healthy aging: factors that predict preservation of

65
Loma Linda University eScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & Creative Works Loma Linda University Electronic eses, Dissertations & Projects 8-2018 Healthy Aging: Factors that Predict Preservation of Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults Imari-Ashley F. Palma Follow this and additional works at: hp://scholarsrepository.llu.edu/etd Part of the Medicine and Health Sciences Commons , and the Psychology Commons is Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by eScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & Creative Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Loma Linda University Electronic eses, Dissertations & Projects by an authorized administrator of eScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & Creative Works. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Palma, Imari-Ashley F., "Healthy Aging: Factors that Predict Preservation of Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults" (2018). Loma Linda University Electronic eses, Dissertations & Projects. 518. hp://scholarsrepository.llu.edu/etd/518

Upload: others

Post on 26-Nov-2021

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Loma Linda UniversityTheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research,Scholarship & Creative Works

Loma Linda University Electronic Theses, Dissertations & Projects

8-2018

Healthy Aging: Factors that Predict Preservation ofCognitive Functioning in Older AdultsImari-Ashley F. Palma

Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarsrepository.llu.edu/etd

Part of the Medicine and Health Sciences Commons, and the Psychology Commons

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by TheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & CreativeWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Loma Linda University Electronic Theses, Dissertations & Projects by an authorized administrator ofTheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & Creative Works. For more information, please [email protected].

Recommended CitationPalma, Imari-Ashley F., "Healthy Aging: Factors that Predict Preservation of Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults" (2018). LomaLinda University Electronic Theses, Dissertations & Projects. 518.http://scholarsrepository.llu.edu/etd/518

LOMA LINDA UNIERSITY

School of Behavioral Health

in conjunction with the

Faculty of Graduate Studies

____________________

Healthy Aging: Factors that Predict Preservation of Cognitive Functioning

in Older Adults

by

Imari-Ashley F. Palma

____________________

A Dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of

the requirements for the degree

Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology

____________________

August 2018

© 2018

Imari-Ashley F. Palma

All Rights Reserved

iii

Each person whose signature appears below certifies that this dissertation in his/her opinion

is adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree Doctor of Philosophy.

, Chairperson

Adam L. Aréchiga, Associate Professor of Psychology

Rebecca E. Ballinger, Licensed Clinical Psychologist

Grace J. Lee, Assistant Professor of Psychology

David A. Vermeersch, Professor of Psychology

iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to thank Dr. Aréchiga who has consistently supported me throughout

my graduate studies and from start to finish with my dissertation. Your mentorship,

patience, and kindness have influenced me greatly and I will forever be grateful for your

leadership and friendship.

I would also like to thank each member of my committee: Dr.’s Lee, Vermeersch,

and Ballinger. I cannot express my deepest gratitude for the help each of you have

offered me throughout this journey. I want to especially thank Dr. Lee for her dedicated

assistance with the editing and revision process. I would not have been able to

successfully complete this milestone without you.

To the most important people in my life: my friends and family, I would like to

thank them for their unconditional support, love, and encouragement for the past six

years. Finally, I would like to thank my Heavenly Father for His always present hand in

my life; carefully leading me in directions that have ultimately placed me where I am

today.

v

CONTENT

Approval Page .................................................................................................................... iii

Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................ iv

List of Figures ................................................................................................................... vii

List of Tables ................................................................................................................... viii

List of Abbreviations ......................................................................................................... ix

Abstract .............................................................................................................................. xi

Chapter

1. Introduction ............................................................................................................. 1

Exploration of a Healthy Aging Profile: Factors that Predict

Preservation of Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults ................................... 1

Age and Cognitive Functioning .................................................................. 4

The Role of Mental Health ......................................................................... 5

The Role of Physical Health ....................................................................... 7

The Role of Life Purpose ............................................................................ 9

Other Factors to Consider ......................................................................... 10

Understanding the Relationships and Current Study ................................ 11

2. Method .................................................................................................................. 14

Participants ...................................................................................................... 14

Materials ......................................................................................................... 15

Demographic Variables ............................................................................ 15

Mental Health............................................................................................ 16

Physical Health ......................................................................................... 16

Life Purpose .............................................................................................. 17

Memory ..................................................................................................... 17

Executive Functioning .............................................................................. 18

Processing Speed ...................................................................................... 19

Statistical Analysis .......................................................................................... 20

3. Results ................................................................................................................... 24

vi

4. Discussion ............................................................................................................. 29

Non-Significant Relationships ........................................................................ 31

Limitations ...................................................................................................... 32

Future Directions and Implications ................................................................. 33

Conclusion ...................................................................................................... 35

References ......................................................................................................................... 38

Appendices

A. Spiritual Index of Well-Being Scale ................................................................. 51

B. 12-Item Short Form Health Survey ................................................................... 52

vii

FIGURES

Figures Page

1. Example of standard z-score bell-curve ................................................................ 21

2. Graphical representation of moderation effect between mental health and

life purpose on processing speed .......................................................................... 27

viii

TABLES

Tables Page

1. Correlations among mental health, physical health, and cognitive domains ........ 25

2. Summary of final regression models for variables predicting cognitive

functioning. ........................................................................................................... 28

ix

ABBREVIATIONS

ARCD Age-Related Cognitive Decline

MCI Mild Cognitive Impairment

AD Alzheimer’s Disease

ADL- Activities of Daily Living

NIH National Institutes of Health

BMI Body Mass Index

MMSE Mini Mental Status Exam

MCS Mental Health Composite Score

SF-12 12-Item Short Form Health Survey

SF-36 36-Item Health Survey

M Mean

SD Standard Deviation

PCS Physical Health Composite Score

SIWB Spiritual Index of Well-Being

RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

TMT A Trails Making Test Trail A

TMT B Trails Making Test Trial B

SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test

DV Dependent Variable

IV Independent Variable

MLR Multiple Linear Regression

MH Mental Health

x

PH Physical Health

LP Life Purpose

xi

ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION

Healthy Aging: Factors that Predict Preservation of Cognitive Functioning

in Older Adults

by

Imari-Ashley F. Palma

Doctor of Philosophy, Graduate Program in Psychology

Loma Linda University, August 2018

Dr. Adam Aréchiga, Chairperson

As the human race continues to defy chronological aging in due part to medical

advancements, an overarching goal for the older adult population and researchers is to

determine how to promote aging well. Cognitive functioning is a heavily researched area

due to the exponentially increasing number of older adults experiencing cognitive

decline. Preservation of cognitive functioning is one aspect of aging well. Three domains

of cognition that have shown to be particularly sensitive to the effects of aging are

memory, executive functioning, and processing speed. The aims of this current study are:

(i) to provide supporting evidence for research that has shown a significant relationship

between mental and physical health and cognitive functioning, (ii) to provide preliminary

evidence that shows a relationship between mental and physical health and life purpose,

and (iii) to examine the relationship between life purpose and cognitive functioning.

Participants were 220 community-dwelling healthy older adults. Results showed that life

purpose moderates the relationship between mental health and cognitive functioning,

such that those who endorsed more mental health issues performed better on tasks of

processing speed when they endorsed a higher sense of life purpose versus those who

endorsed more mental health issues and a lower sense of life purpose (p < .05). There

xii

were no other significant predictors for memory, executive functioning, or processing

speed. Future research should continue to evaluate the value life purpose has on cognitive

functioning and aging well, along with what other factors predict preservation of

cognitive functioning in order to promote living well in late life.

1

CHAPTER ONE

INTRODUCTION

Exploration of a Healthy Aging Profile: Factors that Predict Preservation of

Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults

Healthy aging can be defined in multiple ways. As the human race continues to

defy chronological aging in due part to medical advancements (Olshansky, Beard, and

Börsch-Supan, 2012), the side effects of aging also continue to increase. An overarching

goal for the older adult population and researchers is to determine how to promote aging

well. Preservation of cognitive functioning is one aspect of aging well. Cognitive

functioning is a heavily researched area due to the exponentially increasing number of

older adults experiencing cognitive decline in some facet (age-related cognitive decline,

ARCD; pre-clinical dementia; mild cognitive impairment, MCI; dementia; Alzheimer’s

Disease, AD). Functional and cognitive declines in aging are normal, and it is often

difficult to fully distinguish between preclinical dementia and normal declines due to

aging (Atkinson, and Berish, 2003; Hänninen, et al., 1996; Salthouse; Smith, 2016).

Although ARCD does not always lead to dementia, researchers have shown that early

cognitive decline places one at a significant higher risk for developing dementia (Ferri et

al., 2005; Smith, 2016).

AD is one type of dementia that continues to grow in the amount of research

devoted to better understanding the risk factors, development, prevention, and treatment

(Alzheimer’s Association, 2015). There is an estimated 5 million individuals who have

AD in America (Smith, 2016). Hebert, Weuve, Scherr, and Evans (2013) stated that those

affected by AD will likely triple by 2050 (Division of Population Health, National Center

2

for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, 2015). Being over 65 years old

automatically places one at a greater risk for developing AD (Alzheimer’s Association,

2015), and up to 25%-50% of individuals will have developed, or will have shown signs

of, AD by age 85 years (Hebert, Scherr, Bienias, Bennett, and Evans, 2003). Heron

(2012) reported on the leading causes of death in 2009 and AD was seen to decrease by

4.2%, overall, which potentially revealed promising advancements in decreasing its

prevalence. Unfortunately, rates continue to increase, which is why researchers continue

to study the underlying mechanisms between aging and cognitive decline. While it is

important to further identify factors that lead to cognitive decline, and overall decline in

functioning due to aging, it is equally important to assess factors that can buffer against

early declines.

Researchers tend to operationalize successful aging as being free from physical

and mental illness/disability, maintenance of cognitive functioning, and or ability to stay

physically active (Atkinson et al, 2007; Balboa-Castillo, León-Muñoz, Graciana,

Rodríguez-Artalejo, and Gullar-Castillón, 2011; Barnes, Yaffe, Satariano, and Tager,

2003; Baumgart et al., 2015; Buchman et al., 2012; Cotman and Berchtold, 2002;

Daviglus et al., 2010; Ferreira, Owen, Mohan, Corbett, and Ballard, 2015; Scherder et al.,

2010). Reichstady, Sengupta, Depp, Palinkas, and Jeste (2010) conducted a qualitative

study that assessed how older adults operationalized successful aging and answered an

important question was answered: what do those, who are currently experiencing old age,

have to say about “successful aging?” These researchers found that, for these older

adults, self-acceptance and self-contentedness were the two major themes that were

associated with “successful aging.” These themes encompass ideas, such as, having a

3

sense of life purpose, connectedness with oneself and their environment, having a sense

of life satisfaction, and being content with who one’s life direction. There is not a lot of

research that has investigated the influence of these themes on aging, cognitive decline,

or cognitive preservation. There are some researchers who have assessed these themes in

terms of life meaning/purpose and how this influences mortality in aging (Boyle,

Buchman, Barnes, and Bennett, 2010; Reichstady, Sengupta, Depp, Palinkas, and Jeste,

2010; Krause, 2009), but it would be meaningful to investigate how this construct plays a

role in aging, and possibly cognitive functioning in late life.

As cognitive preservation is the outcome variable of interest in this study, it is

important to assess constructs that have been shown to reliably predict, or have

association with, cognitive decline and functioning across the lifespan. Physical health

and mental health functioning are two of these constructs. Researchers have consistently

shown that maintenance of better physical health is associated with better cognitive

functioning (Buchman et al., 2012; Erickson, Gildengers, and Butters, 2013; Scherder et

al., 2010). They have also shown support for significant mental health issues negatively

influencing cognitive functioning, and vice versa (Geeerlings et al., 2000; Jorm, 2000;

Steffens et al., 2006). How then do researchers interested in the promotion of aging well

integrate established research on the influence of physical health and mental health on

cognitive functioning and the hypothesis that aging well can be defined by having a sense

of life purpose? McKnight and Kashdan (2009) described the importance of fitting life

purpose into a coherent model on what it means to sustain health and well-being. They

described life purpose as, “a cognitive process that defines life goals and provides

personal meaning…that may help explain disparate empirical social science findings” (p.

4

242). Again, while there is more research devoted to assessment of how optimization of

mental health and physical health functioning predicts a decreased likelihood of cognitive

decline and impairment later in life, it would be meaningful to also assess how life

purpose later in life contributes to this growing body of research, as researchers have

shown its important role in aging well for older adults. Before doing so, it is important to

reiterate the relationships between the variables of interest (cognitive functioning, mental

health, physical health, and life purpose).

Age and Cognitive Functioning

Three domains of cognition that have shown to be particularly sensitive to the

effects of aging are memory, executive functioning, and processing speed. There are

various forms of memory, such as, working memory, short-term memory, and long-term

memory. Various brain regions work together to ensure proper memory functioning

including the hippocampus, amygdala, cingulate gyrus, thalamus, hypothalamus,

epithalamus, and mammillary body. All of these regions make up the limbic system,

which is in the medial temporal lobe of the brain (Erickson, Gildengers, and Butters,

2013; Mastin, 2010). Executive functioning encompasses multiple processes such as

regulatory control, working memory, reasoning, problem solving, and planning

(Salthouse, Atkinson, and Berish, 2003). It is typically associated with the prefrontal

cortex (PFC) in the frontal lobe region of the brain (Stuss and Alexander, 2000).

Cognitive processing speed is a term used to describe how efficiently one is able to

examine and complete a simple cognitive task (Deary, Johnson, and Starr, 2010).

Salthouse (1996) theorized that processing speed is the underlying, and entirely separate,

5

domain of cognitive functioning that experiences initial decline, which therefore

adversely influences other cognitive domains such as executive functioning and memory.

Processing speed has been shown to be associated with white matter of the left middle

frontal gyrus, bilateral parietal lobes, and bilateral temporal lobes. White matter fiber

tracts have been hypothesized to play an integral role in the transmission of information

and control signaling, which are necessary for efficiency in overall cognitive performance

tasks (Turken et al., 2008). While many research studies have assessed the relationship

between specific types of variables (e.g., physiological, structural, social, environmental,

psychological) and cognition, it is important to understand a person wholly and how an

individual’s different domains contribute to healthy aging and cognitive preservation.

The Role of Mental Health

The construct of mental health encompasses a variety of factors. Psychological

pathology is one aspect of poor mental health that has been continuously been linked to

acute and chronic cognitive decline and impairment in the literature (Geerlings et al.,

2000; Jorm, 2000; Starkstein, Mayberg, Leiguarda, Preziosi, and Robinson, 1992;

Steffems et al., 2006; Wilson et al., 2002). Psychological pathology can be described as

having symptoms that met criteria for any of the Diagnostic Statistical Manual, Fifth

Edition (2013) disorders. Mental health has also been interchangeably described as well-

being, quality of life, and general happiness. These terms pertaining to mental health are

operationalized in various ways in the literature. Engagement in social activities and

socialization with others is one operationalization of mental health. Researchers, in

general, have shown that regular engagement in social activity and connectedness with

6

others is associated with greater health benefits and decreased likelihood of cognitive

decline in older age (Bassuk, Glass, and Berkman, 1999; Béland, Zunzunegui, Alvarado,

Otero, and del Ser, 2005; Cracchiolo et al., 2007; Dickinson, Potter, Hybels, McQuoid,

and Steffens, 2011; Everard, Lach, Fisher, and Baum, 2000; Holahan and Holahan, 1987;

James, Wilson, Barnes, and Bennett, 2011; Kawachi and Berkman, 2001; Leung, Orrell,

and Orgeta, 2015; Yeh and Liu, 2003; Zunzunegui, Alvarado, Del Ser, and Otero, 2003).

Decreases in social engagement has been shown to be associated with worse cognitive

performance over time, which supports the notion that an enriched environment of being

surrounded by others can strengthen cognition (Small, Dixon, McArdelle, and Grimm,

2012).

Mental health has also been operationalized as subjective well-being. Research in

the area of chronic illness and palliative care has shown that higher levels of subjective

well-being is associated with higher levels of general contentedness of life and more of a

positive outlook, which does not necessarily slow down the progression of the chronic

illness or mortality, but can ameliorate the impact of the pain and or dying process

(Boyle, Buchman, Barnes, and Bennett, 2010; Krause, 2009; Reichstady, Sengupta,

Depp, Palinkas, and Jeste, 2010; Wong, 2007). The absence of chronic sadness, or having

more frequent feelings of happiness, is another definition of good mental health. Again,

research continues to provide supporting evidence for greater feelings of happiness being

linked to a myriad of positive health benefits and longevity (Crivelli, Della Bella, and

Lucchini, 2016; Post, 2005; Røysamb, Tambs, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Neale, and Harris,

2003). It is evident that good mental health later in life can benefit an individual in

multiple ways; therefore, it would be important to continue to assess how to strengthen

7

the benefits of good mental health in order to increase its efficacy on preservation of

cognitive functioning in late life.

The Role of Physical Health

Another aspect of health includes physical health. While physical health

experiences natural decline during aging, researchers continues to consistently support a

significant positive association between physical health and cognitive functioning

(Atkinson et al., 2007; Barnes, Yaffe, Satariano, and Tager, 2003; Cotman and Berchtold,

2002; Grigsby et al., 1998; Hultsch, Hammer, and Small, 1993; Hultsch, Hertzog, Small,

and Dixon, 1999). Physical health is operationalized in various ways across research

studies. Grigsby, Kaye, Baxter, Shatterly, and Hamman (1998) researched the importance

of executive functioning ability in aging individuals on activities of daily living (ADLs),

ADLs being a measure of physical health. They concluded that aging adults who had

higher performance on measures of executive functioning also scored higher on measures

of ADLs. Hultsch, Hertzog, Small, and Dixon (1999) also showed that active engagement

in various forms of ADLs buffered against cognitive decline through consistent

intellectual stimulation.

Researchers have also used self-report measures of health and level of activity to

assess the relationship between physical health and cognitive functioning. One study

found that self-reports of health and activity level were predictive of measures of

cognitive functioning in older adults, and particularly for processing speed ability

(Hultsch, Hammer, and Small, 1993). The presence or absence of physical illness is

another way physical health has been operationalized. Tilvis et al. (2004) sought out to

8

assess the predictive value of various physical health risk factors of cognitive decline.

Their results showed that “general ill health” was significantly correlated with cognitive

declines in individuals 75 years and older. Taken together, this evidence suggests that

despite the way physical health is defined, there appears to be consistent research that

supports a significant direct association with cognitive functioning in older adults.

Physical health has also been operationalized as level of fitness, or engagement in

physical activity. Engagement in leisure activities falls under a wide spectrum (e.g., low

to high levels of physical activity). As is evident, operationalizing leisure activities is

highly subjective, but studies continue to support that higher endorsed levels are

associated with higher levels of cognitive preservation (Ferreira, Owen, Mohan, Corbett,

and Ballard, 2015; Fritsch, McClendon, Smyth, Lerner, Friedland, and Larsen, 2007;

Scatmeas and Stern, 2003). Physical health can also be defined by level of fitness in older

adults such as cardiorespiratory fitness. Barnes, Yaffe, Satarino, and Tager (2003)

assessed the predictive value of baseline health on cognitive functioning in older adults.

They found that higher baseline cardiorespiratory fitness predicted maintenance of

cognitive functioning. Other researchers have more specifically defined physical health in

older adults as the efficiency of mobility. Atkinson et al. (2007) investigated the

multifaceted relationships between cognitive functioning, gait speed, and other factors of

mental health (e.g., depression). Their results showed support for cognitive decline being

predictive of a decline in gait speed. This association was reduced by depression, which

indicated a need to further understand the mechanisms and underlying relationships

between physical health and cognitive functioning in older adults.

9

The Role of Life Purpose

The role of life purpose throughout the lifespan has gained more attention in the

literature, including its effects on cognitive functioning in older adults (Lavretsky, 2010).

Life purpose has been defined and measured in various ways. Spirituality, or belief in a

higher power and structuring one’s life goals around service to, or promotion of, this

higher power is one way “life purpose” has been defined (Daaleman and Frey, 2004).

Life purpose has also been measured as an individual’s internalized belief that he or she

has the ability and resources to effectively carry out the necessary steps to obtain certain

goals (Bandura, 1993). The two aforementioned definitions of life purpose are similar. It

is helpful to look at the later as having more of an intrinsic mechanism and the former as

having more of an influence from a transcendent source. Negative perceptions about

aging, and the notion that life purpose decreases as age increases, have been shown to

have a significantly negative influence on cognitive functioning in older adults

(Robertson, King-Killimanis, and Kenny, 2016). Researchers have also shown that

increased life purpose in older adults is associated with maintenance of physical health

and decrease in mental health concerns (Holahan and Holahan, 1987; Mendes de Leon,

1996; Seeman, Unger, McAvy, and Mendes de Leon, 1999). More specific research on

cognitive functioning in older adults has shown a significant direct association between

life purpose and cognitive functioning as well (Seeman, McAvay, Merrill, Albert, and

Rodin, 1996). In addition, researchers have shown that increased levels of life purpose

may also slow down progression of cognitive decline in those developing AD (Coin et

al., 2010; Kaufman, Anaki, Binns, and Freedman, 2007).

10

It is important to continue to add to this body of research in order to better

understand the influence of life purpose. Some researchers have assessed the importance

of a positive sense of self in aging and that this helps explain better functional health in

older adults (Levy, Slade, and Kasi, 2002). Increased perceived self-efficacy and positive

attitude about aging in general can influence how one interacts with his or her

environment, which may also lower the risk of functional health decline. The

mechanisms by which these relationships operate are likely numerous; therefore, it is

important to further investigate how these factors relate to cognitive functioning, and

overall aging.

Other Factors to Consider

Research on cognitive functioning in older adults is vast and continuously

growing. The general goal for researchers is to better understand the mechanisms

underlying cognitive decline and cognitive preservation in order to better inform

interventions that help make the aging process less burdensome. The variables

aforementioned only contribute to a fraction of better understanding the mechanisms that

may help explain, or support, preservation of cognitive functioning in older adults. Other

important factors to consider when investigating cognitive functioning include level of

education and age. Researchers have found maintenance of cognitive functioning in older

adults can be predicted through age and having a higher level of education (Liévre, Alley,

and Crimmins, 2008; Yaffe et al., 2009). Increasing age, within itself, decreases an

individual’s likelihood to maintain baseline cognitive functioning later in life

(Alzheimer’s Association, 2015). Early education promotes strengthening of cognitive

11

resources during a key developmental period, which contributes to lessening the

influence of ARCD (Zahodne, Stern, and Manly, 2015). A higher level of education has

also been shown to contribute to increased cognitive reserve (León, García-García, and

Roldán-Tapia, 2016; Stern, 2002; Stern, 2003; Stern, 2009; Tucker and Stern, 2011;

Whalley, Deary, Appleton, and Starr, 2004). Cognitive reserve is related to resiliency.

The theory of cognitive reserve originated from the observation that there was a

discrepancy between brain pathology and cognitive performance in older adults (León,

García-García, and Roldán-Tapia, 2016; Stern, 2002; Stern, 2003; Stern, 2009; Tucker

and Stern, 2011; Whalley, Deary, Appleton, and Starr, 2004). There is on-going research

on further understanding what factors contribute to cognitive reserve, how to best

measure it, if it can be strengthened over time, and or if there is a pivotal time when it is

established. Cognitive reserve is made up of various factors, such as, baseline

intelligence, years of education, occupation, engagement in leisure activities (physically

and intellectually stimulating), and social involvement (Ferreira, Owen, Mohan, Corbett,

and Ballard, 2015; Scatmeas and Stern, 2003; Stern, Albert, Tang, and Tsai, 1942; León,

García-García, and Roldán-Tapia, 2016; Richards and Sacker, 2010; Zahodne, Stern, and

Manly, 2015). In other words, all of the variables of interest in this study are included

under the umbrella of cognitive reserve.

Understanding the Relationships and Current Study

The National Institute of Health (NIH) (2010) has recognized the increasing rates

of dementia and ARCD in older adults as a serious issue that needs further research to

investigate ways to help prevent or attenuate declines. Cognitive functioning is complex;

12

it encompasses attention, learning, memory, language, visuospatial skills, and executive

functioning (Daviglus et al., 2010). The 2010 NIH report on AD and ARCD synthesized

that researchers continues to show that: (1) poor health (e.g., high blood pressure,

diabetes) has been linked to cognitive decline but more well-designed studies need to be

conducted to assess other medical and health related factors; (2) there is limited and

inconsistent research findings on the associations between higher levels of social and

cognitive engagement (i.e., strengthening of mental health) in later life on cognitive

decline; and (3) the benefits of physical fitness on cognition is still in the preliminary

stages (Daviglus et al., 2010). The importance of continued investigation in this area of

research is to “bridge the gap” between research and practice, and to add another

dimension to the current research on these variables and cognition, life purpose. Small

(2016) mentioned the importance of translating what it is known about cognitive

functioning into clinical practice of detection and prevention. He emphasized the

importance of educating the community about factors that can help to buffer against

decline and ways to recognize early signs. The aims of this current study are: (i) to

provide supporting evidence for research that has shown a significant relationship

between mental and physical health and cognitive functioning, (ii) to provide preliminary

evidence that shows a relationship between mental and physical health and life purpose,

and (iii) to examine the relationship between life purpose and cognitive functioning. In

terms of directionality, it is hypothesized that:

1. Mental health and life purpose are directly correlated.

2. Physical health and life purpose are directly correlated.

3. Mental health and cognitive functioning are directly correlated.

13

4. Physical health and cognitive functioning are directly correlated.

5. Life purpose and cognitive functioning are directly correlated.

6. Less endorsement of mental health issues predicts average to high average

cognitive functioning.

7. Less endorsement of physical health issues predicts average to high average

cognitive functioning.

8. Higher levels of perceived life purpose predict average to high average cognitive

functioning.

9. Life purpose moderates the relationship between mental/physical health and

cognitive functioning, such that better perceived mental/physical health had a

much stronger effect on cognitive functioning when an individual had greater

perceived life purpose.

14

CHAPTER TWO

METHOD

Participants

Participants were 365 community-dwelling older adults who were recruited for a larger

randomized, single blind, dual center, controlled study under the direction of the Loma

Linda University Department of Nutrition. The larger study’s aim was to collect baseline

data in order to assess what impact daily ingestion of walnuts, over a two year time period,

would have on ARCD and macular degeneration in a healthy aging population. In brief,

walnuts contain long n-3 fatty-acids (LC n-3 PUFA), which have been postulated to be

integral during brain development and prevention, or slowing, of ARCD and age-related

macular degeneration (AMD). The primary aim was to investigate factors that prevent or

slow down ARCD and AMD in healthy aging adults (Sabaté and Rajaram, 2011). For the

current study, baseline data from the Loma Linda University site was used. Participants

were recruited via flyers and posters posted in geriatric clinics, site hospitals, local

churches, and community centers. Exclusion criteria included:

Illiteracy or inability to understand the protocol or undergo neuropsychological

tests.

Morbid obesity (BMI 40 kg/m2).

Uncontrolled diabetes (HbA1c >8%).

Uncontrolled hypertension (on-treatment blood pressure ≥150/100 mm Hg).

Prior cerebrovascular accident.

Any relevant psychiatric illness, including major depression.

Advanced cognitive deterioration, dementia.

15

Other neurodegenerative diseases (i.e., Parkinson’s disease).

Any chronic illness expected to shorten survival (heart failure, chronic liver disease,

kidney failure, blood disease, cancer).

Bereavement in the first year of loss.

Bad dentures unless fixable dental prostheses are used.

Allergy to walnuts.

Customary use of fish oil or flaxseed oil supplements.

Eye-related exclusion criteria.

Participants were informed about the study protocol at an initial meeting in where informed

consent was obtained, clinical history was taken, a physical examination was done, and the

Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) was administered to screen for potential cognitive

impairment. Participants who had a MMSE score < 24 were referred to a physician and

were not eligible for participation (Sabaté and Rajaram, 2011). At baseline, there were a

total of 365 participants who met inclusion criteria, were recruited and completed all

baseline measurements and testing.

Materials

Demographic Variables

Participants completed a clinical history questionnaire at the outset of the study,

which included age (M = 69, SD = 3.17), gender (67.1% female), ethnicity (77.2%

White, 10.3% Hispanic, 6.9% Black, 4.2% Asian, 1.4% Other) and level of education in

years (M = 15.96, SD = 2.50).

16

Mental Health

The Mental Health Composite Score (MCS) subscale on the 12-Item Short Form

Health Survey (Ware, Kosinski, and Keller, 1996; SF-12) was used to measure mental

health. The SF-12 is an abbreviated version of the SF-36. Its main purpose is it to

measure multiple health dimensions (Utah Department of Health, 2001, p. 3). It has been

shown to be a reliable and valid measure of general health when assessing a large sample

(American Thoracic Society, 2007). For this study, higher scores indicate better mental

health. It has been reported that sample norms for older adults between ages 65 year –

75+ years range from 55.0 – 56.5 (Utah Department of Health, 1996). Studies have

shown that average scores on the SF-12 subscales change overtime with age and type of

population being assessed (e.g., healthy versus sick individuals, young adults versus older

adults). MCS scores have been shown to generally increase overtime (Ware, Kosinski,

and Keller, 1996; Utah Department of Health, 2001). Therefore, it is important to obtain

an overall mean for the sample under investigation in order to generate a meaningful

interpretation. Gandek et al. (1998) reported the test-retest reliability of the MCS to be

0.76 in a sample assessed in the United States. The majority of the sample for this study

was within the average ranges on the MCS (M = 56.26, SD = 5.74).

Physical Health

The Physical Composite Scores (PCS) subscale on the 12-Item Short Form Health

Survey (Ware, Kosinski, and Keller, 1996; SF-12) was used to measure physical health.

Higher scores indicate better physical health. It has been reported that sample norms for

older adults between ages 65 year – 75+ years range from 42.7 – 44.6 (Utah Department

17

of Health, 1996). PCS scores tend to decrease overtime (Ware, Kosinski, and Keller,

1996; Utah Department of Health, 2001); therefore, it is important to obtain an overall

mean for the sample under investigation in order to generate a meaningful interpretation.

Gandek et al. (1998) reported the test-retest reliability of the PCS to be 0.89 in a sample

assessed in the United States. The majority of the sample for this study was fell within

above average ranges on the PCS (M = 50.37, SD = 8.04).

Life Purpose

The Life Scheme subscale on the 12-item Spiritual Index of Well Being

(Daaleman, Cobb, and Frey, 2001; SIWB) was used to measure life purpose (see

Appendix A) It is divided into two sub-scales: self-efficacy and life-scheme. A Likert

scale ranging from one (“Strongly Agree”) to five (“Strongly Disagree”) is utilized on the

SIWB. Higher overall scores indicate higher perceived life purpose. Daaleman, Frey,

Wallance, and Studenski (2002) reported the 12-item SIWB to have a reliability of 0.80

on Life Scheme subscale. For this sample, the minimum score for Life Purpose was 16,

while the maximum score was 35 (M = 31.92, SD = 4.12).

Memory

The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Rey, 1941) was used to assess

memory. In this measure, a list of 15 unrelated words are repeated five times (List A).

Between each trial, the participant is asked to repeat as many words as he or she can

recall. An additional list of 15 unrelated words are read after the multiple repetition of the

original list, and the participant is asked to repeat as many words as he or she can recall

18

from this new list (List B). The participant is then asked to recall as many of the words

from the original list as he or she can without repetition from the administrator. After a

30-minute interval, the participant is asked to verbally repeat, and visually recognize, as

many words as he or she can recall from the original list once more. Psychometrically,

the RAVLT has been shown to have good validity and reliability with a Cronbach’s

Alpha coefficient of 0.80 (de Sousa Magalhães, Fernandes Malloy-Diniz, and Cavalheiro

Hamdan, 2012). The RAVLT has also been shown to be effective in early identification

of AD in individuals with subjective memory complaints, and in differentiation between

preclinical phase AD patients, MCI individuals, and normal aging individuals (Estévez-

González et al., 2003). In a healthy population between the ages of 24-81 years,

performance on the RAVLT was shown to decrease with age, but females and

participants with a higher education tended to perform better compared to males and

participants with lower education, across all age ranges (Van der Elst, van Boxtel, van

Breukelen, & Jolles, 2005). For the current study, the total scores for Trials 1-5 (M = 49,

SD = 10.10) were converted to standard z-scores using the sample mean. The Delayed

Recall scores (M = 9.87, SD = 3.35) were also converted to standard z-scores using the

sample mean. The two standard z-scores were then added and averaged to obtain the final

score used for memory (M = 0.00, SD = 1.43).

Executive Functioning

The Trails Making Test, Trail B (TMT B) and the Stroop Color Word Test

(Stroop, 1935) were combined to measure executive functioning. The TMT is a widely

used neuropsychological test of visual attention and task switching. TMT B is used as a

19

measure of executive functioning, as participants are required to organize information

efficiently and rapidly in order to complete the task successfully (Tombaugh, 2004).

Psychometrically, the TMT B has been shown to have good retest reliability for TMT B

(0.86-0.94) (Wagner, Helmreich, Dahmen, Lieb, and Tadic, 2011). The TMT has also

been shown to be negatively correlated with age and level of education (Tombaugh,

2004). The Stroop Color Word Test is a well-known and heavily used

neuropsychological test that measures cognitive interference (Scarpina and Tagini, 2017).

The construct can also measure executive functioning, as it requires the examinee to read

aloud the color a word is printed in, and inhibit themselves from reading the actual name

of the color printed (e.g., if the word “RED” is printed in the color blue, the examinee

must say, “blue”). The Stroop Color Word Test has been shown to have good test-retest

reliability at 0.86 (Siegrist, 1997). For the current study, the scores in seconds for TMT B

(M = 95.59, SD = 47.91) were converted to standard z-scores using the sample mean and

negative/positive signs were reversed via multiplication by -1 to increase meaningful and

standard interpretation. The Stroop Color Word scores, in number of correctly stated

color names (M = 34.52, SD = 8.56), were also converted to standard z-scores using the

sample mean. The two standard z-scores were then added and averaged to obtain the final

score used for executive functioning (M = 0.00, SD = 1.25).

Processing Speed

The Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT; Smith, 1968; Smith 1982) and the

Trails Making Test, Trail A (TMT-A) were combined to measure cognitive processing

speed. The SDMT was developed to assess for neurological impairment. It is also widely

20

used to assess attention, visual scanning, and motor speed (Sheridan et al., 2006). The

participants are instructed that they will have 90 seconds to pair specific numbers with

given symbols (a key of which numbers match to any given symbol is listed at the top of

the page). Sheridan et al. (2006) reported this single score measure to have a test/retest

reliability and correlation with the Wechsler Digit Symbol Test, a similar measure, of .80

in a normal sample. Benedict et al. (2012) also reported the SDMT to have a test/retest

reliability of .80. The TMT is a widely used neuropsychological test of visual attention

and task switching. TMT-A can measure processing speed as it requires the examinee to

connect scattered numbers on a paper using a pencil, in consecutive order, as fast as

possible. The TMT-A has been shown to have good test-retest reliability between 0.76-

0.82 (Wanger, Helmreich, Dahmen, Lieb, and Tadíc, 2011). For the current study, the

scores in seconds for TMT-A (M = 34.09, SD = 11.77) were converted to standard z-

scores using the sample mean and negative/positive signs were reversed via

multiplication by -1 to increase meaningful and standard interpretation. The SDMT

scores, in number of correctly paired symbols to numbers (M = 47.97, SD = 9.08), were

also converted to standard z-scores using the sample mean. The two standard z-scores

were then added and averaged to obtain the final score used for processing speed (M =

0.01, SD = 1.31).

Statistical Analysis

SPSS 20.0 was used to perform all statistical analyses. The outcome variables, or

dependent variables (DVs), were combined as described above, by converting each score

in a standard z-score and taking the average of the two scores for each DV. Higher z-

21

scores, more specifically z-scores equal to or greater than -0.5, indicate average to above

average memory (see Figure 1).

Figure 1. Example of standard z-score bell-curve (Neurofeedback Center of Rochester

(2018).

Hierarchical multiple linear regression (MLR) was used to analyze the hypothesized

predictive value of the above specified IVs on the DVs. Outliers were checked for using

the calculated D-Cooks, DFBETA and DFFITS values. If any participant exceeded the

standard cut off score of 1.0 (psych.unl.edu, 2018), that participant was deleted for the

given model. In addition, participants who did not complete baseline measures for any of

the variables of interest were excluded from the study. Many participants did not

complete the SF-12 questionnaire or SIWB. After checking for outliers and including

22

only participants that completed all baseline measure of interest in this study, there were

a total of 220 participants who were included. After conducting a Power Analysis for this

sample size, the analysis showed that there was very good statistical power (α = 0.80) to

detect an anticipated effect size of 0.80 (Soper, 2018). Multicollinearity was also

checked. Nonessential multicollinearity, or high correlations between main effects and

interaction terms, can change the regression coefficients’ magnitude and sign, standard

errors can become large, and interpretation of coefficients or significance tests can

become less accurate (Morrell, 2013).

Pearson’s r correlational analyses for each model were conducted to analyze the

hypothesized correlations between variables. Three separate hierarchical MLR analyses

were conducted to investigate the three cognitive domains (memory, executive

functioning, and processing speed). Before analysis, each continuous IV was centered to

increase meaningful interpretation of results. Centering predictor variables (𝑋𝑖– 𝑋¯

) helped

to remove nonessential multicollinearity and reduce correlations among predictors.

(Morrell, 2013). It also helped to create a more meaningful interpretation. For each

model, age and years of education were entered in step one of the hierarchical MLR to

control for the effects of these covariates. Mental health, physical health, and life purpose

were entered in the second step to test for main effects. All moderating relationships

(Mental health x Life purpose, Mental health x Physical health, and Life purpose x

Physical health) were entered in step three. The first hierarchical MLR model tested the

following equation:

23

Memory = B0 + B1(Mental health) + B2(Physical health) + B3(Life purpose) +

B4(Mental health x Life purpose) + B5(Mental health x Physical health) + B6(Life

purpose x Physical health).

The second MLR model tested the following equation:

Executive functioning = B0 + B1(Mental health) + B2(Physical health) + B3(Life

purpose) + B4(Mental health x Life purpose) + B5(Mental health x Physical

health) + B6(Life purpose x Physical health).

The third MLR model tested the following equation:

Processing speed = B0 + B1(Mental health) + B2(Physical health) + B3(Life

purpose) + B4(Mental health x Life purpose) + B5(Mental health x Physical

health) + B6(Life purpose x Physical health).

24

CHAPTER THREE

RESULTS

For this study, the participants’ ages ranged between 62–80 years of age (M = 69,

SD = 3.17). Participants were majority female (67.1% female) and identified as White

(77.2% White, 10.3% Hispanic, 6.9% Black, 4.2% Asian, 1.4% Other). This sample’s

level of education ranged between 8–20 years (M = 15.96, SD = 2.50). The IVs for this

study included, mental health (M = 56.26, SD = 5.74), physical health (M = 50.37, SD =

8.04), and life purpose (M = 31.92, SD = 4.11). The DVs for this study, measured in z-

scores, included, memory (M = 0.00, SD = 1.43), executive functioning (M = 0.00, SD =

1.25), and processing speed (M = 0.01, SD = 1.31).

Pearson’s r correlation analyses were conducted to test the first five hypotheses.

Hypothesis one, mental health and life purpose are directly correlated, was not supported

(p > .05). Hypothesis two, physical health and life purpose, was supported (r = 0.188, p <

.01), meaning that for every one-point increase in perceived life purpose, there is a 0.188

increase in perceived physical health. Hypothesis three, mental health and cognitive

functioning are directly correlated, was not supported (p > .05). Hypothesis four, physical

health and cognitive functioning are directly correlated, was not supported (p > .05).

Hypothesis five, life purpose and cognitive functioning are directly correlated was not

supported (p > .05). See Table 1 for a summary of results.

25

Table 1. Correlations among mental health, physical health, and cognitive domains.

1

2

3

4

5

6

M

SD

1.Memorya - .305** .281** -.019 .089 -.015 000 1.427

2. Executive functioningb - - .575** .021 .100 .082 .003 1.249

3. Processing speedc - - - .115 .021 .076 .006 1.306

4. Mental healthd

5. Physical healthe

6. Life purpose f

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-.149*

-

-

-.040

.188**

-

56.26

50.37

31.92

5.742

8.043

4.106

N = 220. *p < .05. **p < .01. aMemory measured using the combined scores from the sum of trials 1-5 on the RAVLT

and RAVLT Delayed Recall. Z-score transformation was used to standardize the combined

score. bExecutive functioning measured using the combined scores from the TMT Trail B and

Digit Span on the WAIS-III. Z-score transformation was used to standardize the combined

score. cProcessing speed measured using the combined scores from the SDMT and sum of

the F, A, and S trials of the COWAT. Z-score transformation was used to standardize the

combined score. dMental health measured using the MCS subscale on the SF-12. For this study, higher

scores indicate less endorsed mental health concerns. ePhysical health measured using the PCS subscale on the SF-12. For this study, higher

scores indicate less endorsed physical health concerns. fLife purpose measured using the Life-scheme subscale on the Subjective Index of Well-

being questionnaire. For this study, higher scores indicate higher perceived life purpose.

Hierarchical MLR was used to assess hypotheses six, seven, eight, and nine: the

hypothesized main effects and moderating relationships between mental health, physical

health, and life purpose on the three domains of cognition (memory, executive

functioning, and processing speed). Hypothesis six, less endorsement of mental health

issues predicts average to high average cognitive functioning, was not supported for the

memory, executive functioning, or processing speed models (p > .05). Hypothesis seven,

26

less endorsement of physical health issues predicts average to high average cognitive

functioning, was not supported for the memory, executive functioning, or processing

speed models (p > .05). Hypothesis eight, higher levels of perceived life purpose predict

average to high average cognitive functioning, was not supported for the memory,

executive functioning, or processing speed models (p > .05). Hypothesis nine, life

purpose moderates the relationship between mental/physical health and cognitive

functioning, such that better perceived mental/physical health had a much stronger effect

on cognitive functioning when an individual had greater perceived life purpose, was

partially supported for the processing speed model. There was a significant interaction

effect between mental health and life purpose on processing speed (B = -0.011, t = -

3.069, p < .05, 95% CI [-0.018, -0.004]). The optimal linear combination of mental

health and life purpose uniquely accounted for 3.7% of the total variance in processing

speed performance (sr2 = 0.037). Processing speed performance was different at different

levels of mental health issue endorsement and perceived life purpose endorsement. In

other words, higher levels of perceived life purpose explained the relationship between

endorsement of more mental health issues and better performance on measures of

processing speed (see Figure 2).

27

Figure 2. Graphical representation of moderation effect between mental health and

life purpose on processing speed.

Hypothesis nine was not supported for the memory or executive functioning models (p >

.05). See Table 2 for a summary of all final regression models.

-15.

-10.

-5.

0.

5.

10.

15.

Less (-1 SD) More (+1 SD)

Pro

cess

ing

Sp

eed

Mental Health Issues

Less Life Purpose Endorsement (-1 SD)

28

Table 2. Summary of final regression models for variables predicting cognitive functioning.

95% Confidence

Interval for B

Model B SE B β sr2 pr2 Lower

Bound

Upper

Bound

Memorya

Mental health (MH) -.011 .017 -.044 .002 .002 -.045 .023

Physical health (PH) .014 .012 .079 .006 .005 -.011 .038

Life purpose (LP) -.018 .022 -.056 .003 .003 -.062 .026

MH x PH -.001 .002 -.046 .002 .002 -.004 .002

MH x LP .005 .004 .075 .005 .005 -.004 .013

PH x LP -.001 .002 -.049 .002 .002 -.006 .003

R2adjusted .036

F 2.036*

Executive functioningb

Mental health (MH) .004 .015 .018 ..000 .000 -,026 .033

Physical health (PH) .016 .011 .100 .009 .010 -.006 .037

Life purpose (LP) .008 .020 .029 .001 .001 -.030 .047

MH x PH -.003 .001 -.130 .015 .017 -.005 .000

MH x LP -.005 .004 -.085 .006 .007 -.012 .003

PH x LP -.001 .002 -.035 .001 .001 -.005 .003

R2adjusted

.084

F 3.512**

Processing speedc

Mental health (MH)

.027

.015

.122

.014

.016

-.002

.056

Physical health (PH) .002 .010 .013 .000 .000 -.019 .023

Life purpose (LP) .008 .019 .026 .000 .001 -.030 .045

MH x PH -.001 .001 -.035 .001 .001 -.003 .002

MH x LP -.011** .004 -.205 .037 .043 -.018 -.004

PH x LP -.003 .002 -.107 .010 .012 -.007 .001

R2adjusted .133

F 5.202***

Note. Unstandardized B values represent directionality of relationship. aMemory measured using the combined scores from the sum of trials 1-5 on the RAVLT and RAVLT

Delayed Recall. Z-score transformation was used to standardize the combined score. bExecutive functioning measured using the combined scores from the TMT Trail B and Digit Span on the

WAIS-III. Z-score transformation was used to standardize the combined score. cProcessing speed measured

using the combined scores from the SDMT and sum of the F, A, and S trials of the COWAT. Z-score

transformation was used to standardize the combined score.

*p < .05, ** p < .01; memory model (n = 205), executive functioning model (n = 207), processing speed

model (n = 207).

29

CHAPTER FOUR

DISCUSSION

Results of this study showed partial support for the outlined aims and hypotheses.

Hypothesis two, physical health and life purpose are directly correlated, was supported in

this study. There have been recent studies that have investigated the influence that an

increased sense of life purpose can have on physical health in older adults. These

researchers have highlighted the importance of assessing how psychological strengths can

act as possible aids in maintaining good physical health in late life, as opposed to

continuing to focus on how psychological distress and psychiatric disorders negatively

influence physical health in late life. These studies have shown that a higher sense of life

purpose has a relationship with improved physical functioning for older adults (Kim,

Kawachi, Chen, and Kubzansky, 2017; Ryff, 2017). These researchers have mentioned

that there may be other underlying mediating factors, such as the possibility that older

adults who have a greater sense of life purpose may have more motivation to be social,

engage in activities, and be health conscious. Still, these relationships need to be further

explored.

The hypothesis that life purpose moderates the relationship between

mental/physical health and cognitive functioning was partially supported. The moderating

effect of life purpose was only significant for the relationship between mental health and

cognitive processing speed, specifically. As stated prior, McKnight and Kashdan (2009)

endorsed the notion that life purpose is a cognitive process within itself that essentially

helps to exercise an individual’s mental toughness, ability to plan ahead, and ability to

goal set. It can be argued that regularly engaging in cognitive processes that involve

30

higher perceived life purpose can help maintain cognitive processing speed. This theory

would need to be further explored, but may help explain the significant relationship found

in this study. Boyle, Buchman, Barnes, and Bennett (2010) conducted a study to

investigate the role of life purpose on reduced risk for AD. In their longitudinal study,

they found that having a greater sense of life purpose was associated with a lower

incidence of developing AD in comparison to those individuals who developed AD, or

signs/symptoms of AD and did not have a sense of life purpose. While this association

does not necessarily indicate that life purpose will prevent the development of AD or

cognitive decline in general, it does exemplify a relationship between these two

constructs that should not be ignored. This study, conducted by

Boyle, Buchman, Barnes, and Bennett (2010), is one of the first of its kind to look at how

life purpose, a construct that is not biological, physiological, medical, or tangible, can

significantly influence something that is (i.e., cognitive decline in later life).

While all of the final models for memory performance, executive functioning

performance, and processing speed performance were significant, only about 3%, 8%,

and 13% of the variance was explained for each model. This suggests that the variables of

interest in this current study add a small percentage of meaningful predictive value to

what help preserve cognitive functioning in older age, but more research needs to be done

to investigate what other variables may contribute to preservation of cognitive

functioning in older adults. Results did show that life purpose was a significant moderator

for the relationship between mental health and life purpose for the processing speed

model, as explained above.

31

Non-Significant Relationships

As shown above, many of the hypothesized relationships were not supported. For

the majority, the literature does support the idea that better mental health and better

physical health are associated with less cognitive decline and better cognitive

performance (Baumgart et al., 2015; Buchman, Boyle, Yu, Shah, Wilson, and Bennett,

2012; Cracchiolo et al., 2007; Lautenschlager et al., 2008; Lee, 2000). It is important to

look at how mental health, physical health, and cognitive decline/performance are

defined in the literature assessing the relationships between these variables, as it can

change the interpretations and, therefore, the conceptualization of these relationships. In

terms of research in the area of mental health and cognitive functioning, there appears to

be more evidence that supports anxiety as more of a predictive factor for development of

cognitive impairment (Beaudreau and O’Hara, 2009; Mantella, 2007). In this study,

anxiety was not specifically measured. The DV, mental health, may have provided more

meaningful information if an anxiety measure was added.

Seeman, Lusignolo, Albert, and Berkman (2001) showed that, in their

longitudinal study, a high baseline of social support predicted better cognitive functioning

at the follow-up assessment. In this current study, social engagement was a small

component in how mental health was measured, but it was not particularly highlighted;

therefore, the specific influence of social engagement on cognitive performance could not

be teased apart. It could be that social support is a more meaningful variable to assess

when investigating how to preserve cognitive functioning in older adults.

For this study, how mental health and physical health were operationalized could

have been improved by utilizing more specific measures. For example, measures of

32

depression, anxiety, health-related anxiety, quality of life, ADLs, physiological

measurements that meet criteria for good health in older adults (e.g., cardiovascular

fitness, low blood pressure, healthy body mass index). Using the SF-12 may not have

been sensitive enough to detect meaningful significant relationships between health and

cognitive performance in the sample used. To reiterate, because this sample was healthier

than the general older adults population, more sensitive measures would have helped to

detect smaller but more valuable information.

Life purpose also may not have been measured in the best way possible for this

study. This study was somewhat exploratory in terms of investigating how the construct

of life purpose fits into the relationship between health and cognitive functioning in older

adults. It could be hypothesized that life purpose influences other constructs that fall

underneath the “mental health umbrella,” such as happiness, general quality of life, or

self-worth for example. Or, life purpose could be influencing other constructs, such as,

level of motivation, self-efficacy, and resiliency. In this study, a higher sense of life

purpose did explain better performance on processing speed when participants endorsed

more mental health issues, which means that the construct of life purpose could be a

meaningful variable to continue to assess in terms of how else it effects aging and

possible cognitive functioning.

Limitations

Establishing causal relationships was not possible in this study for multiple

reasons. Methodologically, cross-sectional data does not establish a pre-determined time

sequence of events, which would need to occur to confidently establish that x causes y

33

(Deary, Johnson, and Starr, 2010). Also, given the literature on mental health, psychical

health, life purpose, and cognitive functioning, including this current study, it cannot be

said that one construct causes the other, as these constricts are constantly changing for

individuals over time, across the lifespan; therefore, at any point in time, one construct

could be “causing” the other, but the reverse may be true given a different point in time.

This current study also intentionally assessed healthy older adults, which is meaningful,

but it also does not provide the opportunity to assess how these same relationships would

occur in older adults who may have had clinically significant mental health pathology,

poor physical health, and or below average cognitive functioning. Due to moderate

sample size of this study, there was a higher likelihood to detect significant effects, and

while there were more non-significant relationships, any significant relationship should

be interpreted with caution. Lastly, there may be a more comprehensive and meaningful

way to measure mental health, physical health, and life purpose. The investigator was

limited in using the measures that were used in the larger study where these variables

were collected.

Future Directions and Implications

While many of the results were shown to be non-significant, there is still

important information that can be gleaned from this study. Life purpose is a variable that

impacts cognitive functioning in older adults in some facet. While it is yet to be clear

how life purpose impacts cognitive functioning, it appears that the influence is positive,

which would mean there are meaningful possible clinical implications. For example,

therapy centered on development of life purpose could be a clinical intervention within

34

itself. This type of therapy would center on increasing self-efficacy, problem solving

ability, motivation, and short-term and long-term goal setting. It would be a strengths-

based approach to help increase life purpose and maintain mood stability. Future research

could assess the utility of this type of intervention for quality of life and cognitive

functioning in older adulthood by conducting a longitudinal controlled study. Clinical

providers who already have a part of an older adult’s care (e.g., physicians, nurses) could

also start to implement increasing the factors aforementioned that help to increase a sense

of life purpose.

Life purpose, as it relates to aging and cognitive performance in older adults, is

still not a heavily researched area. There is some promising research and theories that

suggest that an increased sense of having meaning later in life, and continuing to strive

towards goals, has the potential to increase overall quality of life and therefore the aging

process. The hope in this study was to show that an increased sense of life purpose could

also be directly and or indirectly related to actual measures of cognitive functioning in

older adults. As the results showed, there was some support for an indirect relationship.

An increased sense of life purpose appeared to help those participants who endorsed

more mental health issues perform somewhat better on measures of processing speed.

Because of the cross-sectional design of this study, the possible long-term effects

of having fairly healthy mental/physical health and higher levels of life purpose at

baseline on cognitive functioning could not be assessed. It could be hypothesized for

future studies that older adults with healthy levels of mental health, physical health, and

higher life purpose maintain their baseline cognitive performance scores, or even

improve.

35

It is also important to consider the bidirectional relationships between the DVs

within this study. In other words, without a more controlled longitudinal study design, it

is difficult to ascertain which variable is influencing the other. Because of the cross-

sectional design of this study, it is likely that mental health, physical health, and life

purpose influence each other. It would be expected that better physical health influences

better mental health, which would then influence better cognitive functioning, but based

on the literature and this study the relationships between these constructs are more

dynamic. How life purpose fits into this equation needs further investigation. It could be

hypothesized that having better mental health gives an individual enough motivation and

energy to continue to set goals in life and therefore feel a sense of life purpose, which

may influence cognitive functioning in that the mind is continuing to be cognitively

stimulated through goal setting, planning, problem solving, and new learning. As

explained prior, mental health can be defined in a variety of ways and can encompass

multiple constructs within it. While this study ensured that the variable of life purpose

was distinctly different from how mental health was measured, and results did show that

life purpose did explain the relationship between high mental health concerns and

cognitive processing speed, more research and controlled studies need to be conducted to

better understand this relationship.

Conclusion

The aims of this current study were to: (i) to provide supporting evidence for

research that has shown a significant relationship between mental and physical health and

cognitive functioning, (ii) to provide preliminary evidence that shows a relationship

36

between mental and physical health and life purpose, and (iii) to examine the relationship

between life purpose and cognitive functioning. These aims were partially reached in

that life purpose does appear to add a valuable contribution to how we understand aging

and cognition in late life. The natural decline of mental health and physical health in

older adults has been shown to impact cognition and decrease overall perceived quality of

life, which can then lead to multiple other issues that may indirectly impact overall

functioning in late life (Røysamb, Tambs, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Neale, and Harris,

2003; Strine, Chapman, Balluz, Moriart, Mokdad, 2008). As stated in the outset of this

study, the overarching goal is to increase knowledge of what it means to age well, and if

promotion of life purpose can add a positive influence on these relationships, it would be

important to then investigate how this can be applied for the older adult population.

Life purpose can be operationalized in a variety of ways, apart from how it was

defined this study. Emmons (2003) described how creating life meaning/purpose is

distinctly human characteristic, and highlighted the importance of understanding this

construct better in order to promote this throughout the lifespan in hopes of increasing

quality of life and aging well. Emmons stated that while the goal in life for many is to “be

happy,” researchers have shown that happiness is a by-product of engagement in

“worthwhile projects and activities” (p. 106) that help to create life purpose.

The aging process has historically been associated with a decline in overall

functioning, disease, and mortality. With advancements in medicine, people have been

living longer due to curing or controlling of disease for example. Unfortunately, living

longer chronological lives may include cognitive decline and a potential higher risk of

developing low perceived quality of life, as continued living in older age does not always

37

equate to living healthy or well. It is important to continue to assess what factors can help

to preserve cognitive functioning in older age. Researchers have shown that having

continued life purpose can create a domino effect of positive health and quality of life

outcomes in late life. This study also supported the potential that having a greater sense

of life purpose in late life can have on cognitive functioning and potentially overall

quality of life.

38

REFERENCES

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing and interpreting

interactions. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Alzheimer’s Association. (2015). What is alzheimer’s? Retrieved from

http://www.alz.org/alzheimers_disease_what_is_alzheimers.asp.

American Thoracic Society. (2008). SF-12 health survey. American Thoracic Society

Quality of Life Resources. Retrieved from

http://qol.thoracic.org/sections/instruments/pt/pages/sf12.html.

Atkinson, H. H., Rosano, C. Simonsick, E. M., Williamson, J. D., Davis, C., Ambrosius,

W. T., Rapp, S. R., Cesari, M., Newman, A. B., Harris, T. B., Rubin, S. M.,

Yaffe, K., Satterfield, S., & Kritchevsky, S. B. (2007). Cognitive function, gait

speed decline, and comorbidities: The health, aging, and body composition study.

Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 62(8), 844 – 850.

Balboa-Castillo, T, León-Muñoz, L. M., Graciana, A., Rodríguez-Artalejo, F., & Gullar-

Castillón, P. (2011). Longitudinal association of physical activity and sedentary

behavior during leisure time with health-related quality of life in community-

dwelling older adults. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 9(47).

doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-9-47.

Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self0efficacy in cognitive development and functioning.

Educational Psychologist, 28(2), 117 – 148. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3.

Barnes, D. E., Yaffe, K., Satariano, W. A., & Tager, I. B. (2003). A longitudinal study of

cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy older adults. Journal of

the American Geriatric Society, 51(4), 459 – 465. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-

5415.2003.51153.x.

Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carrillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kin, H., & Johns, H. (2015).

Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and

dementia: A population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal

of the Alzheimer’s Association, 11(6), 718-726. doi: 10.1016%2Fj.jalz.2015.

Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York: Guilford Press.

Bassuk, S. S., Glass, T. A., & Berkman, L. F. (1999). Social disengagement and

incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Annals of

Internal Medicine, 131(3), 165 – 173. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-

00002.

Bates, M. E. & Labouvie, E. (2008). FAS and CFL forms of verbal fluency differ in

39

difficulty: A meta-analytic study. Applied Neuropsychology, 15(2), 87-106, 97-

106. doi: 10.1080/09084280802083863.

Beaudreau, S. A. & O’Hara, R. (2009). The association of anxiety and depressive

symptoms with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults.

Psychology and Aging, 24(2), 507-512.

Béland, F., Zunzunegui, M., Alvarado, B., Otero, A., & del Ser, T. (2005). Trajectories of

cognitive decline and social relations. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological

Sciences, 60(6), P320 – P330. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.6.P320.

Benedict, R. H., Smerbeck, A., Parikh, R., Rodgers, J., Cadavid, D., & Erlanger, D.

(2012). Reliability and equivalence of alternate forms for the symbol digit

modalities test: Implications for multiple sclerosis clinical trials. Multiple

Sclerosis, 18(9), 1320-1325. doi: 10.1177/1352458511435717.

Bittner, R. M. & Crowe, S. F. (2009). The relationship between working memory,

processing speed, verbal comprehension and FAS performance following

traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 21(7), 709-719.

doi: 1 0.1080/02699050701468917.

Boyle, P., Buchman, A. S., Barnes, L. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2010). Effect of a purpose in

life on risk of incident Alzheimer Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment in

community-dwelling older persons. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 63(3), 304-310. doi:

10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.208

Buchman, A. S., Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Shah, R. C., Wilson, R. S., & Bennett, D. A.

(2012). Total daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in

older adults. Neurology, 78(17). Doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35.

Cadar, D. Stephan, C. M., Jagger, C., Johansson, B., Hofer, S. M., Piccinin, A. M., &

Muniz-Terrera. (2016). The role of cognitive reserve on terminal decline: a cross-

cohort analysis from two European studies: OCTO-Twin, Sweden, and Newcastle

85+, UK. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31, 601 – 610. doi:

10.1002/gps.4366.

Coin, A., Perissinotto, E., Girardi, A., Inelmen, E. M., Enzi, G., Manzato, E., & Sergi, G.

(2010). Does religiosity protect against cognitive and behavioral decline in

Alzheimer’s dementia? Current Alzheimer Research, 7(5), 445 – 452. doi:

10.2174/156720510791383886.

Cotman, C. W. & Berchtold, N. C. (2002). Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance

brain health and plasticity. Trends in Neuroscience, 25(6), 295 – 301. doi:

10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02143-4.

Cracchiolo, J. R. Mori, T., Nazian, S. T., Tan, J., Potter, H., & Arendash, G. W. (2007).

40

Enhanced cognitive activity—over and above social or physical activity—is

required to protect Alzheimer’s mice against cognitive impairment, reduce Aβ

deposition, and increase synaptic immunoreactivity. Neurobiology of Learning

and Memory, 88(3), 277 – 294. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.07.007.

Crivelli, L., Della Bella, S., Lucchini, M. (2016). Happiness and health. In: Bruni L,

Porta P-L (Eds.), Handbook of research methods and applications in happiness

and the quality of life. Elgar, Cheltenham (pp. 372–399).

Daaleman, T. P. & Frey, B. B. (2004). The Spirituality Index of Well-Being: A new

instrument for health-related quality of life research. Annals of Family Medicine,

2, 499-503. doi: 10.1370/afm.89.

Daaleman, T. P., Frey, B. B., Wallace, D. & Studenski, S. A. (2002). The Spirituality

Index of Well-Being: Development and testing of a new measure. Journal of

Family Medicine, 51(592), 499-503.

Daaleman, T. P., Cobb, A. K., & Frey, B. B. (2001). Spirituality and well-being: an

exploratory approach to the patient perspective. Soc Sci Med, 53, 119-27.

Daviglus, M. L., Bell, C. C., Berrettini, W., Bowen, P. E., Connolly, S. Jr., Cox, J. C.,

Dunbar-Jacob, J. M., Granieri, E. C., Hunt, G., McGarry, K., Patel, D., Potosky,

A. L., Sanders-Bush, E., Silberberg, D., & Trevisan, M. (2010). National institute

of health state-of-the-science conference statement: Preventing Alzheimer disease

and cognitive decline. Annals of Internal Medicine, 153(3), 176 -181. doi:

10.7326/0003-4819-153-3-201008030-00260.

Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., & Starr, J. M. (2010). Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of

cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 25(1), 219-228. doi: 10.1037/a0017750.

de Sousa Magalhães, S., Fernandes Malloy-Diniz, L., & Cavalheiro Hamdan, A. (2012).

Validity convergent and reliability test-retest of the rey auditory verbal learning

test. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 9(3), 129.

Dickinson, W. J., Potter, G. G., Hybels, C. F., McQuoid, D. R., & Steffens, D. C. (2011).

Change in stress and social support as predictors of cognitive decline in older

adults with and without depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,

26(12), 1267 – 1274. doi: 10.1002/gps.2676.

Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and

Health Promotion. (2015, March 5). Alzheimer’s disease. Retrieved from

http://www.cdc.gov/aging/aginginfo/alzheimers.htm.

Emmons, R. A. (2003). Personal goals, life meaning, and virtue: Wellsprings of a

41

positive life. In C. L. M. Keyes, & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive

psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 105-128). Washington DC: American

Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10594-005

Erickson, K. I., Gildengers, A. G., & Butters, M. A. (2013). Physical activity and brain

plasticity in late adulthood. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15(1), 99–108.

Estévez-González, A., Kulisevsky, J., Boltes, A., Otermín, P., & García-Sánchez, C.

(2003). Rey verbal learning test is a useful tool for differential diagnosis in the

preclinical phase of Alzheimer’s disease: comparison with mild cognitive

impairment and normal aging. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,

18(11), 1021-1028.

Everard, K. M., Lach, H. W., Fisher, E. B., & Baum, M. C. (2000). Relationship of

activity and social support to the functional health of older adults. Journal of

Gerontology: Social Sciences, 55(4), S208 – S212. doi:

10.1093/geronb/55.4.S208.

Ferreira, N., Owen, A., Mohan, A., Corbett, A., & Ballard, C. (2015). Associations

between cognitively stimulating leisure activities, cognitive function and age-

related cognitive decline. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 30, 422 –

430. doi: 10.1002/gps.4155.

Ferri, C. P., Prince, M., Brayne, C., Brodaty, H., Fratiglioni, L., Ganguli, M., Hall, K.,

Hasegawa, K., Hendrie, H., Huang, Y., Jorm, A., Mathers, C., Menezes, P. R.,

Rimmer, E., & Scazufca, M. (2005). Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi

consensus study. Lancet, 366(9503), 2112-2117.

Fritsch, T., McClendon, M. J., Smyth, K. A., Lerner, A. J., Friedland, R. P., & Larsen, J.

D. (2007). Cognitive functioning in healthy aging: The role of reserve and

lifestyle factors early in life. The Gerontologist, 47(3), 307 – 322. doi:

10.1093/geront/47.3.307.

Gandek, B., Ware, J. E., Aaronson, N. K., Apolone, G., Bjomer, J. B., Brazier, J. E.,

Bullinger, M., Kaasa, S., Leplege, A., Prieto, L., & Sullivam, M. (1998). Cross-

validation of item selection and scoring for the SF-12 Health Survey in nine

countries: results from the IQOLA Project. International Quality of Life

Assessment. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 51(11), 1171 – 1178.

Geerlings, M. I., Bouter, L. M., Schoevers, R. A., Beekman, A. T. F., Jonker, C., Deeg,

D., J. H., Van Tilburg, W., Adér, H. J., & Schmand, B. (2000). Depression and

risk of cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease: Results of two prospective

community-bases studies in the Netherlands. The British Journal of Psychiatry,

176(6), 568-575. doi: 10.1192/bjp.176.6.568.

Grigsby, J., Kaye, K., Baxter, J., Shatterly, S. M., & Hamman, R. F. (1998). Executive

42

cognitive abilities and functional status among community dwelling older persons

in the San Luis Valley health and aging study. Journal of the American Geriatric

Society, 46(5), 590 – 596. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1998.tb01075.x.

Hänninen, T., Koivisto, K., Reinikainen, K. J., Helkala, E., Soininen, H., Mykkänen, L.,

Laakso, M., & Riekkinen, P. J. (1996). Prevelence of ageing-associated cognitive

decline in an elderly population. Age and Ageing, 25(3), 201 – 205. doi:

10.1093/ageing/25.3.201.

Hebert, L. E., Scherr, P. A., Bienias, J. L., Bennett, D. A., & Evans, D. A. (2003).

Alzheimer disease in the US population: prevalence estimates using the 2000

census. Archives of Neurology, 60(8), 1119-1122.

Heron, M. (2012). Deaths: Leading causes for 2009. National Vital Statistics Reports,

61(7), 1-95.

Hindle, J. V., Hurt, C. S., Burn, D. J., Brown, R. G., Samuel, M., Wilson, K. C., & Clare,

L. (2016). The effects of cognitive reserve and lifestyle on cognition and dementia

in Parkinson’s disease – a longitudinal cohort study. Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(1),

13 – 23. doi: 10.1002/gps.4284.

Holahan, C. K. & Holahan, C. J. (1987). Self-efficacy, social support, and depression in

aging: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 42(1), 65 – 68.

doi: 10.1093/geronj/42.1.65.

Hultsch, D. F., Hammer, M., & Small, B. J. (1993). Age differences in cognitive

performance in later life: Relationships to self-reported health and activity life

style. Journal of Gerontology, 48(1), P1 – P11. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.1.P1.

Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J. & Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: Engaged

lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14(2),

245-263. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.14.2.245.

James, B. D., Wilson, R. S., Barnes, L. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2011). Late-life social

activity and cognitive decline in old age. Journal of the International

Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 998-1005. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000531.

Jorm, A. F. (2000). Is depression a risk factor for dementia or cognitive decline?

Gerontology, 46(4), 2190227. doi: 10.1159/000022163.

Kaufman, Y., Anaki, D., Binns, M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Cognitive decline in

Alzheimer disease: Impact of spirituality, religiosity, and QOL. Neurology,

68(18), 1509 – 1514. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000260697.66617.59.

Kawachi, I. & Berkman, L. F. (2001). Social ties and mental health. Journal of Urban

Health, 78, 458. doi:10.1093/jurban/78.3.458.

43

Keyes, C. L. M. (1998). Social well-being. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61, 121–140.

Kim, E. S., Sun, J. K., & Peterson, C. (2013). Purpose in life and reduced incidence of

stroke in older adults: ‘The health and retirement study.’ Journal of

Psychosomatic Research, 74(5), 427-432.

doi: 10.1016/j.psychores.2013.01.013.

Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling. New York,

NY: Guilford Press.

Krause, N. (2009). Meaning in life and mortality. The Journals of Gerontology, 64B(4),

517-527. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp047.

Lautenschlager, N. T., Cox, K. L., Flicker, L., Foster, J. K., van Bockxmeer, F. M., Xiao,

J., Greenop, K. R., & Almeida, O. P. (2008). Effect of physical activity on

cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer Disease. JAMA, 300(9),

1027-1037. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.9.1027.

Lavretsky, H. (2010). Spirituality and aging. Aging Health, 6(6), 749 – 769. doi:

10.2217/ahe.10.70.

Lee, Y. (2000). The predictive value of self assessed general, physical, and mental health

on functional decline and mortality in older adults. Journal of Epidemiology &

Community Health, 54, 123-129.

León, I., García-García, J., & Roldán-Tapia, L. (2014). Estimating cognitive reserve in

health adults using the cognitive reserve scale. PLoS One, 9(7), e102632. doi:

10.1371/journal.pone.0102632.

León, I., García-García, J., & Roldán-Tapia, L. (2016). Cognitive reserve and ageing.

Anales de Psicología, 32(1), 218 – 223. doi: 10.6018/analesps.32.1.182331.

Leung, P., Orrell, M., & Orgeta, V. (2015). Social support group interventions in people

with dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of the

literature. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 30(1), 1 – 9. doi:

10.1002/gps.4166.

Levy, B., R., Slade, M. D., & Kasi, S. V. (2002). Longitudinal benefit of positive self-

perceptions of aging on functional health. Journal of Gerontology: Psychosocial

Sciences, 57(5), P409 – P417. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.5.P409.

Liévre, A., Alley, D., & Crimmins, E. M. (2008). Educational differential in life

expectancy with cognitive impairment among the elderly in the United States.

Journal of Aging and Health, 20(4). doi: 10.1177/089826430831557.

Lockwood, K. A., Alexopoulos, G. S., van Gorp, W. (2002). Executive dysfunction in

44

geriatric depression. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(7), 1119-1126. doi:

10.1176.appi.ajp.159.7.1119.

Mantella, R. C., Butters, M. A., Dew, M. A., Mulsant, B. H., Begley, A. E., Tracey, B.,

Shear, M. K., Reynolds, C. F. III, & Lenze, E. J. (2007). Cognitive impairment in

late-life generalized anxiety disorder. Psychiatry, 15(8), 673-679. doi:

10.1097/JGP.0b013e31803111f2.

Mastin, L. (2010). Parts of the brain. The Human Memory. Retrieved from

http://www.human-memory.net/brain_parts.html.

McKnight, P. E. & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Purpose in life a sa system that creates and

sustains health and well-being: An integrative, testable theory. American

Psychological Association, 13(3), 242-251. doi: 10.1037/a0017152.

Morrell, H. M. (2013). Path analysis. [PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved from

https://llu.instructure.com/.

Niolon, R. [Web article post]. (2005, August 24). Introduction to the WAIS-III.

Retrieved February 28, 2018 from

http://www.psychpage.com/learning/library/intell/wais_history.html.

Nucci, M., Mapelli, D., & Mondini, S. (2012). Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire

(CRIq): a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clinical and

Experimental Research, 24(3), 218 – 226. doi: 10.3275/7800.

Neurofeedback Center of Rochester. (2018). [Web article post]. What is z-score

training? Retrieved on March 5, 2018 from

http://neurofeedbackcenterofrochester.com/what-is-z-score-training/.

Nygren, B., Jonsén, L. A., Gustafson, Y., Norberg, A., & Lundman, B. (2010).

Resilience, sense of coherence, purpose in life and self-transcendence in relation

to perceived physical and mental health among the oldest old. Aging & Mental

Health, 9(4), 354-362. doi: 10.1080/1360500114415.

Olshansky, S. J., Beard, J., & Börsch-Supan, A. (2012). Pursuing health ageing: What

healthy ageing involves. The longevity dividend: Health as an investment. In J.

Beard, S. Biggs, D. Bloo,, L. Fried, P. Hogan, A. Kalache, & J. Olshansky (Eds.),

Global population ageing: Peril or promise? (pp. 57-60). Retrieved on March 5,

2018 from the Program on the Global Demography of Aging Working Paper No.

89 http://www.hsph.harvard.edu/pgda/working.htm.

Patterson, J. (2011). F-A-S Test. In: Kreutzer J.S., DeLuca J., Caplan B. (Eds)

Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer, New York, NY.

doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_886.

Post, S. G. (2005). Alturism, happiness, and health: it’s good to be good.

45

International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12(2), 66-77. doi:

10.1207/s15327558ijbm12024.

Reichstady, J., Sengupta, G., Depp, C. A., Palinkas, L. A., & Jeste, D. V. (2010). Older

adults’perspectives on successful aging: Qualitative interview. The American

Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(7), 567-575.

doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181e040bb.

Richards, M. & Sacker, A. (2010). Lifetime antecedents of cognitive reserve. Journal or

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(5), 614 – 624.

doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.614.14581.

Robertson, D. A., King-Killimanis, B. L., & Kenny, R. A. (2016). Negative perceptions

of aging predict longitudinal decline in cognitive function. Psychology and Aging,

31(1), 71 – 81. doi: 10.1037/pag0000061.supp.

Rockefeller College University at Albany. [Web PDF post]. (2007, September 4). Outlier

and DFBETA. Retrieved February 29, 2018 from

https://www.albany.edu/faculty/kretheme/PAD705/SupportMat/DFBETA.pdf.

Ross, T. O., Calhoun, E., Cox, T., Wenner, C., Kono, W., & Pleasant, M. (2007). The

reliability and validity of qualitative scores for the Controlled Oral Word

Association Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(4), 475-488.

doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.026.

Røysamb, E., Tambs, K., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Neale, M. C., & Harris, J. R. (2003).

Happiness and health: Environmental and genetic contributions to the relationship

between subjective well-being, perceived health, and somatic Illness. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1136-1146.

doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1136.

Ryff, C. D. (2017). The benefits of purposeful life engagement on later-life physical

function. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(10), 1046-1046.

doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2136.

Sabaté, J., & Rajaram, S. (2011). Effects of daily ingestion of walnuts for 2 years on age-

related cognitive decline and macular degeneration in healthy elderly subjects: A

randomized, single blind, dual center, controlled trial (Unpublished research

grant proposal). Loma Linda University, Loma Linda, CA, USA.

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in

cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403-428. doi: 10.1037/0033-

295X.103.3.403

Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a

46

potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, Vol 132(4), 566-594. doi: 10.1037/0096-

3445.132.4.566.

Scarpina, F. & Tagini, S. (2017). The Stroop color and word test. Frontiers in

Psychology, 8, 557. Htto://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557.

Scatmeas, N. & Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(5), 625 – 633.

doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576.

Scherder, E. J. A., Van Paasschen, J., Deijen, J.-B., Van Der Knokke, S., Orlebeke, J. F

K., Burgers, I., Devriese, P.-P., Swaab, D. F., & Sergeant, J. A. (2010). Physical

activity and executive functions in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment.

Aging & Mental Health, 9(3), 272-280. doi: 10.1080/13607860500089930.

Seeman, M. (1991). Alienation and anomie. In J. P. Robinson, & P. R. Shaver (Eds.),

Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 291–371). San

Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Seeman, T., McAvay, G., Merrill, S., Albert, M., & Rodin, J. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs

and change in cognitive performance: MacArthur studies on successful aging.

Psychology and Aging, 11(3), 538 – 551. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.11.3.538.

Seeman, T. E., Unger, J. B., McAvy, G., & Mendes de Leon, C. F. (1999). Self-efficacy

beliefs and perceived declines in functional ability: MacArthur studies of

successful aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 54B(4),

P214 – P222. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54B.4.P214.

Sheridan, L. K., Fitzgerald, H. E., Adams, K. M., Nigg, J. T., Martel, M. M., Puttler, L.

I., Wong, M. M., & Zucker, R. A. (2006). Normative symbol digit modalities test

performance in a community-based sample. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology, 21(1), 23-28. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.003.

Siegrist, M. (1995). Effects of taboo words on color-naming performance on a stroop test.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81(2 Pt 2), 1119-1122.

doi: 10.2466/pms.1995.81.3f.1119.

Singh-Manoux, A., Marmot, M. G., Glymour, M., Sabia, S., Kivimäki, M., Dugravot, A.

(2011). Does cognitive reserve shape cognitive decline? Annals of Neurology,

70(2), 296 – 304. doi: 10.1002/ana.22391.

Small, B. J., Dixon, R. A. McArdelle, J. J., & Grimm, K. J. (2012). Do changes in

lifestyle engagement moderate cognitive decline in normal aging? Evidence from

the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 144 – 155. doi:

10.1037/a0026579.

47

Small, G. W. (2016). Detection and prevention of cognitive decline. American Journal of

Geriatric Psychiatry. Advance online publication.

doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.08.013.

Smith, G. E. (2016). Healthy cognitive aging and dementia prevention. American

Psychologist, 71(4), 268 – 275. doi: 10.1037/a0040250.

Soper, D. (2018). A-priori sample size calculator for multiple regression. Retrieved from

https://www.danielsoper.com/statcalc/calculator.

Sumowski, J. F., Rocca, M. A., Leavitt, V. M., Dackovic, J., Mesaros, S., Drulovic, J.,

DeLuca, J., & Fillippi, M. (2014). Brain reserve and cognitive reserve protect

against cognitive decline over 4.5 years in MS. Neurology, 82(20), 1776 – 1783.

doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000433.

Starkstein, S. E., Mayberg, H. S., Leiguarda, R., Preziosi, T. J., & Robinson, R. G.

(1992). A prospective longitudinal study of depression, cognitive decline, and

physical impairments in patients with Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology,

Neurosurgey & Psychiatry, 55, 377-382. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.5.377.

Steffens, D. C., Otey, E., Alexopoulos, G. S., Meryl A. Butters, M. A., Cuthbert, B.,

Ganguli, M., Geda, Y. E., Hendrie, H. C., Krishnan, R. R., Kumar, A., Lopez, O.

L., Lyketsos, C. G., Mast, B. T., Morris, J. C., Norton, M. C., Peavy, G. M.,

Petersen, R. C., Reynolds, C. F., Salloway, S., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., &

Yesavage, J. (2006). Perspectives on depression, mild-cognitive impairment, and

cognitive decline. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(2), 130-139.

doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.130.

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the

reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(3),

448 – 460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813248.

Stern, Y. (2003). The concept of cognitive reserve: A catalyst for research. Journal of

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(5), 589 – 593. doi:

10.1076/jcen.25.5.589.14571.

Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015 – 2028.

doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004.

Stern, Y., Albert, S., Tang, M., & Tsai, W. (1942). Rate of memory decline in AD is

related to education and occupation. Neurology, 53(9), 1526 – 632X. doi: 10.

1212/WNL.53.9.1942.

Strine, T. W., Chapman, D. P., Balluz, L. S., Moriart, D. G., & Mokdad, A. H. (2008).

48

The associations between life satisfaction and health-related quality of life,

chronic illness, and health behaviors among U.S. community-dwelling adults.

Journal of Community Health, 33(1), 40-50. doi: 10.1007/s10900-007-9066-4.

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.

Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a

conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63(3), 289-298.

doi: 10.1007/s004269900007.

Tilvis, R. S., Kähönen-Väre, M. H., Jolkkonen, J., Valvanne, J., Pitkala, K. H., &

Strandberg, T. E. (2004). Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged

people over a 10-year period. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 59(3),

M268 – M274. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.3.M268.

Tombaugh, T.N.T.N. (2004). Trail Making test A and B: Normative data stratified by age

and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology : The Official Journal of the

National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 19(2), 203–214. doi:10.1016/s0887-

6177(03)00039-8.

Tucker, A. M. & Stern, Y. (2011). Cognitive reserve in aging. Current Alzheimer

Research, 8(3), 1 – 7.

Turken, A. U., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Bammer, R., Baldo, J., Dronkers, N. F., &

Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cognitive processing speed and the structure of white

matter pathways: Convergent evidence from normal variation and lesion studies.

Neuroimage, 42(2), 1032-1044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.057.

University of Nebraska Lincoln [Web PDF post]. (2007, July 24). Multiple regression

diagnostics. Retrieved February 28, 2018 from

http://psych.unl.edu/psycrs/statpage/regdiag_eg.pdf.

Utah Department of Health (2001). Interpreting the SF-12. Utah Health Status Survey.

Retrieved from

http://health.utah.gov/opha/publications/2001hss/sf12/SF12_Interpreting.pdf.

Van der Elst, W., van Boxtel, M. P., van Breukelen, G. J., & Jolles, J. (2005). Rey's

verbal learning test: normative data for 1855 healthy participants aged 24-81

years and the influence of age, sex, education, and mode of presentation. Journal

of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11(3), 290-302.

Wagner, S., Helmreich, I., Dahmen, N., Lieb, K., & Tadic, A. (2011). Reliability of three

alternative forms of the trail making tests a and b. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of

Neuropsychologists, 26)4), 314-321. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr024.

49

Walker, L. A. S., Cheng, A., Beard, J., Berrigan, L. I., Rees, L. M., & Freedman, M. S.

(2012). Tests of information processing speed. International Journal of Multiple

Sclerosis Care, 14(2), 92-99. doi: 10.7224/1537-2073-14.2.92.

Ware, J. J., Kosinski, M., & Keller, S. D. (1996). A 12-Item Short-Form Health Survey:

construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Medical

Care, 34(3), 220 – 233.

Whalley, L. J., Deary, I. J., Appleton, C. L., & Starr, J. M. (2004). Cognitive reserve and

the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing Research Reviews, 3(4), 369 – 382.

doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.001.

Wilson, R. S., Barnes, L. L., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Aggarwal, N. T., Schneider, J. S.,

Bach, J., Pilat, J., Beckett, L. A., Arnold, S. E., Evans, D. A., & Bennett, D. A.

(2002). Depressive symptoms, cognitive decline, and risk of AD in older persons.

Neurology, 59(3). doi: 10.1212/WNL.59.3.364.

Wong, P. T. P. (2007). [Web article post]. Meaning of life and meaning of death in

successful aging. Retrieved on March 5, 2018 from the International Network of

Personal Meaning at http://www.meaning.ca/archives/archive/art_successful-

aging_P_Wong.htm.

Wongpakaran, T., Wongpakaran, N., & Ruktrakul, R. (2011). Reliability and validity of

the multidimensional scale of perceived social support (MSPSS): Thai version.

Clinical Practice & Epidemiology in Mental Health, 12(7), 161 – 166.

doi: 10.2174/1745017901107010161.

Yaffe, K., Fiocco, A. J., Lindquist, K., Vittinghoff, E., Simonsick, M., Newman, A. B.,

Satterfield, S., Rosano, C., Rubin, S. M., Ayonayon, H. N., Harris, T. B., & For

the Health ABC Study. (2009). Predictors of maintaining cognitive function in

older adults. American Academy of Neurology, 72(23). Doi:

10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a92c36.

Yeh, S. J. & Liu, Y. (2003). Influence of social support on cognitive function in the

elderly. BMC Health Services Research, 3(9), 1 – 9. doi: 10.1186/1472-6963-3-9.

Zahodne, L. B., Stern, Y., & Manly. (2015). Differing effects of education on cognitive

decline in diverse elders with low versus high educational attainment.

Neuropsychology, 29(4), 649 – 657. doi: 10.1037/neu0000141.

Zhu, J., Tulsky, D. S., Price, L., & Chen, H.-Y. (2001). WAIS-III reliability data for

clinical groups. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7,

8620866.

Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G. & Farley, G. K. (1988). The Multidimensional

Scale of Perceived Social Support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30-41.

50

Zunzunegui, M., Alvarado, B. E., Del Ser, T., & Otero, A. (2003). Social networks,

social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in

community-dwelling Spanish older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Social

Sciences, 58(2), S93 – S100.

51

APPENDIX A

SPIRITUAL INDEX OF WELL-BEING (DAALEMAN AND FREY, 2004)

52

APPENDIX B

12-ITEM SHORT FORM HEALTH SURVEY (WARE, KOSINSKI, AND

KELLER, 1996; SF-12)