moussay_dosseville_2002.pdf

15
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/10965627 Circadian rhythms during cycling exercise and finger-tapping task ARTICLE in CHRONOBIOLOGY INTERNATIONAL · DECEMBER 2002 Impact Factor: 2.88 · DOI: 10.1081/CBI-120015966 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS 26 DOWNLOADS 166 VIEWS 128 6 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: Fabrice Dosseville Université de Caen Basse-Normandie 46 PUBLICATIONS 175 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Jacques Larue Université d'Orléans 33 PUBLICATIONS 620 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Bruno Sesboüé Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Caen 50 PUBLICATIONS 518 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Damien Davenne Université de Caen Basse-Normandie 103 PUBLICATIONS 1,821 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Available from: Fabrice Dosseville Retrieved on: 22 June 2015

Upload: joao-carlos-alencar

Post on 14-Sep-2015

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Seediscussions,stats,andauthorprofilesforthispublicationat:http://www.researchgate.net/publication/10965627

    Circadianrhythmsduringcyclingexerciseandfinger-tappingtaskARTICLEinCHRONOBIOLOGYINTERNATIONALDECEMBER2002ImpactFactor:2.88DOI:10.1081/CBI-120015966Source:PubMed

    CITATIONS26

    DOWNLOADS166

    VIEWS128

    6AUTHORS,INCLUDING:

    FabriceDossevilleUniversitdeCaenBasse-Normandie46PUBLICATIONS175CITATIONS

    SEEPROFILE

    JacquesLarueUniversitd'Orlans33PUBLICATIONS620CITATIONS

    SEEPROFILE

    BrunoSesboCentreHospitalierUniversitairedeCaen50PUBLICATIONS518CITATIONS

    SEEPROFILE

    DamienDavenneUniversitdeCaenBasse-Normandie103PUBLICATIONS1,821CITATIONS

    SEEPROFILE

    Availablefrom:FabriceDossevilleRetrievedon:22June2015

  • CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS DURING CYCLINGEXERCISE AND FINGER-TAPPING TASK

    S. Moussay,1 F. Dosseville,1 A. Gauthier,1 J. Larue,1

    B. Sesboue,2 and D. Davenne1,*

    1Centre de Recherches en Activites Physiques et Sportives (CRAPS

    UPRES EA 2131), UFR Sciences et Techniques des Activites Physiques

    et Sportives, Universite de Caen Basse Normandie, Caen, France2Institut Regional de Medecine du Sport, CHU Caen Cote de Nacre,

    Caen, France

    ABSTRACT

    The aim of this study was to follow the circadian fluctuation of the

    spontaneous pedal rate and the motor spontaneous tempo (MST) in a sample

    of highly trained cyclists. Ten subjects performed five test sessions at various

    times of day. During each test session, subjects were required to perform (i) a

    finger-tapping task, in order to set the MST and (ii) a submaximal exercise on

    a cycle ergometer for 15 min at 50% of their Wmax. For this exercise, pedal rate

    was freely chosen. Spontaneous pedal rate and heart rate (HR) were measured

    continuously.

    The results demonstrated a circadian variation for mean oral temperature,

    HR, and MST. Under submaximal exercise conditions, HR showed no

    significant time-of-day influence although spontaneous pedal rate changed

    significantly throughout the day. Circadian rhythm of oral temperature and

    pedal rate were strongly correlated. Moreover, a significant positive

    correlation was found between MST and pedal rate. Both parameters may

    be controlled by a common brain oscillator. MST, rest HR, and pedal rate

    changes follow the rhythm of internal temperature, which is considered to be

    1137

    DOI: 10.1081/CBI-120015966 0742-0528 (Print); 1525-6073 (Online)Copyright q 2002 by Marcel Dekker, Inc. www.dekker.com

    *Corresponding author. D. Davenne, UFR STAPS Caen, Universite de Caen Basse-Normandie,

    Boulevard du Marechal Juin, 14032 Caen Cedex, France. E-mail: [email protected]

    CHRONOBIOLOGY INTERNATIONAL

    Vol. 19, No. 6, pp. 11371149, 2002

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • the major marker in chronobiology, therefore, if there is a relation between

    MST and pedal rate, we cannot rule out partial dependence of both parameters

    on body temperature. (Chronobiology International, 19(6), 11371149, 2002)

    Key Words: Circadian changes; Cyclic activities; Cycling; Motor spon-

    taneous tempo; Spontaneous pedal rate

    INTRODUCTION

    The natural or spontaneous cadence adopted in all cyclic activities (running,

    walking, or swimming) shows fine temporal coordination. To account for the

    required accuracy in time processing, some authors[1 6] have suggested the

    existence of an internal clock sending periodic signals and serving as a time

    reference. When freely chosen by the subject, these periodic signals occur at a given

    frequency, which has been called personal tempo,[7 9] referent period,[10] or

    motor spontaneous tempo (MST).[11 13] Thus, internal tempo or MST is

    considered to be specific to each individual and may directly reflect a persons own

    internal clock frequency. It may be described as a central pace-maker, used as a

    parameter to organize different internal cognitive and behavioral processes.[8,14]

    The origin of the MST could be genetic[15] or may result from a personal

    construction, built upon neurophysiological bases.[16] The average MST period is

    around 600 msec and is characterized by strong intra-individual stability.[11,17]

    However, Oleron et al.[13] identified a diurnal variation in MST with a peak at

    midday with low values observed early in the morning and late in the afternoon.

    Kikkawa[18] confirmed the increase in MST from 07:00 to 13:00h and a continuous

    decrease from 13:00 to 22:00h, however, these fluctuations did not reach a

    significant level.

    The influence of MST on preferred pedal rate selection in cycling has never

    been studied. Cycling is a strongly stereotypical rhythmical movement, and

    numerous studies have addressed the problem of how the spontaneous crank rate is

    chosen. Some authors have highlighted the importance of the rating of perceived

    exertion (RPE) drawn from central and peripheral origins. For instance, Pandolf and

    Noble[19] demonstrated a parabolic relationship between RPE and cadence with the

    lowest value recorded at 100 revolutions per minute (rev min21) at a power output

    of 200 W. These results were confirmed by Marsh and Martin.[20] In other studies,

    the minimal metabolic cost[21] or minimal muscular fatigue was taken into account.

    Several studies[22 26] show that muscular moments produced and EMG activity

    level (recorded throughout a complete pedaling cycle) are minimized for pedal rates

    ranging from 90 to 100 rev min21. Since pedal force decreases as a function of pedal

    rate, a frequency of 90100 rev min21 would minimize the level of muscular

    contraction produced, thus reducing muscular fatigue.[27]

    In a recent study,[28] the crank rate freely chosen by cyclists to develop a

    moderate power output [less than 60% of their maximal aerobic power (MAP)]

    MOUSSAY ET AL.1138

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • significantly increased from 06:00 to 18:00h]. Similarly, for a light power output

    (2030% of MAP), cyclists spontaneously choose a pedaling rate of 75 rev min21

    at 06:00h and 95 rev min21 at 18:00h. These results may be compared to those of

    Atkinson et al.,[29] which provided evidence for a time-of-day fluctuation in

    working cadence in athletic subjects. Furthermore, these fluctuations were

    correlated with the circadian rhythm of the core temperature.

    In the study of Moussay et al.,[28] cyclists spontaneously decreased their

    pedaling rate in the morning, forcing them to produce a higher pedal torque to

    keep up with the imposed work-load. The maximal muscular peak torque observed

    in the afternoon (17:55h)[30] is not consistent with the reduction of muscular strain

    during the pedaling cycle demonstrated by Patterson and Moreno.[25] Muscular

    strain would appear not to be the main factor in choosing a pedaling rate at a low

    power output, other factors need to be explored. The influence of MST on

    spontaneous pedaling rate adopted during cyclic activities is one of these.[2]

    It seems an obvious link as a cyclists spontaneous pedaling rate will be

    somewhere between 90 and 100 rev min21 depending on power output;[31 33] this

    means a period ranging between 660 and 600 msec, which is similar to the average

    MST period.[12] Hence MST could be an important factor for the spontaneous

    pedaling rate. The aim of this study was to monitor the circadian variation of

    pedaling rate and MST in a population of highly trained cyclists in order to detect a

    possible relationship between MST and pedaling rate.

    METHODS

    Subjects

    Ten male cyclists (age: 22.2 ^ 2.4 yr; body mass: 68.6 ^ 5 kg; height:

    179.5 ^ 5.4 cm) volunteered to take part in this experiment. This study was

    granted ethical approval by the ethics committee, CHU Cote de Nacre, Caen,

    France. All subjects provided written informed consent after the study procedures

    were explained in detail. All subjects had at least 3yr experience in competitive

    cycling. Most trained for 810 h a week.

    Subjects were selected from among 25 cyclists as either moderately

    morning n 6 or neither type n 4; from their responses to the self-assessment questionnaire of Horne and Ostberg[34] which determines morning-

    nesseveningness.

    Experimental Procedure

    First, Wmax was assessed by a maximal continuous ramp test performed at

    15:00h. The individual Wmax was later used to set the individual power output for

    the submaximal steady state exercise.

    CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS DURING CYCLING 1139

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • Two weeks later, oral temperature, heart rate (HR), pedal rate, and MST

    were recorded during five test sessions carried out at the following times of day:

    06:00, 10:00, 14:00, 18:00, and 22:00h. To avoid the effect of sleep deprivation,

    no test session was carried out at 02:00h. Each subject performed five test sessions,

    the timing of which was randomized. For each subject, there was only one session

    per day with a minimal period of 24 h between each session. As proposed by

    Baxter and Reilly,[35] subjects ate a meal 2 h before the tests, at 14:00 and 22:00h,

    and they were woken up at 05:00h for the test at 06:00h. Before this particular test

    they were allowed to drink a glass of water.

    In addition, subjects were asked to adhere as closely as possible to their usual

    sleeping habits with a minimum of 6 h (mean 7:30 ^ 0:45h) of sleep taken on the

    night preceding each test. Subjects were not engaged in any fatiguing exercise

    during the protocol. During the protocol, the laboratory temperature remained

    constant (19 ^ 18C).

    Data Collection

    In the first part of the protocol, the cyclists performed a maximal continuous

    ramp test; they rode on a cycle ergometer (Cateyew, CS-1000, Osaka, Japan) for

    5 min at a work-rate of 150 W to warm-up. Then the work-rate was increased by

    15 W min21 steps until exhaustion. Wmax was determined from the last step

    completed by the subject.

    During the second part of the protocol, each of the five test sessions was

    performed on the same ergometer in order to detect a time-of-day effect on

    temperature, HR, MST, and pedaling rate. Each test session started with a 20min

    rest period during which subjects remained supine. At the end of this rest period,

    HR (Polarw, X-trainer Plus, Kempele, Finland) and oral temperature (Omronw,

    Wegalaon, Netherlands, accuracy: 0.058C) were recorded. Then, the subjects satcomfortably on a chair; they were required to perform a finger-tapping task in

    order to evaluate the MST. As described by Fraisse,[12] the subjects had to tap at

    the most comfortable pace for 30 sec, and the only constraint was to keep the

    pacing as regular as possible. During each session, this test was completed three

    times, with a rest interval of 1 min, to check the reliability of data collection.

    Finally, subjects performed a submaximal exercise on an electrical braked cycle

    ergometer (Cateye). Exercise started with a 5min warm-up period at 70 W, after

    which subjects were asked to develop 50% of their Wmax for 15 min. Throughout

    the exercise, they were free to choose their own crank rate. Spontaneous pedaling

    rate and HR were measured continuously (Polar) and averaged every 5 sec.

    Data Analysis

    For each test session, the most regular of the three 30sec trials (minimal SD)

    was taken for analysis. Out of this 30sec period, only the last 20 sec were

    MOUSSAY ET AL.1140

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • considered for analysis since it takes a little under 10 sec to stabilize the

    rhythm.[12] Instantaneous tap-to-tap frequency was computed and averaged over

    the 20sec recording for each subject. Mean MST is reported as taps per minute

    (taps min21).

    During the submaximal test, pedaling rate and HR were continuously

    recorded. Mean values were calculated from the 3rd to the 15th min, considering

    that it took around 3 min to reach a stable state which was maintained until the end

    of the exercise. All results are presented as mean ^ SD.

    Statistical Analysis

    In order to identify a circadian rhythm and demonstrate that the rhythm

    amplitude differed from zero, each parameter was submitted to a one-way analysis

    of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures, with the factor time as a repeated

    measure. A protected least significant difference (PLSD) Fischer test was selected

    as a post hoc analysis. A least squares regression analysis, using the cosinor

    method, was employed to determine the best-fit of a combined 24h period cosine

    function[27] of the form Y ti M A cosvti w, where M is the mesor (i.e., themean level), A is the amplitude measured as half of the peak-to-trough variation,

    and w is the acrophase (i.e., the time when the maximum level occurs, referencedto local 00:00h). The existence of a sinusoidal circadian rhythm was confirmed or

    rejected on the basis of the confidence interval for each parameter. Cosine

    functions were compared using as reference values, samples of the 10 subjects

    recorded during the five test sessions. Probability ( p ) values of less than 0.05 were

    taken to indicate statistical significance.

    RESULTS

    Maximal Incremental Test

    In the first part of the protocol, when the maximal continuous ramp test was

    performed, subjects achieved a mean Wmax of 412 ^ 32 W. Maximal HR

    measured at exhaustion was 189 ^ 6 beats min21.

    Circadian Functions

    Temperature

    A significant circadian rhythm was found at rest for oral temperature

    (Fig. 1A).

    Values ranged from 35.7 ^ 0.18C at 06:00h to 36.3 ^ 0.18C at 14:00h, withsignificant time-of-day effect F4;36 3:9; p 0:031: Post hoc analysisindicated a significant difference between data collected at 06:00h and those

    CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS DURING CYCLING 1141

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • collected in the afternoon. From cosinor analysis, the 24h mean temperature

    (mesor) was 36.1 ^ 0.28C, with a peak-to-trough amplitude of 0.6 ^ 0.28C,representing a total fluctuation of 1.6% around the mesor. The highest value

    (acrophase) was estimated at 16:44 ^ 1:50h (Table 1).

    Heart Rate

    Rest HR showed a marked circadian fluctuation F4;36 7:10; p 0:0003(Fig. 1B). The lowest value, recorded at 06:00h, was significantly different from

    other values recorded during the day. Values ranged from 49 ^ 1.2 beats min21 at

    06:00h to 58 ^ 1.3 beats min21 at 14:00h. The fitted cosine curve indicated a

    circadian variation with an acrophase at 15:19 ^ 1:52h, a 24h mesor of

    54 ^ 1.2 beats min21, and a peak-to-trough amplitude of 7 ^ 1.2 beats min21 or

    12.43% of the mesor (Table 1). During submaximal exercise at 50% of Wmax, HR

    Figure 1. Diurnal rhythms of (A) oral temperature, (B) heart rate at rest, (C) MST, and (D) pedal

    rate. Mean values (^SD) are shown. Best fit curve between the experimental data () and the cosinefunction curve ( ) is shown.

    MOUSSAY ET AL.1142

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • values ranged from 134 ^ 4.6 beats min21 at 06:00h to 137 ^ 5.2 beats min21 at

    14:00h but did not show any significant circadian variation F4;36 1:74; p 0:16:

    Motor Spontaneous Tempo

    There was a significant time-of-day effect on MST F4;36 4:8; p 0:003(Fig. 1C). Post hoc analysis demonstrated a significant increase in MST from

    06:00h (117.4 ^ 9.7 taps min21) to 18:00h (155.1 ^ 15.6 taps min21) and a

    decrease in the evening from 18:00 to 22:00h (128.9 ^ 18.8 taps min21). The

    mesor for finger-tapping, estimated from the cosine function, was 135.6 ^

    9.8 taps min21 and tap-to-tap interval was 442 msec. Peak-to-trough amplitude

    was 40.2 ^ 8.7 taps min21 (29.64%) of the mesor and the acrophase was

    estimated at 16:52 ^ 1:52h (Table 1).

    Pedal Rate

    Pedal cadences spontaneously adopted by the cyclists showed a stable rate

    during each of the test sessions after 3 min of adaptation at the start of the exercise.

    The pedaling rate changed significantly at different times of day F4;36 2:82;p 0:038 (Fig. 1D).

    Values ranged from 90.1 ^ 2.9 rev min21 at 06:00h to 96.5 ^ 2.8 rev min21

    at 18:00h. The lowest frequency was recorded at 06:00h and differed significantly

    from other values observed during the day. The mesor was 93.9 ^ 2.7 rev min21,

    representing 638 msec for a complete revolution, the acrophase occurring at

    17:49 ^ 2:16h. Cosinor analysis demonstrated an amplitude of 5.9 ^

    1.2 rev min21 throughout the day (Table 1), representing 6.28% of the mesor

    value.

    Table 1. Results of ANOVA for Repeated Measures with the Factor Time and Presentation of the

    Characteristics of Circadian Rhythms (w Acrophase, A Peak-to-Trough Amplitude, M Mesor)ANOVA Characteristics of Circadian Rhythms

    F(dl) p w (h) A M

    Temperature (8C) 3.09(4,36) 0.031 16:44 ^ 1:50 0.6 ^ 0.2 36.12 ^ 0.2Rest HR (beats min21) 7.1(4,36) 0.0003 15:19 ^ 1:52 6.7 ^ 1.2 53.9 ^ 1.2

    MST (taps min21) 4.84(4,36) 0.0003 16:52 ^ 1:52 40.2 ^ 8.7 135.6 ^ 9.8

    Pedal rate (rev min21) 2.82(4,36) 0.038 17:49 ^ 2:16 5.9 ^ 1.2 93.9 ^ 2.7

    CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS DURING CYCLING 1143

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • Correlation Analysis Between Circadian Rhythms

    Temperature cosine variation was significantly correlated with pedaling rate

    r 0:96; p 0:009 and rest HR r 0:9; p 0:037: A significant correlationwas also found between MST and pedaling rate r 0:87; p 0:048:

    There were no significant differences in the acrophases of oral temperature,

    HR, MST, or pedal rate F3;21 0:89; p 0:46:

    DISCUSSION

    The peak and time course of the diurnal fluctuation in oral temperature are in

    agreement with data in the literature.[36 38] The amplitude of the temperature

    fluctuation (0.6 ^ 0.28C) is within the range of those measured by Ilmarinenet al.[39] and Reilly and Down.[40] Such variations are classically observed in

    physically active individuals.[29] The observed biphasic fluctuation characterized

    by an asymmetry of the curve (a substantial rise between 06:00 and 10:00h, when

    no significant variation was observed between 14:00 and 22:00h) is consistent with

    the observations of Gauthier et al.[41]

    Furthermore, the observed diurnal variation of HR at rest confirms a number

    of studies showing a peak value ranging from 15:00 to 17:00h.[36,42 44] However,

    the circadian rhythm of HR at rest was lost completely during submaximal

    exercise. This seems to confirm the decrease of amplitude in circadian rhythms of

    many physiological parameters such as heart rate, oxygen uptake, and ventilation,

    during moderate or heavy submaximal exercise.[45]

    As for MST, the mean MST period was 442.5 ^ 31.9 msec whereas the

    literature indicates MST periods ranging from 380 to 880 msec with an average of

    600 msec.[12] This discrepancy might be due to the fact that our subjects were

    highly trained, as it has been reported that the level of physical fitness may affect

    MST.[12,46]

    High intra-individual stability from one day to another is commonly

    reported.[12] However, only two studies have tested a time-of-day effect on MST:

    one was a single case study,[13] and the other[18] suggested a trend but failed to

    highlight any statistical difference. Our results clearly demonstrate a significant

    circadian variation in MST, the large amplitude in MST [29.49%

    (40.2 ^ 8.7 taps min21) of the mesor value] possibly explained by the subjects

    physical fitness.[29,47]

    The population was fairly homogenous and the mean Wmax(412.5 ^ 32.5 W) is in line with those already observed in highly trained

    cyclists.[48]

    Pedaling rate measured throughout the day fluctuated between 90.1 ^

    2.9 rev min21 at 06:00h and 96.5 ^ 2.8 rev min21 at 22:00h. The mesor value

    (93.9 ^ 2.7 rev min21) is in line with that already observed (85100 rev min21) in

    previous studies with expert cyclists.[31 33] This is consistent with a rate that

    MOUSSAY ET AL.1144

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • minimizes neuromuscular fatigue.[23] This fact might play an important role in

    choosing the preferred pedaling rate for a cycling exercise.

    Interestingly, the diurnal fluctuation in crank rate is not synchronized with

    the circadian rhythm of muscular peak torque,[30] as cyclists chose a lower pedal

    rate in the morning when muscular torque is at its lowest values. Thus, factors

    other than just muscular force need to be taken into account in order to explain the

    decrease in the morning pedaling rate.

    One possible explanation is to assume a relationship between rhythmicity of

    temperature and pedaling rate throughout the day r 0:96; p 0:009:Circadian fluctuations in the core temperature may modify the musculo-skeletal

    system:[49,29]

    In the morning, when body temperature is minimal, articular suppleness is

    reduced.[50,51] This may reduce the velocity of movements during a

    submaximal cycling exercise.

    There is a linear relationship between nerve conduction velocity and body

    temperature.[52,53] Nerve conduction velocity increases throughout the

    day[54] while the time to peak and the relaxation time decrease by 8.3

    and 10.7%, respectively,[55] making for better muscle properties in the

    evening. Neuromuscular efficiency changes synchronously with core

    temperature fluctuation.[56]

    An alternative explanation is that a reduced pedaling rate in the morning

    could be of benefit to intra- and inter-muscular coordination requirements. Indeed,

    Neptune et al.[57] pointed to the importance of the temporal characteristics in burst

    onset/offset on muscles coordination. Another argument for a time-of-day

    influence on movement coordination is given by Thor[58] who demonstrated a jet-

    lag effect in the timing and coordination of movements.

    The days biggest difference was observed between the 06:00h test session

    and other experiment times. This effect could be an artifact since subjects were not

    allowed to have a meal before this test session and were woken up before their

    habitual waketime (

  • (638 ^ 18 msec) and a tap-to-tap interval (442 ^ 31.9 msec) are of different

    durations. Obviously, the two movements differ in terms of motion amplitude and

    applied force. Indeed, a moderate cycling exercise involves larger forces and

    amplitudes of motion than those required for a finger-tapping task. However, the

    maximal rate of reciprocal movements is not necessarily dependent upon the

    inertial properties of the limb. The preferred frequency of intra-limb coordination

    is subject to a relaxation oscillator arising from neuromuscular dynamics.[60] Thus,

    the overt frequency might differ between effectors. Nevertheless, our results show

    a similarity in pedaling and finger-tapping frequencies which could be linked to a

    common central clock.

    In conclusion, this study confirms that rest HR, MST, and spontaneous

    pedaling rate during moderate exercise change during the day. Furthermore, MST,

    rest HR, and pedal rate fluctuate synchronously with body temperature, which is

    held to be a major marker in chronobiology, although there is no evidence for a

    causal relationship between fluctuations. Hence, the significant correlation

    between MST and pedal rate fluctuations during the day suggests that both

    parameters may be controlled by a common brain oscillator such as the

    suprachiasmatic nucleus.

    REFERENCES

    1. Treisman, M. Temporal Discrimination and the Indifference Interval: Implications

    for the Mental Internal Clock. Psychol. Monogr. 1963, 77 (13), 131.

    2. Wilson, F.R. Tone Deaf and All Thumbs; Viking Press: New York, 1986.

    3. Macar, F. Time Psychophysics and Related Models. In Time, Mind and Behaviour;

    Michon, J.A., Jackson, J.L., Eds.; Springer-Verlag: New York, 1985; 112130.

    4. Treisman, M.; Faulkner, A.; Naish, P.L.N.; Brogan, D. The Internal Clock: Evidence

    for a Temporal Oscillator Underlying Time Perception with Some Estimates of Its

    Characteristic Frequency. Perception 1990, 19, 705743.

    5. Gibbon, J. Origins of Scalar Timing. Learn. Motiv. 1991, 22, 338.6. Grondin, S. From Physical Time to the First and Second Moments of Psychological

    Time. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127 (1), 2244.

    7. Boltz, M.G. Changes in Internal Tempo and Effects on the Learning and

    Remembering of Event Durations. J. Exp. Psychol: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1994, 20,

    11541171.

    8. Temperley, N.M. Personal Tempo and Subjective Accentuation. J. Gen. Psychol.

    1963, 78, 267287.

    9. Vanneste, S.; Pouthas, V.; Wearden, J.H. Temporal Control of Rhythmic

    Performance: A Comparison Between Young and Old Adults. Exp. Aging Res.

    2001, 27 (1), 83102.

    10. Drake, C.; Jones, M.R.; Baruch, C. The Development of Rhythmic Attending in

    Auditory Sequences: Attunement, Referent Period, Focal Attending. Cognition

    2000, 77, 251288.

    11. Fraisse, P.; Chambron, H.; Oleron, G. Note sur la Constance et lEvolution

    Genetique du Tempo Spontane Moteur. lAnnee Psychol. 1955, 55, 2534.

    MOUSSAY ET AL.1146

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • 12. Fraisse, P. Les Rythmes Moteurs Spontanes Tempo et Balancement. In Psychologie

    du Rythme; PUF: Paris, 1974; 4362.

    13. Oleron, G.; Fraisse, P.; Siffre, M.; Zuili, N. Les Variations Circadiennes du Temps

    de Reaction et du Tempo Spontane au Cours dUne Experience hors du

    TempslAnnee Psychol. 1970, 70, 347356.

    14. Mishima, J. Introduction to the Morphology of Human Behaviour. The Experimental

    Study of Mental Tempo; Tokyo Publishing Co: Tokyo, 1965.

    15. Oliviero, A. Nei Labirinti Della Mente. Roma-Bari: Sgittari Laterza, 1989.

    16. Perilli, G.G. Subjective Tempo in Adults with and without Psychiatric Disorders.

    Music Ther. Perspect. 1995, 13, 104109.

    17. Rimoldi, A. Personal Tempo. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1951, 46, 280303.

    18. Kikkawa, M. Performance and Time of Day: Diurnal Variations in Physiological and

    Psychological Tests. J. Child Dev. 1978, 14, 1624.

    19. Pandolf, K.B.; Noble, B.J. The Effect of Pedalling Speed and Resistance Changes on

    Perceived Exertion for Equivalent Power Outputs on the Bicycle Ergometer. Med.

    Sci. Sports 1973, 5 (2), 132136.

    20. Marsh, A.P.; Martin, P.E. Perceived Exertion and the Preferred Cycling Cadence.

    Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1998, 30 (6), 942948.

    21. Hagberg, J.M.; Mullin, J.P.; Giese, M.D.; Spitznagel, E. Effect of Pedalling Rate on

    Submaximal Exercise Responses of Competitive Cyclists. J. Appl. Physiol. 1981, 51

    (2), 447451.

    22. Takaishi, T.; Yasuda, Y.; Moritani, T. Neuromuscular Fatigue During Prolonged

    Pedalling Exercise at Different Rates. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1994,

    69 (2), 154158.

    23. Takaishi, T.; Yasuda, Y.; Ono, T.; Moritani, T. Optimal Pedaling Rate Estimated

    from Neuromuscular Fatigue for Cyclists. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1996, 28 (12),

    14921497.

    24. Coast, J.R.; Cox, R.H.; Welch, H.G. Optimal Pedalling Rate in Prolonged Bouts of

    Cycle Ergometry. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1986, 18 (2), 225230.

    25. Patterson, R.P.; Moreno, M.I. Bicycling Pedalling Forces as a Function of Pedalling

    Rate and Power Output. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1990, 22 (4), 512516.

    26. Redfield, R.; Hull, M.L. On the Relation Between Joint Moments and Pedalling

    Rates at Constant Power in Bicycling. J. Biomech. 1986, 19 (4), 317329.

    27. Sawka, M.N.; Foley, M.E.; Pimental, N.A.; Toner, M.M.; Pandolf, K.B.

    Determination of Maximal Aerobic Power During Upper-Body Exercise. J. Appl.

    Physiol. Respir. Environ. Exerc. Physiol. 1983, 54 (1), 113117.

    28. Moussay, S.; Gauthier, A.; Davenne, D.; Sesboue, B. Variations Diurnes de

    Parame`tres Biomecaniques au Cours dUne Epreuve Maximale Aerobie Effectuee

    sur Bicyclette Ergometrique. 1er Colloque Pluridisciplinaire: Espace du Geste et de

    la Posture, Caen, France, April 1011, 2000.

    29. Atkinson, G.; Coldwells, A.; Reilly, T.; Waterhouse, J. A Comparison of Circadian

    Rhythms in Work Performance Between Physically Active and Inactive Subjects.

    Ergonomics 1993, 36 (13), 273281.

    30. Gauthier, A.; Davenne, D.; Gentil, C.; Van Hoecke, J. Circadian Rhythm in the

    Torque Developed by Elbow Flexors During Isometric Contraction. Effect of

    Sampling Schedules. Chronobiol. Int. 1997, 14 (3), 287294.

    CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS DURING CYCLING 1147

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • 31. Faulkner, J.A.; Roberts, D.E.; Elk, R.L.; Conway, J. Cardiovascular Responses to

    Submaximum and Maximum Effort Cycling and Running. J. Appl. Physiol. 1971, 30

    (4), 457461.

    32. Coast, J.R.; Welch, H.G. Linear Increase in Optimal Pedal Rate with Increased

    Power Output in Cycle Ergometry. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1985, 53

    (4), 339342.

    33. Cavanagh, P.R.; Sanderson, D.J. The Biomechanics of Cycling: Studies of the

    Pedalling Mechanics of Elite Pursuit Riders. In Science of Cycling; Burke, E.R., Ed.;

    Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, 1986; 91122.

    34. Horne, J.A.; Ostberg, O. A Self-Assessment Questionnaire to Determine

    MorningnessEveningness in Human Circadian Rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol.

    1976, 4 (2), 97110.

    35. Baxter, C.; Reilly, T. Influence of Time of Day on All-Out Swimming. Br. J. Sports

    Med. 1983, 17 (2), 122127.

    36. Faria, I.E.; Drummond, B.J. Circadian Changes in Resting Heart Rate and Body

    Temperature, Maximal Oxygen Consumption and Perceived Exertion. Ergonomics

    1982, 25 (5), 381386.

    37. Reilly, T. Assessment of Some Aspects of Physical Fitness. Appl. Ergon. 1992, 22,

    291294.

    38. Waterhouse, J.M.; Minors, D.S.; Folkard, S. Estimating the Endogenous Component

    of the Circadian Rhythm of the Rectal Temperature in Human Undergoing Normal

    Sleep/Activity Schedules. J. Interdiscip. Cycle Res. 1993, 24 (3), 197211.

    39. Ilmarinen, J.; Ilmarinen, R.; Korhonen, O.; Nurminen, M. Circadian Variation of

    Physiological Functions Related to Physical Work Capacity. Scand. J. Work

    Environ. Health 1980, 6 (2), 112122.

    40. Reilly, T.; Down, A. Investigation of Circadian Rhythms in Anaerobic Power and

    Capacity of the Legs. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 1992, 32 (4), 343347.

    41. Gauthier, A.; Davenne, D.; Martin, A.; Cometti, G.; Van Hoecke, J. Diurnal Rhythm

    of the Muscular Performance of Elbow Flexors During Isometric Contractions.

    Chronobiol. Int. 1996, 13 (2), 135146.

    42. Reilly, T.; Atkinson, G.; Waterhouse, J. Biological Rhythms and Exercise; Oxford

    University Press: New York, 1997.

    43. Cohen, C.J. Human Circadian Rhythms in Heart Rate Response to a Maximal

    Exercise Stress. Ergonomics 1980, 23 (6), 591595.

    44. Taillard, J.; Sanchez, P.; Lemoine, P.; Mouret, J. Heart Rate Circadian Rhythm as a

    Biological Marker of Desynchronisation in Major Depression: A Methodological

    and Preliminary Report. Chronobiol. Int. 1990, 7 (4), 305316.

    45. Cable, N.T.; Reilly, T. Influence of Circadian Rhythms on Arm Exercise. J. Hum.

    Mov. Stud. 1987, 13, 1327.

    46. Kawagishi, K.; Hirai, T. Relation of Personal Tempo and Heart Rate, Respiration

    Rate and Repetitive Movement. Br. Osaka Univ. Health Sports Sci. 1980, 11, 714.

    47. Harma, M.L.; Ilmarinen, J.; Yletyinen, I. Circadian Variation of Physiological

    Functions in Physically Average and Very Fit Dayworkers. J. Hum. Ergol. 1982, 11

    (Suppl.), 3346.

    48. Craig, N.; Walsh, C.; Martin, D.T.; Woolford, S.; Bourdon, P.; Stanef, T.; Barnes, P.;

    Savage, B. Physiological Tests for Elite Athletes/Australian Sports Commission;

    Gore, C.G., Ed.; Human Kinetics: Australia, 2000; 258277.

    MOUSSAY ET AL.1148

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016

  • 49. Shephard, R.J. Sleep, Biorhythms and Human Performance. Sports Med. 1984, 1,

    1137.

    50. Reilly, T. Human Circadian Rhythms and Exercise. Crit. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 1979,

    18, 165180.

    51. Reilly, T.; Atkinson, G.; Coldwells, A. The Relevance to Exercise Performance of

    the Circadian Rhythm in Body Temperature and Arousal. Biol. Sport 1993, 10,

    203216.

    52. Bagley, R.S.; Wheeler, S.J.; Gay, J.M. Effects of Age on Temperature-Related

    Variation in Motor Nerve Conduction Velocity in Healthy Chickens. Am. J. Vet.

    Res. 1995, 56 (6), 819821.53. Rutkove, S.B.; Kothari, M.J.; Shefner, J.M. Nerve, Muscle, and Neuromuscular

    Junction Electrophysiology at High Temperature. Muscle Nerve 1997, 20 (4),

    431436.

    54. Ferrario, V.F.; Tredici, G.; Crespi, V. Circadian Rhythm in Nerve Conduction

    Velocity. Chronobiologia 1980, 7 (2), 205209.

    55. Martin, A.; Carpentier, A.; Guissard, N.; Van Hoecke, J.; Duchateau, J. Effect of

    Time of Day on Force Variation in a Human Muscle. Muscle Nerve 1999, 22 (10),

    13801387.

    56. Freivalds, A. Investigation of Circadian Rhythms in Selected Psychomotor and

    Neurological Functions. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor,

    MI, 1979.

    57. Neptune, R.R.; Kautz, S.A.; Hull, M.L. The Effect of Pedaling Rate on Coordination

    in Cycling. J. Biomech. 1997, 30 (10), 10511058.

    58. Thor, D.H. Time of Day, Circadian. In Encyclopedia of Sports Sciences and

    Medicine; Larson, L., Ed.; MacMillan: New York, 1968; 124125.

    59. Stockton, I.D.; Reilly, T.; Sanderson, F.H.; Walsh, T.J. Investigations of Circadian

    Rhythms in Selected Components of Sports Performance. Communication to the

    Society of Sports Sciences, Crewe and Alsager College, 1978.

    60. Carson, R.G.; Rick, S.; Winston, D.B.; Abernathy, B.; Susnners, J.J. The Timing of

    Intralimb Coordination. J. Mot. Behav. 1999, 31 (2), 113118.

    Received January 28, 2002

    Returned for revision March 21, 2002

    Accepted June 18, 2002

    CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS DURING CYCLING 1149

    2002 Marcel Dekker, Inc. All rights reserved. This material may not be used or reproduced in any form without the express written permission of Marcel Dekker, Inc.

    MARCEL DEKKER, INC. 270 MADISON AVENUE NEW YORK, NY 10016