesh, and Almog 1997).

Garland et. al. (1988) proposed a theory which contradicted Locke (1968), stating that

performance and goal difficulty exhibit a monotonically positive relationship, suggesting

performance can only improve with increased difficulty of set goals. Locke (1991, 1994)

responded to the criticism of his theory by pointing out shortcomings of the studies which could

not confirm the link between goal attainability and performance. Bar-Eli et al. (1997) also

suggested that the length of the performed experiments could have been too short in order to see

significant differences in the outcomes of those with realistic and those with unattainable goals.

Bar-Eli et al. (1997) used field experiments to examine how goal difficulty and

attainability affect performance. In their setting, competitive high school track athletes were

given four different goal conditions: ‘do’ or ‘do your best’ (no goals), ‘improve by 10%’ (easy),

‘improve by 20%’ (difficult/realistic) and ‘improve by 40%’ (improbable/unattainable). The

results show that those with specific goals performed better than those without set goals.

Additionally, those with difficult yet realistic goals increased their performance more than those

with unrealistic goals or goals that were too easy. The major downside to most athletic

Page 8: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

8

performance studies, similar to weight loss studies, is their reliance on experiments which often

contain a relatively small number of observations which may not produce robust results. Our

study uses a natural experiment setting and involves a large dataset over many years.

The closest study to our work is one conducted by Harding and Hsiaw (2014) who

developed a theoretical model of consumer demand for an energy conservation program that

involves non-binding, self-set goals. They also present empirical evidence supporting the notion

that goals must be realistic in order to motivate performance. In their study, consumers who

chose realistic goals when it came to energy conservation consistently saved more, achieving

savings of nearly 11% more than those choosing very low or unrealistically high goals.

Finally, it is worth noticing that marathon data has received recent attention as it presents

researchers with an opportunity to study goal setting and performance in a real-world, non-

experimental setting. Markle, Wu, White, and Sackett (2015) use marathon data to study

Prospect Theory and found that setting a goal before running a marathon improved a runner’s

performance on average by 6 minutes. Even though their study found that goal setting improves

performance, they did not find the Boston marathon qualifying time to be an influence on

participant satisfaction from running a marathon.5

3. Data

To investigate the relationship between goal attainability and performance, we use

publicly-available data on finish results from many marathons over 45 years. We obtained this

data by taking the finishers of three BQ marathons: Oklahoma City Memorial Marathon (2001-

2014), California International Marathon (1990-2013), and Grandma’s Marathon (2001-2014),

5 There are other recent papers on behavioral biases using marathon running data, see Allen, Dechow, Pope, and Wu (2014).

Page 9: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

9

and finding all race results from these finishers on the website Athlinks.com, a vast repository of

race data. All three marathons are certified as BQ marathons, and the California International and

Grandma’s marathon are among those BQ marathons cited as “good for qualifying”.6 We used

these races because of their status and with their racing histories we could collect names of

marathon runners who are likely to be eager to qualify for Boston. Complete racing histories

were obtained for most of these finishers from 1970-2015, providing repeat observations and

experience data.

The following variables were included in the race results of each observation: the

participant’s first and last name, age, gender, race name, year, date, and finish time. We were

able to match participants by their name and city of residence from year to year in each marathon

and determine participants who ran two or more marathons. Some marathons in certain years

were missing either age or gender variables, yet, we were able to reconstruct most of the missing

data for marathoners participating in two or more races. We also had to exclude runners from our

final data set who had a common first and last name as it was impossible to determine which

individual participated in which race.

The key information in our analysis is the Boston qualifying standards which are

determined by the Boston Athletic Association and differ based upon the participant's gender as

well as the age of each participant on the date of the Boston Marathon for which they are

qualifying. Table 1 presents BQ standards based on athlete’s age and gender. The most recent

6 Currently there are more than one thousand certified marathon courses which runners can use to Boston Qualify. The Boston Athletic Association reports which marathons are usually known as the best to BQ, based either on the number of qualifying individuals or percent of qualifying individuals. Usually these marathons are flatter and have more favorable running weather conditions.

Page 10: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

10

standards were adopted for the 2013 Boston Marathon. Prior to 2013, all age group qualifying

times were five minutes longer.7

[Insert Table 1 here]

The cutoff date to attempt qualifying is the second week of September, slightly more than

half a year prior to the Boston Marathon.8 This means any officially certified marathon held

between the third week of September and the second week of the next September is considered

to be a qualifier for the Boston Marathon in the following year. For example, those running a

marathon in December 2011, if qualified will be eligible to participate in the 2013 Boston

Marathon. Those running in April 2012, if qualified, are also eligible to run the Boston Marathon

in 2013.

The timing of the qualification process along with the change in the qualifying time by

age groups leads to some complex age issues which we must consider in our study. The main

challenge in our investigation is presented by the absence of a birthday date for each participant.

The rules of qualifying state that the qualifying marathon finish time should be less than the

predetermined qualifying time for the age group the runner is in during the Boston Marathon, not

during the qualifying marathon. We can estimate BQ time for most of the participants, but for

some who are close in their age to their next age group it may not be possible. For example, a

female who turned 34 in September 2015 and will be 35 in April 2017 for the Boston Marathon

for which she is trying to qualify, may use results from a December 2015 marathon race. Thus,

7 Once every several years the BQ times are adjusted to limit the number of runners in the field. As the number of runners that BQ each year increases, the organizers make the qualifying time more difficult (faster time) to limit the size of the field. Prior to 2013, the standards were adjusted once in 2003, but only for those runner over the age of 55. The BQ times in Table 1 are correct for all runners since 2003, but they are different for this small portion of runners in our sample who are over age 55 and ran prior to 2003. http://www.baa.org/news-and-press/news-listing/2011/february/boston-athletic-association-announces-new-registration-process.aspx. 8 The Boston Marathon is usually held on the third Monday of April.

Page 11: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

11

she needs to run under 3 hours and 40 minutes to BQ, as that's the qualifying time for the 35-39

age group. A different female runner who turned 33 in March 2015 and is also using December

2015 marathon to qualify for Boston, also needs to run that marathon under 3 hours and 40

minutes to qualify as she also will be 35 during the 2017 Boston marathon. Even though one

female was 34 during December qualifying marathon and another one was still 33, both of them

were using the same qualifying standards. So, without knowing a person's birthday it is

challenging to determine what their qualifying time is and if they qualified or not. For

consistency purposes we assumed that those of age 34, 39, 44, 49, 54, 59, 64, 69, 74, and 79 are

qualifying using the next age group qualifying times. While this introduces inevitable

measurement error, there is no other way for us to estimate the age of each runner at the next

Boston Marathon. In order to address the potential for bias, we conduct age falsification tests to

ensure our results are not driven by any measurement error.

Our dependent variable is the marathon finish time, measured in minutes. We estimate

several models using different independent variables. The main independent variables in each

model indicate if a runner entered a new Boston Qualifying age group and if her qualifying

standards became more challenging, stayed the same, or got easier. Initially we use a variable

showing if the runner entered a new qualifying age group. Since runners entering the new BQ

age group have more realistic qualifying goals, they may put more effort into qualifying, and as a

result improve performance. Thus, we expect this variable to have a negative significant

coefficient. The modification of this variable is a set of dummy variables indicating if the

qualifying time increased or decreased, and by how much. Using the runner’s age and current

qualifying time, we are also able to determine by how much the qualifying standards have

changed. Boston qualifying standards get easier by either 5, or 10, or 15 minutes. At the same

Page 12: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

12

time, since qualifying standards have changed in 2013, we had a group of runners for whom

standards became more challenging by 5 minutes.

Additional examination of the data provided us with guidance for the econometric

specifications. Looking at the average finish times by age, we did not observe a significant

difference between those that changed Boston Qualifying age group. Performance should

improve the most when the goals are challenging yet realistic. If we consider a sample of those

for whom the qualifying time changes then three scenarios are possible. First, there will be some

runners which are already close to qualifying for Boston and thus, lowering the qualifying

standards for them will make the goal more attainable but relatively easy. There are also those

who are quite far away from qualifying and adjusting their qualifying time by 5-15 minutes will

not make the goal of qualifying more realistic. Finally, there are runners who are somewhat close

to qualifying, thus their goal is still challenging and adjusting qualifying times will make it more

realistic. We expect to see the last group improve their performance the most compared to any

other group.

When it comes to running marathons, there is no single scheme that will determine how

much one can improve, which makes it difficult to determine for which groups of runners it will

be “easy” or “challenging” to qualify for Boston (Gaudette 2013). Bar-Eli et al. (1997) used

10%, 20%, 40% improvement targets as a measure of goal difficulty, but in a marathon, a 10%

improvement is a difficult goal to attain (i.e. a 10% improvement from the most common finish

time of 4 hours would be 24 minutes). The improvement target is based on the number of

minutes by which a runner missed her qualifying time in the previous marathon. Of course, it is

entirely possible that a 5 minute improvement may be very difficult for some and quite easy for

Page 13: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

13

others. For this reason, we include a runner’s previous personal record (or personal best)

marathon finish time to control for overall ability.

Table 2 shows the difference in finish time between runners for whom qualifying

standards did not change, and for whom qualifying standards either decreased by 5 minutes or

increased by 5, 10, and 15 minutes respectively. All differences are compared to runners for

whom the qualifying standards did not change, whom we excluded from the table. Each column

groups runners based on how close they were to qualifying for Boston in the previous race. Each

row groups runners according to the change in qualifying time based on them entering a new age

group. The difference in finish time is measured in minutes.

[Insert table 2 here]

The patterns in each column and across columns show that runners have interesting

reactions to their change in BQ time relative to their previous finish time. The most noticeable

thing about the results is that regardless of their previous finish time, all runners for whom

qualifying standards became more challenging, had a slower finish time compared to those for

whom those standards did not change. This observation is consistent with goal setting theory

and suggests that once the goal became more challenging, the runners performed worse. We also

notice that runners who missed their qualifying time by less than 5 minutes and gained 5 minutes

of qualifying time, on average, were slower than those for whom qualifying time did not change.

While for runners who missed qualifying time by more than 5 minutes the result is reversed.

Those who gained 5 minutes of qualifying time were on average faster than those for whom

qualifying time did not change. This suggests that runners near the BQ time that experienced a

more attainable, but still difficult goal (they needed to improve by 5-10 minutes to BQ), tended

to improve more than most other runners. At the same time, for those who missed qualifying by

Page 14: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

14

less than 5 minutes and at the same time gained 5 minutes of qualifying time, the goal became

too easy, and thus, there was no need for those runners to try harder.

Finally, we observe that those who gained 10 or 15 minutes were on average slower than

those for whom qualifying time did not change. Even though this observation is somewhat

contradictory to the goal setting theory, it is important to keep in mind that those runners are

usually older than the average runner for whom the qualifying time did not change. Thus,

without controlling for the age it is difficult to conclude whether these runners were faster or

slower than those for whom qualifying time did not change.

We account for the attainability of a qualifying goal by including a variable measuring

how much a runner missed qualifying in the previous race. A runner may also estimate their

qualifying chances by the amount of time it took them to run their best race, so we take into

account the runner’s personal best marathon time and use it as another proxy to control for a

runner’s qualifying chance. As a result, we lose the first observation of each runner, but this

allows us to better control for personal ability.

The other independent variables include runner characteristics such as age and gender. In

order to control for a runner’s experience, we use the number of marathon and non-marathon

races completed by the runner in the past. We use the number of races in the particular year as a

proxy measuring runner’s preparedness for the marathon. Training for a marathon requires a

serious commitment from a runner, but we have no way of observing if the runner was actually

seriously training (i.e. completing their scheduled runs). Those who participate in numerous

races may also be more likely to complete their training plan for the marathon. Additionally, we

construct the variable showing if the runner has a potential to qualify based on her previous

marathon finish time and her current Boston qualifying time.

Page 15: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

15

The initial dataset of all runners, from all years and all races contains more than 5.5

million observations, but this includes many races which are not marathons. When cleared of all

races but marathons and narrowed to the range of finishing times between three and five hours

with multiple observations for each runner, the final sample contains 145,544 observations, but

given the need to establish an initial race time, the observations in each regression are less.9

Variable definitions are presented in Table 3. Summary statistics are included in Table 4.

[Insert Table 3 here]

[Insert Table 4 here]

The average time for a runner in our dataset is 240 minutes, which is 20 minutes less than

the reported overall average marathon time10, but that is largely due to the fact that we excluded

races in which a marathoner finished in over 5 hours as the chances of qualifying are minimal.11

We also observe that 17% of our sample qualified for Boston at some point. The average time by

which the runners missed their BQ time is 25 minutes and the average time by which a runner’s

personal best race missed their BQ time is 12 minutes. Both of these statistics in our sample

allow us to believe that qualifying is within reach for an average runner in our sample.

4. Methodology

9 We have also determined participants who had the same last and first name, yet were either from a different city or showed a large gap in the age while participating in marathons. We have assumed that these participants happened to have the same first and last name but were actually different people and thus are considered separate runners. 10 http://runrepeat.com/research-marathon-performance-across-nations 11 If we kept slower runners in the dataset, the average finish time was 261 minutes which is close to the 264 minutes average reported finish time for marathon runners http://www.livestrong.com/article/1002323-average-time-run-marathon/

Page 16: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

16

We estimate the effect of a change in goal attainability on two separate models measuring

performance. First, we estimate the effect of a change in age group on a runner’s performance

during the marathon. The model uses the following general form:

𝑅𝑎𝑐𝑒_𝑡𝑖𝑚𝑒𝑖𝑗𝑡 = 𝛼0 + 𝛼1 ∑ 𝐵𝑄_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒𝑖𝑡 + 𝛼2𝐴𝑔𝑒𝑖𝑡 + 𝛼3𝐺𝑒𝑛𝑑𝑒𝑟𝑖 + 𝛼4𝑀𝑖𝑠𝑠𝑒𝑑𝑖𝑗𝑡−1

+ 𝛼5𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑖𝑒𝑛𝑐𝑒𝑖𝑡 + 𝛼6𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑖𝑒𝑛𝑐𝑒_𝑆𝑞𝑖𝑡 + 𝛼7𝑅𝑎𝑐𝑒𝑠_𝑖𝑛_𝑦𝑒𝑎𝑟𝑖𝑡

+ 𝛼8𝑅𝑢𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑛𝑔_𝑃𝑅𝑖𝑡 + 𝛼9𝑀𝑖𝑛_𝑅𝑎𝑐𝑒_𝑇𝑖𝑚𝑒𝑖𝑡 + 𝛼10 ∑ 𝑀𝑜𝑛𝑡ℎ𝑖𝑗 𝜖𝑖𝑗𝑡

Where 𝑅𝑎𝑐𝑒_𝑡𝑖𝑚𝑒𝑖𝑗𝑡 is athlete i’s finish time in year t at marathon j. 𝐵𝑄_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒𝑖𝑡 is a

set of variables showing if an athlete aged into a new Boston qualifying age group. We use

several regression specifications to estimate the effect of a new age group on marathon finish

time. In the first specification, we use 𝐵𝑄_𝑔𝑟𝑜𝑢𝑝_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒𝑖𝑡 variable indicating if a runner

simply entered a new age group. In the second specification we add a dummy variable

𝐵𝑄_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒_𝑛𝑒𝑔5𝑖𝑡 indicating if qualifying standards got tougher for a runner as a result of

change in change in qualifying structure for the 2013 Boston Marathon. In the third specification

we use a set of dummy variables 𝐵𝑄_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒_𝑛𝑒𝑔5𝑖𝑡, 𝐵𝑄_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒5𝑖𝑡, 𝐵𝑄_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒10𝑖𝑡, and

𝐵𝑄_𝑐ℎ𝑎𝑛𝑔𝑒15𝑖𝑡. These variables indicate if the qualifying standards for a runner decreased by 5

minutes, did not change, increased by 5, 10, or 15 minutes respectively.

𝐴𝑔𝑒𝑖𝑡 and 𝐺𝑒𝑛𝑑𝑒𝑟𝑖are athlete i’s corresponding age and gender (equal to 1 if female, and

0 if male) during the year t. We expect both, 𝐴𝑔𝑒𝑖𝑡 and 𝐺𝑒𝑛𝑑𝑒𝑟𝑖 variables to be positively related

to marathon finish time on average, older runners and females have slower finish times

compared to younger runners and males respectively. Variable 𝑀𝑖𝑠𝑠𝑒𝑑𝑖𝑗𝑡−1 measures by how

much the runner missed the BQ time in their previous marathon, giving the improvement

necessary if the qualifying time is not lowered. The E𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑖𝑒𝑛𝑐𝑒𝑖𝑡 term counts the total number

Page 17: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

17

of races the runner has completed before race j in our dataset. This includes marathons and other

races shorter than marathons. The variable 𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑖𝑒𝑛𝑐𝑒_𝑠𝑞𝑢𝑎𝑟𝑒𝑑𝑖𝑡 is the square of

𝐸𝑥𝑝𝑒𝑟𝑖𝑒𝑛𝑐𝑒𝑖𝑡. The variable 𝑅𝑎𝑐𝑒𝑠_𝑖𝑛_𝑦𝑒𝑎𝑟𝑖𝑡 provides the total races the runner completes in

year t. The 𝑅𝑢𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑛𝑔_𝑃𝑅𝑖𝑡 and 𝑀𝑖𝑛_𝑅𝑎𝑐𝑒_𝑡𝑖𝑚𝑒𝑖𝑡 variables are intended to capture the racer’s

maximum potential to that point, considering the best marathon time and the best overall time of

the runner prior to the current race. ∑ 𝑀𝑜𝑛𝑡ℎ𝑖𝑗 is a set of dummy variables indicating in which

month runner i participated in the marathon j. We use month dummies to control for seasonal

differences in races.

The error term is assumed to be normally distributed, and estimation is performed with a

random-effects least squares regression. Several models are estimated with the independent

variables slightly altered as described above.

5. Results

Table 5 provides the results of a random effects least-squares regression. Our dependent

variable in each of the specifications is the marathon finish time in minutes for each runner in an

individual race. Coefficients on month are excluded for space considerations.

[Insert Table 5 here]

In each of the regressions we observe that age is positively and significantly related to a

runner’s marathon finish time. That is not a surprise as we expect older runners to have slower

marathon finish times. The gender variable also has a predicted positive sign, indicating that on

average, females have slower finish times by more than 11 minutes compared to the male racers.

The experience variable is positive and significantly related to the finish time, while the square

of experience is negatively related to the finish time. This result is somewhat surprising as it

Page 18: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

18

suggests that past participation in numerous races hurts one performance. One possible

explanation for such a result is that the experience variable consists of all races a runner

participated in, including 5-Ks and Half Marathons, which are less strenuous and require less

training. Thus, it is possible that slower runners may choose to participate in these races more

often. However, the negative sign on Experience_sq indicates there is a level of experience from

which time begins to improve. As a group the experience terms indicate that experience does not

help finish time initially, but leads to an improvement in time as experience increases. Another

interesting observation is that the number of races a marathoner had during that particular year

(Races_in_year) is negatively related to their finish time, which indicates that runners who

compete in many races may be more serious about their training and thus perform better while

running a marathon. Running_PR and Min_Race_time variables are both positively related to the

finish time, indicating that runners with faster personal records will have faster marathon finish

times.

Column (1) presents the results of an econometric specification in which our main

variable of interest, BQ_group_change shows what we would expect, a significant negative

relationship with the finish time. As the qualifying for Boston Marathon becomes more

attainable, runners will put more effort in finishing a marathon in faster time. On average,

entering a new age group decreased finish time by 0.67 minutes.

Since qualifying standards became more challenging for those trying to qualify for the

2013 Boston Marathon, we wanted to see if that change had an effect on finish times of the

runners. Column (2) presents the results of that regression, in which BQ_change_neg5 is the

dummy variable indicating if the qualifying time decreased by 5 minutes, that is in order to

qualify a runner must run 5 minutes faster. This change would be applicable to all runners who

Page 19: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

19

did not change age groups and who ran qualifying marathons after September 2011. What we

observe is a positive and significant relationship between this variable and finish time, which

indicates that as the goal became more challenging, on average, runners had a greater marathon

finish time. The runners who experienced an increase in qualifying standards on average slowed

down by 1.41 minutes compared to runners that did not experience this change.

Column (3) expands on the simplest regression form and includes a set of dummy

variables indicating if Boston qualifying standards got harder by five minutes or got easier by

either five, ten, or fifteen minutes. The results suggest that for those who experienced more

difficult qualifying standards, finish time was slower by about 1 minute compared to those for

whom finish times did not change. Results also show that those who entered a new age group

and as a result received five-minute slower qualifying standards were more likely to decrease

their finish time, on average by 1.9 minutes compared to those for whom qualifying standards

did not change. Those for whom qualifying standards got easier by 10 minutes also had faster

finish times compared to the “no-change” group. The results also indicate that the runners for

whom qualifying standards changed by 15 minutes did not have significantly faster times than

those for whom the standards did not change. There are two things that can explain this result.

First, those who have a 15 minute reduction in qualifying time are older than those that have a 5

or 10 minute advantage. Second, it suggests that as the goal becomes not only attainable but also

somewhat easy, runners did not put as much effort into accomplishing it. These results suggest

that there is a non-linear relationship between goal difficulty and effort as we observe the goal

becoming easier, at first runners tend to speed up, but when the goal becomes relatively easy,

runners tend to slow down. This finding supports the hypothesis of improved performance under

challenging, yet realistic goals.

Page 20: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

20

We further investigate how changes in qualifying time affects runners who are close

to accomplishing their goal of qualifying for Boston Marathon by excluding slower runners who

did not show potential for qualifying. Table 6 presents the results of the estimation, where

runners who have a finishing time with a difference from qualifying greater than two standard

deviations away from the sample average maximum improvement are dropped. This is intended

to ensure that the results are not being driven by runners with little chance of ever qualifying for

Boston. This specification means that in our sample if a runner began their marathon career by

missing qualifying by 56 minutes or more, they were deemed unlikely to ever qualify, and

dropped from the sample. The results show that runners with the best chance of qualifying

experience the same reduction in finish time from more realistic qualifying standards. Results are

qualitatively similar to the baseline regression, with the coefficient on BQ_group_changes

slightly lessened, and the individual 5 and 10 minute change variables strengthened marginally.

One noticeable difference is that for runners who had a chance to qualify and for whom

qualifying standards changed by 15 minutes, finished on average 1 minute slower than those for

whom qualifying standards did not change. These results once again confirmed our assumption

of improved performance under realistic and challenging goals.

[Insert Table 6 here]

Concerns may still remain that we are not identifying the age change effect on marathon

results. In results available by request, we create falsification tests that replace the

BQ_group_change variable with ages both 1 and 2 years before the original variable. For

example, whereas a runner who is 34 is counted as changing age groups in the main

specification, we try ages 33 and 32 to try to find a similar effect. In the basic regression with

these falsification tests, we find a significant, small increase in finish times from moving into

Page 21: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

21

these age groups in the years prior to the change in age group. These results account for the

standard controls, including an increase in age, and show an opposite effect to the changing of

the age bracket, indicating that our data is not suffering from measurement error, and if there is

any bias in our results, it is toward attenuation.

6. Discussion

The goal of this paper is to examine the relationship between realistic goals and

performance. Using the data from numerous different marathons and analyzing the difference in

performance of runners over the course of many years, we find strong support to the hypothesis

that performance improves as the goals change from unattainable to realistic. We see that

lessening qualifying standards had a strong positive relationship with performance and that in

cases in which the standard became more challenging, we see a strong negative relationship with

performance. From an economic standpoint, this is an interesting result as it relates to effort and

performance. As managers seek to create policies to maximize effort from their employees, our

paper suggests performance goals need to be carefully chosen, and that managers should

implement attainable goals. Goals that are too easy or difficult may cause a drop or no change in

performance.

Some of the difficulty in measuring attainment of goals arises from small sample sizes

and an inability to control for external influences. In using marathons, we can observe years of a

running career, controlling for the level of experience and previous results. Additionally, because

of the varying changes to the Boston qualifying time, we have variation in goals that allows

identification of improvement with attainability.

Page 22: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

22

In addition to the large sample used, our study benefits from the clarity and simple

measurement of the goal of BQ. The goal is clear and the runner can always be sure of what they

need to achieve, and by how much they must improve to achieve their goal. Past empirical

studies on the importance of the attainability of goals may have suffered from less of a clear path

to goal attainment. For example, setting an attainable weight loss target may not be enough if the

person does not know how to achieve it. Improvement in running may be more straightforward,

and in this process realistic goals prove important.

The collection of such a large dataset creates the potential for measurement error, but our

results are robust to sensitivity and falsification tests. Our empirical results are clear, but future

research could focus on different stages of the marathon. For example, we would like to see if

the results hold when instead of measuring performance by the marathon finish time, we use the

time it took a marathoner to complete an early stage of the marathon, the 5 or 10 kilometer mark.

These stages may help to remove some of the problems which are unknowable to the researcher,

but could affect finishing times, and potentially strengthen our results.

Page 23: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

23

References

Allen, E. J., Dechow, P. M., Pope, D. G., and Wu, G. (2014). Reference-dependent preferences:

Evidence from marathon runners. (No. w20343). National Bureau of Economic Research.

Ames, G. E., Perri, M. G., and Fox, L.D. (2005). Changing weight-loss expectations: A

randomized pilot study. Eating Behaviors, 6(3), 259–269.

Arbuckle, D. (2014). Goal Setting for Running a Marathon. Available:

http://livehealthy.chron.com/goal-setting-running-marathon-1887.html

Bar-Eli, M., Levy-Kolker, N., Tenenbaum, G., and Weinberg, R.S. (1993). Effect of goal

difficulty on performance of aerobic, anaerobic and power tasks in laboratory and field settings.

Journal of Sport Behavior, 16, 17-32.

Bar-Eli, M., Tenenbaum, G., Pie, J. S., Btesh, Y., and Almog, A. (1997). Effect of goal

difficulty, goal specificity and duration of practice time intervals on muscular endurance

performance, Journal of Sports Sciences, 15(2), 125-135.

Byrne, S. M., Cooper, Z., and Fairburn, C. G. (2004). Psychological predictors of weight regain

in obesity. Behavior research and therapy, 42(11), 1341–1356.

Corgnet, B., Gómez-Miñambres, J., and Hernán-Gonzalez, R., 2015. Goal Setting and Monetary

Incentives: When Large Stakes Are Not Enough. Management Science. 61(12): 2926-2944.

Dalle Grave, R., Calugi, S., Molinari, E., Petroni, M., L., Bondi, M., Compare, A., Marchesini,

G., and QUOVADIS Study Group. (2005). Weight loss expectations in obese patients and

treatment attrition: An observational multicenter study. Obesity Research, 13(11), 1961–1969.

Page 24: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

24

De Vet, E., Nelissen, R., Zeelenberg, M., and De Ridder, D. (2012). Ain’t no mountain high

enough? Setting high weight loss goals predict effort and short-term weight loss. Journal of

Health Psychology, 18(5), 638–647.

Frazier, M. (2012). Qualifying for the Boston Marathon: 5 Essential Steps. Available:

http://www.runyourbq.com/qualifying-for-boston-marathon/

Fabricatore, A. N., Wadden, T. A., and Rohay, J. M. (2008). Weight loss expectations and goals

in a population sample of overweight and obese US adults. Obesity, 16(11), 2445–2450.

Garland, H., Weinberg, R.S., Bruya, L.D., and Jackson, A. (1988). Self-efficacy and endurance

performance: A longitudinal field test of cognitive mediation theory. Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 37, 381-394.

Gaudette J. (2013). The science behind setting a realistic marathon goal time. Competitor

Running. Available at: http://running.competitor.com/2013/07/training/setting-a-realistic-

marathon-goal-time_79229

Goerg, S. J. and Kube, S. (2012). Goals (th)at Work, Goals, Monetary Incentives, and Workers’

Performance. Available at: http://www.coll.mpg.de/pdf_dat/2012_19online.pdf

Gorin, A. A., Pinto, A. M., and Tate, D. F. (2007). Failure to meet weight loss expectations does

not impact maintenance in successful weight losers. Obesity, 15(12), 3086–3090.

Harding, M. and Hsiaw, A. (2014). Goal setting and energy conservation. Journal of Economic

Behavior & Organization, 107, Part A, 209-227.

Hsiaw, A., 2013. Goal-setting and self-control. Journal of Economic Theory, 148(2), pp.601-626.

Page 25: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

25

Johnson, M. (2013). How to Improve Your Half Marathon or Marathon Time, Runner Academy.

Available: http://runneracademy.com/how-to-improve-marathon-time/

Keflezighi, M. and Douglas, S. (2015). How to Set Good Running Goals, Runners World.

Available: http://www.runnersworld.com/advice/how-to-set-good-running-goals

Latham, G. P., Mitchel, T. R., and Dossett, D. L. (1978). Importance of participative goal setting

and anticipated rewards on goal difficulty and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,

63, 163-71.

Latham, G. P. (2000). Motivate Employee Performance through Goal-Setting. In E. A. Locke

(Ed.), The Blackwell Handbook of Principles of Organizational Behavior (pp. 107-119). Malden:

Blackwell Publishers.

Linde, J. A., Jeffery R. W., and Finch E. A. (2004). Are unrealistic weight loss goals associated

with outcomes for overweight women? Obesity Research, 12(3), 569–576.

Locke, E.A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation incentives. Organizational Behavior and

Human Performance, 3, 157-189.

Locke, E.A. (1991). Problems with goal-setting research in sports - and their solution. Journal of

Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 311-316.

Locke, E.A. (1994). Comments on Weinberg and Weigand. Journal of Sport and Exercise

Psychology, 16, 212-215.

Locke, E.A. and Latham, G.P. (1985). The application of goal setting to sports. Journal of Sport

Psychology, 7, 205-222.

Page 26: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

26

Locke, E. A. and Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Locke, E. A. and Latham G. P. (2002). Building a Practically Useful Theory of Goal Setting and

Task Motivation, American Psychologist, 57, 705-717.

Locke, E.A. and Somers R. L. (1987). The effects of goal emphasis on performance on a

complex task. Journal of Management Studies, 24(4), 405-411.

Markle, A., Wu, G., White, R. J., and Sackett, A. M. Goals as Reference Points in Marathon

Running: A Novel Test of Reference Dependence (April 8, 2015). Fordham University Schools

of Business Research Paper No. 2523510. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2523510

or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2523510

Mento, A. J., Steel, R. P., and Karen, R. J. (1987). A meta-analytic study of the effects of goal

setting on task performance: 1966-1984. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 39, 52-83.

Smith, J., Hauenstein, N., and Buchanan, L. (1996). Goal Setting and Exercise Performance.

Human Performance, 9(2), 141-154.

The Working Group. (2000). The Practical Guide Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of

Overweight and Obesity in Adults. National Institutes of Health. National Heart, Lung, and

Blood Institute. North American Association for the Study of Obesity. Available at

http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/files/docs/guidelines/prctgd_c.pdf

Page 27: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

27

Weinberg, R.S., Fowler, C., Jackson, A., Bagnall, J., and Bruya, L.D. (1991). Effect of goal

difficulty on motor performance: A replication across tasks and subjects. Journal of Sport and

Exercise Psychology, 13, 160-173.

Page 28: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

28

Figures

0

200

400

600

800

1000

1200

1400

34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55

Nu

mb

er o

f Fi

nis

her

s

Age

Figure 1: Boston Marathon Finishers by Age in 2012-14

2014

2013

2012

Page 29: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

29

Tables

Age group Men Women Men Women

18-34 190 220 185 215

35-39 195 225 190 220

40-44 200 230 195 225

45-49 210 240 205 235

50-54 215 245 210 240

55-59 225 255 220 250

60-64 240 270 235 265

65-69 255 285 250 280

70-74 270 300 265 295

75-79 285 315 280 310

80 and over 300 330 295 325

Before 2013 (Minutes) After 2013 (Minutes)

Table 1: Boston Qualifying times in minutes by age and gender before and after 2013

Page 30: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

30

Table 2: Average finish time difference by previous results and qualifying changes

Change in Boston

Qualifying time

Missed Boston Qualifying time in the previous race

by 5 minutes by 5-10 minutes by 10-15 minutes by 15-20 minutes

Lost 5 minutes 4.67 4.48 4.23 4.70

Gained 5 minutes 0.67 -1.90 -0.95 -0.86

Gained 10 minutes 3.93 2.78 5.20 3.01

Gained 15 minutes 16.44 18.31 13.79 15.49

The table shows the difference in marathon finish times between runners for whom qualifying time stayed the

same and runners for whom qualifying time has either decreased by 5 minutes, or increased by 5, 10, or 15

minutes. The runners are grouped according to number of minutes by which they have missed BQ in the previous

race.

Page 31: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

31

Table 3: Variable Descriptions

Variable Description

Race time Marathon finish time

BQ group change =1 if participant changed Boston qualifying age group

BQ change neg 5 =1 if Boston qualifying time decreased by 5 minutes

BQ change 0 =1 if Boston qualifying time stayed the same

BQ change 5 =1 if Boston qualifying time increased by 5 minutes

BQ change 10 =1 if Boston qualifying time increased by 10 minutes

BQ change 15 =1 if Boston qualifying time increased by 15 minutes

Age Participant’s age

Gender =1 if female

Missed Time by which the participant missed qualifying for Boston in the previous race

Experience Number of races participant competed in

Experience squared Experience squared

Running PR Fastest previous marathon finish time

Race in a year Number of races participant competed in during particular year

Min race time Fastest previous 5k race

Month_t =1 if ran a marathon in month t (January t=1, February = 2, etc.)

Missed_index_1 = 1 if qualified for Boston in the previous year

Missed_index_2 = 1 if missed qualification by less than 5 minutes

Missed_index_3 = 1 if missed qualification by 5-10 minutes

Missed_index_4 = 1 if missed qualification by 10-15 minutes

Missed_index_5 = 1 if missed qualification by 15-20 minutes

Missed_index_6 = 1 if missed qualification by more than 20 minutes

Page 32: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

32

Table 4: Summary Statistics

Variable Mean Std. Dev Min Max

Race time 240.8440 30.3984 180 300

BQ group change 0.1584 0.3651 0 1

BQ change neg 5 0.0414 0.1991 0 1

BQ change 0 0.7347 0.4415 0 1

BQ change 5 0.1181 0.3228 0 1

BQ change 10 0.0677 0.2513 0 1

BQ change 15 0.0269 0.1619 0 1

Age 42.4854 10.4931 17 82

Gender 0.3318 0.4709 0 1

Missed 25.1605 30.9526 -125.017 114.7333

Experience 29.0423 38.7165 1 377

Experience squared 2342.4130 6481.7850 1 142129

Running PR 228.2195 29.6390 180 300

Race in a year 6.0743 6.2478 1 30

Min race time 47.5014 54.7675 10 297.2167

Month_ind_1 0.0253 0.1572 0 1

Month_ind_2 0.0215 0.1450 0 1

Month_ind_3 0.0557 0.2293 0 1

Month_ind_4 0.1043 0.3057 0 1

Month_ind_5 0.0747 0.2629 0 1

Month_ind_6 0.1386 0.3455 0 1

Month_ind_7 0.0237 0.1521 0 1

Month_ind_8 0.0108 0.1035 0 1

Month_ind_9 0.0338 0.1808 0 1

Month_ind_10 0.2479 0.4318 0 1

Month_ind_11 0.0760 0.2651 0 1

Missed_ind_1 0.1742 0.3793 0 1

Missed_ind_2 0.0428 0.2025 0 1

Missed_ind_3 0.0461 0.2098 0 1

Missed_ind_4 0.0464 0.2103 0 1

Missed_ind_5 0.0459 0.2093 0 1

Page 33: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

33

Table 5. Regression results

(1) (2) (3)

BQ group change -0.671*** -0.619***

(0.179) (0.180)

BQ change neg5 1.392*** 1.096***

(0.336) (0.337)

BQ change 5 -1.945***

(0.210)

BQ change 10 -1.829***

(0.268)

BQ change 15 -0.495

(0.423)

Age 0.757*** 0.759*** 0.767***

(0.0108) (0.0108) (0.0110)

Gender 11.72*** 11.73*** 11.82***

(0.251) (0.251) (0.250)

Missed 0.219*** 0.220*** 0.224***

(0.00378) (0.00378) (0.00379)

Experience 0.172*** 0.170*** 0.172***

(0.00615) (0.00618) (0.00618)

Experience squared -0.000484*** -0.000477*** -0.000483***

(0.0000278) (0.0000279) (0.0000279)

Running PR 0.315*** 0.314*** 0.313***

(0.00467) (0.00467) (0.00467)

Races in a year -0.236*** -0.234*** -0.254***

(0.0179) (0.0179) (0.0181)

Min Race Time 0.0369*** 0.0369*** 0.0367***

(0.00230) (0.00230) (0.00229)

_cons 118.4*** 118.3*** 118.4***

(1.024) (1.024) (1.023)

N 110835 110835 110835

R squared 0.4168 0.4169 0.4184

Standard errors in parentheses *p< .10, **p< 0.05, ***p< .01

Page 34: Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run …faculty.fairfield.edu/rhiller/Research/Marathons_final...Goal Attainability and Performance: Do You Run Faster When You Can Run Slower?

34

Table 6. Results excluding athletes unlikely to qualify

(1) (2) (3)

BQ group change -0.460** -0.395**

(0.200) (0.200)

BQ change neg5 1.606*** 1.471***

(0.372) (0.374)

BQ change 5 -1.168***

(0.236)

BQ change 10 -1.109***

(0.286)

BQ change 15 1.168***

(0.423)

Age 0.936*** 0.938*** 0.935***

(0.0135) (0.0135) (0.0137)

Gender 16.37*** 16.38*** 16.42***

(0.327) (0.327) (0.327)

Missed 0.189*** 0.190*** 0.191***

(0.00528) (0.00529) (0.00531)

Experience 0.154*** 0.151*** 0.153***

(0.00751) (0.00754) (0.00755)

Experience squared -0.000330*** -0.000321*** -0.000324***

(0.0000352) (0.0000352) (0.0000352)

Running PR 0.304*** 0.303*** 0.303***

(0.00664) (0.00665) (0.00665)

Races in a year -0.316*** -0.314*** -0.325***

(0.0225) (0.0225) (0.0226)

Min Race Time 0.0387*** 0.0387*** 0.0386***

(0.00281) (0.00280) (0.00280)

_cons 108.4*** 108.4*** 108.7***

(1.363) (1.363) (1.363)

N 64650 64650 64650

R squared 0.4973 0.4977 0.4984

Standard errors in parentheses *p< .10, **p< 0.05, ***p< .01

This regression excludes runners who have a finishing time with a difference from qualifying greater than two

standard deviations away from the sample average maximum improvement