lucid dreaming and consciousness: a theoretical investigation849789/fulltext01.pdf · lucid...

115
Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation Master Degree Project in Cognitive Neuroscience One year Advanced level 30 ECTS Spring term 2015 Nuno Alexandre Pinto Supervisor: Paavo Pylkkänen Examiner: Antti Revonsuo

Upload: duonghanh

Post on 04-Aug-2018

282 views

Category:

Documents


8 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:A Theoretical Investigation

Master Degree Project in Cognitive Neuroscience

One year Advanced level 30 ECTS

Spring term 2015

Nuno Alexandre Pinto

Supervisor: Paavo Pylkkänen

Examiner: Antti Revonsuo

Page 2: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

Declaration of authorship

Thesis title: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation

Author name: Nuno Alexandre Pinto

The above noted work is submitted to the School of Bioscience at the University of Skövde, as a final Master project toward the degree of Master of Science (M.Sc.) in Cognitive Neuroscience. The project has been supervised by Paavo Pylkkänen.

I, Nuno Alexandre Pinto, hereby declare that:

1. The above noted work has not previously been accepted in substance for any degree andis not being concurrently submitted in candidature for any other degree.

2. The above noted work is the result of my own investigations, except where otherwise stated. Where corrections services have been used, the extent and nature of the corrections have been clearly marked.

Nuno Alexandre Pinto 2015-06-12

II

Page 3: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

Acknowledgements

First and foremost, I would like to thank my beloved better half for all the patience and

insightful comments on the manuscript, and for her cleverness in pointing out ways to retrieve

most precious articles, which greatly benefited the analysis. But mostly, for bearing with me

and my utmost foul moods in the darkest moments of research and writing: thank you Rita!

Secondly, I would like to express my gratitude to my supervisor Paavo Pylkkänen for

the suggestions and insights provided, and also for his availability throughout the process.

Thirdly, I am also grateful for Oskar McGregor seminars, in which he shared his ideas

about thesis writing, and for the subtitle suggestion.

These acknowledgements would not be complete without my expression of gratitude to

all the persons who, in some way or another, contributed to the development of what I am today.

All the experiences, all the dialogues, all the books... All that is seen and yet to be seen.

“Neither outer sense impressions nor inner images produce consciousness; rather

one and the same consciousness illuminates the sense world in waking and the

dream world in sleep.” (Thompson, 2015a, p. 187)

III

Page 4: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

Table of Contents

Abstract V

Introduction 1

Part I – Main concepts definition and lucid dreaming history 3

1. Main concepts definition ….................................................................................... 3

2. Lucid dreaming history …...................................................................................... 6

Part II – Human sleep and dreaming 9

1. Human sleep: NREM and REM …........................................................................ 9

2. Human dreaming: from non-lucid to lucid …....................................................... 15

3. Human dreams: delusional hallucinations or creative imaginations? …............... 30

Part III – The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming 33

1. The use of lucid dreams in cognitive neuroscience research …............................. 33

2. Contemporary cognitive neuroscience research with lucid dreaming.................... 39

Part IV – Lucid dreaming and consciousness 61

1. Bridges between lucid dreaming and consciousness …......................................... 61

2. The protoconsciousness and primary-secondary consciousness hypotheses …..... 66

3. Can lucid dreaming unveil consciousness? …....................................................... 71

4. The continuity of meta-awareness in consciousness research …........................... 78

Concluding remarks and bridges for the future 82

References 85

Appendix A 109

Appendix B 110

IV

Page 5: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

Running head: LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS

Abstract

The purpose of this theoretical investigation is to provide a conceptual and theoretical analysis

of the lucid dreaming phenomenon and its contribution for consciousness research. After a brief

introductory overview, the most relevant concepts are defined and a sketch of lucid dreaming

historical evolution is given. Secondly, an analysis of the main characteristics pertaining human

sleep and dreaming is done, leading to the grasp of the differences between non-lucid and lucid

dreams. Thirdly, one enters into the cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming per se,

approaching the ways of using it in the experimental setting, and also what has been done

contemporaneously, with a presentation of the neuroscientifical research line relevant for this

investigation. Fourthly, the connections between lucid dreaming and consciousness are

examined, in what relates to main theories and the usefulness of this phenomenon in

consciousness research. Lastly, some concluding remarks are in order, laying out some bridges

for the future.

Keywords: Lucid dreaming, Consciousness, Lucidity, Meta-cognition, REM sleep, Dreaming

V

Page 6: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 1

Lucid dreaming is one phenomenon that has accompanied mankind since early times

and, as so many others, was neglected by the scientific and academic world, relegated to a

more mystical or paranormal approach, in the same way as many other phenomena related to

sleep and dreaming (LaBerge & Rheingold, 1991). In the late XIX and XX century, it suffered

from being a nuisance to Freudian psychoanalysis (Tranquillo, 2014), staying dormant to

science up until the work of LaBerge and colleagues (LaBerge, Nagel, Dement, & Zarcone,

1981), who came up with a way to physiologically identify the exact moment lucid dreaming

was happening inside the dreaming subject. From then on, research and experiments on the

subject exploded, somehow replicating what had happened after the discovery of rapid-eye-

movement (REM) sleep and its connection to dreaming in the 1950's (Thompson, 2015b).

Nowadays, cognitive neuroscience of dreaming proposes pre-frontal deactivations

observed in REM sleep as the cause of the cognitive limitations experienced in non-lucid

dreaming. It would seem that there are human brain regions that become selectively deactivated

when compared to the waking state, such as the dorso-lateral pre-frontal cortex (DLPFC) and

the precuneus, and other regions that become more activated, such as the limbic and paralimbic

systems (Hobson & Pace-Schott, 2002; Nir & Tononi, 2010; Schwartz & Maquet, 2002;

Spoormaker, Czisch, & Dresler, 2010). In lucid dreaming state, it has been suggested that the

previously deactivated DLPFC re-activates, allowing for the awareness of being in a dream.

Likewise, it has been proposed that lucid dreaming associates with pre-frontal activations

(Hobson, 2009a; Hobson, Pace-Schott, & Stickgold, 2000; Karim, 2010; Spoormaker et al.,

2010; Tononi, 2009; Voss, Holzmann, Tuin, & Hobson, 2009).

In Hobson's view (2009a), lucid dreaming suggests that consciousness can be split into

two parts: an actor, in the guise of the dreamer, and an observer, in the guise of the awake,

becoming, in the author's opinion, a very attractive phenomenon for scientific investigation in

the realm of consciousness studies. So attractive indeed, that it can be said the human mind may

Page 7: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 2

be capable of being in two states at one time, waking and dreaming. In turn, this should make

possible for the experimental scientist to measure the physiological correlates of lucid dreaming

in the laboratory (Hobson, 2009a). It is also suggested that, to study consciousness, one would

need to track changes in the brain and in the mind simultaneously (Brylowsky, 2010; Hobson,

2005).

In line with what has been previously mentioned, the aim of this thesis is to provide a

conceptual and theoretical analysis of cognitive neuroscience field of lucid dreaming, in what

concerns its direct relation with the field of consciousness studies. The rationale behind this

objective lies in the permanence of the lucid dreaming phenomenon throughout mankind's

recent history, allied with the surge in contemporary cognitive neuroscience lucid dreaming

research connected with a particular aspect of consciousness studies.

For this purpose, it is important firstly to discern the neurophysiological details of

human sleep and dreaming relevant for the aim of the thesis, especially in what ways non-lucid

dreams differ from lucid ones, in a phenomenologically and neurophysiologically way, and how

the path to lucidity is spanned. Part II of this thesis - “Human Sleep and Dreaming” - will

attempt to accomplish this objective. Afterwards, on Part III, an overview of the cognitive

neuroscience of lucid dreaming is provided, focusing first in general guidelines on working with

lucid dreams, and then proceeding to an in-depth analysis of the most relevant experiments

from contemporary research. The last section of the manuscript deals with the main connections

between lucid dreaming and consciousness, relating to the main theory and hypotheses

connecting both fields, at the present moment.

Notwithstanding, it is important however to previously define some main concepts

relevant to this investigation and to have a brief review of the history of lucid dreaming. This

will be done in the next section.

Page 8: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 3

Part I – Main concepts definition and lucid dreaming history

1. Main concepts definition

For a clear understanding of the nomenclature used throughout this theoretical

investigation, central concepts clarification is in order. To accomplish that, succinct definitions

in the form of citations will be given, followed by short analysis, except in the case of lucid

dreaming, which is defined in a somewhat extended manner.

First of all, one can start by trying to clarify the concept of consciousness relevant for

this manuscript purposes, with the help of the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, and its

entry on the subject:

”The words “conscious” and “consciousness” are umbrella terms that cover a

wide variety of mental phenomena. Both are used with a diversity of meanings,

and the adjective “conscious” is heterogeneous in its range, being applied both to

whole organisms—creature consciousness—and to particular mental states and

processes—state consciousness” (Van Gulick, 2014, section 2).

Consciousness seems to be an umbrella term indeed, but for the purposes of this theoretical

investigation, it will be used as state consciousness, so as it seems to be used in contemporary

lucid dreaming research. Nevertheless, one can also refer to consciousness as “the subjective

psychological reality that we experience” (Revonsuo, 2010, p. 295), which may lead to

understanding it as the subjective experience of “I”, as in inner presence. These two definitions

seem to complement each other, as state consciousness may be the subjective reality that is

experienced as the brain moves back and forth through a continuum of states amid the sleep-

wake cycle, and many other so-called normal and altered states of consciousness, during the

temporal frame of a human life. This concept will be mentioned and discussed throughout the

document.

Page 9: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 4

Under the umbrella term of consciousness, one can also find the concepts of

phenomenal consciousness and reflective consciousness. Simply put, phenomenal

consciousness “is the current presence of subjective experiences, or the having of subjective

experiences. An organism possesses phenomenal consciousness if there is any type of

subjective experience currently present for it” (Revonsuo, 2006, p. 37). The notion of

phenomenal consciousness is said to be the most essential concept for the science of

consciousness, as it indicates the immediate presence of qualitative experience for a subject

(Revonsuo, 2006). One step further, there is a higher-order form of consciousness, or reflective

consciousness, which “involves a cognitive operation that adds something to and is separable

from the phenomenal content itself” (Revonsuo, 2006, p. 41). These two concepts are of a

fundamental nature as to discern the core of this theoretical investigation. It is through these

conceptualizations that lucid dreaming research is contemporaneously connected to

consciousness research, as it will be made progressively clear throughout the manuscript,

culminating in Part IV: “Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness”.

Secondly, the concept of dreaming may be succinctly defined as “complex, multimodal,

dynamic and progressive conscious experiences during sleep that are organized in the form of a

sensory-perceptual world or a world simulation” (Revonsuo, 2010, p. 295). This concept will be

discussed in more detail throughout the whole Part II: “Human Sleep and Dreaming”.

Inside dreaming, one finds the phenomenon of lucid dreaming, which can be shortly

defined as “a dream during which the dreamer recognizes that the ongoing experience is a

dream” (Revonsuo, 2010, p. 298). Taking this into account, one can also say that lucid dreams

are dreams in which the dreamer knows he is asleep (Brown, 1934), that is, dreams in which the

dreamer is in a state of consciousness similar in many ways to waking consciousness, but in

which the subject knows that he is still dreaming (LaBerge et al., 1981). Lucid dreamers report

being in possession of all their cognitive faculties, namely: to reason clearly, to remember

Page 10: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 5

conditions of waking life, and to act within the dream upon reflection or according to a plan

decided before going to sleep (Holzinger, LaBerge, & Levitan, 2006). Lucid dreaming seems to

be the stalwart awareness that one is dreaming and that is not really awake. By this standard, it

may even be possible to consider lucid dreaming paradoxical, as it seems to contain elements

from both waking and dreaming states consciousness (Hobson, 2009a).

In day-to-day lives one does not think about being awake while one is, and in a similar

manner, one is not aware that one is dreaming while it is (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000). This

seems to be contrastive with lucid dreaming, in which one knows that is experiencing a dream.

This awareness of dreaming seems to correlate with clarity of memory and reasoning, that

permits the dreamer to recall his waking life and control his own activities (Barrett, 1992; Van

Eeden, 1913).

Moreover, the concept of lucidity in lucid dreams seems to be based upon a progression

toward a completeness of waking abilities, that co-exists with the continuity of the dreaming

mode of experience, and this co-existing and interaction of the two modes constitutes the

lucidity in lucid dreaming (Barrett, 1992). In short, in what relates lucid dreaming, the defining

feature of lucidity is the cognitive realization of the fact that this is a dream (Revonsuo, 2010).

In addition, some authors point out that if when one dreams one is explicitly aware that

is dreaming, then lucid may also take a psychiatric sense, as a condition of clear insight and

correct orientation to reality. There seems to be a clear possession of one's cognitive abilities

while in lucid dreaming, and also a balance of detachment and participation, in which the actor

and observer perspectives may be present simultaneously (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000).

When studying dreams, their degree of lucidity, and its relation to consciousness, it is

also important to define the concept of awareness. Curiously enough, the word awareness in the

English language is a synonym for the word consciousness, and is defined as ”the quality or

state of being aware, consciousness” (Awareness, n.d.). For the purposes of this investigation,

Page 11: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 6

one can distinguish at least three types of awareness:

1. Awareness - as the phenomenal experiences of objects and events (Cicogna & Bosinelli;

2001)

2. Awareness - as self-awareness, the awareness of being one-self (Cicogna & Bosinelli;

2001)

3. Awareness - as meta-awareness, the awareness of mental life itself (Cicogna &

Bosinelli; 2001); or the ability to take explicit note of the current contents of

consciousness (Schooler, Smallwood, Christoff, Handy, Reichle, & Sayette, 2011)

These three types of awareness are important to apprehend, so as to grasp the influence of the

brain on the ability to be aware while it moves through various states. These concepts are also

being used throughout the manuscript.

One last concept to be construed is meta-cognition, or the ability to reflect on and report

one's own mental states (Filevich, Dresler, Brick, & Kühn, 2015). Other coadjuvant concepts

will appear in the text and will be defined on a first appearance basis. All previously mentioned

definitions are of paramount importance, not only for a clear understanding of such subjective

phenomena as the ones covered in this essay, but also as a guidance, taking into account that the

relevant literature is using many concepts without a clear common ground or agreement onto

exact meanings.

After this clarification, the next sub-section will draw a historical sketch of the way

lucid dreams have accompanied mankind literature and reasoning.

2. Lucid dreaming history

Aristotle (350 B.C.E.) was the first to note that sometimes we know that we are

dreaming while we dream, as he wrote on his treatise “On Dreams” that when one is asleep,

there is something in consciousness declaring that what presents itself is but a dream. From the

Page 12: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 7

eighth century onwards, lucid dreaming was cultivated in Tibetan Buddhism, and also known in

Sufism and Indian Yoga (Holzinger, 2009; Holzinger et al., 2006). The dream yogis of Tibet

used lucid dreams as an opportunity to experiment and realize the subjective nature of the

dream state and, as an extension, the waking experience (Holzinger, 2009). It was practised as a

form of yoga - Dream Yoga - and used to exercise the maintenance of consciousness during all

sleep stages. The final objective of the practice was to be able to maintain consciousness during

death so that the practitioner could consciously find the path to the other world (Holzinger et al.,

2006).

The first oneirologist, or pioneer of dream research, to systematically investigate and

collect dream reports on lucid dreaming, is considered to be Léon d'Hervey de Saint-Denys, a

Parisian French academic and professor of Chinese literature and language, that in 1867

anonymously published his 20 years of experience in dream research (Holzinger, 2009;

Holzinger et al., 2006). The term Lucid Dream was created by Frederik Willems van Eeden, a

Dutch psychiatrist, for a lecture given on the 22nd of April of 1913, at a meeting of the Society

for Psychical Research (SPR) in London, in which he spoke about how he had recorded three

hundred and fifty-two lucid dreams (Holzinger, 2009; Holzinger et al., 2006; Van Eeden, 1913).

More than half a century later, Paul Tholey (1991) named this experience Klartraum, or dreams

of clarity, and defined them as those dreams where the dreamer has complete consciousness and

awareness about the fact that he is dreaming, being able to interfere, influence, or even control

or create the dream (Holzinger, 2009; Holzinger et al., 2006).

It is exactly the more than two thousand years of lucid dream phenomena that provide

the rationale for expanding this area of human experience through neuroscience

experimentation (Brylowski, 2010). In that sense, although a challenging phenomenon to study,

lucid dreaming is a methodological paradigm that offers the potential for a deep understanding

of the brain basis of consciousness, and is becoming more and more a part of the mainstream

Page 13: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 8

cognitive neuroscience (Gackenbach, 2010; Gavie & Revonsuo, 2010; Mota-Rolim & Araujo,

2013).

In the next section, human sleep and dreaming are analysed in some detail, in what

concerns the neurophysiological and phenomenological aspects pertinent to this investigation.

Page 14: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 9

Part II – Human sleep and dreaming

One of the universal and sturdy changes in consciousness, that humans can observe in

themselves and in others, is the moment of loss of self-awareness induced by the transition from

waking to sleep. Even when some kind of awareness returns in sleep during dreaming, this self-

awareness is somehow different from wake state consciousness. One easy and simple way to

experience it is when awaking from a dream, as one perceives the velocity of the switch from

the self-awareness of dream state, to the self-awareness of waking state (Muzur, Pace-Schott, &

Hobson, 2002). Notwithstanding, in phenomenological terms, one of the most astonishing

features of conscious experiences in sleep may well be the similarity of the inner world of

dreams to the real world of wakefulness (Nir & Tononi, 2010).

1. Human sleep: NREM and REM

During sleep, the human brain repeatedly cycles through five main stages. The first four

of these sleep stages are conventionally called Non-Rapid-Eye-Movement, or NREM sleep, and

the fifth stage is known as Rapid-Eye-Movement, or REM sleep (Thompson, 2015b). NREM

and REM sleep are clearly distinguishable, and by convention are defined through the

electrophysiological signals detected by means of a combination of three techniques:

electroencephalography (EEG), electrooculography (EOG), and electromyography (EMG)

(Muzur et al., 2002). These techniques taken together are usually called polysomnography

(PSG), even though the term PSG can accommodate other techniques as well. The EEG

technique basically records electrical activity at the scalp, that in turn can be used to identify

each sleep stage associating it to a distinct brain wave pattern. The EOG technique measures the

corneo-retinal standing potential found between the front and the back of the human eye, which

can be used to identify eye-movement direction. The EMG technique essentially records the

Page 15: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 10

electrical activity produced by skeletal muscles, in order to monitor and understand what kind

of muscular activity goes on, if any (Muzur et al., 2002; Thompson, 2015b).

NREM corresponds to an increasing depth of sleep, and is indicated by high-voltage and

low-frequency wave activity that progressively dominate the EEG register. Low frequency

waves prevail in stages three and four, which are the deepest, and are also called slow wave

sleep, delta sleep, or deep sleep. As the sleeping brain moves out of deep sleep, the large, slow

waves begin to change, more and more resembling the waves typical of the waking brain

(Thompson, 2015b). Likewise, this wake-like state has been called paradoxical sleep (Kahn &

Gover, 2010). In consequence, REM sleep corresponds to high or fast frequency and low

amplitude activity in the EEG, singlets and clusters of rapid eye movements in the EOG

channel, and very low levels of muscle tone, or muscle atonia, in the EMG channel. It also

correlates with a selective reactivation and deactivation of neural activity (Kahn & Gover, 2010;

Muzur et al., 2002).

To summarize, one can look at the characteristics of the physiological sleep stages

according to PSG, in a structured manner (Muzur et al., 2002; Thompson, 2015a):

NREM stage I (N1) – Eyes closed, slow eye-rolling movements, EEG alpha waves (8-

12 Hz) dwindle, slower theta waves (4-8 Hz) appear.

NREM stage II (N2) – Eye movements cease, 12-14 Hz bursts (sleep spindles) and brief

high voltage waves (K-complexes) occur.

NREM stage III (N3) – There is a blend of sleep spindles and high-amplitude, slow

frequency delta waves (0,5-4 Hz).

NREM stage IV (N4) – Almost exclusively delta waves (<4 Hz).

REM – High-frequency, low-amplitude waves, limb muscles paralysed, i.e. muscle

atonia, eyes closed with rapid eye movement.

Page 16: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 11

The evidence that the brain is highly active during sleep was first obtained with the

discovery of REM sleep and its correlation with dreaming. In 1953, Aserinsky and Kleitman

discovered that the eye movements, EEG pattern, and autonomic nervous system activity going

on in sleep were significantly correlated and did not occurred randomly, suggesting that these

physiological phenomena and dreaming were manifestations of a particular level of cortical

activity normally occurring during human sleep. Later on, in 1957, Dement and Kleitman

correlated eye movements during sleep with dream activity, creating an objective method for

the study of dreaming (Aserinsky & Kleitman, 1953; Dement & Kleitman, 1957).

During REM sleep, activation seems to be triggered in pontine regions while sensory

input is gated and motor output is suppressed, and for that reason the brain in REM sleep seems

to function as a closed loop system (Hobson & Pace-Shott, 2002). The noticing of this

simultaneous block of sensory inputs and motor outputs, sometimes called input-output gating

(Hobson & Pace-Shott, 2002), was also important, taking into account that this activation in

REM sleep is suggested to occur at regular ninty-minute intervals and is thought to occupy 20%

of sleep (Hobson, 2005; Kahn & Gover, 2010). Moreover, the development of magnetic

resonance-compatible EEG recording systems granted researchers the proficiency to obtain

PSG in strong magnetic fields, and to obtain functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)

data during verified, unambiguous sleep, allowing non-invasive measurements of neural activity

changes with high spatial resolution (Dresler, Spoormaker, Wehrle, & Czisch, 2014b).

NREM sleep appears to be outlined by widespread deactivations in the human brain,

compared to wakefulness, as seen with fMRI-measured cerebral activity (Kaufmann et al.,

2006). However, on the background of these deactivations, specific regional activity can be also

observed with the use of fMRI, such as neural correlates of sleep EEG micro-processes: slow-

waves (Dang-Vu et al., 2008), K-complexes (Caporro et al., 2012; Czisch et al., 2009; Jahnke et

al., 2012), and sleep spindles (Andrade et al., 2011; Caporro et al., 2012; Schabus et al., 2007).

Page 17: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 12

For instance, Andrade and colleagues (2011) have showed that fast sleep spindles appear

to be accompanied by increased neural activity in the thalamus, cingulate and pre-frontal

cortices, and pre- and post-central gyrus (Andrade et al., 2011). Moreover, processing of

external acoustic stimuli is pointed out to be suppressed during spindle activity (Schabus et al.,

2012), whereas the functional coupling between the hippocampus and the neocortex increases

(Andrade et al., 2011), leading to the idea of a spindle specific brain state, which may allow for

an information transfer increase between brain regions associated with memory processes

(Dresler et al., 2014b).

On the other hand, REM sleep is suggested to correlate with increased cerebral blood

flow in the thalamus, visual areas, and limbic regions (Braun et al., 1998; Maquet et al., 1996).

There seems to be reactivation of the limbic, para-limbic, and amygdala regions, thought to

associate with emotions, and also reactivation of the medial pre-frontal cortex (MPFC), implied

to associate with internally motivated behaviour (Braun et al., 1998; Maquet et al., 1996). At the

same time, the precuneus, thought to correlate with with body location, and the DLPFC,

proposed to associate with executive functions, autobiographical memory recall, and goal

selection, appear to be selectively deactivated (Braun et al., 1998; Kahn & Gover, 2010; Maquet

et al., 1996).

REM sleep has recently been divided in two distinct components: phasic REM, in which

one can find periods of eye movement activity and high cortical activation, and tonic REM, in

which one can find periods with few eye movements and relatively low activation (Dresler et

al., 2014b; LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000). Indeed, there appear to be differences in sensory

processing and in thalamocortical activity patterns during phasic and tonic REM sleep periods

in humans (Wehrle et al., 2007). There seems to be no reaction to sensory stimuli in periods

containing bursts of phasic REM activity, whereas acoustic stimulations elicit residual

activations of the auditory cortex during tonic REM sleep (Wehrle et al., 2007). This may

Page 18: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 13

demonstrate a strong decrease in brain reactivity to acoustic stimulation during phasic REM

sleep periods, while the processing to the stimulus is maintained, to some extent, during tonic

REM sleep, when compared to waking state (Dresler et al., 2014b; LaBerge & Gackenbach,

2000; Wehrle et al., 2007).

All in all, there seems to be residual external stimulus processing during tonic REM

periods, whereas in phasic REM sleep the brain apparently functions in a closed loop (Wehrle et

al., 2007). This neural divide may help explain why sometimes external stimuli are incorporated

into the dream narrative, while other times the same stimuli are ignored or lead to awakening

(Dresler et al., 2014b; Wehrle et al., 2007). Likewise, it has been proposed that prototypical

dreaming is bound to phasic REM sleep, whereas tonic REM sleep seems to be associated with

dreaming activity similar to that of NREM sleep, in a qualitative way (Dresler et al., 2014b;

LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; Wehrle et al., 2007).

Moreover, through the study of human sleep disorders one can ascertain that sleep may

be a local brain phenomenon, instead of a global one, and that wakefulness, REM, and NREM

sleep may not be mutually exclusive states. This is to say that different brain regions may be in

different states at the same time (Nir & Tononi, 2010). As an illustration, this can be observed in

a NREM parasomnia – sleepwalking - where thalamocingulate pathways are as active as in

waking state, while the rest of the cerebral cortex is in NREM sleep (Mahowald & Schenk,

2005). It can also be noticed in a REM parasomnia - REM sleep behaviour disorder (RBD) -

where somatic muscle atonia, one of the defining features of REM sleep, is absent, allowing for

dream mentation acting out, often with violent or injurious results (Mahowald & Schenk, 2005).

In a more general way, Hobson (2009b) suggests sleep as a recovery process in

homoeothermic animals, like humans, which seem to require it to maintain body weight and

body temperature (Siegel, 2005). In fact, only mammals and birds are thought to be

homoeothermic, and are also pointed as the only animals to display REM sleep (Hobson,

Page 19: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 14

2009b; Parmeggiani, 2003). Furthermore, the concept that humans appear to only cognize

effectively in a narrow range of brain temperature, may suggest that consciousness probably

depends on homoeothermy and, in turn, homoeothermy may depend on sleep (Hobson, 2009b).

Sleep deprivation is said to cause psychological dysfunction, which seems to support the idea

that the integrity of waking state consciousness depends on the integrity of dream state

consciousness, and that on the brain mechanisms of REM sleep (Hobson, 2009b; Muzur et al.,

2002).

In summary, and according to what has been mentioned and discussed in this sub-

section, sleep seems to be an active brain process, and represents two somehow different states

of being, NREM and REM sleep (Nir & Tononi, 2010). These states are neurophysiologically

different from each other as each is from wakefulness, and it would seem that most parts of the

brain are active during all these three states of being, in a different form (Hobson, 2005). A

reorganization of brain activity and function seems to be going on during these three states, and

research guided by human sleep disorders led to important basic sleep concepts (Mahowald &

Schenk, 2005). These concepts include the idea that sleep presents itself as local brain

phenomenon, instead of a global one, and state dissociation or inter-mixture seem to point out

that these three states are not mutually exclusive, but rather a continuum (Hobson, 2005; Nir &

Tononi, 2010; Rees, Kreiman, & Koch, 2002).

Moreover, and also mentioned before, sleep is pointed out to be an actively regulated

process and could be regarded as a reorganization of neuronal activity (Hobson, 2005; Nir &

Tononi, 2010). It appears to vary between species and during lifespan, suggesting its many

functions. Sleep, as we know it in larger mammals, is thought to correlate with relatively large

brains and with homoeothermy (Hobson, 2005; Hobson 2009b). The study of mental

experiences during sleep offers a unique opportunity to clarify how changes in brain activity

relate to changes in consciousness, because if it was not for sleep, it would be difficult to see

Page 20: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 15

that consciousness as a state depends on the way the brain is functioning (Hobson, 2005;

Mahowald & Schenk, 2005; Nir & Tononi, 2010; Rees et al., 2002). It would seem that ”sleep

and dreaming are a treasury of altered states of consciousness.” (Revonsuo, 2010, p. 251)

2. Human dreaming: from non-lucid to lucid

In Revonsuo's (2006) view, the dreaming brain is an exemplary system for the study of

consciousness, as it brings about all the sphere of subjective experience in a form that mirrors

waking reality. As such, for the science of consciousness, the dreaming brain may be a unique

model system (Revonsuo, 2006). Expanding on the definition given in Part I, sub-section 1,

dreaming can be further defined as ”a universal human mental state characterized by

hallucinatory imagery congruent with a confabulatory, temporally ordered, story-like

experience” (Pace-Schott, 2005, p. 563). In contrast, a more recent approach states dreaming

“exclusively in terms of consciousness and conscious experiences going on during sleep. More

particularly, dreaming is defined as subjective experience during sleep that simulates awake

experiences realistically” (Revonsuo, 2015, p. 57). This totally imaginary experience seems to

be accepted by the dreamer in the same way as waking percepts and events are.

According to Pace-Schott (2005), the importance of dreaming for neuroscience places

itself mainly in two pillars:

1. The single-mindedness and isolation of dreaming (Rechtschaffen, 1978), made evident

when the dreamer is absorbed in the dream world and plot, without any awareness of the

wake state alternative reality, except in lucid dreaming (Pace-Schott, 2005).

2. The fact that in dreams there are images, plots, characters, motor skills (flying),

emotions and memories that are created anew, without any connection with the

memories, abilities, characters or day-to-day tasks from the waking state (Pace-Schott,

2005).

Page 21: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 16

The functional neuroanatomy of human REM sleep and dreaming was mapped by

Maquet and colleagues in 1996, using positron emission tomography (PET) and PSG. The

regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) distribution was recorded as an index of neuronal activity,

and all subjects recalled a dream after awakening from REM sleep. REM sleep associated with

intense neuronal activity, ocular saccades, muscular atonia and dreaming (Maquet et al., 1996).

The study results seemed to point out that the rCBF correlated positively with REM

sleep in the following brain locations: pontine tegmentum, left thalamus, both amygdaloid

complexes, anterior cingulate cortex, and right parietal operculum. The rCBF correlated

negatively with REM sleep in a vast area of the DLPFC, the parietal cortex, the posterior

cingulate cortex, and the precuneus. According to the authors, these results provided the first

comprehensive description of the distribution of cerebral activity during REM sleep in humans

(Maquet et al., 1996).

According to Muzur and colleagues (2002), unveiling the physiological basis of REM

sleep is to grasp that the ascending reticular activating system of the brainstem, as described in

waking state (Moruzzi & Magoun, 1949), is re-engaged during REM sleep, and the neuronal

basis of REM activation is to be found in the reciprocal interaction of aminergic and cholinergic

neurons in the pontine brainstem (McCarley & Hobson, 1975; Muzur et al., 2002).

In waking state, all main neuromodulatory brainstem systems seem to be available and

highly involved in goal-directed behaviour, such as: serotonin, norepinephrine, dopamine, and

acetylcholine (Kahn & Gover, 2010; Muzur et al., 2002). In REM sleep, two of these systems

apparently shut off: the locus coeruleus neurons, which modulate norepinephrine, and the dorso

raphe nucleus neurons, which modulate serotonin (Kahn & Gover, 2010; Muzur et al., 2002).

This change in brain chemistry, where the aminergic system shuts down while the

cholinergic system remains high, seems to be behind the occurrence of the so-called

hallucinatory images, the reduced ability to recognize the implausibility of the dream scenario,

Page 22: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 17

and the reduced ability to stay focused, typical of the non-lucid REM dream which, in a sense,

seems to have a mind of its own (Kahn & Gover, 2010).

Notwithstanding, dreaming may not be exclusively happening in REM sleep, since

NREM sleep awakenings yield reports of mental activity from stages N1, N2, and N3 (Foulkes,

1962; Nielsen, 2000). It is estimated, from an extensive review done by Nielsen (2000), a

general recall rate frequency of 42,5% from NREM sleep awakenings, so-called mental

experiences or sleep mentations. This figure clearly contrasts with the 81,8% recall rate

frequency from REM sleep awakenings (Nielsen, 2000; Pace-Schott, 2005). It is also suggested

that brain activation processes occurring outside PSG recorded REM, so-called covert REM,

may be in the origin of NREM dreaming (Nielsen, 2000; Pace-Schott, 2005).

Nevertheless, this general estimation hides in itself great variations, as a zoom-in

analysis can detail. For instance, reports from NREM N1 are frequent, between 80% to 90% of

the time, but short (Nielsen, 2000). In this sleep stage, vivid hallucinatory experiences are

reported, commonly defined as hypnagogic hallucinations, which are often static, similar to

single snapshots (Hobson & Pace-Schott, 2002; Hobson et al., 2000) that frequently do not

include a self character (Foulkes, 1985). On the other hand, awakenings from NREM N2 and

N3 yield reports of some experienced content in 50% to 70% of the occasions (Nielsen, 2000),

even though it may vary during the night and between subjects (Nir & Tononi, 2010). In

moments when NREM N3 is prevalent, and many large slow waves dominate the EEG,

awakenings appear to return few reports (Stickgold, Malia, Fosse, Propper, & Hobson, 2001).

Regularly, NREM awakening reports are qualitatively different from typical REM sleep

ones, meaning that they are usually short, thought-like, less vivid, less visual and more

conceptual, less motorically animated, under greater volitional control, more plausible, more

concerned with current issues, less emotional and less pleasant (Fosse, Stickgold, & Hobson,

2001; Hobson et al., 2000). The average length of REM sleep reports seems to increase with the

Page 23: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 18

duration of the REM sleep episode, contrary to what happens in NREM sleep (Hobson et al.,

2000).

Moreover, REM dream reports are more frequent, longer, more bizarre, more visual,

more motorically animated, and more emotional than NREM dream reports (Hobson et al.,

2000; Pace-Schott, 2005). It would seem that REM sleep exhibits much more gamma frequency

fast brain waves or rhythms, from 30 Hz to 80 Hz, than NREM sleep (Pace-Schott, 2005), as

measured by EEG (Corsi-Cabrera, Guevara, & Del Río-Portilla, 2008; Corsi-Cabrera, Miro, Del

Río-Portilla, Pérez-Garci, Villanueva, & Guevara, 2003), intracranial EEG (iEEG) (Gross &

Gotman, 1999; Nishida et al., 2005), and magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Corsi-Cabrera et

al., 2008). When in waking state, these oscillations are thought to be associated with attention to

stimuli and active cognition (Jensen, Kaiser, & Lachaux, 2007; Pace-Schott, 2005).

In contrast, NREM sleep, notably N3 and N4, reveals little gamma activity, correlating

instead with slow brain waves produced by recurrent interactions between the thalamus and

cortex (Pace-Schott, 2005). These recurrent interactions produce slower oscillations, as in sleep

spindles and delta waves, which can also be called corticothalamocortical rhythms, and in turn

associate with the cortical slow (<1 Hz) oscillation that groups in time the other

corticothalamocortical oscillations (Achermann & Borbely, 1997; Pace-Schott, 2005; Steriade,

2000). This slow oscillation seems to consist of periods of neuronal quiescence, alternating with

shorter periods of rapid neuronal firing (Destexhe, Hughes, Rudolph, & Crunelli, 2007; Pace-

Schott, 2005). This may be the cause of dreams lower frequency in NREM sleep (Hobson et al.,

2000; Pace-Schott, 2005).

Studies with EEG and MEG suggest that phasic REM sleep is accompanied by brain

activity that may be related to dream phenomena (Pace-Schott, 2005), such as: visual imagery

(Conduit, Crewther, & Coleman, 2004; Ogawa, Nittono, & Hori, 2005), enhanced cognitive

activity and attention (Corsi-Cabrera et al., 2008; Jouny, Chapotot, & Merica, 2000; Nishida et

Page 24: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 19

al., 2005), decoupling of executive control and perception (Cantero, Atienza, Madsen, &

Stickgold, 2004; Corsi-Cabrera et al., 2003, 2008; Perez-Garci, Del Río-Portilla, Guevara, Arce,

& Corsi-Cabrera, 2001), and enhanced emotional processing (Abe, Ogawa, Nittono, & Hori,

2008; Corsi-Cabrera et al., 2008; Ioannides et al., 2004).

REM sleep deprivation or circadian manipulations appear to intensify the occurrence of

dreams in NREM sleep (Nir & Tononi, 2010; Pace-Schott, 2005). REM sleep dreaming seems

to be more related to imagination than to perception, as it is known from lesion studies that

dreaming requires an intact temporo-parieto-occipital junction, and that lesions in this area also

affect mental imagery in wakefulness (Nir & Tononi, 2010; Pace-Schott, 2005).

Picking up the distinctions put forward by Nir and Tononi (2010), one could say that

dream consciousness is fascinating, if one looks into the ways it neurophysiologically differs

from waking consciousness:

1. Reduced voluntary control and volition - In dreaming there seems to be a prominent

reduction of voluntary control of action and thought. The right inferior parietal cortex

(Brodmann's area 40) appears to be deactivated during REM sleep, and this may help to

explain this reduction, since this area seems to have a role in waking volition (Braun et

al., 1997; Desmurget, Reilly, Richard, Szathmari, Mottolese, & Sirigu, 2009; Goldberg,

Ullman, & Malach, 2008; Maquet et al., 1996).

2. Reduced self-awareness and altered reflective thought - Holding contradictory beliefs is

a common feature, as is accepting strange or non-replicable events in wake

consciousness, such as flying (Hobson, Stickgold, & Pace-Schott, 1998). This may be

connected to the deactivation of certain brain regions, such as: the posterior cingulate

cortex, the inferior parietal cortex, the orbito-frontal cortex, and the DLPFC (Braun et

al., 1997; Maquet et al., 1996).

3. High degree of emotionality or emotional involvement - This may be related to the idea

Page 25: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 20

that REM sleep is pointed to correlate with a marked activation of limbic and paralimbic

structures, such as: the amygdala, the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula (Maquet et

al., 1996; Maquet et al., 2000; Nofzinger, Mintun, Wiseman, Kupfer, & Moore, 1997).

4. Altered mnemonic processes - Memory appears to be altered for the dream and within

the dream, and if the dreamer does not wake up, most dreams usually are lost forever,

probably not registered in memory (Nir & Tononi, 2010). Even if the dreamer awakes,

the memory rapidly vanishes in what concerns the greater details, if not written down or

recorded (Nir & Tononi, 2010). The hypo-activity of the pre-frontal cortex, which seems

to be also implicated in mnemonic processes, may have an important role in dream

amnesia (Braun et al., 1997, 1998; Maquet et al., 1996; Nofzinger et al., 1997).

In what concerns the neurophysiology of dreaming and waking consciousness, LaBerge

(2010) claims that dreaming can be ascertained as the special case of perception without the

constraints of external sensory input, as perception can be understood as the special case of

dreaming constrained by sensory input. No matter how one looks at it, conceptualizing

dreaming may be central for conceptualizing consciousness (LaBerge, 2010).

Nevertheless, to most lay-persons who experience agency and voluntary control of

action during waking, it may be probably clear that dream consciousness is somewhat limited.

Even so, waking consciousness may not be that different from dream consciousness, and the

main difference may reside in the absence of meta-cognition in non-lucid dreams. In turn, lucid

dreams are said to be a remarkable phenomena, in which one can regain meta-cognition inside

the dream scenario (Spoormaker et al., 2010).

As seen so far, and picking up some previously mentioned information for reasoning

purposes, cognitive neuroscience of dreaming proposes pre-frontal deactivations observed in

REM sleep as the cause of the cognitive limitations experienced in non-lucid dreaming (Hobson

& Pace-Schott, 2002; Nir & Tononi, 2010; Schwartz & Maquet, 2002; Spoormaker et al., 2010).

Page 26: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 21

Brain regions become selectively deactivated when compared to waking state, such as the

DLPFC and the precuneus, and others become more activated, such as the limbic and

paralimbic systems (Braun et al., 1997; Muzur et al., 2002). Neuroimaging PET studies

(Kajimura et al., 1999; Maquet et al., 1997; Nofzinger et al., 2000) and quantitative EEG

studies (Borbély, 2001; Finelli, Borbély, & Achermann, 2001; Werth, Achermann, & Borbély,

1997), have shown DLPFC selectively deactivating with the deepening of NREM sleep (Hofle

et al., 1997), and maintaining those selective deactivations during REM sleep (Nofzinger et al.,

1999). In waking state, this area seems to correlate with executive abilities such as expectancy

and working memory (Maquet et al., 2005; Muzur et al., 2002).

In lucid dreaming state, the previously deactivated DLPFC is proposed to re-activate,

and empirical evidence supporting this notion was put forward by Voss and colleagues (2009).

Their findings seem to indicate that when lucidity settles in, it is accompanied by a shift in EEG

power, especially in the 40 Hz range in frontal regions, with a larger EEG coherence in

frontolateral and frontal areas (Voss et al., 2009 – this experiment will be analysed in a more

detailed manner in Part III, sub-section 2). Through the use of fMRI, a higher activation has

also been proposed in frontal, temporal, and occipital regions in lucid dreams when compared to

non-lucid ones (Wehrle et al., 2005, 2007). As a result, it has been suggested that lucid

dreaming associates with pre-frontal re-activations (Hobson, 2009a; Hobson et al., 2000;

Karim, 2010; Spoormaker et al., 2010; Tononi, 2009; Voss et al., 2009).

Lucid dreaming may be characterized as a state that includes many aspects of

wakefulness state, such as: meta-cognitive abilities, memory, volition, fully realized agency, and

full awareness of current state, even though some degree of volitional behaviour can be

observed during non-lucid REM sleep (Dresler et al., 2014a; Takahara, Nittono, & Hory, 2006).

Lucid dreaming may be an excellent example of wake-like cognition in dreams, in the way that

it seems characterized by meta-cognitive insight and heightened levels of cognitive clarity

Page 27: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 22

(Noreika, Windt, Lenggenhager, & Karim, 2010). These particular aspects are being mentioned

for more than a century now, since the lucid dream phenomenon has been approached in a more

methodic and scientific way (Brown, 1934; Green, 1968; LaBerge, 1980; Van Eeden, 1913).

Nevertheless, lucid dreaming does not appear to be an all-or-nothing phenomenon, and

it may occur in different degrees, as in a continuum of subjectively experienced lucidity

(Dresler et al., 2014a; Moss, 1986; Noreika et al., 2010). This continuum may stretch from

varying degrees of lucidity (Moss, 1986), from pre-lucid dreams or reflections to lucid control

dreams (Barrett, 1992; Dresler et al., 2014a; Kahan & LaBerge, 1994). It was already

hypothesized in the 1980s that the awareness found in non-lucid dreams could be the beginning

of partial lucidity (Moss, 1986), and that lucidity could in itself be seen as changing

quantitatively along a continuum (Tart, 1985).

Even taking into account that, in practice, lucid and non-lucid dreams seem to be part of

a continuum, and do not appear to be two static and separate states, with the purpose of

characterizing lucid dreams, comparisons between non-lucid and lucid dream reports were done

in the past. One good example of this is a major review done in 1988 by Gackenbach, which

used the content of lucid and non-lucid dreams originating from dream diaries, questionnaires

filled out by the dreamers, and a few lucid dreams collected from sleep laboratories (LaBerge &

Gackenbach, 2000; Gackenbach, 1988). The material was then explored by content analysis,

leading the author to conclude that, when compared to non-lucid dreams, lucid ones presented,

on the average, more auditory and kinaesthetic dream sensations, more sense or higher level of

control, and fewer dream characters (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; Gackenbach, 1988).

A few years later, in 1993, Levitan and LaBerge (as cited in LaBerge & Gackenbach,

2000) analysed self-rated content scales from six hundred and ninety-nine reports coming from

fifty-two veteran lucid dreamers (N = 52). When compared with non-lucid dreams, lucid ones

presented significantly higher levels of control, more positive emotions, higher levels of visual

Page 28: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 23

vividness, clarity of thinking, physical activity, and changes of scene (LaBerge & Gackenbach,

2000). Nevertheless, the authors pointed out that the sample was biased, as it recruited only

members of the Lucidity Institute. In what relates flying dreams, Barrett (1991) mentions that

these dreams are more likely to be reported by lucid dreamers and, when occurring in the same

dream, lucidity seems to precede flight (Barrett, 1991).

The contemporaneous approach to characterize lucid dreams is being done through face-

to-face and online surveys. These surveys aim to obtain broad and representative samples, from

which empirical findings on lucid dreaming, such as: prevalence, frequency, and typical

contents, are produced.

Schredl and Erlacher (2011) studied the frequency of lucid dreaming in a representative

German adults sample (N = 919), through a consultant that conducts surveys. All the

participants were contacted and interviewed at home, utilizing an eight-point rating scale (0 =

never, 1 = less than once a year, 2 = about once a year , 3 = about 2 to 4 times a year, 4 = about

once a month, 5 = about 2 to 3 times a month, 6 = about once a week, and 7 = several times a

week), that included one question: ”How often do you experience so-called lucid dreams (see

definition)?” (Schredl & Erlacher, 2011, p. 105); and one definition: ”During lucid dreaming,

one is, while dreaming, aware of the fact that one is dreaming. It is possible to wake up

deliberately, to control the dream action, or to observe passively the course of the dream with

this awareness.” (Schredl & Erlacher, 2011, p. 105). Dream recall frequency was also measured,

using a seven-point rating scale developed by Schredl (2004), along with socio-demographic

variables (age, sex, education, income, marital status, and population of place of residence).

According to the authors, the majority of respondents were infrequent lucid dreamers, whereas

about 20,1% were frequent lucid dreamers (frequency ≥ once per month) (Schredl & Erlacher,

2011).

Table 1 (next page) gives a full overview of the lucid dream frequency. All in all, and

Page 29: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 24

according to the study results, it would seem that about 51% of the German general population

has experienced a lucid dream at least once in their lives. Moreover, dream recall frequency

positively correlated with lucid dream recall frequency, even though the percentage of lucid

dreams compared to all recalled dreams was only 7,5%. Socio-demographic variables seem to

have no correlation with lucid dream frequency (Schredl & Erlacher, 2011). Interestingly

enough, the positive correlation between lucid dream frequency and dream recall frequency

seems to be an important one, and has been mentioned by previous studies (Blackmore, 1982;

Schredl & Erlacher, 2004; Watson, 2001).

In a more recent approach, Stumbrys and colleagues (Stumbrys, Erlacher, Johnson, &

Schredl, 2014) conducted an online survey to collect more extensive data on the

phenomenology of lucid dreaming. This included lucid dream origination, duration, active or

passive participation in the dream, planned actions for lucid dreams, and other

phenomenological aspects, in relation to participants age, gender, and lucid dream frequency.

Besides this, the authors also aimed to explore whether there were any differences relating

phenomenology between those who started having lucid dreams naturally, and those who

learned lucid dreaming deliberately. The sample for this study included six hundred and eighty-

Page 30: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 25

four participants (N = 684, 406 female and 278 male, age range: 10 to 74 years), who

completed an online questionnaire about lucid dreams.

According to the authors (Stumbrys et al., 2014), the online questionnaire was posted on

a German lucid dreaming Web site (http://www.klartraum.de), and was available in the first

seven months of 2004. The survey was anonymous, but an e-mail was asked, in an attempt to

minimize the risk of multiple responses. It included questions on biographical data and sixteen

items, from which five were open-ended, regarding dreams and lucid dreams. Participants also

estimated their dream recall frequency, on the same seven-point rating scale (Schredl, 2004)

that had been used in the previously presented survey (Schredl & Erlacher, 2011). Likewise,

lucid dreams frequency was evaluated on an eight-point scale (nightmare and creative dreams

were also evaluated), accompanied by a short definition of lucid dream: ”In lucid dreams, one

has awareness that one is dreaming during the dream. Thus, it is possible to wake up

deliberately, or to influence the action of the dream actively, or to observe the course of the

dream passively” (Stumbrys et al., 2014, p. 193).

According to Stumbrys and colleagues (2014), participants who claimed having had a

lucid dream at least once (N = 571), were asked additional questions about their lucid dreams,

such as:

Age of first lucid dream occurrence

By what means first lucid dream occurred

▪ Spontaneously

▪ Through deliberate training

▪ During relaxation techniques, for instance meditation or yoga

Estimation of average duration of lucid dreams (in minutes)

Descriptions of two possible continuations of lucid dreams were given (passive and

Page 31: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 26

active), and an estimation was requested (in percentages), of how often a passive or

active attitude was taken, in the lucid dreams

If actions had ever been tried (e.g., to fly, to speak with dream characters) in the lucid

dreams that had been planned in wakefulness state, and if so:

◦ Provide an approximate number of such efforts

◦ List the types of the planned actions attempted to accomplish

◦ Estimate the percentage of:

▪ How many intended actions in wakefulness were recalled in the lucid dreams

▪ How many of such recalled intents were successfully completed in the lucid

dreams

◦ Reasons for unsuccessful trials

On this survey, conducted by Stumbrys and colleagues (2014), the descriptive data

reveals that from the five hundred and seventy-one participants (N = 571) reporting having had

at least one lucid dream, 60,2% were frequent lucid dreamers (frequency ≥ once per month).

The mean age for the first lucid dream is 14,8 ± 7,8 years, and an average of 3,4 ± 5,6 is

reported. Moreover, 83,4% of first lucid dreams are said to be spontaneous, and happen as early

as age three, but are most likely to happen in the 12-14 age range, and much less likely to occur

after age 25. The mean duration of a lucid dream is estimated to be about 13,9 ± 13,4 minutes,

and lucid dreamers note that they are more likely to take an active role (56,3% ± 32,5%) in the

development of the dream plot, than a passive one. A majority of participants (58,9%) indicate

attempting to accomplish some waking intentions in their lucid dreams, averaging five

intentions per lucid dream. These intentions most often translate into actions impossible in the

wakefulness state, such as flying, which is the most popular one. Lucid dreamers seem to recall

almost half (48,5%) of their waking intentions inside the lucid dream, and less than half of those

recalled intentions (44,1%) are successfully carried out (Stumbrys et al., 2014).

Page 32: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 27

Relating to the effects of gender, age and lucid dream frequency, the authors (Stumbrys

et al., 2014) results indicate that women are more likely to be spontaneous than learned lucid

dreamers, have longer lucid dreams, but are less likely to take an active role in the development

of the dream plot, when compared to men. Furthermore, when predicting lucid dream

phenomenology, the frequency of lucid dreaming seems to be an important factor. Higher

frequency of lucid dreaming associates with an earlier occurrence of the first lucid dream, with

longer lucid dreams, with a higher likeness to try waking intentions in lucid dreams, with a

higher remembrance of such intentions, and a higher rate of accomplishment of those same

intentions. When comparing to spontaneous lucid dreamers, trained ones have shorter lucid

dreams, but a higher probability of taking an active role in the development of the lucid dream

plot, and are also more likely to try some waking intentions in their lucid dreams (Stumbrys et

al., 2014).

In the same line, a very recently published longitudinal study from Schredl and Göritz

(2015) analysed changes in lucid dream frequency in two online surveys with a three-year

period interval between them (concomitant with dream recall and nightmare frequency). An

eight-point scale was used to measure the lucid dreaming frequency (0 = never, 1 = less than

once a year, 2 = about once a year , 3 = about 2 to 4 times a year, 4 = about once a month, 5 =

about 2 to 3 times a month, 6 = about once a week, and 7 = several times a week), and a

definition adopted from a previous study (Schredl & Erlacher, 2004) was presented for the

scale:

”In a lucid dream, one is aware that one is dreaming during the dream. Thus it is

possible to wake up deliberately, or to influence the action of the dream actively,

or to observe the course of the dream passively.” (Schredl & Göritz, 2015, p. 3).

The study had a relatively large sample (N = 1,281) answering both surveys. The authors

consider the percentage of persons with no changes or small changes (±1) very high, as in

Page 33: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 28

63,39%, but nevertheless point out a decline in lucid dream frequency correlating with age

during adulthood, also stating that it is not a representative sample, even though it has a broad

age range (Schredl & Göritz, 2015). Table 2 (below) gives an overview of the results.

Lucid dreams are also thought to correlate with phasic REM (LaBerge & Gackenbach,

2000). In 1986, LaBerge and colleagues (LaBerge, Levitan, & Dement, 1986) picked up

physiological data from seventy-six laboratory lucid dreams of thirteen participants. This data

was used to compare eye movement activity, heart rate, respiration rate, and skin potential

activity, of lucid and non-lucid segments of REM periods. According to the authors, the lucid

segments of the REM periods exhibited significantly higher levels of physiological activation

than the preceding segments of non-lucid REM from the same REM periods. Also, REM

periods in which lucid dreams occurred were more activated than the ones in which it did not

occur (LaBerge et al., 1986). Furthermore, Hoffmann's reflex (H-reflex) amplitude was lower

during lucid REM (Brylowski, Levitan, & LaBerge, 1989), even though one must add that there

are limitations in the way H-reflex studies' results may be interpreted (Misiaszek, 2003). Taking

all this together, Laberge and Gackenbach (2000) suggest lucid REM as ”a paradoxically

deepened state of REM, with increased phasic activation and suppression of spinal reflexes”

Page 34: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 29

(Laberge & Gackenback, 2000, p. 160).

Lucid dreams are pointed out to occur mainly late in the sleep cycle (LaBerge &

Gackenbach, 2000; LaBerge et al., 1986; Van Eeden, 1913), almost exclusively during REM

sleep (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; LaBerge et al., 1986). It also seems to be a learnable skill

and practice with some inductive techniques is thought to make motivated individuals, with a

good dream recall frequency (≥ once a week), to become lucid more easily (LaBerge, 1980;

LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000 – induction techniques are analysed in more detail in Part III,

sub-section 1).

Another intriguing subject is the relationship between time in lucid dreams and real

time, that is, how time elapsed while executing cognitive tasks or motoric actions in lucid

dreams differs from the time elapsed executing the same tasks and actions in wakefulness.

Earlier studies have proposed an equivalence of time for counting in lucid dreams and in

wakefulness, but about 40% more time to perform a motor task (walking, gymnastics, squats) in

lucid dreaming (Erlacher & Schredl, 2004, 2008; LaBerge, 1985). Notwithstanding, more recent

studies propose that, despite the longer absolute durations for tasks involving motor activity, the

relative durations remain the same, thus also proposing that lucid dreaming can be used by

athletes the same way mental rehearsal is used in wakefulness, that is, to increase performance

(Erlacher, Schädlich, Stumbrys, & Schredl, 2014; Erlacher & Schredl, 2010). Erlacher and

colleagues (2014) speculate that motor tasks prolonged durations in lucid dreams might relate to

the lack of muscular feedback, or REM sleep slower neural processing. More research is needed

to clarify this particular aspect.

In accordance with Dresler and colleagues (2014a), contrasting with coma-wake,

anaesthesia-wake, or sleep-wake comparisons, the comparison between non-lucid and lucid

dreaming may be a fundamental key for understanding the neural correlates of consciousness,

since there is no major shift in vigilance state as defined by formal neurophysiological criteria,

Page 35: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 30

as lucid REM sleep is still REM sleep according to the American Association for Sleep

Medicine (AASM) sleep scoring benchmark (Dresler et al., 2014a; Iber, Ancoli-Israel, Chesson,

& Quan, 2007).

3. Human dreams: delusional hallucinations or creative imaginations?

Some authors point out that to create dream egos in dream worlds one needs only his

imagination, as in the ability to fashion mental images and to envision other worlds (Schredl,

2010; Thompson, 2015b). This may mean that, if dreaming has a source in imagination, the

dominant idea of dreams as delusional hallucinations may be, at least, an oversimplification

(Schredl, 2010). According to Thompson (2015b), in a non-lucid dream, one tends to lose

awareness and identify with the dream ego as the ”I”, whereas in a lucid dream, one regains the

awareness of the imagination consciousness.

Along the same lines, Kuiken (2010) and Brylowski (2010) indicate that non-lucid

dreaming may not be a model for psychopathology, as this would endanger a conception of

dreaming that respects its distinctive form of cognitive functionality. On the other hand, it may

well be a model of musical improvisation or involuntary poetry, than one of psychopathology.

In essence, formally defining dreaming as an hallucination may not be helpful for this field of

study (Brylowski, 2010; Kuiken, 2010). Moreover, Schredl (2010) adds that an outside

perspective on dreaming is when one uses the definition of hallucination for dreaming, and this

may not be useful because the brain state during hallucinations seems to be quite different from

the brain state during dreaming (Schredl, 2010).

In contrast, a very recent review by Dresler and colleagues (2015) clearly states that

cortical areas active during lucid dreams exhibit remarkable overlap with brain regions impaired

in psychotic patients. This would bring empirical support to the notion of lucid dreaming as a

model for insight into the psychotic state, inside the conceptualization of non-lucid dreaming as

Page 36: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 31

a model for psychosis. The later concept is in itself based on the notion that non-lucid dreaming

and psychosis share the same characteristics, such as: hallucinations, delusions, bizarre

experiences, disorganized thought, emotional intensifications, and lack of insight into current

state (Dresler et al., 2015). As non-lucid dreams can become lucid, the authors propose that

insight into his own condition can be improved in psychotic patients, by studying the ways

insight establishes itself in lucid dreams, bringing with it lucidity into the current state of the

individual. This is a very interesting avenue of research, which may lead to practical clinical

applications of lucid dream research. Dreams and lucid dreams may after all be a model for

psychopathology.

Erlacher and Chapin (2010) pick up the virtual reality hypothesis of dreaming and also

propose REM dreams as a kind of simulation of the real world on a higher cognitive level. The

dream actions would function as a neural simulation, which would imply that dreaming an

action could alter the waking performance of that action, improving it. In the authors opinion,

autonomic responses during dreams of physical activity parallel autonomic responses during

actual exercise, and the timing of both actions, the time that elapses to perform the actions in

the dream and in the waking state is also related. The authors also point out lucid dreams as an

ideal moment to practice those actions, for improved performance, as it offers the potential to

practice with a body complete with kinaesthetic sensations, in an environment that appears to be

experienced by the dreamer with the same vividness and realism of the waking experience,

which would work out better than mental practice and modern virtual reality simulators

(Erlacher & Chapin, 2010). This line of work seems also to go against the idea of dreams as

delusional hallucinations, at least in a strict sense.

On the other hand, the term virtual reality experience can be misleading, as the same

virtual reality scenario can be experienced by several people, whereas dreaming is generated

within the person's brain and cannot be perceived by others (Schredl, 2010). Moreover, some

Page 37: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 32

authors state that ”delusions” and ”hallucinations” seem to be part of the ordinary sleeping

experience, entirely normal in that context, which may help to defuse the negative connotations

of the words (Brooks & Vogelsong, 2010).

Curiously enough, some of the areas of the brain that are activated and deactivated

during non-lucid REM dreaming have been pointed out to be involved in the spontaneous

generation of music, such as jazz improvisation, which were preliminarily characterized by

deactivation of the DLPFC and simultaneous activation of the MPFC (Limb & Braun, 2008). In

Kahn and Gover's (2010) opinion, this attenuation of the DLPFC may imply a deregulation of

the contents of consciousness, which might benefit creative intuition, allowing unfiltered,

unconscious, or random thought and sensations to emerge, and this may account for some of the

innovation and creativity in non-lucid dreams where the DLPFC is also deactivated.

Thompson (2015b) conveys the concept of creative imagination in a quite curious way:

“You don't get a being with a sense of self by taking a hallucinating brain and

taking on some sensory inputs and motor outputs. You get a being with a sense of

self by taking a brain with a capacity for imagination – for imaging its past and

future – and embedding it within a sensing and moving body.” (Thompson, 2015b,

p. 188)

All in all, among researchers there seems to be a disagreement on the way dreams, non-

lucid and lucid, may be characterized. It would seem that, in a sense, human dreams can both be

delusional hallucinations and creative imaginations, depending on what lane of research one is

in, exactly. Several research routes are apparently open, and more studies are needed to pave

and strengthen those same lanes.

In the next section, the cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming is discussed, in what

concerns the late twentieth century conceptions of lucid dreams, in a very short way, to the most

relevant studies pertaining the aim of this theoretical investigation.

Page 38: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 33

Part III – The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming

Research in the 1960s and 1970s posted lucid dreaming experiences in the frequent

transitory arousals detected during REM sleep, and these micro-awakenings were proposed to

be its physiological basis (LaBerge, 1990; LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000). Nevertheless, it

would seem that one lone researcher suggested that lucid dreams were occurring during the

paradoxical phase of sleep, as REM was called, like any other dreams (Green, 1968; LaBerge

& Gackenbach, 2000). These speculations lacked empirical evidence to support them, and a

physiological way to mark the exact time the lucid dream was taking place was in need

(LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000). Already in the 1960s it had been found that, in many dream

reports, the eye movements recorded during REM corresponded to the direction the subjects

had reported (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; Roffwarg, Dement, Muzio, & Fisher, 1962).

Knowing this, probably it would be possible to use the dreamer volitional capabilities during

the dream to make a pre-arranged eye movement signal, marking the exact time they became

lucid (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000).

1. The use of lucid dreams in cognitive neuroscience research

As mentioned before, in lucid dreams the dreamer appears to possess a consciousness

comparable to waking state, regarding coherence, clarity and cognitive complexity. In the past,

it was questioned if these experiences were happening during sleep, or during brief

hypnopompic periods of wakefulness (LaBerge, 1990). Attempting to answer this question,

LaBerge and colleagues (1981) designed an experiment intended on determining the

physiological conditions in which lucid dreaming occurs. The authors took into account lucid

dreaming definition, reasoning that if dreamers became aware they were dreaming, maybe they

could also signal in their dream, and that signal probably would have a physiological correlate

Page 39: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 34

(LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; LaBerge et al., 1981).

LaBerge and colleagues (1981) experiment had five subjects (N = 5), trained in a

cognitive technique called Mnemonic Induction of Lucid Dreams (MILD), and selected for

their claimed ability of having lucid dreams on demand. The participants were studied in a sleep

laboratory for two to twenty non-consecutive nights. Standard PSG recordings were done,

including EEG, EOG, and chin, left and right wrist EMG. To mark the onset of a lucid dream,

the subjects produced a pre-arranged signal with dreamed eye shifts, which consisted of

extreme horizontal movements in a pattern left-right-left-right (L-R-L-R), that translated in the

EOG register. In total thirty-five lucid dreams were reported after spontaneous wakening from

different stages of sleep: REM in thirty-two cases, NREM N1 in two cases, and during the

transition from NREM N2 to REM in one case. The participants reported signalling during

thirty of these lucid dreams (LaBerge et al., 1981).

After each recording, according to the authors, the reports alluding to signals and the

respective PSG were reviewed by a judge unaware of the times of the reports. The role of the

judge was to determine whether one (or none) of the PSG epochs had a correspondence with the

reported lucid dream signal. In twenty-four of the aforementioned thirty-five cases, the judge

was capable of selecting the appropriate thirty-seconds epochs, out of about a thousand per

PSG, based on the correspondence between reported and observed signals. The chance

probability of this selection ratio was said to be astronomically small (LaBerge et al., 1981).

Since then, similar experiments were done in several other laboratories, providing strong

evidence to the idea of lucid dreaming as a sleep phenomenon (Armstrong-Hickey, 1988;

Dresler et al., 2012; Fenwick, Schatzman, Worsley, Adams, Stone, & Baker, 1984; Ogilvie,

Hunt, Kushniruk, & Newman, 1983; Voss et al., 2009).

With this said, one can question how to know that the subjects were really asleep when

the pre-arranged signal registered in the EOG? The answer appears to be straightforward: the

Page 40: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 35

subjective lucid dream accounts and the physiological sleep measures coincided, that is, ”all

signals associated with lucid dream reports occurred during epochs of unambiguous REM sleep

scored according to the standard criteria” (LaBerge et al., 1981, p. 729). The two main

conclusions arising from LaBerge and colleagues (1981) study were: first, lucid dreaming

seems to ensue mainly during REM sleep, and second, the results point out to the possibility of

lucid dreamers signalling intentionally to the outside world from the dream world while

dreaming (LaBerge et al., 1981). These two inferences opened the door for a new approach to

dream research: it was now possible to carry out experiments in which subjects could mark the

exact time of occurrence of specific lucid dream events. In turn, this allowed for the derivation

of psycho-physiological correlations and methodical testing of hypothesis (LaBerge &

Gackenbach, 2000; LaBerge et al., 1981).

According to some authors (LaBerge & DeGracia, 2000; LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000;

LaBerge et al., 1986), the use of lucid dreams in cognitive neuroscience research requires the

acquaintance or familiarity with several methodological issues, such as:

1. Subjective reports and introspection are known to provide the most direct accounts of

the contents of consciousness, but they are difficult to verify objectively, and

introspection is very far from an unbiased, direct or error-free process of observation

(LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000). In a experiment, it is of utmost importance that

subjects clearly understand the concept of lucid dreaming, as in the inclusion of a

recognition phrase in a sample lucid dream report (Snyder & Gackenbach, 1988).

2. The capacity for dream recall seems to be an excellent predictor of lucid dreaming

ability in untrained participants (Schredl & Erlacher, 2004; Snyder & Gackenbach,

1988).

3. When pinpointing lucid dreams, it would be important to take into account that it

probably associates with phasic REM, as it has been proposed that lucid segments of

Page 41: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 36

Signal-Verified Lucid Dreams (SVLDs) REM periods seem to be characterized by

higher levels of physiological activation, when compared with the preceding segments

of non-lucid REM from the same REM periods (Brylowsky et al., 1989; LaBerge &

Gackenbach, 2000; Laberge et al., 1986).

4. The extreme horizontal eye movement methodology (L-R-L-R) inside the dream, to

produce SVLDs, may be problematic, as it is not easily executed, and may lead to

awakenings, as this eye movement inside the dream is thought to affect what the subject

is seeing in the dream, disrupting the visual imagery (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000).

5. It is posited that SVLDs may start from an ongoing REM non-lucid dream, as in dream-

initiated lucid dream (DILD), or directly from a short awakening, as in wake-initiated

lucid dream (WILD). On average, two-thirds of lucid dreams are thought to be DILDs

and one-third WILDs (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; Laberge et al., 1986), but this is a

preliminary figure, that needs expansion through further research.

6. Lucid dreams may end in four different ways: in awakening, by becoming a non-lucid

dream, by falling in NREM sleep, or in a false awakening (LaBerge & DeGracia, 2000).

Lucid dreaming is pointed out to be a learnable skill (LaBerge, 1980), and a variety of

techniques for the induction of lucid dreams have been developed. Stumbrys and colleagues

(Stumbrys, Erlacher, Schädlich, & Schredl, 2012), presented a taxonomy of lucid dream

induction methods taken from thirty-five studies, assessed for their methodological quality, and

systematically reviewed the evidence for the effectiveness of induction techniques. The authors

concluded that none of the techniques seem to induce lucid dreams reliably and consistently, but

some of them showed potential, according to their meta-analysis method. The expression lucid

dream induction refers to any means that aim to increase the frequency of lucid dreams, and the

authors defined three broad categories in an attempt to present an empirically based

classification of lucid dream induction techniques:

Page 42: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 37

1. Cognitive techniques that include all that is carried out to increase the likelihood of

achieving lucidity in a dream state by training cognitive skills, such as prospective

memory, self-reflection or intention. The techniques are: MILD (Mnemonic Induction of

Lucid Dreams), Reflection or Reality Testing, Intention, Tholey's Combined technique,

Auto-suggestion, Dream Re-Entry, Post-hypnotic Suggestion, and Alpha Feedback.

2. External stimulation techniques that include all types of stimuli possible to trigger

dream lucidity during REM sleep, by presenting a visual, auditory or tactile cue that

might be incorporated in the dream and recognized by the dreamer, such as: light

stimulus, acoustic stimulus, vibro-tactile stimulus, electro-tactile stimulus, and water

stimulus; or by specific activation, such as vestibular bodily stimulation.

3. Drug application methods that aim to alter cholinergic levels of the brain to enhance

lucidity in dreams, such as the application of an acetylcholine esterase inhibitor class

drug (Donepezil).

According to Stumbrys and colleagues (2012), in the realm of the cognitive techniques,

one promising induction method seems to be Tholey's combined technique, which has been

already tested in two studies with high methodological quality (Paulsson & Parker, 2006; Zadra,

Donderi, & Pihl, 1992) and incorporates elements of reflection, as in developing a reflective

frame of mind, intention, as in imagining being in a dream and recognizing it, and auto-

suggestion, as in suggesting oneself to become lucid when falling asleep. This technique seems

to increase significantly the frequency of lucid dreaming, especially for those previously

experienced with lucid dreams, and also to increase the frequency for the participants with no

previous experience, when compared with controls (Paulsson & Parker, 2006; Stumbrys et al.,

2012; Zadra et al., 1992).

Relating to external stimulation techniques, Stumbrys and colleagues (2012) point out

that some stimuli might be effective for lucid dream induction, such as light flashing on the

Page 43: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 38

eyes of a dreamer or an electrical impulse applied on the wrist during REM sleep. However, the

authors urge that these findings should be interpreted with caution, as the results were published

previously to the commercialization of the studied devices (DreamLight, DreamLink,

NovaDreamer, Dreamachine). Drug application methods seem to be intriguing, but very little

research has been conducted on the subject, thus more rigorous studies are needed (Sergio,

1988; Stumbrys et al., 2012).

When considering lucid dream induction research in general, one of the major issues is

to point out which valid criteria defines a successful induction. In sleep laboratory studies, the

valid criteria should be unambiguous predefined eye signals in the EOG register during REM

sleep, together with a dream report received immediately after awakening following signalling,

which would confirm lucidity and volitional eye signals (Stumbrys et al., 2012). The timing of

the dream report may be crucial, as to avoid impairing by sleep inertia, a transitional state

between sleep and wakefulness in which cognitive performance is pointed to be decreased

(Tassi & Muzet, 2000). These first-person subjective reports can be trusted, regarding

accurately conveying internal experiences in sleep, because the estimated time in dream report

correlates well with the time elapsed in REM sleep before the awakening (Windt, 2013). A very

illustrative case for this is the one presented by RBD, where muscle atonia is disrupted, and the

observed movements match the reported dream (Nir & Tononi, 2010).

Relating the probing of the neurobiology of dream contents, use of trans-cranial direct

current stimulation (tDCS) has been suggested, supported by the rationale that neuroimaging

studies solely allow correlative statements about the brain regions involved in a specific

behaviour (Karim, 2010; Noreika et al., 2010). TDCS allows for cathodal stimulation, which

hyper-polarizes neurons diminishing cerebral excitability, and anodal stimulation, which results

in increased cortical excitability (Nitsche & Paulus, 2000). Therefore, in what concerns the pre-

frontal hypothesis of lucid dreaming (Hobson, 2009a), it may be possible to apply anodal tDCS

Page 44: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 39

during REM sleep in order to activate the DLPFC and test if this external modulation of cortical

excitability induces lucidity, or increases lucid dream frequency, on the subject (Noreika et al.,

2010). Conversely, this can also be used vice versa, that is, inhibiting the DLPFC of veteran

lucid dreamers with cathodal stimulation, to discern if lucid dreaming would be prevented or

attenuated (Noreika et al., 2010). These possible findings could give causal support to the

correlative data obtained by neuroimaging studies, thus proving a predominant role of the

DLPFC in lucid dreaming (Karim, 2010; Noreika et al., 2010).

To summarize, researchers seem to be confident on experiments that couple first-person

reports with objective measures such as voluntary eye movements, which seems to be an

acceptable approach to access the phenomenology of the lucid dreaming brain. Meta-awareness,

probably one of the most conspicuous characteristic of human consciousness, is a subjective

experience and an entirely internal percept, but lucid dreams seem to present researchers with a

unique opportunity to combine introspection with a third person approach to the study of meta-

awareness in a state of known physiology, REM sleep dreaming (Voss, 2010). The scientific

investigation of lucid dreaming may hold the promise of returning psychology to its base in

introspection, now safeguarded with powerful third person tools, including quantitative rating

scales, quantitative EEG, and fMRI (Voss, 2010).

The next sub-section will deal with some of the most relevant contemporaneous

cognitive neuroscience research relevant for the purposes of this investigation.

2. Contemporary cognitive neuroscience research with lucid dreaming

In this sub-section, nine of the most recent and relevant studies on lucid dreaming, in

what pertains the aims of this theoretical investigation, will be analysed, in a more in-depth

fashion. These studies comprise a six-year span, from 2009 to 2015, except for the first one

presented, which is a pilot study from 2003, and has been included for its very interesting

Page 45: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 40

attempt on studying mind-body interactions with lucid dreaming.

Lucid dreaming may be a very useful tool indeed for the study of mind-body

interactions (Schredl, 2000). Erlacher, Schredl and LaBerge (2003) studied the physiological

responses to dreamed motor performance in REM lucid dreams, in a pilot study with a single

subject (N = 1) on EEG alpha band. The volunteer was a male (27 years old) with lucid

dreaming training, that spent three non-consecutive nights in the laboratory. The experimental

protocol included signalling the realization of being in a dream, by means of L-R-L-R pattern of

horizontal eye movements during the lucid dream. Afterwards, the subject would sit down in the

dream, again eye signal L-R-L-R to mark the beginning of left hand clenching, open and clench

the left hand four times, L-R-L-R eye signal to mark the end of left hand clenching and the

beginning of the right hand clenching, open and clench the right hand four times, and finally L-

R-L-R eye signal to mark the end of right hand clenching. Also, and as a reference sequence,

the subject was asked to count from zero to four, inside the dream, and to mark this sequence by

L-R-L-R eye signal in the beginning and in the end of the sequence. After the complete task, the

subject needed to awake himself up by means of focusing a spot in the dream, and afterwards

record the dream experience (Erlacher, Schredl, & LaBerge, 2003).

In this experiment (Erlacher et al., 2003), standard PSG were recorded, including 28-

channel EEG (expanded 10-20 system), horizontal and vertical EOG and chin EMG. According

to the authors, their preliminary results seem to point out to an association between hand

movements performed in a lucid dream, and corresponding activations of specific motor areas.

It was also pointed out a decrease in EEG alpha power over bilateral motor areas, while the

lucid dreamer executed left or right hand clenching, in contrast to dream counting (Erlacher et

al., 2003). The authors also point out that these results seem to support the hypothesis that the

same cortical areas are used for motor performance in waking and in lucid dreaming, and also

support the idea that dreamed movements are parallel to movements in waking (Erlacher et al.,

Page 46: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 41

2003). As such, it would seem the phenomenon of lucid dreaming provides an important

research paradigm for studying mind-body interactions (Erlacher et al., 2003). Nevertheless,

one must say that this is a preliminary study and, although very interesting, it has only one

participant. Replication with a large sample would be needed to further expand this knowledge.

In the same line, Voss and colleagues (2009) sought physiological correlates of lucid

dreaming, testing the hypothesis that the brain must change state if the mind changes state. The

rationale behind it stated that, if lucidity can be self-induced, similarly to a trainable skill

(LaBerge, 1980), this would constitute an opportunity to understand the brain basis of conscious

states, and how these states could be changed by voluntary intervention (Voss et al., 2009).

In this experiment (Voss et al., 2009), a group of twenty undergraduate psychology

students from Bonn University (Germany) took part in lucidity training sessions, on a weekly

basis. The authors do not specify what kind of lucidity training these subjects underwent.

Nevertheless, after four months of regular week training, six subjects claimed to be lucid more

than three times per week. Again, the authors do not specify how these claims were made, or

how they were verified. These six participants (N = 6) were invited to Frankfurt University's

sleep laboratory (Germany). Written consent for participation was given, and a fifty Euro

(€50,00) compensation per night was paid to each participant. It would seem that standard PSG

were recorded, with scalp EEG electrodes (placed at 19 positions; 10-20 system), EOG (taken

from the outer canthi of both eyes and supra-orbitally to the left eye), and EMG at the chin.

EEG was recorded for two to five nights per subject (Voss et al., 2009).

According to the authors (Voss et al., 2009), from the six subjects, three (one male aged

22 and two females aged 23 years) were each able to become lucid once in the laboratory

setting. These three recorded lucidity episodes seem to have occurred spontaneously, pointed by

the authors as a result of auto-suggestion. Subjects were trained to report lucidity by a two-set

of L-R-L-R horizontal eye movements, separated by a brief pause, and were also asked to

Page 47: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 42

repeat this pattern several times during the lucid dream. Lucidity was confirmed through

subjective free reports upon awakening (Voss et al., 2009).

The authors point out (Voss et al., 2009) that EEG power, averaged across all electrodes

as a function of frequency, seems to be REM-like in lucid dreaming in lower frequencies; and

appears to rise above REM sleep at higher frequencies, mainly in gamma waves, starting around

28 Hz and peaking at 40 Hz (Voss et al., 2009). These gamma rhythms, or oscillations in the

gamma frequency band of the EEG, are thought to be involved in the binding of neural events

that are spatially separated but temporally correlated, resulting in a unified perceptual

experience (Castro, Falconi, Chase, & Torterolo, 2013; Fries, Nikolić, & Singer, 2007;

Niedermeyer & Da Silva, 2004). This mechanism seems to explain the extensive interactions

between various regions of the cerebral cortex during cognitive processes or motor functions

(Castro, et al., 2013; Fries et al., 2007; Niedermeyer & Da Silva, 2004). The extent of these

interactions can be examined by means of a mathematical algorithm called “coherence”, which

reflects the strength of functional interactions between cortical areas (Castro, et al., 2013; Fries

et al., 2007; Niedermeyer & Da Silva, 2004).

Taking the authors' words (Voss et al., 2009), higher delta and theta activity and lower

alpha power appear to be characteristic of lucid dreaming, and this seems to evince that this

phenomenon occurs in a REM sleep state. The increase in higher frequency power occurring

during lucid dreaming compared to REM sleep apparently shows the difference between both

states, and seems to highlight lucid dreaming as a unique state of sleep (Voss et al., 2009).

Moreover, the findings on power seem to suggest that lucidity may be occurring in a hybrid

state, as the authors put it, with some features of REM sleep, as in delta and theta waves, and

some of waking, as in gamma waves. Likewise, the data obtained is claimed to show lucid

dreaming as a hybrid state of consciousness, with definable and measurable differences from

waking and from REM sleep, mainly in frontal areas of the brain (Voss et al., 2009). Also, the

Page 48: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 43

frontal and frontolateral regions of the brain seem to have a central role in gaining lucid insight

into the dream state and in gaining volitional control (Voss et al., 2009).

This study is commonly presented as a breakthrough, and cited many times (times cited:

83 from Web of Science core collection in June 2015) on subsequent literature in lucid

dreaming, but a major drawback is that the findings are build upon three lucid dreams, each

from one of three subjects. This may be a weak sample from which to retrieve so many

conclusions, and replication studies are needed.

In another study, Voss and colleagues (Voss, Frenzel, Koppehele-Gossel, & Hobson,

2012) proposed a link between the natural occurrence of lucid dreaming and brain maturation.

This link is primarily based on previous reports of an inverse relationship between age and

frequency of lucid dreaming, higher age seems to be linked with less frequency (this link is also

mentioned in Part II, section 2 of this thesis) (Blackmore, 1984; Schredl & Erlacher, 2004,

2011; Watson, 2001). This study sample had six hundred and ninety-four participants (N = 694;

346 female, 348 male; age range: 6 to 19 years old), all students from German primary and

secondary schools. The sample was representative for secondary school type attendance in

Germany (Voss et al., 2012).

As mentioned by Voss and colleagues (2012), the procedure consisted of one-on-one

interviews conducted by trained graduate students of the Psychology Department of Bonn

University (Germany). The participants were provided with a detailed account of lucid

dreaming, and ample time was given to make sure they understood the concept. Afterwards,

subjects were interviewed with a protocol containing three parts: four sleep-related questions,

seven dream-related questions, and two final questions to test their suggestibility. If a lucid

dream was reported, the interviewer would request an example of such a dream plot, in the

narrative form (Voss et al., 2012; please refer to Appendix A for the interview questions).

Relating to lucidity frequency, 51,9% (N = 360) of the total participating students

Page 49: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 44

reported having had at least one lucid episode in their life. From these, only 25% (N = 91) said

to experience a lucid dream once a month or more (Voss et al., 2012). The results are said to

show that lucid dreaming appears to be quite pronounced in young children, and its incidence

rate appears to drop at about sixteen (Voss et al., 2012). With this in mind, the authors presented

preliminary evidence that the state of lucid dreaming may be said to correlate with brain

maturation during childhood and early adolescence (Voss et al., 2012). Reported lucidity seems

to occur in greater number in the young and in those who are intellectually more capable (Voss

et al., 2012).

In addition, controlling the dream plot is mentioned to occur in only one third of the

lucid dreams, which seems to point out that plot control may not be as strong a defining

characteristic of lucid dreaming as lucid insight. Students claiming to have plot control also

reported a higher lucid dreaming frequency (Voss et al., 2012). The authors hypothesize that

lucidity could be a transient state during brain maturation, that is normally lost in adulthood but

still accessible through training (Voss et al., 2012). The frequency of dream recall seems to be

significantly correlated with frequency of lucid dreams, suggesting that the ability to remember

one's dreams facilitates lucid dreaming or the memory of it (Voss et al., 2012), in line with

previously mentioned by Schredl and Erlacher in 2011 (refer to Part II, sub-section 2 for

details).

Also in 2012, Dresler and colleagues presented a study that aimed to investigate the

neural correlates of lucid dreaming by contrasting lucid versus non-lucid REM sleep, using a

combined EEG-fMRI approach. The sample for this study was comprised by four experienced

lucid dreamers (N = 4; all male; age range: 27 to 32 years), that had been recruited through

internet advertisements. All participants were screened for medical conditions contrary to study

aims, gave written consent for participation, and were paid for participating (the amount is not

revealed). Subjects slept in a MRI scanner with simultaneous PSG monitoring during two to six

Page 50: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 45

nights each (Dresler at al., 2012).

The experiment protocol in this study (Dresler et al., 2012) indicated that, immediately

after achieving lucidity in their dream, the subjects should move their eyes L-R-L-R, and

afterwards clench their left hand for about ten seconds, give the L-R-L-R signal again, clench

the right hand, and so on, as long as possible. The fMRI epochs showing lucid dreaming were

defined by the prearranged L-R-L-R signals in the EOG. Lucidity was assumed to start with the

first L-R-L-R signal, indicating also the onset of the dreamed hand movements. All participants

reported experiencing lucidity at least once, but only one subject had two SVLDs with four or

more consecutive sets of L-R-L-R signals (Dresler et al., 2012).

As in a previous study (Voss et al., 2009), Dresler and colleagues' (2012) experiment is

said to have observed increased activity in the right DLPFC during lucid dreaming. This

specific area is stated to associate with self-focused meta-cognitive evaluation (Schmitz,

Kawahara-Baccus, & Johnson, 2004). Other areas found to be strongly activated, when

compared with non-lucid REM sleep, were the precuneus, the bilateral cuneus, the parietal

lobes, and the pre-frontal and occipito-temporal cortices (Dresler et al., 2012).

The strongest increase in activation was reported to be in the precuneus (Dresler et al.,

2012), a brain region thought to be involved in self-referential processing, particularly first-

person perspective and experience of agency (Cavanna & Trimble, 2006). Interesting enough,

the bilateral cuneus and the occipito-temporal cortices are pointed as part of the ventral stream

of visual processing, which in turn appears to participate in several aspects of conscious

awareness in visual perception (Rees et al., 2002). Moreover, the activation of the DLPFC in

association with the parietal lobes might reflect working memory demands related to task

performance (Smith & Jonides, 1998), as in the intentional movement of the eyes and clenching

of the hands in the SVLDs (Dresler et al., 2012).

In neuroanatomical terms, all the regions found to activate in Dresler and colleagues

Page 51: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 46

(2012) experiment belong to the neocortex, which is an exclusively mammalian part of the

cortex, and is larger and more developed in humans than in other primates (Thompson, 2015b).

These areas seem to form an intentional control system for cognition and decision making - a

frontoparietal control system -, which links brain networks that support the ability to pay

attention to the outside world with other networks related to long term memory, and with the

ability to think about oneself, in a way as to remember one's personal past and project oneself

into an anticipated future (Vicent, Kahn, Snyder, Raichle, & Buckner, 2008; Thompson, 2015b).

All in all, Dresler and colleagues (2012) experiment is a very interesting one indeed, in

the way it places the lucid dream under the MRI machine, so to speak. The conundrum here is

that, starting from a very small sample the study became a case study in the end, and can only

be used, at best, as preliminary evidence with functional data.

In 2013, Stumbrys and colleagues (Stumbrys, Erlacher, & Schredl, 2013) applied tDCS

in an attempt to manipulate the activation of the DLPFC during REM sleep, with the aim of

increasing dream lucidity. This was based on the rationale that increased brain activity over

frontal regions during REM sleep is thought to associate with lucidity in dreams (Dresler et al.,

2012; Voss et al., 2009). The intention was to go beyond the correlational data, experimentally

manipulating the activation of the pre-frontal brain cortex, thus testing the neurobiological basis

of dream lucidity (Karim, 2010; Noreika et al., 2010; Stumbrys et al., 2013).

As previously specified (please refer to Part III, sub-section 1), tDCS involves the

uninterrupted administration of weak currents (1mA) through a pair of surface electrodes,

cathode and anode, affixed to the scalp (Nitsche & Paulus, 2000), and is based on the notion

that cerebral excitability diminishes with cathodal stimulation, which is thought to hyper-

polarize neurons, whereas anodal stimulation increases cortical excitability (Nitsche et al.,

2008). This method, unlike trans-cranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), is indicated as not

inducing auditory artefacts, and the voltage needed to hold the current constant decreases after a

Page 52: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 47

short time, becoming sub-threshold for evoking peripheral sensations that could otherwise

interfere, or even awaken, the participant (Noreika et al., 2010).

In Stumbrys and colleagues study (2013), the final sample was composed by nineteen

subjects (N = 19; 13 female and 6 male; age range: 21 to 33 years), recruited through e-mail

advertisements sent to psychology students and known lucid dreamers. The inclusion criteria

for this study were: one or more recalled dreams a week (measured on a 7-point scale), good

sleep, and no serious health problems. Later on, a fourth criterion was added in the form of an

eight-point scale, for the purpose of estimating the frequency of lucid dreaming. The dream

recall frequency median value reported was: several times a week; and the lucid dream

frequency median value reported was: about once a month (Stumbrys et al., 2013). Eleven

subjects were considered frequent lucid dreamers, as the authors took into account Snyder and

Gackenbach (1988) terminology (lucid dream ≥ once a month = frequent lucid dreamer). All

subjects were paid for their participation (the amount is not revealed), and gave written consent

(Stumbrys et al., 2013).

In this study, the procedure required for the participants to spend three consecutive

nights in the sleep laboratory with uninterrupted PSG recording (Stumbrys et al., 2013). The

first night was used for adaptation, screening for sleep disorders, and screening for sensitivity to

the tDCS. In that night, the PSG included EEG, EOG, submental and leg EMG,

electrocardiogram (ECG), and four respiration measures. For the other two subsequent nights,

the experimental ones, the PSG recordings included EEG, EOG, submental EMG, and ECG. In

these two experimental nights, subjects received tDCS stimulation in one, and sham stimulation

on another, in a randomized and counter-balanced order (Stumbrys et al., 2013). Two pairs of

conductive rubber electrodes, inside saline soaked sponges, were used to deliver the

stimulation. The anodes were applied to the DLPFC, and the cathodes were applied to the

supraclavicular area. The lucid dreaming signalling sequence consisted of L-R-L-R-L-R eye

Page 53: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 48

movements, and was to be repeated at a rate of once a minute, while lucidity lasted (Stumbrys

et al., 2013).

Furthermore, upon awakening, a detailed dream report was firstly elicited, seconded by

a self-evaluation of the intensity of positive and negative emotions during the dream, by means

of two four-point scales. Thirdly, participants were to answer two questionnaires verbally: the

Metacognitive, Affective, Cognitive Experience questionnaire (MACE; Kahan & LaBerge,

2011), which contains seven items, scored on a five-point scale, to assess different types of

meta-cognitive activities; and the Dream Lucidity Questionnaire (DLQ; Stumbrys et al., 2013),

which encloses twelve items, scored on a five-point scale, and was constructed for this

experiment by the authors, “to measure different aspects of lucidity within dreams” (Stumbrys

et al., 2013, p. 1216), evaluating different types of awareness, control and remembrance. The

dream reports were later on scored by an external judge, for lucidity and bizarreness. A three-

point scale was used to evaluate the lucidity of the dream, and dream bizarreness was assessed

by a four-point scale (Stumbrys et al., 2013).

The study conducted by Stumbrys and colleagues (2013) yielded a total of one hundred

and nine REM awakenings, but it would seem that the stimulation had disruptive effects in

REM sleep, leading to awakenings when tDCS was applied. Only one SVLD was recorded

during REM sleep, in a tDCS night, in which there were two signals, after three and four

minutes since the beginning of the tDCS stimulation (Stumbrys et al., 2013). In this particular

situation, the subject awakened by herself, after the second eye-signalling, during the third

REM period of the night. Early in the same night, the subject also signalled, but this time from

NREM N2 sleep, which is very intriguing indeed (Stumbrys et al., 2013).

According to the authors (Stumbrys et al., 2013), dream reports were significantly

longer, and rated as more lucid, on tDCS nights than on sham stimulation nights. Also, longer

dream reports were pointed to have more externally-focused meta-cognition. Relating judge

Page 54: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 49

scoring, seven dream reports were rated as having clear indications of lucidity, four from tDCS

nights, and three from sham nights (Stumbrys et al., 2013). The authors argue that, taking into

account the participants self-rating of dreams, the subjective experiences of dreaming were

affected by tDCS stimulation delivery over the DLPFC during REM sleep, resulting in

increased dream lucidity, thus confirming the initial hypothesis (Stumbrys et al., 2013). As such,

the authors conclude that the study provides preliminary empirical support for the causal

involvement of the DLPFC in lucid dreaming, at least on the neurophysiological level

(Stumbrys et al., 2013).

Notwithstanding, the authors also refer that tDCS stimulation effects were not very

strong, and a post-hoc analysis shown pronounced effects only in frequent lucid dreamers

(Stumbrys et al., 2013). It is interesting to point out, however, that these subjects reported

increased awareness that their physical body was asleep, increased awareness that the dream

objects and characters were not real, and also an overall lucidity (Stumbrys et al., 2013).

However, it would seem that infrequent and non-lucid dreamers did not report effects of

increased lucidity, in tDCS stimulation nights (Stumbrys et al., 2013). The authors have done a

very good analysis on the reasons for this, pointing out several aspects, from which it may be

interesting to mention the possibility that activation of a wider network of different brain areas

may be needed to achieve lucidity in dreams (Stumbrys et al., 2013), as in the frontoparietal

control system mentioned earlier. Other possibility lies in that tDCS may have stimulated other

areas besides DLPFC (Stumbrys et al., 2013). In any way, the authors conclude that tDCS might

not be a promising tool for lucid dream induction on a practical level (Stumbrys et al., 2013).

Another relevant study, from Voss and colleagues (Voss, Schermelleh-Engel, Windt,

Frenzel, & Hobson, 2013), set out to develop the Lucidity and Consciousness in Dreams

(LuCiD) scale, in an attempt to better assess the major and minor determinants of dream

lucidity, through the comparison between lucid and non-lucid dreams from REM sleep.

Page 55: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 50

According to the authors, the LuCiD scale is said to be based in theoretical considerations and

empirical observations, analysed and validated through factor-analysis. The authors also claim

that this scale was the first empirically-based approach to measure and define consciousness in

different types of dreams, and its item construction was aimed at identifying differences

between higher and lower levels of consciousness in dreams (Voss & Hobson, 2015; Voss et al.,

2013).

The final version of this scale includes twenty-eight items or statements (please refer to

Appendix B for a full view of LuCiD scale final version), each followed by a six-point scale (0

= strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree). According to the factor analysis performed by the

authors, non-lucid and lucid dream consciousness is said to be describable using the following

eight elements (Voss & Hobson, 2015; Voss et al., 2013; Voss & Voss, 2014):

1. Insight – lucid insight into the fact that what one is currently experiencing is not real,

but is only a dream (scale items: 1, 3, 8, 9, 16, 19).

2. Control – rule over thought and actions in dreams, as in volition but also as in control of

dream plot (scale items: 4, 6, 10, 14, 23).

3. Thought – about other dream characters (scale items: 5, 12, 22).

4. Realism – a sense of perceptual realism related to the similarity between emotions,

thoughts, and events in lucid dreaming and wakefulness, judged after awakening from

the dream (scale items: 7, 17, 20).

5. Memory - access to elements of waking life memory in the dream (scale items: 2, 13,

18, 24).

6. Dissociation – similar to taking a third person perspective (scale items: 11, 15, 21).

7. Negative emotion (scale items: 26, 28).

8. Positive emotion (scale items: 25, 27).

According to the authors (Voss et al., 2013), the scale was constructed through three

Page 56: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 51

main steps: firstly, fifty theory-based items were tested in a first survey; secondly, from those

fifty items, twenty-four were selected through factor-analysis as suited to distinguish between

lucid and non-lucid dreams, plus four new items on emotion were added; thirdly, a new

confirmatory factor analysis was done, combined with a data assessment for extensive scale

validation (Voss et al., 2013). For this purpose, two surveys were done, advertised among

students at Bonn University (Germany), and also to participants of a lucid dream group of the

same University. On the first survey, relating to the first step, one-hundred and fifty-eight dream

reports were used, mainly from paper and pencil questionnaires, but also from online. These

questionnaires were answered by one-hundred and forty-nine participants (N = 149; 118

females and 31 males; mean age = 24,4 ± 0,7 years). Fifty of these dream reports were

reportedly lucid, and one-hundred and eight non-lucid (Voss et al., 2013), but the authors do not

specify how this assessment was made. The procedure relied on asking for a recent single

dream report, and afterwards answer all questions relating to the dream.

The second survey was done with the final version of the LuCiD scale, for validation

purposes. In this survey, one-hundred and fifty-one dream reports were elicited (117 from

laboratory setting and 34 from paper and pencil answers). It would seem that eighty of these

reports come from females, and seventy-one from males. (Voss et al., 2013). The authors do not

state clearly how many participants this second survey had, which is rather confusing. Also, the

authors do not specify how many of the one-hundred and fifty-one dreams from the second

survey were lucid or non-lucid, as it had been done for the first survey. According to Voss and

colleagues (2013), the procedure for the dream reports from paper and pencil was the same as in

the first survey. For the dream reports in the laboratory setting, awakenings from REM sleep

were done. Subjects arrived at 9 p.m. and were instrumented for regular PSG. Afterwards, the

LuCiD scale items were read out by an experimenter, and time was allowed for full

comprehension. REM sleep awakenings started at 3 p.m., and were made after five minutes of

Page 57: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 52

monitored REM sleep. After the dream report, an experimenter read out the questionnaire items,

and marked the answers. Each subject spent up to three non-consecutive nights at the

laboratory, gave informed consent, and received a fifty Euro (€50,00) compensation for each

night (Voss et al., 2013).

In accordance to Voss and colleagues (2013), this study indicates that three of the LuCiD

scale elements are substantially increased in lucid dreaming: insight into the fact that one is

currently dreaming (Insight), control over the dream plot (Control), and dissociation similar to

taking a third-person perspective (Dissociation). Figure 1 (below) shows the mean scores for

non-lucid and lucid dreams, in the eight elements of the LuCiD scale (Voss et al., 2013). One

can only have an approximated idea of the mean scores, since the authors do not provide the

exact numbers in the text of the study.

In close relation to the previously analysed study, Voss and colleagues (2014) attempted

to discern if lucid dreaming is dependent of the presence of gamma activity, as a necessary

condition, or if it can be elicited by other causal ways, such as stimulation with other

frequencies, as a causal enabling condition. In a simpler manner: if gamma activity triggers

Page 58: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 53

lucid dreaming or if lucid dreaming triggers gamma activity. The authors used trans-cranial

alternating current stimulation (tACS), which is said to produce no acoustic noise and no tactile

sensations, to stimulate the fronto-temporal region of the human brain, at various frequencies.

In this study, conducted by Voss and colleagues (2014), twenty-seven healthy volunteers

(N = 27, 15 female and 12 male, age range: 18 to 26 years), inexperienced in lucid dreaming,

screened for psychiatric and sleep disorders, and free of central nervous system acting

medication, were selected as participants. Up to four nights were spent by the subjects at the

sleep laboratory of the Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, at the University Medical

Centre Göttingen (Germany). Throughout all nights, PSG recordings were done with EEG (22

channels), EOG, and submental EMG. The procedure for this experiment was as follows: the

subjects were let to sleep up until 3 a.m. and, from that moment on, tACS was fronto-

temporally applied for thirty seconds, if a period between two to three minutes of uninterrupted,

arousal-free REM sleep occurred. This would happen at a frequency of either 2, 6, 12, 25, 40,

70 or 100 Hz (stabilized at better than ±0,01 Hz) or sham. To avoid order effects, the applied

stimulation frequencies were counterbalanced across subjects and across nights (1-4).

Participants were awakened five to ten seconds after stimulation, and were asked to provide a

full dream report, and afterwards to rate the twenty-eight items of the LuCiD scale. The scale

items were read to subjects by the experimenters.

Voss and colleagues (2014) report that the EEG underwent quantitative analysis for all

stimulation conditions and sham, and REM-like sleep persisted throughout the procedure

phases, up until awakening, documented by unchanged REMs and muscle atonia (PSG). It

would seem that activity in the lower gamma frequency band increased during stimulation with

40 Hz (mean increase between 37-43 Hz = 28%), and also during stimulation with 25 Hz (mean

increase between 22-28 Hz = 12%), although in a lesser degree. Apparently, stimulation in

lower (2, 6, 12 Hz) or higher frequencies (70 or 100 Hz), or sham, did not produce an activity

Page 59: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 54

increase. This lead the authors to speculate that REM sleep was altered through the stimulation

(40 Hz and 25 Hz frequencies), producing a state change similar to the state of lucid dreaming.

The authors also argue that, if one takes into account the subjective ratings of lucidity,

lucid dreams were most prominent during stimulation with 25 Hz (58%) and with 40 Hz (77%).

Moreover, it is also argued that increases in 40 Hz band activity at fronto-temporal sites

correlate significantly with mean scores on the factors Insight and Dissociation, of the LuCiD

scale (Voss et al., 2014; please refer to Figure 2 below). There seems to be a problem here,

because if one compares the mean score for the element Insight of the LuCiD scale in the

previous study (Insight mean score = ~ 3,3; Voss et al., 2013; please refer to Figure 1), with the

mean score for the same element in this study (Insight mean score = ~ 0,6; Voss et al., 2014;

please refer to Figure 2), there is clearly a difference in the criterion used in both studies.

To sum up, Voss and colleagues (2014) propose that current stimulation in the lower

Page 60: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 55

gamma band during REM sleep has an influence in ongoing brain activity, inducing meta-

awareness in dreams. Lucid dream state consciousness seems to be related to synchronous

oscillations around 25 and 40 Hz, suggesting that lower gamma-band activity correlates with

elevated meta-awareness. According to the authors, these results may provide the first causal

evidence of frequency-specific cortical oscillations in humans induced by tACS, also appearing

to demonstrate altered conscious awareness as a direct consequence of induced gamma-band

oscillations during sleep (Voss et al., 2014). It would seem that stimulating the brain in the

gamma frequency range, which is also the frequency band most often associated with conscious

awareness in the awake state, facilitates the ability to engage in lucid dreaming during REM

sleep (Payne, 2014).

Voss and colleagues (2014) experiment is pointed out in the literature (Payne, 2014) as

another major breakthrough in lucid dream and consciousness research, and has the backing of

none other than Nature Neuroscience journal. Nonetheless, the eliciting of lucid dreams is done

through the LuCiD scale, with a different criterion, and are not SVLDs. This may be a problem,

as the experimenters rely only on a subjective report, which is then rated on the LuCiD scale

with a different criterion than the one used when validating the scale, without having it

physiologically verified. These may be very interesting preliminary results indeed, but these

results are also in want of replication, probably with the inclusion of SVLDs. Lower gamma

band stimulation of the fronto-temporal region of the human brain may enhance neuronal

synchronization and set the stage for lucidity in dreams, but the main actor is still needed.

Along the same vein, Dresler and colleagues (2014a) studied different aspects of

volition during non-lucid dreaming, lucid dreaming, and wakefulness, through the use of an

adapted version of the Volitional Components Questionnaire (VCQ) in lucid dreamers. The

authors hypothesized that experienced volition would be generally higher in both wakefulness

and lucid dreaming compared to non-lucid dreaming. For this study, ten healthy subjects (N =

Page 61: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 56

10; 5 female and 5 male; age range: 19 to 47 years) were recruited at the University of Munich

(Germany), or from a database of volunteers of the Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, also in

Munich. All subjects were said to be experienced lucid dreamers (reported mean frequency =

1,9 ± 0,7 lucid dreams per week), and the authors verified lucid dreaming ability by SVLDs in a

sleep laboratory with PSG recordings for five subjects, and assessed by self-report on the other

five subjects (Dresler et al., 2014a).

For measuring volition in different states of consciousness, the authors adapted the

German short version of the VCQ (SelbststeuerungsInventar, SSI-K3; Dresler et al., 2014a).

The VCQ is an instrument that measures different aspects of volitional competence, with the

purpose of assessing the subjective experience of volitional components supporting central

coordination of goal-maintenance and self-maintenance (Kuhl and Fuhrmann, 1998; Dresler et

al., 2014a). The short version of the VCQ includes thirteen sub-scales with four items each, and

subjects are asked to rate the extent to which the item applies to themselves on a four-point

scale (1 = not at all to 4 = wholly). From the thirteen sub-scales of the short version, the authors

chose to use six: self-determination, planning ability, intention enactment, powers of

concentration, self-access, and integration. An overall score consisting of the mean of all six

sub-scales was calculated by the authors, as a measure for the general experience of volitional

capacity (Dresler et al., 2014a).

The procedure for this experiment (Dresler et al., 2014a) consisted on the completion of

the questionnaire at least once for the three states of consciousness: in the morning after

awakening from a non-lucid dream, in the morning after awakening from a lucid dream, and

before going to bed in the evening, after a normal day of wakefulness. This was to be done by

the subject through the rating of the general experience during the respective state, using the

preceding dream or day as a reminder thereof. Non-lucid dreaming state yielded twenty

questionnaires from five different subjects (5s → 20q), lucid dreaming state yielded fourteen

Page 62: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 57

questionnaires from four different subjects (4s → 14q), and wakefulness state yielded fifteen

questionnaires from four different subjects (4s → 15q). For those participants who completed

the questionnaire more than once for one of the three states of consciousness, the authors used

the mean score of the given state for further analysis (Dresler et al., 2014a).

Dresler and colleagues (2014a) report that, after inferential statistical analysis of the

VCQ overall score, volition was strongly experienced by the participants during wakefulness

and lucid dreaming, but considerably less so during non-lucid dreaming. The analysis of the six

sub-scales is reported to reveal a significantly more pronounced self-determination during both

wakefulness and lucid dreaming states, than in non-lucid dreaming state. This sub-scale is

pointed to be the most prototypical volitional component, and inquires to what degree the

subject experiences the ability to act freely according to his will (Dresler et al., 2014a). This

results seems to be in line with the overall score, thus the authors point out this confirms the

study hypothesis that volition is more marked during both wakefulness and lucid dreaming

states, than in non-lucid dreaming state (Dresler et al., 2014a).

This would seem a very interesting study, not for the fact that subjects are reporting their

consciousness states outside a sleep laboratory, which in itself has its strengths and weaknesses.

For once, the subject is in his own natural environment, not surrounded by machinery and

plugged with equipment, thus going through the states in a natural way. On the other hand, the

questionnaires were completed in the morning, in the non-lucid and lucid dreaming state cases,

which may have biased the reports, since the awakening may not have happened immediately

after the occurrence of the dream. Nevertheless, the authors point out this and some other

limitations in the study (Dresler et al., 2014a), and this may be taken as preliminary evidence

for the similarity of the volitional component marked presence, in the wakefulness and lucid

dreaming states.

One of the most recent studies, conducted by Filevich and colleagues (2015), procured

Page 63: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 58

relationships between the neural correlates of lucid dreaming and meta-cognition. In particular,

if pre-frontal gray matter volume positively associated with experienced dream lucidity, and if

brain areas associated with high lucidity showed increased blood oxygen level-dependent

(BOLD) activity during meta-cognitive monitoring. The authors report that the sample

consisted on sixty-nine healthy participants (N = 69; 34 female and 35 male; age range: 18-37

years), recruited from a database by telephone contact. In this recruiting contact there was no

mention of lucid dreaming. From the initial sample, sixty-three participants were included in the

final analyses. None of them had had history of psychiatric illness in the previous tear, and were

all right-handed (Filevich et al., 2015).

The procedure for this experiment, conducted by Filevich and colleagues (2015),

consisted on two phases. In the first phase, to assess their meta-cognitive monitoring, subjects

went through a thought-monitoring task, with two eleven minute runs, concomitant with MRI

scanning. Afterwards, subjects were apprised about lucid dreams, and received a link to an

online questionnaire. For the second phase, to assess their in-dream lucidity in the week

immediately after the scanning, the participants were instructed to complete the online survey

once every morning, recalling their most recent and most lucid dream from the previous night.

The survey included the following components: one question to determine dream recall

frequency (6-point scale), one question about the frequency of lucid dreams (8-point scale;

Schredl & Erlacher, 2004), the LuCiD scale (Voss et al., 2013), computed as the simple sum of

all items, and subtracted from the elements Negative Emotion and Realism, a rumination and

self-reflection questionnaire, two questionnaires on public and private self-consciousness, and a

visual imagery scale (Filevich et al., 2015).

The authors (Filevich et al., 2015) analyses were based on a combination of the LuCiD

scale and frequency scores, established on the assumption that this composite score would

assess both quality and quantity of lucid dream capability, and denominated the resulting

Page 64: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 59

measure trait-lucidity. Subjects were then median split into two groups (N = 31 for each group),

high and low lucidity, according to score. A brain structure comparison, using voxel-based

morphometry (VBM), was then made. Participants placed in the high-lucidity group exhibited

greater grey matter volume in the frontopolar cortex, Brodmann's area (BA) 9/10, when

compared with those in the low-lucidity group. In addition, during thought monitoring, the BA

9/10 regions identified through structural analyses revealed increases in BOLD signal in both

groups, but more strongly in the high-lucidity group. The authors (Filevich et al., 2015) claim

that the results reveal shared neural systems between lucid dreaming and meta-cognitive

function, particularly in thought monitoring. Moreover, the results seem to suggest BA 10 as a

candidate mediator of meta-cognition, and lucid dreaming a specific form of meta-cognition

that uses the same neural mechanisms as thought monitoring.

Several authors (Filevich et al., 2015; Hobson & Voss, 2011; Kahan & LaBerge, 1994)

suggest that, during REM sleep, deactivations of the DLPFC and frontopolar cortex underlie

decreases in meta-cognitive monitoring, whereas in lucid dreaming, subjects regain reflective

capabilities and become meta-cognitively aware of their current state of consciousness. Direct

relationship between the neural basis of lucid dreaming and meta-cognition is often assumed,

but had not been tested yet (Noreika et al., 2010). Specifically, the connection between meta-

cognitive processes in lucid dreaming, and meta-cognitive variability during wakefulness, in

what concerns inter-individual variability in frontopolar cortex neuroanatomy (Filevich et al.,

2015).

In summary, the line of research presented here is a mere fraction of the experiments on

lucid dreaming done until now. These specific studies relate directly to the use of lucid

dreaming for obtaining a deeper knowledge of the neural processes pertaining consciousness as

a state. The overall results seem to point out lucid dreaming as a way to research mind-body

interactions (Erlacher et al., 2003, 2014), and may even considered it a new state of

Page 65: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 60

consciousness, with features of REM sleep and wakefulness (Voss, et al., 2009). A possibility of

correlation with brain maturation is suggested (Voss et al., 2012), and an association with meta-

awareness and meta-cognition appears to be strengthened (Dresler et al., 2012, 2014a; Filevich

et al., 2015; Hobson & Voss, 2011; Voss et al., 2013). Furthermore, it may be possible to induce

meta-awareness, or insight into the fact that one is dreaming, during REM sleep (Voss et al.,

2014), although this last suggestion is in urgent need of a replication.

In the next section, the approaches and connections between lucid dreaming and

consciousness are examined, and also the theoretical foundations and inter-relations between

them.

Page 66: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 61

Part IV - Lucid dreaming and consciousness

1. Bridges between lucid dreaming and consciousness

One can say that consciousness, as initially defined in this investigation (please refer to

Part I, sub-section 1), may be considered state dependent, taking into account how it is reported

to undergo alterations in parallel with sleep changes in the brain. Therefore, one can also say

that consciousness seems to change its intensity and character as the brain changes state during

the sleep-wake cycle. As such, it would now seem clear that the kind of consciousness one

experiences is a function of the state of the brain (Hobson, 2005).

With this said, one approach to scientifically study consciousness may rely on tracking

changes in the brain and changes in the mind simultaneously, and afterwards to map back and

forth between the two domains (Hobson, 2005). This may be done with the use of first-person

data, as it is very difficult to study subjectivity without it, and consciousness is a subjective

state. Scientific laboratory studies of human consciousness using lucid dreams seem to be a

straightforward way to do this, as long as laboratory lucid dreaming is rigorously defined as

phasic REM sleep, and temporal dream recollections consistent with eye movement signals are

counted for analysis (Brylowski, 2010), so as to produce SVLDs.

In Hobson's (2009a) opinion, lucid dreaming is a state of consciousness in the middle of

waking and non-lucid dreaming, and there is empirical evidence for it, pointing out Wehrle and

colleagues (2007) and Voss and colleagues (2009) studies. As mentioned before (Part II, sub-

section 1, and Part III, sub-section 2 respectively), the former study gives preliminary evidence

for a thalamocortical network specific for phasic REM periods (Wehrle et al., 2007), and the

later study provides preliminary evidence that lucid dreaming appears to have EEG power and

coherence profiles that are significantly different from non-lucid dreaming (Voss et al., 2009).

With the measurement of the temporal correlation between frontal and occipital EEG patterns, it

Page 67: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 62

would seem that subjects have more EEG coherence when lucid than when not, being the EEG

coherence considered the degree to which the brain waves are synchronized across regions

(Hobson, 2009b; Voss et al., 2009).

Therefore, lucid dreaming is posited to present itself with more 40 Hz power than non-

lucid dreaming, especially in frontal regions, which are thought to associate with working

memory, self-awareness and volition, the characteristics that are normally absent in non-lucid

dreaming, and seem to be needed for a dream to become lucid (Voss et al., 2009). This kind of

high-frequency EEG activity, both 40 Hz power and fronto-occipital coherence, may represent

the synchronization of cortical neuronal activity that is pointed out to be necessary to effect the

temporal binding believed to be essential to waking consciousness (Hobson, 2009a, 2009b;

Singer, 2001).

The finding of a correlation between the three conscious states - REM dream, lucid

dream, and waking - and 40 Hz power seems to suggest that 40 Hz may be a substrate of

consciousness (Singer, 2014). Cognitive contents need to be selected by attentional mechanisms

to be consciously processed during wakefulness, and attentional selection is said to happen

through the synchronization of neuronal responses in the gamma frequency range (Fries,

Reynolds, Rorie, & Desimone, 2001; Lima, Singer, & Neuenschwander, 2011).

Taking into account the notion of lucid dreaming as an indication that waking and

dreaming can coexist, it would seem that different regions of the same brain underlie waking

and dreaming. When subjects become lucid they appear to increase their self-awareness as the

40 Hz power of the frontal EEG increases (Voss et al., 2009), and this seems to connect with

Singer (2014) hypothesis that waking consciousness results from high frequency

synchronization of the forebrain. Accordingly, dream lucidity could be a by-product of

increased 40 Hz activation in REM sleep (Hobson, 2014).

Notwithstanding, Hobson (2009a) also points out lucid dreaming as an example of

Page 68: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 63

dissociation, but there appear to be conceptual problems with this, as the author asks: how can

the brain be in two different states at once? (Hobson, 2009a) But is this so? Or can one rather

say that there is a continuum instead of defined static states? Nevertheless, state dissociations

are quite a commonplace, as proven by parasomnias, such as sleep-walking, a NREM

parasomnia, where the gait-generator and navigational system of the brain stem may be in full

operation while the cerebral cortex is in NREM sleep stage IV, or as sleep paralysis, a REM

parasomnia, where the dreamer wakes up from REM and is unable to move due to persistent

REM sleep motor inhibition (Mahowald, 2009; Mahowald & Schenck, 2005). These and other

identified dissociations are pointed to be strong evidence that there is more than one state

present in several occasions, like a cross-state fluidity (Hobson 2009a).

On the other hand, Brylowski (2010) disagrees on Hobson's (2009a) referencing lucid

dreaming as a dissociation, stating that this is not helpful and not empirically supported because

there seems to be no consensus on the concept of dissociation, neither in psychiatry nor in

neurosciences, and it has not been proven that lucid dreaming is a pathological state.

Alternatively, the three conscious states of waking, non-lucid dreaming, and lucid dreaming

may instead be part of a unique continuum, and not psychologically distinct, except for

experimental purposes, in which they should be defined as physiologically distinct (Brylowski,

2010). In the same line, Holzinger (2010) mentions that lucid dreams can be viewed as

paradoxical dreams, where paradoxical relates to the concept of adding secondary process

thinking to primary process thinking, which causes a new state to emerge that is deeper than

non-lucid dreaming. As such, lucid dreaming could be described as an associative state, instead

of a dissociative one.

Earlier on, LaBerge and DeGracia (2000) had proposed that consciousness may be

present in all dreams and may also be measurable on a continuum, in which one oscillates

between the two poles of ”lucidity” and ”non-lucidity”. The authors based this assumption on

Page 69: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 64

the concept that individual variation seems to characterize the experience of lucid dreaming, as

it is composed by a flow of subjective events brought forth by brain processes, like all forms of

conscious experience (LaBerge & DeGracia, 2000). It would seem that two factors have a

strong presence in this equation: firstly, the belief systems and learning of the dreamer, that

seem to shape the lucid dream experiences, secondly, the factors that seem independent of the

belief systems and learning of the dreamer.

By the same token, already in 2000 LaBerge and DeGracia attempted to connect lucid

dreaming research with consciousness research, which clearly clashed with Crick and Koch's

1998 article: “Consciousness and Neuroscience”. In this particular article, lucid dreaming was

said to be a state with no special features that made it experimentally advantageous for

neuroscience study of consciousness (Crick & Koch, 1998). Nevertheless, LaBerge and

DeGracia (2000) suggested using the framework of the global workspace model of

consciousness developed by Bernard Baars (1988) for a systematic comparison between waking

state, lucid, and non-lucid dreaming. It was then hypothesized that waking state consciousness

is maintained by a context hierarchy of unconscious elements that initially requires conscious

participation to be formed, but afterwards frames conscious experience.

According to the authors (LaBerge & DeGracia, 2000), in non-lucid dreams, the

dreamer consciousness seems also constrained by unconscious contextual elements during the

dream, although the conceptual situation may be little related with episodic experiences of

waking, even though there seems to be memory transfer from waking to dreaming, but very

little in reverse. In both cases, it is proposed by LaBerge and DeGracia (2000) that the dreamer

assumes he is awake, not questioning the events that surround the experiences. However, in

lucid dreams the dreamer appears to able to recall details of his waking life within the

experience, and vice-versa, in waking state the lucid dreamer remembers the details of the

dreams, seemingly contributing to the episodic memory of the waking personality. Likewise,

Page 70: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 65

there seems to be a continuity of consciously accessible memory that links lucid dreams and

waking experience, as in a two-way memory transfer (LaBerge & DeGracia, 2000).

Lastly, LaBerge and DeGracia (2000) had distinguished lucid and non-lucid dreams by

the contextual structure that underlies dream consciousness, that is, non-lucid dreams can be

characterized by the formation of global contexts that vary from dream to dream, whereas lucid

dreams are characterized by a distinct and durable context, the lucid dream context. Lucidity in

dreaming is pointed out to be the sum of meta-cognition plus the conscious access of memories

from the waking experience.

Contemporaneously, cognitive neuroscience experiments on lucid dreaming seemingly

follow a line of research concomitant with a divide between primary and higher-order or

secondary aspects of consciousness (meta-awareness, abstract thinking, volition, and meta-

cognition; primary and secondary consciousness are further detailed in the next sub-section). A

parallel may be made between this divide and the concepts of phenomenal and reflective

consciousness. Likewise, Dresler and colleagues (2014a) suggest that neuroimaging research

into the neural correlates of lucid dreaming, and its association with meta-cognitive and

volitional processes, may support the notion of this phenomenon as a promising approach for

the investigation of higher-order aspects of consciousness.

Moreover, there seems to be a remarkable overlap between the neural correlates of lucid

dreaming, and areas and networks that serve meta-cognitive and volitional capabilities (Brass,

Lynn, Demanet, & Rigoni, 2013; Roskies, 2010), which are pointed to be the same areas that

show the strongest differences between human and non-human primates (Dijksterhuis & Aarts,

2010; Frith, 2013), helping to build the suggestion that higher-order aspects of consciousness

are most pronounced in humans (Dresler et al., 2014a). In research, the use of combined

neuroimaging methods and refined measures of the degree of lucidity, such as scales that assess

several dimensions of volition and meta-cognition (Kahan & Sullivan, 2012; Voss et al., 2013),

Page 71: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 66

may help disentangle the specific involvement of brain regions in higher-order aspects of

consciousness, adding up to the understanding of the neural correlates that underlie the multiple

facets of human consciousness (Dresler et al., 2014a).

For the sake of argument, if one can have meta-cognition in waking state, but not in

dreaming state, and if there is a lucid dreaming state where one can have meta-cognition and

still be dreaming, then this state would be highly informative for researching higher-order

aspects of consciousness. More so, in Spoormaker and colleagues (2010) opinion, if one

contrasts lucid and non-lucid dreaming, especially to unveil the brain regions whose activity

and functional connectivity are crucial for consciousness. In fact, to Spoormaker and colleagues

(2010), non-lucid and lucid dreaming may be the contrast between primary and secondary or

higher-order consciousness, and lucid dreaming may be critical to the full understanding of the

neural correlates of higher-order consciousness, taking into account that lucid REM sleep is still

REM sleep according to the AASM criteria (Iber et al., 2007). Taking this into account, lucid

dreaming is suggested to provide the only case known where primary consciousness as

experienced in dreams can contrast with secondary consciousness in the same state

(Spoormaker et al., 2010).

In the next sub-section, the concepts of protoconsciousness, primary consciousness, and

secondary consciousness will be further reviewed, in what concerns the contemporary

connections between lucid dreaming research and consciousness research.

2. The protoconsciousness and primary-secondary consciousness hypotheses

The protoconsciousness theory is based on the findings that newborn mammals show

high amounts of REM sleep (Hobson & Schredl, 2011), and attempts to answer the question of

what is the function of dreaming as a subjective experience during sleep (Hobson, 2009b). It

suggests that the development and maintenance of waking consciousness and other higher-order

Page 72: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 67

brain functions depend on brain activity during sleep (Hobson & Schredl, 2011). In short, what

is meant by Hobson (as cited in Revonsuo, 2015) with protoconsciousness is a state that is prior,

and causally essential, to the development of consciousness: I REM, therefore I will be.

Hobson (2009b) contemplates a human brain that has an auto-creative nature, in respect

to image creation and fiction producing. Protoconsciousness theory proposes that, before the

emergence of language and consciousness as one knows it, the brain gets ready to be conscious

by being auto-creative, translating the genetic paradigm into a functional program, by following

the directions of the external world and the directions of its own internal programs (Hobson,

2009b). This means that when humans are born they already possess a brain protoconscious, in

the sense that is ready to be conscious (Vogelsong, 2013).

In a published conversation with Vogelsong (2013), Allan Hobson presents his

reasoning: the mind is an aspect of the body yet to be understood clearly, in what the mind is the

subjective part and the body the objective one, not in a dualistic way, but in a dual aspect

monism. It would seem that the subjective part cannot operate without the objective part, though

the objective part can still exist, even if the subjective part is long absent or altered. Hobson

goes on, pointing out that dualism is very attractive because the mind-brain relation feels like a

dualistic system, in the sense that when one thinks, one does not have any awareness of thinking

as a brain function, as the brain behaving in a certain way. In like manner, Hobson refers that

monism is a very difficult state to achieve, because the subjective experience is very isolated

from the objective part.

Hobson (Vogelsong, 2013) carries on stating that, when dreaming, humans are

confronted with the fact that the brain has changed state, and the way to understand the change

of states in the brain is by comparing the dream experience with the waking experience. The

dual aspect monism is posited to reinstate the first-person subjective accounts into a more

complete science. Moreover, Hobson (Vogelsong, 2013) points out lucid dreaming as a very

Page 73: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 68

important evidence that something like the protoconsciousness hypothesis may be correct,

because in lucid dreaming there is primary consciousness, meaning awareness of self, space,

movement, emotion, that are typically present in non-lucid dreaming, but something else has

been added, in this case a self that observes and changes the dream.

Primary consciousness can be defined as simple awareness that includes perception and

emotion, which is ascribed to most mammals (Hobson, 2009b). Secondary consciousness

depends on language, and includes the following features: meta-awareness, abstract thinking,

volition, and meta-cognition (Edelman, 1992). In adult humans, REM sleep non-lucid dreams

are suggested to have features of primary consciousness, mainly perceptions and emotions, but

lack characteristics of secondary consciousness (Hobson, 2009b).

Intensity and quality of consciousness seem to vary during the sleep-wake cycle in

humans, and whether one is awake and alert with all the secondary aspects of consciousness,

asleep with very diminished awareness, or dreaming with the internally generated perceptions

and emotions of primary consciousness, depends on three physiologically identifiable states of

the brain: waking, NREM sleep, and REM sleep (Hobson, 2009b).

REM sleep non-lucid dreams seem to be implicated only with the immediate present,

with an uncontrolled access to the past or anticipated future (Edelman, 2003; Hobson & Voss,

2011; Voss et al., 2009), and seem to have an abundance in features of primary consciousness,

especially perceptions and emotions, organized in a scenario-like environment (Edelman, 2003;

Hobson, 2009b). These characteristics are used as a rationale to assume non-lucid dreams as a

primary state of consciousness (Hobson, 2009b; Hobson & Voss, 2010, 2011; Voss et al., 2014).

In this primary state, humans usually consider to be awake, not knowing that are dreaming, thus

not recognizing its own true condition, its incoherence, its limitation of thought and its

impoverishment of memory (Hobson, 2009b; Hobson & Voss, 2011; Voss et al., 2009).

Furthermore, it would seem that primary consciousness co-develops with REM sleep and the

Page 74: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 69

latter is a virtual reality generator for the brain and, when active, dream happens (Hobson &

Voss, 2010).

When awake, humans are said to enter a secondary mode of consciousness, which

includes higher order cognitive functions (Edelman, 2003; Hobson, 2009b; Hobson & Voss,

2010, 2011; Voss et al., 2009), thus waking consciousness can be defined as the awareness of

the external world, our bodies and our selves, as in meta-awareness (Edelman, 2003; Hobson,

2009b). In lucid dreaming, elements of secondary consciousness are said to coexist with

primary consciousness (Hobson, 2009a; Hobson & Voss, 2010; Voss et al., 2009, 2014), and this

coexistence is suggested to enable the dreamer to become aware of the fact that he is dreaming

while the dream continues (Hobson, 2009a, 2009b; Voss et al., 2009, 2014).

According to Hobson (2009b), REM sleep is pointed to be a recent behaviour in

evolutionary terms, regulated by the pontine brainstem, a phylogenetically ancient brain

structure, and an example how relatively new physiological functions can depend on old brain

regions. Although evolving separately in mammals and birds, there appears to be an early life

preponderance of REM sleep in both. Edelman (2003), Hobson and Voss (2010) suggest that the

primary consciousness system is activated in REM sleep in all animals that possess its

advantages, but as the brain continued to evolve, and elaborated its thalamocortical circuitry, it

became possible for animals to add secondary aspects of consciousness to their waking

repertory.

Around the twelfth week of human gestation, the volume of REM sleep is said to peak,

subsequently declining after birth, while at the same time cognitive capabilities and waking

time increase. Additionally, primary consciousness seems to decline and secondary

consciousness seems to grow in parallel with the development of the brain and the capacity for

prolonged waking (Hobson, 2009b).

Hobson and Voss (2010) suggest reflective consciousness as the phenomenon that

Page 75: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 70

distinguishes humans and primates from other animals, providing them a secondary form of

consciousness, which seems to be prerequisite for abstract thought and volition. Therefore, the

authors (Hobson & Voss, 2010) point out that consciousness may be at least bimodal, although

it may even be multi-modal, and research on sleep and dreaming has documented the state-

dependency of this bi-modality, that is expressed in the dreaming and waking brain physiology

and phenomenology.

Hobson and Voss (2011) also refer that only higher order animals, which possess the

capability of secondary consciousness, may suffer from psychosis. This leads them to conclude

that humans, and only humans, evince fully developed secondary consciousness, and so only

humans have minds that can become dysfunctional in ways that can be called psychotic.

The study of lucid dreaming may present consciousness research with a unique

opportunity to understand the mechanism of primary and secondary consciousness, and explore

the functional significance of their interaction (Hobson & Voss, 2010). Actually, the brain may

never be in one and only one state, and in fact dissociations abound, meaning that the brain can

be in several states at once, or probably fluctuate in a continuum of states that are pervasive,

and share elements of two or more states (Mahowald, 2009; Hobson & Voss, 2010).

Hobson (2009a) argues that lucid dreaming provides a model for the emergence of

Edelman's (2003) secondary consciousness, and its isolation in studies of lucid dreaming will

enable a revolutionary turn in neuroscientific studies of consciousness. Yet, according to

Kuiken:

”In his adaptation of this theoretical framework, Hobson circumvents Edelman's

proposal that language distinctively shapes secondary consciousness, instead, he

acknowledges that linguistic competence is manifest in both lucid and non-lucid

dreaming – and, by implication, in both primary and secondary consciousness.”

(Kuiken, 2010, p. 22)

Page 76: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 71

Kuiken (2010) continues his critic by also pointing out that it is unclear which neural

networks support the self of primary consciousness, as if there was no specific neural substrate

for the self of non-lucid dreaming. Lastly, the author also mentions that some aspects of meta-

awareness and volition, supposedly characteristic of secondary consciousness, can occur

independently of it.

In summary, the primary and secondary consciousness hypothesis brings the theoretical

support and endorsement to several experiments with lucid dreaming. Non-lucid dreaming is

stated as dominated by primary consciousness features, and the waking state by secondary

consciousness. Primary consciousness is advanced by Hobson and Voss (2010) to be the

necessary cognitive base on which secondary consciousness is built. The lucid dreaming

phenomenon brings about an excellent opportunity to monitor the resurgence of volition, meta-

awareness, and meta-cognition, inside REM sleep, which is usually dominated by perception

and emotion. This hypothesis also appears to give support to the idea of consciousness as a

flutuating phenomenon, in which the brain may produce at least two, or maybe several states

simultaneously. Actually, some avenues of research are opened by this hypothesis, such as to

discern if some primates and some birds do have secondary consciousness, and to what degree.

In the next sub-section, the primary-secondary dynamic will be put in parallel with the

phenomenal-reflective consciousness dynamic, in an effort to discern how the former

hypothesis relates to the already standing conceptualizations in consciousness research.

Afterwards, the emergent hypotheses generated from the marriage of the primary-secondary

theory and contemporaneous lucid dream research will be briefly described.

3. Can lucid dreaming unveil consciousness?

In point two of his wish-list of the greatest breakthroughs forthcoming in a speculated

”Golden Age” of consciousness research, Revonsuo states out precisely what is to be expected

Page 77: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 72

from lucid dreaming:

”We figure out exactly what the differences are between pure phenomenal

consciousness and reflective consciousness. One crucial experiment would be the

brain imaging of ordinary versus lucid dreaming: What happens in the brain when

phenomenal dream experience turns into reflective, self-aware dream

experience?” (Revonsuo, 2010, p. 291)

This seems very interesting indeed, but how exactly can we characterize phenomenal

and reflective consciousness from an experimental point? According to Revonsuo (2010),

phenomenal consciousness is characterized by patterns of qualities that are commonly called

sensations, percepts, emotional experiences and feelings, and sensory-perceptual images. This

immense variety of phenomenal experience goes on in dynamic patterns ever-changing and

complex, across a phenomenal field, with a vivid but limited centre that continuously changes

position against a large phenomenal background.

In contrast, reflective consciousness pertains mostly of auditory-linguistic images that

have a phenomenal surface, and also semantic content, which are symbolic expressions

referring to things outside of themselves, such as experiences in phenomenal consciousness.

Reflective consciousness is serial and limited in its capacity, as in the extreme difficulty of

expressing and formulating several explicit, completely independent trains of thought at the

same time (Revonsuo, 2010).

As far as experimental psychology is concerned, since the 1890s that consciousness was

understood via the concept of states (Voss, 2010). Accrued sleep and dream research have

shown that consciousness is, undoubtedly, state dependent (Voss, 2010). Non-lucid dreaming

appears to be a state of consciousness akin to primary consciousness and lacking frontal lobe

activation (Voss, 2010; Voss et al., 2009). Secondary consciousness may be related to frontal

lobe activation, and is present in lucid dreaming and waking state, eliciting meta-awareness in

Page 78: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 73

both (Voss, 2010; Voss et al., 2009).

In view of the research and arguments presented throughout this investigation, lucid

dreaming raises deep questions indeed, and Thompson's words formulate these questions in a

very interesting way:

”When I talk about ”my” awareness, who exactly is this self? When I say, ”I

realized I was dreaming,” who is ”I”? When I describe ”my” partial dream

control, who is the agent or controller? And how is all of this – the awareness, the

sense of self, and the feeling of agency – related to my sleeping brain and body?

To put these questions in general terms: Who or what is the experiential subject of

lucid dreaming, and how is this subject related to the brain and body?”

(Thompson, 2015b, p. 109).

According to Thompson (2015b), one finds here the necessity to distinguish between the

dreaming self and the dream ego, that is to say, between the “self-as-dreamer” and the ”self-

within-the-dream”. In a non-lucid dream, one seems to identify with his own dream ego and to

perceive, for instance, ”I'm under attack!”. Whereas in a lucid dream, one thinks: ”I'm

dreaming!”, and recognizes that the dreaming self is not the same as the dream ego. The dream

ego appears to function as an avatar within a virtual world, and the dreaming self as its user

(Thompson, 2015b).

Taking these concepts together, it would seem to the external observing academic,

following the debate and research development, that the connection between lucid dreaming

and consciousness depends on the way the phenomena of non-lucid and lucid dreaming relate to

consciousness conceptions and assumed definitions. For instance, non-lucid dreaming seems to

be interwoven with the concept of primary consciousness (Hobson & Voss, 2011), but also with

phenomenal consciousness (Revonsuo, 2010), and pre-reflective self-consciousness (Gallacher

& Zahavi, 2015). In turn, lucid dreams appear to be intertwined with the concept of secondary

Page 79: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 74

consciousness (Hobson & Voss, 2011), and yet also with reflective consciousness (Revonsuo,

2010), or reflective self-consciousness (Gallacher & Zahavi, 2015). For Thompson (2015b), it

seems to be a question of the dreaming self assimilating or not with the dream ego, and

meditative lucid dreaming, or Dream Yoga, is proposed to further study these phenomena.

One can say that these authors appear to be mentioning the same experiential plethora,

albeit with different nuances. It would seem that the continuum phenomenal-reflective

consciousness may in some ways be connected with a state consciousness continuum, a

primary-secondary consciousness continuum, a pre-reflective-reflective self-consciousness

continuum, or even an assimilation continuum between dreaming self-dream ego, where the

brain-mind relation apparently can be in some way quantifiable. Lucid dreaming fits here like a

measuring tool, capable of producing a glimpse of this continuum inner-works, in a

experimentally pliable way.

In this sense, lucid dreams may offer new perspectives for consciousness research, if

operationalized in experimental contrast with non-lucid ones. In this contrast, and in the

analysis of the transition, may lie all the nuances of the phenomenal-reflective consciousness

continuum. Starting from the non-lucid dreams, that apparently demonstrate that primary

consciousness is not specific to the wake state, including sensory imagery and the experience

with a self interacting with the dream world, while disconnected from sensory inputs and motor

outputs (Noreika et al., 2010). And all the way until reaching SVLD state, which apparently

provides rich examples of secondary consciousness, specifically of more sophisticated cognitive

and meta-cognitive processes occurring in sleep (Noreika et al., 2010). These experimental and

conceptual analyses, may provide many theoretical insights, by revealing and exposing different

segments of the continuum.

According to Noreika and colleagues (2010), when researching the transition from non-

lucid to lucid dreams, through the analysis of dream reports associated with neuroimaging

Page 80: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 75

experiments, it may become possible to map the changes in self-related processing and the

neural correlates enabling that same transition. Furthermore, the prospect of experimentally

manipulating and inducing lucid dreams may also bring forth the possibility of analysing certain

aspects of meta-awareness, such as cognitive and mnemonic processes and bodily experiences

(Dresler et al., 2014a; Noreika et al., 2010).

In addition, lucid dreams may also help investigate the neural correlates of dream

behaviour, clarifying how similar bodily experiences and dream behaviour are to their waking

state equivalents, concerning phenomenology and brain activation (Erlacher et al., 2003;

Erlacher & Schredl, 2008; Hobson, 2009a). Finally, the experimental manipulation of dream

content and induction of lucid dreams may also shine a light on the neural correlates of the

sense of agency, the first-person perspective and the narrative or autobiographical self-model

(Dresler et al., 2014a; Noreika et al., 2010).

In line with this purpose, Voss and Hobson (2015) very recently presented four

hypotheses centred around the lucid dreaming phenomenon, deduced from the empirical work

already presented in this theoretical investigation:

1. The Brain Maturation Hypothesis

2. The Hybrid State Hypothesis

3. The Space of Consciousness Model

4. The Gamma Band Hypothesis

Simply put, the brain maturation hypothesis states that lucid dreaming may be a mental

state of exception, that appears to occur in a natural way while the brain matures (Voss &

Hobson, 2015). The rationale behind this proposition is the inverse correlation between age and

lucid dream frequency, and also how difficult it is to experience this phenomenon on a regular

basis without training, both proposed in several studies (Schredl & Erlacher, 2004, 2011;

Stumbrys et al., 2014; Voss et al., 2012). Voss and Hobson (2015) suggest that the peak in

Page 81: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 76

spontaneous occurrence of lucid dreaming seems to be coincident with frontal lobe myelination

final stages, which include synaptic expansion and dendritic growth. It also attempts explaining

the origin, or the why, of lucid dreaming (Hobson, 2009b; Voss & Hobson, 2015). This is an

interesting hypothesis, and opens up an avenue of research which needs more studies, especially

with children and teenagers.

The hybrid state hypothesis posits that, when the brain shifts from a non-lucid to a lucid

dream, the subjective experience of dreaming becomes a hybrid state, with elements from both

waking and dreaming state consciousness (Hobson, 2009a; Hobson & Voss, 2015; Voss et al.,

2009). The dream self seems to simply separate from the on-going flow of mental imagery,

characteristic of non-lucid REM sleep dreaming, and the dreamer may have a variable degree of

control over the dream plot (Hobson & Voss, 2015; Voss et al., 2014). The hybrid state

hypothesis relies upon this concept, which is pointed by Voss and Hobson (2015) to be mainly

supported by empirical evidence produced by some of the previously analysed studies (Dresler

et al., 2012; Voss et al., 2009, 2014 – please refer to Part III, sub-section 2 for more details).

The Space of Consciousness (SoC) model is “based on the assumption that

consciousness is a dynamical process unfolding in a phenomenal state-space continuum” (Voss

& Hobson, 2015, p. 9). In this model, firstly proposed by Voss and Voss (2014), consciousness

is defined as a three-dimensional space (x-y-z) occupied by states that vary as a function of

sensing, judging, and motor control. Sensing (x-axis) is defined as the ability to experience

physical and mental fluctuations. Judging (y-axis) is used to describe shifting levels of higher-

order cognitive skills (meta-awareness, to think about the past, contemplate the future, make

decisions). The motor control dimension (z-axis) refers to the ability to produce body

movement. The SoC model is pointed to be solely phenomenological, as it does not differentiate

between internal and external sources of information or state-dependent neurochemical

modulations (Voss & Hobson, 2015; Voss & Voss, 2014).

Page 82: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 77

As it can be seen in figure 3, Voss and Hobson (2015) differentiate between REM lucid

dream state (equivalent to DILD) and WILD lucid dream state, as proposed by LaBerge

(LaBerge & DeGracia, 2000; LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; LaBerge et al., 1986). These two

states seem to differ mainly in the motor control dimension, even though this is a theoretical

proposal. This is a fascinating model, even though it may need much more refinement and

probably some more dimensions included. With the aid of this, or posterior models, it may be

possible to start positioning and placing several states of consciousness, as they are research and

more information is added.

The gamma band hypothesis pertains to the discovery of the increased activation of the

human brain frontal lobes during lucidity in dreams, in the lower gamma band (40 Hz), the how

of lucid dreaming, suggesting that brain activation in the 40 Hz frequency range relates to

secondary consciousness (Hobson & Voss, 2011; Voss et al., 2012, 2014).

At this moment in time, the path of state-dependent consciousness study is open, based

on the several hypotheses put forward through the empirical research done so far. It would seem

Page 83: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 78

that cognitive neuroscience has a puzzle with some pieces available, but many more are yet to

be retrieved. These pieces will probably fall in the SoC model, or in future versions of it. There

is still a tremendous amount of research to be done, and the model may evolve to a 3D-space in

a spheric perspective, as in a 3D-puzzle, as more axis are added to it.

4. The continuity of meta-awareness in consciousness research

One final note, linked to a relatively neglected grey area of interesting experimental

research, is due. This grey area is settled in the intersection of the body of knowledge already

obtained by sleep, non-lucid dreaming, and lucid dreaming research, with Tibetan Buddhist

experiences of Dream Yoga and Sleep Yoga.

For centuries, the notion that it is possible to become aware or lucid of one's mental

processes through the whole sleep-wake cycle, that is to say, to keep one's meta-awareness, or

continuity in consciousness, in all stages of sleep, has served as a foundation for Tibetan Dream

Yoga and Sleep Yoga (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000; Stumbrys, 2011; Thompson, 2015b).

Both practices aim to develop constant non-dual awareness across wakefulness, dreaming, and

dreamless sleep. It would seem that the development of awareness in dreams through Dream

Yoga would facilitate the development of awareness in dreamless sleep, and the development of

awareness in deep sleep through Sleep Yoga would in turn facilitate awareness in dreams

(Thompson, 2015b).

Presumably, through these practices it would be possible to achieve continuity of

consciousness among waking, dreaming and dreamless sleep, and this is seen in Tibetan

Buddhism as an essential step to higher personal development. Apparently, lucid dreaming may

constitute a possible glimpse in the continuity of consciousness (LaBerge & Gackenbach, 2000;

Stumbrys, 2011; Thompson, 2015b).

This would also mean that NREM lucid dreaming, or lucid witnessing, could be in

Page 84: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 79

theory achievable, thus suggesting a possible continuity in meta-awareness, or consciousness,

across the whole wake-sleep cycle (Stumbrys, 2011; Thompson, 2015b). Curiously enough,

Stumbrys and Erlacher (2012) reported two cases of lucid dreaming during NREM sleep. The

data from the two cases reported comes from another study, in which participants were

instructed to signal L-R-L-R-L-R whenever reaching lucidity. In the first case, a female (26

years), frequent lucid dreamer (≥ once a week) and with good dream recall, signalled during

NREM N2 on the second consecutive night in the sleep laboratory (during the 2nd sleep cycle,

105 mins. after falling asleep). The subject reported no presence of visual imagery, but a

floating sensation without body feeling. In the second case, also a female (26 years old),

infrequent lucid dreamer (2-4 a year) and with good dream recall, signalled also during NREM

N2 on the third consecutive night in the sleep laboratory (during the 1st sleep cycle, 95 mins.

after falling asleep). This subject did five eye signals, but had no recall (Stumbrys & Erlacher,

2012). Actually, already in 1981, LaBerge and colleagues had reported two NREM N1 lucid

dreams, and one in the transition from NREM N2 to REM (for a more detailed analysis of this

experiment, please refer to Part III, sub-chapter 2). This particular aspect is in need of a more

detailed review and further study.

In the same line, other authors (Mason & Orme-Johnson, 2010) working with

Transcendental Meditation (TM), a specific form of mantra meditation created by Maharishi

Mahesh Yogi in the mid-1950s and popularized world-wide in the 1960s and 1970s, point out

that reflective consciousness evolution may not end with lucidity, but may move further along

the continuum to a quieter, uninvolved state of awareness known as witnessing, experienced as

having no boundaries. This may be called transcendental consciousness, one that is experienced

continuously, especially during deep sleep, indicating that a higher state of consciousness has

been achieved (Mason & Orme-Johnson, 2010). Interestingly enough, Hunt and Ogilvie (1988)

had already mentioned that meditators seem to have more lucid dreams than non-meditators,

Page 85: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 80

and also that longer meditative practice appears to correlate with a higher frequency of lucid

dreams. This would need a recent study, so as to delve further into this particular aspect.

Intriguingly enough, Thompson (2015b) mentions some ancient Indian texts, called the

Upanishads, which seem to contain the first recorded map of consciousness, dating from the

sixth or seventh century (B.C.E.). The author points out that these texts put forward three

principal states of the self: the waking state, the dream state, and the state of deep and dreamless

sleep. Later texts added a fourth state, called state of pure awareness. In this literature, waking

consciousness seems to relate to the outer world and takes the physical body as the self. Dream

consciousness relates to mental images produced from memories and takes the dream body as

the self. In deep and dreamless sleep, consciousness is said to be in a dormant state not

differentiated into subject and object. The author also mentions that pure awareness seems to

witness these changing states without identifying with them or with the self that appears in them

(for more on the concept of pure awareness please refer to Thompson, 2015b). By these

standards, the lucid dreamer seems to be in the so-called state of pure awareness, in which he

observes his dream self act upon the dream, being aware that it is a dream (Thompson, 2015b).

This ancient conception is quite intriguing, as it seems to propose the hypothesis of a

continuity of meta-awareness through all the states, by means of experienced meditation, to

achieve a permanent state of self-awareness. In this ancient conceptualization, consciousness

seems to be defined as a ”light”, which illuminates or reveals things so they can be known. For

instance, in waking state it seems to illuminate the outer world, and in dreams illuminate the

dream world (Thompson, 2015b). Nonetheless, even though lucid dreams are said to be more

discontinuous with waking experience, they seem to present continuity in the sense of ”I” and

meta-awareness, that is, continuity of consciousness (Kahan & LaBerge, 1994; Stumbrys,

2011). As mentioned before, there seems to be some kind of consciousness and self-awareness

in all dreams, but non-lucid dreams seem to lack reflective or meta-awareness, contrary to lucid

Page 86: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 81

dreams, who integrate meta-awareness (Cicogna & Bosinelli, 2001; Kahan & LaBerge, 1994;

Stumbrys, 2011).

In short, the idea of continuity through the maintenance of self-awareness, meta-

awareness (and even pure-awareness), along the wake-sleep cycle is extremely intriguing. The

point is that, in cognitive neuroscience terms, and more broadly in philosophical terms, there

must be an experimental definition of what exactly is intended when the authors mention these

aspects of awareness, if a line of research including the continuity of meta-awareness is to be

pursued.

As seen before, phenomenal experience and self-awareness seem to be present in non-

lucid dreams, but meta-awareness arises only in lucid dreams. To pursue a continuity, should

one achieve meta-awareness in the whole sleep-wake cycle? Is this possible in deep sleep and

non-lucid dreams? If so, would not these states become lucid dreams, in some extent? And,

above all, would not this kind of continuity put at stake the concept of state-dependent

consciousness? Further research is needed to thoroughly understand this idea.

Page 87: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 82

Concluding remarks and bridges for the future

The notion that arises out of all concepts, empirical experimentations, and ideas put

forward previously, is that there seems to be limitations to the scientific method in coming to

terms with the felt reality of first-person experience, in the sense of a self endowed with

consciousness (Thompson, 2015b). Nevertheless, research on lucid dreaming presented

throughout this theoretical investigation clearly demonstrates the viability of empirical

experimentations under this same method. These experimentations, as far as lucid dreaming is

concerned, are limited by the low number of subjects available and able to produce SVLDs,

under different protocols. This is the first and foremost difficulty when working with this

phenomenon.

Moreover, as in everything related to consciousness studies and research, the

inescapable challenge is to have a theory that, somehow, accommodates the phenomenon. In the

case of lucid dreaming, the contemporary empirical research goes hand in hand with primary

and secondary consciousness hypothesis, which is based on a naturalistic view of human

evolution.

Another aspect, as in any branch of science, is replication. The most recent findings in

the field of lucid dreams are in sore need of experimental replication. The LuCid scale is a fine

example of this, as it is being experimentally used in adapted and shorter versions, in a very

flexible way, probably very much so. The way it was used to measure tACS results (Voss et al.,

2014) for instance, as to confirm lucidity in the 40 Hz gamma band, exposes clearly the need

for experimental consolidation in this field of research.

Furthermore, the jargon of the field needs also clarification and agreement. This is more

so in connection with consciousness research, where nomenclature abounds and several

expressions are used to refer to aspects that, in it-self, are not clearly defined. For the non-

specialist academic, the first reading moments reveal a tremendous confusion to what is what

Page 88: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 83

exactly, as each experimental research team uses its own vocabulary, and more so when

superimposed with philosophy of mind.

In what relates consciousness as a state, more research is needed in terms of mapping all

states of consciousness, so as to place them in the SoC model, or some other model that may be

presented in the near future. The continuum of states, and its inter-mixture in the mind-brain

relation should be thoroughly interpreted, so as to clarify all the gradations that go between the

experience of pure phenomenal consciousness, to the more refined state of reflective

consciousness. This may include, for instance: the last instants before death, coma, locked-in

syndrome, near-death experiences (NDEs), out-of-the-body experiences (OBEs), non-lucid

dreams, REM-initiated lucid dreams, wake-initiated lucid dreams (WILDs), meditative states

(dream yoga, sleep yoga, transcendental meditation), all the other so-called mystical or higher-

states of consciousness, and others that may be discovered in the future.

To map this continuum of states, research may pass through the merging of the sleep

laboratory with the natural environment of the dreamer, with new devices that allow for PSG

and brain imaging at home, or in a hospital facility, without wires and with the minimum

discomfort and disruption possible. These devices may also send the data through Wi-Fi, to be

processed automatically as it is happening, proceeding also to the awakenings and posterior

subjective report recordings.

In historical terms, on what investigation is concerned, there is no doubt of the

breakthrough importance of LaBerge and colleagues' 1981 experiment. This seemingly gave a

new and fresh impulse to lucid dreaming research. Throughout the 1980s and 1990s a new

surge of experimentations and writings emerged, mainly in the United States. However, this

movement apparently faltered and waned, probably due to lack of funding for new experiments.

Contemporaneously, a new resurgence seems to have happened in Germany, where the

lucid dreaming phenomenon beacon was lighted up again, espoused with consciousness

Page 89: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 84

research. The vast majority, if not all, of research involving lucid dreams seems to go on in

those latitudes, with the support of German foundations and the occasional support from Bial

foundation, in Portugal. Obviously, this needs expanding.

A golden age needs gold, which is to say, all research needs funding, and lucid dreaming

is no exception. The problem is that this particular subject is not a profitable or commercial one.

Nevertheless, one quick search on the internet reveals several sites and forums on lucid

dreaming, teeming with enthusiasts on the subject, trying to learn more and intent on dreaming

lucid. Crowd-funding may be a way of funding research, as science advances for the benefit of

all.

It would seem that, regarding the near future, several paths lead to the bridge of

replication and expansion of knowledge, which in turn leads to the field of re-doing the

experiments presented, expanding the number of subjects and consolidating the knowledge

edifice. The not so near future may involve the creation of new methods in mind, new

machinery simply put, to thoroughly map all states of consciousness in a way that enables us to

finally comprehend where all pieces fit, and how the whole picture of state consciousness looks

like.

Lucid dreaming came a long way, from an obscure and suspected phenomenon,

mentioned only in rare under pseudonym-written books and manuscripts associated with

mystiques and the occult, to an identified and clearly established phenomenon in the scientific

journals of the academic world. Suffice to say that this new consciousness state, embedded in

REM sleep, may hold one of the contemporaneously available keys for consciousness research,

and for navigation on the vast ocean of human subjectivity.

Page 90: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 85

References

Abe, T., Ogawa, K., Nittono, H., & Hori, T. (2008). Neural generators of brain potentials before

rapid eye movements during human REM sleep: a study using sLORETA. Clinical Neu-

rophysiology, 119(9), 2044-2053. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2008.05.008

Achermann, P., & Borbely, A. A. (1997). Low-frequency (< 1Hz) oscillations in the human

sleep electroencephalogram. Neuroscience, 81(1), 213-222. doi:10.1016/S0306-

4522(97)00186-3

Andrade, K. C., Spoormaker, V. I., Dresler, M., Wehrle, R., Holsboer, F., Sämann, P. G., &

Czisch, M. (2011). Sleep spindles and hippocampal functional connectivity in human

NREM sleep. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(28), 10331-10339. doi:

10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5660-10.2011

Aristotle, (350 B.C.E.). On dreams. Retrieved from http://classic.mit.edu/Aristotle/dreams.html

Armstrong-Hickey, D. (1988). A validation of lucid dreaming in school age children. Lucidity

Letter, 7(2), 35-38.

Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and

concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science, 118(3062), 273-274. Retrieved from

http://www.lphslibrary.org/uploads/7/2/9/6/7296009/aserinsky_eye_motility2.pdf

Awareness. (n.d.). In Oxford English Dictionary online. Retrieved from http://www.oed.com.lo-

gin.libraryproxy.his.se/view/Entry/13894?redirectedFrom=awareness#eid

Barrett, D. (1991). Flying dreams and lucidity: An empirical study of their relationship.

Dreaming, 1(2), 129. doi:10.1037/h0094325

Barrett, D. (1992). Just how lucid are lucid dreams?. Dreaming, 2(4), 221.

doi:10.1037/h0094362

Page 91: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 86

Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: University Press.

Blackmore, S. J. (1982). Have you ever had an OBE? The wording of the question. Journal of

the Society for Psychical Research, 51, 292-302. Retrieved from

http://www.susanblackmore.co.uk/Articles/PDFs/JSPR%201982.pdf

Blackmore, S. J. (1984). A postal survey of OBEs and other experiences. Journal of the Society

for Psychical Research, 52(796), 225-244. Retrieved from

http://www.susanblackmore.co.uk/articles/pdfs/jspr%201984.pdf

Borbély, A. A. (2001). From slow waves to sleep homeostasis: new perspectives. Archives

Italiennes de Biologie, 139(1), 53-61. Retrieved from

http://www.architalbiol.org/index.php/aib/article/viewArticle/204

Brass, M., Lynn, M. T., Demanet, J., & Rigoni, D. (2013). Imaging volition: what the brain can

tell us about the will. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 301-312. doi:

10.1007/s00221-013-3472-x

Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesenten, N. J., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., ... & Her-

scovitch, P. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H2

(15) O PET study. Brain, 120(7), 1173-1197. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.7.1173

Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., ... & Herscovitch, P.

(1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid

eye movement sleep. Science, 279(5347), 91-95. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5347.91

Brooks, J., & Vogelsong, J. (2010). The Subjective Side of Lucid Dream Research.

International Journal of Dream Research, 3(2), 100-103. Retrieved from

https://journals.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/viewFile/6133/pdf

Brown, A. E. (1934). Dreams in which the dreamer knows he is asleep. The Journal of

Abnormal and Social Psychology, 31(1), 59. doi: 10.1037/h0062105

Page 92: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 87

Brylowski, A. (2010). Neuropsychiaric perspective and recommendations. International

Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 3-6. Retrieved from https://journals.ub.uni-

heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/viewFile/602/pdf_25

Brylowski, A., Levitan, L., & LaBerge, S. (1989). H-reflex suppression and autonomic activa-

tion during lucid REM sleep: a case study. Sleep, 12(4), 374-378. Retrieved from

http://www.academia.edu/6008247/H-

reflex_Suppression_and_Autonomic_Activation_During_Lucid_REM_Sleep_A_Case_St

udy

Cantero, J. L., Atienza, M., Madsen, J. R., & Stickgold, R. (2004). Gamma EEG dynamics in

neocortex and hippocampus during human wakefulness and sleep. Neuroimage, 22(3),

1271-1280. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.014

Caporro, M., Haneef, Z., Yeh, H. J., Lenartowicz, A., Buttinelli, C., Parvizi, J., & Stern, J. M.

(2012). Functional MRI of sleep spindles and K-complexes. Clinical Neurophysiology,

123(2), 303-309. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.06.01

Castro, S., Falconi, A., Chase, M. H., & Torterolo, P. (2013). Coherent neocortical 40 Hz ‐

oscillations are not present during REM sleep. European Journal of Neuroscience, 37(8),

1330-1339. doi:10.1111/ejn.12143

Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy

and behavioural correlates. Brain, 129(3), 564-583. doi:10.1093/brain/awl004

Cicogna, P., & Bosinelli, M. (2001). Consciousness during dreams. Consciousness and

Cognition, 10(1), 26-41. doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0471

Page 93: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 88

Conduit, R., Crewther, S. G., & Coleman, G. (2004). Spontaneous eyelid movements (ELMS)

during sleep are related to dream recall on awakening. Journal of Sleep Research, 13(2),

137-144. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00397.x

Corsi-Cabrera, M., Guevara, M. A., & Del Río-Portilla, Y. (2008). Brain activity and temporal

coupling related to eye movements during REM sleep: EEG and MEG results. Brain

Research, 1235, 82-91. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.052

Corsi-Cabrera, M., Miro, E., Del Rı́o-Portilla, Y., Pérez-Garci, E., Villanueva, Y., & Guevara,

M. A. (2003). Rapid eye movement sleep dreaming is characterized by uncoupled EEG

activity between frontal and perceptual cortical regions. Brain and Cognition, 51(3), 337-

345. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00037-X

Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8(2), 97-107.

doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.2.97

Czisch M, Wehrle R, Stiegler A, Peters H, Andrade K, Holsboer, F, & Sämann, P. (2009)

Acoustic Oddball during NREM Sleep: A Combined EEG/fMRI Study. PLoS ONE 4(8):

e6749. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006749

Dang-Vu, T. T., Schabus, M., Desseilles, M., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Darsaud, A., ... & Maquet, P.

(2008). Spontaneous neural activity during human slow wave sleep. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences, 105(39), 15160-15165. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801819105

Dement, W., & Kleitman, N. (1957). The relation of eye movements during sleep to dream

activity: an objective method for the study of dreaming. Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 53(5), 339. doi:10.1037/h0048189

Page 94: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 89

Desmurget, M., Reilly, K. T., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese, C., & Sirigu, A. (2009).

Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science, 324(5928), 811-

813. doi: 10.1126/science.1169896

Destexhe, A., Hughes, S. W., Rudolph, M., & Crunelli, V. (2007). Are corticothalamic “up”

states fragments of wakefulness?. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(7), 334-342. doi:

10.1016/j.tins.2007.04.006

Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annual Review of

Psychology, 61, 467-490. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445

Dresler, M., Eibl, L., Fischer, C. F., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I., Steiger, A., ... & Pawlowski,

M. (2014a). Volitional components of consciousness vary across wakefulness, dreaming

and lucid dreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00987

Dresler, M., Spoormaker, V., Wehrle, R. & Czisch, M. (2014b).Your Imaging Group Has

Coined the Term "Dream Imaging." Please Summarize the Concept in Relation to Dream

Theory. In N. Tranquillo (Eds.) Dream Consciousness (pp. 201-203). Switzerland:

Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-07296-8_13

Dresler, M., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I., Koch, S., Holsboer, F., Steiger, A., ... & Czisch, M.

(2012). Neural correlates of dream lucidity obtained from contrasting lucid versus non-

lucid REM sleep: a combined EEG/fMRI case study. Sleep, 35(7), 1017.

doi:10.5665/sleep.1974

Dresler, M., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I., Steiger, A., Holsboer, F., Czisch, M., & Hobson, J.

A. (2015). Neural correlates of insight in dreaming and psychosis. Sleep Medicine Re-

views, 20, 92-99. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.06.004

Page 95: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 90

Edelman, G. M. (2003). Naturalizing consciousness: a theoretical framework. Proceedings of

the National Academy of Sciences, 100(9), 5520-5524. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0931349100

Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic

Books.

Erlacher, D., & Chapin, H. (2010). Lucid dreaming: Neural virtual reality as a mechanism for

performance enhancement. International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 7-10.

Retrieved from http://journals.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/index.php/

IJoDR/article/viewFile/588/pdf

Erlacher, D., Schädlich, M., Stumbrys, T., & Schredl, M. (2014). Time for actions in lucid

dreams: effects of task modality, length, and complexity. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.

doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01013

Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2004). Time Required for Motor Activity in Lucid Dreams.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99(3f), 1239-1242. doi: 10.2466/PMS.99.7.1239-1242

Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2008). Do REM (lucid) dreamed and executed actions share the

same neural substrate?. International Journal of Dream Research, 1(1). Retrieved from

http://archiv.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/volltextserver/8421/1/30_Erlacher.pdf

Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2010). Practicing a motor task in a lucid dream enhances

subsequent performance: A pilot study. The Sport Psychologist, 24(2), 157-167. Retrieved

from http://www.humankinetics.com/acucustom/sitename/Documents/DocumentItem/

18193.pdf

Erlacher, D., Schredl, M., & LaBerge, S. (2003). Motor area activation during dreamed hand

clenching: A pilot study on EEG alpha band. Sleep and Hypnosis, 5, 182-187. Retrieved

from http://www.sleepandhypnosis.org/pdf/122624433.pdf

Page 96: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 91

Fenwick, P., Schatzman, M., Worsley, A., Adams, J., Stone, S., & Baker, A. (1984). Lucid

dreaming: correspondence between dreamed and actual events in one subject during REM

sleep. Biological Psychology, 18(4), 243-252. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(84)90056-5

Filevich, E., Dresler, M., Brick, T. R., & Kühn, S. (2015). Metacognitive mechanisms underlying lucid

dreaming. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(3), 1082-1088. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3342-

14.2015

Finelli, L. A., Borbély, A. A., & Achermann, P. (2001). Functional topography of the human

nonREM sleep electroencephalogram. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(12), 2282-

2290. doi:10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01597.x

Fosse, R., Stickgold, R., & Hobson, J. A. (2001). Brain-mind states: reciprocal variation in

thoughts and hallucinations. Psychological Science, 12(1), 30-36. doi:10.1111/1467-

9280.00306

Foulkes, W. D. (1962). Dream reports from different stages of sleep. The Journal of Abnormal

and Social Psychology, 65(1), 14. doi:10.1037/h0040431

Foulkes, W. D. (1985). Dreaming: A cognitive-psychological analysis. New Jersey: Routledge.

Fries, P., Nikolić, D., & Singer, W. (2007). The gamma cycle. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(7), 309-316.

doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.005

Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E., & Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal syn-

chronization by selective visual attention. Science, 291(5508), 1560-1563. doi:

10.1126/science.1055465

Frith, C. (2013). The psychology of volition. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 289-299.

doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3407-6

Page 97: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 92

Gackenbach, J. (1988). The psychological content of lucid versus nonlucid dreams. In T. Snyder

& J. Gackenbach (Eds.), Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain (pp. 181-220). New York:

Springer

Gackenbach, J. I. (2010). Psychological considerations in pursuing lucid dreaming research.

International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 11-12. Retrieved from

http://journals.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/viewFile/589/pdf

Gallagher, S., Zahavi, D. (2015). Phenomenological Approaches to Self-Consciousness. In E.N.

Zalda (Eds.). The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2015 Edition), Retrieved

from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2015/entries/self-consciousness-

phenomenological

Gavie, J. E., & Revonsuo, A. (2010). The future of lucid dreaming treatment. International

Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 13-15. Retrieved from https://journals.ub.uni-

heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/viewFile/591/pdf

Goldberg, I., Ullman, S., & Malach, R. (2008). Neuronal correlates of “free will” are associated

with regional specialization in the human intrinsic/default network. Consciousness and

Cognition, 17(3), 587-601. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.10.003

Green, C. E. (1968). Lucid dreams. Oxford: Institute of Psychophysical Research.

Gross, D. W., & Gotman, J. (1999). Correlation of high-frequency oscillations with the sleep–

wake cycle and cognitive activity in humans. Neuroscience, 94(4), 1005-1018.

doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00343-7

Hobson, J. A. (2005). Sleep is of the brain, by the brain and for the brain. Nature, 437(7063),

1254-1256. doi:10.1038/nature04283

Page 98: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 93

Hobson, J. A. (2009a). The neurobiology of consciousness: lucid dreaming wakes up.

International Journal of Dream Research, 2(2), 41-44. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2009.2.403

Hobson, J. A. (2009b). REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(11), 803-813. doi:10.1038/nrn2716

Hobson, J.A. (2014). Lecture II: Physiology. In N. Tranquillo (Eds.) Dream Consciousness (pp.

245-246). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

Hobson, J. A., & Pace-Schott, E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal

systems, consciousness and learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(9), 679-693.

doi:10.1038/nrn915

Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: toward a

cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(06), 793-

842. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003976

Hobson, A., & Schredl, M. (2011). The continuity and discontinuity between waking and

dreaming: A Dialogue between Michael Schredl and Allan Hobson concerning the

adequacy and completeness of these notions. International Journal of Dream Research,

4(1), 3-7. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2011.1.9087

Hobson, J. A., Stickgold, R., & Pace-Schott, E. F. (1998). The neuropsychology of REM sleep

dreaming. NeuroReport, 9(3), R1-R14. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199802160-00033

Hobson, J. A., & Voss, U. (2010). Lucid dreaming and the bimodality of consciousness. In E.

Perry, D. Collerton, F. LeBeau, H. Ashton (Eds.), New Horizons in the Neurosciences of

Consciousness (pp. 155-166). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.

Hobson, A., & Voss, U. (2011). A mind to go out of: Reflections on primary and secondary

consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 993-997.

doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.018

Page 99: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 94

Hofle, N., Paus, T., Reutens, D., Fiset, P., Gotman, J., Evans, A. C., & Jones, E. (1997).

Regional cerebral blood flow changes as a function of delta and spindle activity during

slow wave sleep in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(12), 4800-4808.

Holzinger, B. (2009). Lucid dreaming: dreams of clarity. Contemporary Hypnosis, 26(4), 216-

224. doi: 10.1002/ch.390

Holzinger, B. (2010). Why lucid dreaming could also be called paradoxical dreaming.

International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 16. doi: 10.11588/ijodr.2010.1.592

Holzinger, B., LaBerge, S., & Levitan, L. (2006). Psychophysiological correlates of lucid

dreaming. Dreaming, 16(2), 88. doi:10.1037/1053-0797.16.2.88

Hunt, H. T., & Ogilvie, R. D. (1988). Lucid dreams in their natural series. In T. Snyder & J.

Gackenbach (Eds.), Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain (pp. 389-417). New York: Springer

Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A., & Quan, S. (2007). The AASM manual for the scoring of

sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. Westchester:

American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

Ioannides, A. A., Corsi-Cabrera, M., Fenwick, P. B., del Rio Portilla, Y., Laskaris, N. A.,

Khurshudyan, A., ... & Kostopoulos, G. K. (2004). MEG tomography of human cortex

and brainstem activity in waking and REM sleep saccades. Cerebral Cortex, 14(1), 56-72.

doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg091

Jahnke, K., von Wegner, F., Morzelewski, A., Borisov, S., Maischein, M., Steinmetz, H., &

Laufs, H. (2012). To wake or not to wake? The two-sided nature of the human K-

complex. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1631-1638. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.013

Jensen, O., Kaiser, J., & Lachaux, J. P. (2007). Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated

with attention and memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(7), 317-324.

doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.001

Page 100: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 95

Jouny, C., Chapotot, F., & Merica, H. (2000). EEG spectral activity during paradoxical sleep:

further evidence for cognitive processing. NeuroReport, 11(17), 3667-3671. doi:

10.1097/00001756-200011270-00016

Kahan, T. L., & LaBerge, S. (1994). Lucid dreaming as metacognition: Implications for

cognitive science. Consciousness and Cognition, 3(2), 246-264.

doi:10.1006/ccog.1994.1014

Kahan, T. L., & LaBerge, S. (2011). Dreaming and waking: similarities and differences

revisited. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 494-514.

doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.002

Kahan, T. L., & Sullivan, K. T. (2012). Assessing metacognitive skills in waking and sleep: A

psychometric analysis of the Metacognitive, Affective, Cognitive Experience (MACE)

questionnaire. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 340-352.

doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.11.005

Kahn, D., & Gover, T. (2010). Consciousness in dreams. International Review of Neurobiology,

92, 181-195. doi:10.1016/S0074-7742(10)92009-6

Kajimura, N., Uchiyama, M., Takayama, Y., Uchida, S., Uema, T., Kato, M., ... & Takahashi, K.

(1999). Activity of midbrain reticular formation and neocortex during the progression of

human non-rapid eye movement sleep. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(22), 10065-

10073. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/content/19/22/10065.short

Karim, A. A. (2010). Transcranial cortex stimulation as a novel approach for probing the

neurobiology of dreams: Clinical and neuroethical implications. International Journal of

Dream Research, 3(1), 17-20. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2010.1.593

Page 101: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 96

Kaufmann, C., Wehrle, R., Wetter, T. C., Holsboer, F., Auer, D. P., Pollmächer, T., & Czisch, M.

(2006). Brain activation and hypothalamic functional connectivity during human non-

rapid eye movement sleep: an EEG/fMRI study. Brain, 129(3), 655-667. doi:

10.1093/brain/awh686

Kuhl, J., & Fuhrmann, A. (1998). Decomposing self-regulation and self-control: the Volitional

Components Inventory, in J. Heckhausen and C. Dweck (Eds.) Motivation and Self-regu-

lation Across the Life Span. Cambridge: University Press, 15–49. doi:

10.1017/CBO9780511527869.003

Kuiken, D. (2010). Primary and secondary consciousness during dreaming. International

Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 21-25. doi: 10.11588/ijodr.2010.1.594

LaBerge, S. (1980). Lucid dreaming as a learnable skill: A case study. Perceptual and Motor

Skills, 51(3f), 1039-1042. doi: 10.2466/pms.1980.51.3f.1039

LaBerge, S. (1985). Lucid dreaming: The power of being awake and aware in your dreams. Los

Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher.

LaBerge, S. (1990). Lucid dreaming: Psychophysiological studies of consciousness during

REM sleep. In R.R. Bootsen, J.F. Kihlstrom, & D.L. Schacter (Eds.), Sleep and Cognition

(pp. 109-126). Washington, D.C.: APA Press.

LaBerge, S. (2010). Signal-verfied lucid dreaming proves that REM sleep can support reflective

consciousness. International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 26-27. Retrieved from

http://journals.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/viewFile/606/pdf

LaBerge, S., & DeGracia, D. J. (2000). Varieties of lucid dreaming experience. In R. G.

Kunzendorf & B. Wallace (Eds.), Individual differences in conscious experience (pp. 269-

307). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Page 102: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 97

LaBerge, S. & Gackenbach, J. (2000). Lucid Dreaming. In E. Cardena, S.J. Lynn & S. Krippner

(Eds.). Varieties of anomalous experience. Examining the scientific evidence. Washington,

D.C.: American Psychological Association.

LaBerge, S., Levitan, L., & Dement, W. C. (1986). Lucid dreaming: Physiological correlates of

consciousness during REM sleep. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 7(2-3), 251-258

LaBerge, S. P., Nagel, L. E., Dement, W. C., & Zarcone Jr, V. P. (1981). Lucid dreaming verifed

by volitional communication during REM sleep. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52(3),

727-732. doi: 10.2466/pms.1981.52.3.727

LaBerge, S., & Rheingold, H. (1991). Exploring the world of lucid dreaming. New York:

Ballantine Books.

Lima, B., Singer, W., & Neuenschwander, S. (2011). Gamma responses correlate with temporal

expectation in monkey primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(44),

15919-15931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0957-11.2011

Limb, C. J., & Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An

fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS One, 3(2), e1679.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001679.t001

Mahowald, M. W. (2009). What state dissociation can teach us about consciousness and the

function of sleep. Sleep medicine, 10(2), 159-160. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2008.03.003

Mahowald, M. W., & Schenck, C. H. (2005). Insights from studying human sleep disorders.

Nature, 437(7063), 1279-1285. doi:10.1038/nature04287

Maquet, P., Degueldre, C., Delfiore, G., Aerts, J., Péters, J. M., Luxen, A., & Franck, G. (1997).

Functional neuroanatomy of human slow wave sleep. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(8),

2807-2812.

Page 103: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 98

Maquet, P., Laureys, S., Peigneux, P., Fuchs, S., Petiau, C., Phillips, C., ... & Cleeremans, A.

(2000). Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep.

Nature Neuroscience, 3(8), 831. doi: 10.1038/77744

Maquet, P., Péters, J. M., Aerts, J., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Franck, G. (1996).

Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature,

383(6596), 163-166. doi:10.1038/383163a0

Maquet, P., Ruby, P., Maudoux, A., Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Dang-Vu, T., ... & Laureys, S. (2005).

Human cognition during REM sleep and the activity profile within frontal and parietal cortices: a

reappraisal of functional neuroimaging data. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 219-595.

doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50016-5

Mason, L. I., & Orme-Johnson, D. (2010). Transcendental consciousness wakes up in dreaming and

deep sleep. International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 28-32. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2010.1.595

McCarley, R.W. and Hobson, J.A. (1975) Neuronal excitability modulation over the sleep cycle:

A structural and mathematical model. Science,189, 58–60. doi:10.1126/science.1135627

Misiaszek, J. E. (2003). The H reflex as a tool in neurophysiology: Its limitations and uses in ‐

understanding nervous system function. Muscle & Nerve, 28(2), 144-160. doi:

10.1002/mus.10372

Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the

EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1(1), 455-473. doi:

10.1176/jnp.7.2.251

Moss, K. (1986). The dream lucidity continuum. Lucidity Letter, 5(2). Retrieved from

http://www.spiritwatch.ca/lucidity_1986_december.html#5.2g

Page 104: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 99

Mota-Rolim, S. A., & Araujo, J. F. (2013). Neurobiology and clinical implications of lucid

dreaming. Medical Hypotheses, 81(5), 751-756. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2013.04.049

Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 475-481. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01992-7

Niedermeyer, E., & Da Silva, F. L. (Eds.). (2004). Electroencephalography: basic principles,

clinical applications, and related fields. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Nielsen, T. A. (2000). A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep: “covert” REM sleep as

a possible reconciliation of two opposing models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(06),

851-866. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0000399X

Nir, Y., & Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to

neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 88-100.

doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001

Nishida, M., Uchida, S., Hirai, N., Miwakeichi, F., Maehara, T., Kawai, K., ... & Kato, S.

(2005). High frequency activities in the human orbitofrontal cortex in sleep–wake cycle.

Neuroscience Letters, 379(2), 110-115. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.069

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A, … & Pascual-

Leone, A. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain

Stimulation, 1(3), 206-223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex

by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Physiology, 527, 633-639. doi:

10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

Page 105: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 100

Nofzinger, E. A., Mintun, M. A., Wiseman, M., Kupfer, D. J., & Moore, R. Y. (1997). Forebrain

activation in REM sleep: an FDG PET study. Brain Research, 770(1), 192-201.

doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00807-X

Nofzinger, E. A., Nichols, T. E., Meltzer, C. C., Price, J., Steppe, D. A., Miewald, J. M., ... &

Moore, R. Y. (1999). Changes in forebrain function from waking to REM sleep in depres-

sion: preliminary analyses [of 18 F] FDG PET studies. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimag-

ing, 91(2), 59-78. doi:10.1016/S0925-4927(99)00025-6

Nofzinger, E. A., Price, J. C., Meltzer, C. C., Buysse, D. J., Villemagne, V. L., Miewald, J.

M., ... & Kupfer, D. J. (2000). Towards a neurobiology of dysfunctional arousal in depres-

sion: the relationship between beta EEG power and regional cerebral glucose metabolism

during NREM sleep. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 98(2), 71-91.

doi:10.1016/S0925-4927(00)00045-7

Noreika, V., Windt, J. M., Lenggenhager, B., & Karim, A. A. (2010). New perspectives for the

study of lucid dreaming: from brain stimulation to philosophical theories of self-

consciousness. International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 36-45.

doi:10.11588/ijodr.2010.1.586

Ogawa, K., Nittono, H., & Hori, T. (2005). Brain potentials before and after rapid eye move-

ments: an electrophysiological approach to dreaming in REM sleep. Sleep, 28(9), 1077-

1082. Retrieved from http://www.journalsleep.org/ViewAbstract.aspx?pid=26215

Ogilvie, R., Hunt, H., Kushniruk, A., & Newman, J. (1983). Lucid dreams and the arousal con-

tinuum. Sleep Research, 12, 182.

Pace-Schott, E. F. (2005). The neurobiology of dreaming. Principles and Practice of Sleep

Medicine, 5, 563-75.

Page 106: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 101

Parmeggiani, P. L. (2003). Thermoregulation and sleep. Frontiers in Bioscience, 8 (557-567).

doi: 10.2741/1054

Paulsson, T., & Parker, A. (2006). The effects of a two-week reflection-intention training

program on lucid dream recall. Dreaming, 16(1), 22. doi:10.1037/1053-0797.16.1.22

Payne, J. D. (2014). The (gamma) power to control our dreams. Nature Neuroscience, 17(6),

753-755. doi:10.1038/nn.3727

Pérez-Garci, E., Del Río-Portilla, Y., Guevara, M. A., Arce, C., & Corsi-Cabrera, M. (2001).

Paradoxical sleep is characterized by uncoupled gamma activity between frontal and

perceptual cortical regions. Sleep, 24(1), 118. Retrieved from

http://www.journalsleep.org/Articles/240114.pdf

Rechtschaffen, A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams. Sleep, 1(1), 97-109.

Retrieved from http://www.journalsleep.org/Articles/010109.pdf

Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 261-270. doi: 10.1038/nrn783

Revonsuo, A. (2006). Inner presence: Consciousness as a biological phenomenon. Cambridge,

Massachusetts: MIT Press.

Revonsuo, A. (2010). Consciousness: the science of subjectivity. New York: Psychology Press.

Revonsuo, A. (2015). Hard to See the Problem?. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 22(3-4), 52-

67.

Roffwarg, H. P., Dement, W. C., Muzio, J. N., & Fisher, C. (1962). Dream imagery: relationship

to rapid eye movements of sleep. Archives of General Psychiatry, 7(4), 235-258.

doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1962.01720040001001

Page 107: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 102

Roskies, A. L. (2010). How does neuroscience affect our conception of volition?. Annual Re-

view of Neuroscience, 33, 109-130. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153151

Schabus, M., Dang-Vu, T. T., Albouy, G., Balteau, E., Boly, M., Carrier, J., ... & Maquet, P.

(2007). Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye

movement sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(32), 13164-

13169. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703084104

Schabus, M., Dang-Vu, T. T., Heib, D., Boly, M., Desseiles, M., Vandewalle, G., ...& Maquet, P.

(2012). The fate of incoming stimuli during NREM sleep is determined by spindles and

the phase of the slow oscillation. Frontiers in Neurology, 3(40). doi:

10.3389/fneur.2012.00040

Schmitz, T. W., Kawahara-Baccus, T. N., & Johnson, S. C. (2004). Metacognitive evaluation,

self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 22(2), 941-947.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.018

Schredl, M. (2000). Body–mind interaction: Dream content and REM sleep physiology. North

American Journal of Psychology, 2(1)

Schredl, M. (2004). Reliability and stability of a Dream Recall Frequency Scale. Perceptual

and Motor Skills, 98(3c), 1422-1426. doi: 10.2466/pms.98.3c.1422-1426

Schredl, M. (2010). Dreaming and waking: Phenomenological and biological differences.

International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 46-48. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2010.1.585

Schredl, M., & Erlacher, D. (2004). Lucid dreaming frequency and personality. Personality and

Individual Differences, 37(7), 1463-1473. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.003

Page 108: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 103

Schredl, M., & Erlacher, D. (2011). Frequency of lucid dreaming in a representative German

sample. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 112(1), 104-108. doi:10.2466/09.PMS.112.1.104-

108

Schredl, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2015). Changes in Dream Recall Frequency, Nightmare Fre-

quency, and Lucid Dream Frequency Over a 3-Year Period. Dreaming.

doi:10.1037/a0039165

Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A.

(2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 319-326. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006

Schwartz, S., & Maquet, P. (2002). Sleep imaging and the neuro-psychological assessment of

dreams. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 23-30. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01818-0

Sergio, W. (1988). Use of DMAE (2-dimethylaminoethanol) in the induction of lucid dreams.

Medical Hypotheses, 26(4), 255-257. doi:10.1016/0306-9877(88)90129-6

Siegel, J. M. (2005). Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature, 437(7063), 1264-1271.

doi:10.1038/nature04285

Singer, W. (2001). Consciousness and the binding problem. Annals of the New York Academy of

Sciences, 929(1), 123-146. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05712.x

Singer, W. (2014). How Does the Finding of a Correlation Between the Three Conscious States

(REM Dream, Lucid Dream, and Waking) and 40 Hz Power Fit with Your Suggestion

That 40 Hz Is a Substrate of Consciousness?. In N. Tranquillo (Eds.) Dream

Consciousness (pp. 201-203). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

Page 109: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 104

Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1998). Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(20),

12061–12068. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC21765/

Snyder, T. J., & Gackenbach, J. (1988). Individual differences associated with lucid dreaming.

In T. Snyder & J. Gackenbach (Eds.), Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain (pp. 221-259).

New York: Springer.

Spoormaker, V., Czisch, M., & Dresler, M. (2010). Lucid and non-lucid dreaming: Thinking in

networks. International Journal of Dream Research, 3(1), 49-51. Retrieved from

http://journals.ub.uni-heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/view/597/pdf_21

Steriade, M. (2000). Corticothalamic resonance, states of vigilance and mentation.

Neuroscience, 101(2), 243-276. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00353-5

Stickgold, R., Malia, A., Fosse, R., Propper, R., & Hobson, J. A. (2001). Brain-mind states: I.

Longitudinal field study of sleep/wake factors influencing mentation report length. Sleep,

24(2), 171. Retrieved from http://www.journalsleep.org.proxy.lnu.se/ViewAbstract.aspx?

pid=25515

Stumbrys, T. (2011). Lucid dreaming: discontinuity or continuity in consciousness?.

International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 93-97. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9146

Stumbrys, T., & Erlacher, D. (2012). Lucid dreaming during NREM sleep: Two case reports.

International Journal of Dream Research, 5(2), 151-155. doi: 10.11588/ijodr.2012.2.9483

Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., Johnson, M., & Schredl, M. (2014). The phenomenology of lucid

dreaming: an online survey. The American Journal of Psychology, 127(2), 191-204. doi:

10.5406/amerjpsyc.127.2.0191

Page 110: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 105

Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., Schädlich, M., & Schredl, M. (2012). Induction of lucid dreams: A

systematic review of evidence. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1456-1475.

doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.07.003

Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2013). Testing the involvement of the prefrontal

cortex in lucid dreaming: a tDCS study. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1214-1222.

doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.005

Takahara, M., Nittono, H., & Hori, T. (2006). Effect of voluntary attention on auditory

processing during REM sleep. Sleep, 29(7), 975. Retrieved from

http://www.journalsleep.org/Articles/290716.pdf

Tart, C. (1985). What do we mean by "lucidity"?. Lucidity Letter, 4(2). Retrieved from

http://www.spiritwatch.ca/lucidity_1985_december.html#4.2c

Tassi, P., & Muzet, A. (2000). Sleep inertia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4(4), 341-353.

doi:10.1053/smrv.2000.0098

Tholey, P. (1991). Overview of the development of lucid dream research in Germany. Lucidity

Letter, 8(2), 6-30.

Thompson, E. (2015a). Dreamless Sleep, the Embodied Mind, and Consciousness - The

Relevance of a Classical Indian Debate to Cognitive Science. In T. Metzinger & J.M.

Windt (Eds). Open MIND Group. doi: 10.15502/978395857035

Thompson, E. (2015b). Waking, Dreaming, Being: Self and Consciousness in Neuroscience,

Meditation, and Philosophy. USA: Columbia University Press.

Tononi, G. (2009). Sleep and Dreaming. In S. Laureys & G. Tononi (Eds.). The neurology of

consciousness: cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology (pp. 89-107). London:

Academic Press.

Tranquillo, N. (2014). Dream Consciousness. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

Page 111: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 106

Van Eeden, F. (1913). A study of dreams. In Proceedings of the Society for Psychical Research,

26 (47), 431-461. Retrieved from http://dreamscience.ca/en/documents/New

%20content/lucid%20dreaming%20pdfs/vanEeden_PSPR_26_1-12_1913.pdf

Van Gulick, R. (2014). Consciousness. In E.N. Zalda (Eds.). The Stanford Encyclopedia of

Philosophy. Retrieved from

http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2014/entries/consciousness

Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a

frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of

Neurophysiology, 100(6), 3328-3342. doi:10.1152/jn.90355.2008

Vogelsong, J. (2013). A Conversation with Allan Hobson. International Journal of Dream

Research, 6(1), 3-7. doi:10.11588/ijodr.2013.1.9539

Voss, U. (2010). Lucid dreaming: Reflections on the role of introspection. International Journal

of Dream Research, 3(1), 52-53. Retrieved from http://journals.ub.uni-

heidelberg.de/index.php/IJoDR/article/view/598/pdf_22

Voss, U., Frenzel, C., Koppehele-Gossel, J., & Hobson, A. (2012). Lucid dreaming: an age‐

dependent brain dissociation. Journal of Sleep Research, 21(6), 634-642. doi:

10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01022.x

Voss, U. & Hobson, A. (2015). What is the state-of-the-art on lucid dreaming? - Recent

advances and questions for future research. In T. Metzinger & J.M. Windt (Eds.) Open

MIND: 38(T). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group. doi: 10.15502/9783958570306

Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Hobson, A., Paulus, W., Koppehele-Gossel, J., Klimke, A., & Nitsche,

M. A. (2014). Induction of self awareness in dreams through frontal low current

stimulation of gamma activity. Nature Neuroscience. doi:10.1038/nn.3719

Page 112: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 107

Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Tuin, I., & Hobson, J. A. (2009). Lucid dreaming: a state of

consciousness with features of both waking and non-lucid dreaming. Sleep, 32(9), 1191.

Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2737577/

Voss, U., Schermelleh-Engel, K., Windt, J., Frenzel, C., & Hobson, A. (2013). Measuring

consciousness in dreams: the lucidity and consciousness in dreams scale. Consciousness

and Cognition, 22(1), 8-21. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.11.001

Voss, U., & Voss, G. (2014). A neurobiological model of lucid dreaming. In R. Hurd & K.

Bulkeley (Eds.), Lucid Dreaming: New Perspectives on Consciousness in Sleep (pp. 23-

36). California: Praeger.

Watson, D. (2001). Dissociations of the night: Individual differences in sleep-related

experiences and their relation to dissociation and schizotypy. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 110(4), 526. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.4.526

Wehrle, R., Czisch, M., Kaufmann, C., Wetter, T. C., Holsboer, F., Auer, D. P., & Pollmächer, T.

(2005). Rapid eye movement-related brain activation in human sleep: a functional

magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroReport, 16(8), 853-857. doi:10.1097/00001756-

200505310-00015

Wehrle, R., Kaufmann, C., Wetter, T. C., Holsboer, F., Auer, D. P., Pollmächer, T., & Czisch, M.

(2007). Functional microstates within human REM sleep: first evidence from fMRI of a

thalamocortical network specific for phasic REM periods. European Journal of

Neuroscience, 25(3), 863-871. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05314.x

Werth, E., Achermann, P., & Borbély, A. A. (1997). Fronto-occipital EEG power gradients in

human sleep. Journal of Sleep Research, 6(2), 102. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.d01-

36.x

Windt, J. M. (2013). Reporting dream experience: Why (not) to be skeptical about dream

reports. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00708

Page 113: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 108

Zadra, A. L., Donderi, D. C., & Pihl, R. O. (1992). Efficacy of lucid dream induction for lucid

and non-lucid dreamers. Dreaming, 2(2), 85. doi:10.1037/h0094350

Page 114: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 109

Appendix A

Interview questions regarding dream recall and frequency of lucid dreams (Voss et al., 2012, p.

635)

Page 115: Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation849789/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness: A Theoretical Investigation ... Lucid Dreaming and Consciousness:

LUCID DREAMING AND CONSCIOUSNESS 110

Appendix B

Lucidity and Consciousness in Dreams (LuCiD) Scale (Voss et al., 2013, p. 12)