morphological and behavioural variation in ants:...

46
2015 UNIVERSIDADE DE LISBOA FACULDADE DE CIÊNCIAS DEPARTAMENTO DE BIOLOGIA ANIMAL Morphological and Behavioural Variation in Ants: Comparing species, castes and individuals Mestrado em Biologia Evolutiva e do Desenvolvimento Andreia Filipa Eusébio Vincent Teixeira Dissertação orientada por: Patrícia Beldade | Instituto Gulbenkian de Ciência Sara Magalhães | Faculdade de Ciências da Universidade de Lisboa

Upload: dinhduong

Post on 28-Sep-2018

235 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

2015

UNIVERSIDADE DE LISBOA

FACULDADE DE CIÊNCIAS

DEPARTAMENTO DE BIOLOGIA ANIMAL

Morphological and Behavioural Variation in Ants:

Comparing species, castes and individuals

Mestrado em Biologia Evolutiva e do Desenvolvimento

Andreia Filipa Eusébio Vincent Teixeira

Dissertação orientada por:

Patrícia Beldade | Instituto Gulbenkian de Ciência

Sara Magalhães | Faculdade de Ciências da Universidade de Lisboa

2015

UNIVERSIDADE DE LISBOA

FACULDADE DE CIÊNCIAS

DEPARTAMENTO DE BIOLOGIA ANIMAL

Morphological and Behavioural Variation in Ants:

Comparing species, castes and individuals

Mestrado em Biologia Evolutiva e do Desenvolvimento

Andreia Filipa Eusébio Vincent Teixeira

Dissertação orientada por:

Patrícia Beldade | Instituto Gulbenkian de Ciência

Sara Magalhães | Faculdade de Ciências da Universidade de Lisboa

First I would like to thank Patrícia for her patience throughout this year that I spent in her lab.

I would like to thank Roberto for all the teaching and discussion about this amazing non-model

organism and also for encouraging me to pursue what I want as a scientist. Thank you for

showing me the museum specimens’ utility and its importance for science. And especially for

making me want to keep my own insect collection!

Also want to thank professor Élio for his precious time spent on teaching me how to design

degenerated primers and answering to (sometimes not so) useful questions.

And thank Sara Magalhães for all the support and advice in this final effort.

I want also to thank to all VDS lab for all the support and for teaching me how to work as part

of a team.

To all Evo-Devo community at the IGC for providing me the most inspiring year that I could

ever had.

I want to thank all my friends for their enormous patience and support during a very

complicated year, and also apologize for the lack of attention. To Mariana and Miguel for every

laugh they pulled out of me. To Melinho that always remembered me what I can do and above

all what I should do. To Vasco for remembering me that not only scientists-wannabe have a

difficult life. To Barateiro for all the hugs and loud discussions, not only about science.

To my dearest friend Gonçalo that have always been an example and always had the time to

ask how I was doing, even being 16000km and 9 hours in time zone away.

In last I would like to thank my two musketeers for the companionship during this troubled

journey. Thanks to Ana Catarina and Ana Sofia for all the laughs, brain-farts and late hours

spent (not only) at that institute (not always) working.

Especially to Ana Sofia that listened to me so many times, even when she doesn’t want to.

Honestly I don’t know what to say.

In last I what to thank my heroine Filomena, my sweet grandmother that always supported me

and helped me to accomplish my objectives and finish this long journey. And to my

grandfather Augusto, that unfortunately won’t be able to see me closing this chapter of my

life. I wish you were here.

If I could probably these acknowledgements would be bigger than my thesis.

Abstract

Environment plays a fundamental role in living organisms’ adaptation and selection.

Developmental plasticity allows individuals to sense the surrounding environment and produce

different phenotypes in response to biotic and abiotic cues. Individuals are exposed to these

cues during larval phase and transduce it to shape and size adaptations in the adult. Social

insects are a good (non-)model organism to study developmental plasticity since they present

dramatic castes differences. The reason behind ants’ ecological success is division of labour

and each colony is divided in groups according to specific tasks, like reproduction and colony

defence. Morphology appeared early as a caste defining factor, but soon age and physiology

were included in the list. Extreme dimorphism between queens and workers demonstrated

that morphology and behaviour should not be considered independent traits and species

whose worker caste is divided in sub-castes highlighted that more attention should be paid to

within caste variation and specializations. Morphology is determined by growth during

development and thus regulated by juvenile hormone and ecdysone, which together can

reprogram metamorphosis timing. In this work we compare species, castes and individuals

morphology and can have an overview of how much variation is found between and within

groups. Within worker castes we performed a detailed analysis on morphology and behaviour

and found some task division signatures, even in monomorphic castes. Finally we simulated

environmental cues by manipulating endogenous hormonal levels and to know if a

monomorphic species is able to produce intermediate phenotypes when exposed to the same

conditions as dimorphic species. Our morphometric analysis demonstrated that we could find

more differences between than within castes, and also allowed us to suggest the existence of

more caste-related traits in contrast with other more species-related. Intra-caste analysis

suggested the existence of task-specific behavioural groups within monomorphic colonies.

Hormonal manipulation results were affected by the nature of compounds used and also by

the absence of a specific dosage-response curve for species used. However we could also

address some colony specificity in the response to treatments, and differential sensibility to

these.

Key words: behavioural castes, caste determination, developmental plasticity, juvenile

hormone, monomorphic.

Resumo

A morfologia afecta todos os aspectos do ciclo de vida de um organismo. Há uma relação

muito próxima entre o tamanho e forma de um determinado indivíduo e o comportamento

que este adquire num determinado ambiente. O comportamento de cada indivíduo vai

também afectar e moldar o ambiente em que este é seleccionado, tornando todas estas

interacções mais complexas. Contudo alguns indivíduos conseguem ajustar o seu

desenvolvimento e fenótipo ao ambiente em que se encontram, e esta plasticidade fenotípica

permite que um indivíduo, a partir de um genoma, possa produzir diferentes fenótipos de

acordo com os estímulos a que é exposto.

Os estímulos ambientais que provocam alterações no fenótipo do adulto só poderão ser

efectivos se acontecerem em períodos de tempo específico durante o desenvolvimento, em

que o indivíduo é capaz de sentir e traduzir o que se passa no ambiente envolvente e activar

uma via que produz o fenótipo mais adequado. Estes estímulos podem ser bióticos, como a

presença ou ausência de predadores para as Daphnia e a densidade populacional para os

gafanhotos; ou abióticos, como a temperatura em Byciclus e a nutrição na produção de castas

em insectos sociais.

Os Hymenoptera são um grupo maioritariamente constituído por insectos sociais, e foi a sua

enorme diversidade morfológica e comportamental que permitiu a sua adaptação a vários

nichos ecológicos. Um dos factores que mais contribuiu para o sucesso ecológico deste grupo

foi a divisão de tarefas, isto é, cada grupo de indivíduos dentro de uma colónia especializar-se-

á numa determinada tarefa, defesa, forrageio ou responsáveis por cuidar das larvas, por

exemplo. Este grupo ao qual pertencem as vespas, abelhas, térmitas e formigas tem sido

muito útil para perceber como é que esta divisão de tarefas causou o aparecimento de castas.

As formigas têm um sistema haplo-diplóide de determinação de sexo, em que todos os ovos

fertilizados são diplóides e por isso originam fêmeas, enquanto os ovos não fertilizados são

haplóides e originarão machos. Para além do comportamento e sazonalidade que se

encontram associados a cada sexo, as formigas apresentam castas e, por vezes, sub-castas, o

que as torna um modelo adequado para responder a questões relacionadas com plasticidade e

de que forma esta pode ser moldada durante o desenvolvimento.

As colónias são quase sempre constituídas por fêmeas, produzidas por rainhas fertilizadas, e

um dos típicos exemplos do sistema de castas é a diferença de fenótipos que podemos

encontrar entre as rainhas e as obreiras. Ao serem responsáveis por tarefas tão distintas, que

envolvem um consumo energético tão diferente, estes indivíduos permitiram que a selecção

actuasse sobre os fenótipos que melhor se adequavam a cada função. No entanto sabendo

que as fêmeas são geneticamente idênticas podemos afirmar que o desenvolvimento larvar é

o períodos-chave para que o indivíduo identifique os estímulos que o rodeiam e produza o

fenótipo que melhor se adapta ao ambiente.

As alterações ao desenvolvimento são induzidas pela nutrição, e no caso das formigas

sabemos que o que provoca esta alteração é a quantidade de comida que as larvas recebem. A

nutrição vai afectar os níveis hormonais endógenos, neste caso em particular da hormona

juvenil, e uma das consequências é a alteração do limite de peso crítico necessário para que o

indivíduo sofra metamorfose. As manipulações hormonais em larvas de insectos sociais foram

o primeiro passo para compreender como é que estes sinais são interpretados pelo indivíduo e

quais são os processos envolvidos na produção de fenótipos tão distintos. O período larvar

está dividido em cinco estádios: primeiro, segundo e terceiro estádios larvares, pré-pupa e

pupa; e é no final deste período que a metamorfose ocorre. A determinação de castas ocorre

nas fases iniciais, enquanto as especializações associadas a sub-castas de obreiras são

determinadas nas fases mais tardias.

As sub-castas também terão surgido em resposta à selecção, produzindo morfologias ou

secreções particulares de acordo com a função mais necessária à colónia. Isto conduziu à

classificação da espécies de acordo com a variação que podemos encontrar em tamanho e

forma dentro da casta das obreiras, podendo estas serem classificadas como monomórficas,

dimórficas ou polimórficas. As espécies monomórficas apresentam uma ligeira variação em

tamanho entre os indivíduos; as dimórficas apresentam fenótipos muito distintos,

considerados como unidades discretas em resposta a um determinado gradiente ambiental; e

as polimórficas têm uma distribuição muito ampla de tamanho e forma dos indivíduos ao

longo desse mesmo gradiente. É importante perceber qual o grau de plasticidade associado a

cada um destes casos e se os fenótipos produzidos se devem à não exposição a determinados

estímulos ou se o genoma foi seleccionado para responder apenas quando necessário. Será

que diferentes graus de dimorfismo se devem a diferentes sensibilidades aos estímulos

ambientais?

Para respondermos a este tipo de questões devemos primeiro focar-nos na caracterização

detalhada, morfológica e comportamental, das espécies, castas e colónias. Estudos anteriores

demonstraram que o tamanho e forma dos indivíduos são influenciados não só pelo ambiente

mas também pela colónia em que estes se inserem. Isto relembra a importância de estudos

descritivos e comparativos no que respeita à compreensão das bases evolutivas.

Neste estudo fazemos uma comparação morfológica detalhada entre espécies, castas, colónias

e indivíduos, aprofundando os conhecimentos relativamente à variação que pode ser

encontrada e associada a cada um destes níveis de organização. No entanto sabemos também

que a divisão de tarefas e de castas depende não só da fisiologia do indivíduo em fase larvar

mas também em fase adulta. Para além das habituais castas morfológicas podemos também

referir castas comportamentais, que podem ser ou não dependentes da idade dos indivíduos.

O nosso grupo de espécies analisadas incluiu indivíduos com uma casta de obreiras

monomórfica disponíveis em laboratório, Aphaenogaster senilis, e por isso analisámos em

maior detalhe a sua morfologia e comportamento na tentativa de encontrar possíveis sub-

grupos associados à divisão de tarefas. A relação entre a distribuição preferencial dos

indivíduos e a sua complexa rede de interacções já se demonstrou fundamental para a

organização das colónias, por isso usámos como indicador de divisão de tarefas a localização

dos indivíduos na colónia. Verificámos que embora seja possível encontrar algumas diferenças

morfológicas entre indivíduos que se encontram fora e dentro do formigueiro, é relativamente

ao comportamento que se pode sugerir uma real divisão de tarefas nesta espécie. Mostramos

que há uma forte tendência para que indivíduos encontrados fora do formigueiro se

mantenham lá, sendo os possíveis responsáveis pelo forrageio e recolha de comida para a

colónia.

Outra questão importante é a capacidade de espécies monomórficas responderem a um

gradiente de estímulos ambientais que afecte os indivíduos em diferentes fases do período

larvar. Poderão alterações em fases mais tardias resultar no surgimento de fenótipos ou

características intermédias entre as típicas castas? Manipulámos directamente os níveis da

hormona juvenil nas larvas de A. senilis e os nossos resultados demonstram uma ausência de

resposta por parte dos indivíduos. No entanto, a elevada taxa de mortalidade associada aos

nossos tratamentos, a ausência de curvas de dosagem para os compostos utilizados e a

incerteza acerca da efectividade das nossas manipulações representam possíveis problemas e

dificultam explicações para os nossos resultados.

Este trabalho demonstra a importância que os trabalhos descritivos e comparativos continuam

a ter, bem como a relevância que o estudo da morfologia e comportamento dos organismos

mantêm numa era que favorece visões mais abrangentes e questões mais gerais, tendo como

base a genómica e a epigenética. Compreender a história natural dos nossos organismos

modelo, e principalmente dos nossos organismos não-modelo, continua a ser fundamental

para completar e integrar o que nos permitem concluir trabalhos com uma base molecular.

Palavras-chave: castas comportamentais, determinação de castas, hormona juvenil,

monomórfica, plasticidade fenotípica.

8

Table of contents

Introduction ................................................................................................................ 9

Materials and Methods ........................................................................................... 13

Results and Discussion ............................................................................................ 17

Morphological variation between species, castes and colonies .......................... 17

Inter-specific variation ..................................................................................... 18

Intra-specific variation ..................................................................................... 20

Lasius sp. ............................................................................................... 20

Messor barbarus .................................................................................... 21

Aphaenogaster senilis ............................................................................ 22

Morphological and behavioural intra-caste variation ........................................ 22

Morphological analysis .................................................................................... 23

Behavioural analysis ........................................................................................ 24

Hormonal manipulation effects .......................................................................... 26

Survival effect .................................................................................................. 27

Developmental time ........................................................................................ 29

Induced morphological variation...................................................................... 29

Final Remarks and Future Perspectives ................................................................. 34

Bibliography ............................................................................................................. 36

Annex 1 ..................................................................................................................... 38

Support material – Tables, schemes and images .................................................. 38

Annex 2 ..................................................................................................................... 42

Statistical analysis summary ................................................................................ 42

Annex 3 ..................................................................................................................... 43

Hormonal manipulation effects ............................................................................ 43

9

Introduction

Body shape and size are crucial properties of multicellular organisms. They both condition and

reflect modes of life and affect a series of other traits, including behavioural and life-history

traits. They are also highly diversified, with sometimes very extreme variation both within and

between species. Size and shape evolve in response to various selective pressures [1-5]. The

evolutionary potential of these traits is translated from an intricate relationship between

morphology, behaviour and environment [5]. Often an organism’s morphology influences its

behavioural repertoire and can itself affect the environment where individuals are selected.

These changes in the environment can, in turn, lead to morphological specializations and

behaviour adjustments. Morphological studies lost some attention along the years and are

constantly being replaced by detailed analysis at the molecular level, including genetic and

epigenetic characterizations. It is unfortunate that many recent studies seem to overlook the

importance of morphological descriptions that keep up with the detail and sophistication of

molecular analysis that is now standard [6].

Like is typical of quantitative traits, variation in body shape and size depends both on genetic

and environmental factors and on the interactions between them [3, 7]. Especially in insects we

can find several examples of how the external environment can influence shape and size.

Environmental factors that affect the development of body shape and size can be biotic or

abiotic, and include nutrition as a prime example [7]. In dung beetles, for example, nutrition

during larval development affects adult body and male horn size, as well as reproductive

strategies [8]. Phenotypic plasticity is a phenomenon through which the same genotype can

produce different phenotypes in response to external environmental cues and it is very

common in nature. Numerous studies reported examples where organismal developmental

trajectories and outcomes are affected by various external environmental factors, including

temperature, presence or absence of predators, population density and nutrition, as referred

[9].

Social insects, ants in particular, have long been prime examples in evolutionary and

developmental studies, including in relation to behavioural and morphological traits. Even

Charles Darwin looked for the existence of fertile and sterile groups within species as a test for

his theory [10]. Ants’ ecological dominance in terrestrial ecosystems and extreme evolutionary

diversification contrasts with the difficulties inherent to not being classical lab models with

limited analytical tools and resources [11]. Targets of many biological questions regarding social

insects’ biology still care for an answer.

10

Ants have a haplo-diploid sex determination system, where all diploid eggs develop into

females and haploid eggs develop into males [11]. Generally only queens have structural and

behavioural support to fertilization, which includes numerous ovarioles, spermatheca – to

store sperm – and wings, used in conspicuous mating flights. Consequently, generally, only

queens are able to produce diploid eggs. Haploid eggs can be laid by queens and/or workers,

depending on the species of ants. Some species have workers with reproductive potential,

called gamergates, which are responsible by haploid eggs production [12].

Ants and other social insects are characterized by a complex social organization with division of

labour between different groups of individuals within colonies, each one with particular

morphology and function, called castes. Queens are generally winged and ensure the colony

reproduction, while workers are responsible for colony maintenance, including tasks such as

foraging, cleaning and brood care [11]. Queens and workers typically differ in total body size,

albeit not as dramatically in all species, and differ also in body shape [11, 13]. Particularly,

differences in thoracic organization have been described and associated to the specialized

tasks of each caste. In queens we observe an enlarged T2, where the muscles that power the

wings are inserted, in relation to T1 and T3. Workers, on the other hand have a relatively

enlarged first thoracic segment, T1, where the muscles that power head movements are [5].

Moreover, queens of different species can be separated based on T1/T2 ratios in a manner

that associates with whether queens do or not forage when establishing new colonies [5].

Aside striking differences between queens and workers, in many ants there are also sub-

groups or sub-castes of workers with distinct morphologies associated with distinct functions

in the colony life. A good example are some Pheidole species where minor and major workers

have extremely different phenotypes, and further sub-division of major in soldiers or super-

soldiers, castes produced specifically for colony defence [14,15]. On the other hand

Pogonomyrmex, Solenopsis and Messor are characterized by extensive size and shape variation

within the worker caste and thus classified as polymorphic species [16-19]. This wide range of

variation is opposite to what was described for species with discrete castes, where variation

presents modest values [20]. Some ants’ species can even present intermediate characteristics

or inter-castes, presenting individuals with a “hybrid phenotype” between queen and worker

[21].

Worker polymorphisms have been used in allometry studies to better understand how growth

rates can differ between body parts. For example, in Solenopsis species worker size increases

with increasing colony size, but some body parts grow disproportionally [18, 20]. These

differential growth rates within single individuals will allow morphological differentiation, later

resulting in function specializations and division of labour within workers [16]. While division of

11

labour is associated to morphological specializations of castes and sub-castes, the latter is not

strictly necessary for the former. Studies have found that even morphologically monomorphic

castes can still have behavioural and functionally specialized sub-groups [10, 16]. For example,

morphologically monomorphic sub-castes include temporal castes, in which different tasks are

ensured by individuals of different ages [22]. This seems to be more common among

Hymenoptera than morphological castes. Age polyethism has been described in many species

and usually entails that younger workers are responsible for feeding and nursing and spend

most of the time inside the nest, while older workers are responsible for foraging outside the

nest [10, 22]. Aside temporal castes, morphologically monomorphic division of labour can also be

associated to glandular morphology or secretory products and levels [22]. Examples of such

“physiological castes” include specialized pathfinders in Pharaoh ants (Monomorium

pharaonic) and specialized brood-care workers in ponerine ants (Ectatomma tuberculatum) [22,

23]. Finally, in some cases complex patterns and networks of group interactions within a colony

can also contribute to some hierarchic division of labour that does not associate with

morphologically distinct sub-castes [24].

Although the lack of information on sociometry and sociogenesis is considerably high and

evolutionary origin of castes and sub-castes remains a topic of discussion, much has been

discovered regarding developmental plasticity in social insects [25]. Several studies

demonstrated that castes are a product of differential feeding and consequential different

pathways activated during development in response to internal hormonal levels [14, 26-27].

Ants are holometabolous insects and their development is divided in three larval instars

corresponding to the growth phase before metamorphosis [26]. After eclosion body size

remains unchangeable throughout adulthood [10, 26]. It is the quantity and/or quality of

nutrition during the larval phase that determines body size and also caste differentiation.

Nutrition determines caste formation mediated by juvenile hormone levels and DNA

methylation [27-29]. Most studies underlying DNA methylation and changes in expression were

developed in honeybees but some recent works confirmed an important role of caste-specific

methylomes in ants [28]. It is known that nutrition directly impacts hormone endogenous levels

[26]. Larval neurosecretory system translates endocrine dynamics to specific developmental

time points and thresholds defining the minimal conditions required to undergo

metamorphosis [14, 26]. Juvenile phase maintenance is sustained by juvenile hormone while

larvae – pupa – adult transformations are held by ecdysone. JH is produced in corpora allata,

near the brain, and it will be the responsible for developmental time delay, being thus involved

in developmental switches needed for caste determination [30-32]. These developmental

switches can occur earlier or later in development having different effects on phenotypes and

12

it depends on the species. Lab studies that artificially lead to increase (using methoprene, a JH

analogue) or decrease (using precocene) of JH levels, independently of nutrition, have allowed

characterization of the sensitive time points and threshold levels [14, 26]. Precocene will affect

corpora allata cells and decrease JH secretion, meaning that when applied it will induce

precocious metamorphosis and consequently miniature versions of adult insects [33].

Specifically precocene II was proved to be effective on Solenopsis invicta dealation process,

which means it is interfering also with adult individual maturation processes naturally induced

by JH [34]. Environmentally-induced caste determination, between queens and workers, occurs

during first and second larval instar and sub-castes are determined in late larval instars, usually

after individuals were already determined as workers. Only well-fed larvae are able to develop

into queens, since high nutrient intake induces corpora allata to secrete more JH [26].

Reprogramming in late larval instars will allow individuals to grow more and for longer periods,

and will allow them to allocate resources to specific structures breaking the default worker

developmental program [26].

Many studies focused on characterizing the morphology and underlying development of

different castes and sub-castes of ants. Much less attention has been given to intra-caste

differences and to plasticity in males. Here we propose to understand not only how

morphology changes both in size and shape across species with different ecologies but also to

analyse in detail the morphological and behavioural variation present within castes and

colonies. Using different species, each one displaying a different dimorphism degree, will also

allow us to better understand morphological variation according to organizational system with

emphasis on monomorphic species. We will also perform hormonal manipulation experiments

to adress monomorphic species response to artificial environmental gradients and preliminary

observations of haploid genomes response to similar cues.

13

Materials and methods

Biological material

We focused on three ant species with different degrees of female plasticity, reflected also in

extent of queen-worker size dimorphism: Aphaenogaster senilis, with a monomorphic worker

caste; Messor barbarus representing a stronger queen-worker size dimorphism and

polymorphic worker caste; and Lasius sp. also with a strong queen-worker size dimorphism but

with a monomorphic worker caste. All colonies were collected in the vicinity of the host

institution (Oeiras, Portugal – see Annex 1, Table S1). For A. senilis, we maintained colonies in

the laboratory, while for the other two species wild-caught individuals were collected and

immediately analysed. We analysed queen and worker morphology for all three species, and

did experiments in female larvae for A. senilis (Annex 1, Table S1).

All A. senilis lab colonies were kept in incubators under controlled conditions, 27°C, 60%

humidity and a 12:12 hours of light:dark cycle. Colonies were feed with frozen Drosophila and

pieces of orange each two days, with sugar cubes and water provided ad libitum. They were

placed in plastic boxes of 40 x 30 x 30cm and the boxes inside “walls” were covered with fluon,

to prevent ants being able to escape. Each box contained a nest simulator with two

compartments of cylindrical shape (10 x 6cm each one) both made of dark material to provide

darkness and separated from each other by a grid. The ants with their larvae were on the top

compartment, from which they could access the outside for foraging, and the bottom

compartment was kept full of water to provide high humidity (scheme on Fig. S1A, Annex 1).

A. senilis colonies were kept in queen-right and/or queen-less status to control larvae

production. Queen-less colonies were either obtained from the field or generated in the lab by

splitting field-collected queen-right colonies in two groups with the approximate same number

of workers, one with the queen laying fertilized eggs that will develop to be diploid larvae and

later adult females, and one without a queen or juveniles where workers were able to lay

unfertilized eggs that will produce haploid larvae and adult males.

Morphological measurements

We measured a series of morphological traits to assess differences between species, castes,

colonies and individuals (un-manipulated individuals) and between hormonal treatments (only

for females). We measured different body parts and traits for each we expected differences

between castes and species. We measured the length of the first (T1) and second (T2) thoracic

lengths and the total thorax length for each individual, known to distinguish queens from

14

workers as well as different type of queens [5]. We measured length and width of heads to

assess size and shape (length/width) of this structure. Detailed information about

measurements and traits used can be found on Table 2, below. Unfortunately, we could not

always measure all traits in all individuals. Measurements were done with ImageJ 1.48v on

photographs taken of body parts of individual ants. For image acquisition we used a JVC digital

camera mounted on a Leica MZ16 microscope and magnification was adjusted not only to

specific structures but also depending on the species we measured. Detailed information

about measurements and figures illustrating the correct structure/body positioning for

imaging can also be found in Annex 1, fig. S1 C and fig. S2 A-C.

Behavioural assays

To test for potential task division between workers of a species with a monomorphic worker

caste, we tracked the location of individuals found inside and outside the nest in three of our

lab queen-right colonies of A. senilis. In each colony we marked 10-15 individuals found inside

or outside the nest with a different colour dot applied with a toothpick to the dorsal side of the

first thoracic segment: silver for individuals inside the nest and green for individuals outside

the nest. To discard any possible ink effects on colony behaviour, all individuals were marked

at the same time, returned to the original colony and observed in regular intervals of 15

minutes during 2 hours before starting behavioural observations. Ink effects were discarded

based on observation of unchanged behaviour in marked individuals and no rejection by the

rest of the colony (non-marked individuals). We observed 20 to 30 marked individuals during

four consecutive days for day period observations and during one day for night period

Table 2 – Detailed information about measured traits, analysed species and total number of individuals measured for each trait (queens and workers from different colonies). A – A. senilis, M – M. barbarus and L – Lasius sp.; colour code correspondent to colours used on Fig.1.

15

observations. Day period observations were done in the afternoon, and we did five

observations per day in 1 hour intervals. In night period we did four observations divided in 30

minutes intervals. During the observations we took note of the individuals’ location within the

colony by counting number of green and silver dots seen outside the nest (to avoid

disturbances caused by opening a nest). To correct for total number of individuals we also

counted and removed green and silver-marked dead individuals.

Hormonal manipulations

We did manipulations of JH hormones on A. senilis diploid larvae (produced by fertilized

queens and isolated from our queen-right colonies).

For the JH manipulation experiments we staged larvae by size as well as morphology and size

of their mandibles and hairs [35-37]. We decided to manipulate individuals only on second larval

instar (L2) once it has been described as the stage limit for larvae bipotency – able to produce

a worker or a queen development [38].

Three treatments were done by topically applying 1µl of one of three different solutions to

each larva abdominal part (see scheme on Fig. S1 B, Annex 1 [15]. The three treatments

corresponded to: 1) control (CTR) with application of acetone, the organic solvent where the

other compounds were dissolved; 2) a JH- treatment with application of precocene II (Santa

Cruz Biotechnology), an antagonist of juvenile hormone that have a cytotoxic effect on corpora

allata cells decreasing JH production [26]; and 3) a JH+ treatment with application of

methoprene (Sigma-Aldrich), a JH agonist that mimic the original molecule and is able to

activate JH-mediated pathways, reprogramming critical size and delaying metamorphosis

timing [15]. Both methoprene and precocene II were dissolved on acetone to a range of

concentrations, but we only obtained adult individuals from two of the concentrations used,

one for each compound. In JH+ treatment we applied a 5µg/µl concentration, reported as

effective concentration in production of major workers in Pheidole bicarinata, for example [15];

and in JH- treatment we opted for a 2µg/µl concentration, considered appropriate after

experiments where higher concentration of precocene II inhibits dealation in adults in

Solenopsis invicta, correspondent to a delay on reproductive maturation process [34].

In each manipulation round, we collected larvae from one of our colonies, selected those of

the target stage and split them into three groups with the approximate same number of

larvae, one per treatment. After the treatment, the larvae of each group were transferred into

separate small plastic boxes (20 x 20 x 7cm), with 20 workers from the same source colony and

left to develop until adulthood.

16

After eclosion adults were kept in 70% ethanol for measurements.

Data analysis

All analyses were performed with R, version 3.2.2. For all analyses, residuals of the models

have been tested for normality using Shapiro-Wilk (alpha = 0.05), and equality of variances

using Levene’s test. When comparing variances between groups we can quantify the

difference between groups. This data together with distribution plots for each trait allow us to

suggest which group presents more or less variation for that specific trait. We used Levene’s

test to know if we can group colonies when comparing species, based on their homogeneity of

variances. All Levene’s test results are summarized in Table S3. In the first part of results,

relative to morphological variation within species, and between castes and colonies, we used

Kruskal-Wallis test for all traits, after finding some of them that did not meet normal

distribution assumption. We used simple models to test each trait within species (trait~colony

or trait~caste) and between workers of different species. Unbalanced samples do not allow us

to compare every group of individuals in inter-species comparisons, so we selected three

groups of workers with approximate same number of individuals and tested each trait in

function of species (trait~species). For A. senilis we tested whether traits differed between

workers found inside versus outside the nest (trait~location) for both colonies (A1 and A2)

using Wilcoxon test.

Behavioural data showed normal distribution and was analysed using ANOVA (analysis of

variance) to test effect of colour – correspondent to their location when marked – on their

“preferential” location during observation. For our statistical analysis we compared the

probability of finding a specific group of individuals outside, testing a general linear model

(location~observation+colony*colour) and analysing deviance for each period of the day

separately.

Hormonal manipulations comparisons depended on trait. Pearson’s Chi-squared test was used

to test mortality differences between colonies. Developmental time and morphological traits

were tested for possible effects of treatment, colony and their interaction

(trait~colony*treatment) using ANOVA. When significant differences were found, we used

Tukey’s post-hoc pairwise comparisons to establish which groups differed from each other. For

each group of individuals, in this specific case each treatment within colonies, different letters

indicate pairwise comparisons that revealed statistically significant differences.

17

Results and Discussion

Morphological variation between species, castes and colonies

To better understand the morphological variation between ant groups we decided to start

with comparisons between species, castes and colonies. In this first figure panel we wanted to

highlight the differences found in size and shape between and within the three species used,

between and within castes from each of these species and also between colonies and

individuals within castes. We should mention that numerical labels used to distinguish colonies

in different results topics are merely informative to each one of them and does not always

correspond to true colonies ID; these can be found on Annex 1, Table S1.

We should underlie that our Y axis changes its range depending on each trait. In annex 1 we

present some of the images used for each trait measurement, and you can confirm that we are

not expecting the same scale for every trait across castes and species.

Fig. 1 – Morphological differences between species, castes, colonies and individuals. A) Comparison between first (T1) and second (T2) thoracic segment proportions relative to total thorax length (TL), each colour represents species and different symbols represents queens and workers, ◊ and ○ respectively; B) Proportion of T1 relative to T2, for queens and workers from different species, slight colour variation in castes represents individuals from different colonies, correspondence

18

between colours and species maintained for every figure in the panel. C-E) Thoracic structures variation, C) T1 variation, D) T2 variation and E) TL variation across individuals, where ◊ and ○ represents queens and workers, respectively. F-H) Head size and shape variation, F) head length variation, G) head width variation and H) head length/head width across individuals. From C-H) Q and W represents the different castes and the following numbers represent each colony. All traits were measured in mm. Only significant statistical differences are represented, for colonies within (black line) and between (grey dashed line) species; p-value<0.05 *, p-value<0.01 **, p-value<0.001 *** and p-value<0.0001 ****.

Inter-specific variation

Figure 1A and 1B are relative to thoracic segments and were used to understand how first, T1,

and second, T2, thoracic segments can be related and how their proportions change, not only

between castes but also between species. It has been shown that thorax size and shape are

adaptive traits, responsive to caste-specific functions but also to species ecology [5]. Knowing

that queens can be separated in two types according to foraging activity we represented our

data on fig. 1A, enabling distinction between different queen types across species. Proportions

of first and second thoracic segments relative to individuals’ total thorax length – T1/TL and

T2/TL ratios – allowed the division of individuals in two groups. We can observe it specifically

between queen castes, with Aphaenogaster senilis in one group and Messor barbarus and

Lasius sp. in a second group. As demonstrated in Keller et al. we expected that our three

species queens fall in the “reduced T1” queen category, according to their ecology and colony

foundation type. However we can see sub-division within this group and A. senilis fall

somewhere in a “less reduced” T1 queen category, where the investment between neck

strength and flight musculature is balanced, while the other two species insert in the typical

“reduced T1” type of queens, investing in flight and storage musculature. This modest position

for A. senilis could be explained by their apparently primitive condition, characteristic of

species where queens are close to workers in size and the groups are similar in some traits [16].

Distribution and analysis of T1/TL (H(2)T1/TL = 8.42, p-value = 0.0148) and T2/TL in workers,

compared between three worker groups, suggests that thoracic architecture and proportions

are not only caste-specific but also species-specific, in accordance to what was verified in

queens. This can be related to highly conserved tasks and associated behaviour in workers,

independent from their species [5].

In fig. 1B we emphasize the distinction observed between castes thoracic structures, and as

expected we can observe a clear distinction in thorax shape due to first thoracic segment size

relative to second thoracic segment size, represented by T1/T2 proportion. Queens have a

bigger T2, where wings and respective flight muscles are included [5], while workers present an

enlarged T1 as a consequence of more developed neck muscles also required for their caste-

specific tasks [5]. This figure also highlights differences in proportion of these thoracic segments

19

between colonies within the same species. Both queens and workers were collected from

various colonies. In fig. 1 each colour represents one species and each colour variants

represents different colonies from the same species.

Independently of the sample size for each colony we can suggest that distributions of thoracic

proportions of workers are similar. Although some groups reveal a wider range of distribution

and we found significant differences in T1/T2 (H(2)T1/T2 = 8.42, p-value = 0.0149) between the

same three worker groups previously used, the general interval is maintained independently of

the species. The differences found are probably due to the specific groups used for statistical

analysis. As expected we can observe differences in T1/T2 ratios across species queens in the

same figure. Relative to species we can say that Lasius queens have a much smaller T1 in

relation to their T2 size, while Aphaenogaster presents a bigger T1 when compared with T2

size, somehow presenting a thorax more similar to what we can call a worker-like phenotype.

The unbalanced sample for Messor does not allow us to conclude anything about their queen

phenotype and behaviour, but we can hypothesize by looking to figure 1B that it could have an

intermediate position in relation to T1 and T2 size.

To understand how much of this variation can be attributed to species differences, and for this

we analysed three workers groups chosen due to their similar sample size, one belonging to

each species. The significant differences found for homogeneity of variances (Table S2) did not

allow us to group different colonies. The further analysis included head traits once it is known

to be an informative morphological structure about caste and sub-caste specificity. We only

represented statistical significant differences in the respective figures. We found differences

for all head-related traits, both in size (H(2)HL = 103.23, p-value < 0.0001; H(2)HW = 106.71, p-

value < 0.0001) and shape (H(2)HL/HW = 122.77, p-value < 0.0001). Thoracic traits revealed

significant differences for TL (H(2)TL = 102.49, p-value < 0.0001), T1 (H(2)T1 = 100.1, p-value <

0.0001) and T2 (H(2)T2 = 79.923, p-value < 0.0001) length across the three groups of workers.

For this analysis we also tested the three ratios represented on fig. 1A and 1B, and only found

significant differences for T1/TL (H(2) = 8.4204, p-value = 0.0148) and T1/T2 (H(2) = 8.4171, p-

value = 0.0148). These results can seem contradictory to data in figure 1A, where apparently

all the workers fall in a big group with similar thoracic proportions, independently of species,

and also to what we observe in figure 1B where we can’t point out differences in T1/T2 worker

proportions. However it could be the case that differences found for these three specific

groups may not be confirmed using all the individuals represented, or using bigger samples.

Increase of individuals for each sample considered could also result in decrease of variation in

thoracic proportions for each species, and our analysis can classify the differences found as

non-significant. Unfortunately we are unable to take any conclusion in relation to queens, but

20

we would expect to find differences in this group. This expectation is again related not only to

our information in figure A1, but also justified by previous works where has been shown that

queens thorax morphology is dependent on colony funding behaviour and trade-offs that

these individuals found between ground and flight behaviours associated [5].

Intra-specific variation

After comparisons in relation to thoracic structure and shape variation across species and

castes we tried to get some insight on how each trait behaves individually and how much

variation can we find within each caste and colony. For this we used the same three traits

above –T1, T2 and TL – and added three head-related traits for analysis. We analysed head

length and width for size variation, and head length/head width for variation in shape.

Lasius sp.

For Lasius we used individuals from one single colony to compare castes, and as expected we

confirmed the extreme dimorphism present between workers and queens. We can also notice

these extreme size differences by looking at untreated images on Annex 1, Fig. 3. Statistical

analysis demonstrated significant differences for all traits compared between castes and for

almost all of them we could find a considerably different level of variation. The analysis of

variance revealed a statistically significant effect of caste for all traits. We found significant

differences for T2 length (H(1)T2 = 87.796, p-value < 0.0001), as expected due to their

differences in size, and also for T1 length (H(1)T1 = 86.332, p-value < 0.0001) and for TL (H(1)TL =

87.79, p-value <0.0001). Thoracic proportions analysed, T1/T2 (H(1)T1/T2 = 87.787, p-value <

0.0001), T1/TL ((H(1)T1/TL = 87.787, p-value < 0.0001) and T2/TL ((H(1)T1/TL = 87.787, p-value <

0.0001) also revealed significant differences between workers and queens, as expected. In

relation to head traits we could also confirm the dimorphism expected between workers and

queens. Both head length (H(1)HL = 87.791, p < 0.0001) and head width (H(1)HW = 87.795, p <

0.0001) revealed significant differences between castes. Significant differences were also

found for head shape between castes (H(1)HL/HW = 87.787, p < 0.0001) and revealed a tendency

for queens head to be square-shaped (HL/HW = 1.113 ± 0.039, n = 70) while workers show a

more elongated, barrel-like head shape (HL/HW = 1.324 ± 0.045, n = 51). This last suggestion is

made assuming a squared shape when HL/HW present values near 1, and elongated shape

when HL/HW is relatively higher than 1.

21

Messor barbarus

We considered only worker caste for comparisons between M. barbarus individuals due to a

very unbalanced sample for Messor queen caste, represented only by one individual. For the

two colonies used for traits comparisons we found significant differences for first thoracic

segment length (H(1)T1 = 8.1525, p-value = 0.0043), which can be influenced by original colony

size. Although represented just by one individual, Messor queen head data stands out when

compared with the other species queens for its smaller size in relation to bigger workers head.

This is common on polymorphic species and especially on species like this [16]. Other important

factor missing in our analysis is the real size of the colony. It is known that bigger colonies will

present higher mean size for workers, but we could not assess that information since our

sample only represents a little part of a wild colony [25].

To complete our Messor barbarus analysis we should have collected more individuals from

different colonies and analysed other morphological traits in order to better understand how

allometry and/or isometry are contributing to size and shape variation in a polymorphic

species. Other thing that could be affecting our analysis is the age of the colony. It has been

described that polymorphic species colonies start their worker force by producing a

considerable amount of small workers and only later in life they start producing major workers

and, when possible, specializing workers in particular functions [20]. However the major part of

the information in polymorphic species is about Solenopsis invicta and some Pogonomyrmex

species, and we have no evidence that Messor, although being a seed-harvester species, would

morphologically behave similarly to any of those.

What we can tell by looking to the worker groups represented, even with different sample

sizes – W1 with 123 and W2 with 47 individuals –, is that we can see a wider range of size

distribution for colony W1 (fig. 2C-2H). Individuals from both colonies were collected in the

wild and we cannot address foraging preferences to any worker size. We can only ensure that

the collection was unbiased and if the colonies were the same age we should find the same

size distribution, which means an equal representation of minor and major workers, according

to predictions referred above. Consequently we can suggest that colony W1 was founded

previously to W2 and this can be affecting our results. To solve this we should have used

laboratory reared colonies or follow wild colonies for longer periods and expand our

sociometry and sociogenesis information on this species and in particular about the location

where they have been excavated [25].

22

Aphaenogaster senilis

Again we proceed to comparisons between colonies for worker caste, since it was the only

with considerable sample sizes. Queen individuals were used as an indication about what we

could expect from queens’ phenotype variation. In this specific case workers and queens

represented by colony number 3 actually belong to our colony A3 in Table S1.

This species shows significant differences between colonies for all measured traits, both thorax

(H(2)T1 = 7.9361, p-value = 0.0189; H(2)TL = 17.02, p-value = 0.0002) and head-related (H(2)HL =

10.47, p-value = 0.0053; H(2)HW = 26.441, p-value < 0.0001; H(2)HL/HW = 21.683, p-value <

0.0001), with exception for second thoracic segment length (H(2)T2 = 2.3612, p-value = 0.3071).

Although we could think that these differences were due to the laboratory reared individuals,

from colony 1 (W1 group) and colony 2 (W2 group), being compared to wild-caught ones from

colony 3 (W3 group), our statistical analysis shows that the differences found were not only

caused by W3 group individuals.

Looking to queens and workers data, we can suggest that there are more differences in

thoracic traits than in head traits between castes, with the exception for head shape that also

reveals significant differences. Head shape in A. senilis shows a similar variation to what were

found between Lasius castes, and again workers have more barrel-shape heads (HL/HW =

1.698 ± 0.066, n = 91) while queens show a more square-shaped head (HL/HW = 1.472 ± 0.034,

n = 7). Stronger differences found in thoracic traits are supported by previous studies, where

mesothorax was described as the principal allometric growth-center between workers and

queens [16]. In order to better classify Aphaenogaster senilis an allometric analysis would be

useful since the size similarity between workers and queens has been suggested as a primitive

character, as already referred [16].

Morphological and behavioural intra-caste variation

After recognizing important differences between colonies we tried to understand the origin of

these differences, especially if we could find any pattern associated with intra-caste

morphological variation. Could these differences be due to our sample collection? Or are they

associated with individuals’ preferential location in the nest? Is the morphological

specialization according to function limited to polymorphic, seed-harvester species [17]?

It is already known that in the Hymenoptera there are different strategies to respond to one of

the most remarkable features of eusociality – the division of labour [10]. Ants accomplished that

dividing their tasks through different castes, and these castes can be morphological,

physiological or behavioural [22].

23

Morphological analysis

Fig. 2 – Morphological differences between workers found in versus outside of nest. A) Comparison between first (T1) and second (T2) thoracic segment proportions relative to total thorax length (TL), each colour represents individuals’ location/where they were found and marked, grey – inside/green – outside. Empty circles represent A1 (from Aphaenogaster colony 1) individuals and filled circles represent A2 (Aphaenogaster colony 2) individuals. B) Thoracic segments variation for A1 and A2, inside and outside the nest. T1 represented in dark grey (inside) and green (outside), T2 represented in light grey (inside) and green (outside); C) Total thorax length (TL) variation between inside and outside individuals for A1 and A2. D-F) Head size and shape variation, D) head length variation, E) head width variation and F) head length/head width across individuals. Both colonies and colour codes are maintained in all figures. Black lines represent the mean for each group of individuals and only significant statistical differences are represented; p-value<0.05 *, p-value<0.01 **, p-value<0.001 *** and p-value<0.0001 ****. All traits were measured in mm.

First we tried to confirm that our model species Aphaenogaster senilis is morphologically

monomorphic. We collected individuals from two colonies of A. senilis kept in the lab and

sampled them from two different locations inside our colony maintenance boxes, inside and

outside the nest. Using the same traits that were used before for inter and intra-caste

comparisons, we wanted to know if there is some selective pressure for a specific body shape

and/or size prevail according to a specific function. According to previous studies our

expectations will be that inside individuals should be responsible for nursing and outside

individuals should be responsible for foraging [10, 24]. These specific group functions have

already been described for several species, and gave rise to specific morphological and

behavioural adaptations throughout ants’ evolutionary history [10].

Using the same type of graphic used on fig. 1 to highlight possible intra-caste subgroups based

on their thoracic proportions and after our statistical analysis using Wilcoxon test, we can

conclude that there are no differences between individuals from different locations in both

24

colonies, A1 and A2, respectively represented by empty and filled symbols. Apparently there

are no morphological differences between different individuals’ preferential location within

the nest. Preferential location is here referred as the place where individual spend most part of

their time, probably according to their function in the colony, and it is not correspondent to

any demonstrated preference. As we did before both thoracic and head traits were analysed,

and size and shape seem to be maintained across locations. When testing for a location effect

we found some differences in A1 and A2, but none of these were maintained in both colonies.

Individuals from A2 showed differences in T1 length (W = 126, p-value = 0.0333) and

individuals from A1 showed differences in TL (W = 29, p-value = 0.003) and head shape (W =

34, p-value = 0.0008). Although we could not find significant differences for all traits and

across colonies, when the difference appears it is caused by an increase in size of the

individuals found outside the nest. However these differences can be caused by some bias,

once there is a confounding effect associated with inside-nest individuals’ collection. Each time

the nest was opened we can try to be quick enough to pick individuals staying inside and avoid

picking any other individual that moved from the outside to the inside of the nest. In general

these results corresponded to our expectations, showing little morphological variation

between individuals from the same caste.

Behavioural analysis

Aphaenogaster senilis does not seem to have distinct morphological groups, but they could

present behavioural groups. To test the existence of behavioural castes in this monomorphic

species we maintained the two colonies used before in the experiments – A1 and A2 – and

added a third one – A3 – available in the lab at the time, also useful to provide some

robustness to our behavioural experiments. We should underlie that this A3 colony is an

informative code for this behavioural assay, but it corresponds to colony A4 (Annex 1, Table

S1).

For each colony we marked 10-15 individuals outside the nest with green painting and 10-15

individuals inside the nest with silver painting, and during four days and one night counted the

number of individuals found at each location. Our null hypothesis was the absence of

behavioural castes, where all individuals distributed randomly within the colony, without any

preferential location associated with sub-groups and specific division of tasks. In fig. 3 below,

lines representing 50% determine exactly half of the individuals marked, 50% on top is relative

to silver marked individuals and 50% below represents green marked ones. The 25% and 75%

dashed lines represent 50% of individuals that corresponds to a particular colour and are used

25

to follow their behavioural dynamic. If individuals within a colony distributed randomly we

would observe light columns constantly crossing these dashed lines, meaning that individuals

could usually be found in a “wrong” location.

Fig. 3 – Behavioural differences between workers found in versus outside of nest. A-C) Each figure is relative to behavioural observations in one Aphaenogaster colony. Grey represents percentage of individuals marked inside nest and green represents individuals marked outside the nest. Outside individuals are represented from 0-50%, half marked in green; inside individuals are represented from 50-100%. Dark color percentages represent individuals observed where they were marked and light color percentages represent individuals observed in different locations from where they were marked. Black lines mark 50%, which we defined as representation limit for each location group, and black dashed lines mark 25% and 75%, which we expected to be the behavioural variation limit for each group if behavioural groups are maintained. We followed three colonies during four days in day period and one day on night period (highlighted by sun and moon symbols). A) Behavioural variation between workers groups from A1 (Aphaenogaster colony 1), B) behavioural variation between workers groups from A2 (Aphaenogaster colony 2), and C) behavioural variation between workers groups from A3 (Aphaenogaster colony 3). Scheme on top right position summarize our experimental design.

What we can conclude, during our short period of observations, is that the individuals show

fidelity to their group location and possibly to the specific tasks of their group. Individuals

marked in green spent most of their time outside, correspondent to the dark green proportion

always above 25%, and the same is true for individuals marked in silver, where the majority

spent their time inside. We corrected for the total number of individuals marked in each colour

at the end of each day of observations. During the day period we found significant differences

26

in relation to the colour found outside (F(1, 96) = 368.14, p-value < 0.0001), meaning that

individuals’ distribution is really being affected by the group where they first belong. We also

found differences between colonies (F(2, 97) = 12.71, p-value = 0.0017) and these differences tell

us that although individuals present a tendency to maintain their supposed behavioural

groups, the probability of finding the individuals’ in the right place can change across colonies.

Once more we should be careful with the results because these differences can be caused by

some bias introduced by our collection and labelling.

Although our results can suggest the existence of behavioural castes we could not provide any

information about the supposed relationship between individuals’ tasks and their location.

Extended observation time is needed to improve our conclusions about this topic. It would be

important to balance number of observations between different periods of the day since it is

known that circadian rhythms could have strong influence in ants’ behaviour [24]. Different

techniques like video tracking could help us on getting detailed information about behavioural

specializations within colonies and it will allow us to follow specific individuals and their

activity for months.

Hormonal manipulation effects

The follow up to previous questions was to understand the degree of phenotypic plasticity that

is present in Aphaenogaster senilis and to explore in detail intra-caste variation in response to

hormonal manipulations. We wanted to know what could be involved in shaping the reaction

norms for monomorphic workers. Reaction norms are usually associated to discrete

phenotypes in dimorphic ant species or sub-castes [7]. Not only morphological aspects of an

individual but also its life history traits were shown to be affected by a variety of

environmental cues [10, 15]. As referred, juvenile hormone is responsible for castes and sub-

castes determination [15, 26]. We manipulated internal levels of JH, using methoprene treatment

to increase endogenous JH levels [26] and precocene II treatments to decrease them [33]. We

measured not only the phenotypic effect of the treatment on the adults but also the impact of

these on life history traits.

During this experiment several external factors influenced and limited our manipulations, and

it was the case of low reproduction rates in our colonies, loss of individuals and long periods

required for queen-less colonies to lay eggs. Although we are not able to find any good

explanation for low egg laying rates in our queen-right colonies maintained in artificial

conditions during four years, the possible explanation for such low reproduction rates in

queen-less colonies is apparently easy. As referred above A. senilis has gamergates and being

27

responsible for part of colony reproductive potential also results in conflicts between

potentially laying workers. These conflicts are easily associated with our artificial new colony

foundation and it has been described that a queen-less colony – orphaned or originated by

fission – will need a period of 12 weeks to produce their first male pupae, due to policing

behaviour. During this period all eggs and larvae produced will be destroyed by other workers

[37-39].

Our experiments thus included trials with queen-less colonies founded by fission from our

original queen-right colonies, and hormonal manipulation events on male larvae, but as

referred the policing and destruction of our manipulated male larvae could have been the

reason why it never reached a sufficient number of individuals to proceed with the

experiment.

Survival effect

Fig. 4 – Survival rates across hormonal treatments for diploid larvae. A1 (Aphaenogaster colony 1) and A2 (Aphaenogaster colony 2) were the colonies used for manipulations, and JH-, CTR and JH+ represents the three applied treatments, with precocene II, acetone and methoprene, respectively. Each bar represents the percentage of total manipulated diploid larvae, light grey for manipulated individuals that did not eclosed and dark grey for manipulated individuals that completed development (n inside bars).

Our manipulations resulted in extremely low survival rates to all treatments, control included,

and also considerable differences in survival between the two colonies tested. All survival

values specifically associated with colony, treatment and round, are represented in Table 2,

below. Although we could find some differences associated with colony and round, our main

interest was to understand how each treatment and/or compound can affect survival, which

will influence the number of adult individuals measured, and in consequence all final

conclusions. Grouping the total of individuals manipulated in both colonies we get 53% of

28

survival for our control treatment, with acetone only, and 32% and 18% for JH- and JH+,

respectively. Based on these results we can conclude that control treatment have the higher

percentage of survival. This is probably correlated with the use of the other compounds as

pesticides. The concentrations used for each one of these compounds were previously

described, and the same methoprene concentration as already been used in Wheeler and

Nijhout experiments. A similar protocol was used in individuals from Pheidole bicarinata

species and treatments with methoprene induced an increase in production of major workers

[14]. Precocene II concentration was an adapted version from previous experiments on

Solenopsis invicta dealation, where compound efficacy as anti-JH treatment was confirmed [34].

Although the concentration tested on Solenopsis was higher than ours, we preferred to use

lower dosages, due to pesticide nature of the compound. We also tested other concentrations,

increasing precocene II and decreasing methoprene concentrations, in response to the

unexpected survival rates we obtained, but these changes either maintained or decreased

survival levels. The correct way to test this would be to draw a specific dosage-response curve

for our species, Aphaenogaster senilis. Although JH+ treatment had always the lowest survival

rate, the control and JH- treatments survival values changed across colonies. One factor that

could be contributing to these differences is unbalanced number of manipulation

events/rounds, once we have three rounds for colony A1 and only two rounds for colony A2.

These unbalanced samples are consequence of differential reproduction rates for each colony,

which decreased the number of larvae available from each colony and limit their use for each

manipulation event.

Table 2 – Summary of the number of individuals used from each colony in each treatment, and respective percentages of survival. In brackets you can find numbers and percentages associated with each round/event of manipulation, presented in chronological order.

29

Developmental time

Fig. 5 – Hormonal treatments effects on developmental time in diploid larvae. We measured developmental time counting from manipulation day until day of eclosion for each individual. Different symbols represent different colonies, and different colors represent different manipulation events – rounds. JH-, CTR and JH+ are three letter codes to represent three applied treatments, with precocene II, acetone and methoprene, respectively. Black lines represent the mean for each group of individuals and only significant statistical differences are represented; p-value<0.05 *, p-value<0.01 **, p-value<0.001 *** and p-value<0.0001 ****.

We should recall that our experimental results only include females and the first trait for which

we have specific expectations according to treatment is developmental time. Previous studies

reported that higher levels of JH would increase growth and delay metamorphosis [26].

According to our treatments, we expected maintenance in developmental time for control

individuals, and an increase and decrease of time for JH+ and JH- treatment respectively.

Statistical analysis revealed no treatment effect on developmental time (F(2, 0.7) = 0.003, p-value

= 0,997) and we could conclude that there are intrinsic and significant differences in

developmental time across colonies, as represented on fig. 5 above (F(1, 619) = 5.6652, p-value =

0.0223). In the same figure we can also see a tendency inversion on treatment effect across

colonies. Developmental time seems to decrease in response to increasing JH levels on A1 and

seems to do the opposite on A2. Our primary expectations coincide with what was observed in

A2, but the low number of individuals can also influence tendencies found. We could also

notice a round effect.

Induced morphological variation

The second aim of our hormonal manipulations was to understand how these different

hormonal levels will affect the adult phenotype. We used only second instar individuals for

manipulations, and a more careful classification of individuals could have been useful once the

30

size range during this instar is wide. Individuals should have been divided in early, intermediate

and late L2, in order to give us precise information about the specific time window where

juvenile hormone levels can be interfering with developmental pathway selected and final

adult form [14].

Fig. 6 – Effects of JH manipulations on Aphaenogaster senilis diploid larvae. A1 (Aphaenogaster colony 1) and A2 (Aphaenogaster colony 2) correspond to colonies used for hormonal manipulations; and JH- (green), CTR (grey) and JH+ (orange) are three letter codes that represent three applied treatments, with precocene II, acetone and methoprene, respectively. Inside each treatment column we can distinguish sub-columns that correspond to each manipulation event – round. A-C) Head size and shape variation between treatments, A) head length variation, B) head width variation and C) head length/head width across individuals. D-G) Thoracic structures variation, D) first thoracic segment (T1) variation, E) second thoracic segment (T2) variation, F) total thorax length (TL)

variation across individuals, between treatments; and G) Comparison between first (T1) and second (T2) thoracic segment proportions relative to total thorax length (TL), empty ◊ correspond to A1 individuals and filled ◊ correspond to A2 individuals, and the color code for treatments. Black lines represent the mean for each group of individuals and only significant statistical differences are represented; p-value<0.05 *, p-value<0.01 **, p-value<0.001 *** and p-value<0.0001 ****. For some traits differences were analyzed in detail – Tukey’s pairwise comparisons – and letters above columns represent them, same letter represented across treatment columns meaning no significant differences between groups. All traits were measured in mm.

In these experiments, we would expect different treatments resulting in different phenotypes,

and probably in intermediate ones. Since we are dealing with a monomorphic species we

expected a phenotypic response represented by a linear reaction norm, with intermediate

phenotypes between workers and queens, rather than the typical polyphenism representing

two or more discrete phenotypes across an environmental gradient. We maintained the

colonies used in the previous experiments and measured the same traits used until now.

31

All individuals that survived to hormonal treatments were classified as workers due to their

final phenotype. Contrary to expectations not a single individual treated, supposedly the ones

with higher juvenile hormone internal levels, originated a queen or queen-like phenotype. This

was concluded just by phenotype observation and due to absence of queen-specific structures,

like wings, ocelli or numerous ovarioles. On fig. 6G we represented the T1/TL and T2/TL values

to highlight the possible groups of phenotypes that we expected to see as result of these

treatments. Statistical analysis of all thoracic proportions revealed no differences for T1/T2,

but found differences for T1/TL (F(1, 0.03) = 24.35, p-value < 0.0001) and T2/TL (F(1, 0.04) = 37.99, p-

value < 0.0001) between colonies. What we observe is a random distribution of points, similar

to the distribution found for non-manipulated worker individuals in the first figure panel (fig.

2A), and this indicates the absence of specific differences in thoracic structures in response to

treatments. Proportion of T1 in relation to T2 is maintained as expected within castes of a

certain species. In the other six figures we can see each trait specific response to

manipulations, and again only significant differences or effects are represented in these. Head

length and total thorax length do not show significant differences, neither between colonies

nor between treatments within colonies. We found significant differences for the other traits,

and more specifically for head width we only found an effect of colony in interaction with

treatment. These differences were found using the most complex ANOVA model

(HW~colony*treatment) and to find differences among colonies we used Tukey’s range test –

shared letter representing no differences between certain groups – and no considerable

biological results were found when comparing each group to each other. We found a

significant effect of treatment in head shape (F(2, 0.01) = 3.72, p-value = 0.0334) and the same

post-hoc analysis revealed an interesting effect on colony A1, particularly between control and

JH+ treatment once these were the groups that differed significantly (represented on figure by

different letters – a and b) with p-value < 0.05. Also relative to head shape we could not find a

colony associated effect contrary to what was expected when looking for non-manipulated

individuals results.

When testing our model for first thoracic segment we found statistically significant differences,

including an effect of colony (F(1, 0.13) = 44.70, p-value < 0.0001) and a close to significance

effect of treatment (F(2, 0.02) = 3.22, p-value = 0.0519). Once the ANOVA assumptions were met

we proceed to Tukey’s test and confirmed that the differences found were an effect of colony

and not from treatment, since within each colony every treatment group shared the same

letter. In fig. 6E we could see a strong colony effect on T2 (F(1, 0.17) = 39.5, p-value < 0.0001),

which highlight possible response variations between colonies and is in accordance to what we

have found for non-manipulated individuals, as represented on fig. 1D. In summary the only

32

trait that could show some treatment effect was head length/head width, and it only

happened in one colony.

These colony effects found in manipulated individuals are supported by our previous results on

non-manipulated individuals, where we also found differences between colonies for

Aphaenogaster senilis. However, results for second thoracic segment length and head shape

need some special attention since they are not supported by our previous analysis. Our

expectations for head shape would be a worker like phenotype changing to a queen like

phenotype, which means that a barrel-like structure would become closer to a heart-shaped

structure. This expectation is suggested by our first graphics, where Aphaenogaster queens

seem to have a more square/heart-shaped head according to low values for head length/head

width ratio. Previous experiments reported a head shape change caused by an allometric head

growth that follow body size increase not only between queens and workers, but also in

polymorphic workers in several species [16]. We could only find some effect for JH+ treatment

in A1 colony, and contrary to what we could expect the head is responding by increasing in

length. Relative to T2 differences reported for manipulated individuals could be caused by

higher statistical power associated with the test used, in this case ANOVA, when compared

with data analysed for non-manipulated individuals. What is most probably happening is that

our treatments are not having an effect, but differences between colonies were highlighted

when using a more powerful test for differences in comparison with non-parametric Kruskal-

Wallis test.

Such inconsistency in our results can be related with four different things: i) number of

individuals used is insufficient to reveal any effect, ii) unbalanced samples do not allow the

correct analysis and are masking possible effects, iii) the concentration applied from each

compound is insufficient to trigger a response, and iv) manipulations are not causing hormonal

levels to change, due to low absorbance through the cuticle, for example. In general these

problems could be solved just by increasing our sample size, not only increasing the number of

colonies used but also the number of individuals for each one of them. Other thing that could

have been optimized was the time window and number of colonies used to obtain male larvae

from queen-less colonies. One of our project objectives was to test how haploid genomes

respond to the same environmental cues as females and one of the main difficulties was to get

enough male larvae to test this and report any conclusion about the topic. As referred, the

possible existence of policing between Aphaenogaster workers have been reported as the

main cause for queen-less/orphaned colonies need more than 30 weeks to produce the first

male larvae that are able to complete development.

33

To solve the problem with compounds concentration we would need to perform experiments

in order to get a specific dosage-response curve for this species, as done for other species [15].

In general our expectations do not match our observations, and this could be caused by

precocious degradation of methoprene. Previous studies suggested that if methoprene was

applied out of JH-sensitive periods individuals will degrade it and undergo metamorphosis

sooner than expected [15]. If this is the case recent adults’ phenotype will mimic JH- treatment

effect, which is in accordance with the results on fig. 6.

To test whether juvenile hormone was indeed manipulated by our treatments we want

measure the expression levels of Krüppel-homolog 1, or Kr-h1. This gene acts downstream of

Methoprene-tolerant receptor and both of them are extremely conserved in insects. Their

interaction and function were first reported for Drosophila and Tribolium, where elegant RNAi

experiments proved that these elements are involved in metamorphosis regulation [40, 41]. JH is

therefore responsible for juvenile stage maintenance and metamorphosis delay with Met and

Kr-h1 as intermediates. However, due to our time schedule and this time consuming technique

we were not able to finish our sequencing, and the details about this experiment can be found

on Annex 3.

34

Final remarks and future perspectives

This work brought some insight about the importance of inter-specific analysis but above all

about the importance of within caste comparisons, both between colonies and individuals

inside these colonies.

We could observe that differences between castes are bigger than within castes,

independently of their worker nature. However we can also say that allometric analysis is

needed to give some robustness to our results, especially when comparing polymorphic and

monomorphic worker castes. It has been suggested that workers functional and morphological

specializations are a secondary adaptive modification and our observations can confirm it [16].

Claustral queens, who we confirmed to have a reduced T1 in proportion to other thoracic

structures, morphology should be analysed more carefully since different dimorphism degrees

can reveal an important role in their ecology and adaptation. Inter-specific worker analysis

showed significant differences in all traits, confirming our expectations in relation to their

differences in size, even if proportions were maintained. Specifically relative to head shape our

analysis revealed that we should analyse this data more carefully, once queens’ heads proved

to be more squared-shape without being always bigger, what was somehow unexpected.

Intra-specific A. senilis analysis revealed that we can highlight some degree of morphological

difference between workers that spent more time outside than inside the nest, probably

according to their functions, but a more elaborated experimental design can give us more

security about these results. Although morphological differences could not be securely

assumed as real, the same was not verified for behavioural groups within this species.

Behavioural observations allow us to suggest the existence of behavioural castes, even without

the possibility of classifying them as temporal or physiological.

In relation to our hormonal manipulations little can be suggested about developmental

plasticity associated with monomorphic species, which was our primary objective. Although

we can assume some treatment effect, at least in relation to induced mortality, we should also

conclude that it is of extreme importance to assess specific colony behaviour and

characteristics in order to better interpret results about this type of experiment.

Regarding all the problems we had with extremely unbalanced samples and also differences

found in morphology between laboratory maintained and wild-caught individuals, the first

follow up for this study will include bigger sample sizes and previous analysis of effects that

laboratory maintenance can have on colonies. Increased sample sizes are also necessary to

reinforce our hormonal manipulations experiment, together with a previous analysis of a

specific dosage curve response. An important experiment is also to test if our treatments are

35

really inducing an effect on JH internal levels and for this we need to obtain the correct

sequence for our genes of interest, both Kr-h1 and control, and analyse their expression in

treated individuals.

In addition to these tests, we consider other follow up experiments that would be an

increment to this project and also a more complete way to approach our main questions. It

would be interesting to look to other body structures, like eyes and ocelli – that are also

known to be plastic –, wings when present and wing imaginal discs during development – with

and without JH levels manipulation – [42], mandibles – that also present intra-caste variation –

and ovarioles, with particular attention to reproductive potential of workers (including

manipulated individuals) that can be measured by eggs development analysis [43].

It would be an interesting follow up to analyse developmental adaptive plasticity between

different species regarding possible different responses according to dimorphism degree and

within caste variation, but also to compare these responses between diploid and haploid

genomes. Ants represent a perfect model to answer this type of questions because within a

colony we could assess both diploid and haploid versions of the same genome, and given the

nature of plastic females vs. (apparently) less plastic males with such specific life history and

function we could also try to answer questions about genomes robustness. It would be also

interesting to collect more morphological and behavioural information about male ants in

general since they have been overlooked during all these decades of social insects’ studies.

Follow ups suggested would contribute to a broader view of processes involved on, probably,

the most intriguing example of developmental plasticity within insects. Integrative studies

including morphological, behavioural, physiological and molecular approaches are a

requirement. Nowadays, social insects’ biology and ecology, in parallel with their evolutionary

origin, are part of the most interesting research areas in biology. Their complex organization

systems are contributing to our better understanding of society dynamics and complex

networks patterns, and the contribution of these small animals to the community is (still)

immeasurable, just like their diversity.

36

Bibliography

1. Kingsolver, J. G. et al. The strength of phenotypic selection in natural populations. Am. Nat. 157, 245–261 (2001).

2. Nijhout, H. F. The control of body size in insects. Dev. Biol. 261, 1–9 (2003). 3. Moczek, A. P. Phenotypic plasticity and diversity in insects. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.

365, 593–603 (2010). 4. Wilson, E. O. The origin and evolution of polymorphism in ants. The Quarterly review of

biology 28, 136–156 (1953). 5. Keller, R. a, Peeters, C. & Beldade, P. Evolution of thorax architecture in ant castes highlights

trade-off between flight and ground behaviors. Elife 3, e01539 (2014). 6. Keller, R. A. A Phylogenetic Analysis of Ant Morphology (Hymenoptera : Formicidae) with

Special Reference to the Poneromorph. Bulletin Of The American Museum Of Natural History 355, 1–90 (2011). doi:10.1206/355.1

7. Beldade, P., Mateus, A. R. a & Keller, R. a. Evolution and molecular mechanisms of adaptive developmental plasticity. Mol. Ecol. 20, 1347–63 (2011).

8. Moczek, A. & Emlen, D. Male horn dimorphism in the scarab beetle, Onthophagus taurus: do alternative reproductive tactics favour alternative phenotypes? Anim. Behav. 59, 459–466 (2000).

9. Simpson, S. J., Sword, G. a. & Lo, N. Polyphenism in insects. Curr. Biol. 21, R738–R749 (2011). 10. Darwin, C. On The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of

Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. John Murray, Albemarle Street (1859). 11. Hölldobler, B. & Wilson, E. O. The Ants. Harvard University Press (1990). 12. Peeters, C. The occurrence of sexual reproduction among ant workers. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 44,

141–152 (1991). 13. Pie, M. R. & Tschá, M. K. Size and shape in the evolution of ant worker morphology. PeerJ 1,

e205 (2013). 14. Wheeler, D. E. Developmental and Physiological Determinants of Caste in Social

Hymenoptera: Evolutionary Implications. Am. Nat. 128, 13 (1986). 15. Wheeler, D. E. & Nijhout, H. F. Soldier Determination in Pheidole Bicarinata: Effect of

methoprene on caste and size within castes. J. Insect Physiol. 29, 847–854 (1983). 16. Wilson, E. O. The origin and evolution of polymorphism in ants. The Quarterly review of

biology 28, 136–156 (1953). 17. Arnan, X., Ferrandiz-Rovira, M., Pladevall, C. & Rodrigo, A. Worker size-related task

partitioning in the foraging strategy of a seed-harvesting ant species. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 1881–1890 (2011).

18. Tschinkel, W. R. The morphometry of Solenopsis fire ants. PLoS One 8, 1–22 (2013). 19. Heredia, A. & Detrain, C. Worker size polymorphism and ethological role of sting associated

glands in the harvester ant Messor barbarus. Insectes Soc. 47, 383–389 (2000). 20. Tschinkel, W. R., Mikheyev, A. S. & Storz, S. R. Allometry of workers of the fire ant, Solenopsis

invicta. J. Insect Sci. 3, 1–11 (2003). 21. Peeters, C. P. Ergatoid queens and intercastes in ants: Two distinct adult forms which look

morphologically intermediate between workers and winged queens. Insectes Soc. 38, 1–15 (1991).

22. Robinson, E. Physiology as a caste-defining feature. Insectes Soc. 56, 1–6 (2009). 23. Jackson, D. E., Martin, S. J., Ratnieks, F. L. W. & Holcombe, M. Spatial and temporal variation

in pheromone composition of ant foraging trails. Behav. Ecol. 18, 444–450 (2007). 24. Mersch, D. P., Crespi, A. & Keller, L. Tracking Individuals Shows Spatial Fidelity Is a Key

Regulator of Ant Social Organization. Science (80-. ). 340, 1090–1093 (2013). 25. Tschinkel, W. R. Back to basics: Sociometry and sociogenesis of ant societies (Hymenoptera:

Formicidae). Myrmecological News 14, 49–54 (2010). 26. Nijhout, H. F. Insect Hormones. Princeton University Press (1994).

37

27. Schwander, T., Lo, N., Beekman, M., Oldroyd, B. P. & Keller, L. Nature versus nurture in social insect caste differentiation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 275–82 (2010).

28. Bonasio, R. et al. Genome-wide and caste-specific DNA methylomes of the ants camponotus floridanus and harpegnathos saltator. Curr. Biol. 22, 1755–1764 (2012).

29. Alvarado, S., Rajakumar, R., Abouheif, E. & Szyf, M. Epigenetic variation in the Egfr gene generates quantitative variation in a complex trait in ants. Nat. Commun. 6, 6513 (2015).

30. Jindra, M., Palli, S. R. & Riddiford, L. M. The juvenile hormone signaling pathway in insect development. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 58, 181–204 (2013).

31. Hammock, B.D, Quistad, G. B. Metabolism and mode of action of juvenile hormone, juvenoids and other insect growth regulator. (1981).

32. Libbrecht, R. et al. Interplay between insulin signaling, juvenile hormone, and vitellogenin regulates maternal effects on polyphenism in ants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, 11050–5 (2013).

33. Bowers, W.S., Ohta, T., Cleere, J. S. Precocene II inhibits juvenile hormone biosynthesis by cockroach corpora allata in vitro. Nat. Commun. 265, 548–549 (1977).

34. Burns, S. N., Teal, P. E. a, Vander Meer, R. K., Nation, J. L. & Vogt, J. T. Identification and action of juvenile hormone III from sexually mature alate females of the red imported fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. J. Insect Physiol. 48, 357–365 (2002).

35. Wheeler, G. C. & Wheeler, J. ANT LARVAE : REVIEW AND SYNTHESIS. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Washingt. 7, (1976).

36. Passera, L., Roncin, E., Kaufmann, B. & Keller, L. Increased soldier production in ant colonies exposed to intraspecific competition. Nature 379, 630–631 (1996).

37. Boulay, R. et al. Production of sexuals in a fission-performing ant: Dual effects of queen pheromones and colony size. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 1531–1541 (2007).

38. Boulay, R., CerdÁ, X., Fertin, A., Ichinose, K. & Lenoir, A. Brood development into sexual females depends on the presence of a queen but not on temperature in an ant dispersing by colony fission, Aphaenogaster senilis. Ecol. Entomol. 34, 595–602 (2009).

39. Chéron, B., Doums, C., Fédérici, P. & Monnin, T. Queen replacement in the monogynous ant Aphaenogaster senilis: supernumerary queens as life insurance. Anim. Behav. 78, 1317–1325 (2009).

40. Minakuchi, C., Zhou, X. & Riddiford, L. M. Krüppel homolog 1 (Kr-h1) mediates juvenile hormone action during metamorphosis of Drosophila melanogaster. Mech. Dev. 125, 91–105 (2007).

41. Minakuchi, C., Namiki, T. & Shinoda, T. Krüppel homolog 1, an early juvenile hormone-response gene downstream of Methoprene-tolerant, mediates its anti-metamorphic action in the red flour beetle Tribolium castaneum. Dev. Biol. 325, 341–350 (2009).

42. Rajakumar, R. et al. Ancestral Developmental Potential Facilitates Parallel Evolution in Ants. Science 335, 79–82 (2012).

43. Khila, A. & Abouheif, E. Reproductive constraint is a developmental mechanism that maintains social harmony in advanced ant societies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 17884–17889 (2008).

44. Lozano, J. & Belles, X. Conserved repressive function of Krüppel homolog 1 on insect metamorphosis in hemimetabolous and holometabolous species. Sci. Rep. 1, 163 (2011).

45. Cheng, D., Zhang, Z., He, X. & Liang, G. Validation of reference genes in Solenopsis invicta in different developmental stages, castes and tissues. PLoS One 8, e57718 (2013).

46. Lubertazzi, D. The Biology and Natural History of Aphaenogaster rudis. Psyche A J. Entomol. 2012, 1–11 (2012).

47. Brown, M. J. Semi-claustral founding and worker behaviour in gynes of Messor andrei. Insectes Soc. 46, 194–195 (1999).

48. Stille, M. Queen/worker thorax volume ratios and nest-founding strategies in ants. Oecologia 105, 87–93 (1996).

38

Annex 1

Support material – Tables, schemes and images

Fig. S1 – Illustrative schemes of experimental design. A) Scheme of artificial habitat conditions for our laboratory maintained colonies; B) Representation of hormonal treatments application directly on larvae cuticle; C) Adult traits measured exclusively on dorsal view during our experiments, head length (HL) and width (HW) in white, first (T1) and second (T2) thoracic segments length in green and blue, respectively.

39

Tab

le S

1 –

Inf

orm

atio

n a

bo

ut

the

colo

nie

s u

sed

fo

r ex

peri

men

ts.

Co

nd

itio

n i

ndi

cate

s if

th

e co

lon

y w

ere

mai

nta

ined

wit

h (

Q+)

or

wit

ho

ut

the

fou

nd

ing

qu

een

. C

olo

ur

rect

angl

es r

epre

sen

t ea

ch e

xper

imen

t fo

r w

hich

eac

h c

olo

ny

was

use

d.

Blu

e is

use

d f

or

hor

mo

nal

man

ipu

lati

on

s ex

peri

men

ts,

ora

nge

for

mo

rph

om

etri

c an

alys

is b

etw

een

and

wit

hin

sp

ecie

s an

d c

aste

s, a

nd

gre

en

fo

r b

eh

avio

ura

l ass

ays

[46

-48

] .

40

A

B

C

Fig.

S2

Rep

rese

nta

tio

n

of

tran

sect

s de

sign

ed

and

use

d

to

mea

sure

ea

ch

indi

vidu

al

on

Imag

eJ.

Imag

es

corr

esp

on

d

to

Ap

haen

oga

ster

sen

ilis

indi

vid

ual

s. A

) H

ead

len

gth

(w

hite

lin

e) a

nd h

ead

wid

th (

wh

ite

das

hed

lin

e) m

easu

rem

ents

, B

) fi

rst

and

seco

nd

th

orac

ic s

egm

ents

mea

sure

men

ts o

n d

orsa

l vie

w, a

nd C

) to

tal t

ho

rax

len

gth

mea

sure

men

ts v

iew

, alw

ays

on

the

left

sid

e.

41

Fig. S3 - Adult structures in females from three species. A-F) Aphaenogaster senilis body parts; A) Worker head in dorsal view, B) Worker thorax in dorsal view (for first and second thoracic segments measurements), C) Worker thorax in left lateral view (for total thorax length measurements); D) Queen head in dorsal view, E) Queen thorax in dorsal view and F) Queen thorax in left lateral view. G-I) Messor barbarus body parts; G) Worker head in dorsal view, H) Worker thorax in dorsal view and I) Worker thorax in left lateral view; J-O) Lasius sp. body parts; J) Worker head in dorsal view, K) Worker thorax in dorsal view, L) Worker thorax in left lateral view, M) Queen head in dorsal view, N) Queen thorax in dorsal view and O) Queen thorax in left lateral view. All images were acquired at same magnification but treated to compose this panel. Proportions between individuals are not correspondent to the real ones. Each image has its own scale and each white line corresponds to 1mm.

42

Annex 2

Statistical analysis summary

Table S2 – Levene’s test results for intra-caste comparisons for A. senilis and M. barbarus,

only workers used.

Table S3 – Kruskal-Wallis non-parametrical test results for morphological variation analysis; comparisons between species,

castes and colonies correspondents to Fig. 1 data.

43

Annex 3

Hormonal manipulation effects

Hormonal manipulations experiment and its results suggested that compound applications

could be ineffective. Although each round of manipulation has demonstrated a high mortality

rate it is important to refer that this could be due to handling or some other effect that we are

not observing.

To disentangle the effect of treatment for the presence or absence of effect on phenotype

there is a useful experiment that should be done. We know that compounds used are

absorbed when in contact with larval cuticle, but we need to measure how much of these

treatments have a true effect.

We already referred that some genes respond directly to juvenile hormone levels in insects,

which is the case for Krüppel-homolog 1 (Kr-h1), a gene already described as an intervenient in

metamorphosis regulation [44]. Both regulation and amino acid sequence of Kr-h1 are

extremely conserved across insects [44]. Juvenile hormone acts directly through its receptor

Methoprene-tolerant (Met) as demonstrated by Drosophila Met mutants and RNAi on

Tribolium castaneum that produced precocious or miniature adults [40, 41]. This interaction

between ligand and receptor will induce Kr-h1 encoding a putative transcription factor and

allow its action on nucleus and in all individual, delaying metamorphosis [44].

We can thus suggest that Kr-h1 expression levels during development and particularly after

hormonal treatments application (JH-, CTR and JH+) would be a good measurement for our

compounds activity and application effectiveness.

To measure this specific gene expression we will use qPCR in order to quantify juvenile-

hormone responsive elements in comparison with control genes. For this test we used pupae

from both colonies and collected genomic DNA from male and female pools of individuals from

different colonies. Since we do not have the sequenced genome of our model species, A.

senilis, we need to find degenerate primers sequences or design them ourselves. Degenerated

primers are designed using amino acid sequences and are only used when the sequence

resulting of a gene transcript is a highly conserved motif across species inside a taxonomic

group.

Isolation of A.senilis sequence for Krüppel-homolog 1 (Kr-h1) first used degenerate primers

designed for Blatella germanica, a cockroach, generated by RT-PCR using cDNA as template [44].

However, our results for cloning protocol with these primers were not good, and we started

the degenerate primers process from the beginning and tried to maximize process efficiency

following the next steps:

44

1. Sequences alignment, choice of conserved motifs for Kr-h1 and four possible

control genes – EF1, GAPDH, RPL18 and TBP;

2. Design of degenerate primers using CODEHOP, for primer sequences see Table 3;

3. DNA extraction from white pupae with Qiagen – Dneasy Blood and Tissue Kit;

4. Degenerated PCR with gDNA from male and female pools of individuals for target

sequence amplification (PCR program cycle: 95°C for 5min; 35 cycles of 95°C for 20s

– 55°C for 20s - 72°C for 1min; 72°C for 6min);

5. Cloning of PCR products using pGem-T Easy protocol;

6. Plasmid DNA purification with Nucleospin Plasmid protocol (Macherey-Nagel);

7. Cycle sequencing with BigDye Terminator protocol, using reverse primers;

8. Sequence analysis and new alignment, to confirm amplification of genes of interest.

We succeeded and got the proper sequence for our control genes, but our Kr-h1 primers

amplified the wrong region of the genome, with low percentage of identities when aligned

with this conserved region across insects’ groups and in particular when aligned with

Solenopsis sequences used as templates for control genes [45].

The follow up for this process will be the RNA extraction from each larval and pupal instars,

cDNA synthesis and confirmation of sequences expression. The final step would be the

quantification of Kr-h1 expression in different instars in manipulated and un-manipulated

individuals. We would expect for Kr-h1 expression to increase in JH+ treatments and decrease

in JH- treatments.

45

Ta

ble

S4

– G

enes

invo

lved

in m

eth

op

ren

e ac

tivi

ty d

etec

tio

n a

nd

qu

anti

fica

tio

n.