jarrett mehldau

51
1 Motivic Strategies in Improvisations by Keith Jarrett and Brad Mehldau Timothy Page Kirjallinen Työ 1ss10 Kevät 2009 Sibelius-Akatemia

Upload: joep42000

Post on 26-Oct-2014

361 views

Category:

Documents


37 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Jarrett Mehldau

1

Motivic Strategies in Improvisations by

Keith Jarrett and Brad Mehldau

Timothy Page

Kirjallinen Työ 1ss10 Kevät 2009

Sibelius-Akatemia

Page 2: Jarrett Mehldau

2

Contents

I. Introduction

II. Biographical Sketches

III. Method

A. Choice of Source Material

B. Transcription

C. Motivic Analysis: Assumptions and Approaches

D. Motivic Chain Associations

E. Schenkerian perspectives on Motive

IV. Analysis

A. MCA Analyses

B. Motivic Transformation

C. “Motivic Glue”

D. Motivic Parallelisms, Higher-Level Motives

E. Linear Aspects of Motivic Organization

V. Conclusion

Sources

Discography

Page 3: Jarrett Mehldau

3

I. Introduction

As with any art form, "style" in jazz improvisation is the sum total of a countless

number of factors. Alongside time feel, melodic, rhythmic, and harmonic vocabulary, and

preferences regarding improvisational forms, one important category in determining style has

to do with approaches to use of motive in improvised melodies. Keith Jarrett (1945 --) and

Brad Mehldau (1970 --) are two jazz pianists whose playing is highly motivic. They are also

both frequently mentioned in the same sentence: Mehldau has for example been touted as the

“heir apparent to such impressionist masters as Keith Jarrett and Bill Evans” (Collins 2006)

and said to be “reminiscent of Jarrett,” (JazzTimes 1997), while Jarrett has been called

Mehldau’s “obvious forebear,” (CD Universe 2008). It has even been rumored that Jarrett at

one point threatened a lawsuit against the younger pianist for “stealing (his) piano trio

concept.”1 Recordings of the two pianists do in fact reveal clear points of comparison:

Perhaps most immediately perceptible is a shared flexible rhythmic approach, and a

comparable sound color in their pianisim.

In this investigation I will nonetheless take some issue with claims of similarity

between the two players by drawing distinctions between their respective motivic strategies,

or ways in which they use motives to create a sense of coherence in their improvisations.

This will be done via examination and comparison of motivic aspects of Jarrett's and

Mehldau's melodic improvisational technique as exhibited in the right-hand melodic

improvisations from Jarrett's performance of "Just in Time," from his Standards in Norway

album, and Mehldau's performance of "I Didn't Know What Time It Was," from his album

Art of the Trio, Volume I.

I will first map out surface motivic connections in each solo using the categories of

“motivic chain association” proposed by Ekkehard Jost (1970). The motivic chain association

analyses will be used to draw some generalizations about each player’s approach to use of

surface motives in constructing phrases. It will be argued that Jarrett shows a higher tendency

than Mehldau to treat his motivic material transformatively, recycling and accumulatively

manipulating small motivic cells over the course of his phrases, and creating poignant sense

of forward developmental motion. Mehldau, it will be argued, in turn displays a greater

1 From a conversation with Jari Perkiömäki (2008)

Page 4: Jarrett Mehldau

4

propensity to use motivic elements as relatively static points of reference, separated by spans

of music of variable length, that thematically bind his solo together.

Employing a more Schenkerian perspective, I will then discuss some examples of

motivic parallelisms and higher-level motives -- those operating underneath the surface of the

music. Both players will be shown to employ motivic elements, often derived from the

original compositions over which they improvise, on more than one structural level. Finally,

I will give a preliminary comparison of each player's approach to organizing surface motivic

material by way of linear voice-leading events implicit in the harmonic forms over which

they improvise. It will be proposed that Jarrett’s improvised melodies show a higher tendency

than Mehldau’s to be directed by linear voice-leading events inherent in underlying harmony.

II. Biographical Sketches

Keith Jarrett was born on May 8, 1945 in Allentown Pennsylvania2. His musical proclivities

were noticed very early on by his parents, and he began taking piano lessons by the age of

three. By the age of seven he was already giving classical recitals, in which he sometimes

included his own compositions. Significantly, his interest in composing was such that he

arranged some years later to study with Nadia Boulanger -- an opportunity that he ultimately

backed out on. He began to get into jazz at the age of 15, spurred on by experiences listening

to players such as Dave Brubeck and Bill Evans. Two years later he received a scholarship to

study at the Berklee College of music in Boston, which he attended for one year before being

kicked out due to his unwillingness to follow the curriculum. After a period of low profile

club performances in Boston, he moved to New York in 1964. Shortly thereafter, he had his

professional breakthrough when drummer Art Blakey happened to hear him play at a jam

session at the Village Vanguard at a time when Blakey was shuffling personnel in his Jazz

Messengers. Jarrett's tenure with Blakey lasted only four months, but their one recording

together, Buttercorn Lady, shows that he was entirely fluent and competent in the hard bop

idiom.

Jarrett's next regular gig was with saxophonist Charles Lloyd's quartet, an

engagement that lasted nearly through the rest of the 60s. The quartet's sound was rooted in

2 Biographical details in the following three paragraphs are taken from Carr, 1991.

Page 5: Jarrett Mehldau

5

the modal and avant-garde jazz pioneered by John Coltrane during roughly the same time,

and already a big stylistic departure from the more straight-ahead bebop flavor of Blakey's.

After Lloyd, from 1970 to 71, Jarrett played and recorded with Miles Davis during the height

of Davis's so-called fusion period. Then in 1973 he began to give improvised solo concerts.

Jarrett solo improvisations constitute a genre of their own. While drawing on perhaps all of

Jarrett's stylistic influences to date, from gospel to classical to avant-garde jazz, they very

often feature repeating ostinato figures and tend to be highly diatonic, sometimes showing

kinship to the music of minimalist composers such as Philip Glass, Terry Riley, and Steve

Reich. Other notable activity during the 70s included Jarrett's work with Norwegian

saxophonist Jan Garbarek, who pioneered what is often classified today as "Scandinavian

jazz." This music is often characterized by textures that are rhythmically free, and

harmonically modal. At the end of the 70s, Jarrett also began performing classical music

publicly

It was in 1983 that Jarrett made the surprising decision to form a trio devoted to

performing the so-called "standard" jazz repertoire, the popular American songs from movies

and musicals of the 20s, 30s and 40s as well as some of the compositions of bebop players

from the late 40s and 50s. These are the compositions that comprise the harmonic structures

most commonly used by jazz players for their improvisations, from the inception of the

bebop style in the mid-40s through the early 60s. Since 1983, Jarrett has almost exclusively

devoted his involvement in jazz to this so-called "Standards" trio with drummer Jack

DeJohnette and bassist Gary Peacock. It is from this trio's recording of “Just in Time” off of

the Standards in Norway record of 1989 that I have transcribed the Jarrett solo under

consideration.

Brad Mehldau was born in Jacksonville, Florida on August 23, 1970, and spent much

of his childhood in West Hartford, Connecticut. He began playing at the age of four, studied

classical piano from age 6 to his early teenage years, and began to study jazz at the age of 11

after being impressed by an Oscar Peterson recording (Jung, 2008). Another early influence

shortly thereafter was apparently Keith Jarrett's Bremen and Lausanne solo concert

recordings (Jung, 2008). Mehldau's professional jazz studies began in earnest after high

school in 1988 at the New School for Social Research in New York, where among his

teachers were Junior Manse, Kenny Warner, Fred Hersch, and the legendary bebop drummer

Jimmy Cobb. One of Mehldau's first professional engagements was with Cobb's band in

Page 6: Jarrett Mehldau

6

1991, where he apparently displayed his mastery of the hard bop style associated with pianist

Wynton Kelly (Ouellette, 2007). His big publicity breakthrough came in 1994, when he

recorded with saxophonist Joshua Redman. In 1995 he made his recording debut as a

bandleader for a trio, with bassist Larry Grenadier and drummer Jorge Rossi. This album

contained mostly jazz standards, and a couple of Mehldau's original compositions. Already

on his first album, there are aspects of his sound that are audible in all his subsequent

recordings: a classical like touch, a flexible, "laid back" time feel, a predilection for odd

meters in his interpretation of standards, and a harmonic and melodic vocabulary that is an

extension of the bebop idiom. Since the 1995 debut, most of Mehldau's recording output has

centered around his trio, which maintained the same lineup until 2005, when Jeff Ballard

replaced Jorge Rossi on drums.

III. Method

A. Choice of Source Material

Given both Jarrett's and Mehldau's highly diverse backgrounds and stylistic interests,

it is fair to ask how any one performance of either player could be considered sufficiently

representative of his motivic improvisational technique to warrant generalizations.

Furthermore, supposing a sufficiently representative solo to be found for each player, on

what bases can two solos be compared to one another?

The short answer to the first question is that no one solo can be considered

representative of either player's entire improvisational oeuvre. I have purposely limited my

inquiry to Jarrett's bebop performances with his Standards Trio from 1983 onward, and

Mehldau's performances of pieces from the standard repertoire with his own trio from 1995

onward. On the basis of repeated listening to nearly every recording of Jarrett's trio since

1983 (and having done numerous transcriptions of his solos), I am confident in stating that

Jarrett's approach to bebop style improvisation has remained largely consistent over the two

and a half decades since the trio's formation. In the case of Mehldau, the situation is a bit

different. For one, he was only 25 at the time his trio began to record in 1995. In the

recordings he has made with his trio over the last 12 years, it is easy to hear continual

branching and development in the relatively young player's stylistic preferences and

Page 7: Jarrett Mehldau

7

harmonic vocabulary. However, while his repertoire has become more and more diverse and

dominated by his own compositions (as well as arrangements of contemporary and classic

rock tunes), Mehldau still periodically records tunes from the standard repertoire. While there

are aspects of his playing that certainly remain consistent across his entire recorded oeuvre,3 I

observe consistency especially in his interpretation of standards. This consistency lies in,

among others, phrasing, melodic and harmonic vocabulary, and -- most significantly for this

inquiry -- motivic strategies.

From the limited set of solos belonging to performances of jazz standards, I ultimately

sought solos that were not only representative, but also exemplary -- those that especially left

an impression of coherence, and even prior to close analysis sounded especially motivic. On

the basis of countless auditions and cursory analyses, Jarrett's solo on “Just in Time” from the

1989 Standards in Norway recording appeared to offer one of the most compelling recorded

examples of his improvising. A similar process led me to Mehldau's solo on “I Didn't Know

What Time It Was” off his Art of the Trio, Volume 1 from 1997.

In previous scholarly work concerned with comparison of improvisational technique

between two players, it has been customary to use a recording of the same tune by each

player (e.g. DeVieux 1997, 79). In the case of Jarrett and Mehldau, this would have been

possible: both players have recorded, for example, “All the Things You Are,” “Solar,” and “It

Might As Well Be Spring.” However, I decided to sacrifice the convenience of comparing

improvisation over identical harmonic and formal structures for the sake of presenting these

players "at their best" with regards to audible "motivicness" and coherence. In Mehldau and

Jarrett's performances of the tunes just mentioned, I determined that either pianist or both

failed to offer a particularly exemplary solo.

The lead sheets for “Just in Time” and “I Didn’t Know What Time it Was” are

reprinted below from the Real Book:

3 Jazz pianist Robert Glasper states, "give me a quote blindfold test any day, and I’ll pick out Brad in a few hats.” (Oulette 2007)

Page 8: Jarrett Mehldau

8

Ex 1: Irving Berlin, “Just in Time” from The Real Book II, in which it is reprinted without

permission.

Page 9: Jarrett Mehldau

9

Ex 2: Lorenz Hart and Richard Rogers, “I Didn’t Know What Time it Was,” from The Real

Book III, in which it is reprinted without permission.

Page 10: Jarrett Mehldau

10

In the performances under consideration, both players more or less adhere to the versions

above, with some small changes: Mehldau has transposed “I Didn’t Know What Time it

Was” into D-minor, substituted a G7 for the B half-dim.7 in the second measure of the A

sections, substituted an F-minor phrygian chord for the final harmony, and inserted a

progression between I and VI in the 6th bar of every A section, C# maj 7 - F# maj 7 - B maj

7, which is despensed with on the solo choruses. He has also adjusted the meter by adding an

extra beat between beats two and three, creating a 5/4 meter consistently divided into 3+2.

Jarrett’s performance of “Just in Time” deviates from the above version in its lack of

preparation of the Bb major chord in m.20 of the form, and substituting the chords in mm.23-

24 with the progression Eb-min7 - Ab7 - D-min7 - G7.

These small adjustments by no means change the fact that there are considerable

similarities in the types of harmonic progressions employed in both compositions; II-V-I

progressions are ubiquitous, and both compositions contain extended passages based on

cycles of descending fifths. Thus, both compositions offer a similar -- though not identical --

harmonic environment for organization of motivic material.

Rhythmically, however, there are some significant differences: “I Didn’t Know What

Time it Was” maintains the general pattern of 2 chords per bar in Mehldau’s 5/4 setting. “Just

in Time” has a slower harmonic rhythm, generally with one chord per bar. This discrepancy

is slightly mitigated by the extra beat per measure in the Mehldau.

One important factor in the feasibility of an improviser to truly "compose in the

moment," and not rely mostly on well-rehearsed melodic formulas and patterns is tempo.

While ability certainly varies from player to player, many players4 have attested that they are

able to construct thought-out phrases only up to a certain tempo, after which their fingers

"take over" and fall into well-known patterns. Jarrett's interpretation of “Just In Time”

begins at a tempo of quarter note = 230 and slows to about 210 over the course of his solo,

giving an average tempo of around quarter note = 220. Mehldau's performance of “I Didn't

Know What Time It Was” stays steady at about a quarter note = 190. The tempo discrepancy

between the two performances is appreciable, even enough so to be reflected in a difference

between the two players' rhythmic articulation of eighth notes; Jarrett's eighth notes are

noticeably straighter than Mehldau's. Tempo, then, is another variable not perfectly

4 such as my own teachers Andy Jaffe, Mike Kokour, and Samuli Mikkonen.

Page 11: Jarrett Mehldau

11

controlled for in the ensuing comparative analysis of each player's use of motive. But the

tempos are close enough to place both performances in the category of "medium-up-tempo

swing," with both solos clearly dominated by lines subdividable into eighth notes. And the

performances are slow enough so that motivic development is feasible in general -- as each

player empirically demonstrates.

B: Transcription

The current investigation is concerned with motivic aspects of the improvised

melodic lines in the two performances in question. Accordingly, I have based my analysis on

transcriptions that are limited to the right-hand melodies improvised by each player following

their statements of the original melody. References will be made to the harmonies implied by

left-hand chords only in so far as they have bearing on the melodic-motivic features being

dealt with. Jarrett’s entire solo on “Just in Time” lasts eight choruses, in contrast to the two

choruses of Mehldau’s. In order to keep the amount of source material from each player on

comparable footing, I have limited my analysis and transcription to the first four choruses of

Jarrett’s solo, along with the two complete choruses of Mehldau’s.

As Scott DeVeaux has noted, "transcription is interpretation," (DeVeaux 1991, 32)

and the two transcriptions presented here are no exceptions; while traditional notation is

perfectly sufficient to convey information about pitch when the instrument in question is

piano, it has its limitations in conveying nuances in rhythm and articulation. In both

transcriptions I have followed the standard practice of using conventional eighth notes to

notate so-called “jazz eighth notes” – which at medium to fast tempos are generally

performed as something between and . This is less of an approximation in the

case of Jarrett than in that of Mehldau, whose solo is slow enough to allow for eight notes

that are a bit more swung (i.e. sound closer to ). Since eighth notes are treated as

equivalent between the two solos, this particular difference in rhythmic nuance between the

two players is taken to have no bearing on motivic features under consideration.

In the Mehldau transcription, additional rhythmic approximations are necessary, since

Mehldau sometimes shows a tendency to play "around" the pulse, and cram in odd numbers

of notes a melodic runs before landing on the beat. I have approximated these effects by

Page 12: Jarrett Mehldau

12

using tuplets that closely mimic what is heard in the performance. The tuplets used are not

intended to convey motivic content in and of themselves.

As for dynamic articulation, the principal variable taken into account in the

transcriptions is accent. As discussed below, accents can have bearing on whether or not

pitches can be considered to be structural -- which in turn can have motivic implications.

There is of course a continuous dynamic spectrum between an unaccented pitch and a highly

accented pitch. I have tried to notate accents whenever they are especially clear and seem to

have structural significance.

C: Motivic Analysis: Assumptions and Approaches

In his article “'Der Tod das ist ein Kühle Nacht' Op.96/1. Motiivianalyysin

Ongelmia.” (Problems Of Motivic Analysis), Risto Väisänen offers a lively survey of some

of the pitfalls encountered in the discipline of motivic analysis as it has been historically

applied to European art music (see Vaisanen 1998). Among the most pernicious has been a

fixation, especially prevalent in various early to late-mid 20th-century analytical circles, with

demonstrating the "unity" of works to be analyzed. "Unity" in such cases generally refers to

a work's "organic growth" from a small number of "seeds" or motives, and is taken as an

assumed property of the great masterworks (see e.g. Reti 1952, Rufer 1952, Schönberg

1975). Analysis here often consists precisely in demonstrating how entire works in fact grow

out of a single motive. This problematically circular approach is not unheard of in analysis of

jazz improvisation. A particularly notorious example is Gunther Schuller's 1958 analysis of a

Sonny Rollins solo on the tune "Blue Seven," in which he shows how the entire solo

"organically grows" out of a two-note motive stated at the solo's beginning (Schuller 1958).

As Väisänen has argued, the inherent circularity in these sorts of analyses has

practical consequences: the analyst operating in this vein is potentially guided far more by the

goal of demonstrating relations to the motivic seed or seeds posited than by salient features of

the music. And the rules and principles underlying motivic analysis have long been flexible

and indeterminate enough so that its practitioners are able to relate nearly any span of music

back to a given motivic seed (Väisänen 1998, 32-34, 61)

If "unity," and "organicism" were prevailing buzzwords in early to late-mid 20th-

century analysis of European art music, a comparably ubiquitous term in jazz analysis might

Page 13: Jarrett Mehldau

13

be "coherence." On up to the present, jazz analyses abound in which the author seeks to

demonstrate in one way or another the coherence of the music in question. (See e.g. Cogswell

1995, Jost 1970, Korman 1999, Larson 1998, Meehan 2002, Schuller 1958). The prevalence

of such studies might be understood in part as stemming from a defensive stance against

jazz's detractors -- those who have argued that jazz is "incoherent" in comparison to

European art music -- carried over from a time in which jazz and its analysis were still

gaining academic acceptance5. But whatever the motivation behind these coherence studies,

the notion of coherence itself is far less problematic than that of unity. This is especially true

if coherence is understood foremost as an experience produced in the listener/analyst by the

music under investigation, and not some assumed, inherent, and hidden property of the music

to be brought to light by the analyst -- as was often the case with "unity." Coherence, then, is

not something to be proven or demonstrated, but something to be explained: what are the

technical features of the music that might help one experience it as being coherent?

My aim in the present analyses is to show how motivic features in the Mehldau and

Jarrett solos under consideration may have a role in creating a sense of coherence, and

compare the ways in which they do so for each player.

D. Motivic Chain Associations

"After you initiate the solo, one phrase determines what the next is going to be. From the

first note that you hear, you are responding to what you've just played: you just said this on

your instrument, and now that's a constant. What follows from that? And then the next

phrase is a constant. What follows from that? And so on and so forth." (Max Roach in

Berliner 1991, 193)

Drummer Max Roach's comment underscores a common feature of jazz improvisation: in the

course of developing some "idea" or musical element, an improviser might come upon a new

musical element which is subsequently developed. This may in turn lead to a new idea, and

5 Putting jazz improvisation on par with classical music as an object of analysis was almost certainly one of Schuller's motivations behind the article alluded to above. Schuller was a longtime advocate of African-American jazz's acceptance in the white, classical concert world.

Page 14: Jarrett Mehldau

14

so on, so that material is thematically "chained" together over the course of phrases, choruses,

or even entire solos. In appreciation of this phenomenon and its implications for motivic

developmnet, analyst Ekkehard Jost coined the term "motivic chain association" (abbreviated

MCA), a broad concept that in practice seems to encompass nearly any kind of audible

motivic relatedness between elements of a melodic line (see Jost 1970). In his analyses of

free jazz saxophonist Ornette Coleman's solos, Jost categorizes the types of motivic chain

associations as follows (Cogswell 1995. Examples of each category are taken from the

present analyses of Jarrett’s solo on “Just in Time” and Mehldau’s solo on “I Didn’t Know

What Time it Was.”)

1. MCA - rhythm: the repetition of a single rhythmic pattern or the similarity of two rhythmic

patterns

Ex 3:

(Mehldau, mm.5-7, 9-11)

2. MCA - pitch: the repetition of a pitch arrangement or the similarity of two pitch

arrangements:

Ex 4:

(Jarrett, mm. 7,11)

Page 15: Jarrett Mehldau

15

3. MCA - contour: the resemblance in contour between two melodic motives

Ex 5:

(Mehldau mm. 24-5, 29-30)

4. MCA - variation:

A. Initial variation: the creation of two phrases that begin with the same motive

Ex 6:

(Jarrett, mm. 19-21)

B. Terminal variation: the creation of two phrases that end with the same motive

Ex 7:

(Jarrett, mm. 103-106)

Page 16: Jarrett Mehldau

16

5. MCA - Dovetailing: the creation of two phrases for which the ending of the first phrase

and the beginning of the second phrase contain an identical motive

Ex 8:

(Jarrett, mm. 33-37)

6. MCA - Repetition: restatement of a melodic motive in essentially unaltered form

Ex 9:

(Mehldau mm. 5-6)

7. MCA - Step progression: the creation of an embedded melodic line, moving in step, within

a larger melodic line.

Ex 10:

(Mehldau mm.1-8)

These different types of MCA are by no means mutually exclusive; simultaneous

instances of MCA pitch, rhythm, and contour, for example, are of course possible and quite

common.

Page 17: Jarrett Mehldau

17

These categories have been taken up by Michael Cogswell and Norman Meehan in

melodic analyses of improvisations by Coleman as well as free jazz pianist Paul Bley (see

Cogswell 1995, Meehan 2002). Analysis for all three aforementioned analysts largely

consists of identifying the types of motivic chain associations as they occur in the

improvisations in question.

Jost’s system raises some questions, both semantic and substantive: As MCA labels

can be applied to even just two, isolated melodic fragments, the "chain" in the term "motivic

chain association" would often seem superfluous. Moreover, it is not entirely obvious that all

these categories belong in the same list; Depending on one's understanding of the term

"motive" -- an issue glazed over by all three analysts -- the "stepwise progression" category,

for example, might seem out of place. Is a stepwise progression an actual motivic association

between parts of a melody, or is it more a non-motivic structural principle of that melody's

overall organization? More importantly, the exact criteria for identifying "similarity" of a

kind implying motivic chain association are a bit fuzzy. How close, for example, do two pitch

arrangements need to be in intervallic content in order to constitute MCA-pitch? And

whether or it not it portends to be, this is certainly not an exhaustive list of the ways that one

part of a melody can be associated with another part of a melody.

In spite of these shortcomings, the MCA perspective is a fruitful starting point in

mapping out motivic relations between parts of a melody, and one that will be employed in

my own analysis. The aforementioned analysts' relatively haphazard use of the term "motive"

may ultimately be to their credit: implicit in their analyses is an understanding of the concept

that is flexible enough to cover a very wide range of ways in which two or more bits of

melody might be associated with each other and aid in creating a sense of coherence over a

particular span of music. "Motive" is defined phenomenologically in the context of the

surrounding music; an element of melody, whether it be rhythm, pitch, contour, etc., becomes

motivic as soon as it appears to have an appreciable role in generating subsequent material.

This flexible, contextual definition of motive will be invoked in my analyses as well.

As for the issue of similarity criteria, all three analysts implicitly accept a degree of

subjectivity, relying foremost on audibility in deciding what actually counts as an instance of

MCA. But as long as subjective aural impressions can be corroborated by objective features

Page 18: Jarrett Mehldau

18

of the music, this approach should be deemed acceptable6. In my own analyses, audibility is

also one of the primary criteria for identifying and/or verifying instances of MCA. In cases in

which a similarity is deemed significant but somewhat tenuous, I have put the relevant

parameter in parenthesis (i.e. MCA – (p), r = clear rhythmic similarity, less clear pitch

similarity)

In addition to similarity criteria, MCA analysis also requires some principles for

determining when an clear association between two items is not worth notating: Should every

group of two notes in a descending, eighth-note scalar run, for example be marked as MCA-

Repetition? Again, it is context that determines the significance of a motivic association. In

my own analyses, scalar runs are generally treated as “filler” material that may connect items

of higher motivic significance. A single stepwise progression, however, can be considered

motivic in the proper context.

Even with such criteria and principles established, one important area in which Jost's

system needs much supplementation is that of motivic transformation -- the gradual altering

of motivic material over successive statements of that material. While MCA labels can show

that successive statements of motivic material are related, they tell little about how the

motivic material might change from statement to statement, and whether there might be

specific processes underlying that change. This requires some additional vocabulary, which

will be introduced as needed in the analysis.

E. Schenkerian perspectives on Motive

The MCA categories are concerned primarily with motivic features on the surface of

a melody. Their only acknowledgment of different structural levels in a melody occurs in the

category "stepwise progression." Here, the notes that prominently spell out stepwise

progressions are held to be of higher structural importance than the notes between them. But

protracted stepwise progressions are not the only way that structurally important pitches in a

melody might have motivic implications; one possibility not addressed in MCA categories is

6 The converse, however, need not always be the case, i.e. significant motivic relations may exist that are not readily audible. This is especially so in the case of motives that exist on a higher-than-surface level, to be discussed below

Page 19: Jarrett Mehldau

19

that of "motivic parallelism" -- appearances of motives that recur on higher-than-surface

structural levels and/or exist on more than one structural level.

In his later writings, Heinrich Schenker discussed a number of ways in which a

melodic motive can repeat itself on different structural levels of a composition (Burkhart

1978, Cadwallader and Pastille 1992). Charles Burkhart standardized "motivic parallelism"

as an umbrella term for the variety of phenomena discussed by Schenker in this vein

(Burkhart, 1978). In the context of tonal European art music, to which the concept was

originally applied, a motivic parallelism often involves a surface motive being mirrored by

important, structural voice leading events at a higher level. In this context, a pitch is deemed

more or less "structural" based on its harmonic and contrapuntal importance relative to an

underlying harmony or harmonic progression. In mm.1-2 of the Chorale St. Antoni from

Brahms’ Haydn Variations, for example, the initial neighbor note motive on the melodic

surface is nested within the next highest structural level's neighbor-chord progression, which

expands the tonic and spells out the same motive in the melody:

Ex 11: Brahms, Haydn Variations, mm.1-3

Another kind of motivic parallelism concerns motives that are apparent only on

higher-than-surface levels. These "high-level motives," as they have been dubbed by

Cadwallader and Pastille (Cadwallader and Pastille 1992), are "unfolded" in various ways on

lower structural levels, but don't necessarily appear prominently on the surface itself. For

example, in the E-flat minor prelude from Book I of Bach's Well-Tempered Clavier a

descending third is spelled out by pitches of relative structural importance in a number of

ways on various structural levels, but occurs relatively rarely on the surface of the music:

Ex 12 (reprinted from Burkhart 1978, 149):

Page 20: Jarrett Mehldau

20

Cadwallader and Pastille have remarked of such high-level motives that they are

"'basic motives' of the tonal system -- figures that recur at middle ground levels and regulate

the development of lower-level motives in contrasting passages and sections throughout the

tonal system." (Cadwallader and Pastille 1992, 135) In other words, they are not particular to

individual compositions.

Applying similar Schenkerian concepts in analysis of bebop improvisations by

Charlie Parker, Henry Martin has done extensive research in trying to identify motivic

parallelisms like the type exhibited in the Bach example above (see Martin, 1996). While the

current investigation is primarily concerned with surface motivic features, it will also identify

isolated instances of motivic parallelisms similar to those in the both the Bach and Brahms

examples.

One of the challenges in applying Schenkerian perspectives to bebop is deciding

which pitches over a given harmony can be considered structural (Larson, 1998). There is a

tendency for pitches that would be considered unstable in the context of tonal European art

music to be treated as stable in jazz harmony: sevenths, ninths, elevenths, and thirteenths are

frequently (but not always) used as chord tones that do not resolve to adjacent consonances

(thirds, fifths, sixths, and octaves.) Most Schenkerian analysts of bebop acknowledged this

(e.g. Strunk 1996, Larson 1998, Martin 1996), but nonetheless classify stability

hierarchically, considering octaves, fifths, and thirds and sixths to be more structural than

other chord tones that might appear to be treated as consonances in certain contexts (see

Larson 1998, 212-213). On this issue, the current investigation will generally follow their

examples. However, in certain contexts it will employ a slightly more holistic notion of what

constitutes a melodic pitch's "structuralness." In this not so strictly Schenkerian context the

Page 21: Jarrett Mehldau

21

term "structural" can refer simply to audible prominence: "structural pitches" are often

support points in a melody, some of which can be determined by harmony, but some of

which can be determined independent of harmony via a combination of rhythm, articulation,

and/or position in a melodic contour. 7

Since it was developed in conjunction with analyzing the 1960s free jazz of Ornette

Coleman, in which the harmonic structure of compositions performed had little or no bearing

on the melodies improvised, the MCA system in fact remains entirely silent on issues of

harmony and voice leading. But since both performances under consideration here belong

more or less to the bebop idiom, and the compositions over which the improvisations take

place are essentially tonal, harmony certainly has implications for how motivic material is

organized in both cases. One of the conjectures put forth in this investigation is that Jarrett

has a higher tendency than Mehldau to organize his motivic material in such a way that

harmonically structural voice leading events (in the Schenkerian sense of the term) implicit in

the harmonic form are emphasized in his melodic lines. It would be outside the scope of the

study to do a comprehensive Schenkerian voice leading analysis of both solos. But in key

passages, in which either player appears to be particularly guided by voice leading

considerations in his manipulation of motivic material, I will include some basic

Schenkerian-like reductions. In the case of “Just in Time,” a middle-ground reduction of the

original composition is also provided to elucidate what emerges as an important motivic

parallelism operating in multiple passages in Jarrett’s solo.

IV. Analysis

A. MCA-Analysis

Below are the transcriptions of the two solos, including a complete MCA analysis of

each. Abbreviations employed in the analysis are as follows: p = pitch, r = rhythm,

c=contour, i.v.= initial variation, t.v. = terminal variation, dove = dovetailing, step = stepwise

progression). To Jost’s categories I have added additional qualifiers (inv) and (ret) for

situations in which inversion and retrograde relationships are especially manifest.

7 This is in fact the sense in which the concept of "structuralness" is invoked in Jost's

MCA category of stepwise progression mentioned above.

Page 22: Jarrett Mehldau

22

Page 23: Jarrett Mehldau

23

Page 24: Jarrett Mehldau

24

Page 25: Jarrett Mehldau

25

Page 26: Jarrett Mehldau

26

Page 27: Jarrett Mehldau

27

Page 28: Jarrett Mehldau

28

Page 29: Jarrett Mehldau

29

B. Motivic Transformation

The MCA analyses above leave little doubt that both solos are highly motivic. A conspicuous

contrast that emerges between the two, however, is the relative density of different types of

MCA closely linked together in Jarrett’s solo. In the solo on “Just in Time,” small motivic

cells on the surface of the music are constantly recycled, but subtly varied in close proximity

to each other, continually generating new material that is motivically associated with the

immediately preceding material. This lends his improvisation a certain kind of forward,

developmental thrust that is not as prevalent in the Mehldau. This contrast is underscored by

the comparative ubiquity of wholesale repetition in the Mehldau: The above analysis counts

38 occurrences of MCA - rep in the 360 total quarter-note beats of Mehldau’s solo, compared

to 11 occurrences in the 512 beats of Jarrett’s solo.

In practice, tightly linked, varying types of MCA are generally indicative of motivic

transformation, processes in which motivic material undergoes alteration over some number

of statements. Motivic transformation can be in varying degrees multidirectional or

unidirectional. Unidirectional motivic transformation involves processes in which discrete,

minor alterations to some initial motivic material accrue sequentially, preserving in each

successive statement the alterations from each previous statement. In multidirectional

transformation, alterations are not always preserved from statement to statement, and more

freedom is taken with the number of alterations introduced in any given statement. The solo

on “Just in Time” contains copious instances of each, but shows an overall tendency to

towards the unidirectional variety.

The beginning of the solo offers two short, intertwined examples of unidirectional

motivic transformation. In the two-bar lead-in to his solo, Jarrett twice repeats D-C neighbor

note figure, a diatonization of the ubiquitous D-C-sharp neighbor note figure from the

original melody, helping one to hear the first melodic fragment of the solo proper as being

divided into two cells A and B, indicated in the simplified analysis below:

Ex. 13: Jarrett, lead in + mm.1-8

Page 30: Jarrett Mehldau

30

In the next fragment (mm.2-3), a metrically displaced pseudo-sequence of the first, the major

second descent of A becomes a minor third (MCA p, c, r). The third fragment (mm.3-4)

begins with A', then deviates from the two initial cells until the next measure when A' is

inverted and expanded by an extra pitch to become A'' (MCA – p, c(inv), (r)). In m.6,

neighbor note figure B has become double neighbor B' (MCA p, c, r). It recurs in the next

measure, rhythmically augmented and permuted, as B'' (MCA p, c). Underlying both these

brief transformations are the processes of intervallic and rhythmic expansion.

It is interesting to compare this passage to the way in which Mehldau begins his solo:

Ex 14: Mehldau, mm.1-8

After paraphrasing the original melody with figure X, Mehldau seems to immediately diverge

from the motivic material of which X is comprised. But in m. 4, he calls attention, via

articulation and repetition, to the chromatic embellishment tenuously marked X' (a relatively

common formula in bebop improvisation). Apart from the initial half-step descent and lack of

a repeated unison, this figure has the same intervallic content of the opening gesture – in fact

leaving it interpretable as a motivic parallelism. It also has the overall contour of a

descending step, a feature shared by the opening gesture that appears to determine the course

of the rest of the phrase; a structural, downward stepwise progression in D minor in fact

already starts in m. 3, as indicated in the complete MCA analysis above. Z is an element that

is not manifestly related to the opening gesture. But the rhythmic trajectory of repetitions of

Page 31: Jarrett Mehldau

31

Z’ allows a seamless insert in m.6 of another X', which, when combined with a leap to the

repeating note figure Y from the opening gesture, creates Z''. These features all create a high

degree of motivic connectedness and produce a sense of coherence in the passage, but they

don’t comprise the kind of frugal, tightly packed motivic transformation on display in the

opening of the Jarrett solo.

A more long range, though equally transparent example of motivic transformation in

Jarrett’s solo occurs at the beginning of the fourth chorus. As shown in the simplified

analysis below, we have a short melodic fragment A0 that gradually grows into a more

complicated structure over a series of modifications:

Ex 15: Jarrett, mm. 96-110

After its first statement, figure A0 is metrically displaced and incorporates an incomplete

upper neighbor note before it's final pitch, becoming A1. In A2, the first-quarter note of A1 is

embellished by a downward leap of a fourth. After a literal transposition of the figure, it

loses its remaining quarter note, becoming A3. A2 appears again in m.102, its latter half

transposed up by a step. Between each of these successive six statements, there are at most

two modifications. The statement of A4 breaks the unidirectional pattern somewhat,

introducing more degrees of separation, but it is nevertheless easily associated with A1: the

upbeat is augmented to a quarter note, the initial interval is shrunk to a minor third, a quarter

note repetition is added, the whole step between B-flat and C becomes an augmented unison,

and a punctuation mark of a falling sixth is added to the end (incorporating the rhythmic and

enharmonic intervallic separation between the end of A2 and the beginning of A3 in mm.

101-102). If A4 is somewhat of a deviation from more or less unidirectional pattern of the

Page 32: Jarrett Mehldau

32

transformation, the remaining figure A5 picks up where that process left off, reintroducing

the tremolo gesture (this time with an interval of a major second), and substituting

incomplete upper neighbor eighth notes of the other figures with the major sixth downward

leap in quarter notes enharmonically taken from the previous figure. A5’s rhythmic

placement also gives it a heightened association with the departure point of the passage, A0.

It is interesting to note that within the relatively multidirectional transformations between A3

and A5 there is at least one entirely unidirectional process: the shrinking of each figure's

initial interval by a half or whole step, from a perfect fourth to a major second. Finally, in the

beginning of the longer eighth-note run in m.106, the upbeat portion of figure A5 becomes a

diminished triad to the upper neighbor of the following downbeat, and the falling 6th is filled

in by a major triad and chromatic descent, producing A6 -- a fragment that bares very little

resemblance to the passage’s original departure point A0.

An equally striking example of tightly packed unidirectional motivic transformation

occurs in beginning of the Jarrett’s second chorus, shown here with its complete MCA

analysis to better convey it’s motivic intricacy:

Ex 16: Jarrett, mm. 29-48

Page 33: Jarrett Mehldau

33

Figure B0, which grows out of the tail of figure A becomes B1, as part of the dovetailing of

m. 34 into the following phrase. This phrase concludes with the B2, related to B1 by

inversion and reversal of the final eighth note. In B3, the final descent of B2 expands to a

fourth. In mm. 40-41, B3 (or its first three pitches) is dovetailed into a transformation of

itself, labeled B4. The relationship between B3 and B4, marked here as MCA - c, r, is highly

audible because of the similarity of rhythm, the leap down on the final eighth note, and the

outline of a fifth between the first and last pitches. B5 rhythmically augments the final

pitches of B4, contracting the final interval to a fifth. And B6 adds one intermediate and two

final notes to the final pitches of B5.

While instances of motivic transformation certainly occur in Mehldau's

improvisation, they tend to be of a much shorter range than the process just described, with

their final product generally quite similar to their point of departure. At the end of m. 60 in

Mehldau's solo, for example, the four note scalar ascent X undergoes the following

transformation: after being transposed and rhythmically displaced but otherwise unaltered, it

is rhythmically diminished at the beginning of 63, and then intervallically augmented

(appearing in its original rhythm). Running parallel to this brief process is a liquidation of

the eighth note "filler material" between the statements of this idea.

Ex 17: Mehldau, mm.60-63

Another brief and slightly more unidirectional transformation occurs at the end of the

solo:

Ex 18: Mehldau, mm.69-72

The three-note, descending scalar figure in m. 70 is given an extra initial eighth note triplet

(in addition to being rhythmically displaced relative to the meter) in m. 71. In m. 72 the

Page 34: Jarrett Mehldau

34

initial interval is expanded to a third, while the rest of the figure reverses direction, launching

an ascending scalar run to end the form.

Mm. 43 through 46 offer another case of motivic transformation. In this case we start

with a somewhat larger structure that is subsequently liquidated in a multi-directional

fashion.

Ex 19: Mehldau, mm. 43-51

In X1, X0 loses its upper arpeggio, one of the initial repeated notes, and its lowest note, but

gains a chromatic grace note and a rhythmically augmented repetition of the first descending

interval. X2 regains a repeated note and the final note of the descent from X0. X3 gains an

embellishment and once again loses the final note of the descent. X4 expands the initial

interval to a fourth, otherwise mimicking X3. Finally, X5 retrogrades the final arpeggio of

X4 before launching scalar material not immediately related to the original figure. Due to this

transformation’s multidirectional nature and features that remain common between all the

melodic fragments in question -- in particular the descending arpeggiated triad -- the passage

is perhaps heard less as a process than as relatively static “riffing” on an idea. This is

something that occurs frequently in Mehldau’s solo.

C: “Motivic Glue”

Where Jarrett creates a high degree of continuity in his solo by creating motivic

associations as part of transformative processes like the ones described above, Mehldau

shows a tendency to use motivic associations to relate events that are separated by larger

spans of music, which in its own way helps to create a sense of thematic coherence in the

solo as a whole. While the “chain” in “motivic chain association” is a particularly apt

Page 35: Jarrett Mehldau

35

metaphor for the kind of motivic work on display in the Jarrett solo, Mehldau’s approach

often might seem better characterized by the term "motivic glue."

One of the most conspicuous examples of a motivic element that recurs throughout

Mehldau’s solo is the rhythmic idea introduced in m. 5, a repeating figure with a duration of

two quarter-notes, which becomes displaced relative to the 3+2/4 meter as it is repeated

across the bar line:

Ex 20: Mehldau mm.5-7

This rhythm is immediately taken up in the next passage, its pitch content changed into a

descending chromatic scale, where it is used as the common basis for two parallel 4-bar

phrases (the only example of such a phrase construction in either solo under investigation):

Ex 21: Mehldau mm.9-16

After this, the rhythm always appears in conjunction with ascending or descending scalar

material that is locally diatonic:

Ex 22: Mehldau, mm.31-36

In its final appearance, it undergoes the brief transformation already described above:

Page 36: Jarrett Mehldau

36

Ex 23: Mehldau, mm. 69-72

In all the occurrences of this figure, it is not merely the rhythm (or pitch content) of each

repeated unit that is motivic, but also the phenomenon of metric displacement brought about

by its repetitions across the barline -- a feature difficult to convey with the MCA labels.

It should be noted that appearances of this figure account for 25 of the 38 instances of

MCA - rep tallied in Mehldau’s solo. Though it may aid in creating a sense of thematic

coherence in the solo, the multiplicity of passages with extended repetitions of this figure

underscore the solo’s relative lack of forward developmental thrust in comparison to Jarrett’s;

not once does Jarrett repeat an motivic idea over multiple bars without somehow varying it in

the process.

Another type of motivic glue that Mehldau employs throughout the solo is his use of

motivically related "signals" beginning on beat 1 that end many of his phrases:

Ex 24: Mehldau

Interestingly, these closing signals undergo transformation over the course of the solo. But

since they are generally separated by lengthy spans of music and their transformation is fairly

multi-directional, they are not heard as something that moves the music forward; rather, they

are binding points that remind the listener where the music has come from.

The figure in m. seven, ending the solo's second phrase, clearly harkens back to the

solo's opening in m. 2, by virtue of the repeating B-flat, but with the addition of an initial

falling fifth. The same figure, slightly stretched out rhythmically, also ends a phrase in m.

24. Its next occurrence closes the first chorus in m. 36, with the falling fifth inverted to a

Page 37: Jarrett Mehldau

37

rising fourth, and an additional leap added in between the repeating Fs. It is quickly

answered by the end of the next phrase, inverted back to its original contour, with the added

leap turned into a lower neighbor note. To this point, the transformation has been

unidirectional. In m. 57, it takes back its form from m. 24, with an added grace note and the

fifth turned into a major third. Finally, this major third becomes a falling sixth in m. 67. That

these figures refer back to each other so effectively is due both to the consistency of their

rhythmic placement (always beginning on beat 1) and the scarcity elsewhere of material

rooted in the repeating note figure that caps the solo’s opening gesture; the only instances not

included in the above excerpts are in mm. 17 to 18, where Mehldau riffs on a repeating E,

and the figures in mm. 43-47, and mm. 64-65.

Jarrett employs recurring phrase-closing motives as well, but in more local contexts.

In the second half of the second chorus, (mm. 46-63) for example, he ends nearly every

phrase with an ascending or descending leap of a third:

Ex 25: Jarrett mm. 46-65

Jarrett only twice returns to this phrase-closing motive (mm. 110, 120) in subsequent

choruses, so it does not have the same solo-wide, audible cohesive function as does the

phrase-closings of Mehldau’s solo8.

8 The leap of a third may however be considered a yet another surface manifestation of a stepwise descending filled-in third, a motive that would appear to operate prominently on

Page 38: Jarrett Mehldau

38

As the MCA analysis of Mehldau’s solo indicates, motivic associations also exist

between the beginnings of many of his phrases, especially in the latter half of his solo. The

connections are perhaps less audible than those of the closing signals described above, partly

due to differences in their rhythmic placement, but they provide motivic connectivity

nonetheless:

Ex 26: Mehldau

The quarter note figure in m. 26 is the first in the solo to emphatically to use pitches

immediately derived from a stack of fourths. The beginning of phrase that launches the

second chorus in m. 37 begins on the downbeat with eighth-note triplets, but emphasizes

intervals like those in m.26 (Due to the prominence of the c-f fourth in this figure, it has been

tenuously marked as a dovetailing from the end of the previous phrase in the MCA analysis).

The figure in m. 39 more or less preserves the rhythm from that of m. 37, but recycles the

contour and interval content of the figure from m. 26, (with an added F inserted between E

and A). The figure in m. 51 refers more directly back to m. 26, rhythmically diminished but

with identical interval content. Finally, the first eight notes in m. 59 are a literal transposition

of the figure from m. 39.

The MCA analysis of the Jarrett shows that his phrase beginnings, in contrast, are

more likely to involve dovetailing from the end of immediately preceding phrases than

references to earlier phrase beginnings.9 This again underscores the constant forward

developmental motion on display in Jarrett’s solo in comparison to the Mehldau’s. It should

be emphasized, however that Jarret’s solo is not without motivic glue that spans over large

multiple structural levels throughout Jarrett’s solo, and the original melody of “Just in Time”, as will be discussed below in conjunction with higher level motives and motivic parallelisms 9 One important exception to this is the lower neighbor note figure from the melody, which Jarrett uses in one way or another to begin choruses 1, 2, and 3.

Page 39: Jarrett Mehldau

39

sections of the solo. In the course of building phrases that transformatively push motivic

material forward, Jarrett often returns to important motivic elements. Two of the most

ubiquitously referenced elements are the lower neighbor note figure from the original melody

(e.g. mm. 1-3, 32-35, 54, 67, 74), and the stepwise descending third, discussed below in

conjunction with motivic parallelisms and higher level motives.

D. Motivic Parallelisms, Higher-Level Motives.

In example 14 above, it was suggested that the opening gesture X and X' from

Mehldau’s solo could be interpreted as a motivic parallelism. The single, descending step

outlined by both figures is in fact a likely candidate for a higher-level motive operating

throughout the solo. It’s origin clearly lies in the in the original melody, where it is repeatedly

and prominently outlined slightly below the surface, in parallel with the bass (see example 2,

mm. 1-2, 3-4, 4-5, 9-10, 11-12, 13-14, 17-19, 25-26, 27-28, 29-30, 33-36). A single,

descending step is of course an entirely rudimentary element of any music, tonal or

otherwise, that employs scalar material. But if it is recurrently cast in high relief, in relative

isolation from its surroundings, there is no reason that a single, descending step may not be

considered to be motivic.

Ex 27: Mehldau, mm. 1-2

Pitches A and G in the Mehldau’s opening passage above would be low on a traditional

Schenkerian hierarchy of harmonic structuralness, as they both form elevenths with the bass.

But their rhythmic placement (and articulation) lends them an irrefutable structural

importance in their melodic context. It is quite possible to hear echoes of these measure-long

descending steps as they are elaborated in different ways over the course of the solo:

Ex 28: Mehldau, mm.5-7

Page 40: Jarrett Mehldau

40

The upper part of above two-part melody similarly outlines a step from mm. 5 to 6,

the A in m.5 appearing as an anticipation to beat 1 of m.6. The lower part outlines a

descending step from both m.5 to m.6 and m.6 to m.7. Here the structuralness of the pitches

involved in the stepwise motion is determined both rhythmically and harmonically.

In mm. 43-48, Mehldau departs radically from the harmonic form of the original. But

in mm. 43-46 he twice outlines a measure long (minus anticipations) descending step with

the highest points of the arpeggio melody:

Ex 29: Mehldau, mm. 43-46

When he returns to the form harmonically in m.49 (4 bars into the second A section

as in example 28), Mehldau again refers to the opening gesture, with ornamented step from

B-flat to A

Ex 30: Mehldau, mm. 49-50

One might also interpret the following surface appearances of isolated, single

stepwise descents as motivic parallelisms with the opening, especially as they are given

emphasis with articulation:

Ex 31: Mehldau, mm. 22-25

Page 41: Jarrett Mehldau

41

A motive that appears to recur on multiple levels of Jarrett’s solo on “Just in Time” is

the stepwise descending filled-in third. As with Mehldau’s descending step, the origin of this

higher-level motive lies in both the melody and harmony of the original composition. From a

Schenkerian perspective, however, this motive would seem to have even deeper structural

implications in “Just in Time” than does the single descending step in “I Didn’t Know What

Time it Was”:

Ex 32: Berlin, “Just in Time,” middle-ground reduction

In the above middle-ground reduction of the original “Just in Time”, the stepwise descending

third emerges as a motivic element over various prolongational spans: First, it appears in the

bass from mm.1-5 preparing a movement to II. At one structural level higher, it occurs as

parallel tenths between soprano and bass (allowing for registeral transfer in the melody) in

mm. 1, 15, and 17 as the harmony moves from a prolonged tonic to VI via bVII. Next, it

appears closer to the surface in the bass as a movement preparing II in mm. 23-25. Finally it

emerges as the Ur-line of the form as a whole.10

10 However, since Jarrett always prolongs the dominant between choruses to connect them

(i.e. dispensing with the tonic at mm 31-32 of the form), individual choruses aren’t

Page 42: Jarrett Mehldau

42

The improvised melody on Jarrett’s first solo chorus contains multiple explicit

occurrences of the stepwise descending third, on at least two structural levels. Mm. 1-5

elaborate parallel tenths with the falling bass line (with the C in m.2 occurring as a 2.5 beat

anticipation to the next measure, and registrally transferred in m.4).

Ex 33: Jarrett, mm.1-5

The descending third is also already present on the surface in the opening figure in m.1,

comprising a classic example of a nested motivic parallelism.

In the next phrase, the motive is taken up on the surface as a closing figure, which

subsequently undergoes rhythmic transformation and inversion:

Ex 34: Jarrett, mm.9-18

Finally, it occurs as atop a prolongation of II (and ultimately V) at the end of the form,

spelled out in the lowest notes of the melody:

Ex 36: Jarrett, mm. 24-30

harmonically closed. Therefore, the Ur-line will not apply to the background-most structures

of the individual choruses as performed by Jarrett)

Page 43: Jarrett Mehldau

43

Later on, there are numerous other explicit appearances of the motive on or close to the

surface (e.g. mm. 36-7, 45, 49, 51, 56, 58, 88-91). And in the beginning of the fourth chorus,

Jarrett employs the same contrapuntal skeleton (parallel tenths) as in the beginning of the first

chorus:

Ex 37: Jarrett, mm. 96-101

Jarrett’s solo also contains other, isolated examples of different motivic elements that

are repeated in close proximity on slightly different, close-to-surface structural levels. In the

example below, an arpeggio of stacked thirds (D, F, A, C) is decorated by lower neighbors (C

and A themselves functioning as neighbors to Bb), then appears in the next phrase entirely as

a surface element (with the F raised to F# as the leading tone in a V7/VI):

Ex 38: Jarrett, mm.33-36

In the next example, the ubiquitous leap of the third that ends the previous and subsequent

phrases is elaborated as a stepwise descent (decorated with arpeggio material) in mm. 57-58:

Ex 39: Jarrett, mm. 54-63

Page 44: Jarrett Mehldau

44

Below, the consonant leap down of a third in m. 118, decorated by an upper neighbor note, is

stretched out and slightly elaborated at the end of the phrase in m.120:

Ex 40: Jarrett, mm. 117-120

These examples are all superficial motivic parallelisms that may aid in creating continuity in

specific passages, but have little bearing on the solo’s overall thematic coherence or deeper

structures. However, they offer additional evidence of Jarrett’s ability to create motivic

associations between structural levels in local contexts.

E: Linear aspects of motivic organization

“Just in Time” and “I Didn’t Know What Time it Was” have different enough

structural features so that it is perhaps difficult to draw conclusions based on a comparison of

Jarrett’s and Mehldau’s use of higher-level motives in their improvised melodies. But

examining the two solos through the lens of voice-leading in general suggests a contrast

worth a final note: Jarrett seems to show a higher tendency than Mehldau to organize his

motivic material in such a way that basic voice-leading events implicit in the underlying

chord progressions are emphasized.

Page 45: Jarrett Mehldau

45

Since both compositions under consideration contain ample passages based on cycles

of fifths, they also contain passages in which one or more voice in the underlying chord

progression undergoes a linear descent, diatonic or chromatic, in its voice-leading with the

bass. This is illustrated in the rudimentary three-voice harmonization of each tune below:

Ex 41 A-B: rudimentary three-voice harmonization of underlying chord progressions (linear

descents, diatonic or chromatic, indicated with slurs)

A: “I Didn’t Know What Time it Was”

B: “Just in Time”

Page 46: Jarrett Mehldau

46

The extent to which each player’s improvised melodies outline portions of these linear

progressions, then, would have implications regarding their tendencies to emphasize

underlying voice-leading events in general.

In Jarrett’s solo, significant portions of these linear progressions very often emerge as

the frame of his improvised melody, clearly outlined in his manipulation of motivic elements.

A particularly striking example is given once again by the beginning of the fourth chorus:

Ex 42: Jarrett, mm. 96-110

Page 47: Jarrett Mehldau

47

Here, Jarrett organizes his motivic material to outline a descent, allowing for registral

transfers, from D-G over the first 14 bars of the form. The descent is initially diatonic, but

then chromatic from C onwards, and corresponds exactly to the upper voice of the above

three-part harmonization of “Just in Time.” Similar examples, though not as extensive, can

be found throughout the solo. Moreover, in almost all of the stepwise progressions indicated

in the MCA analysis of Jarrett’s solo, each constituent pitch is involved in some underlying

linear voice-leading progression.

“I Didn’t Know What Time it Was” doesn’t contain as long a cycle of fifths as occurs

in the first half of “Just in Time” (it’s longest, from mm.3-6 in Mehldau’s first solo chorus,

contains 6 iterations: E-A-D-G-C-F). But even on this scale, Mehldau’s improvised melodies

outline relatively few pitches from any of the potential linear progressions implicit in the

chord progression. And in contrast to stepwise progressions of Jarrett’s solo, only small

portions of the stepwise progressions that Mehldau employs correspond to voice-leading

events in the underlying harmony. Mehldau’s opening passage offers a telling point of

comparison with the Jarrett example above:

Ex 43:Mehldau, mm.1-8

Page 48: Jarrett Mehldau

48

In the indicated stepwise progression, only two of the constituent pitches participate in an

essential voice-leading progression: Bb-A, or 4^ to 3^ atop a II, V, I progression in F-major,

in m5. Other stepwise progressions in Mehldau’s solo are similarly ungrounded in the

structural voice leading of the underlying harmony.

A more extensive analysis of voice leading phenomena in the two solos is outside the

scope of this study. But the conjectures presented here offer direction for further research.

Correspondence, or lack-thereof, between improvised melodies and structural voice-leading

events in the underlying harmonic progression may indeed have a very significant role in the

motivic strategies - and improvisatory style in general - of the two players in question.

V. Conclusion

When interpreted with an eye to process, MCA analyses of the two solos studied lead

to clear evidence of Jarrett's relative propensity, compared to Mehldau, for tightly woven

motivic work characterized by forward-moving transformation of small motivic fragments.

They also reveal Mehldau's tendency to use motivic material as points of reference that bind

longer spans of music together. Supplementing the MCA categories with Schenkerian

perspectives shows that both players create motivic connections between structural levels,

and ground some of their most important subsurface motives in the original compositions

over which they improvise. Preliminary voice leading analysis of the solos, in turn suggests

that Jarrett shows a higher tendency than Mehldau to guide his improvised melodies by linear

voice-leading progressions implicit in underlying harmony.

Page 49: Jarrett Mehldau

49

To acknowledge that Mehldau is less inclined to execute passages of continual,

closely packed motivic transformation is not to imply that he is somehow a less skilled

improviser. The relative merits of each player in his idiom of course depend on the aesthetic

criteria by which they might be evaluated. When I once commended Jarrett's flair for motivic

development in a conversation with drummer/pianist Jukkis Uotila, for example, Uotila

responded, "yes, but so what," and proceeded to list other factors he deemed more important

in defining the bebop idiom, areas in which he personally felt Jarrett was lacking. And if

Jarrett's improvised melodies are more guided by structural voice leading events implicit in

underlying harmony than are Mehldau's, this does not mean that Jarrett is in general a better

voice-leader or has a better handle on harmonic form. It merely means that Jarrett tends to

articulate harmonic form in a more "classical" fashion than Mehldau. The resulting harmonic

clarity in Jarrett's melodic improvisation can again be understood as an aspect of his style, as

with his propensity for motivic transformation. Harmony in general would in fact be a very

fruitful topic of study for additional comparison between Jarrett and Mehldau. Even in the

limited context of their bebop-centered performances, cursory listening indicates that this is

an area in which two players diverge significantly, with Mehldau seemingly employing a

more expansive and liberal approach than Jarrett.

None of the contrasts between the two players’ motivic improvisatory styles

discussed in this study refute claims that Mehldau has been influenced by Jarrett. But they do

imply that such claims can be easily exaggerated. And they perhaps suggest that the

hypothetical lawsuit referred to above regarding plagiarism of Jarrett's "piano-trio concept"

most likely would not be worth anyone's while.

Sources: Berliner, Paul 1994. Thinking in Jazz: The Infinite Art of Improvisation. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. 193-219 Burkhart, Charles 1978. “Schenker’s ‘Motivic Parallelisms.’” Journal of Music Theory 22 (2):145-175 Carr, Ian 1991. Keith Jarrett: The Man and his Music. London: Grafton Books, 1991

Page 50: Jarrett Mehldau

50

Cogswell, Michael. 1995 "Melodic Organization in Two Solos by Ornette Coleman." Annual Review of Jazz Studies 7: 101 to 144 Collins, Troy 2006. “House On Hill.” All About Jazz. (internet publication) http://www.allaboutjazz.com/php/article.php?id=22449 DeVeaux, Scott. 1997 The Birth of Bebop. Berkeley, California: University Of California Press Jost, Ekkehard 1970. “Zur Musik Ornette Colemans.” Jazzforschung 2: 105-124 Jung, Fred 2008. “A Conversation with Brad Mehldau,” interview published on allaboutjazz.com, http://www.allaboutjazz.com/iviews/bmehldau.html Korman, Clifford 1999. “‘Criss Cross:’ Motivic Construction in Composition and Improvisation” Annual Review of Jazz Studies 10: 103-126 Larson, Steve 1998. “Schenkerian Analysis of Modern Jazz: Questions About Method.” Music Theory Spectrum 20/2: 209-241 Martin, Henry. 1996. Charlie Parker and Thematic Improvisation. Lanham, Maryland: Scarecrow Press Meehan, Norman 2002. “After the Melody: Paul Bley and Jazz Piano After Ornette Coleman.” Annual Review of Jazz Studies 12: 85-116 Moon, Tom 1999. “The Making, Unmaking & Remaking of Brad Mehldau.” JAZZIZ 16 (3): 32-36 Oulette, Dan 2007. “The Mehldau Effect” Down Beat – Jazz Blues and Beyond 74 (1) 32-34, 36-37 Reti, Rudolph 1951. The Thematic Process in Music. New York: Macmillan Rufer, Josef 1954. Composition with Twelve Tones. London: Barrie and Rockliff Schuller, Gunther 1958. “Sonny Rollins and the Challenge of Thematic Improvisation.” In Walser, Robert ed. Keeping Time: Readings in Jazz History. New York: Oxford University Press 1999, 212-222. Originally published in Jazz Review 1 (1) 1958, 6-11, 21. Shatz, Adam 1999. “A Jazz Pianist with a Brahmsian Bent.” New York Times (July 25, 1999) Strunk, Steven 1996. “Linear Intervallic Patterns in Jazz Repertory.” Annual Review of Jazz Studies 8: 63-115 Väisänen, Risto 1998. “Brahmsin ‘Der Tod das ist ein Kühle Nacht’ Op.96/1. Motiivianalyysin Ongelmia.” Musiikki 1: 30-67

Page 51: Jarrett Mehldau

51

Discography:

Keith Jarrett, Standards in Norway. ECM. 1989 Brad Mehldau, Art of the Trio, Volume I. Warner Bros / Wea. 1997