mvpa analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

57
Copyright © 2019 Jensen et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed. Research Article: New Research | Sensory and Motor Systems MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of neuromagnetic responses to words reliably classifies multiple levels of language processing in the brain https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0444-18.2019 Cite as: eNeuro 2019; 10.1523/ENEURO.0444-18.2019 Received: 7 November 2018 Revised: 30 May 2019 Accepted: 28 June 2019 This Early Release article has been peer-reviewed and accepted, but has not been through the composition and copyediting processes. The final version may differ slightly in style or formatting and will contain links to any extended data. Alerts: Sign up at www.eneuro.org/alerts to receive customized email alerts when the fully formatted version of this article is published.

Upload: others

Post on 11-Apr-2022

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Copyright © 2019 Jensen et al.This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in anymedium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

Research Article: New Research | Sensory and Motor Systems

MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherenceof neuromagnetic responses to wordsreliably classifies multiple levels of languageprocessing in the brain

https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0444-18.2019

Cite as: eNeuro 2019; 10.1523/ENEURO.0444-18.2019

Received: 7 November 2018Revised: 30 May 2019Accepted: 28 June 2019

This Early Release article has been peer-reviewed and accepted, but has not been throughthe composition and copyediting processes. The final version may differ slightly in style orformatting and will contain links to any extended data.

Alerts: Sign up at www.eneuro.org/alerts to receive customized email alerts when the fullyformatted version of this article is published.

Page 2: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

1

1. Manuscript Title 1 MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of neuromagnetic responses to words reliably 2 classifies multiple levels of language processing in the brain 3 4 2. Abbreviated Title 5 MVPA analysis of language processing in the brain 6 7 3. List all Author Names and Affiliations in order 8 Mads Jensen1, Rasha Hyder1, Yury Shtyrov1,2 9 1: Center of Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), Department of Clinical Medicine, 10 Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark 11 12 2: Laboratory of Behavioural Neurodynamics, St. Petersburg State University, St. Petersburg, 13 Russia 14 15 4. Author Contributions 16 RH and YS designed research, RH performed research, MJ analysed data, MJ and YS wrote paper. 17 18 5. Correspondence should be addressed to 19 Mads Jensen 20 Center of Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), 21 Nørrebrogade 44, build. 10G, 4th 22 DK-8000 23 Aarhus, Denmark 24 Email: [email protected] 25 26 6. Number of figures: 2 27 7. Number of tables: 4 28 8. Number of Multimedia: none 29 9. Number of words for Abstract: 246 30 10. Number of words for Significance Statement: 118 31 11. Number of words for Introduction: 1793 32 12. Number of words for Discussion: 1754 33 13. Acknowledgements 34 We thank Andreas Højlund, Karen Østergaard, Christelle Gansonre, Alina Leminen, Eino Partanen, 35 Nikola Vukovic, Lilli Kimppa, Christopher Bailey, and Britta Westner for their help at different 36 stages of this work 37 14. Conflict of Interest: Authors report no conflict of interest. 38 15 Funding sources: Danish Council for Independent Research (DFF 6110-00486), Lundbeck 39 Foundation (R164-2013-15801; R140-2013-12951), Arhus University, Institute for Clinical 40 Medicine, Central Jutland Region, MINDLab infrastructure, (14.W03.31.0010) 41

Page 3: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 1 of 47

MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of 1 neuromagnetic responses to words reliably classifies 2 multiple levels of language processing in the brain 3

Abstract 4

Neural processing of language is still among the most poorly understood functions of the 5

human brain, whereas a need to objectively assess the neurocongitve status of the language 6

function in a participant-friendly and noninvasive fashion arises in various situations. Here, 7

we propose a solution for this based on a short task-free recording of MEG responses to a set 8

of spoken linguistic contrasts. We used spoken stimuli that diverged lexically 9

(words/pseudowords), semantically (action-related/abstract) or morphosyntactically 10

(grammatically correct/ungrammatical). Based on beamformer source reconstruction we 11

investigated inter-trial phase coherence (ITPC) in five canonical bands (alpha, beta, and low, 12

medium and high gamma) using multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA). Using this approach, 13

we could successfully classify brain responses to meaningful words from meaningless 14

pseudowords, correct from incorrect syntax, as well as semantic differences. The best 15

classification results indicated distributed patterns of activity dominated by core 16

temporofrontal language circuits and complemented by other areas. They varied between the 17

different neurolinguistic properties across frequency bands, with lexical processes classified 18

predominantly by broad gamma, semantic distinctions – by alpha and beta, and syntax – by 19

low gamma feature patterns. Crucially, all types of processing commenced in a near-parallel 20

fashion from ~100 ms after the auditory information allowed for disambiguating the spoken 21

input. This shows that individual neurolinguistic properties take place simultaneously and 22

involve overlapping yet distinct neuronal networks that operate at different frequency bands. 23

This brings further hope that brain imaging can be used to assess neurolinguistic processes 24

objectively and noninvasively in a range of populations. 25

26

Significance Statement 27

In an MEG study that was optimally designed to test several language features in a non-28

attentive paradigm, we found that, by analysing cortical source-level inter-trial phase 29

Page 4: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 2 of 47

coherence (ITPC) in five canonical bands (alpha, beta, and low, medium and high 30

gamma) with machine-learning classification tools (multivariate pattern analysis, 31

MVPA), we could successfully classify meaningful words from meaningless 32

pseudowords, correct from incorrect syntax, and semantic differences between words, 33

based on passive brain responses recorded in a task-free fashion. The results show 34

different time courses for the different processes that involve different frequency bands. 35

It is to our knowledge the first study to simultaneously map and objectively classify 36

multiple neurolinguistics processes in a comparable manner across language features 37

and frequency bands. 38

39

Introduction 40

A neuropsychological assessment of the neurological and/or cognitive status of a subject or 41

patient is a routine required in a variety of situations. This typically involves elaborate 42

behavioural tests aimed, for instance, at evaluating the extent of developmental disorders, 43

assessing the level of neurodegenerative impairment, neurological damage after a head injury, 44

screening for hearing loss, etc. In order to perform such tests successfully, the subject, on the 45

one hand, must typically have a reasonably clear understanding of what they have to do in a 46

particular procedure; on the other hand, they must also be able to communicate their 47

responses to a given task, by giving e.g. a manual, facial or oral response. This raises the 48

problem of uncooperative subjects, such as those suffering from neurological/organic brain 49

disorders or mental illnesses that cause patients to become unresponsive. A brain-damaged 50

person may be unable to respond verbally because of a collateral lesion-related injury (such 51

as akinetic mutism, paralysis, aphasia). A young child with a developmental disorder may be 52

unable or unwilling to communicate their reaction overtly. A locked-in patient, although not 53

Page 5: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 3 of 47

necessarily unconscious, is unable to respond due to the total loss of their motor control 54

function (Laureys, Owen, & Schiff, 2004; Ragazzoni, Grippo, Tozzi, & Zaccara, 2000). On 55

the other hand, an undetected language impairment could have drastic consequences for 56

(mis)diagnosis of language-unrelated disturbances (Majerus, Bruno, Schnakers, Giacino, & 57

Laureys, 2009). A range of such situations is wide and they create a substantial challenge for 58

diagnosis and assessment of performance, development or recovery in various groups. 59

Clearly, techniques that could reveal the brain processing of language without relying on the 60

individual’s overt behaviour would be helpful in such cases. 61

62

Fast-paced development of non-invasive neuroimaging techniques in recent years has given 63

hopes of assessing the brain status of various cognitive functions objectively, even when 64

overt response may not be possible. Language is a complex phenomenon that, to make a 65

coherent whole, requires multiple information types involving different specific features and 66

properties that rapidly unfold in time with a fine temporal structure and remarkable speed 67

(Friederici, 2002, 2011; Shtyrov, 2010). This, in turn, suggests that for the capturing of 68

cerebral processing of linguistic information, a temporally-resolved neuroimaging method is 69

needed that could faithfully track the dynamic neural activity during language comprehension 70

(e.g. Hagoort, 2008). At present, two main techniques are able to provide such high temporal 71

resolution: magneto- and electroencephalography (MEG, EEG), both capable of registering 72

mass neural activity on a millisecond scale. Whereas the two methods are highly similar, 73

MEG has a certain advantage in the ease of modelling underlying cortical activity, owing to 74

unhindered spreading of magnetic fields (but not electric currents) through the head and skull 75

tissues (Baillet, Mosher, & Leahy, 2001). 76

77

A body of electrophysiological research done in EEG and MEG using various linguistic 78

Page 6: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 4 of 47

materials has provided a rich picture of linguistic processing in the brain. Perhaps the most 79

well-known neurophysiological response to language stimuli is the so-called N400 (Kutas & 80

Hillyard, 1980), usually seen as an index of (lexico-)semantic processes and peaking at ~400 81

ms. Syntactic (grammatical) processing has been associated with an early ELAN (early left 82

anterior negativity) response reflecting the stimulus’ grammaticality from ~100 ms 83

(Friederici, Pfeifer, & Hahne, 1993; Neville, Nicol, Barss, Forster, & Garrett, 1991), as well 84

as later frontal negativities (LAN) with longer latencies (Gunter, Friederici, & Schriefers, 85

2000; Münte, Heinze, Matzke, Wieringa, & Johannes, 1998) and the P600, a late positive 86

shift (Osterhout, Holcomb, & Swinney, 1994). 87

88

Most of these responses (except, to an extent, ELAN) still require at least a degree of overt 89

attention or even focused task performance from the subject. In terms of assessing linguistic 90

processing in a more task-free fashion, a number of studies have attempted to use the so-91

called passive paradigms, in which the subjects are presented with linguistic contrasts without 92

having to perform an overt task and are usually distracted from the auditory input by a video 93

or another unrelated activity (Näätänen, Paavilainen, Rinne, & Alho, 2007; Pulvermüller & 94

Shtyrov, 2006). A series of studies using this approach established MEG/EEG correlates of 95

automatic linguistic access including phonological, lexical, semantic and syntactic levels of 96

information processing (Shtyrov, 2010). These have shown that information-specific 97

linguistic activations can be recorded noninvasively without an explicit task or even an 98

instruction to focus on the speech input. For instance, meaningful native words show stronger 99

activation in the core language system than meaningless acoustically similar pseudowords, 100

which putatively indicates activation of word-specific memory traces (Pulvermüller et al., 101

2001). Furthermore, these activations show both semantic specificity (e.g. by involving motor 102

cortex activation and deactivation when presenting action-related words (Hauk, Shtyrov, & 103

Page 7: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 5 of 47

Pulvermüller, 2008; Moseley, Pulvermüller, & Shtyrov, 2013; Shtyrov, Butorina, Nikolaeva, 104

& Stroganova, 2014) and sensitivity to grammatical properties (ELAN-like activity in 105

response to syntactic violations (Hasting, Kotz, & Friederici, 2007). 106

107

Such studies of neurolinguistic processeing typically focus on one single process (e.g. syntax 108

or semantics) at a time. For this approach to be more practically-oriented, it would seem 109

essential to develop a task-free paradigm that can assess multiple types of linguistic 110

information processing in a single short participant-friendly session. Furthermore, previous 111

studies have mostly investigated neurolinguistic processes using ERP/ERFs - that is, evoked 112

activity in a rather narrow frequency band (typically 1-30 Hz), - and it would thus appear 113

advantageous to open up the frequency spectrum to maximise possibilities for registering 114

brain reflections of language. Oscillations in different frequency bands have been shown to 115

reflect multiple cognitive processes and are considered to be a vehicle for neural 116

communication (Fries, 2005; Varela, Lachaux, Rodriguez, & Martinerie, 2001). Different 117

frequency bands have been ascribed different functional roles in the neocortical information 118

processing (Cannon et al., 2013; Sauseng & Klimesch, 2008), where higher bands could be 119

related to local computations (Buzsáki & Schomburg, 2015; Buzsáki & Silva, 2012) and 120

lower bands to longer-range connectivity (Von Stein & Sarnthein, 2000). That different 121

neurolinguistic processes involve different frequency bands have been shown in a range of 122

studies. For instance, Bastiaansen & Hagoort (2006) argued that retrieval of lexical 123

information and unification of semantic and syntactic information are underpinned by 124

different oscillatory networks, while Kösem and van Wassenhove (2017) suggested that 125

oscillations (3-8 Hz) are related to acoustic properties. Teng, Tian, Rowland, & Poeppel 126

(2017) shows that humans can track spoken language dynamics at both and oscillations. 127

Using electrocorticography (ECoG), Towle and collegues (2008) have shown an increase in 128

Page 8: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 6 of 47

high (70-100 Hz) in relation to hearing a word compared to a tone. Luo and Poeppel (2007) 129

showed that phase information in the oscillatory dynamics can track and discriminate spoken 130

sentences. However, although the interest in time-frequency analysis of electrophysiological 131

data has been steadily rising in recent years, language-related oscillatory dynamics still 132

remains relatively unexplored, with most studies in the field focussing on evoke 133

potentials/fields instead. 134

135

In an attempt to close the gap between these research strands and at the same time bridge this 136

research with applied/clinical needs, we set out to combine these different approaches and 137

designed a simple short paradigm that simultaneously includes lexical, semantic and syntactic 138

contrasts, in order to assess different levels of linguistic processing in a single session. This 139

paradigm is tested here by recording MEG responses in a sample of healthy adult participants 140

as a first step to establishing its applicability. The participants were presented with spoken 141

stimuli, which were either meaningless pseudowords or meaningful words with different 142

semantics (action-related verb vs. concrete visual noun) and that either could be syntactically 143

correct or included a morphosyntactic stem-affix violation. These were presented without any 144

stimulus-related task, while the subjects’ attention was diverted away from the auditory 145

stimulus stream. 146

147

Furthermore, in order not to restrict our results to narrow-band evoked activity, we analysed 148

activation in a wide range of frequency bands, from to high . We focussed on the phase 149

part of the oscillatory activity, as phase synchrony has been theorised to be directly related to 150

neural computations (Fries, 2005; Lopes da Silva, 2006; Palva, Palva, & Kaila, 2005; Varela 151

et al., 2001). In order to quantify coordinated neural phase activity related to neurolinguistic 152

processing, we determined phase synchrony by analysing Inter-Trial Phase Coherence (ITPC) 153

Page 9: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 7 of 47

of the MEG responses. ITPC is a measure of how aligned the phase angles over individual 154

trials are. It therefore provides information about mass-synchronised neural activation (and in 155

this sense is to a degree similar to ERP/ERF) without restricting it to a specific power peak or 156

band. ITPC can inform us about the phase synchrony over trials: i.e., if ITPC is high, it means 157

that the phases become aligned when performing a particular perceptual or cognitive 158

computation. We investigated the ITPC in different frequency bands allowing for a 159

comprehensive assessment of the neurolinguistic dynamics. To gain anatomic specificity, 160

single-subject MRIs were used to model cortical source activity, and ITPC values were 161

calculated in individual source space. 162

163

Given the vast amount of information about neural activity created by analysing the ITPC 164

across both time and frequency bands, we used Multivariate Pattern Analysis (MVPA) for an 165

unbiased statistical assessment of the data (Bzdok, Altman, & Krzywinski, 2018), where by 166

‘unbiased’ we mean that that the researcher does not have to choose when and where to 167

test for differences. MVPA (see Methods for more detail) is a machine learning technique 168

based on predicting data rather than on parameter estimation (as used in traditional factorial 169

analyses, e.g. ANOVA or t-tests). The MVPA algorithm will first try to extract a pattern from 170

a subset of the data, which can then be used to predict new, previously unseen data. This 171

allows for a data-driven analysis without a priori hypotheses about spatial or temporal 172

location of the signal of interest. For instance, we might allow the decoding algorithm to train 173

itself on MEG recording trials of meaningful vs. meaningless word form stimuli (i.e. find 174

specific patterns of features in the data most valuable for detecting word-pseudoword 175

differences) and then ask whether it can correctly predict the same distinction in a different 176

data subset; this can be done both within and across subjects. Crucially, such an algorithm, if 177

successful, may in principle be used to estimate data predictability in one subject (e.g. 178

Page 10: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 8 of 47

patient) from another one (e.g. healthy norm) and thus determine both normal functionality 179

and functional abnormalities, or assess the same or different states of a particular processing 180

system in the same individual. 181

182

In sum, we present here a short (< 30 min) task-free paradigm in which auditory linguistic 183

stimuli are presented to MEG subjects, without explicitly requiring their attention, and the 184

resulting data are analysed using MVPA-based machine learning algorithm to classify brain 185

responses in different frequency bands as reflecting meaningful lexical input as well as its 186

semantic and syntactic properties. 187

188

Methods 189

Participants 190

The experiment was conducted according to the principles of the Helsinki Declaration and 191

was approved by Central Jutland Region Committee on Health Research Ethics. MEG data 192

were acquired in seventeen healthy right-handed (handedness assessed using Oldfield (1971)) 193

native speakers of Danish (age range 18–27 years, 12 females) with normal hearing and no 194

record of neurological impairments. All participants gave written consent before the start of 195

the experiment and received payment for their participation. 196

Stimuli 197

Since we wished to address a range of different neurolinguistic processes – those at lexical, 198

semantic and syntactic levels – we chose stimulus items which could enable us to contrast a 199

combination of different linguistic phenomena while controlling for acoustic features (see 200

Fig. 1 for examples of the stimuli used). To this end, we followed a previously suggested 201

Page 11: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 9 of 47

strategy (Gansonre, Højlund, Leminen, Bailey, & Shtyrov, 2018) and selected a set of spoken 202

Danish-language stimuli which (i) belonged to different lexical and semantic categories 203

(action-related verb, abstract verb, object-related noun and meaningless pseudoword), (ii) 204

were close in terms of phonology so we could compare them directly with minimal 205

acoustic/phonetic confounds, and which (iii) could be modified morpho-syntactically in the 206

exact same way and nonetheless exhibit different linguistic properties (i.e., grammatically 207

correct vs. incorrect) such that we could test the very same contrasts in different linguistic 208

contexts. 209

210

211

Figure 1

(A) Examples of spectrograms of spoken stimuli used in the experiment. (B) Examples of

waveforms plotted on top of each other.

212

These requirements led to the choice of four main base stimuli: bide ([biːðə], to bite), gide 213

Page 12: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 10 of 47

([giːðə], to bother), mide ([miːðə], a mite), *nide ([niːðə], *pseudoword). These were 214

presented as such. Note that they have identical CVCV phonological structure and only differ 215

in the first consonant. The second syllable [ðə], which allows recognition of the lexical items, 216

is the same across all items. To ensure that the full recognition of each particular word form 217

in the restricted experimental context is only possible at the second syllable, we also 218

included, in a 1:1 ratio with all other stimuli, all four first syllables in isolation: [biː], [giː], 219

[miː] and [niː]. These served as fillers to ensure identical acoustic divergence point across the 220

four types, to be used for time-locking brain activity to, and were not analysed as such. 221

222

The above quadruplet provided us with a way to address both lexical and semantic contrasts. 223

By estimating the brain activity elicited by the same word-final syllable [ðə], we could 224

compare, on the one hand, word vs. meaningless pseudoword activation, putatively indicating 225

lexical access, which we expected to find its reflection in an automatic activation of the core 226

left temporo-frontal language system (Tyler & Marslen-Wilson, 2008). On the other hand, by 227

comparing action vs. non-action items we could address semantically-specific aspects of 228

these activations. Previous EEG, MEG and fMRI research has indicated automatic 229

involvement of the brain’s motor system in the comprehension of action-related verbs 230

(Pulvermüller, 2005; Pulvermüller & Fadiga, 2010); we therefore expected more pronounced 231

centro-frontal activity for the action verb bide, but not the concrete noun mide. 232

233

Based on the above base forms, we produced further stimuli that included a balanced 234

morphosyntactic contrast. We took advantage of Danish morphology and the fact that the 235

morphemes -(e)t and -(e)n can be used to express the past participle of verbs and definiteness 236

on common nouns. This enabled us to compare the inflected items based on their syntactic 237

congruence or incongruence, e.g. -n in miden vs. *giden, and -t in gidet vs. *midet (where * 238

Page 13: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 11 of 47

indicates a violation of the stem/affix syntactic agreement). Note that each of these pairs have 239

identical codas (t/n) that lead to grammatical/morphosytactic violation in a counterbalanced 240

fashion: each of them is correct in combination with one but not the other stem. These were 241

presented, in equal proportion along with the other stimuli above, to make sure syntactic 242

properties are only recognised at the very last consonant. To balance for these acoustic 243

modifications, we also included similar items based on other forms (bide[n/t] and nide[n/t], 244

all meaningless), which were used to make a balanced design, but not analysed as such. 245

246

The stimuli were made based on a digital recording of a male native speaker of Danish in an 247

anechoic chamber (recording bandwitdh: 44k Hz, 16bit, stereo). The first and second 248

syllables of four CVCV stimuli were recorded independently, in order to avoid possible co-249

articulation effects, and cross-spliced together, such that the second syllables were physically 250

identical across all items. The second syllable commenced at 300 ms after the onset of the 251

first one, and this was the earliest time (the so-called disambiguation point) when any lexical 252

or semantic effects could be expected in the MEG data. 253

254

To produce the morphosyntactic items, ending in [t] or [n], we cross-spliced recordings of 255

these two morphemes onto the four main stems, in order to obtain words either violating or 256

respecting rules of Danish morphology such that the exact same phonemes completed 257

syntactic or asyntactic forms in a counterbalanced fashion. These morphemes became distinct 258

at 408 ms after the word onset, and this was therefore the earliest time any morphosyntactic 259

contrasts could affect the brain responses. 260

261

The sounds were matched for loudness, with a 1.93 dB drop between the first and the second 262

Page 14: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 12 of 47

syllables so that our stimuli sounded as natural as possible and normalised to have identical 263

power (measured as root-mean-square, RMS). All sound editing was done using Adobe 264

Audition CS6 software (Adobe Inc., San Jose, CA). 265

266

In sum, the stimulus set included four CV syllables, four CVCV stems, four CVCV+[t] and 267

four CVCV+[n] forms, all strictly controlled for phonological and acoustic properties. These 268

were combined, in a pseudorandom fashion, in a single auditory sequence ensuring that the 269

stimuli’s lexical, semantic and syntactic properties were available at stringently defined 270

times. 271

272

Procedure 273

The MEG recording was conducted in an electromagnetically shielded and acoustically 274

attenuated room (Vacuum Schmelzer Gmbh, Hanau, Germany). During the recordings, 275

participants were instructed to focus on watching a silent film and to pay no attention to the 276

sounds. The auditory stimuli were controlled using Neurobehavioral Systems Presentation 277

v16 (www.neurobs.com) and presented through in-ear-tubes (Etymotic ER-30) binaurally at 278

50 dB above individual auditory threshold. 279

280

All sixteen stimuli were presented equiprobably in a single data acquisition seesion inter-281

mixed in a continuous manner, with 100 pseudorandom repetitions of each stimulus resulting 282

in 1600 epochs in total. The stimulus onset was fixed at 1000 ms (The fixed ISI was based 283

on previous studies of automatic neurolinguistics processing using non-attend designs, 284

which served as the starting point for the current paradigm). The total recording time was 285

28 minutes. 286

Page 15: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 13 of 47

287

MEG data were acquired with an Elektra Neuromag Triux MEG setup (Elekta Neuromag Oy, 288

Helsinki, Finland), with 102 magnetometers and 204 planar gradiometers; for eye movement 289

and heartbeat artifact detection 2 bipolar EOG and 1 bipolar ECG recordings were taken. 290

Cardinal landmarks and additional head points were digitised using a Polhemus FASTRAK 291

setup (Polhemus, Vermont, USA). Data were recorded at 1000 Hz, a high pass filter of 0.1 Hz 292

and low pass of 330 Hz were applied online. Head position and head movements were 293

continuously tracked using four Head Position Indicator Coils (HPIs). The participants were 294

lying still on a non-magnetic patient bed, with their head as close to the top of the helmet as 295

possible, the MEG dewar being in supine position. 296

Data preprocessing 297

All data were preprocessed using MNE-python version 0.16 software package (Gramfort et 298

al., 2013). First, continuous data were bandpass filtered from 1 to 95 Hz, downsampled to 299

500 Hz, and epoched into single-trial epochs of 1000-ms duration, starting 100 ms before and 300

ending 900 ms after stimulus onset. Bad channels were automatically detected and 301

interpolated, epochs with excessive bad channels discarded and outlier trials removed using 302

an automatic approach (as implemented in Autoreject utility, see Jas, Engemann, Bekhti, 303

Raimondo, & Gramfort, 2017, for details). On average per subject there were 23.24 bad 304

channels (median: 32, std: 10.93) and 12.9 (std: 18.66) bad epochs. No signal-space 305

separation transformation (SSS, also known as maxfiltering) was applied at any stage of 306

the preprocessing. Thereby cleaned epoch data were bandpass-filtered into five frequency 307

bands (Dalal et al., 2011): α within 8-12 Hz, β (13-30 Hz), γ-low (30-45 Hz), γ-medium (55-308

70 Hz) and γ-high (70-90 Hz). 309

Page 16: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 14 of 47

Source reconstruction 310

For each participant, a T1 and T2 structural magnetic-resonance images (MRIs) were 311

obtained using a Siemens Tim Trio 3T MR scanner. The images were segmented with 312

separate surfaces created for the grey matter, inner skull and skin using SimNIBS utility 313

(Thielscher, Antunes, & Saturnino, 2015). For each subject, individual 3-layer boundary 314

element model (BEM) was calculated together with individual forward models. A common 315

template grey matter surface was created by averaging all of the study participants, using 316

FreeSurfer software (Dale, Fischl, & Sereno, 1999). 317

Source reconstruction was carried out using an LCMV beamformer (Van Veen, Van 318

Drongelen, Yuchtman, & Suzuki, 1997) using planar gradiometer data and previously 319

developed (Westner, 2017; Westner & Dalal, 2017) Hilbert beamformer. The decision to use 320

only gradiometers was choosen as mixing channel types is not trivial due to magnetometers 321

and planar gradiometers producing values of different scales, furthermore planar 322

gradiometers are less sensitive to external magnetic sources and have a better signal-to-noise 323

ratio compared to magnetometers (Hari, Joutsiniemi, & Sarvas, 1988). First, for each 324

frequency band of interest (α, β, γ-low, γ-medium, and γ-high), the epochs were bandpass-325

filtered for each subject without subjtracting the evoked signal from the single trials, as we 326

were interested in investigating the complete information contained in the responses time-327

locked to the auditory stimuli. Secondly, an adaptive filter was created by combining 328

responses to all the stimuli in the paradigm, using a 3-layer BEM and a cortically constraint 329

source space. The adaptive filter was computed using a data covariance matrix based on all 330

time points of the bandpass-filtered epochs; the covariance matrix was not regularized prior 331

to inversion. Source orientation was optimised by using the orientation of maximum signal 332

power (cf. Sekihara & Nagarajan, 2008). Neural Activity Index (NAI, cf. Sekihara & 333

Nagarajan, 2008), which incorporates the weight normalisation using a unit-gain 334

Page 17: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 15 of 47

beamformer, was selected as the output value. Third, after the adaptive filters were created, 335

the single-trial epoched data were Hilbert-transformed and the adaptive filter was applied to 336

the complex data, providing a source reconstruction of the Hilbert-transformed single-trial 337

data. Lastly, we calculated Inter-trial Phase Coherence (ITPC) of thereby obtained single-trial 338

source space data. This was done for each time point and in each source space location 339

indenpendtly using the equation below: 340

ITPC =

where n is the number of trials, eik provides a complex polar representation of the phase angle 341

k on trial r for the time-frequency point tf, where frequency is the frequency band (for a 342

thorough review, see Cohen, 2014, Chapter 19, esp. pp. 244-245). Thie resulted in a single 343

ITPC time course for each point in the source space for each subject. 344

345

After the source space ITPC data were calculated for each subject, the individual data were 346

morphed onto a common template surface (5124 vertices) created as the mean over all 347

individual participants for data standardisation across the group. Finally, the data were 348

smoothed with a 10 ms rolling window mean in temporal dimension for each source 349

independently. 350

Multivariate pattern analysis 351

For each participant, the morphed ITPC time series for each point in the source space was 352

extracted based on the contrast in question. Common for all the Multivariate Pattern Analysis 353

(MVPA) was the classification over time, i.e. for each time point a classifier pipeline was 354

applied giving a classification score over time. We used the entire cortical source space for 355

the classification at each time point. As it was comprised of 5124 vertices, it gave 5124 356

Page 18: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 16 of 47

features per time point. 357

358

The classifier pipeline was constructed in MNE-python using scikit-learn utility (Pedregosa 359

et al., 2011) and composed of three steps: First, the features were standardised (z-scored); the 360

standardisation was done across all vertices in the source space at each time point 361

independently using the training set and then applired to the test set. Second, feature selection 362

was done using cross-validated Lasso model (stratified folds, n=4) to create a sparse feature 363

space that was adaptive for each time point, i.e. allowing for number of relevant features to 364

be different for different time points. Lastly, a logistic regression (C=1) was used to classify 365

the two contrasts, and Receiver Operating Characteristic Area Under the Curve (ROC-366

AUC) was used as the classification score. The pipeline was applied across subjects, 367

meaning that we obtained a decoding score over time for all participants and, hence, we only 368

looked for effects that could hold across the subjects in the tested population. 369

370

MVPA can easily overfit data (i.e. become biased towards a specific response) and, in order 371

to prevent that from happening, cross-validation was used for the pipeline. Simply put, cross-372

validation makes use of all the data by first splitting the data into two smaller data sets. One 373

set, called the training set, is used for standardising the features and fitting (training) the 374

MVPA model. The other data set, called the test set, is then used to test accuracy of the fitted 375

model (i.e. trained model) by trying to predict the class of the new label and, by comparing 376

the prediction to the actual class of the test set, we can calculate the ROC-AUC for the 377

model. By creating new training and test sets from the full data set, it is possible to use all the 378

data for both training and testing (see Varoquaux et al. 2017 for more details and strategies 379

for cross-validating brain imaging data). 380

Page 19: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 17 of 47

381

All the steps in the pipeline were cross-validated with stratified folds (n=5). Stratified folds 382

imply that ratio of classes in the all the data is maintained in all the cross-validated folds; e.g. 383

in the lexical condition there are 3 real words and one pseudoword for each participant, so in 384

the stratified folds the ratio 3:1 would remain such that even when shuffled there will always 385

be a 3:1 ratio. For the semantic condition and morphosyntax comparison the ratio was 1:1, 386

i.e. 50%. We had 17 participants in the study and it was, therefore, impossible to have an 387

equal number in each of the cross-validation folds and to make cross-validation splits that 388

have the exact same number of participants in all folds and keep the ratio the same. 389

However, by using stratified folds we kept the balance between the folds as equal as possible. 390

391

To test for statistical significance of the classification we used permutation tests (Ojala & 392

Garriga, 2010). A permutation test was performed for each time point independently. First, 393

the arrangement of the labels was shuffled, e.g. action verb and object noun in the semantic 394

condition, such that the data might be from an action verb but the label tell the MVPA 395

algorithm that it is an object noun. By repeatedly shuffling the labels and running the 396

classification (n=2000) we can build a null distribution of random ROC-AUC scores. This 397

null distribution is interpreted as the distribution of what the ROC-AUC score could be just 398

by chance. Hence, we can then assess the classification score we had from the real labels by 399

comparing to the null distribution. If number of random scores that are better than the actual 400

classification score is less than or equal 5%, the classification score is said to be better than 401

chance, where 5% is the level choosen. 402

403

To optimise the computational time, only classification scores that surpassed the threshold 404

Page 20: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 18 of 47

(calculated, for each frequency band independently, as the mean score of the baseline plus 1.5 405

standard deviations of the baseline) were tested for statistical significance. So, for each time 406

point where the ROC-AUC score was above the threshold we made a permutation test (n = 407

2000). As the minimum p-value that can be obtained with a permutation test is dependent on 408

the number of permutations run, the p-value is defined as pu = ( )( )

, where b is the number 409

of times the permutation was equal or more extreme then the observed classification value 410

and m is the number of permutations, and 1 is added to b and to m to ensure that the 411

estimated p-value is not zero and to avoid division by zero (see Puolivali, Palva, & Palva 412

(2018); and Phipson & Smyth (2010)). So, the p-value in a permutation test does not behave 413

in the same way as a p-values in a parametric test, it is rather a description of observed 414

probability that the given classifier is better than chance. Since we perform a series of 415

permutations, i.e. one for each time point, we need to consider the multiple comparision 416

problem, i.e. the problem that some tests will be significient purely by chance and not be a 417

true effect. We opted to apply cluster threshold correction, i.e. there needs to be a continuous 418

range of significant time points (at least 10 ms or longer) to accept it as a non-random effect. 419

The 10 ms threshold is based on previous language and auditory research (Edmonds & 420

Krumbholz, 2014; Hagoort & Brown, 2000; Wang, Zhu, & Bastiaansen, 2012), typically 421

using 10 ms as the shortest bin duration to test. It should be noted that only two of the thirteen 422

clusters reported as significant at 10 ms, the rest are longer, ranging from 12 ms to 42 ms. 423

424

For each linguistic contrast we fitted the classification pipeline independently. For the lexical 425

contrast, we tested if we could classify lexical features, i.e. if the participant heard a real 426

word vs. an acoustically similar pseudoword (see the Stimuli section above). In the semantic 427

contrast, we tested classification of the action verb vs. object noun. In the syntactic contrast, 428

we tested whether we could correctly classify ungrammatical vs. grammatically correct items 429

Page 21: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 19 of 47

(irrespective of their acoustic features, i.e. *midet, *giden vs. miden, gidet). Each contrast 430

was tested from the relevant disambiguation point (DP: 300 ms for lexis and semantics and 431

408 ms for morphosyntax) through the end of the epoch. Latencies reported in the Results 432

section below are measured relative to DP. 433

Results 434

Lexical contrast 435

For the lexical contrast (see Figures 2, 3, 4, Table 1) we found that it was possible to 436

significantly decode words vs. pseudowords in the β, γ-low, γ-medium and γ-high bands. The 437

earliest significant decoding was achieved in the γ-medium band already at 62-76 ms after 438

the disambiguation point. The pattern of classifier-selected features in the left hemisphere 439

included the frontal lobe (BA 44, border of BA 6 / BA 8, BA 4), parietal BA 7, junction of BA 440

7 / 39, and BA 39/19. In the right hemisphere, features in temporal lobe BA 22 and frontal 441

BA 9, BA4 were selected. The highest ROC-AUC score was in the γ-low band at 224 ms after 442

DP (ROC-AUC: 94.35%, SD: 4.5%). The cluster including this AUC-ROC peak spanned 443

from 222 ms to 238 ms and involved a broad pattern of features including, in the left 444

hemisphere, temporal lobe (BA 22), frontal areas (BA 11, border of BA10/47, BA44 445

dorsal/posterior), and parietal areas (BA 40, BA 7 as well as BA 3/1/2). In the right 446

hemisphere, it included the temporal lobe (BA 22, BA 23, BA 43), frontal areas (BA 11, 447

border of BA 9/10/46, posterior dorsal BA44), and parietal areas (BA 39, BA 40, BA 7, BA 448

1/2). No significant classification results were obtained in the α-band. 449

Page 22: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 20 of 47

450

Figure 2

Top: heatmap of significant clusters across three linguistic contrasts, five frequency bands

and time. Lexical condition in blue colours, semantic in green, and syntax in red. Bottom:

surface topogpraphy of significant effects. For all conditions, colours go from lighter to

darker as latency becomes longer.

451

Table 1

452

Lexical

band peak (%)

peak std (%)

Peak time (ms)

Cluster start (ms)

Cluster end (ms)

Cluster length (ms)

Cluster mean (%)

Cluster std (%)

-medium 88,53 6,43 66 62 76 14 83,20 4,57 -low 94,35 4,50 224 222 238 16 85,02 9,68

-high 87,88 9,29 358 350 366 16 80,19 4,95 -low 87,65 11,08 440 432 448 16 81,34 4,99 -low 87,71 8,83 516 510 534 24 81,34 4,36

85,97 14,95 538 530 546 16 80,25 4,32 Table 1: Table of significant clusters in the lexical condition sorted by time from the 453

divergence point. Peak is highest ROC-AUC scores of the cluster. Peak std is the standard 454

Page 23: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 21 of 47

deviation of cross-validation folds for the peak ROC-AUC score. Peak time is the time of the 455

peak from DP. Cluster start is the start time of the cluster from DP. Cluster end is the end 456

time of the cluster from DP. Cluster length is the length of the cluster. Cluster mean is the 457

mean ROC-AUC score of the cluster. Cluster std is the standard deviation of the Cluster mean 458

across cross-validation folds. 459

460

461

Figure 3 462

Page 24: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 22 of 47

463

Figure 3. (A) Model patterns: interpreting coefficients of a machine learning model is not

trivial and a high coefficient value does not necessitate a high signal value in the MEG data

(see Haufe et al., 2014 for details). “Model patterns” are a way to highlight the signal in a

Page 25: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 23 of 47

neurophysiological sensible way that is directly interpretable compared to the raw

coefficients (Haufe et al., 2014). We show top and bottom 5% of the patterns in the -low

band from 222 to 238 ms. Blue colours are areas of activation able to predict real words and

yellow/red are areas used to predict pseudo word. (B) Average top and bottom 5% of ITPC

difference; blue colours indicate higher ITPC for real words and yellow/red indicate higher

ITPC for pseudo word -low band from 222 to 238 ms. (C) Average ITPC over time; solid

lines are the average of the selected features, dashed lines are the average of all vertices in

the source space. Time zero is the divergence point, when stimuli could be recognised from

the available acoustic information.

464

465

Figure 4. Heatmap of ROC-AUC scores for all bands in lexical condition. Note that chance

in this condition is 75%. Time is relative to DP

466

Semantic contrast 467

In the semantic contrast (see figures 2, 5, 6, Table 2), the peak classification score was found 468

in the α-band at 106 ms after DP (AUC-ROC: 91.11%, SD: 12.96%). The cluster including 469

Page 26: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 24 of 47

the peak AUC-ROC was between 80-122 ms after the onset of the second syllable 470

disambiguating the semantics of the particular form. The cluster comprised a pattern of 471

features including both temporal (BA 22) and frontal areas (BA 9, BA 44, BA 45, BA46) of 472

the left hemisphere. In the right hemisphere, in addition to the temporal (BA 21, BA 22, BA 473

38, BA 42) and the frontal lobe (BA 6, BA 9, BA 11, BA 44), it also involved parietal areas 474

(BA 7 and BA 2). 475

Table 2

476

Semantics

band peak (%)

peak std (%)

Peak time (ms)

Cluster start (ms)

Cluster end (ms)

Cluster length (ms)

Cluster mean (%)

Cluster std (%)

91,11 12,96 106 80 122 42 68,21 9,77 75,00 19,08 138 134 146 12 69,62 5,25

-low 84,58 9,01 224 220 230 10 71,85 7,06 70,00 13,43 256 254 264 10 66,53 3,37 85,83 3,74 584 574 590 16 71,71 7,62

Table 2: Table of significant clusters in the Semantic condition sorted by time from the 477

divergence point. See the legend of table 1 for an explanation of the columns. 478

479

Page 27: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 25 of 47

480

Figure 5. (A) Model patterns (see also Figure 4 legend): top and bottom 5% of the patterns in

Page 28: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 26 of 47

the the α band from 80 to 122 ms. Blue colours are areas used to predict action verb and

yellow/red are areas used to predict object noun. (B) Average top and bottom 5% of ITPC

difference, blue colours indicateing higher ITPC for action verb and yellow/red indicating

higher ITPC for object noun from 80 to 122 ms. (C) Average ITPC over time, solid lines are

the average of the selected features, dashed lines are the average of all vertices in the source

space. Time zero is the divergence point, when stimuli could be recognised from the available

acoustic information.

481

482

Figure 6. Heatmap of ROC-AUC scores for all bands in semantic condition.

483

(Morpho)syntactic contrast 484

The results of MVPA classification of grammatically correct vs. incorrect inflections (see 485

figure 2, 7, 8, table 3) showed the peak ROC-AUC score in the γ-low band at 90 ms after the 486

syntactic DP (ROC-AUC: 69.22%, SD: 13.05%). The significant classification cluster on the 487

left hemisphere included the temporal lobe (BA 21, BA 22, BA 37, BA 41), frontal areas (BA 488

4, BA 9 BA 10, BA 11), as well as parietal areas (BA 40/7/2), and occipital area BA 19. In the 489

Page 29: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 27 of 47

right hemisphere, it involved temporal (BA 21, 22, 38, 42), frontal (BA 44, 11, 4, 6, 9), 490

parietal (BA 39, BA 40, 2, 1, 7), and occipital (BA 18, 9) areas. 491

Table 3

Syntax

band peak (%)

peak std (%)

Peak time (ms)

Cluster start (ms)

Cluster end (ms)

Cluster length (ms)

Cluster mean (%)

Cluster std (%)

-low 69,22 13,05 90 85 98 12 62,57 9,16 -low 68,36 11,57 302 300 314 14 64,04 5,10

Table 3: Table of significant clusters in the syntax condition sorted by time from the 492

divergence point. See the legend of table 1 for an explanation of the columns. 493

494

Page 30: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 28 of 47

495

Figure 7. (A) Model patterns: top and bottom 5% of the patterns in the -low band from 84

to 98 ms. Blue colours are areas used to predict correct syntax and yellow/red are areas used

to predict incorrect syntax. (B) Average top and bottom 5% of ITPC difference. Blue colours

indicate higher ITPC for correct syntax and yellow/red indicate higher ITPC -low band

from 84 to 98 ms (C) Average ITPC over time; solid lines are the average of the selected

Page 31: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 29 of 47

features, dashed lines are the average of all vertices in the source space. Time is relative to

the divergence point.

496

Figure 8. Heatmap of ROC-AUC scores for all bands in syntax condition.

497

Classicification of amplitude data 498

We also tested ad hoc if we could decode language properties using amplitude data. We only 499

found significant clusters in semantic in the -medium band from 500 ms to 512 ms after DP. 500

For the morphosyntactic condition we found that we could classify correctly in the -high 501

bands (from 114 ms to 128 ms after DP) and band (from 248 ms to 262 ms after DP). As 502

we were not able to decode all conditions successfully, we have left out the amplitude results 503

from further discussion. 504

505

Discussion 506

In this study, we suggested and tested a paradigm which, in the absence of focussed attention 507

Page 32: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 30 of 47

on the auditory input or any explicit stimulus-focussed behavioural task, addressed different 508

levels of neurolinguistic information processing in the brain using a carefully crafted set of 509

spoken stimuli with strictly controlled acoustic/psycholinguistic contrasts. We registered the 510

brain’s activity elicited by these speech stimuli using high-density whole-head MEG set-up, 511

and analysed it using machine learning-based multivariate pattern analysis techniques applied 512

to inter-trial phase coherence in a range of frequency bands, at the level of cortical sources 513

calculated using individual MRIs. The results indicated that, by using this approach, we were 514

able to successfully classify lexical, semantic and morphosyntactic contrasts (see Table 4 for 515

an overview of results). These effects were exhibited in different frequency bands and at 516

different times. Below, we will briefly discuss these results in more detail. 517

518

Table 4

Condition

Lexical condition Semantic condition Syntax condition

Band Peak score

Peak std

Peak time

Peak score

Peak std

Peak time

Peak score

Peak std

Peak time

- - - 91,11 12,96 106 - - -

85,97 14,95 538 75,00 19,08 138 - - -

-low 94,35 4,50 224 84,58 9,01 224 69,22 13,05 90

-medium 88,53 6,43 66 - - - - - -

-high 87,88 9,29 358 - - - - - - 519

Table 4: Table of peak scores for each bands and condition, dash (-) indicate no signifact 520

cluster. Peak score is highest ROC-AUC scores of the cluster. Peak std is the standard 521

deviation of cross-validation folds for the peak ROC-AUC score. Peak time is the time of the 522

peak from DP. 523

Page 33: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 31 of 47

524

Lexical contrast 525

In the lexical contrast, we found clusters of significant decoding performance (with a peak 526

classification score of ~94%) over the entire duration of the analysis epoch starting from ~60 527

ms after the divergence point. These predominantly occurred in the range (including all 528

three sub-bands tested) and were underpinned by activity in bilateral temporo-parietal and 529

frontal clusters. This early onset of lexical effects here is in line with previous ERF and ERP 530

studies that used similar non-attend auditory designs (MacGregor, Pulvermüller, van 531

Casteren, & Shtyrov, 2012; Pulvermüller & Shtyrov, 2006; Shtyrov & Lenzen, 2017) and 532

showed that the brain responds differently to meaningful words vs. meaningless pseudoword 533

stimuli from ~50-80 ms after the acoustic input allows to identify the lexicality of stimuli, 534

with additional lexicality-driven activity spanning across peaks until ~400 ms. Such 535

ERP/ERF results (Shtyrov, Kujala, & Pulvermüller, 2010; see also, e.g. Shtyrov, Pihko, & 536

Pulvermüller, 2005) have reported similar perisylvian configuration of bilateral source 537

activations; importantly, here we find them for higher-frequency phase coherence values, not 538

reported previously. 539

540

Increased activity in the band (quantified as spectral power or as event-related 541

synchronisation) has previously been linked to lexical access possibly underpinning 542

synchronisation of neural elements making up distributed word memory circuits 543

(Lutzenberger, Pulvermüller, & Birbaumer, 1994; Pulvermüller, Preissl, Lutzenberger, & 544

Birbaumer, 1996; see also, e.g.. Tavabi, Embick, & Roberts, 2011). What our findings suggest 545

is that this process may involve multiple synchronisation steps expressed as phase resetting 546

(an important mechanisms in information processing, Canavier (2015) ) at different times, 547

Page 34: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 32 of 47

frequencies and neuroanatomical locations. 548

These previous studies have typically found a single power peak in low dynamics, whereas 549

we here find a series of activations at frequencies up to 90 Hz, potentially reflecting the 550

specific advantages of machine learning techniques in classifying distributed clusters of 551

activity. We also found a single significant cluster at the end of the epoch (>500 ms post-552

DP). This is fully in line with previous research highlighting the role of oscillations in 553

lexico-semantic storage and processing (e.g. Bakker, Takashima, van Hell, Janzen, & 554

McQueen, 2015; Brennan, Lignos, Embick, & Roberts, 2014), although our data suggest that 555

this activity is secondary in relation to the almost immediate phase resetting in the band. 556

Note that while a number of above studies have also identified -band activity in relation to 557

some aspects of speech processing (e.g. syllable tracking (Luo & Poeppel, 2007, 2012)), we 558

optimised our recording for time (with a view of potential applied use) that led to short 559

baselines not suitable for analysis. There is, however, compelling evidence of a relation 560

between θ and γ-band activity (Canolty et al., 2006), and future research using similar 561

paradigms could investigate whether the current ITPC findings may also have a counterpart 562

in the range. 563

Semantic contrast 564

The semantic contrast between the action verb and non-action noun has indicated activity 565

from ~100 ms after the divergence point in , and, to a smaller degree, in a lower range, 566

with peak classification scores of ~91, 75 and 85%, respectively. This mostly involved 567

temporo-frontal cortices, largely overlapping with the core language systems, but importantly, 568

indicated elevated frontal involvement, including clusters of activity in inferior-frontal 569

(BA44, 45) and motor (BA6) areas, which is compatible with the motor system involvement 570

in action word processing, posited previously (Pulvermüller et al., 2006). Previous EEG and 571

Page 35: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 33 of 47

MEG research into the brain basis of action-related semantics has found ERF and ERP 572

correlates between 80-200 ms indicating near-immediate and largely automated activation of 573

the motor strip in action word comprehension (Shtyrov et al., 2014; Shtyrov, Hauk, & 574

Pulvermüller, 2004). In line with this previous research, the features indicated here by the 575

semantic contrast did not include many in the temporal lobe but mostly in the inferior-frontal 576

and prefrontal areas in the left and right hemispheres. While the timing of this activation was 577

generally similar – and thus largely parallel – to the lexical processes above, the spectral 578

composition was different, with significant ITPC findings in a lower frequency range. 579

Changes in and especially power have previously been reported as related to single word 580

semantics, including action word semantics in particular (Bakker et al., 2015; Vukovic & 581

Shtyrov, 2014). What we show here is that the phase resetting likely linked to these changes 582

can reliably classify words with different meaning. In oscillatory space our results are in line 583

with those previously reported by Mamashli and colleagues (Mamashli, Khan, Obleser, 584

Friederici, & Maess, 2018) who investigated functional connectivity across regions of interest 585

and found activity in the , and low bands, while Haarman & Camaron (2005) reported a 586

change of coherence in the 10 – 14 Hz range while retaining a sentence. 587

Syntax contrast 588

Successful classification of the morphosyntactic contrast was achieved in the -low range 589

exclusively and was found to commence at ~100 ms after the syntactic divergence point. 590

While, in terms of the absolute stimulus timing, this difference was later than the lexical and 591

semantic findings above, it is important to note the syntactic disambiguation in the stimulus 592

itself was also possible at a later time, as it was determined by the final consonant (n/t). Thus, 593

in terms of the relative timing, the syntactic properties appear to be assessed roughly in 594

parallel to other tested features, overall in line with the view positing near-simultaneous onset 595

of neurolinguistics processing of different information types (Hagoort, 2004; Marslen-596

Page 36: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 34 of 47

Wilson, 1987). The timing is in line with the findings of ELAN literature that suggested 597

syntactic parsing to sometimes start as early as 50 ms in an automatic fashion (Herrmann, 598

Maess, & Friederici, 2011). Findings of links between band activity and syntactic parsing 599

have been reported in the literature previously (Lewis, Wang, & Bastiaansen, 2015), 600

including morphosyntactic processing of the kind broadly similar to that required in 601

comprehension of complex words used here (Levy, Hagoort, & Démonet, 2014). Importantly, 602

we used a fully balanced set of contrasts, with highly matched word stimuli in which 603

morphosyntactic (in)correctness was carried by physically the same phonemes, i.e. [n] and [t] 604

equally employed in both sets. This activation was underpinned by broad temporo-frontal 605

networks in both hemispheres, in line with previous literature (Bozic, Fonteneau, Su, & 606

Marslen-Wilson, 2015; Friederici, 2011; Hickok & Poeppel, 2007). We have also found 607

activity in parietal and occipital areas, not commonly reported in syntax studies and therefore 608

requiring validation in future research; notably, the values here do not reflect absolute activity 609

(e.g. activity) as such but rather reliable classification of activation, however small it may be. 610

Some studies investigating oscillatory neural activity in relation to syntactic processing have 611

found that there is a link between power and syntactic properties of phrases (e.g. Leiberg, 612

Lutzenberger, & Kaiser, 2006). While we do not find any similar activity in the range for 613

our morphosyntactic condition, that may be due to the use of single word stimuli with 614

morphosyntacitc modifications (rather than phrases with more elaborate syntax) in our 615

paradigm. 616

Of the three linguistic contrasts tested, the morphosyntactic one had the lowest decoding 617

percentage (peak at ~69%). One possible reason for this is that it used two acoustically 618

different stimuli, words ending in [n] and [t], with the grammatical correctness independent 619

of the word ending. The different acoustic properties — later onset of [t] than [n] and 620

different amplitude envelopes of the two — may have smeared the effect in time leading to 621

Page 37: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 35 of 47

poorer (but nevertheless significant) classification results. Future studies could use other 622

contrasts and different languages in order to improve classification results. 623

MVPA of MEG ITPC data as a tool for objective assessment of neurolinguistics 624

processes 625

Previous literature has shown that passive paradigms can be used to investigate language 626

processing (Gansonre et al., 2018; Shtyrov et al., 2012), an important first step towards 627

assessing participants and patient groups that have difficulties responding verbally or in other 628

behavioural ways. However, the next necessary step is to optimise the analysis of data 629

obtained in such paradigms. Important issues arise when trying to automate MEG analysis 630

both at preprocessing stage and at the level of identifying significant effects and differences 631

either within or between groups. What we present here is a tentative proposal to solve these 632

issues. By using the automated data cleanup protocol combined with a single-trial 633

beamformer reconstruction, we reduce the number of manual steps needed; and further, by 634

applying MVPA we avoid having to a priori select time and ROIs for the statistical 635

assessment of the difference between groups. The focus on oscillatory dynamics and ITPC 636

provides us the ability to assess activity in different frequency bands simultaneousy, resulting 637

in a more detailed picture of the neural activity related to neurolinguistic processing. 638

Previously, such analyses have been hampered by the number of tests that are needed to 639

statistically evaluate the differences across groups/conditions. In order to solve this problem, 640

we used MVPA, as this approach makes use of the powerful machine learning techniques 641

capable of assessing the effects statistically in an unbiased fashion. So, by combining ITPC 642

and MVPA we have the possibility to assess data in a detailed and yet exploratory manner. 643

644

Common to the effects across all conditions is that they last a relatively short time compared 645

Page 38: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 36 of 47

to the length of the trials. It is worth remembering that we are looking for clusters of time 646

where we can decode a difference in the signals that are compared, which may help explain 647

temporally sparse results. It is also worth noting that one cannot from a significant cluster 648

alone conclude what exact processes underpin it. However, comparing the ITPC data and the 649

patterns of the MVPA models together may provide an overall picture of the activity 650

undelyining the process at hand. 651

652

Conclusions 653

Using a passive paradigm, we probed several different neurolinguistic properties. Separating 654

the data into different frequency bands and looking at ITPC, we found that, by using MVPA, 655

we could classify lexical, semantic, and syntactic information processing. The best 656

classification results varied between the different neurolinguistic properties both in time and, 657

importantly, frequency bands, with lexical processes classified predominantly by broad , 658

semantic distinctions – by and , and morphosyntax – by low feature patterns. Crucially, 659

all types of processing commenced in a near-parallel fashion from ~100 ms after the auditory 660

information allowed for disambiguating the spoken input. This shows that individual 661

neurolinguistic processes take place near-simultaneiously and involve overalapping yet 662

distinct neuronal networks that operate at different frequency bands. Further investigations 663

are needed to understand the precise relation of the time courses, frequency bands, neuronal 664

subsrates and neurolinguistic properties, and to test the applicability of this approach to 665

detecting linguistic anomalies in various populations. 666

Competing interests 667

The authors declare that no competing interests exist. 668

Page 39: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 37 of 47

669

References 670

Baillet, S., Mosher, J. C., & Leahy, R. M. (2001). Electromagnetic brain mapping. IEEE 671

Signal Processing Magazine, 18, 14–30. https://doi.org/10.1109/79.962275 672

Bakker, I., Takashima, A., van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J. M. (2015). Changes in 673

Theta and Beta Oscillations as Signatures of Novel Word Consolidation. Journal of 674

Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(7), 1286–1297. 675

https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00801 676

Bastiaansen, M., & Hagoort, P. (2006). Oscillatory neuronal dynamics during language 677

comprehension. In C. Neuper & W. Klimesch (Eds.), Event-Related Dynamics of 678

Brain Oscillations (pp. 179–196). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-679

6123(06)59012-0 680

Bozic, M., Fonteneau, E., Su, L., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2015). Grammatical analysis as 681

a distributed neurobiological function: Bihemispheric Grammatical Analysis. 682

Human Brain Mapping, 36(3), 1190–1201. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22696 683

Brennan, J., Lignos, C., Embick, D., & Roberts, T. P. L. (2014). Spectro-temporal correlates 684

of lexical access during auditory lexical decision. Brain and Language, 133, 39–685

46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.006 686

Buzsáki, G., & Schomburg, E. W. (2015). What does gamma coherence tell us about inter-687

regional neural communication? Nature Neuroscience, 18, 484–489. 688

https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3952 689

Buzsáki, G., & Silva, F. L. (2012). High frequency oscillations in the intact brain. Prog 690

Neurobiol, 98, 241–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.02.004 691

Bzdok, D., Altman, N., & Krzywinski, M. (2018). Statistics versus machine learning. 692

Nature Methods, 15, 233. 693

Page 40: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 38 of 47

Canavier, C. C. (2015). Phase-resetting as a tool of information transmission. Current 694

Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 206–213. 695

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.12.003 696

Cannon, J., McCarthy, M. M., Lee, S., Lee, J., Börgers, C., Whittington, M. a, & Kopell, N. 697

(2013). Neurosystems: brain rhythms and cognitive processing. European Journal 698

of Neuroscience, n/a-n/a. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12453 699

Canolty, R. T., Edwards, E., Dalal, S. S., Soltani, M., Nagarajan, S. S., Kirsch, H. E., … Knight, 700

R. T. (2006). High Gamma Power Is Phase-Locked to Theta Oscillations in Human 701

Neocortex. Science, 313, 1626–1628. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1128115 702

Cohen, M. X. (2014). Analyzing neural time series data: theory and practice. MIT Press. 703

Dalal, S. S., Vidal, J. R., Hamamé, C. M., Ossandón, T., Bertrand, O., Lachaux, J.-P., & Jerbi, K. 704

(2011). Spanning the rich spectrum of the human brain: slow waves to gamma 705

and beyond. Brain Structure and Function, 216(2), 77–84. 706

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-011-0307-z 707

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. 708

Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage, 9, 179–194. 709

https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0395 710

Edmonds, B. A., & Krumbholz, K. (2014). Are Interaural Time and Level Differences 711

Represented by Independent or Integrated Codes in the Human Auditory Cortex? 712

Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 15(1), 103–114. 713

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162-013-0421-0 714

Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends 715

in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84. 716

Friederici, A. D. (2011). The Brain Basis of Language Processing: From Structure to 717

Function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357–1392. 718

Page 41: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 39 of 47

https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00006.2011 719

Friederici, A. D., Pfeifer, E., & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials during 720

natural speech processing: Effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic 721

violations. Cognitive Brain Research, 1(3), 183–192. 722

Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through 723

neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 474–480. 724

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011 725

Gansonre, C., Højlund, A., Leminen, A., Bailey, C., & Shtyrov, Y. (2018). Task-free auditory 726

EEG paradigm for probing multiple levels of speech processing in the brain. 727

Psychophysiology, e13216. 728

Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic 729

expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive 730

Neuroscience, 12(4), 556–568. 731

Haarmann, H., & Cameron, K. (2005). Active maintenance of sentence meaning in 732

working memory: Evidence from EEG coherences. International Journal of 733

Psychophysiology, 57(2), 115–128. 734

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.017 735

Hagoort, P. (2004). Integration of Word Meaning and World Knowledge in Language 736

Comprehension. Science, 304(5669), 438–441. 737

https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1095455 738

Hagoort, P. (2008). The fractionation of spoken language understanding by measuring 739

electrical and magnetic brain signals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal 740

Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 1055–1069. 741

https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2159 742

Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. (2000). ERP effects of listening to speech: semantic ERP effects. 743

Page 42: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 40 of 47

13. 744

Hari, R., Joutsiniemi, S. L., & Sarvas, J. (1988). Spatial resolution of neuromagnetic 745

records: theoretical calculations in a spherical model. Electroencephalography 746

and Clinical Neurophysiology, 71(1), 64–72. 747

Hasting, A. S., Kotz, S. A., & Friederici, A. D. (2007). Setting the Stage for Automatic 748

Syntax Processing: The Mismatch Negativity as an Indicator of Syntactic Priming. 749

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(3), 386–400. 750

https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.386 751

Haufe, S., Meinecke, F., Görgen, K., Dähne, S., Haynes, J.-D., Blankertz, B., & Bießmann, F. 752

(2014). On the interpretation of weight vectors of linear models in multivariate 753

neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 87, 96–110. 754

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.067 755

Hauk, O., Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2008). The time course of action and action-756

word comprehension in the human brain as revealed by neurophysiology. Journal 757

of Physiology-Paris, 102(1–3), 50–58. 758

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.013 759

Herrmann, B., Maess, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Violation of syntax and prosody—760

Disentangling their contributions to the early left anterior negativity (ELAN). 761

Neuroscience Letters, 490(2), 116–120. 762

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.039 763

Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature 764

Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393–402. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113 765

Jas, M., Engemann, D. A., Bekhti, Y., Raimondo, F., & Gramfort, A. (2017). Autoreject: 766

Automated artifact rejection for MEG and EEG data. NeuroImage, 159, 417–429. 767

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.030 768

Page 43: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 41 of 47

Kösem, A., & van Wassenhove, V. (2017). Distinct contributions of low- and high-769

frequency neural oscillations to speech comprehension. Language, Cognition and 770

Neuroscience, 32(5), 536–544. 771

https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1238495 772

Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect 773

semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205. 774

Laureys, S., Owen, A. M., & Schiff, N. D. (2004). Brain function in coma, vegetative state, 775

and related disorders. The Lancet Neurology, 3(9), 537–546. 776

Leiberg, S., Lutzenberger, W., & Kaiser, J. (2006). Effects of memory load on cortical 777

oscillatory activity during auditory pattern working memory. Brain Research, 778

1120(1), 131–140. 779

Levy, J., Hagoort, P., & Démonet, J.-F. (2014). A neuronal gamma oscillatory signature 780

during morphological unification in the left occipitotemporal junction. Human 781

Brain Mapping, 35(12), 5847–5860. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22589 782

Lewis, A. G., Wang, L., & Bastiaansen, M. (2015). Fast oscillatory dynamics during 783

language comprehension: Unification versus maintenance and prediction? Brain 784

and Language, 148, 51–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.01.003 785

Lopes da Silva, F. H. (2006). Event-related neural activities: what about phase? Progress 786

in Brain Research, 159, 3–17. 787

Luo, H., & Poeppel, D. (2007). Phase Patterns of Neuronal Responses Reliably 788

Discriminate Speech in Human Auditory Cortex. Neuron, 54(6), 1001–1010. 789

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.004 790

Luo, H., & Poeppel, D. (2012). Cortical Oscillations in Auditory Perception and Speech: 791

Evidence for Two Temporal Windows in Human Auditory Cortex. Frontiers in 792

Psychology, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00170 793

Page 44: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 42 of 47

Lutzenberger, W., Pulvermüller, F., & Birbaumer, N. (1994). Words and pseudowords 794

elicit distinct patterns of 30-Hz EEG responses in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 795

176(1), 115–118. 796

MacGregor, L. J., Pulvermüller, F., van Casteren, M., & Shtyrov, Y. (2012). Ultra-rapid 797

access to words in the brain. Nature Communications, 3(1). 798

https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1715 799

Majerus, S., Bruno, M.-A., Schnakers, C., Giacino, J. T., & Laureys, S. (2009). The problem 800

of aphasia in the assessment of consciousness in brain-damaged patients. 801

Progress in Brain Research, 177, 49–61. 802

Mamashli, F., Khan, S., Obleser, J., Friederici, A. D., & Maess, B. (2018). Oscillatory 803

dynamics of cortical functional connections in semantic prediction. Human Brain 804

Mapping. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24495 805

Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. 806

Cognition, 25(1–2), 71–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)90005-9 807

Moseley, R. L., Pulvermüller, F., & Shtyrov, Y. (2013). Sensorimotor semantics on the spot: 808

brain activity dissociates between conceptual categories within 150 ms. Scientific 809

Reports, 3, 1928. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep01928 810

Münte, T. F., Heinze, H.-J., Matzke, M., Wieringa, B. M., & Johannes, S. (1998). Brain 811

potentials and syntactic violations revisited: No evidence for specificity of the 812

syntactic positive shift. Neuropsychologia, 36(3), 217–226. 813

Näätänen, R., Paavilainen, P., Rinne, T., & Alho, K. (2007). The mismatch negativity 814

(MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review. Clinical 815

Neurophysiology, 118(12), 2544–2590. 816

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026 817

Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F. (1991). Syntactically based 818

Page 45: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 43 of 47

sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. 819

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(2), 151–165. 820

Ojala, M., & Garriga, G. C. (2010). Permutation tests for studying classifier performance. 821

The Journal of Machine Learning Research, 11, 1833–1863. 822

Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-823

path sentences: evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. 824

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 786. 825

Palva, J. M., Palva, S., & Kaila, K. (2005). Phase synchrony among neuronal oscillations in 826

the human cortex. J Neurosci, 25, 3962–3972. 827

https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4250-04.2005 828

Pedregosa, F., Varoquaux, G., Gramfort, A., Michel, V., Thirion, B., Grisel, O., … Duchesnay, 829

E. (2011). Scikit-learn: Machine Learning in Python. Journal of Machine Learning 830

Research, 12, 2825–2830. 831

Phipson, B., & Smyth, G. K. (2010). Permutation P-values Should Never Be Zero: 832

Calculating Exact P-values When Permutations Are Randomly Drawn. Statistical 833

Applications in Genetics and Molecular Biology, 9(1). 834

https://doi.org/10.2202/1544-6115.1585 835

Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews 836

Neuroscience, 6(7), 576–582. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706 837

Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a 838

cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(5), 351–360. 839

https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2811 840

Pulvermüller, F., Huss, M., Kherif, F., Moscoso del Prado Martin, F., Hauk, O., & Shtyrov, Y. 841

(2006). Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds. Proceedings of 842

the National Academy of Sciences, 103(20), 7865–7870. 843

Page 46: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 44 of 47

https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0509989103 844

Pulvermüller, F., Kujala, T., Shtyrov, Y., Simola, J., Tiitinen, H., Alku, P., … Näätänen, R. 845

(2001). Memory traces for words as revealed by the mismatch negativity. 846

Neuroimage, 14(3), 607–616. 847

Pulvermüller, F., Preissl, H., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1996). Brain rhythms of 848

language: nouns versus verbs. European Journal of Neuroscience, 8(5), 937–941. 849

Pulvermüller, F., & Shtyrov, Y. (2006). Language outside the focus of attention: The 850

mismatch negativity as a tool for studying higher cognitive processes. Progress in 851

Neurobiology, 79(1), 49–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.04.004 852

Puolivali, T., Palva, S., & Palva, J. M. (2018). Influence of multiple hypothesis testing on 853

reproducibility in neuroimaging research. BioRxiv. 854

https://doi.org/10.1101/488353 855

Ragazzoni, A., Grippo, A., Tozzi, F., & Zaccara, G. (2000). Event-related potentials in 856

patients with total locked-in state due to fulminant Guillain–Barré syndrome. 857

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37(1), 99–109. 858

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00098-2 859

Sauseng, P., & Klimesch, W. (2008). What does phase information of oscillatory brain 860

activity tell us about cognitive processes? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 861

32(5), 1001–1013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.03.014 862

Sekihara, K., & Nagarajan, S. S. (2008). Adaptive Spatial Filters for Electromagnetic Brain 863

Imaging. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-79370-0 864

Shtyrov, Y. (2010). Automaticity and attentional control in spoken language processing: 865

Neurophysiological evidence. The Mental Lexicon, 5(2), 255–276. 866

https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.5.2.06sht 867

Shtyrov, Y., Butorina, A., Nikolaeva, A., & Stroganova, T. (2014). Automatic ultrarapid 868

Page 47: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 45 of 47

activation and inhibition of cortical motor systems in spoken word 869

comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(18), 870

E1918–E1923. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1323158111 871

Shtyrov, Y., Hauk, O., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Distributed neuronal networks for 872

encoding category-specific semantic information: the mismatch negativity to 873

action words: Distributed memory traces for semantic information. European 874

Journal of Neuroscience, 19(4), 1083–1092. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0953-875

816X.2004.03126.x 876

Shtyrov, Y., Kujala, T., & Pulvermüller, F. (2010). Interactions between Language and 877

Attention Systems: Early Automatic Lexical Processing? Journal of Cognitive 878

Neuroscience, 22(7), 1465–1478. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21292 879

Shtyrov, Y., & Lenzen, M. (2017). First-pass neocortical processing of spoken language 880

takes only 30 msec: Electrophysiological evidence. Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(1), 881

24–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2016.1156663 882

Shtyrov, Y., Pihko, E., & Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Determinants of dominance: Is language 883

laterality explained by physical or linguistic features of speech? NeuroImage, 884

27(1), 37–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.003 885

Shtyrov, Y., Smith, M. L., Horner, A. J., Henson, R., Nathan, P. J., Bullmore, E. T., & 886

Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Attention to language: Novel MEG paradigm for 887

registering involuntary language processing in the brain. Neuropsychologia, 888

50(11), 2605–2616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.012 889

Tavabi, K., Embick, D., & Roberts, T. P. L. (2011). Spectral–temporal analysis of cortical 890

oscillations during lexical processing: NeuroReport, 22(10), 474–478. 891

https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283476b84 892

Teng, X., Tian, X., Rowland, J., & Poeppel, D. (2017). Concurrent temporal channels for 893

Page 48: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 46 of 47

auditory processing: Oscillatory neural entrainment reveals segregation of 894

function at different scales. PLOS Biology, 15(11), e2000812. 895

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2000812 896

Thielscher, A., Antunes, A., & Saturnino, G. B. (2015). Field modeling for transcranial 897

magnetic stimulation: A useful tool to understand the physiological effects of 898

TMS? Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2015 37th Annual 899

International Conference of the IEEE, 222–225. IEEE. 900

Towle, V. L., Yoon, H.-A., Castelle, M., Edgar, J. C., Biassou, N. M., Frim, D. M., … Kohrman, 901

M. H. (2008). ECoG gamma activity during a language task: differentiating 902

expressive and receptive speech areas. Brain, 131(8), 2013–2027. 903

https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn147 904

Tyler, L. K., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2008). Fronto-temporal brain systems supporting 905

spoken language comprehension. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 906

B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 1037–1054. 907

https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2158 908

Van Veen, B. D., Van Drongelen, W., Yuchtman, M., & Suzuki, A. (1997). Localization of 909

brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial 910

filtering. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 44(9), 867–880. 911

https://doi.org/10.1109/10.623056 912

Varela, F., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., & Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase 913

synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 914

229–239. https://doi.org/10.1038/35067550 915

Varoquaux, G., Raamana, P. R., Engemann, D. A., Hoyos-Idrobo, A., Schwartz, Y., & Thirion, 916

B. (2017). Assessing and tuning brain decoders: Cross-validation, caveats, and 917

guidelines. NeuroImage, 145, 166–179. 918

Page 49: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

Page 47 of 47

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.038 919

Von Stein, A., & Sarnthein, J. (2000). Different frequencies for different scales of cortical 920

integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. 921

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 38(3), 301–313. 922

Vukovic, N., & Shtyrov, Y. (2014). Cortical motor systems are involved in second-923

language comprehension: Evidence from rapid mu-rhythm desynchronisation. 924

NeuroImage, 102, 695–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.039 925

Wang, L., Zhu, Z., & Bastiaansen, M. (2012). Integration or Predictability? A Further 926

Specification of the Functional Role of Gamma Oscillations in Language 927

Comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. 928

https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00187 929

Westner, B. U. (2017). The Hilbert beamformer pipeline. Retrieved from https://brittas-930

summerofcode.blogspot.com/2017/08/the-hilbert-beamformer-931

pipeline_29.html 932

Westner, B. U., & Dalal, S. S. (2017). Faster than the brain’s speed of light: Retinocortical 933

interactions differ in high frequency activity when processing darks and lights. 934

BioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/153551 935

936

937 938

Page 50: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of

� � � �

Page 51: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of
Page 52: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of
Page 53: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of
Page 54: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of
Page 55: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of
Page 56: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of
Page 57: MVPA analysis of intertrial phase coherence of