hysterical conversion and brain function

Upload: alexandru-si-irina-organ

Post on 06-Jul-2018

229 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    1/21

    S.

    Laurry:s

    Ed.

    Progress

    n Rrailt

    Researrh.

    Vol. 150

    lssN

    0079-6123

    Coplrighi

    (C

    2005

    Elsevi::r J.V

    All rights

    rcserved

    CHAPTER

    23

    HysterLcalonversionand brain

    function

    Patrik

    Vuilleumier*

    l-aboratorl,.for

    Behatioral

    Neurolagy

    and

    ltrwgingl

    of Cognition,

    Clfuic

    of Neurology

    &

    Department

    of Neurosciences,

    Ltniuersit),"trteclical

    Center,

    Faatttli of

    Psyainiy

    & Educution

    Sciences,

    uniuersity

    of

    Geneua,

    Genetta,

    Switzerland

    Abstract:

    Hysterical

    conversion

    disorders

    epresent

    functional"

    or

    unexplained

    neurological

    deficits

    such

    as

    paralysis

    or somatosensory

    osses

    hat are

    not explained

    by

    organic

    esions

    n the

    nervous

    system,

    but

    arise

    n the

    context

    of

    "psychogenic'n

    tress

    or emotional

    conflicts.

    After

    more

    than a

    century

    of

    both

    clinical and theoretical nterest,the exact nature of such emotional disordersresponsibleor hysterical

    symptorus,

    and

    their functional

    consequenes

    on

    neural

    systems

    n the

    brain,

    still

    remain

    argely

    unknown.

    Aor*u.r,

    several

    ecentstudies

    have

    used unctional

    brain

    imaging

    echniques

    such

    as

    EEG,

    fMRI,

    PET,

    or SPECT)

    in the attempt

    to identify

    specific

    neural

    correlates

    associated

    with

    hysterical

    conversion

    symptoms.

    This article

    presents

    general

    overview

    of these

    indings

    and of

    previousneuropsychologically

    based

    accounts

    of hysteria.

    Functional

    neuroimaging

    haS revealed

    selective

    decreases

    n the activity

    of

    frontal

    and subcortical

    circuits

    nvolved

    n motor

    control

    during

    hysterical

    paralysis,

    decreases

    n

    somato-

    sensory

    ortic€s

    during

    hysterical

    anesthesia,

    r

    decreases

    n visual

    cortex

    during

    hysterical

    blindness.

    Such

    changes re

    usually

    not

    accompanied

    y

    any signifrcant

    changes

    n

    elementary

    tages

    of sensory

    or

    motor

    processing

    s measured

    y evoked

    potentials,although

    some

    changes

    n

    later stages

    f

    integration

    (such

    as

    it:00

    t"rplnses)

    havebeen

    eported.

    On

    the

    other hand,

    several

    euroimaging

    esults

    have

    shown ncreased

    activation n limbic regions, uchascingulateor orbitofrontalcortexduring

    conversion

    symptoms

    affecting

    difl'erent

    sensory

    or motor

    modalities.

    Taken

    together,

    hese

    data

    generallydo not

    support

    previous

    pro-

    posals

    hat

    hysteria

    rnight

    involve

    an

    exclusion

    of sensorimotor

    epresentations

    rom awareness

    hrough

    attentional

    processes. he1,1a11t"t

    eem

    o

    point

    to

    a modulation

    of such

    representations

    y

    primary

    affective

    or

    stress-related

    actors,

    perhaps

    nvolving

    primitive reflexive mechanisms

    of

    protection and

    alertness

    hat are

    partly

    independent

    of conscious

    control.

    and

    mediated

    by

    dynamic

    modulatory

    inter-

    actions

    between

    imbic

    and sensorimotor

    networks.

    A better

    understanding

    of the

    neuropsychr:biological

    bases

    f hysterical

    conversion

    disorder

    might

    therefore

    be obtained

    by t'uture

    maging

    studies

    hat

    compare

    different

    conversionsymptoms

    and

    employ

    functional

    connectivity

    analyses.

    his should

    not

    only

    lead to

    improve

    clinical

    management

    f these

    patients,

    but

    also

    provide

    new

    insights

    on the brain

    mechanisms

    f

    self-awareness.

    Introduction

    Sincemore

    than a

    century,

    hysteria

    has

    ctlntinu-

    ousll'

    fascinated

    both

    clinicians

    and

    theorists

    n-

    terestedn altercd

    states

    of self-consciousness.

    s a

    *Corrcsponding

    author.

    Tel.:

    1-41

    (0)22

    379-s.381;

    Far

    *41

    (0)223795.402:

    E-mail:

    [email protected]

    DOI: 10.0l ti/S0079-6123(0ti0023-:

    medical

    condition

    in

    which

    patients may

    present

    with various

    somatic

    symptoms

    without

    a

    recog-

    nized organic

    illness,

    hysteria

    still

    constitutes

    a

    poorly

    understood

    class

    of disorders

    at

    the

    border

    between

    psychiatryand

    neurology,

    with

    many

    dif-

    ferent

    appearances

    nd

    names.

    A variety

    of clas-

    sifications

    and

    explanations

    have been

    proposed

    over

    the

    years,

    vith different

    emphasison

    psycho-

    logical

    or neurobiological

    actors.

    but

    none is

    still

    309

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    2/21

    3 1 0

    Fig.

    l. Illustration

    of

    paraparesis

    astasia-abasia) f hysterical

    origin

    (drawn

    by

    Jean-Martin

    Charcot

    n the

    1870s).

    entirely

    satisfactory

    see

    Halligan

    et al.,

    2001 or a

    recent

    general

    overview).

    In some

    r€spects,

    he most

    important

    chang€

    sinceclassical

    descriptions

    of

    hysteria

    by Charcot

    and others

    at theend

    ofthe

    l9th c€ntury

    (see

    Figs.

    I and 2) is

    related

    to the

    fact that

    the term

    "hys-

    teria" was recently eliminated from the official

    psychiatric

    erminology.

    Thus,

    hysteria

    s now

    re-

    ferred to as

    "conversiondisorder"

    in the

    DSM-IV

    classification

    (American Psychiatric

    Association,

    1994,Diagnostic

    and Statistical

    Manual

    of lfental

    Disorders),

    where

    t is

    definedas a

    loss or

    distor-

    tion of a

    neurological

    unction

    (e.g.,

    paralysis

    or

    anesthesia)

    hat is

    not explained

    by

    an organic

    neurological

    esion or medical

    disease, rising

    in

    relation

    to some

    psychological

    stress

    or

    conflict'

    Fig. 2.

    "L'anesth6sie

    yst6rique",

    a cruel

    demonstrationofan-

    esthesian a patientat I-a Sal$triere, while n a stateof hysteria

    (etched

    rom

    photograph

    by

    P. R6gnard,

    1887,ks maladies

    6pid6miques

    e 'esprit

    Sorcellerie,magn6tisme,

    morphinisme,

    d6lire des

    grandeurs.

    Plon-Nourrit, Paris).

    but

    not

    consciously

    produced

    or intentionally

    feigned.However,

    although

    he term'oconversion"

    implies a

    specific

    mechanism

    whereby

    a

    primary

    psychological

    disorder

    is

    converted

    into bodily

    s)imptoms,

    he

    exact

    pr@esses

    esponsible

    or

    trig-

    gering

    his

    phenomenonand altering

    the

    patients'

    awar€ness

    f their

    bodily state

    still

    remain un-

    known. In fact, when considering he possiblene-

    urobiological

    changes

    associated

    with

    hysterical

    conversion

    symptoms,

    many

    of the current

    hy-

    potheses

    re

    not very

    different

    from those

    elabo-

    rated

    n the l9th

    century.

    Moreover,

    n contrast

    to

    other

    psychiatric

    conditions

    (such

    as

    depression,

    anxiety,

    compulsion,

    or

    phobia),

    surprisingly

    tbw

    investigations

    have

    been

    carried

    out to examine

    whether

    the "functional"

    symptoms

    experienced

    by

    hysterical

    patients

    might

    correspond to

    any

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    3/21

    3 l l

    underlying

    "functional

    changes"

    n cerebral

    activ-

    ity,

    as

    can be evaluated

    or

    instance

    sing

    a variety

    of

    modern

    neuroimaging

    echniques.

    The

    aim of

    this

    chapter

    s to

    present

    a

    general

    overview of the relationshipsbetweenhysterical

    conversion

    and

    brain

    function,

    focusing

    particu-

    larly on

    previous attempts

    o

    determine

    some

    ne-

    urophysiological

    correlates

    of

    hysterical

    conversion

    disorders.

    This

    review

    will

    primarily

    concentrate

    on

    patients

    with

    unexplained

    neuro-

    logical

    deficits

    n

    sensory

    and/or

    motor

    functions

    (i.e.,

    hysterical

    paralysisor anesthesia),

    ince

    such

    disorders

    are the

    most

    frequent

    and most

    easily

    described

    n terms

    of

    specifrc

    neurological

    path-

    ways. Similar

    approaches

    n

    other

    domains

    (e-g.,

    visual loss, deafness,or amnesia)will also be

    briefly

    mentioned,

    but

    other

    conversion

    disorders

    associated

    ith more

    oopositive"

    symptoms

    such

    as

    pseudo-seizure nd

    abnormal

    movem€nts

    will

    not

    be

    discussed

    ere,

    since

    even

    ess

    s known

    about

    their

    possible unctional

    neural

    correlates

    for re-

    view

    see

    Prigatano

    et al.,

    2002;

    Reuber

    et

    al.,

    2003).

    Finally,

    a

    few studies

    have

    examined

    he

    functional

    neuroanatomy

    of dissociations

    nduced

    by hypnosis

    (Maquet et al.,

    1999;

    Faymonville

    et

    al.,

    2000;

    Halligan

    et al.,

    2000;Kupers

    et al.,

    this

    volume),

    but their

    implications

    for hysterical

    def-

    icits are

    still

    in

    part

    unclear

    and

    will

    be discussed

    briefly

    A

    better

    understanding

    of

    patients with hyster-

    ical conversion

    has

    important

    implications

    for

    several

    easons,

    oth

    clinical

    and

    theoretical.

    First,

    such

    disorders

    are

    very common

    in clinical

    prac-

    tice,

    causing

    frequent

    problems

    n

    diagnosis

    and

    therapy

    for

    neurologists

    as

    well as

    psychiatrists,

    and

    resulting

    n important

    medical

    costs and

    ma-

    jor

    socio-economic

    burden

    for

    the

    patients and

    their

    relatives.

    Second,

    conversion

    disorders

    aise

    important theoretical questions concerningthe re-

    lationships

    between

    body

    and

    mind,

    and

    the

    ne-

    urobiological

    and

    psychological

    mechanisms

    underlying

    self,-awareness.

    From

    the

    perspective of

    clinical

    neurology,

    resulting

    from

    their

    brain

    lesion

    (Vuilleumier,

    2000a,

    2004; Marcel

    et al.,

    2004).

    Anosognosia

    cannot

    be

    explained

    by

    psychogenicactors

    alone,

    or

    by

    general

    confusion,

    but

    it is often

    associated

    with a lack of emotional concernquite similar to

    "la belle

    indifference"

    that

    has

    typically

    been

    de-

    scribed

    n

    hysteria.

    Moreover,

    ike

    anosognosia

    or

    hemiplegia,

    hysterical

    paralysisand hysterical

    an-

    esthesia

    re

    often

    thought

    to affect

    the

    letl more

    than

    the

    right

    limbs,

    without

    this

    being

    entirely

    accounted

    or

    a more

    frequent

    dominance

    of

    the

    right

    hand

    (Galin et al.,

    19771'

    tern,

    1983;

    Pas-

    cuzzi,

    1994;

    Gagliese

    et al.,

    1995),

    although

    a re-

    c€nt m€ta-analysis

    as

    questioned he

    existence

    f

    such

    an asymmetry

    Stone

    et

    a1.,2002).

    t is also

    intriguing o.tot"

    that

    theleurological

    @

    ass@la

    osognosra,,togn

    O it'

    *E€-f -bJ-F4 @

    lr

    4

    hvoiqt

    [o- -esly--ttuila ele]-.

    Sral$

    Aa*ArcsareS6,^'but

    lso

    blindness,

    mnesiq,.-of

    idigoir

    aphasid

    O['enkei,"

    -1995;

    VriiliJumier,

    20OOa).

    tlhoGtr

    such

    similarities

    do

    not

    indicate

    any

    clear

    relationships

    between

    hese

    different

    dis-

    orders,

    hese

    parallels

    between

    neurology

    and

    psy-

    chiatry

    highlight

    the

    fact

    that

    some

    behavioral

    abilities

    may

    dissociate

    rom

    the subjective

    expe-

    rience

    of

    these

    abilities,

    and

    that

    such

    dissociations

    are

    likely to arise from specificchanges n brain

    furrction

    -

    be

    they

    due

    to

    certain

    types of

    organic

    damage

    or

    to

    certain

    psycho-affective

    tates.

    Incidence

    and

    evolution

    Hysterical

    conversion

    disorders

    are

    thought

    to

    represent

    4o/o

    of all diagnoses

    n

    generalhospi-

    tals

    throughout

    w€stern

    countries.

    This

    frequency

    has

    remained

    remarkably

    stable

    across

    the

    past

    decades espitemany important changes n med-

    icine.

    Thus,

    a retrospective

    urvey

    at

    the

    National

    Hospital

    of

    Neurology

    and

    Neurosurgery

    n

    Lon-

    don,

    Queen

    Square,

    rom

    1955

    o 1975

    evealed

    a

    relatively

    constant

    ate of

    patients nvestigated

    or

    conversion

    or

    "functional"

    symptoms,

    ranging

    from

    0.85

    to 1.55oh

    over

    decades

    (Trimble,

    l98l).

    A

    similar

    result

    was

    found

    by

    another

    study

    examining

    the diagnoses

    made

    on

    7836

    successive

    utpatient

    referrals

    at Charing

    Cross

    diGdciatiitniffii@

    6f

    performance

    re

    not unusual

    n

    patientswith-

    .+.--_-__..--

    organlc

    braln

    Fslons,

    a

    rganTc*t5iffi

    an-

    edge

    and-even

    expligLgy_legy

    se]erl

    hanjgP

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    4/21

    312

    Hospital

    between

    19'17and

    1987

    (Perkin,

    1989),

    which

    revealed

    a stable

    ncidence

    of 3.8%

    of con-

    version

    disorders

    over the

    years.

    In

    Switzerland,

    Frei

    (1984)

    also estimated

    that the

    proportion

    andpresentation f hysterical onversiondisorders

    among

    patients

    seen

    n

    a

    large

    public

    hospital

    was

    similar

    in the

    1920s s compared

    with the

    1980s.

    However,

    it

    is

    important

    to

    emphasize

    hat the

    type ofconversion

    disorders

    may

    significantly

    vary

    across

    ifferent

    medical

    settings

    and

    referral

    sourc-

    es. This

    might

    explain

    why

    the incidence

    of hys-

    teria may appear

    o vary in

    somedomains

    but not

    are n€ver

    seen

    fto",

    t9ggl.

    Neurologists

    probably seemany

    more

    patients with

    relatively acute

    and

    limited

    symp-

    toms,

    as

    compared

    with

    psychiatristswho

    tend

    to

    see

    patients with more

    chronic

    and

    multiform

    dis-

    orders

    (Marsden, 1986).

    Moreover,

    although

    there

    is a

    good

    consensus

    between

    neurologists

    as to

    clinical

    syndromes

    eflecting

    non-organic

    diseases

    (irrespective of which term

    is actually

    preferred

    to

    describe

    these

    syndrom€s,

    e.g., hysterical,

    func-

    tional,

    or

    psychogenic),here

    s

    usually

    ess

    agree-

    m€nt among

    psychiatrists

    (Mace

    and

    Trimble,

    l99l).

    Furthennore,

    although

    most n€urologists

    would agree

    with

    the DSM-IV

    statement

    that

    "typically, individual

    conversion

    symptoms

    are of

    short

    duration'',

    many

    psychiatrist have

    rather

    observed

    hat

    conversionsymptoms

    often

    tend to

    persist

    or reoccur

    in association

    with

    other

    psy-

    chiatric comorbidities

    and

    pathological personal-

    ity traits

    (Ron, 1994;

    Binzer

    and

    Kullgren,

    1998;

    Stone

    et al.,

    2003).

    However,

    in most cas€s

    seenby

    neurologists,

    a

    rapid

    remission

    of initial

    symptoms

    is observed

    after appropriate behavioral treatment. The like-

    lihood of spontaneous

    emission

    has repeatedly

    been

    found

    to

    be

    -50-60Yo

    after

    I or

    2

    years

    {Singh

    and I-ee,

    199?;Binzer

    and Kullgren,

    1998;

    Crimlisk

    et

    al.,

    1998)

    or even

    gleater

    (Folks et al.,

    1984).

    Among

    young

    patients

    (< 27

    year-old),

    only 3Yo

    have

    symptoms

    or more

    than

    I

    month

    (Turgay,

    1990).

    The

    duration

    ofeffective

    behavi-

    oral

    treatment

    in such cases

    s usually

    between

    Personality

    disorder

    appears

    as a

    particularly-

    @ C r i m l i s k e t a l .

    1998;

    Crimlisk

    et al., 1998;

    Stone

    et al.,

    2003).

    [-qgs).

    t is ilieiffiirnportant

    to

    identity

    pot€n-

    tial

    risk factors

    during

    the

    initial

    evaluation

    of

    patients,

    n

    order

    to

    prevent he

    development

    of a

    more

    chronic

    handicap.

    Body,

    mind, and

    brain

    diseases

    A

    potential

    limitation

    to

    our

    current understand-

    ing of hysterical

    conversion

    comes

    rom

    a

    lack of

    clear definition

    of the

    boundaries

    with sorne

    elat-

    ed disorders.

    From

    the

    perspective f

    psychiatry,a

    number

    of

    problems

    are still

    unresolved

    concern-

    ing the terminology

    and

    classification

    f functional

    symptoms

    without

    an

    organic

    cause. n

    DSM-IV,

    the

    concept of

    hysteria

    has

    been broken

    down

    to

    different disorders, including somatoform disor-

    ders on one

    hand

    and

    dissociative

    disorders

    on

    the

    other

    hand,

    with

    conversion

    disorders

    being

    considered

    as

    a

    specific category

    of somatoform

    disorders

    involving

    sensorimotor

    symptoms

    or

    pseudo-seizures,istinct

    from

    other somatization

    symptorns

    or

    psychogenic

    ain

    disorders.

    Further-

    more, depression

    and

    chronic

    fatigue

    syndrome

    would

    fall

    under

    other diagnostic

    categories

    although

    in clinical

    practice there

    is often

    some

    overlap

    between

    all

    suchconditions.

    For example,

    . Furthermore.

    version

    disorders

    are defined

    as-d.gft"]q@'-@g

    t - ' - - *

    a

    ibecffit neurological

    Sl9lign-;gg[*qs---molQr

    dtien.$h

    or-

    sbm€ttisGnsory

    perception,

    Py9. 9-

    -

    ioniiGreifTniiead--unde

    enlc

    memory

    loss

    ls consl(]€fe(l

    _ll*:19._.--

    ine-"ataEmytor-"dissoaaf

    G-?1i-ofr

    E-re''oq

    :*

    - : j

    _

    ' -

    - _ - r '

    -

    * : -

    - ' : - c

    "

    t&G l

    +emory

    s

    obviously.?lig

    spe.sifrp

    fain

    fuiiiiion

    (see

    Markowitsch,

    1999,

    2003

    for

    a

    --.--'-:--..-.: .--

    associated

    with

    a favorable

    prognosis,

    acutenesS

    f

    presentation, nset

    St

    many patients recoveringfto-j,o y9lg 94

    2 to X

    weeks

    (Speed,

    1996).

    Soveral

    actors

    are

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    5/21

    Eames

    (1992) described

    a

    seriesgf

    , 9Z,g,g.ge s

    € - > - * . -

    fr65 were

    ad'iiiittecl

    d a

    iEhebilitation

    ward

    4nd-

    neuropsychological

    perspective of

    psychogenic

    amnesia).

    Therefore,

    unlike

    DSM-IV,

    the

    Interna-

    tional Classification

    of Diseases

    ICD-10) of the

    World Health

    Organization

    has

    included

    all

    sen-

    sorimotor and memory symptoms of presumed

    psychogenicorigin

    under

    the same

    general cate-

    gory

    of

    dissociative

    disorders.

    conversron

    casionally

    be

    a

    "complication"

    of

    organic

    brain

    disease,

    nd

    Schilder

    1935)

    wrote that brain

    dys-

    function could sometimesnduce

    "organic

    neurot-

    ic

    attitudes".

    More

    recently,

    1gi4tnggf$-$-9lp-U

    patients

    exhibited

    at

    diffuse brain lesions

    (Eames,

    1992).

    Similarly,

    Gould

    et al.

    (1986)

    re-

    ported that

    among

    a

    prospective

    eries

    of 30

    pa-

    tients

    with an

    acute

    hemispheric

    stroke,

    "atypical

    signs"

    usually

    suspected

    o

    reflect

    non-organic

    "functional"

    origin

    were

    observed

    n

    approximate-

    ly

    20% of

    cases

    e.g.,

    changrng

    deficit,

    patchy sen-

    sory loss, or "give-away" weakness).Finally,

    multiple

    sclerosis

    (e.g.,

    Nicolson

    and

    Feinstein,

    1994)

    and

    epilepsy

    (e.g.,

    Devinsky

    and

    Gordon,

    1998)

    constitute

    two

    other

    frequent

    neurological

    diseases

    n

    which

    not

    only

    some

    truly

    "organic'o

    manifestations

    may

    sometimes

    e

    difficult

    to dis-

    tinguish

    from

    non-organic

    disorders,

    but

    some

    pa-

    tients

    may

    also

    present with a

    combination

    of

    apparently

    both

    "organic" and

    "psychogenic"

    manifestations

    at

    the

    same

    ime.

    These

    occasional

    31 3

    associations

    between

    hysterical

    conversion

    and

    neurological

    diseases

    are

    not

    only

    challenging

    for current

    "dichotomous"

    classification

    schemes,

    but

    in fact

    might

    potentially

    provide valuableclues

    about the neurocognitivemechanismsby which

    awareness

    f

    a

    function

    can

    be dissociated

    rom

    actual

    abilities

    in

    a

    patient.

    However,

    the

    co-

    existence

    of these

    neurological

    diseases

    nd

    con-

    version

    might

    also

    ust

    be a

    coincidence,

    urely

    due

    to

    their

    high

    incidence

    or

    to any

    other

    general

    stress

    actors.

    Nevertheless,

    espite

    hese

    problems of

    defini-

    tions

    and

    associations,

    hysterical

    conversion

    is

    only

    rarely

    falsely

    diagnosed

    or an

    occult

    neuro-

    logical

    condition.

    Although

    a

    few early

    studies

    suggested

    hat

    up

    to 20o/o

    f

    patients

    initially

    di-

    agnosed

    with motor

    conversiondeficitsmay sub-

    sequently

    develop

    a

    real organic

    neurological

    disease

    xplaining

    their

    original

    symptoms

    (Slat-

    er,

    1965;

    Mace

    and

    Trimble,

    1996),

    several

    ecent

    studies

    have

    now clearly

    established

    hat

    such

    ates

    were

    overestimated

    nd

    that

    only

    l-5%

    of hyster-

    ical

    patients may

    present with a

    underlying

    but

    occult

    organic

    cause

    e.g.,

    Crimlisk

    et al.'

    1998;

    Stone

    et al.,

    2003).

    The rarity

    of organic

    diseases

    detected

    n current

    follow-up

    studies

    s

    probably

    due

    to

    several

    actors,

    including

    a

    better

    charac-

    terizationof neurologicaldiseases nd the availa-

    bility

    of

    more

    sensitive

    non-invasive

    diagnostic

    procedures.

    History

    and

    theories

    The

    term of

    "hysteria"

    was forged

    by the

    ancient

    Greeks

    to

    indicate

    that

    physical

    disturbances

    n

    the

    uterus

    were

    the

    primary cause of

    psychic

    symptomsn women.By contrast,all modern the-

    ories

    of

    conversion

    disorders

    acknowledge

    hat

    not

    only

    both

    men

    and

    women

    may

    be

    affected,

    but

    also

    a

    primary

    psychological

    disturbance

    s

    prob-

    ably

    responsible

    or

    triggering

    subjective

    physical

    s5rmptoms,

    nd

    perhaps also

    {br

    triggering

    some

    associated

    hanges

    n the

    neurophysiological

    tate

    of

    the

    central

    nervous

    system.

    However,

    the

    exact

    psychological

    actors

    to blame

    still

    remain

    disput-

    ed, and

    the

    possible implication

    of a

    particular

    From

    a neurological

    perspective, tU -JgIl

    ins that

    conversion

    may

    sometimes

    c6-eiist

    with a

    real

    organ

    ;"*pluined-by

    thit

    brain

    hsio.rtg

    Gowers

    .._+

    .c

    ----#-re

    6g9ij-already

    recognized

    hat hysteria

    could

    oc-

    Itrsi-"

    de-"liy3fdifit:Ii'ld"*

    behavi6-i'

    driii.3ig=.

    ihb

    colifi

    FortIi€ii'rcnutmttti6n;aiqtF -s99,-by--=qti-

    tol'ati"ratingscales-t+i9$'...[..1a-6.ffstq:*-s.dt-;C-

    hiviors'

    couiii-?ni'iiide

    exaggeration,

    secondary

    gain

    expectancy,

    i

    non-ggSgigp"age$f

    offe

    fldTrlii.'Inieiisf

    riglyjhoTa[tyru-qf

    patientspre-

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    6/21

    314

    brain functions in

    generating

    an abnormal

    phys-

    ical'experience till remainsunresolved.

    Since the l9th

    century, a myriad

    of different

    theories have been

    put

    forward to explain

    how

    hysterical motor or sensorydeficitsmight be im-

    plemented

    n the brain in terms

    of specific ana-

    tomical

    circuits, or in

    relation

    to

    putative

    cognitive

    architectures.However,

    most of these heories est

    on

    purely

    speculative

    rounds,

    and little empirical

    work has been conducted o

    test theseneuropsy-

    chological

    hypotheses.Although

    it is beyond

    the

    scope

    of this chapter

    o discuss ll of

    these heories

    in

    great

    detail

    (for

    more complete

    reviews, see

    Merskey,

    1995;Halligan

    et al.,

    2001),a few con-

    jectures

    on the neural

    substratesof hysteria

    that

    have been

    proposed

    by influential

    neuroscientists

    will be briefly

    illustrated. Although many

    old the-

    ories

    are susceptibleo misinterpretations

    ased

    on

    our

    current

    knowledge, t is nteresting

    o note

    that

    some of them

    might still appear

    attractive

    f they

    were rephrased

    using more

    modern concepts

    and

    terminologies

    f.rom current

    cognitive and

    aflective

    neurosciences.

    n

    fact, several ecent

    hypotheses

    about the

    possible

    cerebral

    correlates

    of hysterical

    conversion,

    proposed

    on the basis

    of new

    func-

    tional neuroimaging

    esults,can be traced

    back to

    strikingly similar ideas hat

    were elaborated

    more

    than

    a century ago, but naturallyphrased n terms

    of models of

    brain

    physiology

    rom

    that time.

    One of the first

    and best known

    hypothesison

    the

    cerebral mechanismsof

    hysterical

    conversion

    was

    proposed

    by Charcot in the

    early 1890s

    see

    Charcot, 1892;

    Widlocher, 1982;

    White, 1997).

    Giting

    activity of motor

    or sensory

    a[h@

    with-

    out any

    p€rmanent

    structural damage.

    Hysteria

    was thus consideredas a "neurosis" among other

    functional

    llnesses uch as

    epilepsyor

    Parkinson's

    disease.Charcot

    suggested hat such

    functional

    changes n the

    nervous system

    could be induoed

    by

    particular

    ideas, suggestions,

    or

    psychological

    states,as

    demonstratedby the

    effect of hypnosis

    on

    hysterical

    symptoms.

    Thus,

    hysterical

    paralysis

    could result from an

    inability to

    form a

    oomental

    image"

    of movement,

    or instead rom

    an abnormal

    "mental

    image" of

    paralysis.

    n

    essence"hese deas

    are echoing

    he more recent

    proposals

    of a

    selective

    impairment

    n action

    "representations" or inten-

    tional

    motor

    planning

    (e.g.,

    Spence,

    1999),

    al-

    though

    both the terminology

    and

    concepts f brain

    function haveconsiderably hangedsince hen.

    At the same

    ime as Charcot,

    Jun:lj 9{)gbg

    orooosed hat

    hvstericaldehcitswere

    the result of

    - : - 4

    "fixed

    ideas"

    that could take

    control

    over mental

    dr motor

    functions. Flowever.

    he added

    that such

    between

    distinct do-

    way to the classical

    heory of

    Freud and

    Breuer

    (1895),

    ater refined by Freud

    alone

    Freud,

    1909),

    who

    formulated a

    purely psychodynamic

    ascount

    with only minimal

    reference

    o the nervous

    systern.

    In brief. accordins

    o Freud

    and Breuer.hvsterical

    d"

    and

    eftry-o: --ftltgEt

    (one

    becoming ominated

    y

    the unconscious

    fixed

    idea). Such views

    paved

    the

    transformed

    nto

    bodilv

    complaints

    with symbolic

    vhlues.For

    this reasbn.

    hvsterical

    ef-rcitsid

    not

    obey anatomical

    constraints

    as observed

    with or-

    ganic

    neurological

    esions.

    This view has then

    nat-

    urally

    led to

    the

    term

    of

    "conversion disorder",

    and n many

    respectss still

    prevailing

    oday.

    Only

    later

    did Freud emphasize

    hat the

    unconscious

    affectivemotives usuallyhad their origin in sexual

    concerns

    rom early childhood.

    An attempt to

    integrate hese

    different cerebral

    and

    psychological

    perspectives

    was made by the

    Frenchneurologist

    Babinski

    n

    1912,

    who

    also

    irst

    described anosognosia

    a,fter

    brain damage two

    sequent interpretation

    stage.

    A more sophisticated

    neuro-anatomical

    scheme

    was later

    proposed

    by

    iZ-eai ould ariseat an

    u

    inducing

    a dissocia

    ons within

    the central

    nervous system,

    af-

    Pavlov

    1,928-1941,

    933),

    who

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    7/21

    cerebral enters

    possibly

    mediatingemotionalor

    -----

    rearncdcondrtroned esp gql_ q d

    lelg

    to a re-

    _---ffi

    active

    inhibition

    of

    cortical

    inputs

    imposed

    by_l he_

    frontal corte>i This inhibition was responsible or

    fitGtrffifalysis or anesthesia, hose duration

    was

    proportional

    to the amount

    of

    resources e-

    pleted

    n the individual.

    A number of more recent

    accounts based on

    neurophysiological speculations have

    similarly

    proposed

    a role for inhibitory

    or

    "filtering"

    mech-

    anismsas a likely neural

    substrate or

    generating

    hysterical

    conversion

    deficits. Thus, in line with

    Babinski and Favlov,

    Sackeim

    et

    al.

    (1979)-arld

    Stern

    (

    I

    983) hvooth"sizffffiT6iilor moti-

    vational

    processes

    might induce

    a

    selectiveblock-

    ----+--:-.__-.%

    ase

    or

    clrstortlon) I sensory nd/ormotor rnpglls,

    =#

    resultlng

    n

    therr

    exclusronrom conscrous

    ware-

    n"rr.

    i

    greater

    involvement

    of

    the right hemi-

    sphere in emotion might

    account

    for more

    frequent symptoms on the left side

    of the body

    observed

    n

    some

    series

    Stern,

    1983;seeStone et

    t972

    uncuons mechanisms

    the level

    of thalamic

    mrclei.under the influence

    315

    lsignals

    from

    limbic systems

    within orbitofrontal

    ,,

    and cingulate

    cortex during volitional movements

    i

    (Marshall

    et al., 1997).Again, such nhibition

    was

    lascribed

    to unconscious

    motivational factors

    Finallv- from the mid-1970so mi,

    ing to the influenceof the

    research n split-brain

    ical

    between t

    uvsrygg-_in_e-qg-

    impairment in the

    rigbt

    (Stern,

    1983)or left hem-

    l*i;tt"."*fpm:H;itiffi-

    "FrqS

    D:-fiowevei,

    oniy

    aiEriCii-thai onversion isordersmlelt'ilGlve

    t

    .,.#,*.-**@

    @..->===*

    dysfunction in the

    'omonitoring

    and controlli4g

    diiaaifif

    b:r?o;sri5GEsf

    .

    idulrin's l

    -

    i disso

    ciafivtiltilF

    il,t imffi

    "ff"ffi

    . , - . . " r y

    gt-qt g @-&iqq-(*s

    4-lo-

    stta

    eq4..&:ri

    l.

    1999),

    whereas

    Spiegel

    1991)

    nsteademphasized

    f _ e _

    __

    anattentlonalmechailsm_..l+e^4_ Atg4*b:"tb9=*a ejr-

    or cingllatg

    -aql[er.

    Likewise,KLtpjryn"1[ $CI

    nence ot'

    perceptron

    a-nd/or voluntary

    actlon,

    4

    presumably

    tnrough abnorrnal coordlnatlon

    oI

    particular

    neurocognitive

    modules. Oakley

    (1999)

    also referred

    to neuropsychologicalmoAels6ffi-

    tentlon o

    suggesthat dunng hystena, n lntern4l

    very

    l

    performance

    on

    ..._-=4

    n-urop-pfiological tests

    was otTered n support

    of these nterhemispheric

    ypotheses

    Flor-Henry

    et

    al . , 1981).

    In sum, most

    theoretical models trying to link

    hystericaldisorders

    with

    specific

    neuropsycholog-

    ical or neurophysiologicalmechanisms

    have es-

    sentiallybeen

    nspired by speculative rguments

    or

    analogies

    with

    general

    models

    of the brain and

    mind, rather than by the convergence f systematic

    empirical

    research. t is striking

    that relatively few

    studissover

    the

    past

    decades

    have exploited ne-

    urophysiological

    echniques

    see

    below) that

    pro-

    vide objective

    neasures

    fbrain

    functions

    (such

    as

    electroencephalogram

    EEG)

    or

    brain imaging),

    allowing

    a better identification

    of neurobiological

    factors associated

    with hystericalconversion.

    This

    is

    all

    the more surprising

    since the

    well-defined

    neurological-like symptoms

    of conversion

    (e.g.,

    paralysis,

    anesthesia,

    lindness,etc.)

    should

    po-

    tentially be amenable o a precise nvestigation

    with

    well-defined

    predictions

    about

    the site and

    type of neurophysiological

    ysfunction.

    In

    partic-

    ular, the advent

    of new

    tirnctional

    imaging tech-

    niques should now

    allow a refine

    assessment f

    functional correlates

    in brain activity

    potentially

    associated

    with

    hysterical conversion,

    and thus

    go

    beyond he

    dichotomous

    question

    of "organic"

    versus

    "non-organic"

    disease.

    A better

    know-

    ledge

    of such

    functional correlates

    might

    provide

    tation

    oenti6f- ex€cutive

    atten

    from awareness v a

    motor

    ontif Effi ffi;GtiotiITiste-

    i"ioiuine

    f6ritaFnnt

    ifr'su6i6iiud.

    r

    nuir

    ""

    rfi

    ri

    n;

    "

    ; J

    1

    D='dvitrq99)a-nifMurrii,a-ll"una

    "oil"ugu;

    (1997)

    \

    ,

    as well as Spence

    1999)

    and Spence

    nd colleagues

    f

    .

    (2000)

    also speculated that

    representationsof

    N

    i

    motor action miebt b€ inhibited

    in

    patients

    with

    I

    I

    hysterical

    paralysis,

    with activation n their

    motor

    I

    \

    cortex being actively suppressed

    by abnormal

    \

    r

    (Galin

    et

    al.. 1977). The

    greater

    occurrence of

    left

    r #

    hemrbodVdrsorders ed to the ldea that the trans-

    #

    IEiof sensory

    or motor

    inputs

    mieht

    btimpairg

    a1.,2002).

    Other researchersuchas

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    8/21

    6

    constraints on

    psychodynamic

    heories

    conversion,

    with

    greater

    biological and neuro-

    plausibility,

    and might also improve the

    assessment

    nd management f

    patients.

    orrelates

    of conversion

    EEG

    provided

    one

    of

    the first tools to meas-

    n activity, a number of studies from the

    o

    1970sonward

    have

    used this

    technique

    lectrophysiologicalmeasureso

    patients

    with hyster-

    conversion. These studies can

    generally

    be

    n two broad categories: hose aiming

    intact electrophysiological e-

    despite subjective unctional

    losses,and

    rying to determinesomeabnormal

    pattern

    with functional symptoms.

    or brainstem auditory evoked

    potentials

    are

    usually found to disclose ormal am-

    and latencies n the pr€sence f subjective

    blindness, or deafness, respectively

    Howard and Dorfman,

    1986;

    Drake,

    1990).

    results suggesting a

    paradoxical

    amplification

    rather than attenuation of tactile evoked respons-

    es during hysterical anesthesia

    Moldofsky

    and

    b _ ,

    ly early

    stages f cortical

    processing.

    Moreover,

    hese arly

    SEPsmay not necessarily orrelate

    with

    subjective

    perceptual

    experience, ince

    SI

    responses an still

    be

    elicited by unperceived timuli in

    patients

    with

    brain tumors involving the

    parietal

    lobe but spar-

    ing SI

    @reissl

    t al.,

    2001).

    Also, in

    patients

    n a

    vegetative

    state, devoid of any

    conscious

    perrep-

    tion,

    preserved

    I activation

    has

    been

    shown using

    functional imaging and simultaneously

    ecorded

    SEPs

    Laureys

    et al., 2002).Thesedata emphasize

    that

    SI

    activation does not necessarilymean con-

    scious omatosensory

    erception.

    On the other

    hand.

    a few other

    studies have re-

    ported

    subtle changes in

    paradigms

    that were

    slightly

    more

    sophisticated han

    just

    detection of

    simple actile stimuli.

    For instance, actile stimuli

    close o

    percepual

    threshold

    may fail to

    produce

    normal evoked

    potentials

    n

    patients

    with sensory

    conversion s)mptoms, even when stimuli above

    threshold still

    produce

    normal responses

    Levy

    and Mushin,

    1973). In addition, anomalies

    in

    the rate of habituation to repeated

    stimulations

    were observedn hysterical

    onversionusing SEPs

    (Moldofsky

    and

    England, 1975)as well

    as

    skin-

    conductanoe reactivity

    (Horvath

    et

    al.,

    1980).

    In

    normal subjects, responseswere

    found to de-

    creaseover time

    when comparing late blocks

    of

    stimuli relative o initial blocks. Such

    habituation

    could also be observed n

    patients

    with high levels

    of anxiety (Horvath et al., 1980), but was not

    present

    in

    patients

    with hysterical conversion,

    indicating that the latter tended

    to

    process

    frequent and exprcted

    stimuli as if they

    were

    still novel.

    Another

    r€cent

    rted

    ri5e

    P500

    conBonent

    (Lorenz

    et al.,

    component

    is typically elicited by

    clearly indicatejhalpd_mety

    sen-

    pathways

    are both

    qtnrcjurally-andftuqtiqo-

    in the

    patients.

    In

    the

    somatosensory

    been studied

    using

    MEG)

    in order to dis-

    activations

    n

    primary (SI)

    and

    secondary

    cortical areas

    (Hoechstetter

    et

    al.,

    2002),

    data from healthy

    people

    typically show a

    modulation of SII but not SI activity

    ra-sk-relatedactors. However,

    in

    patients

    with hyrloig[tpg@

    showe6; ndffiaf

    patter":of

    rfiponse

    r"t

    characteristic onpprr"nt.

    g"oeruied

    iiU"otn

    Sf

    controlaterally and ipsilaterally to

    the

    pafrlats

    relative to

    Cn{eclT,

    nTotfr-Srant su@-ffiCiGr

    FGid aialirnneige

    ithearlierEEG

    England,1975). aken

    the common

    or instance"

    standard soma

    potentials

    (SEPs),

    visual

    evoked

    potentials

    -case

    stud

    evoked Dotentia

    Tery""tfra

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    9/21

    novel

    stimuli

    n an "oddball"

    taskor by

    infrequent

    targets within

    a stream

    of successive

    timuli,

    and

    presumably

    eflects

    a normal orienting

    responseo

    relevant

    stimuli.

    Lorenz et

    al.

    (1998)

    designed

    an

    elegantEEG paradigm in which they repeatedly

    stimulated he

    unaffected eft

    hand of a man

    with

    hysterical sensory

    oss

    on the right hand,

    and oc-

    casionally

    applied a "deviant"

    stimuli on

    either he

    affected right

    hand, or on

    another finger of the

    $P*qlilg e:seje:

    infrequent stimuli

    on that side

    (i.e.,

    Iike

    the con-

    version

    patient),

    a P300was

    still normally evoked

    oy tnese o"uran@

    hand, indicating that

    a reduction of P300

    n the

    patient

    was

    not due

    to malingering or control by

    voluntary

    inhibition.

    Interestingly,

    reducedP300 responseso

    tactile

    "oddball" stimuli

    have also been observed

    n

    pa-

    tients

    who

    present

    with

    a

    "segmental

    exclusion

    syndrome"

    (Beis

    et al., 1998).These

    patients

    ex-

    hibit

    an abnormally

    prolonged

    under-use nd

    pain

    of

    their upper limb

    after sufferinga relatively

    mi-

    nor injury

    to

    peripheral

    body tissues

    n one hand

    or one finger,

    and such unctional exclusion

    of the

    limb cannot

    be explainedby the severity

    of

    injury.

    Although this syndrome

    s different from conver-

    sion disorder, t has similarly

    beenconsidered

    s a

    maladaptive

    deficit

    with a

    partly psychogenic

    r-

    igin, and abnormal P300 esponses €re nterpret-

    ed as

    reflecting

    some

    kind of attentional nhibition

    or hemineglect

    n motor

    behavior

    (Beis

    et al.,

    1998).Accordingly,

    anomalies n P300

    have been

    f,ound or undetected

    stimuli not

    only in

    patients

    with spatial hemineglect

    after right

    parietal

    lobe

    lesions

    Lhermitte

    et al., 1985),but

    also

    n

    patients

    with

    Parkinson's

    diseasewith

    impairments n in-

    tentional

    motor

    planning

    (Kropotov

    and Ponoma-

    rev,

    99l; Sohnet al., 1998).

    317

    Evoked

    potentials

    in other sensory

    modalities

    such as vision and

    audition have been

    ess often

    studied

    beyondelementary

    ensory esponses,

    er-

    haps

    because onversion

    disorders n

    such modali-

    tiesare ess requent,more variable,and/or seenby

    physicians

    ther than

    neurologists.n

    patients

    with

    functional blindness,

    a P300 was still

    evoked

    by

    unreported visual

    stimuli but with smaller

    ampli

    tude

    (Towle

    et al., 1985).

    By contrast,

    a study of

    auditory

    processing

    n

    patients

    with

    hysterical

    deafness sported

    a reducedP300

    response,with

    preservation

    of earlier Nl

    and N2 auditory

    re-

    sponses Fukuda

    et al., 1996).

    Some anomalies

    were also reported

    in

    patients

    with

    various som-

    atization symptoms

    for the auditory

    mismatch

    negativity(MM1.0, which is normally evoked by

    deviant

    stimuli in

    a series of repetitive sounds

    (James

    et

    al., 1989).

    Still other studies

    reported

    more

    general

    changes n EEG

    spectra

    n

    patients

    with conversion

    nd somatoformdisorders.

    nclud-

    distribution

    over

    right

    and lefllrontal

    - + _ * : + /

    y,

    motor conversion

    disordershave

    been

    investigated

    by transcranial

    magnetic stimulation

    bffih6sC"cb

    slds"did-rat

    changewhen

    pa_treats

    ecovered

    la$.qh-eil

    lveak-

    ness.There is

    also anecdotal evidence hat con-

    ffin patients may show an abnormal

    contingent negative variation (CI.W)

    component

    in

    EEG, which is normally

    evoked

    during

    motor

    preparation

    in response o a cue

    prior

    to

    an ex-

    Considered all ical

    da-

    t

    absenceof

    af-

    such

    esultsare usually aken to indicate

    hat mo-

    tor

    pathways

    are structurally

    and functionally in-

    tact in

    these

    patients.

    Only one recent

    studv

    &und

    a decreased xcitabilitffit

    Fgt-q$p-ry:€.:1ygf

    g*yU--.t'"e-q,e_e_;"r'

    eral left

    hystericalweakness

    Foong

    et al., 1997b),

    same unaffected

    left hand.

    The

    patient

    sbowed

    a

    normal P300

    esponseor

    dEfr6-ntJtffii-on:iG

    ffid*painful tactile stimuli, demonst

    (TMS),

    typically

    6voked

    tials

    over the motor co

    ffisfiis

    et a\., 1999).Again,

    pected

    o-be-judgedstimulus

    (Drake,

    1990).

    are

    Iectrng

    h€_p- ngg-sensoryor

    pnmary

    motol$-

    #

    Ems

    -Instead.

    anv

    chan-sesln-biain

    function

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    10/21

    3 1 8

    associatedwith

    conversion

    misht involve

    hipher

    f - - - i

    _

    h

    l_"dlo{.-pl-o-9"-e..s_s.lug,93._lppJ€Le$eg=*.1-_*F irq

    sensoryandJ_q_1-mo.Jpr.--SiSnal$._=.a1q._.ig -e"g"r.p" _ej

    di'tli

    moie'ioriipir*'

    information

    related to the

    meanng nd-.q i-rer.e-i-dr--;ji sdi-asa;eld;,

    (e.g.,

    motivational

    significanc€,

    ovelty, expected-

    ness,

    etc.). This might

    relate

    to

    the

    reduced

    P300 response

    ound in

    a

    few

    different studies

    using different

    paradigms.

    However,

    EEG and

    MEG

    investigations

    still remain remarkably

    scarce,

    and reported findings

    have too rarely

    been replicated to

    allow firm conclusions

    about

    any

    putative

    neural

    correlates

    ofrspecifrcconver-

    sion

    symptoms.

    Hemodynamic

    rain imaging

    Over

    the

    past

    l0

    years,

    functional

    brain imaging

    has literally exploded into

    innumerable

    paths

    of

    new research

    on the cerebralbases

    of

    various

    be-

    havioral functions

    in

    humans, ncluding not only

    perceptual

    and motor

    processes

    ccessibleo ex-

    ternal

    objective assessment,

    ut also much more

    complex mental

    operations elated

    to internal af-

    fective

    states

    Damasio

    et

    al.,

    2000),

    perceptual

    or

    motor magery

    Kosslyn

    et

    al.,

    1995;

    Ehrsson t al.,

    2003),and €ven unconscious rocessing r prelbr-

    ences

    Elliott

    and Dolan, 1998).This functional

    brain

    mapping approachhas.also

    oensuccessfully

    extended o

    a

    variety

    of

    psychiatric

    conditions

    such

    asdepression,

    bsessive-compulsive

    isorders.

    ho-

    bia,

    pos{-traumatic

    stressdisorders, chizophrenia,

    or hallucinations

    (e.g.,

    Frith

    and

    Dolan,

    1998;

    Parsey and Mann,

    2003; Kircher, this volume).

    Surprisingly,

    however,

    very

    few neuroimaging

    studies have been

    performed

    in

    patients

    with

    conversion symptoms. despite

    the fact that sucb

    symptomsmight often be very well suited to neuro-

    imaging nvestigations.

    Most

    neuroimaging studies

    of

    conversion

    used

    SPECT

    (single

    photon

    emission computerized

    tomography)

    or PET

    (positron

    emission tomo-

    graphy),

    and t-ocused

    n motor rather than sensory

    conversion

    symptoms.These

    echniques llorv on-

    ly

    a

    few

    brain scans o

    be taken and

    provide

    an

    estimateof activity

    averaged ver severalminutes,

    indirectly

    obtained

    by a measure f cerebralblood

    flow

    changes uring

    a restingstateor during a task

    period.

    The

    first of such SPECT studies

    was car-

    ried

    out by Tiihonen

    and colleagues

    Tiihonen

    et

    al.,

    1995)

    n

    a

    woman who

    had a long history

    of

    left hemisensory isturbances f presumedhyster-

    ical origin,

    and reported both decreases

    rytg4

    p_gg"t"i"9tt""{y-."Afg9'_".g'*

    A

    tivity when

    the affected hand of the

    patient

    was

    . . r : : " ' l - . - : - * ; - - * i . - * . * - - " . . - - .

    s ,iuulateClas

    compaEd=-ffilFlffiiF sym'metiiC

    pattern

    after recovery).

    However, his singleob-

    servation

    was mor€

    qualitative

    than truly

    quanti-

    tative,

    and not statistically analyzed.

    Similarly,

    another SPECT study reported a seriesof five

    pa-

    tients with

    heterogeneous onversion symptoms,

    including

    not only limb weakness ut

    also

    vertigo

    and

    gait

    disturbances

    $anci

    and Kostakoglu,

    1998), n whom

    brain scansat rest showed

    a re-

    duction in

    activity for several cortical regions,

    predominantly

    in left

    parietal

    and left temporal

    lobes,

    but with a

    great

    variability across

    patients.

    A more systematic

    SPECT study

    was

    conducted

    in our

    own center, n a

    group

    of seven

    patients

    who

    were

    prospectively

    elected asedon the

    pres-

    enceof

    an isolated and

    oofocal"

    motor conversion

    disorder, with

    a

    recent

    onset and short duration

    (<

    2 months)

    Vuilleumier

    et al., 2001).Strict se-

    lection

    criteria

    were

    used including: unilateral

    weaknessn upper and lower limb, with or with-

    out sensory oss n the same erritory, but without

    any other

    psychogenic

    or

    neurological symptoms

    (such

    as headache,vertigo, blurred vision, etc.),

    without

    any

    past

    history of major

    psychiatric

    or

    neurologicaldisease, nd without any

    organic

    le-

    sion

    as

    determinedby

    extensivemedical nvestiga-

    tions

    (i.e.,

    brain and spine MRI, SEPs.MEPs,

    VEPs,

    EMG, etc.). These

    patients

    were

    followed

    up for 6 months after the onsetof their symptoms,

    and underwent brain SPECT scanning in three

    different conditions: (l) a baseline est condition

    (To),

    with eye closed

    and no stimulation,

    rvhen

    motor symptomswere

    present;

    2)

    a

    passive

    acti-

    vation

    condition

    (Tl),

    with bilateral vibrotactile

    stimuli

    (50

    Hz)

    applied

    to both

    the

    affected

    and

    unaffected limbs simultaneously,

    when

    motor

    symptomswere

    pr€sent;

    and

    (3)

    the

    sameactiva-

    tion sondition

    (T2),

    again with bilateral vibrotac-

    tile stimulation of the affected and unaffected

    limbs,

    after motor symptoms

    had recovered

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    11/21

    (in

    four

    patients;

    three others

    had

    persisting

    or

    new symptoms at

    6 months follow-up).

    The

    rationale of vibrotactile stirnulation

    was to

    pro-

    vide

    an

    indirect activation of both sensory and

    motor areas in the brain (since such stimuli

    provide

    inputs

    not only

    to

    cutaneous but also

    deep endon fibers), n a completely

    passive,

    ym-

    metric, and

    reproducible

    manner.

    All voxel-based

    analyses n this study were

    done on a

    group

    basis using statistical

    parametric

    mapping

    (SPM)

    (Friston

    et al.,

    1995).

    A first analysis comparing activation

    by bilat-

    eral

    vibrotactile

    stimulation

    to resting baseline

    (Tl

    >T0) revealed elatively symmetric

    ncreasen

    cerebral blood

    flow in frontal and

    parietal

    areas

    involved in somatosensory nd motor functions,

    both

    ipsilaterally and contralaterally

    o the motor

    conversion symptoms

    (Vuilleumier

    et a-1., 001).

    This result

    converges

    with

    previous

    electrophysio-

    logical data indicating

    intact sensorimotor

    path-

    ways in

    such

    patients.

    A second,more

    interesting

    analysis

    compared activation by bilateral

    vibro-

    tactile stimulation after

    recovery relative to the

    same timulation

    during symptoms

    T2>Tl), pro-

    viding

    a

    direct measur€ f changes

    n

    brain

    activity

    specificallyassociated

    ith hystericalmotor weak-

    ness, rrespective fany other distinctive

    pattern

    of

    brain function in these

    patients (e.g.,

    related to

    depression,

    anxiety, or other

    personality

    charac-

    teristics). This

    cr€ases ln

    in the

    motor defrcit"

    rv

    her, the degreeof decreasesn caudate

    nu-

    "l""S

    u"dJE

    wa-J-ffififfim-"laGd

    with

    ilii-oqlation

    oF

    "Uf'gsfi.{';iffittre*t'"

    regions n patientswho did not r€cov€r6 months

    later, relative to thosewho

    subsequently

    ecovered

    (Fig.

    3B). This reducedactivation

    n contralateral

    basal

    ganglia-thalamic

    circuits

    might therefore

    provide

    a

    plausible

    substrate for the subjective

    motor conversion deficits. Finally,

    we

    also

    per-

    formed a reversecomparisonof

    vibrotactile stim-

    ulation

    during symptoms relative to recovery

    Ol>T2).Tfril_rbpy_qd."o-ply-1gdd.

    n-sJess.e.$.

    nss-

    m1 o ep=9.{y,_cp$-er_a"an rp 4Je$l_ts-t}ssgmp.l.oms,

    4 l'_l"eg9l_tggl9 .9:f

    gry_qetgg_grthe

    ffinot;eem

    suppr€sre

    f f a : -

    (as preniouslfffr]oSul:-Iiidwig,

    1972; Sierra

    31 9

    rPPhYsrsJ-oCY

    and Berrios, 1999)but instead appearsenhanced

    (Moldofsky

    and England,

    1975: Hoechstetter

    er

    a1.,2002).

    These

    selectiveanomalies n

    subcortical brain

    regions

    during motor conversion

    (Vuilleumier

    et

    al.,

    2001)are intriguing since

    hese egionsare in-

    terconnected

    nto functional

    loops forming a cor-

    tico*striato-thalamo-cortical

    circuit

    which is

    critical

    for voluntary motor action

    (Alexander

    et

    al., 1986)

    and since the striatum

    (especially

    cau-

    date

    nucleus)

    constitutes an essentialneural

    site

    within such oops wheremotivational signalscan

    modulate motor

    preparation

    activity

    (Mogenson

    et

    al., 1980;

    Kawagoeet al., 1998;Haber,

    2003;see

    Fig.

    ).

    It is

    therefore conceivable

    hat thesecir-

    cuits might become unctionally

    suppressed uring

    hystericalmotor

    conversionunder the

    influenceof

    a

    particular

    affective

    or motivational states,

    re-

    sulting

    in an impaired "motor

    readiness"or

    im-

    paired

    "motor intention" for

    the

    affccted

    imb(s).

    Notably, in humans, ocal

    tesions

    e.g.,

    stroke) af-

    fecting the

    basal

    ganglia

    (Watson

    et

    al.,

    1978;

    Healtonet al., 1982) nd

    thalamus

    Laplane

    et al.,

    1986;

    on Giesenet al., 1994)are implicated

    n a

    syndrome of unilateral motor neglect,

    n which

    patients present

    with impairments in motor

    use

    that cannot be explained

    by

    primary

    weakness ut

    rather

    reflect

    a lack of motor intention

    or

    plan-

    ning. Sucha lossof

    motor intentionhasalsobeen

    implicated

    in

    the failure of some brain-damaged

    patients

    o becomeaware of their

    (real) paralysis,

    i.e., anosognosia

    for hemiplegia

    (Gold

    et al.,

    1994;

    Vuilleumier, 2000b),suggesting hat subjec-

    tive experience

    of conscious motor action

    and

    volition might be linked to basal ganglia or

    thalamus unction. Also

    in support of this, direct

    electric

    stimulation of lateral thalamic nuclei

    by depth

    electrodes may trigger

    contralateral

    movementswith a subjective

    experience f volun-

    tary action

    (H6caen

    et al., 1949).Finally, changes

    in basal

    ganglia

    activity

    have also been impli-

    .cated

    n immobilization

    behaviors exhibited by

    animals to

    protect

    an injured limb

    (De

    Ceballos

    et al., 1986).

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    12/21

    320

    o: =

    Decreased

    activation

    d urin

    g

    co n0alatelal

    se

    nsorlmotor

    sym

    ptoms

    relative

    o recovery

    (T2

    >

    Tl)

    Caudde

    oo

    6

    dl

    P

    E

    (D

    o

    ?

    (A )

    D€ffcit

    Flecovery

    $1)

    (r2)

    Deficit

    Recovery

    (rl)

    {r2)

    Deficit

    Recovery

    (r1)

    ff2)

    lt

    d)

    g

    (B)

    s0

    80

    70

    60

    50

    Ilecreased

    cdvafion

    urlng

    conoalateral

    ymptoms

    scan

    Tl)

    predicfi

    g clinical

    evolution

    Symptoms

    fier4

    montfts:

    $

    eenistentln=3palienG)

    ffi

    lmproved

    n=

    4

    patients)

    Fig. 3. Changes

    n brain activity

    associated

    with

    hysterical

    paralysis.

    (A)

    Selective

    decreases

    n activity

    were found

    in the thalamus,

    caudate, and

    putamen

    (upper

    row)

    in the hemisphere

    contralateral

    to the

    limb affected

    by sensorimotor

    symptoms.

    Measures

    of

    regional cerebral blood

    flow

    (rCBF) were obtained

    by SPECT scans

    n seven

    patients

    (ower

    row) during

    bilateral

    sensorimotor

    stimulation

    by

    vibrotactile

    stimuli

    when their symptoms

    were

    present (f

    I scan),

    and in four

    patients when their symptoms

    had abated

    3-4 months later

    (I2

    scan).

    Brain activity

    was

    decreased

    n all these

    subcortical

    regions n all

    patients

    n T'l as

    compared

    with the same

    regions

    n T2 or with homologous

    regions n the

    bcmisphere psilateral

    to the

    symptoms n

    T1.

    (B)

    Such

    decreases

    n the thalamus

    and

    caudate

    (but

    not in the

    putamen)

    were

    more severe

    n the initial scan

    fl)

    in the

    three

    patients

    who had

    persisting

    symptoms

    at

    follow-

    up

    4 months tater, as compared

    with.four other

    patients

    who had

    recovered,suggesting

    lat the

    severity of

    decrease

    at the time of

    initial

    symptoms, may

    predict

    the

    duration of their

    symptoms.

    (From Vuilleumier et al.,

    2001).

    Thalamus

    Putamen/Pailidum

    Cilc€,/e

    Thalamus

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    13/21

    321

    Motor

    commands

    Modulations

    bymofivafon

    f,

    or emotlon

    \ \ \

    z

    \t-* r-'r ---)/;

    \-*"(

    \

    ---'',-'

    it[[[E\ | -

    f f i ) ) -

    VL-z

    m

    prinErymotorarea

    TUIUIIETI

    & oallidum E

    Supdemertary otor rea

    ffi

    Prsmotor

    area

    lffi

    Preftontalcortex

    I

    Linbicarea

    Fig. 4.

    Schematic llustration of cortico-subcortico-cortical loops.

    These circuits link

    various

    areas n frontal cortex

    to

    the caudate

    nucleus,

    putamen

    and

    pallidum,

    halamus,and

    then back o the cortex,allowing a modulation and coordination of motor commands

    initiated in the cortex during movement

    execution- but

    presumably

    also during more complex

    cognitive

    operations. Such oops

    provide

    several neural sites,

    particularly

    in

    the striatum/caudate,

    where

    neural signals can be modulated by allective and motivational inputs

    from many other brain regions

    (such

    as

    orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate cortex, or amygdala), constituting a cerebral system hought

    to

    be critical for the integration of volitionally guided and emotionally triggeredexpressionsof behavior.

    ,/

    Motor

    (f

    execut'on

    \\

    S

    halamus

    prod

    uced i nclqglgd_agUyalrp:Lof--:rentroge&l

    fiffiffi*Aortex, including

    right anterior cingulate

    dndTEmbrbitttfibii&l cbffi;*fii"h

    .I,.

    "inorr"*

    duiill|

    irioVein€nt-biEtlirtion- lith

    the unaffected

    ri-ghtEg. II ivasccincludCtihat during initiation of

    m6ttii"action on the affected side, some signals

    might be

    generated

    in the limbic ventro-medial

    frontal and cingulate cortex due to affective or

    motivational factors, and that such signalsmight

    actively inhibit the activation of motor cortex,

    preventing

    the execution of normal movements

    (Konishi

    et a1.,1999;Paus,2001).However,

    t

    is

    possible

    hat this increase n frontal ;ndqlguEg

    afnvationmghr

    fr

    pncm

    tnese

    egions

    ricTr-

    as-diffi

    -ulty-,

    rqni-

    tbimE-oTleilure_-&us-gt-al-.--lt9-8;van.V.een

    200-a)-ortiriflict

    (Dehaene

    t al.,

    2003;Badre

    and

    WeEner,

    O0+; ue to ambiguous

    ask demands

    (r

    ffi

    biatum

    Other functional changes

    n brain areas elated

    to voluntary motor control havebeensuggested

    y

    two PET

    studies

    n

    patients

    with

    hystericalmotor

    deticits

    (Marshall

    et

    al.,

    1997;

    see also Spence,

    1999;

    Spence t al.,

    2000; or

    review). Marshall et

    al.

    (1997)

    studied

    a single

    patient

    with a history of

    unilateral

    left

    leg

    weakness

    ersisting

    more than 2

    years,

    n a

    task requiring

    either to

    prepare

    or to

    execute movementswith either the left (affected)

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    14/21

    (i.e.,

    "try

    to move even

    f

    you

    cannot").

    In any

    healthy subject who perfonned the same motor

    preparation

    and execution

    ask as n the

    preceding

    study, but now

    49.

    hlp1ggg_qUgglstiol

    of uni-

    lateral

    paralysis

    (Halligan

    et al., 2000).

    Although

    this result suggests

    ome similarity

    betweenhyp-

    notic suggestionand conversion

    symptoms,

    this

    classic elationship

    (entertained

    since Charcot by

    many others:

    see Bliss, 1984;

    Spiegel, l99l;

    Oakley, 1999)may

    still not be firmly established

    (see,

    .g.,

    Persinger, 994;Foong

    et al.,1997a).

    A subsequent

    PET study therefore

    compared

    three

    patients

    who had

    hysterical weakness

    two

    patients

    n

    left arm and

    one

    patient

    in right

    arm),

    with four healthy

    control subjectswho

    were in-

    structed o feign

    motor weakness f the right

    hand

    (Spence

    t al., 2000).

    All

    participants

    had to

    per-

    form regularly

    paced

    movementswith

    a

    joystick

    held

    n

    their affected

    or pseudo-affected)

    and. As

    1€I9 P,

    Patients

    it

    co d

    -+

    dgglgl*4activity in

    left

    prefrontal

    cortex relative

    to the

    decreased

    activity

    in-rignt

    "prefroiittfE6itex-r€E-

    ti ve t

    o co

    n

    ve

    Si6-fr

    -tfi

    6if ilTlie au h

    o rs su

    glEildd

    that leTtfionlatd66ffiIffi*n in hysteria may reflect

    the role of these

    cortical areas n motor

    planning

    (Spence,

    999).However.

    qft frontal hypoactivity

    ^ . - r u

    is also frequently

    seen n other conditions

    such as

    SpryssEu-

    evueis,

    2T031

    Ali"patfiil

    in t[i"

    Study had a history of

    depression, lthough none

    needed reatrnent

    at the time of scanning.More-

    over, these eft frontal anomalies id

    not

    provide

    a

    direct functional

    substrate for the contralateral

    motor

    deficit

    itself, since he

    affectedside differed

    across

    he

    patients.

    But nevertheless,ome

    mpair-

    ment in internal representationsof voluntary

    movements s likely to be

    present

    n these

    patients,

    as also suggested y

    purely

    behavioral

    studies hat

    compared

    mental motor imagerywith the

    affected

    and unaffected

    hands

    in individuals

    with motor

    conversion

    Maruff

    and Velakoulis,2000; Roelofs

    et

    al.,2002).

    Finally,

    only two recent

    studies used functional

    magnetic resonan@maging

    (fMRI)

    to examine he

    functional

    patterns

    of brain

    activity associatedwith

    hysterical

    conversion,concerning

    de{icits n somato-

    sensory

    processing Mailis-Gagnon

    er

    al.,

    2003)

    _- - __J_

    .+-

    -

    ---.---"-g_\

    and visual

    pejgpption

    Werring

    et

    al., 2004).

    Mailis-

    Gagnon et al.

    (2003)

    studied

    four

    patients

    with

    chronicdeficits n sensation f touch and/or chronic

    pain

    a"ffecting

    one

    or

    more limbs on

    one or both

    sides,

    while they undenvent

    fMRI scanning during

    blocks of brush

    and mildly noxious

    stimulation on

    their affected and

    unaffected body

    parts.

    Results

    revealed

    different

    patterns

    for different brain

    areas.

    First,

    unlike stimulation

    on intact imbs

    (which

    were

    always

    perceived

    nd

    reported),

    both noxious

    and

    non-noxious

    stimuli

    on

    the

    une"Al;u-f*rri"r,

    ye @&t}djgp-t :e e

    the

    halamus,nsular,

    nferior rontal,

    and

    posterior

    iingnlatb'

    Coitic

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    15/21

    an

    attempt

    to move

    the

    parulyzed limb

    in

    motor

    conversion

    or hypnosis

    (Marshall

    et

    al.,

    1997;Halhgan

    t a1.,2000).

    ikewise,

    recent

    MRI

    cortex,

    temporal

    poles

    as

    well q;

    on

    fitrs"Tftffi

    #-----@

    different

    from

    previous

    findings

    for sensorimotor

    deficits,

    this

    pattern

    was again

    interpreted

    as

    gen-

    erallyconsistent

    with the dea

    hat

    conversion

    might

    involve inhibitory modulationof visual processing

    through

    increases

    n the activity

    of limbic

    areas.

    Altogether,

    these

    new functional

    neuroimaging

    data

    provide

    compelling

    evidence

    hat

    "function-

    al"

    symptoms

    n

    patients with hysterical

    conver-

    sion

    may at

    least in

    part

    correspond

    to

    specihc

    components

    n brain

    function,

    potentially

    under-

    lying

    an abnormal

    awareness

    f

    perceptualand/or

    motor

    abilities.

    Here

    again,

    here are

    still

    too

    few

    studies,

    using

    different

    paradigms

    in

    different

    types

    of

    patients,

    such

    hat no

    definite

    conclusion

    tivation

    as

    r, further systematicstudies are needed o

    better

    tease

    apart the neural

    correlates

    associated

    with

    subjective

    det'icits

    hemselves

    rom

    other

    as-

    p€cts potentially associated

    with

    coexisting

    disor-

    ders, such

    as changes

    in

    mood

    or anxiety"

    personality

    characteristics,

    xpectations,

    ttention,

    coping

    reactions

    as

    well asany activity

    conceivably

    related

    to unconscious

    affective

    motives

    and

    con-

    flicts

    that

    were

    postulated

    by

    Freudian

    psychody-

    namic accounts.

    More

    questions

    nd

    nerv

    directions

    A better

    understanding

    of

    functional

    changes n

    brain

    activity

    during

    hysterical

    conversion

    s cer-

    tainly important not only becausehis may yield

    new

    insights

    into neural

    correlates

    of

    subjective

    experience

    and

    awareness,

    ut also

    because

    his

    may

    provide new

    constraints

    on

    theoretical

    ac-

    counts

    of

    conversion,

    n a

    neurobiologically

    plau-

    sible

    ramework,

    and

    therefore

    ead

    o improve

    the

    diagnosis

    and

    management

    f

    patients.

    There

    s

    no

    doubt

    (and

    perhaps

    no surprise)

    hat an

    altered

    experience

    f

    bodily

    functions

    might

    be associated

    with specific

    modifications

    n the

    brain

    networks

    normally

    responsible

    or

    generating

    our

    conscious

    experience f such unctions,asalso demonstrated

    for

    even

    more

    extreme

    disturbances

    n bodily

    awareness

    Vuilleumier

    et

    al.,

    1997;Blanke

    et al.,

    2004;Blanke,

    his volume).

    Demonstrating

    specific

    neural

    correlates

    of hysterical

    onversion

    may also

    help

    reassure

    he

    patients

    as

    well as

    their

    caretak-

    ers, ncluding

    nursesand

    doctors

    who sometimes

    show

    negative

    or

    unsympathetic

    reactions

    when

    confronted

    with complaints

    unaccompanied

    by

    visible organic

    pathology.

    As

    already

    suggested

    y

    James

    1896),

    this may

    eventually

    help convince

    some

    skeptics

    hat

    hysteria

    s a

    "real disease,

    ut a

    mental disease".

    Importantly,

    neurobiological

    findings

    should

    lead

    to

    refine our

    current

    psychopathologicalex-

    planations of conversion,

    by

    suggesting

    possible

    mechanisms

    by

    which

    the

    mind can

    produce

    changes

    n

    the brain

    and body

    functions.

    Such

    mechanisms

    re likely

    to

    involve

    dynamic

    nterac-

    tions

    between

    neural

    systems

    mediating

    specific

    functions

    (e.g.,

    motor,

    somatosensory,

    r visual

    processing) nd neural

    systems

    esponsible

    or af-

    fective

    evaluations

    and

    reactions,

    based

    on current

    aswell aspastexperiencese.g., imbic areasn the

    broad

    sense, uch

    ascingulate

    gyrus,

    orbitofrontal

    cotrtex,

    or amygdala).

    Such

    interactions

    between

    distributed

    brain areas

    might

    be usefully

    investi-

    gated

    by neuroimaging

    studies

    using

    connectivity

    analysis

    (e.g.,

    Friston,

    1994;

    Mclntosh

    and

    Gonzalez-Lima,

    1994).

    Various statistical

    tools

    now

    exist o

    describe

    ow

    neural activity

    in one

    or

    several

    egions

    can

    either

    influence,

    or

    instead

    be

    contingent

    upon,

    neural

    activity

    in other

    regions.

    other

    hand.

    increased

    activity

    was found

    S

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    16/21

    324

    New methods

    allowing

    inferences

    bout

    the

    direc-

    tionality

    of such

    nfluences

    suchas dynamic

    caus-

    al

    modeling,

    see

    Friston et

    al., 2003)

    might

    prove

    particularly valuable n

    this context.

    Thus, in our own study of hystericalparalysis

    (Vuilleumieret al.,

    2001)demonstrating

    educed

    activation

    in the

    contralateral

    basal

    ganglia-

    thalamic

    circuits,

    which

    became

    normal

    again af-

    ter

    recovery

    rom

    paralysis,we were

    also

    able to

    show

    that such

    changes

    o-varied

    with concomi-

    tant changes

    n other

    distant

    brain areas,

    where

    activity

    was not

    globally

    reduoed

    or enhanced

    but

    rather appeared

    differentially

    coupled

    with

    the ba-

    sal

    ganglia-thalamic ircuits

    during

    paralysis.This

    was

    demonstrated

    using

    a type of

    principal

    com-

    ponent analysis scaledsubprofilemodeling;Mo-

    eller

    and Strother,

    99l) that

    allowed

    us to

    identify

    three

    distinct

    networks

    of regions

    whose

    activity

    tended

    o covary

    together across

    all

    subjects

    and

    all sessions,

    rrespective

    of their absolute

    evel of

    activity.

    Critically,

    this

    analysis

    dentified a

    net-

    - -

    work including

    he cau

  • 8/17/2019 Hysterical Conversion and Brain Function

    17/21

    generalization

    of the

    findings

    from one

    study to

    another.

    Future studies might

    therefore

    ruitfully

    com-

    pare patterns

    of brain

    activity in

    conversion

    pa-

    tients showing different types of deficits (e.g..

    motor or

    somatosensory

    oss)

    to

    determine

    more

    directly

    what are the

    changes elated

    to specific

    symptoms,and

    conversive

    eactions n

    general

    for

    instance, esting

    whether some

    effects n anterior

    cingulate and

    frontal cortex may

    arise

    rrespective

    of

    the type

    of deficit).

    New studies

    should also

    use

    different

    possible

    approaches,

    ,or instance

    by ex-

    amining

    brain function

    more systematically

    uring

    and after

    conversion

    deficits in

    the same ndivid-

    uals; by manipulating

    more

    explicitly

    factors re-

    lated to attention, expectationor inhibitiory and

    by investigatingany

    differences

    n brain reactivity

    to stressful

    probes

    presented

    o these

    patients

    even

    when hey have

    ecovered rom

    specificconversion

    symptoms.

    In addition,

    we need to

    b€tter under-

    stand the

    cerebral mechanisms

    nvolved

    in other

    psychiatric

    disturbances

    haracterized

    by dissoci-

    ative symptoms,

    such as

    psychogenic

    amnesia

    or

    "mnestic

    block"

    (Markowitsch,

    2003;

    Glisky et al.,

    2004), or depersonalization

    disorders

    affecting

    p€rception

    of the

    self and/or

    of the environment

    in stressful ituations

    Lanius

    et a1.,2002;

    Reinders

    et al., 2002).

    New imagingdata

    may help to clarify

    the

    possible

    relationships of these

    disorders

    with conversiondisorders

    and other types

    ofpsy-

    chogenic

    "block"

    affecting

    conscious ensoryand

    motor functions.

    Interestingly, unctional neuro-

    imaging

    correlatesof dissociative

    esponses ave

    also highlighted

    a key role of

    medial

    prefrontal

    areas for integrating

    perceptual

    or motor

    repre-

    sentations

    with a subjective sense

    of conscious

    control

    (Lanius

    et a1.,2002;

    Reinderset al., 2003).

    Conclusions

    In

    summary, although

    hysterical conversion

    has

    rernained a common

    and fascinating

    disorder at

    the

    border

    between

    neurologyand

    psychiatry,

    ,ew

    systematic and controlled

    studies have

    yet

    been

    conducted

    using modern neuroimaging

    ools.

    This

    is

    surprising

    given

    the variety of

    currently availa-

    ble techniques

    ERPs,

    MEG,

    PET, SPECT, MRI)

    325

    the

    rich

    productivity of imaging

    research n

    neuropsychiatric

    domains.

    A better

    under-

    ing of functional

    changes

    n brain activity

    ing hysterical

    conversion

    will undoubtedly

    pro-

    unique insights into neural mechanismsot

    consciousness,

    ut should

    also

    mprove the

    gnosis,

    management,

    and assessment

    f

    prog-

    ;is n these

    patients. Finally,

    further research

    n

    field might certainly

    contribute

    to strengthen

    links between

    psychiatry

    and

    brain sciences.

    thanks

    to F. Assal,

    C. Chicherio,

    T. Landis,

    S. Schwartz

    for their

    precious

    collaboration,

    to P. Halligan for manyvaluable discussions

    of this

    work

    was supported

    by

    grants

    from

    Swiss

    National Foundation.

    Rbferences

    Atexander,

    G.E., Delong,

    M.R. and Strick,

    P.L.

    (1986)

    Par-

    [llel

    organization

    of functionally

    segregated

    ircuits linking

    lbasal

    anglia

    and cortex.

    Ann.

    R.ev.Neurosci.,

    9:

    357-381.

    AlTrerican sychiatric

    Association.

    1994).

    Diagnostic and

    Sta-

    tistical

    Manual of Mental

    Disorders:

    APA,

    Washington,DC.

    Bdbinski,J.

    (1914)

    Contribution

    a

    l'6tudedes roubles

    mentaux

    dans

    I'hemipl6gie organique

    (anosognosie). Revue Ne-

    furologique,

    27 845-848.

    B{dre, D. and Wagner,

    A.D.

    (2004)

    Selection,

    ntegration,and

    lonflict

    monitoring: assessing

    he nature

    and

    generality

    of

    prefrontal

    cognitive

    control mechanisms.

    Neuron.

    4l:

    473.J.87.

    Bdis,J.M., Andr6, J.M.,

    Vielh, A. and

    Ducrocq, X.

    (1998)

    Event-related

    otentials

    n

    the segmental

    xclusion yndrome

    [f

    the

    upper limb.

    Electromyogr. CIin.

    Neurophysiol., 38:

    247-252.

    Bibzer,

    M. and Kullgren,

    G.

    (1998)

    Motor

    conversion isorder:

    I

    prospective

    - to 5-year ollow-up

    study. Psychosomatics

    39: 519*527.

    Blbnke,O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L. and.Seeck,M. (2004)Out-

    of-body

    experience and

    autoscopy

    of neurological

    origin.

    Brain. 127:243-258.

    Bliss,

    E.L.

    (1984)

    Hysteria

    and

    hypnosis.J. Nerv.

    Ment. Dis..

    172:203-206.

    Bdidges,K.W. and

    Goldberg, D.P.

    (198