impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › uq:382128 ›...

38
1 Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction in a mouse model of frontotemporal dementia Ann van der Jeugd 1,2 , Ben Vermaercke 2 , Glenda M. Halliday 3 , Matthias Staufenbiel 4 , and Jürgen Götz 1,* 1 Clem Jones Centre for Ageing Dementia Research (CJCADR), Queensland Brain Institute (QBI), University of Queensland, St Lucia Campus (Brisbane), QLD 4072, Australia; 2 Laboratory of Biological Psychology, Brain and Cognition, University of Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium; 3 Neuroscience Research Australia, Barker Street, Randwick, NSW 2031, Australia; 4 German Centre for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, D- 72076 Tübingen, Germany *Author for correspondence: Tel: +61-7-334.66329; Fax: +61 7 3346 6301; Email: [email protected] Abstract Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) is a neurodegenerative disorder, a major subset of which is characterized by the accumulation of abnormal forms of the protein tau, leading to impairments in motor functions as well as language and behavioral alterations. Tau58-2/B mice express human tau with the P301S mutation found in familial forms of FTLD in neurons. By assessing three age cohorts of Tau58-2/B mice in a comprehensive behavioral test battery, we found that the tauopathy animals showed age-dependent signs of impulsivity, decreased social exploration and executive dysfunction. The deficit in executive function was first limited to decreased spatial working memory, but with aging this was extended to impaired instrumental short-term memory. Tau pathology was prominent in brain regions underlying these behaviors. Thus, Tau-58-2/B mice recapitulate neurological deficits of the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), presenting them as a suitable model to test therapeutic interventions for the amelioration of this variant.

Upload: others

Post on 25-Jun-2020

9 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  1  

Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive

dysfunction in a mouse model of frontotemporal dementia

Ann van der Jeugd1,2, Ben Vermaercke2, Glenda M. Halliday3, Matthias Staufenbiel4,

and Jürgen Götz1,*

1Clem Jones Centre for Ageing Dementia Research (CJCADR), Queensland Brain

Institute (QBI), University of Queensland, St Lucia Campus (Brisbane), QLD 4072,

Australia; 2Laboratory of Biological Psychology, Brain and Cognition, University of

Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium;  3Neuroscience Research Australia, Barker Street,

Randwick, NSW 2031, Australia; 4German Centre for Neurodegenerative Diseases

(DZNE), Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, D-

72076 Tübingen, Germany

*Author for correspondence: Tel: +61-7-334.66329; Fax: +61 7 3346 6301; Email:

[email protected]

Abstract

Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) is a neurodegenerative disorder, a major

subset of which is characterized by the accumulation of abnormal forms of the protein

tau, leading to impairments in motor functions as well as language and behavioral

alterations. Tau58-2/B mice express human tau with the P301S mutation found in

familial forms of FTLD in neurons. By assessing three age cohorts of Tau58-2/B mice

in a comprehensive behavioral test battery, we found that the tauopathy animals

showed age-dependent signs of impulsivity, decreased social exploration and

executive dysfunction. The deficit in executive function was first limited to decreased

spatial working memory, but with aging this was extended to impaired instrumental

short-term memory. Tau pathology was prominent in brain regions underlying these

behaviors. Thus, Tau-58-2/B mice recapitulate neurological deficits of the behavioral

variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), presenting them as a suitable model to

test therapeutic interventions for the amelioration of this variant.

Page 2: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  2  

Keywords: Behavior; executive function; frontotemporal dementia; frontotemporal

lobar degeneration; social exploration; tau

Highlights

• Tau58-2/B mice present with a tau pathology in cortical areas implicated in

bvFTD.

• Tau58-2/B mice exhibit motor and sensory gating anomalies at an early age.

• With aging the mice show decreased sociability but no altered exploration.

• The mice further display increased risk-taking behaviors and executive

deficits.

• First limited to spatial working memory, it later extends to instrumental

memory.

1. Introduction

Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) is a form of dementia that is characterized

by atrophy of the frontal and temporal lobes and deposition of the microtubule-

associated protein tau (Galimberti and Scarpini, 2010; Rademakers et al., 2012;

Sieben et al., 2012). Under physiological conditions, tau is mainly localized to the

axon of mature neurons where it binds to and stabilizes microtubules; however, tau

has also been found in the dendritic compartment where the protein has a role in

targeting the kinase Fyn to the dendritic spines (Ittner et al., 2010). Under

pathological conditions such as FTLD, tau becomes hyperphosphorylated and forms

filaments that eventually form microscopic lesions known as neurofibrillary tangles

(NFTs) (Ittner et al., 2015). FTLD has been difficult to diagnose due to the

heterogeneity of the associated symptoms, which affect behavior, language and

cognition (Bigio, 2013; Mendez, 2004; Seltman and Matthews, 2012).

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) as the clinical presentation of FTLD is the

second most common type of presenile dementia and the fourth most common type of

senile dementia, although it is among the more costly due to its symptom

characteristics (Neary et al., 2005; Seltman and Matthews, 2012). Clinically, FTD is

divided into three subtypes: a behavioral variant (bvFTD) that affects social skills,

Page 3: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  3  

emotions, personal conduct, and self-awareness, semantic dementia that compromises

language comprehension, and motor variants leading to muscle wasting (Hodges et

al., 2004). BvFTD presents with changes in social behavior and conduct, such as loss

of social awareness and social withdrawal, restlessness and poor impulse control

leading to compulsive behaviors including stereotyped hair-pulling and skin picking

(Eslinger, Moore, Anderson, & Grossman, 2011; Lindau et al. 2000; Mendez &

Perryman, 2002; Pressman & Miller, 2014; Snowden et al., 2001, 2003). At later

stages, FTD patients develop deficits in executive function: they have problems

planning, coordinating and executing simple tasks (Harciarek and Cosentino, 2013;

Huey et al., 2009; Johns et al., 2009; Moy et al., 2004; Stopford et al., 2012). In

addition to the characteristic behavioral changes, the clinical features of FTD can be

complicated by neurological signs, such as motor neuron signs, parkinsonism,

and gait disturbances, with some patients developing motor problems resulting from

motor neuron pathology (Devenney et al., 2015; Merrilees et al., 2010).

To elucidate the contribution of hyperphosphorylated tau pathology to the

behavioral manifestations and cognitive deficits observed in FTD, we analyzed the

tau expression pattern and studied the effects of tau overexpression by subjecting

P301S tau transgenic Tau58-2/B (Tg) mice to a behavioral test battery probing for

signs and symptoms relevant for FTD. We analyzed Tau58-2/B mice of three age

groups to investigate whether with progressive tau accumulation this would be

associated with more pronounced behavioral changes. We found that this

murine model is histologically characterized by progressive deposition of

hyperphosphorylated forms of tau in neurons. In addition, we found deficits in

behaviors modeling the symptoms of bvFTD, including increased impulsivity and

risk-taking behaviors, decreased social interest, and executive dysfunction. The

distinctive behavioral phenotype was augmented with advanced age, reflecting the

age-dependent tauopathy that characterizes the Tau58-2/B mice. Together, this set of

FTD-like phenotypic traits and a distinct brain pathology renders the Tau58-2/B strain

a suitable model for bvFTD, with the prospect of determining whether therapeutic

interventions also ameliorate the phenotype that characterizes the behavioral variant

of FTD.

2. Materials and methods

Page 4: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  4  

2.1. Mouse models. Tau58-2/B mice (Tg) express the human 0N4R tau isoform

together with the P301S mutation under control of a murine Thy1.2 promoter on a

C75Bl/6 background (Tau58, Novartis Institutes for Biomedical Research, Basel,

Switzerland). Mice from the Novartis colony were separately shipped to Australia to

establish independent colonies. One colony was mainly analyzed histologically (van

Eersel et al., 2015). We established a separate colony by continued breeding of Tau58

onto a C57BL/6 background, generating Tau58-2/B mice. Non-transgenic littermates

(wild-type, Wt) were included as controls.

2.2. Experimental design. For behavior, mice were divided into the following age

groups: 2-3 months, 6-7 months, and 10-11 months. Animals were housed in groups

of 2–5 animals per cage. They were kept on a 12  h light/dark cycle (light on at

8:00  AM, with testing during the light period) with free access to food and water

unless otherwise stated. For behavioral experiments, the Tau58-2/B mice were

directly compared with Wt littermates (5 males and 5 females per genotype per age-

group), and tested cross-sectionally using the following experimental sequence:

SHIRPA screen, open field, social exploration, light/dark box, Y-maze, and puzzle-

box (Supplementary Table 1, Supplementary Table 2). For histological studies (n=3

per age group), the 6-7 month group was used after the behavioral data had been

obtained (by then 7-8 months old) and the 10-11 month group after the behavioral

data had been obtained (by then 11-12 months old). A separate batch of 16 months

old Tau58-2/B mice was included for histology. All animal experiments were

approved by the Animal Ethics Committees of the University of Queensland and all

procedures complied with the statement on animal experimentation issued by the

National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia.

2.3. Histology. Animals were anesthetized, perfused, and brains removed and post-

fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde at 4 °C overnight, dehydrated, and then embedded in

paraffin. Seven µm-thick brain sections were cut with a microtome in the frontal or

sagittal plane and mounted on silane-coated slides. Brains of 7-8, 11-12, and 16

month-old Tau58-2/B mice were analyzed for the presence of pathological tau species

(Xia et al., 2015) (Fig 1). The following anti-tau antibodies were used: pSer235

(1:500, Thermo Scientific), pSer422 (1:500, Thermo Scientific), AT8

Page 5: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  5  

(pSer202/pThr205 Tau, 1:500, Thermo Scientific), AT100 (pThr212/pSer214 Tau,

1:500, Thermo Scientific), AT180 (pThr231/pThr235 Tau, 1:500, Thermo Scientific),

and HT7 (pan Tau, 1:500, Thermo Scientific), which recognizes total tau.

Bielschowsky silver staining was employed to reveal NFTs (Ke et al., 2009).

2.4. Murine neurobehavioral screen and motor abilities. A comprehensive

modified SHIRPA screen (Rafael et al., 2000; Rogers et al., 1997) was performed.

Using this test battery, physical characteristics, sensory reactions and reflexes, as well

as motor abilities were investigated (for a complete checklist and photos of the setups

used see Supplementary Data). The collected physical characteristics included the

body weight and general appearance. Next, mouse behavior was observed which

included transfer behavior, spontaneous activity, and the occurrence of tremors,

palpebral closure and gait (Supplementary Fig 1). Subsequently, reactions to simple

stimuli such as touch escape, trunk curl, and the reaching reflex, Preyer reflex, and the

toe pinch reflex were assessed. Finally, basic motor abilities were measured using the

grip strength, wire hanging and accelerating Rotarod tests.

2.5. Exploratory behavior, sociability and anxiety. General exploration and social

exploration (Callaerts-Vegh et al., 2012; Crawley, 1985) were examined using a 30 ×

30 cm2 square arena of Plexiglas. Animals were placed in the experimental room

30  min before the test started. They were then placed individually in the arena (600

lux) for 10  min, where their movement was recorded using EthoVision video tracking

equipment and software (Noldus, Wageningen, The Netherlands). Total distance

traveled and velocity were included as measures of locomotor activity. Time spent

exploring the center (within an imaginary inner square of 20 x 20 cm) and the corners,

and the number of visits to the center and corners were recorded as a measure of

anxiety-like behaviors. In addition, heat maps were generated using a custom-made

program to examine anxiety and thigmotaxis (wall-hugging behavior) (Van Der Jeugd

et al., 2013).

For the assessment of sociability, the open field arena contained a cage with a

diameter of 10 cm and a height of 20 cm holding an unfamiliar mouse of the same

gender. Again, total distance traveled and velocity were included as measures of

locomotor activity. The time exploring the stranger mouse (within an imaginary

Page 6: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  6  

annulus of 5 cm around the cage containing the mouse), and the number of visits to

the center of the arena was recorded as a measure of social exploration.

2.6. Anxiety, impulsivity and risk-taking behaviors. The light/dark box arena

consisted of a Plexiglas box divided by a small underpass into two compartments: a

larger brightly lit zone (58 cm long, 28 cm wide, 750 lux) and a smaller covered dark

zone (15 cm long, 28 cm wide) (Crawley, 1985; Tasan et al., 2009). Prior to the test,

animals were habituated to the dark for 30 min. They were then placed in the dark

chamber of the apparatus facing away from the door and tracked for 5 min using

EthoVision software (Noldus). The latency to the first entry into the light, the time

spent in the illuminated area, and the number of transitions were recorded as

indications of impulsivity and anxiety. In order to assess risk-taking behaviors in

more detail (Baeta-Corral and Giménez-Llort, 2014; Varty et al., 2002), the frequency

of stretch-attendance postures (with the animal stretching forward into the underpass

and then retracting to its original position) was also monitored.

2.7. Executive function. Spatial working memory was tested with the spontaneous

alternation behavior (SAB) paradigm (Hughes, 2004; Lalonde, 2002; Naert et al.,

2013). Testing occurred in a Y-shaped maze with three white, transparent plastic arms

arranged at 120° angles (9 cm wide, 40 cm long arms, 30 cm deep). The mouse was

placed in the start arm of the Y-maze and allowed to freely explore the three arms in a

5 min trial (extra maze cues were present on the walls). The number of arm entries

and the number of triads were recorded in order to calculate the percentage of SAB.

An entry occurred when all four limbs were within the arm.

To assess problem solving capacities and short-term memory (STM), the mice

were subjected to the puzzle box (Ben Abdallah et al., 2011; Galsworthy et al., 2005).

The arena consisted of a Plexiglas box divided into two compartments by a small

underpass: a larger brightly lit zone, the start box (58 cm long, 28 cm wide, 750 lux)

and a smaller covered dark zone, the goal box (15 cm long, 28 cm wide). Mice

underwent a total of six trials over two consecutive days, with three trials per day: the

day started with a training session followed by two puzzle sessions (on day 1 these

were two dig trials, and on day 2 two plug trials). Mice were deprived of food two

days prior to the test (while their body weight was maintained at approximately 85%

of its initial weight), and food pellets were presented in the goal box. On training

Page 7: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  7  

trials, mice were introduced into the start zone, and taught to move into the goal zone

through a narrow underpass (~ 4 cm wide) separating the two compartments. For the

burrowing trials, the underpass was filled with sawdust and the mice had to dig their

way through. For the plug trials, a square, cotton plug that mice had to pull with their

paws to enter the goal zone obstructed the underpass. Time to enter the goal box was

measured (with a cut-off of 3 min, thus a 180 s penalty was given when the animal

did not solve the puzzle).

2.8. Statistics. ANOVA was used to determine genotype, and gender and interaction

effects between the three age groups. All data are represented as means (± SEM), and

p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant (* for differences between genotypes,

# for differences between gender).

3. Results

3.1. Tau transgenic Tau58-2/B mice present with a widespread, progressive

tauopathy

In tauopathy, progressive hyperphosphorylation of tau, in particular at pathologic

epitopes, occurs as disease progresses, concomitant with NFT formation. To

determine the progression of tau pathology in Tau58-2/B mice, the mice were

histologically analyzed with a set of phospho-tau-specific antibodies (pS235, pS422,

AT8, AT100, and AT180) as well as human tau-specific antibody HT7 (Fig 1A-E). At

8 months of age, Tau58-2/B mice display subtle deposition of hyper-phosphorylated

tau mainly in the CA1 region of the hippocampus and the cortex (Table 1). With age,

the mice developed a more pronounced tau pathology, with hyperphosphorylated tau

being found in the cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum as shown for 12 months of

age. As the mice aged even further, histological analysis revealed NFT deposits;

Bielschowsky silver staining of sagittal sections demonstrated stained NFT-like

structures and neuronal cell bodies in the hippocampus, throughout the whole frontal

cortex including the anterior cingulate cortex and orbitofrontal cortex, and in the

cerebellum and brainstem (Fig 1B'-E').

3.2. Higher arousal and reduced neuromotor abilities in Tau58-2/B mice

Page 8: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  8  

Assessing the physical characteristics of the mice we found that the Tg mice had a

lower body weight than age-matched Wt mice, at all ages tested (three categories: 2-3

months (F1,19=13.71, p = 0.003), 6-7 months (F1,19=39.98, p = 0), and 10-11 months

(F1,19=21.21, p = 0.001)) (Table 2). Evaluating the existence of whiskers we did not

find a difference between Tg and Wt animals. However, when assessing the

constitution of the fur this showed that the 10-11 month-old Tg mice had more bald

patches and wounds than their Wt littermates, which was not seen for the two younger

age groups (10-11 months: F1,19=48.52, p < 0.001). When observing the mice for

habituation of activity in a new environment, Tg mice were indistinguishable from Wt

age-controls. However, we noted some tremors and a less fluid gait in 6-7 and 10-11

month-old Tg mice compared to age-matched Wt mice (tremor: 6-7 months:

F1,19=7.33, p = 0.02; 10-11 months: F1,19=25.96, p < 0.001; gait: 10-11 months:

F1,19=5.15, p = 0.038).

In terms of sensorimotor reflexes, we found no difference in visual acuity by

reaching reflex, or nociceptive sensation as measured by toe pinch and trunk curl.

However, when evaluating the acoustic acuity we found that this elicited a stronger

flinch response to the sound in 6-7 and 10-11 month-old Tg mice as compared to their

respective age-matched Wt controls (6-7 months: F1,19=41.25, p < 0.001; 10-11

months: F1,19=27.40, p < 0.001). When evaluating Tg mice for the touch escape

response, we also found a higher response in 10-11 month-old Tg mice (10-11

months: F1,19=5.15, p = 0.038). Finally, assessing the motor abilities of the mice, we

found that 10-11 month-old Tg mice were impaired in muscular grip strength

(maximal grip: 10-11 months: F1,19=31.67, p < 0.001; mean grip: 10-11 months:

F1,19=108.07, p < 0.001). Also, in the wire hang test, aged Tg mice fell off faster than

Wt mice (6-7 months;: F1,19=6.74, p = 0.025; 10-11 months: F1,19=39.62, p < 0.001).

Finally, in the accelerating Rotarod test, a similar outcome was observed (maximal

latency on the rod: 6-7 months: F1,19 = 21.21, p = 0.001; 10-11 months: F1,19=28.10, p

< 0.001; - mean latency on the rod: 6-7 months: F1,19=26.1, p < 0.001; 10-11 months:

F1,19=37.11, p < 0.001). When investigating the main effect of gender (Supplementary

Results), differences were observed for weight: at all ages males weighed more than

females, irrespective of their genotype. However, in Tg male mice we observed more

tremors, bald patches and wounds compared to their female Tg and male Wt controls.

Also, in the grip test, both Wt and Tg males outperformed females as measured by

Page 9: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  9  

maximum and mean pulling force. Overall, Tau58-2/B mice presented with an overall

increase in nervous behavior and arousal at young ages, neuromotor deficits at adult

ages, and a lower body weight at all ages.

3.3. Tauopathy mice show no alterations in general exploratory behavior or

anxiety

To examine general exploratory behavior and anxiety, mice were observed in the

open field test. Total path length and velocity were included as measures of locomotor

activity; however, no changes in velocity or overall distance traveled were found

between the genotypes at any of the ages tested (Fig 2A). Time spent in the center and

corners were recorded as a measure of anxiety, and were found to be unaltered

between genotypes at all ages examined (Fig 2B). Numbers of center and corner visits

were unaltered; this was also confirmed by displaying the data in heat plots (Fig 2C).

No gender differences were found in this test. As indicated above, these findings

showed no differences in locomotor activity because the overall spontaneous

locomotion and general motor activity was not altered between the genotypes, nor

was there any evidence for an anxiolytic or anxiogenic profile in the Tau58-2/B mice.

3.4. Tauopathy mice have deficits in social exploration

The social exploration test is used to study social interactions in mice. No change in

total distance travelled or velocity was found in Tg compared to Wt mice at any age

tested (Fig 3A). The time spent in the center of the field (within an imaginary annulus

of 5 cm around the cage containing the stranger mouse) was recorded as a measure of

social exploration. Interestingly, decreased exploration of stranger mice in 6-7 and 10-

11 month-old Tg mice was observed compared to Wt littermates (6-7 months: F1,19 =

12.19, p = 0.006; 10-11 months: F1,19=4.70, p = 0.047) (Fig 3B). This was confirmed

by the number of entries into the center of the arena which differed between

genotypes: Tg mice visited the center with the stranger mouse less frequently than Wt

mice (6-7 months: F1,19 = 14.63, p = 0.03; 10-11 months: F1,19=14.70, p = 0.037) (data

not shown). Heat plots verified that there were fewer activity hot spots near the cage

containing the stranger mouse in Tg mice from 6 months onwards (Fig 3C). No

gender differences were found in this test. Thus, importantly, although Tg mice did

Page 10: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  10  

not show altered overall exploration levels per se, they were more reluctant to explore

another mouse.

3.5. Increased impulsivity and risk-taking behaviors in tauopathy mice

The light/dark box is used in the assessment of anxiety and risk-taking behaviors. The

latency to the first entry into the light and the time spent in each of the two

compartments was assessed to measure impulsivity and anxiety. We found that Tg

mice spent more time in the illuminated area than Wt mice at all ages (2-3 months:

F1,19=7.34; p = 0.019; 6-7 months: F1,19 = 11.11, p = 0.007; 10-11 months: F1,19=16, p

= 0.0001) (Fig 4A). Moreover, we found Tg mice to emerge faster from the

illuminated area than Wt mice (2-3 months: F1,19=14.09, p = 0.003; 10-11 months:

F1,19=5.06, p = 0.032) (Fig 4B). We found a gender main effect in the latency to enter

the illuminated area at 6-7 months (F1,19 = 5.024, p = 0.041), with females entering the

illuminated area faster than males. There was no difference in the number of

transitions between the dark and light compartments between the groups (data not

shown). Furthermore, we found fewer stretch-attend postures in Tg mice at 6-7 and

10-11 months compared to their age-matched Wt littermates (6-7 months: F1,19 = 9.73,

p = 0.01; 10-11 months: F1,19=17.64, p = 0.001) (Fig 4C). Hence, one may conclude

that the Tg mice act more impulsively and erratically.

3.6. Tauopathy mice have deficits in executive function

To assess executive function we first subjected the mice first to the Y-maze that

measures spatial working memory. We found that total entries into the Y-maze arms

did not differ between Tg and Wt mice, at any of the ages tested (data not shown).

However, the percentage of SAB was significantly different between the genotypes,

with Tg mice making fewer alternations compared to age-matched Wt mice, with an

interaction effect for gender at 6-7 months (main genotype effect: F1,19 = 15.63, p =

0.002; genotype x gender interaction effect: F1,19 = 6.82, p = 0.024), and at 10-11

months (main genotype effect: F1,19 = 53.39, p < 0.001; genotype x gender interaction

effect: F1,19 = 5.602, p = 0.032) (Fig 5A). Heat plots verified this lack of SAB in the

Tg mice (Fig 5B). Next, in the puzzle box paradigm, mice were presented with

several puzzles of increasing difficulty. In the first phase, a simple emergence test

Page 11: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  11  

from the brightly lit arena into the dark goal box, none of the mice were impaired

(data not shown). However, in the second phase, that required the mice to dig their

way to reach the goal box, Tg mice failed the test from 10 months onwards (F1,19 =

16.446, p = 0.001) (Fig 5C). Moreover, on the next day thus requiring intact STM,

both the 6-7 and 10-11 month-old Tg mice were found to be impaired (6-7 months:

F1,19 = 12.46, p = 0.005; 10-11 months: F1,19 = 30.44, p < 0.01) (Fig 5D). No gender or

genders x genotype interaction effects were found in this test. In conclusion, the Tg

mice display specific executive function deficits as shown by working and STM

impairments.

4. Discussion

Various aspects of tauopathy have been modeled in mice, but the behavioral aspects

that specifically characterize FTD have been less explored. In our study we found that

the P301S human tau transgenic Tau58-2/B strain displays a bvFTD-like phenotype.

Specifically, we found that this murine model is characterized by the deposition of

hyperphosphorylated forms of tau in neurons in frontal and temporal brain areas, and

presents with consistent deficits in behaviors resembling the symptoms of bvFTD,

including decreased social behavior, impulsivity and increased risk-taking behaviors

and impairments in executive function.

Previous studies have shown that motor performance is impaired in Tau58-2/B

mice as assessed in the Rotarod, beam and horizontal pole tests (van Eersel et al.,

2015). In the present study, we were able to replicate the progressive genotype-linked

differences in the Rotarod (however at half the speed in our study), wire hang and

grip strength tests, but now add to this a more comprehensive behavioral analysis,

addressing aspects of FTD-like behaviors. For instance, we found that Tau58-2/B

animals had tremors and displayed a less fluid gait. Indeed, in FTD patients,

extrapyramidal symptoms including akinesia, Parkinsonian gait and resting tremor

have been reported (Diehl-Schmid et al., 2007). Upon inspection of the cerebellum

and brainstem, regions known to contribute to motor problems, we found tau

pathology in the older Tg mice.

Moreover, we found evidence for increased arousal and reactivity to sensory

stimuli, an aspect of sensory gating that is observed in individuals with FTD and

Alzheimer's disease (Gibbons et al., 2008; Grunwald et al., 2003; Knight et al., 1999;

Page 12: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  12  

Landqvist Waldö et al., 2014; Perriol et al., 2005; Takeuchi et al., 2011). In our study,

we observed that Tau58-2/B mice were more sensitive to an unexpected stimulus, as

measured by a higher startle response in the Preyer reflex and touch escape assay. In

two related P301S murine models it had been discovered that prepulse inhibition

(PPI), another marker of sensorimotor gating, is altered (Koppel et al., 2014;

Takeuchi et al., 2011). However, we were prevented from performing a PPI assay

with our mice because of the motor weakness that characterizes the aged Tg animals.

Sensory gating is highly dependent on the prefrontal cortex (Chao and Knight, 1995;

Knight et al., 1999), a brain area for which we found massive immuno-reactivity for

hyperphosphorylated tau using a series of epitope-specific antibodies, and NFTs as

visualized with Bielschowsky.

Impairment of social interactions, including social withdrawal, loss of

empathy and social misconduct, is one of the core characteristics of FTD (Eslinger et

al., 2011; Hodges, 2013; Passant et al.; Rankin et al., 2005; Savage et al., 2014). We

found that Tau58-2/B animals spend a significantly shorter amount of time with a

stranger mouse than Wt mice. This is in accordance with other studies that found

deficits in social interactions in mouse models resembling clinical FTD, such as in

progranulin-deficient, and senescence-accelerated-prone (SAMP) mice (Filiano et al.,

2013; Meeker et al., 2013; Takeuchi et al., 2011; Yin et al., 2010). It has been

reported that orbitofrontal dysfunction is related to both apathy and disinhibition in

FTD patients (Peters et al., 2006). In accordance with this we noted increased tau

immuno-reactivity and NFT formation by silver staining in 12 month-old Tg animals.

It is noteworthy that with the open field test we did not find evidence for an

altered general exploration in our tau transgenic model. As the exploration of a

stranger mouse is performed in the same arena as the open field test, we conclude that

the decrease in sociability we see in the Tg mice is restricted to exploration of a

stimulus animal.

Our results revealed that in the light/dark box, the Tg mice spent more time in

the illuminated compartment than their age-matched Wt counterparts. This is in

contrast to the findings of the open field test where the Tg mice spent an equal

amount of time in the center and corners compared to the Wt mice. Moreover, when

we assessed marble burying in the mice, we found that Tg mice bury roughly the

same number of marbles within a 30 min period as their age-matched Wt littermates

(data not shown). The marble burying task can be used as an indicator of both

Page 13: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  13  

obsessive compulsive-like behavior and anxiety-like behavior; however, the neuronal

circuitry of this behavior has not been clearly elucidated (Njung’e and Handley,

1991). For some tau transgenic strains, it has been shown that the mice can be

anxiolytic and/or thigmotactic correlating with a pathology in the amygdala (Baeta-

Corral and Giménez-Llort, 2014; Egashira et al., 2005; Sterniczuk et al., 2010; Van

der Jeugd et al., 2013). Incidentally, obtaining coronal sections, we also found a

prominent accumulation of tau in the amygdala of the Tau58-2/B mice (data not

shown).

Interestingly, our Tg mice emerged faster into the illuminated area than their

Wt littermates after being habituated to the dark half an hour prior to the test. This

could indicate increased (motor) impulsivity and behavioral disinhibition in the Tg

mice. More evidence for impulsivity comes from the increase in risk-taking behavior

that was observed in our Tg animals by decreased stretch-attendance postures in a

fearful situation. In individuals with FTD, cases of pathological stealing, an

excessive gambling or alcohol consumption have been reported (Cruz et al., 2008;

Grochmal-Bach et al., 2009; Manes et al., 2010; Mendez, 2011; Pompanin et al.). All

of these violations can be regarded as disinhibitory deficits, which are pathologically

related to impairments in specific prefrontal cortex regions. For instance, the

ventromedial prefrontal cortex is associated with emotion regulation, the anterior

cingulate cortex with emotional empathy, and the orbitofrontal cortex with

compulsive behavior (Knutson et al., 2015; Mendez, 2010; Starkstein and Robinson,

1997). Related to this, we observed bald patches and in some cases even skin wounds

in 75% of the Tau58-2/B male animals, but only in one Wt mouse. This could be due

to excessive barbering, an abnormal repetitive behavior, which is analogous to human

compulsive hair pulling and skin picking, as commonly observed in FTD patients

(Pompanin et al., 2014). To rule out the possible effect of hetero-barbering or

aggression, in a future study repetitive self-grooming and aggression could be

examined separately. Increased aggression could also be explained by the higher

arousal in male Tg mice as observed by the touch escape and Preyer reflex. In later

stages of the disease, a decline in executive function is often seen in FTD patients

(Eslinger et al., 2011; Harciarek and Cosentino, 2013; Huey et al., 2009; Johns et al.,

2009; Seltman and Matthews, 2012; Shea et al., 2014). To evaluate executive

function, the animals were first subjected to the Y-maze test, which depends on their

intrinsic motivation to explore a novel environment. Notably, our 6-7 and 10-11

Page 14: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  14  

month-old Tg mice showed a working memory deficit in the Y-maze. In rodents, it

has been shown that the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus are engaged in this spatial

working memory task (Dillon et al., 2008; Naert et al., 2013; Reisel et al., 2002).

Moreover, mice were subjected to several problem-solving tasks requiring not only

intact spatial STM but also an instrumental response. The sequence over two days

(day 1 consisting of two dig trials, day 2 two plug trials) allowed assessing STM for

species-specific instrumental responses (Cowan, 2008). We found that only the 10-11

month-old Tg mice performed worse than their Wt counterparts on the dig trials on

day 1. However, when tested the next day on the plug trials, now also the 6-7 month-

old Tg mice failed, thus suggesting an STM deficit at this age. In support, staining of

brain sections with a panel of antibodies revealed hyperphosphorylated tau species

and NFTs in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in the Tg mice.

In FTLD, tau is abnormally phosphorylated, forming fibrillar aggregates that

can be visualized with silver impregnation methods as NFTs. In our Tau58-2/B mice,

we found evidence for progressive tau pathology leading to NFTs, including

phosphorylation of pathological epitopes in brain areas relevant for FTLD.

Bielschowsky silver staining confirmed the presence of NFTs - this was in accordance

with another recently published study using Tau58 mice (van Eersel et al., 2015).

In conclusion, we assessed the behavior of three age groups of Tau58-2/B

mice with a progressive tau hyperphosphorylation in frontal and temporal regions,

focusing on evaluating FTD signs and symptoms. We found that aged Tau58-2/B

mice mimic the three core characteristics of FTD: increased impulsivity and risk-

taking behavior, impairment of social interactions as shown by decreased social

exploration time, and mental rigidity as revealed by poorer working memory and an

inability to resolve problem solving tasks. This finding presents Tau58-2/B mice as a

model to validate therapeutic interventions to ameliorate the behavioral symptoms of

FTD.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Estate of Dr Clem Jones AO, as well as grants from

the Australian Research Council (ARC) [DP130101932] the National Health and

Medical Research Council of Australia (NHMRC) [APP1037746, APP1003150] and

an ARC LIEF grant [LE100100074] to JG. AVdJ and BV are both supported by a

Page 15: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  15  

FWO postdoctoral fellowship. We thank Linda Cumner, Tishila Palliyaguru, Trish

Hitchcock, and the animal care team for animal maintenance, Gerhard Leinenga and

Dr. Daniel Blackmore for advice regarding the experimental setup, Dr. Robert

Sullivan for histological preparations, Matthew Pelekanos for help with histology,

Luke Hammond for help with imaging, and Rowan Tweedale for reading of the

manuscript.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary data to this article can be found online.

Page 16: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  16  

References

Baeta-Corral, R., Giménez-Llort, L., 2014. Bizarre behaviors and risk assessment in

3xTg-AD mice at early stages of the disease. Behavioural brain research 258,

97–105. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.10.017

Ben Abdallah, N.M.-B., Fuss, J., Trusel, M., Galsworthy, M.J., Bobsin, K., Colacicco,

G., Deacon, R.M.J., Riva, M.A., Kellendonk, C., Sprengel, R., Lipp, H.-P., Gass,

P., 2011. The puzzle box as a simple and efficient behavioral test for exploring

impairments of general cognition and executive functions in mouse models of

schizophrenia. Experimental neurology 227, 42–52.

doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2010.09.008

Bigio, E.H., 2013. Making the diagnosis of frontotemporal lobar degeneration.

Archives of pathology & laboratory medicine 137, 314–25.

doi:10.5858/arpa.2012-0075-RA

Callaerts-Vegh, Z., Leo, S., Vermaercke, B., Meert, T., D’Hooge, R., 2012. LPA(5)

receptor plays a role in pain sensitivity, emotional exploration and reversal

learning. Genes, brain, and behavior 11, 1009–19. doi:10.1111/j.1601-

183X.2012.00840.x

Chao, L.L., Knight, R.T., 1995. Human prefrontal lesions increase distractibility to

irrelevant sensory inputs. Neuroreport 6, 1605–10.

Cowan, N., 2008. What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and

working memory? Progress in brain research 169, 323–38. doi:10.1016/S0079-

6123(07)00020-9

Crawley, J.N., 1985. Exploratory behavior models of anxiety in mice. Neuroscience

and biobehavioral reviews 9, 37–44.

Cruz, M., Marinho, V., Fontenelle, L.F., Engelhardt, E., Laks, J., 2008. Topiramate

may modulate alcohol abuse but not other compulsive behaviors in

frontotemporal dementia: case report. Cognitive and behavioral neurology  :

official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology 21, 104–

6. doi:10.1097/WNN.0b013e31816bdf73

Devenney, E., Vucic, S., Hodges, J.R., Kiernan, M.C., 2015. Motor neuron disease-

frontotemporal dementia: a clinical continuum. Expert review of

neurotherapeutics 15, 1–14. doi:10.1586/14737175.2015.1034108

Page 17: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  17  

Diehl-Schmid, J., Schūlte-Overberg, J., Hartmann, J., Förstl, H., Kurz, A.,

Häussermann, P., 2007. Extrapyramidal signs, primitive reflexes and

incontinence in fronto-temporal dementia. European journal of neurology  : the

official journal of the European Federation of Neurological Societies 14, 860–4.

doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2007.01773.x

Dillon, G.M., Qu, X., Marcus, J.N., Dodart, J.-C., 2008. Excitotoxic lesions restricted

to the dorsal CA1 field of the hippocampus impair spatial memory and extinction

learning in C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiology of learning and memory 90, 426–33.

doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2008.05.008

Egashira, N., Iwasaki, K., Takashima, A., Watanabe, T., Kawabe, H., Matsuda, T.,

Mishima, K., Chidori, S., Nishimura, R., Fujiwara, M., 2005. Altered depression-

related behavior and neurochemical changes in serotonergic neurons in mutant

R406W human tau transgenic mice. Brain research 1059, 7–12.

doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.08.004

Eslinger, P.J., Moore, P., Anderson, C., Grossman, M., 2011. Social cognition,

executive functioning, and neuroimaging correlates of empathic deficits in

frontotemporal dementia. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical

neurosciences 23, 74–82. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.23.1.74

Filiano, A.J., Martens, L.H., Young, A.H., Warmus, B.A., Zhou, P., Diaz-Ramirez,

G., Jiao, J., Zhang, Z., Huang, E.J., Gao, F.-B., Farese, R. V, Roberson, E.D.,

2013. Dissociation of frontotemporal dementia-related deficits and

neuroinflammation in progranulin haploinsufficient mice. The Journal of

neuroscience  : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33, 5352–61.

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6103-11.2013

Galimberti, D., Scarpini, E., 2010. Genetics and biology of Alzheimer’s disease and

frontotemporal lobar degeneration. International journal of clinical and

experimental medicine 3, 129–43.

Galsworthy, M.J., Paya-Cano, J.L., Liu, L., Monleón, S., Gregoryan, G., Fernandes,

C., Schalkwyk, L.C., Plomin, R., 2005. Assessing reliability, heritability and

general cognitive ability in a battery of cognitive tasks for laboratory mice.

Behavior genetics 35, 675–92. doi:10.1007/s10519-005-3423-9

Gibbons, Z.C., Richardson, A., Neary, D., Snowden, J.S., 2008. Behaviour in

amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis  : official publication

Page 18: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  18  

of the World Federation of Neurology Research Group on Motor Neuron

Diseases 9, 67–74. doi:10.1080/17482960701642437

Grochmal-Bach, B., Bidzan, L., Pachalska, M., Bidzan, M., Łukaszewska, B., Pufal,

A., 2009. Aggressive and impulsive behaviors in Frontotemporal dementia and

Alzheimer’s disease. Medical science monitor  : international medical journal of

experimental and clinical research 15, CR248–54.

Grunwald, T., Boutros, N.N., Pezer, N., von Oertzen, J., Fernández, G., Schaller, C.,

Elger, C.E., 2003. Neuronal substrates of sensory gating within the human brain.

Biological psychiatry 53, 511–9.

Harciarek, M., Cosentino, S., 2013. Language, executive function and social

cognition in the diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia syndromes. International

review of psychiatry (Abingdon, England) 25, 178–96.

doi:10.3109/09540261.2013.763340

Hodges, J.R., 2013. Alzheimer’s disease and the frontotemporal dementias:

contributions to clinico-pathological studies, diagnosis, and cognitive

neuroscience. Journal of Alzheimer’s disease  : JAD 33 Suppl 1, S211–7.

doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-129038

Hodges, J.R., Davies, R.R., Xuereb, J.H., Casey, B., Broe, M., Bak, T.H., Kril, J.J.,

Halliday, G.M., 2004. Clinicopathological correlates in frontotemporal dementia.

Annals of neurology 56, 399–406. doi:10.1002/ana.20203

Huey, E.D., Goveia, E.N., Paviol, S., Pardini, M., Krueger, F., Zamboni, G., Tierney,

M.C., Wassermann, E.M., Grafman, J., 2009. Executive dysfunction in

frontotemporal dementia and corticobasal syndrome. Neurology 72, 453–9.

doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000341781.39164.26

Hughes, R.N., 2004. The value of spontaneous alternation behavior (SAB) as a test of

retention in pharmacological investigations of memory. Neuroscience and

biobehavioral reviews 28, 497–505. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.006

Ittner, L.M., Halliday, G.M., Kril, J.J., Götz, J., Hodges, J.R., Kiernan, M.C., 2015.

FTD and ALS-translating mouse studies into clinical trials. Nature reviews

Neurology 11, 360–366. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2015.65

Ittner, L.M., Ke, Y.D., Delerue, F., Bi, M., Gladbach, A., van Eersel, J., Wölfing, H.,

Chieng, B.C., Christie, M.J., Napier, I.A., Eckert, A., Staufenbiel, M.,

Hardeman, E., Götz, J., 2010. Dendritic function of tau mediates amyloid-beta

Page 19: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  19  

toxicity in Alzheimer’s disease mouse models. Cell 142, 387–97.

doi:10.1016/j.cell.2010.06.036

Johns, E.K., Phillips, N.A., Belleville, S., Goupil, D., Babins, L., Kelner, N., Ska, B.,

Gilbert, B., Inglis, G., Panisset, M., de Boysson, C., Chertkow, H., 2009.

Executive functions in frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body dementia.

Neuropsychology 23, 765–77. doi:10.1037/a0016792

Ke, Y.D., Delerue, F., Gladbach, A., Götz, J., Ittner, L.M., 2009. Experimental

diabetes mellitus exacerbates tau pathology in a transgenic mouse model of

Alzheimer’s disease. PloS one 4, e7917. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007917

Knight, R.T., Staines, W.R., Swick, D., Chao, L.L., 1999. Prefrontal cortex regulates

inhibition and excitation in distributed neural networks. Acta psychologica 101,

159–78.

Knutson, K.M., Dal Monte, O., Schintu, S., Wassermann, E.M., Raymont, V.,

Grafman, J., Krueger, F., 2015. Areas of Brain Damage Underlying Increased

Reports of Behavioral Disinhibition. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical

neurosciences appineuropsych14060126.

doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.14060126

Koppel, J., Jimenez, H., Azose, M., D’Abramo, C., Acker, C., Buthorn, J.,

Greenwald, B.S., Lewis, J., Lesser, M., Liu, Z., Davies, P., 2014. Pathogenic tau

species drive a psychosis-like phenotype in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s

disease. Behavioural brain research 275, 27–33. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.030

Lalonde, R., 2002. The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation.

Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 26, 91–104.

Landqvist Waldö, M., Santillo, A.F., Gustafson, L., Englund, E., Passant, U., 2014.

Somatic complaints in frontotemporal dementia. American journal of

neurodegenerative disease 3, 84–92.

Lindau, M., Almkvist, O., Kushi, J., Boone, K., Johansson, S.E., Wahlund, L.O.,

Cummings, J.L., Miller, B.L., 2000. First symptoms--frontotemporal dementia

versus Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders 11, 286–

93. doi:17251

Manes, F.F., Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., Bekinschtein, T.A., Hodges,

J.R., 2010. Frontotemporal dementia presenting as pathological gambling.

Nature reviews Neurology 6, 347–52. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2010.34

Page 20: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  20  

Meeker, H.C., Chadman, K.K., Heaney, A.T., Carp, R.I., 2013. Assessment of social

interaction and anxiety-like behavior in senescence-accelerated-prone and -

resistant mice. Physiology & behavior 118, 97–102.

doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.05.003

Mendez, M.F., 2011. Pathological stealing in dementia: poor response to SSRI

medications. The Journal of clinical psychiatry 72, 418–9.

doi:10.4088/JCP.10l06440gry

Mendez, M.F., 2010. The unique predisposition to criminal violations in

frontotemporal dementia. The journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry

and the Law 38, 318–23.

Mendez, M.F., 2004. The accuracy of clinical criteria for the diagnosis of

frontotemporal dementia. International journal of psychiatry in medicine 34,

125–30.

Mendez, M.F., Perryman, K.M., 2002. Neuropsychiatric features of frontotemporal

dementia: evaluation of consensus criteria and review. The Journal of

neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences 14, 424–9.

Merrilees, J., Klapper, J., Murphy, J., Lomen-Hoerth, C., Miller, B.L., 2010.

Cognitive and behavioral challenges in caring for patients with frontotemporal

dementia and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis  :

official publication of the World Federation of Neurology Research Group on

Motor Neuron Diseases 11, 298–302. doi:10.3109/17482961003605788

Moy, S.S., Nadler, J.J., Perez, A., Barbaro, R.P., Johns, J.M., Magnuson, T.R., Piven,

J., Crawley, J.N., 2004. Sociability and preference for social novelty in five

inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes, brain,

and behavior 3, 287–302. doi:10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x

Naert, A., Gantois, I., Laeremans, A., Vreysen, S., Van den Bergh, G., Arckens, L.,

Callaerts-Vegh, Z., D’Hooge, R., 2013. Behavioural alterations relevant to

developmental brain disorders in mice with neonatally induced ventral

hippocampal lesions. Brain Research Bulletin 94, 71–81.

doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.01.008

Neary, D., Snowden, J., Mann, D., 2005. Frontotemporal dementia. The Lancet

Neurology 4, 771–80. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70223-4

Njung’e, K., Handley, S.L., 1991. Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model

of anxiety. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior 38, 63–7.

Page 21: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  21  

Passant, U., Elfgren, C., Englund, E., Gustafson, L.,. Psychiatric symptoms and their

psychosocial consequences in frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimer disease and

associated disorders 19 Suppl 1, S15–8.

Perriol, M.-P., Dujardin, K., Derambure, P., Marcq, A., Bourriez, J.-L., Laureau, E.,

Pasquier, F., Defebvre, L., Destée, A., 2005. Disturbance of sensory filtering in

dementia with Lewy bodies: comparison with Parkinson’s disease dementia and

Alzheimer's disease. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry 76,

106–8. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2003.035022

Peters, F., Perani, D., Herholz, K., Holthoff, V., Beuthien-Baumann, B., Sorbi, S.,

Pupi, A., Degueldre, C., Lemaire, C., Collette, F., Salmon, E., 2006.

Orbitofrontal dysfunction related to both apathy and disinhibition in

frontotemporal dementia. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders 21, 373–9.

doi:10.1159/000091898

Pompanin, S., Jelcic, N., Cecchin, D., Cagnin, A.,. Impulse control disorders in

frontotemporal dementia: spectrum of symptoms and response to treatment.

General hospital psychiatry 36, 760.e5–7.

doi:10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2014.06.005

Pressman, P.S., Miller, B.L., 2014. Diagnosis and management of behavioral variant

frontotemporal dementia. Biological psychiatry 75, 574–81.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.006

Rademakers, R., Neumann, M., Mackenzie, I.R., 2012. Advances in understanding

the molecular basis of frontotemporal dementia. Nature reviews Neurology 8,

423–34. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2012.117

Rafael, J.A., Nitta, Y., Peters, J., Davies, K.E., 2000. Testing of SHIRPA, a mouse

phenotypic assessment protocol, on Dmd(mdx) and Dmd(mdx3cv) dystrophin-

deficient mice. Mammalian genome  : official journal of the International

Mammalian Genome Society 11, 725–8.

Rankin, K.P., Kramer, J.H., Miller, B.L., 2005. Patterns of cognitive and emotional

empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cognitive and behavioral

neurology  : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive

Neurology 18, 28–36.

Reisel, D., Bannerman, D.M., Schmitt, W.B., Deacon, R.M.J., Flint, J., Borchardt, T.,

Seeburg, P.H., Rawlins, J.N.P., 2002. Spatial memory dissociations in mice

lacking GluR1. Nature neuroscience 5, 868–73. doi:10.1038/nn910

Page 22: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  22  

Rogers, D.C., Fisher, E.M., Brown, S.D., Peters, J., Hunter, A.J., Martin, J.E., 1997.

Behavioral and functional analysis of mouse phenotype: SHIRPA, a proposed

protocol for comprehensive phenotype assessment. Mammalian genome  : official

journal of the International Mammalian Genome Society 8, 711–3.

Savage, S.A., Lillo, P., Kumfor, F., Kiernan, M.C., Piguet, O., Hodges, J.R., 2014.

Emotion processing deficits distinguish pure amyotrophic lateral sclerosis from

frontotemporal dementia. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal

degeneration 15, 39–46. doi:10.3109/21678421.2013.809763

Seltman, R.E., Matthews, B.R., 2012. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration:

epidemiology, pathology, diagnosis and management. CNS drugs 26, 841–70.

doi:10.2165/11640070-000000000-00000

Shea, Y.F., Ha, J., Chu, L.-W., 2014. Comparisons of clinical symptoms in

biomarker-confirmed Alzheimer’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and

frontotemporal dementia patients in a local memory clinic. Psychogeriatrics  : the

official journal of the Japanese Psychogeriatric Society. doi:10.1111/psyg.12103

Sieben, A., Van Langenhove, T., Engelborghs, S., Martin, J.-J., Boon, P., Cras, P., De

Deyn, P.-P., Santens, P., Van Broeckhoven, C., Cruts, M., 2012. The genetics

and neuropathology of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Acta

neuropathologica 124, 353–72. doi:10.1007/s00401-012-1029-x

Snowden, J.S., Bathgate, D., Varma, A., Blackshaw, A., Gibbons, Z.C., Neary, D.,

2001. Distinct behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and semantic

dementia. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry 70, 323–32.

Snowden, J.S., Gibbons, Z.C., Blackshaw, A., Doubleday, E., Thompson, J.,

Craufurd, D., Foster, J., Happé, F., Neary, D., 2003. Social cognition in

frontotemporal dementia and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia 41, 688–

701.

Starkstein, S.E., Robinson, R.G., 1997. Mechanism of disinhibition after brain

lesions. The Journal of nervous and mental disease 185, 108–14.

Sterniczuk, R., Antle, M.C., Laferla, F.M., Dyck, R.H., 2010. Characterization of the

3xTg-AD mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease: part 2. Behavioral and cognitive

changes. Brain research 1348, 149–55. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.011

Stopford, C.L., Thompson, J.C., Neary, D., Richardson, A.M.T., Snowden, J.S., 2012.

Working memory, attention, and executive function in Alzheimer’s disease and

Page 23: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  23  

frontotemporal dementia. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous

system and behavior 48, 429–46. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.12.002

Takeuchi, H., Iba, M., Inoue, H., Higuchi, M., Takao, K., Tsukita, K., Karatsu, Y.,

Iwamoto, Y., Miyakawa, T., Suhara, T., Trojanowski, J.Q., Lee, V.M.-Y.,

Takahashi, R., 2011. P301S mutant human tau transgenic mice manifest early

symptoms of human tauopathies with dementia and altered sensorimotor gating.

PloS one 6, e21050. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021050

Tasan, R.O., Lin, S., Hetzenauer, a., Singewald, N., Herzog, H., Sperk, G., 2009.

Increased novelty-induced motor activity and reduced depression-like behavior

in neuropeptide Y (NPY)-Y4 receptor knockout mice. Neuroscience 158, 1717–

1730. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.11.048

Van der Jeugd, A., Blum, D., Raison, S., Eddarkaoui, S., Buée, L., D’Hooge, R.,

2013. Observations in THY-Tau22 mice that resemble behavioral and

psychological signs and symptoms of dementia. Behavioural Brain Research

242, 34–39. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.12.008

Van Der Jeugd, A., Vermaercke, B., Lo, A.C., Hamdane, M., Blum, D., Buée, L.,

D’Hooge, R., 2013. Progressive age-related cognitive decline in tau mice.

Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 37, 777–788. doi:10.3233/JAD-130110

Van Eersel, J., Stevens, C.H., Przybyla, M., Gladbach, A., Stefanoska, K., Chan,

C.K.-X., Ong, W.-Y., Hodges, J.R., Sutherland, G.T., Kril, J.J., Abramowski, D.,

Staufenbiel, M., Halliday, G.M., Ittner, L.M., 2015. Early-onset axonal

pathology in a novel P301S-Tau transgenic mouse model of frontotemporal lobar

degeneration. Neuropathology and applied neurobiology. doi:10.1111/nan.12233

Varty, G.B., Morgan, C.A., Cohen-Williams, M.E., Coffin, V.L., Carey, G.J., 2002.

The gerbil elevated plus-maze I: behavioral characterization and

pharmacological validation. Neuropsychopharmacology  : official publication of

the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 357–70.

doi:10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00312-3

Xia, D., Li, C., Götz, J., 2015. Pseudophosphorylation of Tau at distinct epitopes or

the presence of the P301L mutation targets the microtubule-associated protein

Tau to dendritic spines. Biochimica et biophysica acta 1852, 913–24.

doi:10.1016/j.bbadis.2014.12.017

Yin, F., Dumont, M., Banerjee, R., Ma, Y., Li, H., Lin, M.T., Beal, M.F., Nathan, C.,

Thomas, B., Ding, A., 2010. Behavioral deficits and progressive neuropathology

Page 24: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  24  

in progranulin-deficient mice: a mouse model of frontotemporal dementia.

FASEB journal  : official publication of the Federation of American Societies for

Experimental Biology 24, 4639–47. doi:10.1096/fj.10-161471

Page 25: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  25  

Figure legends

Figure 1. Tau58-2/B mice present with a widespread, progressive tauopathy. (A)

Drawing of a mouse sagittal section representing the affected brain regions and their

underlying functions. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; HC, hippocampus; OFC,

orbitofrontal cortex; PFC, prefrontal cortex; and OFC, orbitofrontal cortex. (B)

Representative sagittal section of the brain of a 12 month-old Tau58-2/B mouse

stained with HT7 for total tau. (B’) Consecutive section impregnated with

Bielschowsky silver staining to visualize NFTs. (C-E, C’-E’) Corresponding higher

magnification images derived from hippocampus (C, C'), prefrontal cortex (D, D'),

and cerebellum (E, E'). Scale bars: 100µm.

Figure 2. Tauopathy mice show no alterations in explorative activity or anxiety.

(A) Total distance moved over a 10 min session in the open field test reveals no

differences in anxiety between wild-type (Wt) and transgenic (Tg) mice. (B) Tau Tg

mice spend the same amount of time as the Wt littermates in the center for all three

age groups assessed. (C) Heat maps reveal no thigmotactic or altered anxiety-like

behavior in Tg compared to Wt mice at any age tested. Representative traveled paths

per genotype are shown.

Figure 3. From 6 months onwards, tauopathy mice display deficits in social

exploration. (A) In the social exploration test, tau Tg mice are indistinguishable from

Wt mice at all age comparisons, when comparing total distance traveled. (B) From 6

months onwards, Tg mice spend less time in the annulus around the cage containing

the stranger mouse compared to the age-matched Wt mice, indicative of impaired

social interaction. (C) Heat maps confirm the decrease in social exploration in Tg

mice. Representative traveled paths per genotype are shown. Brackets indicating

differences between genotypes (*), and gender (#); p* < 0.05; p** < 0.01; p*** <

0.001.

Figure 4. Tauopathy mice show increased impulsivity and risk-taking behaviors.

(A) Tau Tg mice spend more time in the light compartment as compared to Wt mice.

(B) As shown by the decreased latency in the light/dark box, dark-habituated tau Tg

mice emerge faster from the dark into the light than their age-matched Wt counter-

Page 26: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  26  

parts. (C) From 6 months of age onwards, Tau58-2/B mice show an increased risk-

taking behaviors compared to Wt mice. Brackets indicating differences between

genotypes (*), and gender (#); p* < 0.05; p** < 0.01; p*** < 0.001.

Figure 5. Decreased working memory and adaptive problem solving capacities in

aged tau Tg mice. (A) From 6 months onwards, tau Tg mice make fewer alternations

in the Y-maze. (B) Heat maps confirm the decrease in alteration in 10-11 months-old

Tg mice. Representative traveled paths during the first minute for each genotype are

shown. (C) From 10 months onwards, Tg mice take longer to solve the digging

problem than the age-matched Wt mice as shown by an increased latency to enter the

goal box that contains the food reward. (D) The executive function deficit in aged tau

Tg mice is verified in the second puzzle on day 2 when tau Tg mice fail to solve the

plug test as compared to age-matched Wt mice. Brackets indicating differences

between genotypes (*), and gender (#); p* < 0.05; p** < 0.01; p*** < 0.001.

Page 27: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  27  

Tables

Table 1. Progressive tau hyperphosphorylation and NFT formation in Tau58-2/B

mice.

Table 2. Differences in general appearance, arousal, and neuromotor per-

formance in Tau58-2/B mice.

Page 28: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

Cerebellum

ACC

Brainstem

A

B

C

D

C’

D’

E’E

Figure 1

B’

Page 29: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

A

C

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

Dis

tanc

e tra

vele

d (c

m)

Wt femaleWt male

Tg femaleTg male

2-3 months 6-7months 10-11 months

Wt

Tg

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

50

100

150

200

250

Tim

e sp

ent i

n ce

nter

(s)

B

Figure 2

Page 30: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

A B

C

2-3 months 6-7months 10-11 months

Wt

Tg

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

Dis

tanc

e tra

vele

d (c

m)

Wt femaleWt male

Tg femaleTg male

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

100

200

300

Tim

e sp

ent i

n ce

nter

(s)

*** ** *

Figure 3

Page 31: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

A B

C

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

100

200

300

400

500

Tim

e sp

ent i

n lig

ht (s

) Wt femaleWt male

Tg femaleTg male

* ** **** ** ***

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

20

40

60

80

Late

ncy

to li

ght (

s)

#

** ** * *

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

20

40

60

80

Stre

tche

d po

stur

es (#

)

Wt maleWt female

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

20

40

60

80

Str

etch

ed p

ost

ure

s (#

)

***TgWt

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

20

40

60

80

Str

etch

ed p

ost

ure

s (#

)

***TgWt

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

20

40

60

80

Str

etch

ed p

ost

ure

s (#

)

***TgWt

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

20

40

60

80

Str

etch

ed p

ost

ure

s (#

)

***TgWt

** ** *** ***

Tg femaleTg male

Figure 4

Page 32: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

A B

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

20

40

60

80

100

Alte

rnat

ions

(%) Wt female

Tg female

Wt maleTg male# #

#

#

** ** *** ***

C

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

50

100

150

200

Late

ncy

to s

olve

dig

(s)

*** ***

2-3 months 6-7 months 10-11 months0

100

200

300

Late

ncy

to s

olve

plu

g (s

)

** ** *** ***

10-11 months

Wt Tg

D

Figure 5

Page 33: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  1  

Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive

dysfunction in a mouse model of frontotemporal dementia

Ann van der Jeugd1,2, Ben Vermaercke2, Glenda M. Halliday3, Matthias Staufenbiel4,

and Jürgen Götz1,*

Supplementary Data

Supplementary Results

Gender effects in SHIRPA murine neurobehavioral screen and motor abilities

tests. At any age tested, male mice, regardless of their genotype, weighed more than

female mice (gender main effect at 2-3 months, F1,19=36.42, p < 0.001; 6-7 months:

F1,19=34.33, p < 0.001; 10-11 months: F1,19=31.52, p < 0.001; gender x genotype

interaction effects not significant). Moreover, 10-11 month-old Tg male mice had

more bald patches and wounds than female mice and their Wt counterparts (missing

fur: gender main effect: F1,19=20.83, p < 0.001; gender x genotype effect: F1,19=9.75,

p = 0.007; wounds: gender main effect: F1,19=9.83, p = 0.007; gender x genotype

interaction effect: F1,19=9.83, p = 0.007). Also, male Tg mice of 6-7 months of age

displayed more tremors than their female and Wt counterparts (gender main effect:

F1,19=7.33, p = 0.02; gender x genotype interaction effect: F1,19=7.33, p = 0.02).

Finally, we found higher values in the grip strength test for males than for females

(grip mean: gender main effect at 2-3 months: F1,19=4.83, p = 0.048; gender x

genotype interaction effects not significant - grip max: gender main effect at 6-7

months: F1,19=6.12, p = 0.031; gender x genotype interaction effects not significant).

Page 34: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  2  

Supplementary Figure 1

Behavioral setups. (A) Open field test, (B) social exploration test, (C) light/dark box,

(D) Y-maze, (E) puzzle box dig trial, and (F) puzzle box plug trial.

Page 35: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  3  

Supplementary Table 1

Tasks used to compare Tau58-2/B mice to wild-type littermate controls

Page 36: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  4  

Supplementary Table 2

SHIRPA Murine neurobehavioral screen (based on (Rogers et al., 1997))

Physical factors and gross appearance

• Body weight (g)

• Presence of whiskers o 0 = None o 1 = A few o 2 = Most, but not a full set o 3 = A full set

• Appearance of fur (not counting patches of missing fur)

o 0 = Ungroomed and dishevelled o 1 = Somewhat dishevelled o 2 = Well-groomed (normal)

• Patches of missing fur on body

o 0 = None o 1 = Some o 2 = Extensive

• Wounds

o 0 = None o 1 = Signs of previous wounding o 2 = Slight wounds present o 3 = Moderate wounds present o 4 = Extensive wounds present

Observation of behavior in a novel environment (within a 3 min trial in a tub cage)

• Transfer behavior o 0 = Coma o 1 = Prolonged freeze (>10 sec.), then slight movement o 2 = Extended freeze, then moderate movement o 3 = Brief freeze (a few seconds), then active movement o 4 = Momentary freeze, then swift movement o 5 = No freeze, immediate movement o 6 = Extremely excited

• Spontaneous activity

o 0 = None, resting o 1 = Casual scratch, groom, slow movement o 2 = Vigorous scratch, groom, moderate movement o 3 = Vigorous, rapid/dart movement o 4 = Extremely vigorous, rapid/dart movement

Page 37: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  5  

• Tremor

o 0 = None o 1 = Mild o 2 = Marked

• Palpebral closure

o 0 = Eyes wide open o 1 = Eyes ½ closed o 2 = Eyes closed

• Gait

o 0 = Normal o 1 = Fluid but abnormal o 2 = Limited movement only o 3 = Incapacity

Reactions to simple stimuli and reflexes

• Touch escape (finger stroke from above, starting light and getting firmer) o 0 = No response o 1 = Mild (escape response to firm stroke) o 2 = Moderate (rapid response to light stroke) o 3 = Vigorous (escape response to approach)

• Trunk curl (grip tail and lift about 30 cm)

o 0 = Absent o 1 = Present

• Reaching reflex (forelimbs extension when lowered by tail from 15cm height)

o 0 = None o 1 = Upon nose contact o 2 = Upon vibrasse contact o 3 = Before vibrasse contact o 4 = Early vigorous extension

• Preyer reflex (loud handclap 30 cm above mouse, watch for pinna reflex)

o 0 = None o 1 = Active retraction, moderate brisk head flick o 2 = Hyperactive, repetitive flick

• Toe pinch (apply gentle lateral compression on hind foot of a tail lifted mouse)

o 0 = None o 1 = Slight withdrawal o 2 = Moderate withdrawal, not brisk o 3 = Brisk, rapid withdrawal o 4 = Very brisk repeated extension and flexion

Grip strength and motor coordination

Page 38: Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive ... › view › UQ:382128 › UQ382128_OA.pdf · ! 1! Impulsivity, decreased social exploration, and executive dysfunction

  6  

Grip strength was measured using a T-shaped bar connected to a digital dynamometer (Ugo Basile, Comerio, Italy). Mice were placed in such a way that they grabbed the bar spontaneously and were softly pulled backwards by the tail until they released their grip. Ten such readouts were recorded; mean and max pulling force were noted for each mouse. The wire hang test seeks to evaluate motor function. The animal is placed on a wire cage top, which is then inverted and suspended above the home cage for 30 seconds. Grip is scored as following:

o 0 = Active grip o 1 = Difficulty to grasp, but hangs on o 2 = Grasps, but falls of within 10 seconds o 3 = Unable to grasp, falls within seconds o 3 = Falls immediately

Motor coordination and equilibrium were tested using an accelerating Rotarod (Ugo Basile, Comerio, Italy). Mice were tested on four trials, during which the rod accelerated from 4 to 20 rpm in 5 minutes. Consecutive trials were separated by a 2-minute intertrial interval. Latency to falling off the rod was recorded up to 5 minutes; mean and max walking latencies were noted for each mouse.