myers-briggs® preferences and academic success in the

196
Graduate eses and Dissertations Iowa State University Capstones, eses and Dissertations 2013 Myers-Briggs® preferences and academic success in the first college semester Debra K. Sanborn Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: hps://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd Part of the Educational Assessment, Evaluation, and Research Commons , Higher Education Administration Commons , Higher Education and Teaching Commons , and the Personality and Social Contexts Commons is Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, eses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate eses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Sanborn, Debra K., "Myers-Briggs® preferences and academic success in the first college semester" (2013). Graduate eses and Dissertations. 13095. hps://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/13095

Upload: others

Post on 21-Oct-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Graduate Theses and Dissertations Iowa State University Capstones, Theses andDissertations

2013

Myers-Briggs® preferences and academic success inthe first college semesterDebra K. SanbornIowa State University

Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd

Part of the Educational Assessment, Evaluation, and Research Commons, Higher EducationAdministration Commons, Higher Education and Teaching Commons, and the Personality andSocial Contexts Commons

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State UniversityDigital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State UniversityDigital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationSanborn, Debra K., "Myers-Briggs® preferences and academic success in the first college semester" (2013). Graduate Theses andDissertations. 13095.https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/13095

Myers-Briggs® preferences and academic success

in the first college semester

by

Debra K. Sanborn

A dissertation submitted to the graduate faculty

in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

Major: Education (Educational Leadership)

Program of Study Committee: Daniel C. Robinson, Major Professor

Larry H. Ebbers Sharon K. Drake

Mack C. Shelley, II Loren W. Zachary

Iowa State University

Ames, Iowa

2013

Copyright © Debra K. Sanborn, 2013. All rights reserved.

ii

DEDICATION

This

dissertation

is dedicated to

Chad,

Delaney, and Deckard.

iii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................................. vii

LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................................................. xi

ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................ xii

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................. 1 Statement of the Problem .............................................................................................. 2 Purpose of the Study ..................................................................................................... 2 Research Questions ....................................................................................................... 4 Theoretical and Conceptual Framework ....................................................................... 5 Methodology ................................................................................................................. 8 Delimitations and Limitations ....................................................................................... 9 Significance of the Study ............................................................................................ 10 Definition of Terms ..................................................................................................... 11 Summary ..................................................................................................................... 11

CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW ................................................................................ 12 Introduction ................................................................................................................. 12 Typological Models .................................................................................................... 12 Myers-Briggs Type Indicator ...................................................................................... 13 Elements of Psychological Type ................................................................................. 15 Extraversion and Introversion ......................................................................... 16 Sensing and Intuition ...................................................................................... 17 Thinking and Feeling ...................................................................................... 17 Judging and Perceiving ................................................................................... 18 Feeling and Perceiving .................................................................................... 19 Problem in Review ...................................................................................................... 20 Myers-Briggs Research Perspective ........................................................................... 22 Learning style .................................................................................................. 23 Type and learning style ................................................................................... 24 Type, academic success, and persistence ........................................................ 28 Summary ..................................................................................................................... 33

CHAPTER 3. METHODOLOGY .......................................................................................... 35 Overview ..................................................................................................................... 35 Research Questions and Hypotheses .......................................................................... 38 Research Design .......................................................................................................... 41 Setting ......................................................................................................................... 43 Population and Sample ............................................................................................... 43 Reliability and Validity ............................................................................................... 44 Creation of the Research Dataset ................................................................................ 45

iv

Study Variables ........................................................................................................... 46 Dependent ....................................................................................................... 46 Independent ..................................................................................................... 46 Data Analysis .............................................................................................................. 47 Ethical Considerations ................................................................................................ 48 Limitations .................................................................................................................. 50 Delimitations ............................................................................................................... 50 Summary ..................................................................................................................... 51 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS ........................................................................................................ 52 Overview ..................................................................................................................... 52 Design Classification .................................................................................................. 53 Analysis of Research Questions and Hypotheses ....................................................... 54 Student background characteristics ................................................................. 54 Gender ................................................................................................. 55 STEM major ........................................................................................ 57 High-school percentile rank, composite ACT score means, and standard deviations .................................................................. 57 Summary ............................................................................................. 58 Differences in Myers-Briggs preference by cohort year and for the total Population ........................................................................................... 59 Differences in Myers-Briggs preference by male or female by cohort year and for total population ....................................................................... 78 Research sample type preference comparison with base sample ........ 79 Male type preference compared with base sample ............................. 79 Female type preference compared with base sample .......................... 82 Male and female 2004 distribution compared with base sample ........ 84 Male and female 2005 distribution compared with base sample ........ 86 Male and female 2006 distribution compared with base sample ........ 89 Male and female 2007 distribution compared with base sample ........ 91 Male and female 2008 distribution compared with base sample ........ 93 Male and female 2009 distribution compared with base sample ........ 96 Male and female 2010 distribution compared with base sample ........ 98 Male and female 2011 distribution compared with base sample ........ 99 Differences in Myers-Briggs preference for students with STEM majors by cohort year and for total population ............................................. 102 Research sample STEM and non-STEM major type preference comparison ............................................................................ 103 STEM major type preference comparison with base sample ............ 105 Non-STEM major type preference comparison with base sample ... 106 STEM and non-STEM 2004 distribution compared with base Sample ................................................................................... 108 STEM and non-STEM 2005 distribution compared with base Sample ................................................................................... 110

v

STEM and non-STEM 2006 distribution compared with base sample ................................................................................... 112 STEM and non-STEM 2007 distribution compared with base sample ................................................................................... 114 STEM and non-STEM 2008 distribution compared with base sample ................................................................................... 117 STEM and non-STEM 2009 distribution compared with base sample ................................................................................... 119 STEM and non-STEM 2010 distribution compared with base sample ................................................................................... 121 STEM and non-STEM 2011 distribution compared with base sample ................................................................................... 123 Differences in academic aptitude of ACT and high school percentile rank and Myers-Briggs preference by cohort year and for research sample ............................................................................................... 126

Differences in first-semester grade point and Myers-Briggs preference by cohort year and for research sample ............................................ 134 Relationship of ACT, high school percentile rank, and Myers-Briggs

preference to first-semester grade point by cohort year and for research sample ................................................................................. 143

Multicollinearity and singularity ....................................................... 143 Models examining MBTI, ACT and percentile rank as variables to GPA ............... 145 Summary ............................................................................................................................... 154 CHAPTER 5. DISCUSSION ................................................................................................ 157 Findings ..................................................................................................................... 159 Majority of students in the study have ENFP preferences ............................ 160 Differing frequency of type preferences for male and female students ........ 161 ENFP most frequent type for STEM and non-STEM majors ....................... 162 Variances exist between ACT and class rank and Myers-Briggs preference .......................................................................................... 163 Variances exist between first semester GPA and Myers-Briggs preference 163 ENFP preference has negative impact for some students ............................. 164 Conclusions ............................................................................................................... 164 Implications for Practice ........................................................................................... 166 Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research .......................................... 168 Validity challenges ........................................................................................ 173 Summary ................................................................................................................... 174 APPENDIX A. HUMAN SUBJECTS APPROVAL ............................................................ 176

APPENDIX B. MBTI SAMPLE ITEMS ............................................................................. 176

vi

REFERENCES ..................................................................................................................... 178

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS .................................................................................................... 182

vii

LIST OF TABLES

Table 3.1 Population sample for each cohort year ................................................................... 45 Table 3.2. Variables, coding scale, and source file of the data ................................................. 47 Table 3.3 Research questions, variables, and method of analysis ........................................... 49 Table 4.1. Gender distribution of students who enrolled fulltime, fall 2004–fall 2011 ............ 56 Table 4.2. Learning community distribution of students in STEM majors, who enrolled fulltime, fall 2004–fall 2011 .................................................................................... 56 Table 4.3. High school percentile rank, composite ACT score means, and standard deviations of students who enrolled fulltime, fall 2004–fall 2011 .......................... 57 Table 4.4. Type distribution of research sample (N=775) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 61 Table 4.5. Type distribution of 2004 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 64 Table 4.6. Type distribution of 2005 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 65 Table 4.7. Type distribution of 2006 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 67 Table 4.8. Type distribution of 2007 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 70 Table 4.9. Type distribution of 2008 cohort (N = 99) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 72 Table 4.10. Type distribution of 2009 cohort (N = 100) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 73 Table 4.11. Type distribution of 2010 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 75 Table 4.12. Type distribution of 2011 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ...................................................... 76

viii

Table 4.13. Male/female distribution of research sample (N = 775) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 80 Table 4.14. Male/female distribution of 2004 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 85 Table 4.15. Male/female distribution of 2005 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 87 Table 4.16. Male/female distribution of 2006 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 80 Table 4.17. Male/female distribution of 2007 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 92 Table 4.18. Male/female distribution of 2008 cohort (N = 99) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 94 Table 4.19. Male/female distribution of 2009 cohort (N = 100) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 97 Table 4.20. Male/female distribution of 2010 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual .............................................. 99 Table 4.21. Male/female distribution of 2011 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ............................................ 100 Table 4.22. STEM and non-STEM distribution of research sample (N = 775) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 104 Table 4.23. STEM and non-STEM distribution of 2004 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 109 Table 4.24. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2005 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 111 Table 4.25. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2006 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 113 Table 4.26. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2007 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 115 Table 4.27. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2008 Cohort (N = 99) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 118

ix

Table 4.28. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2009 cohort (N = 100) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 120 Table 4.29. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2010 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 122 Table 4.30. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2011 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual ........................ 124 Table 4.31. Cross tabulation means and standard deviations comparing ACT composite by MBTI for population, 2004-2011 .......................................................................... 127 Table 4.32. One-way ANOVA summary comparing the ACT composite by MBTI for research population and cohort years 2004-2011 .................................................. 129 Table 4.33. Summary of post hoc Tukey-Kramer HSD comparing research population ACT composite to the MBTI ................................................................................. 130 Table 4.34. Cross tabulation of means and standard deviations comparing percentile rank by

MBTI for the population, 2004-2011 ..................................................................... 132 Table 4.35. One-way ANOVA summary for each year, 2004-2011, and research sample for percentile rank compared to the MBTI ............................................................ 133 Table 4.36. Cross tabulation of means and standard deviations comparing first-semester grade point by MBTI for the research population and cohort years, 2004-2011 ... 136 Table 4.37. One-way ANOVA summary of each year, 2004-2011, and research sample for ACT composite in comparison to the MBTI .................................................... 137 Table 4.38. Summary of post hoc Tukey-Kramer HSD comparing first semester grade point to MBTI .............................................................................................. 139 Table 4.39. Contingency table analysis comparing Myers-Briggs preference to 2.0 grade point ....................................................................................................................... 140 Table 4.40. Contingency table analysis comparing 2005 cohort Myers-Briggs preference to 2.0 grade point ................................................................................................... 141 Table 4.41. Multiple regression of MBTI, ACT composite, and percentile rank for first- semester grade point for research sample, ascending R2 ....................................... 146 Table 4.42. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point for the research population .......... 146

x

Table 4.43. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2004 cohort (N = 97) ................... 148 Table 4.44. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2005 cohort (N = 96) ................... 149 Table 4.45. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2006 cohort (N = 95) ................... 149 Table 4.46. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2007 cohort (N = 96) ................... 150 Table 4.47. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2008 cohort (N = 99) ................... 151 Table 4.48. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2009 cohort (N = 100) ................. 151 Table 4.49. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2010 cohort (N = 97) ................... 152 Table 4.50. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance predicting students’ first semester grade point, 2011 cohort (N = 95) ................... 152

xi

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 4.1. Euler diagram of student group classifications, illustrating variable relationship categories among students ............................................................. 55

xii

ABSTRACT

This research examined aspects of Myers-Briggs® preferences and academic success

in the first college semester. Academic aptitude as measured by precollege characteristics of

ACT and class rank, academic performance during the first semester of college, and Myers-

Briggs preference were analyzed for their significance within a learning community at a

Midwest research university. Academic performance and Myers-Briggs preference were

compared between students based upon grade point success in the first semester, fall 2004 to

fall 2011. Statistical analyses were completed to determine if there is a relationship between

type preference and academic success. ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Perceiving, was the most frequent type preference for students in the sample.

ENFP was found to negatively relate to first-semester grade point for the research population

and two cohorts. Identifying a trend toward specific type preferences related to academic

achievement may provide support for the student population and enhance retention

interventions.

1

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

Student development theory helps one to understand differences among students who

are considering higher education. Understanding differences of psychological type and how

type pertains to learning style of students may also enhance student success. The assessment

of psychological type is based on the theory that human behavior is not random and that

patterns of mental functions exist in the population (Jung, 1971). Following this conceptual

foundation, the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator® (MBTI®) has become the most widely used

instrument for determining type preferences in business, personal coaching, and on college

campuses.

Psychological type assessment can been helpful to enable the detection of

interpersonal roadblocks and miscommunication related to type preferences, particularly for

students in the transition from high school to college. Hunter (2006) posited that “attention

to student characteristics, needs, behaviors, and experiences is central to creating and

sustaining successful transition initiatives” (p. 9). Through intentional examination of type

distribution and type theory related to learning preferences, opportunities emerge for

enabling students to understand more about themselves in the college transition.

During the past four decades, Myers-Briggs® type theory and the Myers-Briggs

assessment have became well known and widely utilized in a variety of education and

business settings. The accurate and ethical application of type theory has considerable value

for practitioners and research in many topic areas (Evans, Forney, Guido, Patton, & Renn,

2010; Hammer, 1996; VanSant, 2003; Stricker, Schiffman, & Ross, 1965).

2

Statement of the Problem

Specific, consistent differences in learning style personality temperament are

represented in typological theory (Robinson & Taylor, 2003). Although typological theories

of student development are helpful to understand characteristics unique to the individual

student, they are not able to explain individual changes or beliefs.

Myers-Briggs profile assessments collected from students during a first-year seminar

course were informally analyzed and sorted for a review of academic success and persistence

in the cohort. Through this process, it was discovered that the student population had

specific over-represented type preferences for two Myers-Briggs preferences in comparison

to national samples. An additional area of comparison was made of Myers-Briggs

preferences for students who had not achieved a 2.0 grade point average in the first semester

that also revealed over-represented type preferences. A disproportionate number of students

with the over-represented type preferences was also revealed to be withdrawing from the

institution prior to completing a degree. These circumstances suggest that psychological type

preferences could be a variable relating to student success for this student cohort within their

university. Additionally, if additional analysis of preference anomalies could be found as a

variable affecting academic success, then perhaps steps could be taken to positively enhance

the educational experience of students with these type preferences.

Purpose of the Study

Psychological type assessment can been helpful in detecting interpersonal roadblocks

and miscommunication related to type preferences, particularly for students in the transition

from high school to college. Through intentional examination of type distribution and type

3

research related to learning preferences, opportunities emerge to enable students to

understand more about themselves during the college transition.

Kalsbeek (1986, 2003) reported use of a tracking tool that aids universities in

reviewing Myers-Briggs data in comparison with available student data to provide a research

base for retention strategies. The study revealed that certain Myers-Briggs preferences were

significant in their influence on first semester grades. Type data were also found to correlate

with entering student profiles based on reasons for attending college, performance on college

admission standardized tests, and first-term academic achievement. The goal of the current

research was to reproduce portions of Kalsbeek’s study related to academic aptitude and

grade point correlation with Myers-Briggs preferences for a similar student sample. As

student retention and performance are important issues for all colleges and universities who

wish to recruit and graduate students, this study could aid in determining if students with

specific type preferences have more academic difficulty in their path to a college degree and,

if so, identify steps to ease this challenge.

This study examined whether student Myers-Briggs preferences correlate to academic

success in the first college semester. Descriptive and inferential statistics, and standard

multiple regression were used to analyze the relationship between first semester grade point

and variables of gender, STEM major, and academic aptitude. Ideally, this information will

provide introduction to further research for the use of type in early identification of students

requiring academic intervention.

4

Research Questions

If student type preferences and learning styles can be defined as incongruent to

institutional culture, lack of engagement may occur. As academic disengagement is a major

factor behind a decline in grades (Keup, 2006), identifying trends for academic success

related to psychological type that could be useful for student development.

The objective of this study was to investigate the collected type preferences for a

student group to analyze type preference distributions, learning style, and potential effects of

type on academic success in the first college semester. The following research questions

outlined the study:

1. What are the academic demographics of the students in the study, including academic

aptitude (ACT and high school class rank) and first-semester grade point average?

2. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students

in the study by each cohort year and for the research population in comparison to the

distribution of a national sample?

3. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences by gender

for students in the study by each cohort year and for the research sample in

comparison to the distribution of a national population?

4. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students

with STEM majors by each cohort year and the research population in the study in

comparison to the distribution of a national population?

5. Are there statistically significant differences in academic aptitude of ACT and high

school class rank and Myers-Briggs preference for students by each cohort year and

for the research population in the study?

5

6. Are there statistically significant differences in academic aptitude and Myers-Briggs

preference for students in the study by grade point and ability to achieve a 2.00 grade

point in the first college semester by each cohort year and for the research

population?

7. Is there a correlation of ACT, high school class rank, or Myers-Briggs preference to

first semester grade point by each cohort year and the research population?

These questions guided this quantitative study to assess perceived effects of type and

learning style. Combined with measures of academic aptitude, the results of this study could

aid student academic progress and persistence.

Theoretical and Conceptual Framework

Typological models of student development can be used to explain differences of

psychological type and how these differences affect student success. Three common

typological models of student development include: Holland’s Person-Environment Theory,

Kolb’s Theory of Experiential Learning, and the Myers-Briggs Type. Although typological

models cannot explain individual changes or beliefs, they are useful for understanding

differences and characteristics unique to each student and how these differences may

influence student development and academic success.

Holland’s (1997) Person-Environment Theory typological model seeks to explain

vocational behavior and suggests that our culture allows persons to be categorized by

personality type. Kolb’s (1984) Theory of Experiential Learning arranges individuals

according to a learning style model based upon how they learn and develop.

6

The basic premise of Myers-Briggs type is that individuals can have different

motivations and processes for getting through the day, but they will follow certain polar

configurations. The preference pairs include where a person gets his or her energy,

categorized as Extraversion and Introversion; how an individual takes in information or

Sensing and Intuiting; the decision-making process of Thinking and Feeling; and the

orientation to and organization in the outer world of Judging and Perceiving. Individuals use

each aspect of the personality pairs daily, but have a preference for one that is more

comfortable or useful to the self.

There is substantial literature related to the typology theoretical framework and

academic success variables related to student Myers-Briggs preferences. As student retention

and performance are important issues for all colleges and universities, this review may aid in

determining if students with specific type preferences have more academic difficulty in their

path to a college degree and, if so, identify steps to ease this challenge.

In a conceptual framework of this research, it is import to reflect the dichotomies of

psychological type in consideration to learning styles and influences of academic success.

Learning style is important to the composition of student success in college (Chesborough,

2005; 2009; DiTiberio & Hammer, 1993). Learning style can be described as the different

ways a student will approach critical thinking and the assimilation of new information or how

they process information. Although learning style is fairly consistent for most individuals, it

can be enhanced with usage, and developed by individual strengths that contribute to student

understanding (Evans et al., 2010).

Lawrence (2009) defined four learning styles and dispositions of learning related to

psychological type preferences. Cognitive style is the preferred function or pattern for

7

information processing. Attitude and interest patterns affecting the participation in learning

are the second aspect of learning style. A disposition to seek learning environments that

match interests is the third aspect, while a disposition to successfully use learning tools

completes the quadrant.

The Myers-Briggs indicator, although not an identifier of physical trait learning, is

beneficial in that it assesses learning preferences and processes rather than the learning

behaviors of most learning style inventories (Jensen, 2003). Jensen described the Myers-

Briggs as the most comprehensive assessment of learning style assessments attributing to

instrument norming, length of development time, and sixteen specific approaches to student

learning.

In an examination of 107,000 college students enrolled in 59 college majors,

Schaubhut and Thompson (2011) created a Myers-Briggs type table summary information

from a comparison to a database collection of all fulltime college students completing the

assessment. They posited that an understanding of psychological type preference may be

helpful for students as they plan their academic and institutional choices.

Allen (2007) addressed the debate of whether to measure learning or the learner by

suggesting that psychological type can be used to enhance learning strategies and incorporate

learning style into the demands of learning. Following two decades of research within a

community college study skills center, Allen hypothesized that students learn the same way

they think. As thinking is a concept of psychological type, then learning – like thinking – is,

therefore, innate.

The evolution of student learning and expectations has demonstrated opportunities for

learning style development and teaching growth. Erickson and Strommer (2005) posited that

8

research university models of creating and delivering knowledge do not successfully address

the learning styles of current students. They encouraged educators to incorporate small

group discussion, personal writing exercises, case studies, problem-based learning, and

experiential learning to meet student learners where they are in the classroom.

An understanding of psychological type as made available by the Myers-Briggs can

be a mechanism for assisting students and college administrators to understand learning and

student success in various post-secondary institutions. As university cultures are wide and

varying, establishing student fit within that culture through use of Myers-Briggs is one

method for enhancing student satisfaction and progress to graduation.

Methodology

A quantitative inquiry using secondary data was employed to investigate the

questions in this study. Descriptive and inferential statistics were used to examine Myers-

Briggs profile assessments in comparison with student and institutional data for academic

aptitude and first-semester grade point. The study was structured as quasi-experimental time

series design in that measures of the student cohort were collected before and after the

assessment (Creswell, 2009). The Myers-Briggs instrument (Form M) was collected from

794 first-semester freshmen at a Midwest research university. Students were sorted by age

for inclusion in the study and a population sample was reviewed for type preferences. The

assessment was given as a complement to a first-year seminar course lecture related to

learning style. Students were classified as in-state resident students in undergraduate majors

throughout six colleges. Although each achieved regular admission to the university, they

differed in range of high school rank and ACT scores. An additional comparison was made

9

of Myers-Briggs preferences for students with less than a 2.0 grade point in the first

semester.

A distribution of Myers-Briggs Learning Styles for the full student cohort and less

than 2.0 grade point cohort were developed for measure against the percentage distribution of

a U.S. sample. To compare the means for learning style and academic aptitude through

ACT, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) was utilized for the full cohort and less than 2.0

group. Finally, a multiple regression was implemented for the variables of class rank, ACT,

first-semester grade point, and preference for the research sample and each cohort group.

Regression models are valuable as they help to determine how variables are related to each

other to understand their relationship (De Veaux, Velleman, & Bock, 2012).

Delimitations and Limitations

This study was designed to capture data for an identified group of students at this

university; thus, it should be carefully interpreted before comparison to other student groups

or institutions. The student cohort was deemed of sufficient sample size to address the data,

but may not be representative of the student population based upon demographic and

socioeconomic status. Limitations of this study include the structure of the Myers-Briggs as

a self-reporting instrument. The assumption was made that the respondents were of normal

mental health and objectively reported their preferences when completing the assessment.

Additionally, as respondents were asked to complete the Myers-Briggs assessment as part of

a first-year seminar assignment on learning styles as opposed to self-selecting to complete

the instrument, an assumption was made that the respondents objectively reported their

preferences and were not influenced by the assignment directive.

10

Significance of the Study

Macdaid (2003) designated type practitioners and educators regularly using the

Myers-Briggs as able and obligated to contribute to the data and research of the instrument.

Each collection of descriptive data or hypothesis testing adds to the body of literature

supporting the reliability and validity of the Myers-Briggs. Macdaid suggested type

distribution, correlations, differences, and case studies as solid areas for initiating and

contributing to type research.

Comparisons of Myers-Briggs preferences for students who are not able to achieve a

2.0 grade point average and who may be withdrawing from an institution prior to completing

a degree may offer insight to improving student success. These circumstances define the

potential hypothesis that psychological type preferences are a variable to success for this

student cohort within the university.

There is evidence that statistical analysis of type distributions for a specific

population may help identify whether Myers-Briggs preferences correlate to student

academic difficulty in the first college year. The review of the literature supports that type

preferences, particularly preferences for perceiving, may have an effect on student academic

progress and that identification and understanding of these preferences may assist in

compensating for student learning differences. Type theory reveals that preferences are not

related to ability or motivation. As such, identifying a trend toward specific type preferences

in participants who are not successful in the first college year may provide analysis pertinent

to the participant population in the research.

Preferences of psychological type are equal in their validity and strength to the

individual and no type is of more advantage than another. This research sought to confirm

11

these questions with a goal to develop adaptive programming aimed at increasing student

success.

Definition of Terms

This section defines frequent terminology used in this study and the assessment of

measurement.

Academic aptitude: The term to explain the academic demographic first-year students bring

to the university based upon their ACT standardized test score and high school class rank.

MBTI®: The psychological assessment instrument administered to measure an individual’s

Myers-Briggs preferences. It is a registered product of Consulting Psychologists Press

(CPP), Inc.

Myers-Briggs®: Defines the preferences measured by the MBTI® instrument. Denotes the

authors of the assessment, Katharine Briggs and Isabel Briggs Myers. It is a registered

trademark of the MBTI® Trust, Inc.

Summary

This study builds on prior research to add to the knowledge of whether Myers-Briggs

preferences and academic aptitude correlate to academic success in the first college semester.

Chapter 2 summarizes the literature on the benefits and usage of Myers-Briggs for

understanding individual learning style. Chapter 3 presents the quantitative methodology and

methods used for designing and conducting this study. Chapter 4 provides the descriptive

and inferential results of the study. Finally, Chapter 5 summarizes the research results, and

provides conclusions and recommendations for future research, practice, and policy.

12

CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW

Introduction

As student development theory helps one to understand differences in students served

in higher education, typological models of student development can be used to explain

differences of psychological type and how these differences affect student success.

Typological models do not explain individual changes or beliefs, but are useful for

understanding individual differences and characteristics unique to each student and how

these differences may influence student development and academic success. These models

are also helpful in describing interpersonal interactions and can be employed in designing

educational experiences, team building, training, conflict management and leadership

functions (Evans, Forney, Guido, Patton, & Renn, 2010). Four common typological models

complementing student development include: (a) the Strong-Cambell Interest Inventory; (b)

Holland’s Person-Environment Theory; (c) Kolb’s Theory of Experiential Learning; and (d)

the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator.

Typological Models

Campbell, Borgen, Eastes, Johansson, and Peterson (1968) expanded elements of the

Strong Interest Inventory to enable a clearer explanation and usage of typological interest

inventories in daily application. Their longitudinal study included test-retest measures of two

population samples over multiples years to demonstrate that vocational inventories can also

be descriptive of the individual. Campbell and Holland (1972) investigated a merging of

vocational inventories with personality types and found that rankings on the Strong

Vocational Interest Blank corresponded with the personality scales models set forth by

13

Holland. The Holland personality scale provides a general framework of the individual while

the vocational inventory measures strengths allowing a much more individualized view of the

subject (Cambell & Holland, 1972). Campbell later became a coauthor of the Strong-

Campbell Interest Inventory.

Holland’s (1997) Person-Environment Theory as a typological model seeks to explain

vocational behavior and suggests that our culture enables persons to be categorized by

personality type. The instrument based on Holland’s theory identifies characteristics and

behaviors to define six personality types based upon a 66-item survey. Holland posited that

if an individual then chooses a career or work environment that is similar to his or her

personality type and values, then he or she is more likely to be successful and satisfied in

their endeavor.

Kolb’s (1984) Theory of Experiential Learning arranges individuals according to a

learning style model based upon how they learn and develop. The Kolb assessment is a 12-

item self-scoring instrument that helps a participant to understand preferred learning style

and approaches to problems, conflict and communication. The Kolb model is best known for

its four learning styles: Accommodating, Assimilating, Converging, and Diverging, but is a

developmental theory in that it explains the stages and complexity through which individual

development occurs (Evans et al., 2010).

Myers-Briggs Type Indicator

The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) is the most widely used instrument for

determining psychological type preferences utilized in business and personal coaching and

has common usage on the college campus. The 93-item Form M instrument is the most

14

frequently used assessment of the MBTI battery. The assessment is based upon the theory

that human behavior is not random and that innate patterns of mental functions exist in the

population (Jung, 1971). The basic premise of type is that individuals can have different

motivations and processes for getting through the day, but that they will follow certain

configurations. The MBTI instrument was conceived by the mother-daughter team of

Katherine Briggs and Isabel Briggs Myers with the foundation of Jung’s orientation and

organization to the outer world, or judging and perceiving preferences (Myers, 1980). Briggs

and Myers designed the original assessment to guide individuals in understanding the “value

of differences” (Myers, p. 201). The MBTI asks a series of self-report forced-choice

questions (Appendix B) to define opposing preferences for personal energy, acquiring

information, making decisions, and organizing one’s world. Based upon responses to these

questions, an individual is assigned a type preference for each pair of opposites which when

combined will become one of sixteen distinct four-letter type codes. When assessing

psychological type, proper facilitation requires that an individual is encouraged to determine

their best-fit type, accepting that environment, academics, and self-knowledge may influence

this best fit.

Psychological type assessment can been helpful in allowing the detection of

interpersonal roadblocks and miscommunication related to type preferences, particularly for

students in the transition from high school to college. Hunter (2006) posited that “attention

to student characteristics, needs, behaviors, and experiences is central to creating and

sustaining successful transition initiatives” (p. 9). Through intentional examination of type

distribution and type theory related to learning preferences, opportunities emerge for

enabling students to understand more about themselves in the college transition. Although

15

the MBTI is not designed to be a predictor, examining type distributions and preference

anomalies to enhance student services and resources may lead to increased student success

and progress toward degree.

During the past four decades, Myers-Briggs type theory and the MBTI have became

well known and widely utilized in a variety of education and business settings. As MBTI

popularity has grown, misinterpretation and misuse of the assessment by untrained

individuals and in easily accessible unmeasured online formats has also increased.

Regardless of these challenges, the accurate and ethical application of type theory has

considerable value for practitioners and research in many topic areas (Evans, Forney, Guido,

Patton, & Renn, 2010; Hammer, 1996; VanSant, 2003). Substantial critiques of Myers-

Briggs theory including assessments of reliability, validity, and factor analysis support its

value (Capraro & Capraro, 2002; Pittenger, 2005; Reynierse & Harker, 2005; Stricker,

Schiffman, & Ross, 1965).

Elements of Psychological Type

There are four MBTI type dichotomies, or opposite preference pairs, and each has a

different influence on learning. The word preference is used to refer to the innate tendency

one has in each of the psychological dichotomies (Myers, 1980). The principle of preference

is frequently illustrated in type facilitations by asking participants to write their signature

with their non-dominant hand. Generally, participants will describe this exercise as

awkward, uncomfortable, or not a preferred activity, but one they are able to complete. Just

as an individual has a preferred hand for writing, each individual also has a preference for

daily functions, but is able to operate out of preference, as needed. The preference pairs

16

include where a person gets their energy, categorized as Extraversion and Introversion; how

an individual takes in information or Sensing and Intuiting; the decision-making process of

Thinking and Feeling; and the orientation to and organization in the outer world of Judging

and Perceiving. Individuals use each aspect of the personality pairs daily, but have a

preference for one that is more comfortable or useful to the self. As addressed previously, an

individual is assigned a type preference for each pair of opposites which, when combined,

create 16 individual four-letter type codes.

Extraversion and Introversion

Extraversion and Introversion are expressions of where an individual gathers personal

energy. Extraversion (E) is the energy that develops from engaging with people, objects and

events. Externally expressing interests and interacting with others is invigorating for

extraverts. Extraverts learn best in situations that include movement, action and

conversation, and prefer to connect theories and facts with personal experience. Introversion

(I) is a reflective, inward coordination with thoughts and ideas. Introverts look internally for

thoughts and energy. They think best in quiet or solitude and prefer advance notice before

sharing or acting in a learning situation.

Dunning (2008) encouraged Extraverts to practice active listening and reading

strategies to remain focused in lectures or while studying. Study and discussion groups are

particularly helpful for students with this preference, although Extraverts may wish to limit

questions or comments so that everyone in a group has an opportunity to participate.

Introverted preference students should find time to think and study in quiet, uninterrupted

locations so that they may process information. When possible, Introverts should seek

17

agendas and other information in advance in order to think things through before responding.

As Introverts can be viewed as quiet and unengaged, Dunning encouraged these students to

use “nonverbal cues to demonstrate participation” (p. 17) while engaging in group processes.

Sensing and Intuition

Sensing and Intuition are the functions for attaining information. The Sensing (S)

perception is the process of awareness and accumulating information through the physical

senses. Sensing is a pragmatic function relying on details, sequenced lists, and consistency.

Sensors learn best with sequential learning from concrete to abstract and tend to excel at

memorization. The Intuitive (N) perception is future oriented and uses hunches and sees

possibilities to provide explanations. Intuitive preference persons value patterns and abstract

ideas and learn through imaginative tasks and theoretical topics with ease.

Sensing preference students learn best by summarizing subject matter; finding

practical application for big picture ideas or themes; and “creating specific, short-term

learning goals” (Dunning, 2008, p. 18). Students who prefer Intuition can increase detail

oriented fact retention with flash cards or summarized outlines and should focus on

supporting facts with ideas and connections when writing or making presentations. Intuitive

types must “avoid being distracted by tangentially related information” (p. 19) and can

enhance their learning by focusing on the specifics required of academic assignments.

Thinking and Feeling

Thinking and Feeling are the decision-making or judgment processes of type.

Thinking (T) is the objective decision-making process that uses specific standards to analyze

information or situations and then improve the situations or performance. Thinking

18

preference individuals are motivated in learning by logic and respect for their competence.

The Feeling (F) decision-making preference is more subjective and based upon personal

values for accommodating harmony and the improvement of personal conditions for others.

Individuals with Feeling preference are motivated in learning by personal encouragement,

values and the human dimension of a topic or lesson.

The logical and analytical Thinking preference student seeks credible reasoning.

Dunning (2008) encouraged this student to take the time to create questions and comments

without debating over topics and issues. As the Thinking preference generally relies on

critique rather than appreciation, listening and understanding are helpful for the learning

processing of this student. Dunning noted that students with a preference for Feeling achieve

the most from learning when it validates their individual perspective. Feeling preference

students are most successful when they are encouraged and supporting in their learning.

Judging and Perceiving

Judging (J) is the process of engaging with the outer world preferring organization,

structure, and a planned life. Those preferring Judgment tend to experience time in specific

segments. They are driven to seek closure or finish tasks in those specific time periods.

Judging preference learning thrives on task completion, structured learning and specific

goals. The Perceiving (P) preference values autonomy, flexibility and spontaneity. They

experience time as an uninterrupted flow and are open to new information as they experience

and process. They prefer open learning environments that rely less on deadlines and

structure.

19

The student with a Judging preference is frequently eager to get things completed.

Dunning (2008) suggested that students with this preference should avoid open and over-

scheduling, making decisions too quickly, and should “plan for inevitable interruptions to

minimize academic stress” (p. 22). The openness of the learning schedule for the Perceiving

preference student be a challenge if essential time is not available for a student to do the

work required. Dunning advised the Perceiving student to recognize his or her flow

approach to learning, but to allow for structure and organization as required.

Feeling and Perceiving

The student population prompting this research and literature review had over-

represented type preferences for Feeling and Perceiving in comparison to national samples.

There are four MBTI types that encompass the Feeling and Perceiving preferences, ISFP,

ESFP, INFP and ENFP. Nardi (2001) described persons with Introversion, Sensing, Feeling

and Perceiving (ISFP) and Extraverted, Sensing, Feeling and Perceiving (ESFP) as

experiential and improvisational. Students with these preferences like to see the big picture

early in a learning concept so that they may decide relevancy. They appreciate respect in the

classroom and appreciate being able to ask questions to clarify learning. The ISFP and ESFP

appreciate immediate feedback and most enjoy learning in the context of how it will help

them accomplish a task or help others. They learn best when the content is challenging but

fun, allowing “the freedom and independence to explore” (p. 25).

The Introverted, Intuitive, Feeling, Perceiving (INFP) and Extraverted, Intuitive,

Feeling, Perceiving (ENFP) persons were recognized by Nardi (2001) as being perceptive

and inspirational. They respond easily to others’ behaviors or emotions and are able to

20

mediate communication and meanings in relationships. The INFP and ENFP learn most

efficiently when there is connection to their values and personal experiences. They seek

meaning and goals in learning and achievement and learn best through self-discovery and

personal development. The INFP and ENFP students appreciate authentic and encouraging

feedback and want to be “recognized for the unique perspective they bring to an assignment

or task” (p. 37).

Problem in Review

MBTI profile assessments taken during a first-year seminar course guide this interest

in whether knowledge of type can be asset to student success. The MBTI instrument (Form

M) was administered to approximately 800 entering first-year students at Iowa State

University. The assessment was a complement to a first-year seminar course lecture related

to learning style. The resulting profiles, collected by this researcher over an eight-year

period, were informally analyzed and sorted for a review of academic success and persistence

in the cohort. Through this process, it was discovered that the student population had

specific over-represented type preferences for Feeling and Perceiving in comparison to

national samples. An additional area of comparison was made of MBTI preferences for

students who had not achieved a 2.0 grade point average in the first semester. From the

collected profiles, 110 students with less than a 2.0 in the first semester were grouped

according to personality type. Consequently, there were a higher number of students from

this less than 2.0 group with the over-represented type preferences for Feeling and

Perceiving. A disproportionate number of students with the Feeling and Perceiving

preferences were also found to be withdrawing from the institution prior to completing a

degree. These circumstances suggested the possibility that psychological type preferences

21

could be a variable relating to student success for this student cohort within their university.

Additionally, it was suspected that if preference anomalies could be found to be a variable

affecting academic success, then perhaps steps could be taken to positively enhance the

education experience of students with these type preferences. Maxwell (2005) defined this

generation of theory from data as grounded theory methodology. Grounded theory, first

defined by Glaser and Strauss (1967), is a reverse of traditional research in that data

collection inductively drives the hypotheses questions.

Utilizing the collected type preferences for the cohort group, the following research

questions framed this study:

1. What are the academic demographics of the students in the study, including academic

aptitude (ACT and high school class rank) and first-semester grade point average?

2. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students

in the study by each cohort year and for the research population in comparison to the

distribution of a national sample?

3. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences by gender

for students in the study by each cohort year and for the research sample in

comparison to the distribution of a national population?

4. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students

with STEM majors by each cohort year and the research population in the study in

comparison to the distribution of a national population?

5. Are there statistically significant differences in academic aptitude of ACT and high

school class rank and Myers-Briggs preference for students by each cohort year and

for the research population in the study?

22

6. Are there statistically significant differences in academic aptitude and Myers-Briggs

preference for students in the study by grade point and ability to achieve a 2.00 grade

point in the first college semester by each cohort year and for the research

population?

7. Is there correlation of ACT, high school class rank, or Myers-Briggs preference to

first semester grade point by each cohort year and the research population?

Although ability and motivation are primary factors influencing student academic

success, an understanding of the impact of type preferences can assist services for all

students. If a student’s type preferences and learning styles as defined by type are

incongruent to institutional culture, lack of engagement may occur. As academic

disengagement can lead to distraction and lack of motivation, major factors behind a decline

in grades (Keup, 2006), identifying trends for academic success related to psychological type

could be useful for student development.

This literature review sought information related to the aforementioned theoretical

framework and whether academic success and academic progress variables may relate to

student MBTI preferences. As student retention and performance are important issues for all

colleges and universities who wish to recruit and graduate students, this review may aid in

determining if students with specific type preferences have more academic difficulty in their

path to a college degree and, if so, identify steps to ease this challenge.

Myers-Briggs Research Perspective

As mentioned previously, Macdaid (2003) designated type practitioners and educators

utilizing the Myers-Briggs assessment as responsible to contribute to the data and research of

23

the instrument. Each collection of descriptive data or hypothesis testing adds to the body of

literature supporting the reliability and validity of the MBTI. Macdaid suggested type

distribution, correlations, differences, and case studies as solid areas for initiating and

contributing to type research.

Myers, McCaulley, Quenk, and Hammer (1998) indicated that type distribution tables

are the basic method for reporting MBTI information on groups and that the descriptive

information of the type table provides evidence of construct validity. Type distributions are

most commonly analyzed through the self-selection ratio type table, or SRTT, (McCaulley,

1985). Self-selection ratio type tables are used to determine the frequency of a type in

comparison to a base population and provide a measurement of the overrepresentation or

underrepresentation of a type preference for a group.

Learning style

In a conceptual framework of this research, it is import to reflect the dichotomies of

psychological type in consideration to learning styles and influences of academic success.

Learning style is important to the composition of student success in college (Chesborough,

2005; 2009; DiTiberio & Hammer, 1993). Learning style can be described as the different

ways a student will approach critical thinking and the assimilation of new information or how

they process information. Although learning style is fairly consistent for most individuals, it

can be enhanced with usage, and developed by individual strengths that contribute to student

understanding (Evans et al., 2010). Differences in attentional processing such as absorbing

the big picture as opposed to small bites of information, encoding strategies, or the

24

organizing of information and self-regulation of learning, can be contributing factors in

diverse learning styles (Svinicki, 2004).

Lawrence (2009) defined four styles and dispositions of learning related to

psychological type preferences. Cognitive style is the preferred function or pattern for

information processing. Attitude and interest patterns affecting the participation in learning

are the second aspect of learning style. A disposition to seek learning environments that

match interests is the third aspect, while a disposition to successfully use learning tools

completes the quadrant. Lawrence advised that although a student’s learning strategies and

behaviors may be required to change from situation to situation, their type preferences do not

change and “aspects of learning style that are a reflection of one’s type can be expected to

persist across situations” (p. 39). Lawrence defined these learning preferences as aligned

with MBTI type preferences or the mental processes of type theory. He suggested that the

sixteen individual types are also sixteen methods of mental processing, each with individual

strengths, and all with value for the individual learner.

Type and learning style

Identifying a correlation of MBTI preferences as a gauge of academic success in

college requires a review of literature within the framework of MBTI usage and examination

among college students. Academic success in the first college semester is widely believed to

affect the eventual success or graduation of the new college student. Tinto (1975) suggested

that a necessary factor in this success for the college student is finding out whether an

institution fits their needs allowing them to become integrated into the campus culture.

College students will frequently adapt to the education culture of an institution to find

25

success (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2005). It is this integration function that can determine

whether a student continues toward a degree and is retained by a college or university.

The MBTI indicator, although not an identifier of visual, auditory, and kinesthetic

learning styles is beneficial in that it assesses learning preferences and processes rather than

the learning behaviors of most learning style inventories (Jensen, 2003). Jensen described

the MBTI as the most comprehensive assessment of learning style assessments attributing to

instrument norming, length of development time, and sixteen specific approaches to student

learning. He asserted that as the MBTI is a personality type assessment, and type is generally

static, it is more useful than learning style assessments for measuring student behavior or

performance which may fluctuate, dependent upon the learning experience. As institution

type and instructor type preferences can frequently differ from student type preferences, an

understanding of type theory can assist educators and learners in goals of student success.

Jenson encouraged instructors not to abandon their preferred teaching styles but instead to be

more flexible in how they teach, augmenting lecture with class discussion, adding theoretical

discussion to facts and figures, and “allowing for unstructured learning when applicable,” (p.

133).

In an examination of 107,000 college students enrolled in fifty-nine college majors,

Schaubhut and Thompson (2011) created MBTI type table summary information from a

comparison to a database collection of all fulltime college students completing the MBTI.

They noted that an understanding of psychological type preference may be helpful for

students as they plan their academic and institutional choices. Although students should not

choose a major or a university on the basis of type alone, assessing an academic environment

in relation to MBTI preferences is a useful tool for post-secondary education planning.

26

Schaubhut and Thompson suggested that instructors and educators may also find usefulness

in examining the type preferences of students in specific majors to become more

synchronized with the learning styles of students choosing those programs.

Alaoutinen, Heikkinen, and Porras (2012) studied computer science students at

Finnish and Egyptian universities enrolled in a five-day intensive computer-coding course.

Students were identified by learning style and assessed for satisfaction with learning

following the course and found that introverted, reflective (I) and intuitive, big picture (N)

learners did particularly well in the intensive education format. Alaoutinen et al. suggested

that, although intensive courses with group work are common, they are not widely used and

their focus on collaboration may be of advantage to some learners.

Differences were found among personality style for Swiss university students in the

visual arts, music, and psychology as examined by Haller and Courvoisier (2010). Mean

scores found psychology and music students as more extraverted while visual arts students

were identified as more introverted. Haller and Courvoisier suggested that creative students

have a different personality and thinking style and that some areas of study seem to require

different personality profiles. This is valuable information for students in that it

demonstrates a tendency to choose a field of study that fits individual strengths and

personality. The challenge then becomes when students with less self-knowledge self-select

into fields of study that do not match their strengths.

Allen (2007) addressed the debate of whether to measure learning or the learner by

suggesting that psychological type can be used to enhance learning strategies and incorporate

learning style into the demands of learning. Following two decades of research within a

community college study skills center, Allen hypothesized that students learn the same way

27

they think. As thinking is a concept of psychological type, learning, like thinking, is

therefore innate. By comparing the preference pairs to learning taxonomy, Allen

demonstrated elements of type preferences and different learning styles by linking them to

concrete and abstract thinking. By focusing on how to assist students in the full use of their

individual type preferences, Allen illustrated that educators can meet individual learning

needs and encourage learner growth.

In a study of MBTI influences connected to social work student learning style,

Chesborough (2009) found an overrepresentation of Extroverted Sensing students that was

contrary to an earlier collection of type data for social workers that demonstrated more

Introverted Intuiting preferences. Chesborough asserted that as many prior social workers

become social work educators, conflicts in instructional and learning styles may occur in the

classroom and that an understanding of type could assist in the educational success of social

work students. As instructors cannot consistently teach to individual type preferences,

Chesborough recommended matching students for study and discussion groups based on type

preferences to enhance learning.

Chesborough (2005) also conducted a university study of male football and basketball

scholarship athletes and found that Sensing, Thinking and Perceiving functions were strongly

preferred. Additionally, it was found that the ISTP, ESTP and ESTJ types were significantly

over-represented in the athlete sample. She emphasizes the implication of this study in that

student athletes, frequently recognized as at-risk, may actually be different learners requiring

a different learning environment. This assertion follows the generally recognized assumption

that most classroom instruction is primarily reliant on deductive delivery. As a potential

solution to these different learning preferences, Chesborough presented a redesigned learning

28

plan that enhances the ability of Sensing, Thinking and Perceiving individuals in achieving

academic success, but is sensible and applicable for many college classrooms. First,

educators can be encouraged to provide learning conditions that use more applied approaches

to learning and conditions that make use of inductive rather than deductive strategies. They

should utilize audiovisual materials and other media and rely less on straight lecture format

for presentation of information. Instructors should make stronger use of computer-assisted

learning and interactive materials to “provide immediate, specific feedback on learning

problems and individual learning” (p. 38). Additionally, the use of experiential learning such

as field trips and laboratory experiments along with alternatives to paper writing is desirable

and Chesborough encouraged institutions to coordinate the use of type-alike student learning

assistants or tutors within athletic academic programs.

The evolution of student learning and expectations has demonstrated opportunities for

learning style development and teaching growth. Erickson and Strommer (2005) posited that

research university models of creating and delivering knowledge do not successfully address

the learning styles of current students. They encouraged educators to incorporate small

group discussion, personal writing exercises, case studies, problem-based learning and

experiential learning to meet student learners where they are in the classroom. “Sufficient

variety of instructional strategies helps to maintain interest as well as appeal to the various

learning strengths of a diverse class” (p. 255).

Type, academic success, and persistence

As students move toward campus integration they seek congruence and comfort in a

campus culture. Type theory and the MBTI can be helpful in moving students toward this

29

goal. Kalsbeek (1986, 2003) reported on a tracking tool that aids university communities in

reviewing MBTI data in comparison with available student data to provide a research base

for retention strategies. Students at a medium-size private university were administered the

MBTI at the time of their housing application or during first-year orientation. They

consented to having their scores merged with ACT/SAT scores and other entry and

demographic data sources. Academic results, program of study and enrollment status of the

students were tracked in subsequent semesters. While it is not surprising that results from the

tracking research found ACT/SAT scores as the best predictor of academic performance in

the first semester, the research also revealed that preferences for Introversion, Perception and

Intuition were found to be statistically significant in their influence on first semester grades.

Type data were also found to correlate with entering student profiles as to reasons for

attending college, performance on college admission standardized tests and first-term

academic achievement. Each of these correlations is helpful to campus retention efforts by

explaining possible shifts in college entry data and academic success. As failure to find

academic success is a major factor in student persistence, Kalsbeek (2003) emphasized that

the MBTI instrument is useful for academic success programs. It can be used to identify

special challenges for students, as a method for responding to students in need of academic

support, and for “facilitating a good educational fit between the learner and the instructor” (p.

109).

In a second study examining the type preferences of students in social work majors,

Campbell and White (2009) discovered students of color studying social work were

significantly more likely to have Sensing, Sensing-Thinking, and Introverted-Sensing than

traditional-aged college students in a sample of senior undergraduates. Students in the study

30

were also less likely to prefer the Intuitive preference than the sample population. As

suggested by Chesborough (2009), many social work educators have more Introverted

Intuiting preferences and Campbell and White found this type conflict to be potentially

detrimental to the success of minority students in social work studies. They theorized that

Sensing preference students generally are successful in the social work field, but that the

propensity for Intuiting preference students to score higher on timed aptitude tests may

potentially eliminate many Sensing preference students from admission to social work

programs. As Sensing preference students tend to prefer linear and hands-on learning,

Campbell and White recommended social work programs offer more “engagement in

concrete tasks and early opportunities for students to engage in practical experiences” (p. 61).

In a nine-year study of MBTI type, choice of major and academic performance of

nearly 6,300 students at a private university, DiRienzo, Das, Synn, Kitts, and McGrath

(2010) researched whether certain MBTI types were specific to majors, if type correlated to

academic success and if certain MBTI type preferences outperform other types. Their results

indicated a significant relationship between Type, major, and academic success including a

finding that Judging types had higher average grade points overall and that all Introverted,

Feeling, and Judging types outperformed other students in the study with the exception of

Business students. Students with a preference for Perceiving were generally found to have

lower average grade points. Additionally, students with Intuitive, Feeling and Perceiving

preferences demonstrated a likelihood to choose study in the fine arts while those with

preferences for Extraversion, Sensing and Thinking more frequently chose business majors.

The size and scope of this study helps to provide practical information regarding MBTI type

for advising and support of academic success.

31

Following a ten-year study of entering freshman at a liberal arts college to determine

if there were MBTI type patterns among withdrawing students, Barrineau (2005) found

students with preferences for Perceiving, Intuition and Perceiving, and specifically, the ENFP

type preference (Extraverted, Intuiting, Feeling and Perceiving), were moderately, but

significantly, more likely to withdraw from college. DiTiberio and Hammer (1993) defined

the ENFP college student as disliking routine and as one who may avoid necessary details.

This student may fluctuate among many college majors before choosing and impulsively

become involved in many projects, sometimes not finishing them. Barrineau found that in

addition to identifying student type preferences related to attrition, students with the Judging

preference were significantly underrepresented among students leaving the university. As

the study reported that 69% of students who failed to persist to graduation had a preference

for Perceiving, it provides solid analysis for addressing academic support, mentoring

possibilities and transition issues with students of similar preferences. Barrineau suggested

that institutions should create type-alike mentoring programs between first-year students and

upper classification students, enforce stricter deadlines on majors and courses to push

students toward graduation, and develop “type and learning style training for faculty and

advisors to address type attrition concerns” (pp. 31-32).

Students labeled as low achieving based upon standardized testing were found as able

to compensate and achieve above-expected collegiate performance levels by using learning

preference tools to complement their psychological type preferences (Hill, 2006). Students

admitted to a six-week remedial academic program at a midsized four-year university and

who eventually achieved a 3.0 or better were included in the study. The Sensing preference

in successful students in the sample was found to outnumber the Intuitive type by three to

32

one. Hill argues this finding is noteworthy as colleges frequently measure academic ability

through standardized assessments of ability, favoring students with Intuitive preferences. He

encourages institutions to identify non-cognitive factors for predicting academic performance

and to consider the influence of these results in course design and instruction. The challenge

then, according to Erickson and Strommer (2005), is that faculty cannot be expected to meet

the different learning preferences of all students all of the time, but they can offer a variety of

educational activities and assignments to engage students with a variety of learning styles.

Students of all preferences can find academic success, a factor that should be a recognizable

foundation of any student retention initiative.

Beckham (2012) conducted a qualitative study of academically successful Perceiving

preference college students compared with a content analysis of college success and study

skills texts. She found that although Perceiving students valued learning and were

academically successful using their own learning styles and approaches, some were less

confident and less positive about their skills as a result of institutionalized methods and

expectations of student success. Beckham described Perceiving students as viewing time in a

continuous process where they work in a connected flow on a project. Frequently, this flow

will begin at what may be characterized as at the last minute or directly before a deadline or

project due date. This is in contrast to those with a Judging preference who view time in

increments and divide large tasks into smaller parts. Beckham stated that this finite Judging

time management process is supported in much of the academic skills and success literature

but is difficult to achieve for those with the Perceiving preference. She argued that we

cannot invalidate the learning of students who spend less time on study or writing simply

because they complete these tasks differently. If students with Perceiving preferences

33

ultimately persist to graduation and are satisfied with their education, institutions should not

be able to lessen the value of the experience. Beckham recommended that college success

courses, texts and educators be willing to utilize unconventional instruction strategies to meet

the needs of all students.

As outlined by Tinto (1975, 2012), a key factor for persistence is the integration to

campus academic and social cultures experienced by a student in the transition to college.

An understanding of psychological type as made available by the MBTI instrument can be a

mechanism for assisting students to understand learning and student success in the post-

secondary institution. As university cultures are wide and varying, establishing student fit

within that culture through use of the MBTI is one method for enhancing student satisfaction

and progress to graduation.

Summary

What began as an assignment in a first-year seminar course lecture related to learning

style has revealed that a student cohort with MBTI preferences for Feeling and Perceiving

could provide analysis for contribution to the field of typological student development

research. Delving further into a comparison of MBTI preferences for students who are not

able to achieve a 2.0 grade point average and who may be withdrawing from an institution

prior to completing a degree may offer insight to improving student success. These

circumstances defined the hypothesis that psychological type preferences are a variable to

success for this student cohort within the university.

There is evidence that statistical analysis of type distributions for a specific

population may help identify whether MBTI preferences correlate to student academic

34

difficulty in the first college year. This review of the literature supports that type

preferences, particularly preferences for Perceiving, may have an effect on student academic

progress and that identification and understanding of these preferences may assist in

compensating for student learning differences. Type theory reveals that preferences are not

related to ability or motivation. As such, identifying a trend toward specific type preferences

in participants who are not successful in the first college year may provide analysis distinct to

the participant population in the research.

Preferences of psychological type are valid in their strength to enhance individual

perception of style and learning. This research ascertained the value of the Myers-Briggs

assessment in helping students to better understand their fit within their institution with a

goal of enhancing persistence.

35

CHAPTER 3. METHODOLOGY

Overview

The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship of Myers-Briggs Type

preferences to first-semester grade point average for new direct from high school Iowa

students enrolling at Iowa State University as recipients of the Hixson Award from fall 2007

through fall 2011. Myers-Briggs preferences represent a typological model of student

development theory. Although typology does not explain individual changes or beliefs, the

models are useful for understanding individual differences and characteristics and how these

differences may influence student development and academic success.

Myers-Briggs profile assessments collected from eight first-year student cohorts

enrolled in a first-year seminar course were informally analyzed and sorted for a review of

academic success and persistence in a student cohort. Through this process, it was

discovered that the student population had over-represented type preferences for two Myers-

Briggs preferences in comparison to national samples. An additional comparison found

evidence of over-represented type preferences for students who had not achieved a 2.0 grade

point average in the first semester and among those students withdrawing from the institution

prior to completing a degree. These circumstances suggest that psychological type

preferences could be a variable relating to student success for this student cohort within their

university, and invite additional statistical analysis.

Kalsbeek (1986, 2003) reported on a tracking tool that aids universities in reviewing

Myers-Briggs data in comparison with available student data to provide a research base for

retention strategies. The study revealed certain Myers-Briggs preferences to be significant in

36

their influence on first-semester grades. Type data were also found to correlate with entering

student profiles regarding reasons for attending college, performance on college admission

standardized tests, and first-term academic achievement.

The goal of this study was to reproduce portions of Kalsbeek’s research with data

collected on Myers-Briggs preferences for a first-year student cohort sample. As student

retention and performance are important issues for all colleges and universities that wish to

recruit and retain students through graduation with the bachelor’s degree, this study might aid

in determining if students with specific type preferences have more academic difficulty in

their path to a college degree, and, if so, identify steps to ease this challenge.

Measures of the student cohort were studied with data measuring student personality

types before and after the Myers-Briggs assessment in what Creswell (2009) referred to as a

quasi-experimental time series design. The Myers-Briggs instrument (Form M) collected

from approximately 800 first-year students were reviewed for type preferences. The

assessment was given as a complement to a first-year seminar course lecture related to

learning style. The student participants were classified as in-state resident students in a

variety of majors throughout six undergraduate colleges. They each achieved regular

admission to the university, but varied in range of high school rank and ACT scores. Data

from that form were correlated with pre-enrollment measures of academic aptitude, ACT,

and high school class rank, and then with first-semester grade point. An additional analysis

was made of Myers-Briggs preferences for students with less than a 2.0 grade point average

in the first semester.

A quantitative inquiry using secondary data was utilized to investigate the questions

for this study. The survey questions and analysis of the results were informed by the review

37

of current literature in the field. Descriptive and inferential statistics were employed to

examine Myers-Briggs profile assessments in comparison with student and institutional data

for academic aptitude and first-semester grade point. Analyses of consistencies or anomalies

in cohort type preferences were also conducted.

The JMP Pro 10 statistical software package from SAS was used to investigate the

quantitative variables and relationships between independent and dependent variables.

Logistic regression models were used to determine the variables that affect first-semester

academic success.

The student population in this study was comprised of new, direct from high school,

first-time fulltime Iowa resident students who enrolled at Iowa State University beginning

fall 2004 through fall 2011. The secondary database was comprised of eight years of student

data, with approximately 100 students in each year.

Descriptive statistics were calculated for specific independent variables. Analysis of

variance (ANOVA) was utilized for the full sample and separately for students with less than

a 2.00 cumulative grade point average to compare the means for type preference and

measures of academic aptitude. Multiple Regression was implemented for the variables of

class rank, ACT, and preference pairs for the full cohort and less than 2.0 group. As stated

in Chapter 1 egression models are valuable as they help us to see variables and their

relationships to enhance understanding (De Veaux, Velleman, & Bock, 2012).

The primary method for analyzing type preferences is the self-selection ratio type

table (SRTT; McCaulley, 1985). The SRTT is used to measure the frequency of type in a

collected sample against the frequency of that type in a base population.

38

The analyses assessed categorical and continuous variables. The independent

variables included continuous variables, such as ACT, high school rank, or first-semester

grade point, and discrete variables, such as Myers-Briggs Type preference. A complete list

of variables are located in a codebook table (see Table 3.2).

Self-selection ratio type tables were used to aid in reporting the results. Contingency

tables were created to assess the association between categorical variables (e.g., gender).

Histograms were created to show frequencies of variables collected for each year.

Additional research is needed to explore student learning preferences and predictors

affecting first-semester student success and retention. This study might help to further

illuminate these learning preferences and potential barriers to degree attainment.

Additionally, this research may provide educators and advisors of similar student programs

and of learning communities with typological development information to support students in

the transition to college. [you are the humble researcher]

This chapter describes the methodological approach employed for this study. The

research questions, hypotheses, research design, setting, population, comparison sample, data

collection, variables, data management, and method of data analyses are outlined. The

ethical issues, limitations, and delimitations of the study also are reported.

Research Questions and Hypotheses

If student type preferences and learning styles are found to be incongruent with

institutional culture, lack of engagement may occur. As academic disengagement is a major

variable behind a decline in grades and increased attrition (Keup, 2006), identifying trends

for academic success related to psychological type can be useful for student development.

39

The purpose of this study was to investigate the collected type preferences for a

student cohort to analyze type preference distributions, learning style, and potential effects of

type on academic success in the first college semester. The following research questions

guided this study. If a research question is not descriptive and seeks inference, a

corresponding hypothesis is included. Each hypothesis is written in the traditional null

hypothesis form postulating no difference between variables (Creswell, 2009).

1. What are the academic demographics of the students in the study, including academic

aptitude (ACT and high school class rank) and first-semester grade point average?

2. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students

in the study by each cohort year in comparison to the distribution of a national

population?

Hypothesis 1: There is no difference between the Myers-Briggs preferences

for students in the study and the national population.

3. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences by gender

for students in the study by each cohort year and across groups in comparison to the

distribution of a national population?

Hypothesis 2: There is no difference between the Myers-Briggs preferences

by gender for students in the study by cohort year or across groups and the

national population.

4. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students

with STEM majors by each cohort year in the study in comparison to the distribution

of a national population?

40

Hypothesis 3: There is no difference between the Myers-Briggs preferences

for students with STEM majors in the study and the national population.

5. Are there statistically significant differences in academic aptitude of ACT and high

school class rank and Myers-Briggs preference for students by each cohort year and

across groups in the study?

Hypothesis 4: There is no difference in ACT and high school class rank and

Myers-Briggs preference for students by each cohort year and across groups

in the study.

6. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preference for student

grade point in the first college semester in comparison by each cohort year and across

groups? Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preference for

students who are able to achieve a 2.00 grade point in in the first college semester by

each cohort year and across groups?

Hypothesis 5: There is no difference in Myers-Briggs preference for students

in the study by grade point in the first college semester in comparison by each

cohort year and for the research population.

Hypothesis 6: There is no difference in Myers-Briggs preference for students

who are able to achieve a 2.00 grade point in the first college semester in

comparison by cohort year and for the research population.

7. Is there correlation of ACT, high school class rank, or Myers-Briggs preference to

first semester grade point by each cohort year and across groups.?

41

Hypothesis 7: There is no correlation of ACT, high school class rank, or

Myers-Briggs preference, to first semester grade point by each cohort year and

across groups.

These research questions and hypotheses framed this quantitative study to provide

information on the effect of Myers-Briggs learning preferences and academic success in the

first semester. The findings of this study should add to the body of knowledge regarding

typological connections in student development and academic achievement for students

enrolling at Iowa State University.

Research Design

The framework for this study provides the plans and procedures for completing this

research. According to Creswell (2009), there are three components involved with the

design: (1) researcher paradigm; (2) strategy of inquiry; and (3) research method. The

problem addressed in this study reflects the need to identify and assess the various causes

(variables) that influence outcomes (type preferences related academic success in the first

college semester).

Creswell (2009) defined this approach as postpositivist in that it begins with a

research theory, followed by data collection, and then a statistical analysis of the data.

Postpositivists seek to identify and assess causes that are influencing outcomes. This

researcher began by noticing trends in Myers-Briggs assessments, developed theories, and

then requested to collect data to test the theories.

The strategy of inquiry for this study was structured as a quasi-experimental time

series design in that measures of the student cohort were studied from before and after the

42

Myers-Briggs assessment (Creswell, 2009). The Myers-Briggs instrument (Form M) was

collected from first-year students from the fall 2004 through fall 2011 semesters and

reviewed for type preferences. Descriptive and inferential statistics were employed to

examine a secondary database of Myers-Briggs profile assessments in comparison with

student and institutional data for academic aptitude and first-semester grade point. Analyses

of consistencies or anomalies in cohort type preferences were conducted.

The JMP Pro 10.0 statistical software package was used to investigate the quantitative

variables and relationships between independent and dependent variables. Logistic

regression models were used to determine which variables affect first-semester academic

success.

Descriptive statistics were calculated for specific independent variables. To compare

the means for type preference and academic aptitude through ACT, an analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was utilized to compare means across groups for the full cohort and across groups

for students with less than 2.00 grade point. Finally, multiple regression was implemented

for the variables of class rank, ACT, and preference pairs for the full cohort and less than 2.0

group.

The student population in this study was comprised of new, direct from high school,

first-time fulltime Iowa resident students who enrolled at Iowa State University beginning

fall 2004 through fall 2011. The database was comprised of eight years of student data, with

approximately 100 students each year. The analyses assessed categorical and continuous

variables. The independent variables included continuous variables, such as ACT, high

school rank, or first-semester grade point and discrete variables, such as Myers-Briggs Type

preference.

43

Astin’s (1984) Input-Environment-Outcome model provided a conceptual framework

to research variables that impact student success in the first semester of college. This

framework recognizes students’ precollege characteristics as inputs, students’ academic

learning experiences during their first semester of college as environmental, and cumulative

GPA earned after the first semester as an outcome.

This research design sought to determine causes that define outcomes. It examined a

secondary database of student data and Myers-Briggs preferences of a first-year student

learning community, and descriptive and inferential statistical analyses were utilized to

identify variables influencing student success in the first college semester.

Setting

The setting for this study was Iowa State University, an AAU and Carnegie RU/VH

comprehensive public research university located in a small Midwest city. Iowa State was

established in 1858 and currently enrolls approximately 31,000 students, 82% of whom are

undergraduate students in six colleges. The University is the nation’s first land-grant

institution, is accredited by the North Central Association of the Higher Learning

Commission, and is one of three major public universities in Iowa. Sixty-eight percent of

undergraduates are Iowa residents.

Population and Sample

The student population in this study was comprised of new, direct from high school,

first-time, fulltime Iowa resident students who enrolled at Iowa State beginning fall 2004

through fall 2011. They were selected for enrollment in the Hixson Awards Program and

participated in a required first-year seminar learning community course. They were

44

primarily first-generation students, defined for federal educational program purposes as

having parents without an earned bachelor’s degree. The students also had primarily low

Expected Family Contribution (EFC) as determined by the Free Application for Federal

Student Aid (FAFSA) and were typically full Pell Grant eligible.

The Hixson Opportunity Awards (Hixson Awards) were created in 1995 for Iowa

high school students whose challenging environments typically preclude higher education,

and are the largest privately funded scholarship at Iowa State. One hundred high school

seniors are selected from across the state (ideally, one from each county) to receive this

yearly award (one-half tuition and fees for four years) representing each county of the state,

rural and urban communities. The Hixson Program serves 400 students annually (first-year

through senior classification) and supports the university mission by strengthening

undergraduate programs, sustaining a supportive university community, and enhancing

student life through programming for intellectual growth and persistence. The Hixson

Program has among the highest retention and graduation rates for learning communities at

the institution.

The secondary database compilation was comprised of eight years of first-year

student data, with approximately 100 students each year. Students under the age of 18 years

at the time of enrollment were excluded from the study. Table 3.1 provides the population

for each cohort year.

Reliability and Validity

During the past four decades, Myers-Briggs type theory and the MBTI® assessment have

became well known and widely utilized in a variety of education and business settings.

45

Table 3.1. Population sample for each cohort year

Cohort Year 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 Total

Population 97 96 95 96 99 100 97 95 775

As MBTI popularity has grown, misinterpretation and misuse of the assessment by

untrained individuals and in easily accessible unmeasured online formats has also increased.

Regardless of these challenges, substantial critiques of the Myers-Briggs framework,

including assessments of reliability, validity, and factor analysis, support its value (Capraro

& Capraro, 2002; Pittenger, 2005; Reynierse & Harker, 2005; Stricker, Schiffman, & Ross,

1965).

The application process for admission to Iowa State University established much of

the reliability and validity of the academic data. All high school students are required to

apply for admission and submit high school transcripts or supporting documents prior to

being offered admission. These data will become a framework for the study. The data came

directly from student registration information, admission records, and end of semester

records. Data were also collected from self-reported information, such as gender, and race or

ethnicity.

Creation of the Research Dataset

The dataset for this research was comprised of students enrolled in a specific first-

year student learning community. The students were selected as a convenience sample for

the study as this researcher coordinates and instructs the selected learning community and

46

first-year seminar course where the Myers-Briggs instrument was facilitated. The students

were enrolled fulltime their first fall semester to seek their first bachelor’s degree.

Extracting required information from each student’s admission and academic record

and comparing it with the reported Myers-Briggs preference of the student became the basis

for the secondary dataset of this study. The Myers-Briggs information were supplied to the

Office of the Registrar so that student identifiers could be removed to protect student privacy

following data extraction. When the data were extracted, a codebook was created that

identifies details for each variable. Text values were replaced with numeric data when

possible, all labels and values were assigned, and data were verified against the original

extracted file.

Study Variables

Dependent

This study has one dependent variable—the first-semester grade point average of

the students. This grade point variable is continuous, and is reported as actual GPA received.

Independent

The independent variables used in this study are organized into three areas: (a)

student attributes, or background demographic characteristics; (b) academic aptitude, or high

school academic background characteristics; and (c) survey and academic environmental

characteristics that occurred while enrolled for their semester at Iowa State. Table 3.2

provides a listing of the variables.

47

Table 3.2. Variables, coding scale, and source file of the data

Data Analysis

The study framework identified independent variables for this study in three areas: (a)

student attributes, or background demographic characteristics; (b) academic aptitude or high

school academic background characteristics; and (c) survey and academic environmental

characteristics that occurred while enrolled for their semester at Iowa State. Descriptive,

comparative, and inferential statistical analyses were conducted on the quantitative data

collected from student records and Myers-Briggs preferences. The analyses were comprised

of measures of categorical and continuous variables. Various descriptive and inferential

statistical analyses were completed, including Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) and logistic

(including step-wise) regression models. A check for multicollinearity was completed to

ensure that independent variables were not redundant with one another. If there is

redundancy of a variable, it loses any predictive value over another independent variable

(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).

Variable Coding/scale Source File

Gender

Dichotomous 1=female 0=male

Admissions

STEM Major

Dichotomous 1=yes 0=no

Admissions

ACT Composite Score

Continuous Admissions

H.S.%ile Rank

Continuous Admissions

Myers-Briggs Preferences

Continuous Learning Community

Cumulative GPA end of first college semester

Continuous Registrar

48

Table 3.3 provides a breakdown of the research questions, variables, and method of

analysis. For research questions 2 through 4, type distributions were completed with self-

selection ratio type table (SRTT) analysis, the primary method for measurement of type

distribution (McCaulley, 1985). SRTT determines the over- or underrepresentation of a

research sample in comparison to a national base type preference sample. The ratio

numerator represents the percentage of that type in the research sample while the

denominator is the type percentage in the base population. This was followed by a chi-

square test and simulated distribution of the test statistic to determine probability for the

frequency of a given type preference occurring by chance.

Ethical Considerations

The University Registrar approved the Release of Student Information for Research

for the project on July 9, 2012. The study received approval for meeting requirements of

federal regulations and ISU policies governing human subjects research from the Iowa State

Institutional Review Board (IRB) on July 12, 2012. A copy of the IRB approval is provided

in Appendix A.

This researcher protected participant rights and confidentiality. Due to the secondary

database’s sensitive nature of containing student information, the data were reported in

aggregate terms to maintain the anonymity of individual student records.

Myers-Briggs survey participant data were de-identified by the Registrar to assure

data integrity. At no time are individuals named or otherwise identified in reports or

presentations. Additionally, this researcher has accumulated more then 20 fulltime years

49

Table 3.3. Research questions, variables, and method of analysis

Variables

Research Questions Independent Dependent Method of Analysis 1. What are the demographics of the students in the study including academic aptitude (ACT and high school class rank) fall 2004 to 2009?

Background characteristics gender ACT class rank class size

Descriptive

2. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students in the study by each cohort year and the whole study in comparison to the distribution of a national population?

Gender type preference STEM entry year

SRTT Chi-Square Simulation of Test Statistic

3. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences by gender for students in the study by each cohort year and across groups in comparison to the distribution of a national population?

Gender type preference STEM entry year

SRTT Chi-Square Simulation of Test Statistic

4. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preference for students with STEM majors in the study in comparison to the distribution of a national population?

2004, 2005, 2006, 2007 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011

SRTT Chi-Square Simulation of Test Statistic

5. Are there statistically significant differences in academic aptitude of ACT and high school class rank and Myers-Briggs preference for students by each cohort year and across groups in the study?

2004, 2005, 2006, 2007 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011

Inferential ANOVA Tukey-Kramer HSD Cross-Tabulation

6. Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preference for students in the study and GPA in comparison by cohort year and across groups?

2004, 2005, 2006, 2007 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011

Mean GPA conclusion of first college semester

Inferential ANOVA Tukey-Kramer HSD Cross-Tabulation

7. Is there correlation of ACT, class rank, or Myers-Briggs preference to first semester GPA by each cohort year and across groups?

2004, 2005, 2006, 2007 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011

Mean GPA conclusion of first college semester

Pearson correlation Multiple logistic regression

50

working in a higher education environment and is well practiced in FERPA regulations and

the importance of confidentiality.

Limitations

This study was designed to capture data for an identified group of students at this

university and should be carefully interpreted before comparison to other student groups or

institutions. As all students under the age of 18 years at the time of enrollment were

excluded from the sample, the results are not fully representative. Additional limitations of

this study include the structure of Myers-Briggs as a self-reporting instrument. The

assumption must be made that the respondents are of normal mental health and objectively

report their preferences when completing the assessment. Additionally, as respondents were

asked to complete the Myers-Briggs assessment as part of a first-year seminar assignment on

learning styles as opposed to self-selecting to complete the instrument, the assumption was

made that the respondents were objectively reporting their preferences and were not

influenced by the assignment directive.

Delimitations

A delimitation of this research is that the scope of the study was confined to students

in a specific first-year student learning-community who enrolled fulltime each fall semester

from 2004 through 2011 at Iowa State University. The study sample was deemed of

sufficient sample size to address the data, but may not be representative of the student

population based upon demographic and socioeconomic status. Other learning communities,

colleges, or universities would also have similar student populations from which to draw

information. Creation of this database also required that certain students be delimited or

51

excluded from the study if they were not 18 years of age at the semester of enrollment or

academic aptitude information was missing from the dataset.

Summary

The methodological approach suggested for this study was highlighted in this chapter.

Outlines were provided for research questions, hypotheses, research design, and study

setting. Additionally, the population, sample, data collection, variables, data management,

and proposed data analysis were reviewed. Ethical considerations, and limitations and

delimitations of the study were also addressed.

52

CHAPTER 4. RESULTS

Overview

This chapter provides an overview of the quantitative findings of this study

comparing student academic aptitude and Myers-Briggs preference with ability to achieve a

2.00 grade point in the first semester. Understanding differences of psychological type and

how type pertains to learning style in students may enhance student success. The chapter is

organized into seven sections. Each section is based upon a research question and is

supplemented by six corresponding hypotheses statements. Section RQ 1 examines the

background characteristics and academic demographics of students who enrolled fulltime at

Iowa State beginning each fall semester from fall 2004 through 2011 (also referred to as the

research population). The descriptive reporting includes male or female, high school

percentile class rank, composite ACT scores, size of high school graduating class and

whether students selected a STEM major. Percentages are reported for all of these

characteristics.

Section RQ 2 reports whether statistically significant differences are present in the

distribution of type and among each cohort year and for the full sample in comparison to the

national sample distribution. Section RQ 3 reports the possibility of a statistically significant

difference between male and female students in comparison to the national sample

distribution. Section RQ 4 examines whether STEM majors in the sample are found with

demonstrated difference in comparison to the national sample distribution. Tables and

figures highlight any change over the eight years of the study.

53

Section RQ 5 evaluates whether a statistically significant difference is present in the

mean ACT and high school class percentile rank in comparison to Myers-Briggs preferences

for cohort year and for the research population. Section RQ 6 analyzes if there are

significant differences in academic aptitude and Myers-Briggs preferences based on ability to

achieve a 2.00 grade point for each cohort and the research population. Section RQ 7 reports

the Stepwise regression results between students’ composite ACT score, percentile class

rank, Myers-Briggs preferences and achievement of a 2.00 grade point at the conclusion of

the first college semester.

Psychological type assessment can been helpful in detecting interpersonal roadblocks

for students in the transition from high school to college. This examination of type

distribution and type research related to learning preferences may help students understand

more about themselves in the college transition.

Design Classification

The study was structured as quasi-experimental time series design in that measures of

the student cohort were collected before and after the assessment (Creswell, 2009). The

Myers-Briggs instrument (Form M) was collected from 775 first-semester freshmen at a

Midwest research university and reviewed for type preferences. The assessment was given

as a complement to a first-year seminar course lecture related to learning style. Student high

school rank and ACT scores were collected from prior to enrollment. An additional

comparison examined variables based upon greater or less than a 2.0 GPA in the first

semester. The framework used to organize this study is retrospective cohort analysis, with

the outcomes of the research population being reviewed without following specific cases. It

54

is also between-group design in that the descriptive and inferential statistical analyses are

compared for the research sample, between the cohorts of the research sample, and with a

national base population. The research questions focus on statistically significant differences

between the sample and base populations (Figure 1). This design is necessary for

determining the correct statistical analyses for the research questions.

There are several different groupings of students based on achievement of a 2.0 grade

point in the first semester, Type preference, and STEM major:

1. ! 2.00 first semester grade point 2. " 2.00 first semester grade point 3. Composite ACT 4. High School percentile rank 5. STEM major 6. Non-STEM major 7. Myers-Briggs Type Preferences (16 options)

Analysis of Research Questions and Hypotheses

Student background characteristics

Research Question 1: What are the academic demographics of students in the research population, including academic aptitude (ACT and high school class rank) and first-semester grade point average?

This first research question examined the background characteristics of cohort

population size and male/female delineation in Table 4.1. The number and percentages of

students in this population who enrolled in STEM or non-STEM majors is provided in Table

4.2. For each of the eight years, precollege academic aptitude as measured by the mean and

55

Figure 4.1. Euler diagram of student group classifications, illustrating

variable relationship categories among students

standard deviation of the students’ high school percentile rank and the composite ACT score

is provided (see Table 4.3). The mean first-semester grade point average in this research

population was 2.94, with a standard deviation of 0.761.

Gender

Overall, males comprised 49.8% of the study population whereas 50.1% were female.

Between fall 2004 and fall 2011, the male population ranged between 42% and 61%, and the

1 See Table 4.36. Cross tabulation of means and standard deviations comparing first semester grade point by

MBTI for the population, 2004-2011.

56

Table 4.1. Gender distribution of students who enrolled fulltime, fall 2004–fall 2011 Female Male Year entered n % n % Total

2004 50 52 47 48 97 2005 43 45 53 55 96 2006 55 58 40 42 95 2007 51 53 45 47 96 2008 39 39 60 61 99 2009 51 51 49 49 100 2010 52 54 45 46 97 2011 48 51 47 49 95

Total 389 50.1 386 49.8 775

N = 775 Table 4.2. Learning community distribution of students in STEM majors, who enrolled fulltime, fall 2004–fall 2011 Female Male Year entered n % n % Total

2004 53 55 44 45 97 2005 33 34 63 66 96 2006 49 52 46 48 95 2007 45 47 51 53 96 2008 46 46 53 54 99 2009 46 46 53 53 100 2010 35 36 62 64 97 2011 39 41 56 59 95

Total 346 45 529 55 775

N = 775 female population between 39% and 58%. The average yearly number of students in each

cohort during these eight years was 96.88 students. The race of the students in this

population was predominately White; because only a very small number of students in this

study were from other racial groups, race was not utilized as a variable for this study.

57

Table 4.3. High school percentile rank, composite ACT score means, and standard deviations of students who enrolled fulltime, fall 2004–fall 2011

Year entered n High school

class%ile rank mean

SD ACT composite Mean SD

2004 97 80.72 15.39 24.55 3.54

2005 96 80.71 12.85 23.90 3.96

2006 95 82.12 12.69 24.02 3.68

2007 96 81.88 13.55 24.90 3.52

2008 99 80.34 14.88 24.52 3.22

2009 100 82.29 13.03 24.57 3.66

2010 97 82.59 12.84 24.08 3.56

2011 95 80.13 12.90 23.87 3.89

8-year aveage 775 81.35 13.53 24.31 3.64

N = 775

STEM major

Iowa State University offers degree programs in more than 100 majors.

Approximately two-thirds of those majors are in Science, Technology, Engineering or

Mathematics (STEM). Students with declared majors in STEM comprised 55% of the study

population. Between fall 2004 and fall 2011, the STEM population ranged between 44% and

62% of each cohort, and the non-STEM population between 33% and 53%. Non-STEM

majors outnumbered STEM majors in only two cohorts, 2004 and 2006.

High-school percentile rank, composite ACT score means, and standard

deviations

The mean high-school graduating class percentile rank in this research population

was 81.35, with a standard deviation of 13.53. The percentile rank ranged from 80.13 to

82.59 during this eight-year period. The composite ACT was 24.31, with a standard

58

deviation of 3.64, whereas the composite ACT scores ranged from 23.87 to 24.90 during this

eight-year period.

The mean high school GPA of the students who enrolled fulltime from fall 2004

through fall 2011 was 3.63. As high school GPA was assessed as a component of university

admission policies in only three of the eight years of the research sample, this variable was

not utilized in the demographic collection.

Summary

The population in this study was comprised of 775 students. The students were new

direct from high school Iowa students who enrolled fulltime at Iowa State beginning each fall

semester from 2004 through 2011, and were selected to participate in the learning community

by way of receiving the program scholarship. The following list summarizes the key

background characteristics of this population that answer Research question 1: What are the

academic demographics of students in the research population, including academic aptitude

(ACT and high school class rank) and first-semester grade point average?

1. A slight majority of the students in the full research sample were female (50.1%);

(49.8%) were male.

2. A majority of the students were enrolled in STEM majors (55%); (45%) were non-

STEM.

3. The mean composite high school graduating class percentile rank was 81.35, with a

standard deviation of 13.53.

4. The mean composite ACT score was 24.31, with a standard deviation of 3.64.

59

Differences in Myers-Briggs preference by cohort year and for the total population

Research Question 2: Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for students in the study and by each cohort year in comparison to the distribution of a national population?

H0 1: There is no difference between the Myers-Briggs preferences for

students in the study and by cohort year and the national population.

To measure Research Question 2, type distributions were completed with self-

selection ratio type table (SRTT) analysis, the primary method for measurement of type

distribution (McCaulley, 1985). SRTT determines the over- or under-representation of a

research sample in comparison to a national base type preference sample. The ratio

numerator represents the percentage of that type in the research sample while the

denominator is the type percentage in the base population. This was followed by a chi-

square test and simulated distribution of the test statistic to determine a probability for

frequency of a given type preference occurring by chance.

Following the SRTT and chi-square analysis of type preference for each cohort year

and the full research sample, a simulation calculation of the distribution of the test statistic

from the multinomial or null distribution was calculated for each type table. The simulation

can be explained as rolling multisided dice; for example, a die with 775 sides being rolled, or

sampled, 100,000 times. The test statistic distribution was computed using the entire

research sample of 775 students, and for the eight cohort years. Of the nine samples of

students, 2004-2011, and the full research sample, only the 2010 cohort group was found not

significantly different from the base sample type population.

60

Each block in the 16 block type table examined in research question 2 contains the

name of the type, the percentage of the sample with preferences for this type and the

percentage of the base population or expected frequency for this type. The final figure in the

block is the index, or observed to expected frequency for this type. A probability figure is

included with the index if statistical significance of the ratio is found through 2 ! 2 chi-

square analysis with one degree of freedom and is highlighted in the following analysis. .

The simulated distribution of the test statistic from the null population is included for each

table as some cell frequencies are five or less.

Table 4.4 illustrates the SRTT for the full research sample of 775 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table is significant at 0.000 indicating that it is unlikely to occur by chance in the

sample. Of the 16 types, 7 types were underrepresented in comparison to the base population

sample, 8 were over-represented, and 1 type preference was equal to the base population.

Eleven types were found to be statistically significant for the research sample—ISTJ, ISFJ,

INFJ, ISFP, INFP, ESTP, ENFP, ENTP, ESTJ, ESFJ, and ENFJ.

ISTJ, or Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging was found to occur in 5.5%

of the research population. The index or ratio at 0.47 was less than the expected occurrence

of 8.1% in the base population, and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 24.467, df = 1, N = 43, p < .001). ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling

and Judging, was found to occur in 7.3% of the research population. The index or ratio at

0.53 was also less than the expected occurrence of 13.8% in the base population and is

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 24.272, df = 1, N = 56, p < .001). INFJ, or

Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 3.3% of the research

61

Table 4.4. Type distribution of research sample (N=775) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

population. The index or ratio at 2.2 was greater than the expected occurrence of 1.5% in the

base population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 15.388, df = 1, N

= 25, p < .001).

ISFP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 4.4%

of the research population. The index or ratio at 0.50 was one half the expected occurrence

of 8.8% found in the base population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation

62

(X! =17.15, df = 1, N = 34, p < .001). INFP, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and

Perceiving was found to occur in 8.3% of the research population. The index or ratio at 1.89

was greater than the expected occurrence of 4.4% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 26.217, df = 1, N = 64, p < .001).

ESTP, or Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to occur in

7.5% of the research population. The index or ratio at 0.67 was less than the expected

occurrence of 8.7% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 18.27, df = 1, N = 58, p < .01). ENFP, the preference for Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 17.9% of the research

population. The index or ratio was 2.21 times more than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in

the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! =

92.557, df = 1, N = 139, p < .001). ENTP, or Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and

Perceiving, was found in 5.8% of the research population. The index or ratio of 1.81 was

more than the expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 16.453, df = 1, N = 45, p < .001).

The ESTJ preference, Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was also

found in 5.8% of the research population. The index or ratio of 0.67 was less than the

expected occurrence of 8.7% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 7.458, df = 1, N = 45, p < .001). ESFJ, Extraverted Sensing with

Feeling and Judging, comprised 6.7% of the research population. The index or ratio of 0.54

indicated fewer ESFJ preferences in the sample than the expected occurrence of 12.3% in the

base population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 19.691, df = 1, N

= 52, p < .001). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur

63

in 6.7% of the population (X! = 54.936, df = 1, N = 52, p < .001). The index or ratio was 2.68

times more than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation.

Table 4.5 shows the SRTT for the 2004 cohort of 97 students in comparison to the

national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table

was significant at 0.000 indicating little occurrence chance in the sample. Of the 16 types, 10

types were underrepresented in comparison to the base population sample and 8 types were

over-represented. Four types were found to be statistically significant for the research

sample—ISFJ, INFP, ENFP and ENFJ.

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 6.2% of

the 2004 population. The index or ratio wass 0.38, or less than the expected occurrence of

13.8% in the base population and significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.253, df

= 1, N = 5, p < .05). INFP, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in

19.6% of the population. The index or ratio was 4.45 times more than the expected

occurrence of 4.4% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 50.851, df = 1, N = 5, p < .001). ENFP, the preference for

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 16.5% of the

research population. The index or ratio was 2.04 times more than the expected occurrence of

8.1% in the base population, and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation

(X! = 8.439, df = 1, N = 16, p < .01). ENFJ, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 10.3% of the research population. The index or

ratio was 4.12 times higher than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and

64

Table 4.5. Type distribution of 2004 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 23.662, df = 1, N = 10, p

< .001).

The 2005 cohort is highlighted in Table 4.6 with the SRTT for 96 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.000, indicating little occurrence chance in the sample. Of

the 16 types, 7 types were underrepresented in comparison to the base population sample and

65

Table 4.6. Type distribution of 2005 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

9 types were over-represented. Seven types were found to be statistically significant for the

research sample—ISFJ, INFJ, ISTP, INFP, ENFP, ENTP, and ENFJ.

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 6.3% of

the 2005 population (X! = 3.965, df = 1, N = 6, p < .05). The index or ratio was 0.47, or less

than the expected occurrence of 13.8% in the base population, and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and

66

Judging, was found in 6.3% of the population (X! = 14.44, df = 1, N = 6, p < .001). The

index or ratio was 4.2 times more than the expected occurrence of 1.5% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation. ISTP, or

Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was not found in the 2005 population (X!

= 5.184, df = 1, N = 0, p < .05). The index or ratio of zero was less than the expected

occurrence of 5.4% in the base population, and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-

square calculation. INFP, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in

10.4% of the population. The index or ratio was 2.36 times more than the expected

occurrence of 4.4% in the base population, and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 7.898, df = 1, N = 10, p < .01).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 18.8% of the 2005 population. The index or ratio was 2.32 times more than

the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found to be significant at

0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 13.442, df = 1, N = 18, p < .001). ENTP, the preference

for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to occur in 7.3% of the

population. The index, at 2.28, was greater than the expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.023, df =

1, N = 7, p < .05). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging was found to

occur in 7.3% of the research population (N = 7). The index or ratio was 2.92 times more

than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population, and was found to be significant

at 0.01 in chi-square calculation. (X! = 8.817, df = 1, N = 7, p < .01).

The 2006 student cohort is depicted in Table 4.7 with the SRTT for 95 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

67

Table 4.7. Type distribution of 2006 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

full type table was significant at 0.000, indicating little occurrence chance in the sample. Of

the 16 types, 8 types were underrepresented in comparison to the base population sample, 7

types were over-represented, and 1 was equal to the base sample. Seven types were found to

be statistically significant for the research sample—ISFJ, INFJ, ISFP, INFP, ENFP, ESFJ,

and ENFJ.

68

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 6.3% of the

2006 population (X! = 3.856 df = 1, N = 6, p < .05). The index or ratio was 0.46, or less than

the expected occurrence of 13.8% in the base population, and was found to be significant at

0.05 in chi-square calculation. INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was

found in 4.2% of the population (X! = 4.653, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05). The index or ratio was

4.2 times more than the expected occurrence of 1.5% in the base population and was found to

be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. ISFP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling

and Perceiving, was 2.1% of the 2006 population (X! = 4.838, df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). The

index was less than the expected occurrence of 8.8% in the base population and was found to

be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. INFP, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling

and Perceiving, was found in 9.5% of the population. The index or ratio was 2.16 times

more than the expected occurrence of 4.4% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.558, df = 1, N = 9, p < .05).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 23.2% of the 2006 population. The index or ratio was 2.86 times greater

than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found to be significant

at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 26.593, df = 1, N = 22, p < .001). ESFJ, the

preference for Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 4.2% of

the 2006 population. The index of 0.34 was less than the expected occurrence of 12.3% in

the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! =

5.054, df = 1, N = 7, p < .05). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was

found to occur in 9.5% of the research population. The index or ratio was 3.80 times more

69

than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was found to be significant

at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 18.480, df = 1, N = 9, p < .001).

The 2007 student cohort is depicted in Table 4.8 with the SRTT for 96 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.000, indicating little occurrence chance in the sample. Of

the 16 types, 7 were underrepresented in comparison to the base population sample, 8 types

were over-represented, and 1 was equal to the base sample. Seven types were found to be

statistically significant for the research sample—ISTJ, INFJ, INFP, ENFP, ENTP, ESFJ, and

ENFJ.

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was found to occur in 5.5% of

the 2007 population (X! = 5.944 df = 1, N = 3, p < .05). The index was 0.47, or less than the

expected occurrence of 11.6% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05

in chi-square calculation. INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found

in 3.3% of the population (X! = 8.801, df = 1, N = 5, p < .01). The index or ratio was 2.2

times more than the expected occurrence of 1.5% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation. INFP, which is Introverted Intuition with

Feeling and Perceiving, was found in 8.3% of the population. The index or ratio was 1.89

times more than the expected occurrence of 4.4% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.400, df = 1, N = 9, p < .05).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 24.2% of the 2007 population. With an index 2.21 times greater than the

expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population, ENFP was found to be significant at

0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 29.806, df = 1, N = 23, p < .001). ENTP, or Extraverted

70

Table 4.8. Type distribution of 2007 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to occur in 5.8% of the 2007 population.

The index or ratio was 1.81 times greater than the expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.023, df =

1, N = 7, p < .05). ESFJ, or Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to

occur in 6.7% of the 2007 population. With an index of 0.54, it was less than the expected

occurrence of 12.3% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-

71

square calculation (X! = 3.925, df = 1, N = 5, p < .05). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 7.2% of the 2007 cohort. The index or ratio was

2.68 times more than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was found

to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation. (X! = 8.817, df = 1, N = 7, p < .01).

The 2008 student cohort is depicted in Table 4.9 with the SRTT for 99 students with

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.000, indicating little occurrence chance in the sample. Of

the 16 types in the table, 10 were underrepresented in comparison to the base population

sample while 6 types were over-represented. Four types were found to be statistically

significant for the research sample—ISFJ, ESTP, ENFP, and ENFJ.

ISFJ, Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 2.0% of the 2008

population (X! = 9.955, df = 1, N = 2, p < .01). The index was 0.15, or less than the expected

occurrence of 13.8% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-

square calculation. ESTP was the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and

Perceiving and was found in 12.1% of the population (X! = 14.084, df = 1, N = 12, p < .001).

The SRTT ration was 2.81 times more than the expected occurrence of 4.3% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation.

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to be 21.2% of the 2008 population. The index or ratio was 2.62 times greater than the

expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001

in chi-square calculation (X! = 21.013, df = 1, N = 21, p < .001). ENFJ, or Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 7.0% of the 2008 research

population. The index or ratio was 2.80 times more than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in

72

Table 4.9. Type distribution of 2008 cohort (N = 99) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation. (X! =

8.308, df = 1, N = 7, p < .01).

The 2009 student cohort is depicted in Table 4.10, with analysis for 100 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.017200, indicating little occurrence chance in the sample.

Of the 16 types, 9 were underrepresented in comparison to the base population sample and 8

73

Table 4.10. Type distribution of 2009 cohort (N = 100) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

types were over-represented. Three type preferences were found to be statistically significant

for the research sample—ISTP, ISFP, ENFP, and ESFJ.

ISTP, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to occur in

11.0% of the 2009 population (X! = 5.807 df = 1, N = 11, p < .05). The index was 1.85, or

greater than the expected occurrence of 5.4% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. ISFP, Introverted Sensing with Feeling and

74

Perceiving, was found to occur in 3.0% of the 2009 population (X! = 3.823 df = 1, N = 3, p <

.05). The index was 0.34, or less than the expected occurrence of 8.8% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. ENFP, the

preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in

19.0% of the 2009 population. The index or ratio was 2.35 times greater than the expected

occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 14.668, df = 1, N = 19, p < .001). ESFJ, the preference for

Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 5.0% of the 2009

population. The index of 0.41 was less than the expected occurrence of 12.3% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.333, df =

1, N = 5, p < .05).

The 2010 student cohort is outlined in Table 4.11 with the SRTT for 97 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table did not reveal significance. Of the 16 types, 5 were underrepresented in

comparison to the base population sample, 10 types were over-represented, and 1 was equal

to the base sample. Two types were found to be statistically significant for the cohort—

ENTP and ENFJ.

ENTP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was

found in 8.2% of the 2010 population. The index or ratio was 2.56 times greater than the

expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 7.723, df = 1, N = 8, p < .01). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition

with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 4.1% of the 2010 population. The index

75

Table 4.11. Type distribution of 2010 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

was 1.64 times more than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was

found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.452, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05).

The 2011 student cohort in Table 4.12 has the SRTT for 95 students in comparison to

the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table

was significant at 0.000003, indicating little occurrence chance in the sample. Of the 16

types, 7 were underrepresented in comparison to the base population sample, 8 types were

76

Table 4.12. Type distribution of 2011 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

over-represented, and 1 was equal to the base sample. Nine types were found to be

statistically significant for the research sample—ISFP, ESTP, ENTP, and ESFJ.

ISFP, Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 2.1% of

the 2011 population (X! = 4.838 df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). The index was 0.24, or less than the

expected occurrence of 8.8% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in

chi-square calculation. ESTP, the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and

77

Perceiving, was found to occur in 14.7% of the 2011 population. The index or ratio was 3.42

times greater than the expected occurrence of 4.3% in the base population and was found to

be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 26.065, df = 1, N = 14, p < .001).

ENTP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 7.4% of the 2011 population. The index or ratio was 2.31 times greater

than the expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base population and was found to be significant

at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.158, df = 1, N = 7, p < .05). ESFJ, the preference for

Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 4.2% of the population.

The index of 0.34 was less than the expected occurrence of 12.3% in the basepopulation and

was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.054, df = 1, N = 4, p <

.05).

Null hypothesis 1 was rejected because there were statistically significant differences

in the distribution of type preferences in each cohort group of students and for the full

research sample. Although the test statistic distribution simulation conducted by cohort year

found the 2010 cohort group type preference not significantly different from the base sample

type population, it did contain two type preferences, ENFJ and ENTP, found to be

significantly different from the base sample.

In the full research sample of 775 students, 7 type preferences were found to be a

lower percentage than the national base sample, 8 preferences were found to be greater than

the base sample, and 1, ENTJ, was found to have an equal percentage in the research sample

as in the national base sample. For the eight cohort years in the study, only 3 type preference

comparisons of 128 (eight cohort years, 16 type preferences) were found to be equal to the

base national sample for the preference, 2 INTJ and 1 ENTJ.

78

Differences in Myers-Briggs preference by male or female by cohort year and for total

population

Research Question 3: Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for male and female students in the study by each cohort year and for the research population in comparison to the distribution of a national population?

H0 2: There is no difference between the Myers-Briggs preferences for male

and female students in the study and by each cohort year and the national

population.

To measure Research Question 3, type distributions were completed with self-

selection ratio type table (SRTT) analysis followed by a chi-square test and simulated

distribution of the test statistic to determine a probability for frequency of a given type

preference table occurring by chance. Of the eight cohorts, 2004-2011, and the full research

sample of students, only the self-selection type ratio table for females in the 2009 and 2010

cohort groups were not statistically different from the base sample national population.

Each category block in the 16 type tables illustrated in Research Question 3 contains

the type preference and three rows of type percentage and frequency information for the

research sample and base population. Row one includes the number of male students with

the type preference in the sample, the percentage of males with this preference, and the index

or ratio of that percentage to the base population sample of males with the same type

preference. Row two is comprised of the percentage population of the base sample with the

type preference, the percentage of males in the base sample with the preference, and the

percentage of females in the base sample with the type preference. Row three includes the

number of female students with the type preference in the sample, the percentage of females

in the cohort with this preference, and the index or ratio of that percentage to the base

79

population sample of females with the same type preference. A probability figure is included

for the male and female indexes if statistical significance of the ratio is found through chi-

square analysis with one degree of freedom. The simulated distribution of the test statistic

from the null population is mentioned for each table as some cell frequencies have five or

fewer students. The following are descriptions of the SRTT and type preferences with

significance for each cohort.

Research sample type preference comparison with base sample

Table 4.13 shows the SRTT for the full research sample of 775 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.000, indicating that it was unlikely to occur by chance in

the sample. Of the 16 types, 9 types were underrepresented for males in comparison to the

base population sample and 7 were over-represented. Seven types were underrepresented for

females in comparison to the base population sample and 9 were over-represented. Eleven of

16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for males in the research

sample, and 9 of 16 type preferences were statistically significant for females in the research

sample. Only preferences for ISTP, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving,

INTP, Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, and ESFP, Extraverted Sensing

with Feeling and Perceiving, were not found statistically different from the base population

sample for either male or female students.

Male type preference compared with base sample

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was presented in 6.0% of males

in the full research population. The index, or ratio, at 0.37, was the smallest SRTT for males,

80

Table 4.13. Male/female distribution of research sample (N = 775) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

and was less than the expected occurrence of 16.4% of males with this type in the base

population, and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 25.66, df

= 1, N = 23, p < .001). ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in

3.6% of males in the research population. The index or ratio at 0.44 was also less than the

expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and is significant at 0.01 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 9.562, df = 1, N = 14, p < .01).

81

INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was preferred for 2.8% of

males in the research population. The index or ratio at 2.33 was more than double the

expected occurrence of 1.2% for males in the base population and was significant at 0.01 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 8.904, df = 1, N = 11, p < .01). ISFP, or Introverted Sensing

with Feeling and Judging, was found for 4.7% of males in the research population. The

index or ratio at 0.62 demonstrated it was less than the expected occurrence of 7.6% found

for males in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X!

=4.358, df = 1, N = 18, p < .05). INFP, which is Introverted Intuition with Feeling and

Perceiving, was found to occur in 8.5% of the research population. The index or ratio at 2.07

indicated a greater than the expected occurrence of 4.1% in the base population for males and

was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 18.724, df = 1, N = 33, p

< .001).

ESTP, or Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving was found to occur for

10.9% of males in the research population. The index or ratio at 1.95 was higher than the

expected occurrence of 5.6% in the base population and was found significant at 0.001 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 19.267, df = 1, N = 42, p < .001). ENFP, or Extraverted Intuition

with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in 17.4% of males or 67 students, and was the most

frequent type preference for male students in the study. The index or ratio of 2.72 was more

than the expected occurrence of 6.4% of males in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 72.441 df = 1, N = 67, p < .001). ENTP,

or Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found in 7.8% of males. The

index or ratio of 1.95 was more than the expected occurrence of 4.0% of males in the base

82

population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 13.842 df

= 1, N = 30, p < .001).

For the ESTJ preference, Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, males

totaled 6.7% of the research population. The index or ratio of 0.57 was less than the

expected occurrence of 11.2% in the base male population and was found to be significant at

0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 6.848, df = 1, N = 26, p < .01). ESFJ, the preference for

Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, comprised 4.7% of males in the research

population. The index or ratio of 0.63 indicated fewer ESFJ preferences in the sample than

the expected occurrence of 7.5% in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 4.172, df = 1, N = 18, p < .05). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 4.7% of the male population (X! = 22.458, df = 1,

N = 18, p < .001). The index or ratio was 2.94 times more than the expected occurrence of

1.6% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation.

Female type preference compared with base sample

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 10.8% of

females in the research population. The index or ratio at 0.56 was fewer than the expected

occurrence of 19.4% for females in the base population and is significant at 0.001 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 14.864, df = 1, N = 42, p < .001). INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with

Feeling and Judging, was preferred for 3.6% of females in the research population. The

index or ratio at 2.25 indicated this preference occurred with more frequency than the 1.6%

83

found for females in the base population and was significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation

(X! = 9.813, df = 1, N = 14, p < .01).

INTJ, or Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, was found for 2.3% of

females in the research population. The index or ratio at 2.56 demonstrated a greater than

expected occurrence of 0.9% was found for females in the base population and was

significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! =8.643, df = 1, N = 9, p < .01). ISFP, or

Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving, was found for 4.1% of females in the

research population. The index or ratio at 0.41 demonstrated less than the expected

occurrence of 9.9% found for females in the base population and was significant at 0.001 in

chi-square calculation (X! =13.149, df = 1, N = 16, p < .001). INFP, or Introverted Intuition

with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in 8.0% of females in the research population. The

index or ratio at 1.74 was greater than the expected occurrence of 4.6% in the base

population for females and was found significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! =

9.587, df = 1, N = 31, p < .01).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found for 18.5% of females in the research population, which at 72 students was the most

frequent preference for female students in the study. This index or ratio was 1.91 times more

than the expected occurrence of 9.7% of females in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 31.207, df = 1, N = 72, p < .001). ESFJ,

the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, comprised 5.1% of females

in the research population. The index or ratio of 0.51 indicated fewer females with ESFJ

preferences in the sample than the expected occurrence of 16.9% in the base population and

was significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 15.295, df = 1, N = 34, p < .001).

84

ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 8.7% of the

female population (X! = 35.113, df = 1, N = 34, p < .001). The index or ratio was 2.64 times

more than the expected occurrence of 3.3% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation. ENTJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling

and Judging, was found in 2.3% of the female population (X! = 8.643, df = 1, N = 9, p < .01).

The index or ratio was 2.56 times more than the expected occurrence of 0.9% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation.

Male and female 2004 distribution compared with base sample

Table 4.14 shows the SRTT for the 2004 cohort of 97 students in comparison to the national

base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was

significant at 0.000 (male) and 0.000001 (female), indicating little occurrence of chance in

the sample. Of the 16 types, 9 types were underrepresented for males in comparison to the

base population sample and 7 were over-represented. Eight types were underrepresented and

8 were over-represented for females in comparison to the base population sample. Three of

16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for males and for females in the

research sample.

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was presented in 4.3% of males

in the full research population. The index or ratio at 0.26 was less than the expected

occurrence of 16.4% of males with this type in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.229, df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). INFP, or

Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in 23.4% of males in the 2004

population. The index or ratio was 5.70 times more than the expected occurrence of 4.1% of

85

Table 4.14. Male/female distribution of 2004 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

males in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 42.624, df = 1, N = 11, p < .001). ENFP, the preference for Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 17.0% of the 2004 male

research population. The index or ratio was 2.66 times more than the expected occurrence of

6.4% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation

(X! = 8.333, df = 1, N = 8, p < .01).

86

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 6.0% of females

in the 2004 population. The index or ratio at 0.31 was fewer than the expected occurrence of

19.4% for females in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation

(X! = 4.628, df = 1, N = 3, p < .05). INFP, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and

Perceiving, was found to occur in 16.0% of females in the 2004 population. The index or

ratio at 3.48 was greater than the expected occurrence of 4.6% in the base population for

females and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 14.126, df =

1, N = 8, p < .001). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to

occur 16.0% of the 2004 female population (X! = 24.438, df = 1, N = 8, p < .001). The index

or ratio was 4.85 times more than the expected occurrence of 3.3% in the base female

population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation.

Male and female 2005 distribution compared with base sample

The 2005 cohort is shown in Table 4.15 with the SRTT for 96 students in comparison

to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type

table was significant at 0.00065 (male) and 0.000077 (female), indicating little occurrence of

chance in the sample. Of the 17 types, 8 were underrepresented and 8 were over-represented

for males in the cohort. Seven types were underrepresented for females in comparison to the

base population sample and 9 were over-represented. Five of 16 type preferences were found

to be statistically significant for males in the 2005 sample, and 4 of 16 type preferences were

found to be statistically significant for females.

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was not found among males in

the 2005 population. The index or ratio at 0.0 was less than the expected occurrence of

87

Table 4.15. Male/female distribution of 2005 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

16.4% of males with this type in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01

in chi-square calculation (X! = 8.700, df = 1, N = 0, p < .01). INFJ, or Introverted Intuition

with Feeling and Judging, was found in 5.7% of the population (X! = 8.703, df = 1, N = 3, p <

.005). The index or ratio was 4.75 times more than the expected occurrence of 1.2% in the

base population and was found to be significant at 0.005 in chi-square calculation. ISTP, or

Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was also not found in the 2005 population

88

(X! = 4.51, df = 1, N = 0, p < .05). The index or ratio of zero was less than the expected

occurrence of 5.4% in the base population and was found significant at 0.05 in chi-square

calculation.

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 20.1% of males in the 2005 population. The index or ratio was 3.14 times

more than the expected occurrence of 6.4% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 17.083, df = 1, N = 11, p < .001). ENTP,

the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to occur in

11.3% of males in the 2005 population. The index or ratio was 2.83 times more than the

expected occurrence of 4.0% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001

in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.101, df = 1, N = 6, p < .01).

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 4.7% of females

in the 2005 population. The index or ratio at 0.24 was lower than the expected occurrence of

19.4% for females in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation

(X! = 4.820, df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging,

was found in 7.0% of females in the 2005 population. The index or ratio at 4.38 was greater

than the expected occurrence of 1.6% for females in the base population and was significant

at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.733, df = 1, N = 3, p < .01). INFP, or Introverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 11.6% of females in the 2005

population. The index or ratio at 2.52 was greater than the expected occurrence of 4.6% in

the base population for females and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 4.606, df = 1, N = 5, p < .05). Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and

Judging, or ENFJ, was found to occur 11.6% of the 2005 female population (X! = 9.026, df =

89

1, N = 5, p < .01). The index or ratio was 3.52 times more than the expected occurrence of

3.3% in the base female population and was found to be significant at at 0.01 in chi-square

calculation.

Male and female 2006 distribution compared with base sample

The 2006 student cohort is depicted in Table 4.16 with the SRTT for 95 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.00007 (male) and 0.000003 (female), indicating little

occurrence of chance in the sample. Of the 16 types, 8 were underrepresented and 8 were

over-represented for males and females in the cohort. Four of 16 type preferences were

found to be statistically significant for males in the 2006 sample, and 2 of 16 type

preferences were found to be statistically significant for females.

INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found in 5.0% of males

in the 2006 population. The index or ratio at 4.17 was greater than the occurrence of 1.2% in

the base male population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.813, df

= 1, N = 2, p < .05). INFP, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in

12.5% of males in the 2006 population (X! = 6.883, df = 1, N = 5, p < .01). The 3.05 index

was greater than the expected occurrence of 4.1% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation. ESFP, or Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and

Perceiving, was found to occur for 17.5% of males in the 2006 population. The index or

ratio at 2.54 indicated a higher than expected occurrence of 6.9% in the base population and

was found significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 6.514, df = 1, N = 7, p < .05).

ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in 20.0% of males in the

90

Table 4.16. Male/female distribution of 2006 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

2006 population. The index of 3.13 was larger than the expected occurrence of 6.4% in the

base male population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! =

11.56, df = 1, N = 8, p < .001).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 25.5% of the 2006 female population. The index or ratio was 2.63 times

greater than the expected occurrence of 9.7% in the base female population and was found to

91

be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 14.044, df = 1, N = 14, p < .001).

ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 12.7% of the

2006 female population. The index or ratio was 3.85 times more than the expected

occurrence of 3.3% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 14.743, df = 1, N = 7, p < .001).

Male and female 2007 distribution compared with base sample

The 2007 student cohort is illustrated with male and female type preference in Table 4.17

with the SRTT for 96 students in comparison to the national base sample. The simulated

distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.000005 (male) and

0.005760 (female), indicating the small occurrence of chance in the sample. Of the 16 types,

we are underrepresented and 8 were over-represented for males in the cohort. Of the 16

types, 7 were underrepresented and 9 were over-represented for females in the cohort. Four

of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for males in the 2007 sample,

whereas 3 of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for females.

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was found to occur in 2.2% of

the 2007 male population (X! = 5.516, df = 1, N = 1, p < .05). The index was 0.13, or less

than the expected occurrence of 16.4% in the base male population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and

Judging, was found in 6.6% of males (X! = 11.207, df = 1, N = 3, p < .001). The index or

ratio was 5.5 times more than the expected occurrence of 1.2% in the base male population

and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation. ENFP, the preference for

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 26.7% of the 2007

92

Table 4.17. Male/female distribution of 2007 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

male population. With an index that was 4.17 times greater than the expected occurrence of

6.4% in the base population, ENFP was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 28.88 df = 1, N = 12, p < .001). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling

and Judging, was found to occur in 11.1% of the 2007 male cohort. The index or ratio was

6.94 times more than the expected occurrence of 1.6% in the base population and was found

to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 25.44, df = 1, N = 5, p < .001).

93

ESTP, which is Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to

occur in 9.8% of females in the 2007 population. The index or ratio was 3.27 times greater

than the expected occurrence of 3.0% in the base female population and was found to be

significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.870, df = 1, N = 5, p < .01). ENFP, the

preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in

21.6% of the 2007 female population. With an index of 2.23 times greater than the expected

occurrence of 9.7% in the base population, ENFP was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 7.394, df = 1, N = 11, p < .01). ENTP, or Extraverted Intuition with

Thinking and Perceiving, was found in 7.8% of 2007 females. The index or ratio was 3.25

times greater than the expected occurrence of 2.4% in the base population and was found to

be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 6.334, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05).

Male and female 2008 distribution compared with base sample

The 2008 student cohort is provided for male and female type preference in Table

4.18 with the SRTT for 99 students in comparison to the national base sample. The

simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.000

(male) and 0.000029 (female), indicating the small occurrence of chance in either sample.

Of the 16 types, 10 were underrepresented and 6 were over-represented for males in the

cohort. For females in the cohort, 9 types are underrepresented and 7 are over-represented.

Three of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for males in the 2008

sample, whereas 4 of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for

females.

94

Table 4.18. Male/female distribution of 2008 cohort (N = 99) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

ISFJ, Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was not found in the 2008 male

population (X! = 4.86, df = 1, N = 0, p < .05). The index was 0.0, or less than the expected

occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found significant at 0.05 in chi-square

calculation. ESTP, or preference for Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was

found in 20.0% of the population (X! = 22.217, df = 1, N = 12, p < .001). The SRTT ratio

was 3.57 times more than the expected occurrence of 5.6% in the base population of males

95

and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation. ENFP, the preference for

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to be 20.0% of the 2008 male

population. The index or ratio was 3.13 times greater than the expected occurrence of 6.4%

in the base population and was found significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! =

17.34, df = 1, N = 12, p < .001).

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 5.1% of females

in the 2008 population. The index or ratio at 0.26 was lower than the expected occurrence of

19.4% for females as in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 4.098, df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). ENFP, or Extroverted Intuition with Feeling

and Perceiving, was found to occur in 23.1% of females in the 2008 population. The index

or ratio at 2.38 was greater than the expected occurrence of 9.7% in the base population for

females and is found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.209, df = 1, N

= 9, p < .01). Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, or ENFJ, was found to occur

12.8% of the 2008 female population (X! = 10.670, df = 1, N = 5, p < .01). The index or ratio

was 3.88 times more than the expected occurrence of 3.3% in the base female population and

was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation. Extraverted Intuition with

Thinking and Judging, or ENTJ, was found for 7.7% of the 2008 female population (X! =

20.064, df = 1, N = 3, p < .001). The index or ratio was 3.52 times more than the expected

occurrence of 0.9% in the base female population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in

chi-square calculation.

96

Male and female 2009 distribution compared with base sample

The 2009 student cohort is presented by male and female type preference in Table

4.19 with the SRTT for 100 students in comparison to the national base sample. The

simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.000061

for males, indicating a small occurrence of chance in the sample. The simulated distribution

of the test statistic for the full type table was 0.03369 for females. Of the 16 types, 11 were

underrepresented and 5 were over-represented for males in the cohort. For females in the

cohort, 8 types were underrepresented and 8 were over-represented. Three of 16 type

preferences were found to be statistically significant for males in the 2009 sample, whereas 2

of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for females.

ISFJ, Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was not found to occur in the

2009 male population (X! = 3.97 df = 1, N = 0, p < .05). The index was 0.0, or less than the

expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base male population, and was found to be significant at

0.05 in chi-square calculation. ESTP, or Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving,

was found to occur in 12.2% of males in the 2009 population. The index or ratio was 2.18

times greater than the expected occurrence of 5.6% in the base male population and was

found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 3.879, df = 1, N = 6, p < .05).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to

occur in 20.4% of the 2009 male population. The index or ratio was 3.19 times greater than

the expected occurrence of 6.4% in the base population and was found to be significant at

0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 19.675, df = 1, N = 11, p < .001).

97

Table 4.19. Male/female distribution of 2009 cohort (N = 100) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

ISTP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 9.8% of females

in the 2009 population. The index or ratio at 4.26 revealed this percentage was higher than

the expected occurrence of 2.3% for females in the base population, and was significant at

0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 12.538, df = 1, N = 5, p < .001). Extraverted Sensing

with Feeling and Judging, or ESFJ, was found to occur 3.9% of the 2009 female population

(X! = 5.084, df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). The index or ratio was .23, or less than the expected

98

occurrence of 16.9% in the base female population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in

chi-square calculation.

Male and female 2010 distribution compared with base sample

The 2010 student cohort by male and female type preference in Table 4.20 provides

the SRTT for 97 students in comparison to the national base sample. The simulated

distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.000033 for males,

indicating a very small occurrence of chance in either sample. The simulated distribution of

the test statistic for the full type table was 0.6317 for females. Of the 16 types, 8 were

underrepresented and 8 were over-represented for males in the cohort. For females in the

cohort, 10 types were underrepresented and 6 were over-represented. One of 16 type

preferences was found to be statistically significant for males in the 2010 sample, whereas 2

of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for females.

ENTP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was

found in 17.8% of the 2010 male population. The index or ratio was 4.45 times greater than

the expected occurrence of 4.0% in the base male population and was found to be significant

at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 21.356, df = 1, N = 8, p < .001). ISTP, or Introverted

Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 7.6% of females in the 2010 population.

The index or ratio at 3.30 was higher than the expected occurrence of 2.3% for females in the

base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 6.533, df = 1, N =

4, p < .05). Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, or ENTJ, was found to occur in

3.8% of the 2010 female population (X! = 4.980, df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). The index or ratio

99

Table 4.20. Male/female distribution of 2010 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

was 4.22, or greater than the expected occurrence of 0.9% in the base female population, and

found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation.

Male and female 2011 distribution compared with base sample

The 2011 student cohort in Table 4.21 has the SRTT for 95 students in comparison to

the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table

was significant at 0.000024 for males and 0.003890 for females, indicating a small

100

Table 4.21. Male/female distribution of 2011 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

occurrence of chance in either sample. Of the 16 types, 10 were underrepresented, 5 were

over-represented, and 1 was equal to the national sample for males in the cohort. For females

in the cohort, 8 types were underrepresented, 7 were over-represented, and 1 preference was

equal to the percentage in the national sample. One of 16 type preferences was found

statistically significant for males in the 2011 sample, whereas 4 of 16 type preferences were

found statistically significant for females.

101

ESTP, which is Extroverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to

occur in 21.3% of males in the 2011 population. The index or ratio at 3.80 was greater than

the expected occurrence of 5.6% in the base population for males and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 20.652, df = 1, N = 10, p < .001).

Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, or INFJ, was found to occur in 6.3%

of the 2011 female population (X! = 6.458, df = 1, N = 3, p < .05). The index or ratio was

3.94 times more than the expected occurrence of 1.6% in the base female population and was

found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. Introverted Intuition with Thinking

and Judging, or INTJ, was found to occur 6.3% of the 2011 female population (X! = 15.360,

df = 1, N = 3, p < .001). The index or ratio was 7.0 times more than the expected occurrence

of 0.9% in the base female population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation. ESTP, the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving,

was found to occur in 8.3% of the 2011 female population. The index or ratio was 2.77 times

greater than the expected occurrence of 3.0% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.551, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05). ENFP, the

preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in

20.8% of the population. The index of 2.14 demonstrated a greater than the expected

occurrence of 9.7% in the base female population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 6.119, df = 1, N = 10, p < .05).

Null hypothesis 2 was rejected because there were statistically significant differences

in the distribution of type preferences among males and females in each cohort group of

students and for the full research sample. Although the test statistic distribution simulation

conducted by cohort year found the 2009 and 2010 female cohort group type preferences not

102

significantly different from the base sample type population, each cohort demonstrated two

type preferences found to be significantly different from the base sample.

ENFP, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was the most frequent

type preference for the research population, with males at 17.4% (N = 67) and females at

18.5% (N = 71). The least frequent preference for male students was ENTJ, or Extraverted

Intuition with Thinking and Judging, at 1.3% of the population (N = 5). The least frequent

preference for female students was INTP, or Introverted Intuition with Thinking and

Perceiving, at 1.5% (N = 6). In the full research sample and eight cohort years, only one type

preference for males or females was found to be equal to the base national sample for the

preference, ISTP for male students and ESTJ for female students, both in the 2010 cohort

year. Research Question 3 revealed significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for

male and female for students in the study by each cohort year and for the research population

in comparison to the distribution of a national population.

Differences in Myers-Briggs preference for students with STEM majors by cohort year

and for total population

Research Question 4: Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences students with STEM majors in the study by each cohort year and for the research population in comparison to the distribution of a national population?

H0 2: There is no difference between the Myers-Briggs preferences for

students with STEM majors in the study and by each cohort year and the

national population.

To measure Research Question 4, type distributions with self-selection ratio type

table (SRTT) analysis were completed for students with STEM majors (N = 429) and non-

103

STEM majors (N = 346) followed by a chi-square test and simulated distribution of the test

statistic to determine a probability for frequency of a given type preference table occurring

by chance. Each category block in the sixteen type tables illustrated in Research Question 4

contains the type preference and three rows of type percentage and frequency information for

the research sample and base population. Row one includes the number of STEM students

with the type preference in the sample, the percentage of STEM students with this

preference, and the index or ratio of that percentage to the base population sample with the

same type preference. Row two is the percentage of the base sample population with the

type preference. Row three includes the number of non-STEM students with the type

preference in the sample, the percentage of non-STEM students in the cohort with this

preference, and the index or ratio of that percentage to the base population sample with the

same type preference. A probability figure is included for the STEM and non-STEM indexes

if statistical significance of the ratio is found through chi-square analysis with one degree of

freedom. The simulated distribution of the test statistic from the null population is

mentioned for each table as some cell frequencies have five or fewer students. The following

are descriptions of the SRTT and type preferences with significance for each cohort.

Research sample STEM and non-STEM major type preference comparison

Table 4.22 shows the SRTT for the full research sample of 775 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for

STEM and non-STEM tables was significant at 0.000, indicating that the results are unlikely

to occur by chance in the sample. Of the 16 types, 6 were underrepresented for STEM

majors in comparison to the base population sample and 10 were over-represented. Nine

104

Table 4.22. STEM and non-STEM distribution of research sample (N = 775) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

types were underrepresented for non-STEM majors in comparison to the base population

sample and 7 were over-represented. Nine of 16 type preferences were found to be

statistically significant for STEM majors in the research sample, whereas 9 of 16 type

preferences were statistically significant for non-STEM majors in the research sample.

105

STEM major type preference comparison with base sample

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was the preferred type in 5.1%

of students with STEM majors in the full research population. The index or ratio at 0.44 was

the smallest SRTT for non-STEM majors, and was less than the expected occurrence of

11.6% of individuals with this type in the base population. The index or ratio was found to

be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 15.519, df = 1, N = 22, p < .001). ISFJ,

or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 7.2% of students in STEM

majors in the research population. The index or ratio at 0.52 was also less than the expected

occurrence of 13.8% in the base population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 13.433, df = 1, N = 31, p < .001).

ISFP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found for 4.0% of

students in STEM in the research population. The index or ratio at 0.45 was less than the

expected occurrence of 8.8% found in the base population and was significant at 0.001 in

chi-square calculation (X! =11.446, df = 1, N = 17, p < .001). INFP, or Introverted Intuition

with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 7.7% of students in STEM in the research

population. The index or ratio at 1.75 indicated a greater than the expected occurrence of

4.4% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation

(X! = 10.519, df = 1, N = 33, p < .01).

ESTP, or Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to occur for

5.8% of students in STEM in the research population. The index or ratio at 2.0 was higher

than the expected occurrence of 4.3% in the base population and was found significant at

0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 18.802, df = 1, N = 37, p < .001). ENFP, or Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found for 17.0% of students in STEM and was the

106

most frequent type preference for STEM majors in the study. The index or ratio of 2.10

revealed a more than expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population, and was found to

be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 42.273 df = 1, N = 73, p < .001).

ENTP, or Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found in 6.8% of STEM

majors. The index or ratio of 2.13 revealed more than the expected occurrence of 3.2% in the

base population. and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! =

17.088, df = 1, N = 29, p < .001). ESFJ, the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Feeling

and Judging, comprised 6.0% of STEM students in the research population. The index or

ratio of 0.49 indicated fewer ESFJ preferences for STEM majors in the study than the

expected occurrence of 12.3% in the base population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 13.603, df = 1, N = 26, p < .001).

Non-STEM major type preference comparison with base sample

ISTJ, or Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was found in 6.1% of

students with non-STEM majors in the research population. The index or ratio at 0.53 was

fewer than the expected occurrence of 11.6% in the base population and was significant at

0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 9.098, df = 1, N = 21, p < .01). ISFJ, or Introverted

Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 7.2% of non-STEM students in the research

population. The index or ratio at 0.52 was fewer than the expected occurrence of 13.8% in

the base population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 10.802, df =

1, N = 25, p < .001). INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was preferred

by 3.6% of non-STEM students in the research population. The index or ratio at 3.07

indicated this preference occurred with more frequency than the 1.5% found in the base

107

population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 22.431, df = 1, N = 16,

p < .001).

ISFP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving, was found for 4.9% of

non-STEM majors in the research population. The index or ratio at 0.56 demonstrated a less

than expected occurrence of 8.8% in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-

square calculation (X! =5.907, df = 1, N = 17, p < .05). INFP, or Introverted Intuition with

Feeling and Perceiving, was found in 9.0% of non-STEM students in the research population.

The index or ratio at 2.05 indicated a greater than expected occurrence of 4.4% in the base

population and was found significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 16.424, df = 1,

N = 31, p < .001).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found for 19.1% of non-STEM students in the research population, which at 66 students was

the most frequent preference for non-STEM students in the study. This index or ratio was

2.35 times more than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found

to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 51.571, df = 1, N = 66, p < .001).

ESTJ, the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, comprised 3.8% of

non-STEM students in the research population. The index or ratio of 0.43 indicated fewer

non-STEM majors with ESTJ preferences in the sample than the expected occurrence of

8.7% in the base population and was significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! =9.715,

df = 1, N = 13, p < .01). ESFJ, the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and

Judging, comprised 7.5% of non-STEM students in the research population. The index or

ratio of 0.61 indicated fewer non-STEM students with these preferences in the sample than

the expected occurrence of 12.3% in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-

108

square calculation (X! = 6.469, df = 1, N = 26, p < .05). ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 8.7% of non-STEM population (X! = 52.148, df =

1, N = 30, p < .001). The index or ratio was 3.47 times more than the expected occurrence of

2.5% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation.

STEM and non-STEM 2004 distribution compared with base sample

Table 4.23 provides the SRTT for the 2004 cohort of 97 students by STEM or non-

STEM major in comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the

test statistic for the full type tables was significant at 0.005770 (STEM) and 0.000 (non-

STEM), indicating little occurrence of chance in the sample. Of the 16 types, 7 types were

underrepresented for STEM majors in comparison to the base population sample and 9 were

over-represented. Eight types were underrepresented and 8 were over-represented for non-

STEM majors in comparison to the base population sample. One of 16 type preferences was

found to be statistically significant for STEM majors, whereas 3 preferences for the non-

STEM majors were found significant in the 2004 cohort.

INFP, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found in 15.9% of

STEM majors in the 2004 population. The index or ratio was 3.61 times more than the

expected occurrence of 4.4% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001

in chi-square calculation (X! = 13.689, df = 1, N = 7, p < .001).

INFP was found to occur in 22.6% of non-STEM majors in the 2004 population. The

index or ratio at 5.14 indicated a greater than the expected occurrence of 4.4% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 40.133, df

109

Table 4.23. STEM and non-STEM distribution of 2004 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

= 1, N = 12, p < .001). ENFP, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur 17.0% of the 2004 non-STEM population (X! = 5.171, df = 1, N = 9, p < .05).

The index or ratio was 2.10 times more than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. ENFJ, or

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur 13.2% of the 2004 non-

STEM population (X! = 24.172, df = 1, N = 7, p < .001). The index or ratio was 5.28 times

110

more than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation.

STEM and non-STEM 2005 distribution compared with base sample The 2005 cohort is shown in Table 4.24 with the SRTT for 96 students in comparison

to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type

tables was significant at 0.000058 (STEM) and 0.00221 (non-STEM), indicating little

occurrence of chance in the sample. Of the 16 types, 9 were underrepresented and 7 were

over-represented for STEM majors in the cohort. Eight types were underrepresented for non-

STEM majors in comparison to the base population sample and 8 were over-represented.

Five of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for STEM majors in the

2005 sample, whereas 3 of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for

non-STEM majors.

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was found among 3.2% of

STEM majors in the 2005 population. The index or ratio at 0.28 was less than the expected

occurrence of 11.6% with this type in the base population and was found to be significant at

0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 3.848, df = 1, N = 2, p < .05). INFJ, or Introverted

Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found in 4.8% of the population (X! = 4.423, df = 1,

N = 3, p < .05). The index or ratio was 3.22 times more than the expected occurrence of

1.5% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation.

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 17.5% of STEM majors in the 2005 population. The index or ratio was

2.16 times more than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found

111

Table 4.24. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2005 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 6.825, df = 1, N = 11, p < .01).

ENTP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to

occur in 7.9% of STEM majors in the 2005 population. The index or ratio was 2.47 times

more than the expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.50, df = 1, N = 5, p < .05). ENFJ, the

preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving was also found 7.9% of

STEM majors in the 2005 population. The index or ratio was 3.16 times more than the

112

expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 7.225, df = 1, N = 5, p < .01).

INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found in 9.1% of non-

STEM majors in the 2005 population. The index or ratio at 6.07 was greater than the

expected occurrence of 1.5% in the base population and was significant at 0.001 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 12.5, df = 1, N = 3, p < .001). INFP, or Introverted Intuition with

Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 12.1% of non-STEM majors in the 2005

population. The index or ratio at 2.75 indicated a greater than expected occurrence of 4.4%

in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! =

4.167, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05). ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling

and Perceiving, was found to occur in 21.2% of non-STEM majors in the 2005 population.

The index or ratio was 2.62 times more than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 6.848, df =

1, N = 7, p < .01).

STEM and non-STEM 2006 distribution compared with base sample

The 2006 student cohort is depicted in Table 4.25 with the SRTT for 95 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.00007 (STEM) and 0.000003 (non-STEM), indicating

little occurrence of chance in the sample. Of the 16 types, 6 were underrepresented, 9 were

over-represented, and 1 was equal to the national sample for STEM majors. Of the 16 types,

9 were underrepresented and 7 were over-represented for non-STEM majors in the cohort.

Four of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for STEM majors in the

113

Table 4.25. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2006 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

2006 sample, whereas 2 of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for

non-STEM majors.

INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found in 6.5% of STEM

majors in the 2006 population. The index or ratio at 4.3 was greater than the occurrence of

1.5% in the base population and was significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.733,

df = 1, N = 3, p < .01). ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found

in 23.9% of STEM majors in the 2006 population. The index of 2.95 was larger than the

114

expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001

in chi-square calculation (X! = 14.403, df = 1, N = 11, p < .001). ESFJ, or Extraverted

Sensing with Feeling and Judging was not found in the 2006 STEM population. The index

or ratio at 0.0 indicated a less than expected occurrence of 12.3% in the base population and

was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.7, df = 1, N = 0, p < .05).

ENFJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to occur in 8.7% of the

2006 STEM population. The index or ratio at 3.48 indicated a higher than expected

occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-

square calculation (X! = 6.533, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05).

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 19.1% of the 2006 non-STEM population. The index or ratio was 2.36

times greater than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found to

be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 12.25, df = 1, N = 11, p < .001). ENFJ,

or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging was found to occur in 8.7% of the 2006

non-STEM population. The index or ratio was 3.48 times more than the expected occurrence

of 2.5% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square

calculation. (X! = 12.033, df = 1, N = 5, p < .001).

STEM and non-STEM 2007 distribution compared with base sample

The 2007 student cohort is illustrated with STEM and non-STEM type preference in

Table 4.26 with the SRTT for 96 students in comparison to the national base sample. The

simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.000001

(STEM) and 0.005810 (non-STEM), indicating the small occurrence of chance in the sample.

115

Table 4.26. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2007 cohort (N = 96) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

Of the 16 types, 10 were underrepresented and 6 were over-represented for STEM majors in

the cohort. Of the 16 types, 7 were underrepresented and 9 were over-represented for non-

STEM majors in the cohort. Three of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically

significant for STEM and non-STEM majors in the 2007 cohort.

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was found to occur in 2.0% of

the 2007 STEM population (X! = 4.069, df = 1, N = 1, p < .05). The index was 0.17, or less

116

than the expected occurrence of 11.6% in the base population and was found to be significant

at 0.05 in chi-square calculation. ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling

and Perceiving, was found to occur in 29.4% of the 2007 STEM population. With an index

that was 3.63 times greater than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population,

ENFP was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X! = 28.978 df = 1, N =

15, p < .001). ENTP, or Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found to

occur in 9.8% of the 2007 STEM cohort. The index or ratio was 3.06 times more than the

expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 7.225, df = 1, N = 5, p < .01).

INFJ, or preference for Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found to

occur in 6.7% of non-STEM majors in the 2007 population. The index or ratio was 4.47

times greater than the expected occurrence of 1.5% in the base population and was found to

be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.915, df = 1, N = 3, p < .01). ENFP, the

preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in

17.8% of the 2007 non-STEM population. With an index that was 2.20 times greater than

the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population, ENFP was found to be significant at

0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.378, df = 1, N = 8, p < .05). ENFJ, or Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Judging, was found in 8.9% of 2007 non-STEM majors. The

index or ratio was 3.56 times greater than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base

population and was found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.645, df =

1, N = 4, p < .01).

117

STEM and non-STEM 2008 distribution compared with base sample

The 2008 student cohort is shown with STEM and non-STEM type preference in

Table 4.27 with the SRTT for 99 students in comparison to the national base sample. The

simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.000013

(STEM) and 0.005810 (non-STEM), indicating the small occurrence of chance in either

sample. Of the 16 types, 7 were underrepresented and 9 were over-represented for STEM

majors in the cohort. For non-STEM majors in the cohort, 10 types were underrepresented

and 6 were over-represented. Five of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically

significant for STEM majors in the 2008 sample, whereas 4 of 16 type preferences were

found to be statistically significant for non-STEM majors.

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in the 2008 STEM

population (X! = 5.437, df = 1, N = 1, p < .05). The index was 0.14, or less than the expected

occurrence of 13.8% in the base population, and was found significant at 0.05 in chi-square

calculation. ISFP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving, was not found among

the STEM majors in the 2008 population. The index or ratio at 0.0 was lower than the

expected occurrence of 8.8% in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 4.7, df = 1, N = 0, p < .05). ESTP, or preference for Extraverted Sensing

with Thinking and Perceiving, was found in 17.0% of the population (X! = 19.517, df = 1, N

= 9, p < .001). The SRTT ratio was 3.95 times more than the expected occurrence of 4.3% in

the base population, and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation.

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to be

18.9% of the 2008 STEM population. The index or ratio was 2.33 times greater than the

expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population, and was found significant at 0.01 in chi-

118

Table 4.27. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2008 Cohort (N = 99) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

square calculation (X! = 7.556, df = 1, N = 10, p < .01). Extraverted Intuition with Thinking

and Judging, or ENTJ, was found for 5.7% of the 2008 STEM population (X! = 4.424, df = 1,

N = 3, p < .05). The index or ratio was 3.17 times more than the expected occurrence of

1.8% in the base population, and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation.

ISFJ, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 2.2% of non-

STEM majors in the 2008 population. The index or ratio at 0.16 was lower than the expected

119

occurrence of 13.8% in the base population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square

calculation (X! = 6.3, df = 1, N = 1, p < .05). INFJ, or Introverted Intuition with Feeling and

Judging, was found in 6.5% of non-STEM majors in the 2008 population. The index or ratio

at 4.33 was greater than the expected occurrence of 1.5% in the base population and was

significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.733, df = 1, N = 3, p < .01). ENFP, or

Extroverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to occur in 23.9% of non-

STEM majors in the 2008 population. The index or ratio at 2.95 indicated a greater than the

expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001

in chi-square calculation (X! = 14.403, df = 1, N = 11, p < .001). Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Judging, or ENFJ, was found to occur 8.7% of the 2008 non-STEM population

(X! = 6.533, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05). The index or ratio was 3.48 times more than the expected

occurrence of 2.5% in the base non-STEM population, and was found to be significant at

0.05 in chi-square calculation.

STEM and non-STEM 2009 distribution compared with base sample

The 2009 student cohort is exhibited by STEM and non-STEM type preference in

Table 4.28 with the SRTT for 100 students in comparison to the national base sample. The

simulated distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.1707 for

STEM majors and 0.012630 for non-STEM majors. Of the 16 types, 9 were

underrepresented and 9 were over-represented for STEM majors in the cohort. For non-

STEM majors in the cohort, 8 types were underrepresented and 8 were over-represented.

One of 16 type preferences was found to be statistically significant for STEM majors in the

120

Table 4.28. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2009 cohort (N = 100) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

2009 sample, whereas 4 of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for

non-STEM majors.

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 18.5% of the 2009 STEM population. The index or ratio was 2.28 times

greater than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population, and was found to be

significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.127, df = 1, N = 10, p < .01).

121

ISTP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was found in 13.0% of non-

STEM majors in the 2009 population. The index or ratio at .42 revealed a percentage less

than the expected occurrence of 5.4% in the base population, and was significant at 0.05 in

chi-square calculation (X! = 4.90, df = 1, N = 6, p < .05). ISFP, or Introverted Sensing with

Feeling and Perceiving, was not found for non-STEM majors in the 2009 population. The

index or ratio at 0.0 revealed this percentage was less than the expected occurrence of 8.8%

in the base population, and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.0, df = 1,

N = 0, p < .05). ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving,

was found to occur in 19.6% of the 2009 STEM population. The index or ratio was 2.42

times greater than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population, and was found to

be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation (X! = 7.592, df = 1, N = 9, p < .01).

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, or ENFJ, was found to occur 8.7% of the

2009 non-STEM population (X! = 6.533, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05). The index or ratio was 3.48,

or greater than the expected occurrence of 2.5% in the base population and was found to be

significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation.

STEM and non-STEM 2010 distribution compared with base sample

The 2010 student cohort by STEM and non-STEM type preference in Table 4.29

reveals the SRTT for 97 students in comparison to the national base sample. The simulated

distribution of the test statistic for the full type table was significant at 0.004880 for STEM

majors, indicating a small occurrence of chance in the sample. The simulated distribution of

the test statistic for the full type table was 0.6997 for non-STEM majors. Of the 16 types, 7

were underrepresented and 9 were over-represented for STEM majors in the cohort. For

122

Table 4.29. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2010 cohort (N = 97) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

non-STEM majors in the cohort, 10 types were underrepresented and 6 were over-

represented. Three of 16 type preferences were found to be statistically significant for STEM

majors in the 2010 sample.

ISTJ, or Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was the preferred type in

1.6% of students with STEM majors in the 2010 cohort. The index or ratio at 0.14 was less

than the expected occurrence of 11.6% of individuals with this type in the base population.

The index or ratio was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 5.260, df

123

= 1, N = 1, p < .05). ISTP, or Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was found

in 14.5% of students in STEM majors in the 2005 group. The index or ratio at 2.69 was

greater than the expected occurrence of 5.4% in the base population and is significant at 0.01

in chi-square calculation (X! = 9.529, df = 1, N = 9, p < .01). ENTP, the preference for

Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was found in 11.3% of the 2010 STEM

population. The index or ratio was 3.53 times greater than the expected occurrence of 3.2%

in the base population, and was found to be significant at 0.001 in chi-square calculation (X!

= 12.727, df = 1, N = 7, p < .001).

STEM and non-STEM 2011 distribution compared with base sample

The 2011 student cohort shown in Table 4.30 provides the SRTT for 95 students in

comparison to the national base sample. The simulated distribution of the test statistic for the

full type table was significant at 0.000052 for STEM majors and 0.001840 for non-STEM

majors, indicating a small occurrence of chance in either sample. Of the 16 types, 8 were

underrepresented, 7 were over-represented, and 1 was equal to the national sample for STEM

majors in the cohort. For non-STEM majors in the cohort, 9 types were underrepresented

and 7 were over-represented in comparison to the national sample. Two of 16 type

preferences were found statistically significant for STEM majors in the 2011 sample,

whereas 5 of 16 type preferences were found statistically significant for non-STEM majors.

ISFP, or Introverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving, was not found in STEM

majors in the 2011 cohort. This was less than the expected occurrence of 8.8% in the base

population and was significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.9, df = 1, N = 0, p <

.05). ESTP, the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, was found

124

Table 4.30. STEM/non-STEM distribution of 2011 cohort (N = 95) and SRTT comparison with population norms from the 1998 MBTI manual

in 19.6 % of the 2011 STEM population. The index or ratio was 4.56 times greater than the

expected occurrence of 4.3% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.001

in chi-square calculation (X! = 30.817, df = 1, N = 11, p < .001).

ISTJ, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was not found for students with

non-STEM majors in the 2011 population. The index or ratio at 0.0 was less than the

expected rate of 11.6% of individuals with this type in the base population. The index or

125

ratio was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.5, df = 1, N = 0, p <

.001). Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, or INFJ, was found to occur in 7.7%

of the 2011 non-STEM population (X! = 9.844, df = 1, N = 3, p < .01). The index or ratio

was 5.13 times more than the expected occurrence of 1.5% in the base population and was

found to be significant at 0.01 in chi-square calculation. Introverted Intuition with Thinking

and Judging, or INTJ, was also found to occur 7.7% of the 2011 non-STEM population (X! =

5.796, df = 1, N = 3, p < .05). The index or ratio was 3.67 times more than the expected

occurrence of 2.1% in the base population and was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-

square calculation. ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and

Perceiving, was found to occur in 17.9% of the 2011 non-STEM population. The index of

2.21 demonstrated a greater than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base population and

was found to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 4.513, df = 1, N = 7, p <

.05). ENTP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was

found to occur in 10.3% of the 2011 non-STEM population. The index of 3.22 demonstrated

a more frequent than the expected occurrence of 3.2% in the base population and was found

to be significant at 0.05 in chi-square calculation (X! = 6.533, df = 1, N = 4, p < .05).

Null hypothesis 3 was rejected because there were statistically significant differences

in the distribution of type preferences among STEM majors in each cohort and among seven

of eight cohorts for non-STEM majors and for the full research sample. Although the test

statistic distribution simulation conducted by cohort year found the 2009 STEM and 2010

non-STEM cohort group type preferences not significantly different from the base sample

type population, only the 2010 non-STEM cohort had no type preferences significantly

different from the base sample.

126

ENFP, or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was the most frequent

type preference for the research population, with STEM majors at 17.0% (N = 73) and non-

STEM majors at 19.1% (N = 66). The least frequent preference for STEM students was

ENTJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, at 1.86% of the population (N =

8). The least frequent preference for non-STEM students was INTJ, or Introverted Intuition

with Thinking and Judging, at 1.16% (N = 4). Considering the full research sample and eight

cohort years, only two type preference for STEM majors were found to be equal to the base

national sample for the preference, ESTP in the 2006 cohort year, and ENTJ in the 2011

cohort. Research Question 4 revealed significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for

STEM and non-STEM students in the study by cohort year and for the research population in

comparison to the distribution of a national population.

Differences in academic aptitude of ACT and high school percentile rank and Myers-

Briggs preference by cohort year and for research sample

Research Question 5: Are there statistically significant differences in the Academic Aptitude as measured by ACT and high school percentile rank in graduating class and Myers-Briggs preference by each cohort year or for the research population?

H04: There is no significant difference in ACT and high school percentile

rank and Myers-Briggs preference by cohort year or for the research

population.

To answer Research Question 5, cross-tabulations, one-way analysis of variance

(ANOVA), and Tukey-Kramer HSD post hoc tests were computed using the research sample

of 775 students and eight cohort years to determine the distribution of Myers-Briggs type to

the ACT composite and high school graduating class percentile rank of students. Table 4.31

1

Table 4.31. Cross tabulation means and standard deviations comparing ACT composite by MBTI for population, 2004-2011 ! ! "##$! ! ! "##%! ! ! "##&! ! ! "##'! ! ! "##(! ! ! "##)! ! ! "#*#! ! ! "#**! ! +,-,./01!2.345,!

6789! !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%"

:;<=! *#! "%>$#! ?>?$! !!'! "?>$?! ?>"*! !!)! "&>%&! $>&*! !!'! "$>$"! ">&$! !!'! "$>")! ?>#)! !!!!%! "$>&#! ">)'! !!$! "'>"%! ?>''! !!?! "?>??! ?>#&! !!%"! "%>##! ?>$$!

:;<@! *%! "?>$#! ">?%! *(! "?>()! ?>&(! ""! "?>?&! ?>*$! "?! "$>$?! ?>*'! "*! "$>&'! ?>&(! !!*)! "?>()! ?>)#! !!(! "?>?(! ?>'#! *"! "$>?#! ?>'#! *?)! "?>)'! ?>?&!

:;8=! !!"! "*>%#! ">*"! !!"! "%>%#! &>?&! !!*! "&>##! ! !!"! ")>%#! #>'*! !!$! "&>%#! ">&%! ! ! ! !!"! "#>##! ">(?! !!*! ?">##! ! !!*$! "%>%#! $>?%!

:;8@! !!$! ")>##! *>$*! !!'! "'>")! $>)"! !!&! "%>??! $>&(! !!'! "&>")! $>&$! !!?! "#>&'! ?>#&! !!!!?! ?#>?#! #>%(! !!(! "?>"%! ">('! !!'! "$>'*! $>"?! !!$%! "%>%*! $>?%!

:2<=! !!'! "?>'*! ">'%! *?! "">&)! $>#'! !!$! "$>"%! ?>?#! !!%! "%>"#! %>&'! !!'! "">##! ?>$&! !!!!%! "&>&#! $>"(! !!'! "?>*$! ?>$$! !!%! "*>'%! ?>(&! !!%"! "?>*'! ?>'%!

:2<@! !!$! "*>##! ">&%! !!(! "*>'%! $>*?! *"! "*>'%! ?>''! !!)! "$>##! ?>&$! !!'! "$>$?! ?>))! !!!!)! "">%&! ?>"*! !!'! "*>(&! ?>*(! *"! "">&'! $>"?! !!&(! "">&#! ?>&'!

:2<=! !!$! "&>'%! ">""! !!$! "%>%#! ?>('! !!?! "&>##! ?>&*! !!?! "%>??! ">()! !!$! "%>%#! ?>('! !!*#! "%>*#! ?>%*! !!)! "$>'(! ">$)! !!(! "$>*?! $>((! !!$%! "%>*&! ?>?&!

:28@! !!$! "%>%#! %>"#! !!?! "?>??! *>%?! !!%! "?>$#! ?>&%! !!'! "&>%'! *>%*! *"! "?>(?! ">#$! !!!!'! "?>$?! ?>*#! !!&! "%>*'! ">?*! *$! "?>%#! ?>?*! !!%(! "$>""! ">(*!

9;<=! !!*! "">##! ! !!&! "$>*'! ?>'&! !!$! "?>##! ">$%! !!%! "%>##! ?>(*! !!?! "">&'! *>%?! !!!!*! "#>##! ! !!"! "%>%#! &>?&! !!?! "">##! &>##! !!"%! "?>%&! ?>%)!

9;<@! *)! "%>**! $>%(! *#! "$>(#! ?>)$! !!)! "$>##! $>##! !!)! "$>%&! ?>*'! !!%! "%>(#! *>&$! !!!!$! "$>%#! ?>('! !!%! "&>##! ?>&'! !!?! "$>&'! $>%*! !!&$! "$>()! ?>''!

9;8=! !!*! "(>##! ! !!?! "&>##! *>##! !!"! "$>%#! #>'*! !!"! *)>##! *>$*! !!"! "'>##! $>#(! !!!!*! "&>##! ! !!"! "$>%#! ?>%?! !!$! "%>"%! ">'%! !!*'! "$>((! ">)(!

9;8@! !!"! "?>%#! #>'*! !!"! ?*>##! *>$*! !!?! ")>##! ?>##! !!"! "*>##! $>"$! !!$! "'>##! $>#(! !!!!$! "(>%#! ?>('! !!$! "&>%#! &>"%! !!?! "'>##! *>'?! !!"$! "&>)&! $>"*!

92<=! !!$! "?>"#! ">&(! !!&! "*>&'! $>#(! !!&! "?>*'! ">(&! !!'! "%>##! ?>('! !!"! "(>%#! ">*"! !!*"! "$>##! $>##! **! "">##! ">*)! !!'! "?>%'! %>%#! !!%&! "?>$?! ?>'$!

92<@! !!&! "">(?! ?>$?! !!$! "">##! *>$*! !!"! "&>##! %>&&! !!$! "(>'%! *>%#! !!&! "?>??! *>"*! !!!!?! "%>##! *>##! !!'! "%>'*! ?>"#! !!"! "?>%#! ?>%$! !!?$! "$>%?! ?>*%!

928=! !!&! "%>??! ?>((! !!?! "">??! ">?*! !!&! "">(?! ">*$! !!?! "&>??! ">#(! !!(! "$>"%! ">)"! !!!!&! "$>&'! ?>??! !!&! "&>%#! $>#$! !!%! "*>(#! *>&$! !!$?! "$>?#! ?>"#!

928@! !!'! "%>*$! ?>$$! ! ! ! !!*! "&>##! ! !!*! "">##! ! !!$! "'>##! *>&?! !!**! "%>("! ?>""! !!)! "?>**! ?>#"! !!&! "%>%#! $>%)! !!?)! "%>#%! ?>?&!

!8AB.5! )'! "$>$&! ">)*! )&! "">(?! ?>**! )%! "$>'#! ">)'! )&! "$>''! ">(*! ))! "$>($! ">'$! *##! "?>$$! ">'$! )'! "$>")! ?>&"! )%! "$>?&! ?>%"! ''%! "$>%%! ?>%'!

127

128

exhibits the cross-tabulation Myers-Briggs preference and number of students with those

preferences, the mean composite ACT for students with the preference, and the standard

deviation for each of the cohort groups of students for the years 2004 to 2011. The mean

ACT for the research sample was 24.55 with a standard deviation of 3.57 and interquartile

range of 0.64. Of the eight cohort groups of students, the ACT composite was highest for the

2008 cohort at 24.84.

Table 4.32 provides a summary of the one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)

results which reveals that the full research population ACT composite comparison to Myers-

Briggs type had a significant difference score when measured for each of the eight cohort

years and research sample, F (15, 759) = 3.722, p < .001) at the p = .05 level. The one-way

ANOVA results indicated that there was not a significant difference or change for the ACT

composite score earned by students in each of the eight individual cohort years in comparison

to MBTI, possibly due to sample size.

Tukey-Kramer HSD post hoc tests were computed to determine if a significant

difference existed for the ACT Composite for students in comparison to Myers-Briggs

preference by cohort year or for the research population. The results shown in Table 4.33

indicate nine instances in which significance did appear between the means of ACT and

Myers-Briggs preference for students in the full research population. No significance for

ACT and type were found by individual cohort year with Tukey. Table 4.33 reveals that the

mean difference between INTP and ESFP within the research population was 4.36, a

significant value, as indicated with a p value < .001, at the p = .05 level. Recall that Table

4.31 revealed the mean ACT composite for the research population was 24.55; for INTP,

129

Table 4.32. One-way ANOVA summary comparing the ACT composite by MBTI for research population and cohort years 2004-2011

Source df SS MS F p

Population MBTI 15 699.516 46.634 3.722 <.0001* Within groups 759 9510.618 12.531 Total 774 10210.134

2004 MBTI 15 249.910 16.661 1.411 0.162 Within groups 81 956.130 11.804 Total 96 1206.040

2005 MBTI 14 328.370 23.455 1.638 0.086 Within groups 81 1159.790 14.318 Total 95 1488.156

2006 MBTI 15 260.945 17.396 1.356 0.192 Within groups 79 1015.010 12.848 Total 94 1275.958

2007 MBTI 15 252.843 16.856 1.462 0.140 Within groups 80 922.115 11.526 Total 95 1174.958

2008 MBTI 15 235.784 15.719 1.675 0.072 Within groups 83 778.943 9.385 Total 98 1014.727

2009 MBTI 14 267.845 19.132 1.533 0.117 Within groups 85 1060.662 12.478 Total 99 1328.510

2010 MBTI 15 291.295 19.420 1.695 0.068 Within groups 81 928.046 11.457 Total 96 1219.340

2011 MBTI 15 198.359 13.224 0.865 0.605 Within groups 79 1208.126 15.293 Total 94 1406.484

* p < .05

130

Table 4.33. Summary of post hoc Tukey-Kramer HSD comparing research population ACT composite to the MBTI

Type for Population Mean Difference Std. Error p

INTP to ESFP 4.355 0.840 < .001*

ESFJ 3.785 0.874 0.0018*

ISFJ 3.530 0.864 0.0049*

ENFP 2.987 0.782 0.0137*

ENTP to ESFP 2.908 0.680 0.0023* ESTJ to ESFP 2.553 0.680 0.0174* ISTP to ESFP 2.448 0.711 0.0491* ENFJ to ESFP 2.397 0.652 0.0227* INFP to ESFP 2.288 0.616 0.0201*

*p < .05

Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, mean ACT was 26.96; a difference of

4.355 from ESFP, Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving, with a mean of 22.60.

The difference for INTP to ESFJ, Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Judging, was

from 26.96 to 23.17 (ESFJ). The difference for INTP to ISFJ, Introverted Sensing with

Feeling and Judging, was 26.96 to 23.43 (ISFJ). The difference for INTP to ENFP,

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was 26.96 to 23.97 (ENFP).

ENTP, Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, with mean ACT of 25.50,

had a difference of 2.908 from ESFP, Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving, with

a mean of 22.60. ESTJ, Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, with mean ACT of

25.16, had a difference of 2.553 from ESFP. ISTP, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and

Perceiving, with a mean ACT of 25.05, showed a difference of 2.448 from ESFP. ENFJ,

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, with a mean ACT of 25.00; had a difference

of 2.397 from ESFP, with a mean of 22.60. INFP, Introverted Intuition with Feeling and

131

Perceiving, and a mean ACT of 24.89; had a difference of 2.288 from the ESFP mean of

22.60.

Table 4.34 illustrates the cross-tabulation Myers-Briggs preference and number of

students with those preferences, the mean composite high school percentilerank for students

with the preference, and the standard deviation for each of the cohort groups of students for

the years 2004 to 2011. The mean percentile rank for the research sample was 82.24 with a

standard deviation of 12.82 and interquartile range of 4.43. Of the eight cohort groups of

students, the percentile rank was highest for the 2006 cohort at 85.29.

Table 4.35 provides a summary of the one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) results

reporting that the full research population percentile class rank comparison to Myers-Briggs

type had a significant difference score when measured for each of the eight cohort years and

research sample, F (15, 759) = 2.522, p = .0012) at the p = .05 level. The one-way ANOVA

results demonstrate that there was no significant difference for the percentile class rank

earned by students in each of the eight individual cohort years in comparison to MBTI,

potentially due to sample size. Tukey-Kramer HSD post hoc tests computed to determine if a

difference existed for the percentile rank for students in comparison to Myers-Briggs

preference by cohort year or for research population produced no significant results.

Hypothesis 4 was rejected. As shown in Tables 4.32 and 4.33, there was significant

difference in ACT composite score. As indicated in Table 4.35, there was a significant

mean

ACT composite for the research population was 24.31, and nine Myers-Briggs preference

groups in the research population had a mean ACT above that score, it is not surprising that

128

Table 4.34. Cross tabulation of means and standard deviations comparing percentile rank by MBTI for the population, 2004-2011 ! ! "##$! ! ! "##%! ! ! "##&! ! ! "##'! ! ! "##(! ! ! "##)! ! ! "#*#! ! ! "#**! ! +,-,./01!2.345,!

6789! !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%"

:;<=! *#! ($>)#! *#>"#! !!'! (&>")! *#>*&! !!)! )#>##! !!(>**! !!'! (%>%'! **>?)! !!'! ')>(&! *&>?&! !!%! (*>$#! *%>#(! !!$! ('>"%! **>%?! !!?! ')>??! !!'>&$! !!%"! ($>)#! **>")!

:;<@! *%! &(>(*! "#>"#! *(! ''>*'! *$>(?! ""! '(>##! *%>*"! "?! '(>*'! *%>"(! "*! (*>??! *?>&(! !!*)! '(>#%! **>(?! !!(! (?>&?! **>"%! *"! (#>&'! *?>"&! *?)! ''>)?! *$>)$!

:;8=! !!"! '">##! !!)>)#! !!*! '%>##! "*>"*! !!*! )&>##! ! !!"! ()>%#! !!$>)%! !!$! )*>"%! !!?>%)! ! ! ! !!"! '(>%#! *#>&*! !!'! )*>##! ! !!*$! ($>$?! **>$"!

:;8@! !!$! )?>'%! !!&>&%! !!'! (*>")! *">?(! !!&! ($>(?! *#>%#! !!'! )">")! !!'>"#! !!?! '*>??! "">*)! !!!!?! (?>##! *">%?! !!(! '%>?(! **>"(! !!%! (*>##! *&>$#! !!$%! (">)?! *?>"%!

:2<=! !!'! (%>")! **>")! *?! (#>"?! *#>)&! !!$! (%>%#! !!)>$'! !!%! '(>##! *&>((! !!'! (">*$! *#>'%! !!!!%! (%>"#! !!%>)'! !!'! ((>##! **>"#! !!'! (">"%! !!)>**! !!%"! (?>#$! *#>(#!

:2<@! !!$! (?>&'! !!)>#'! !!(! '$>?(! *#>#(! *"! '&>*'! *$>*"! !!)! '?>()! *%>"$! !!'! '%>")! *&>$#! !!!!)! (#>""! *&>)"! !!'! (">%'! *(>$*! *"! ''>##! *%>%&! !!&(! ''>"%! *$>&%!

:2<=! !!$! ()>##! !!'>('! !!$! (">%#! *'>$*! !!?! )%>##! !!?>&*! !!?! (%>&'! !!'>#)! !!$! ()>'%! !!)>)*! !!*#! ''>)#! *&>*#! !!)! (?>$$! *">"#! !!(! (%>&?! *#>%*! !!$%! ($>$)! *">$$!

:28@! !!$! (%>"%! !!(>'?! !!?! ''>??! *(>?$! !!%! '(>&#! *#>'*! !!'! ('>$?! !!)>"'! *"! '(>$"! *">(*! !!!!'! '#>(&! *&>(*! !!&! (*>&'! *?>#?! *$! (?>?&! *&>(?! !!%(! ')>(?! *?>)$!

9;<=! !!*! )">##! ! !!&! ')>%#! !!(>%'! !!$! (%>"%! !!)>%$! !!%! ('>##! !!'>#'! !!?! '?>??! *&>&"! !!!!*! ''>##! ! !!"! ''>%#! ?#>$*! !!?! (?>##! **>*$! !!"%! (*>($! **>$(!

9;<@! *)! (?>')! *%>)'! *#! (?>?#! *%>&(! !!)! (#>()! *?>?'! !!)! ($>**! *?>?%! !!%! '$>$#! *)>*#! !!!!$! '&>'%! *#>(*! !!%! (">(#! *$>#&! !!?! ($>(?! *$>?&! !!&$! (">*?! *$>$(!

9;8=! !!*! ))>##! ! !!?! )&>##! !!">##! !!"! )*>##! !!*>$*! !!"! &?>##! *&>)'! !!"! )$>##! !!">(?! !!!!*! )'>##! ! !!"! )$>%#! !!$>)%! !!$! ''>%#! !!)>$'! !!*'! ('>##! *?>??!

9;8@! !!"! &)>##! !!'>#'! !!"! )$>%#! !!&>?&! !!?! )*>??! !!%>(&! !!"! '#>%#! !!?>%$! !!$! ($>'%! **>?"! !!!!$! )">%#! !!$>(#! !!$! (">%#! *?>*?! !!?! (*>?'! !!$>)?! !!"$! ($>$"! *#>(?!

92<=! !!%! (?>&#! *#>"$! !!&! (#>*'! **>"?! !!&! '(>&'! **>$*! !!'! (*>'*! *?>*$! !!"! )">%#! !!">*"! !!*"! ('>##! !!)>&)! **! (?>$%! !!'>$)! !!'! (*>%'! *#>)'! !!%&! (?>"?! *#>*#!

92<@! !!&! '&>%#! *?>&*! !!$! '*>'%! !!%>%#! !!"! ($>##! *%>%&! !!$! (*>##! *'>"#! !!&! '">(?! *&>*$! !!!!?! )">&'! !!&>**! !!'! '$>")! *(>$"! !!"! &'>%#! *$>(%! !!?$! '&>'&! *$>&&!

928=! !!&! (%>##! *$>##! !!?! )?>&'! !!">#(! !!&! (#>??! *">(#! !!?! )*>??! !!'>"?! !!(! ($>&?! *?>?$! !!!!&! (%>??! !!(>#'! !!&! (&>&'! *$>&%! !!%! (#>##! *">*'! !!$?! (%>#"! **>'"!

928@! !!'! '">%'! *&>'&! ! ! ! !!*! ()>##! ! !!*! (%>##! ! !!$! '">##! ?#>%$! !!**! ()>"'! **>*$! !!)! (?>%&! **>&(! !!&! '?>(?! **>("! !!?)! (#>&)! *%>($!

!8AB.5! )'! (">'&! *#>*#! )&! (">"*! **>*"! )%! (%>")! (>(&! )&! (">*$! *#>?&! ))! (*>**! *?>&*! *##! (?>&*! **>""! )'! (">(&! *?>?)! )%! (#>$(! **>*)! ''%! (">"$! *">("!

132

133

Table 4.35. One-way ANOVA summary for each year, 2004-2011, and research sample for percentile rank compared to the MBTI

Source df SS MS F p

Population MBTI 15 6722.14 448.142 2.522 .0012* Within groups 759 134871.80 177.697 Total 774 141593.94

2004 MBTI 15 5440.980 362.732 1.699 0.0672 Within groups 81 17284.505 213.389 Total 96 22725.485

2005 MBTI 14 2866.277 204.734 1.2934 0.230 Within groups 81 12821.557 158.291 Total 95 15687.833

2006 MBTI 15 2810.054 187.337 1.202 0.289 Within groups 79 12317.672 155.920 Total 94 15127.726

2007 MBTI 15 3622.799 241.520 1.398 0.169 Within groups 80 13817.701 172.721 Total 95 17440.500

2008 MBTI 15 3175.605 211.707 0.948 0.516 Within groups 83 18532.718 223.286 Total 98 21708.323

2009 MBTI 14 3500.398 250.028 1.598 0.096 Within groups 85 13298.192 156.449 Total 99 16798.590

2010 MBTI 15 1696.724 113.115 0.6484 0.826 Within groups 81 14130.781 174.454 Total 96 15827.505

2011 MBTI 15 1151.931 76.795 0.419 0.969 Within groups 79 14486.554 183.374 Total 94 15638.484

* p < .05

134

significance in scores were found within the study. Study results revealed in Table 4.33

indicate nine instances in which significance did appear between the means of ACT and

Myers-Briggs preference for students in the full research population. Additionally, as the

mean percentile rank for the research sample was 82.24 with a standard deviation of 12.82, a

difference in percentile class rank measurements for the population was not unexpected.

Significance was found for each pre-college academic aptitude variable with

comparison within the full research population. Although no significance for ACT and

percentile rank and type preference were found by individual cohort year, the results prompt

consideration of these results for first-year college students. There may be need for an

additional understanding and reflection upon learning style related to type and where

opportunities for improving academic strengths may be focused. These data are important

for college administrators, specifically in terms of orientation and preparation for the first

college year.

Differences in first-semester grade point and Myers-Briggs preference by cohort year

and for research sample

Research Question 6: Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preference for student grade point in the first college semester by each cohort year and across groups? Are there statistically significant differences in Myers-Briggs preference for students who are able to achieve a 2.00 grade point in in the first college semester by each cohort year and across groups?

H05: There is no difference in Myers-Briggs preference for students in the

study by grade point in the first college semester in comparison by

each cohort year and for the research population.

135

H06: There is no difference in Myers-Briggs preference for students who are

able to achieve a 2.00 grade point in the first college semester in

comparison by cohort year and for the research population.

For a comparison of Myers-Briggs preference with first semester grade point, cross-

tabulations, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), and Tukey-Kramer HSD post hoc tests

were computed using the full research sample of 775 students and eight cohort years to

determine the distribution of Myers-Briggs type to the first-semester grade point of students

in the study. Table 4.36 provides a cross-tabulation of Myers-Briggs preference and number

of students, with the preference, mean first-semester grade point for students with the

preference, and the standard deviation for each of the cohort groups of students for the years

2004 to 2011 and the full research sample. The mean first-semester grade point for the

research sample was 2.94 with a standard deviation of 0.76 and interquartile range of 0.32.

Of the eight cohort groups of students, the first-semester grade point was highest for the 2004

cohort at 3.09 and lowest for the 2007 cohort at 2.80.

Table 4.37 provides a summary of one-way ANOVA results which reveal that the full

research population grade point comparison to the Myers-Briggs preference had significant

difference when measured, F (15, 759) = 3.230, p < .0001) at the p = .05 level. Significance

was also found for the 2004 cohort, F (15, 81) = 2.184, p = 0.0134. There was no significant

difference found for first semester grade point earned by students in the 2005-2011

individual cohort years in comparison to Myers-Briggs.

Tukey-Kramer HSD post hoc tests were computed to determine if significant

difference existed for the first semester grade point for students in comparison to Myers-

128

Table 4.36. Cross tabulation of means and standard deviations comparing first semester grade point by MBTI for the population, 2004-2011

! ! "##$! ! ! "##%! ! ! "##&! ! ! "##'! ! ! "##(! ! ! "##)! ! ! "#*#! ! ! "#**! ! +,-,./01!2.345,!

6789! !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%" !" #" $%"

:;<=! *#! >?%#! #?">! !!'! >?*(! #?&"! !!)! >?>#! #?>'! !!'! >?*"! #?&%! !!'! >?#'! #?'"! !!%! "?')! #?')! !!$! >?*#! *?##! !!>! "?)'! #?>(! !!%"! >?*(! #?%)!

:;<@! *%! "?%#! #?(&! *(! "?>$! #?()! ""! "?&#! #?(*! ">! "?&'! #?(&! "*! "?&"! *?*#! *)! >?(*! #?''! !!(! "?%)! *?"%! *"! "?)*! #?(#! *>)! "?&*! #?)"!

:;8=! !!"! "?("! #?&>! !!"! "?)"! *?%#! !!*! >?"'! ! !!"!! >?&)! #?>%! !!$! >?&%! #?"(! ! ! ! !!"! >?%#! #?"$! !!*! $?##! ! !!*$! >?$*! #?&"!

:;8@! !!$! >?#)! #?$%! !!'! "?)>! #?'$! !!&! >?*&! #?%)! !!'! >?#(! *?*>! !!>! "?*#! #?("! !!>! >?*)! #?$&! !!(! "?%#! #?&)! !!'! "?((! #?&"! !!$%! "?('! #?'%!

:2<=! !!'! >?")! #?'"! *>! "?(*! #?&$! !!$! >?*#! #?&"! !!%! "?)>! #?((! !!'! "?>"! *?"%! !!%! "?('! #?&(! !!'! "?>%! *?>'! !!%! >?*%! #?%>! !!%"! "?(*! #?)#!

:2<@! !!$! "?&*! #?&*! !!(! "?'$! #?")! *"! "?)"! #?(*! !!)! "?'(! #?&)! !!'! "?>"! *?*>! !!)! "?)#! #?($! !!'! "?$#! #?'$! *"! "?(%! #?(*! !!&(! "?'>! #?''!

:2<=! !!$! >?"#! #?$)! !!$! >?"(! #?'(! !!>! >?%>! #?>#! !!>! *?)$! *?##! !!$! >?%%! #?%#! *#! >?*#! #?%$! !!)! >?**! #?>%! !!(! "?&"! #?&(! !!$%! >?#>! #?&'!

:28@! !!$! "?('! #?*'! !!>! >?"%! #?'>! !!%! >?#%! #?"(! !!'! >?*&! #?'$! *"! "?'%! #?%%! !!'! "?%'! #?&#! !!&! "?&&! #?')! *$! "?')! #?)>! !!%(! "?'&! #?''!

9;<=! !!*! >?(%! ! !!&! >?>#! #?$&! !!$! "?&%! #?'>! !!%! >?$(! #?*'! !!>! >?*'! #?%)! !!*! >?%)! ! !!"! >?&>! #?%>! !!>! >?$&! #?>*! !!"%! >?"&! #?%$!

9;<@! *)! >?*#! #?%'! *#! "?'"! *?#)! !!)! "?)#! #?$&! !!)! "?('! #?'$! !!%! "?&*! #?$$! !!$! >?"%! #?&#! !!%! "?"(! #?)*! !!>! >?*>! #?)*! !!&$! "?()! #?'>!

9;8=! !!*! $?##! ! !!>! >?%)! #?*>! !!"! "?'*! #?">! !!"! *?#$! *?$&! !!"! >?>#! #?)$! !!*! >?)>! ! !!"! "?&*! *?&(! !!$! >?*>! *?*'! !!*'! "?)'! *?*&!

9;8@! !!"! "?'#! #?(*! !!"! >?&"! #?>#! !!>! "?>)! *?%#! !!"! "?""! #?#"! !!$! "A#"! #?$&! !!$! >?%>! #?>&! !!$! "?'$! *?#)! !!>! "?("! #?$#! !!"$! "?()! #?(#!

92<=! !!"! "?'$! #?"(! !!&! "?&&! #?$*! !!&! >?*)! #?&)! !!'! "?("! *?*>! !!"! >?(*! #?"(! *"! "?($! #?&%! **! >?""! #?%&! !!'! "?($! #?'$! !!%&! "?)%! #?&)!

92<@! !!%! "?&(! #?'#! !!$! "?&>! #?>(! !!"! >?*"! #?%$! !!$! "?("! *?$'! !!&! "?>#! #?(>! !!>! >?%>! #?$*! !!'! "?''! #?&%! !!"! >?#(! #?"%! !!>$! "?''! #?''!

928=! !!&! >?%#! #?$'! !!>! "?*&! #?&'! !!&! "?)>! #?&*! !!>! >?(#! #?"*! !!(! "?)'! #?'>! !!&! "?)$! #?%*! !!&! >?"#! #?(*! !!%! "A&$! #?$'! !!$>! >?*#! #?&$!

928@! !!'! "?)'! #?'*! ! ! ! !!*! >?&"! ! !!*! "?>>! ! !!$! "?*'! *?#(! **! "?)&! *?#'! !!)! "?'"! #?'$! !!&! "?>$! #?)*! !!>)! "?'>! #?)#!

!8BC.5! )'! >?#)! #?$(! )&! >?#*! #?&$! )%! >?#>! #?%>! )&! "?(#! #?'"! ))! "?(%! #?'>! *##! >?#%! #?%%! )'! "?($! #?($! )%! "?)'! #?&"! ''%! "?)$! #?'&!

136

137

Table 4.37. One-way ANOVA summary of each year, 2004-2011, and research sample for ACT composite in comparison to the MBTI

Source df SS MS F p

Population MBTI 15 29.925 1.995 3.230 <.0001* Within groups 759 468.862 0.618 Total 774 498.787

2004 MBTI 15 12.369 0.825 2.184 0.0134* Within groups 81 30.581 0.378 Total 96 42.951

2005 MBTI 14 11.774 0.841 1.563 0.108 Within groups 82 43.575 0.538 Total 96 55.350

2006 MBTI 15 7.512 0.501 1.050 0.416 Within groups 79 35.675 0.477 Total 94 45.187

2007 MBTI 15 18.488 1.233 1.649 0.080 Within groups 80 58.308 0.748 Total 95 76.796

2008 MBTI 15 17.194 1.146 1.488 0.129 Within groups 83 63.962 0.771 Total 98 81.156

2009 MBTI 14 5.905 0.422 0.794 0.674 Within groups 85 45.179 0.532 Total 99 51.083

2010 MBTI 15 11.754 0.784 1.069 0.398 Within groups 81 59.385 0.733 Total 96 71.139

2011 MBTI 15 6.055 0.404 0.671 0.805 Within groups 79 47.523 0.602 Total 94 53.578

* p < .05

138

Briggs preference by cohort year or for the research population. The results shown in Table

4.38 indicate four instances in which significance appeared between the means of grade point

and Myers-Briggs preference for students in the full research population and one instance of

significance for the 2004 cohort. No significance for grade point and type were found in

2005 to 2011 cohort years with Tukey post-hoc test.

As illustrated in Table 4.36, the mean grade point for the research population was

2.86. This can be compared with Table 4.38, which illustrates the significant differences for

grade point found in Tukey post-hoc test. In the full research population, ENTJ, Extraverted

Intuition with Thinking and Judging, the mean GPA was 3.41; a difference of 0.804 from

ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, with a mean GPA of 2.61. The

difference for INFJ, Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, with a mean GPA of

3.26, from ENFP, was 0.650. For ENFJ, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, and

a mean of 3.18, to ENFP, the difference was 0.579. The difference for ISTJ, Introverted

Sensing with Thinking and Judging, with a mean GPA of 3.10, to ENFP, was 0.494. Table

4.38 also provides the mean grade point for the 2004 cohort was 3.01. In the Tukey post-hoc

test, illustrated in Table 4.38, ENFJ, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, with a

mean GPA of 3.50, has a difference of 1.001 from ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling

and Perceiving, with a mean GPA of 2.50.

To review Myers-Briggs preference with student achievement of a 2.0 grade point in

the first semester, contingency analysis was performed to explore the distribution of the two

nominal categories along with a cell chi-square and is displayed in Table 4.39. or the full

research population of 775 students enrolling from 2004 to 2011, 105 students were unable to

achieve a 2.0 grade point in the first semester. Of the students unable to achieve a 2.0 grade

139

Table 4.38. Summary of post hoc Tukey-Kramer HSD comparing first semester grade point to MBTI

Type for Population Mean Difference Std. Error p

ENTJ to ENFP 0.804 0.220 0.0251* INFJ to ENFP 0.650 0.171 0.0142* ENFJ to ENFP 0.579 0.128 0.0008* ISTJ to ENFP 0.494 0.137 0.0296*

Type for 2004 Cohort Mean Difference Std. Error p

ENFJ to ENFP 1.001 0.248 0.0108* *p < .05

point in the first semester, 32 students, or 30.48%, share the Myers-Briggs preference for

ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, in comparison to the National

Sample type distribution, where 8.1% of the population has the ENFP preference.

Measurement of Pearson chi-square test of the contingency analysis produced significance at

0.0076 indicating a small likelihood that the relationship between type and achievement of a

2.0 grade point in the first semester occurred by chance for the research population. The cell

chi-square for the 32 students below a 2.0 grade point with ENFP preference was found to be

9.2070, which is significant at 0.005.

Cohort years 2004 to 2009 were each found with ENFP as the most frequent type

preference for students not achieving a 2.0 grade point in the first semester. For the 2005

cohort of 96 students, shown in Table 4.40, 12 students were unable to achieve a 2.0 grade

point in the first semester. Of the students unable to achieve a 2.0 grade point in the first

semester, six students, or 50.00%, shared the Myers-Briggs preference for ENFP, Extraverted

Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving. Measurement of Pearson chi-square test of the

132

Table 4.39. Contingency table analysis comparing Myers-Briggs preference to 2.0 grade point for research sample !"#$%&'"%()&*&!")#+$&*&,"-&*&!.))&!/0&1&

2345& 2346& 23'5& 23'6& 2745& 2746& 27'5& 27'6& 8345& 8346& 83'5& 83'6& 8745& 8746& 87'5& 87'6& '9':;&

& & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & &

<6:& =>& ?>@& ?A& AB& C=& =D& CA& CD& 1=& =E& ?C& 11& =?& 1D& C?& A>& E@>&

& &&EFC=& ?AFD?& &&?FED& &&=F>A& &&=FD?& &&@FCD& &&=F==& &&EF?B& &&&AF1A& &&@F1A& &&?FD?& &&1FDC& &&EF=D& &&AFE?& &&=F1B& &&AFD@& DEFC=&

& BEF?=& @EFBD& B1FDE& DEFE@& DEF=C& D=F1B& B=F=E& D1F@E& ?>>F>>& D@F=>& D1FA=& B?FE@& B?F>@& D1FA=& B=FA=& @EFB1& &

& &&@FCE& ?=FB@& &&?FBC& &&=FD1& &&EF@1& &&DFEE& &&EFC?& &&@F?E& &&&AF@A& &&DFAE& &&1F>B& &&AF1D& &&@FE?& &&CF?D& &&EF?1& &&CFCD& &

& >F=EE1& ?FCC1B& >F>EE=& >F>>>1& >F>>>& >F>?>=& >FCA?C& >F>B?=& >F=A>D& >F>>D?& >F>AA>& >F>@==& >F?AD1& >F>EE?& >FABA@& >FC>BE& &

& & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & &

G&1F>& 1& A1& ?& E& @& ?>& 1& ?>& >& D& A& 1& =& E& 1& D& ?>=&

& >F1E& &&CF?A& >F?A& &&>F@@& &&>FB>& &&?F1B& >F1E& &&?F1B& >F>>& &&?F>A& &&>FAB& >F1E& &&>FE=& &&>F@@& >F1E& &&?F?E& ?AF==&

& AFD=& 1AF>1& @F?C& ?AFAA& ?AFCE& ?CF@?& CFCC& ?@F1C& >F>>& ?1F=>& ?@FE=& DFAA& &&DFBA& ?@FE=& CFE=& 1AF>B& &

& ?FB>& A>FCD& >FBC& &&=F@?& &&EFE@& &&BF=1& ?FB>& &&BF=1& >F>>& &&@FE1& &&1FDE& ?FB>& C?F@E& &&=F@?& ?FB>& &&DF=@& &

& AFE?1D& !"#$%$& >FC1C>& >F>>?=& >F>>>A& >F>E@1& 1FB=1B& >F=DAD& AFAD@?& >F>=?B& >F1?>D& >FCD?D& >FDD11& >FC1F?E& 1F=?1C& 1FE?A=& &

& & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & &

& =1& ?A>& ?C& C=& =1& ED& C=& =B& 1=& EC& ?D& 1C& =E& AC& CA& A>& @@=&

& EF@?& ?@FBC& ?FD?& =FD?& EF@?& DF@@& =FD?& @FCD& AF1A& DF1E& 1F?B& AF?>& @F1A& CFAB& =F==& =F>A& &&Sig. at .005

140

137

Table 4.40. Contingency table analysis comparing 2005 cohort Myers-Briggs preference to 2.0 grade point !"#$%&'"%()&*&!")#+$&*&,"-&*&!.))&!/0&1&

2345& 2346& 23'5& 23'6& 2745& 2746& 27'5& 27'6& 8345& 8346& 83'5& 83'6& 8745& 8746& 87'5& 87'6& '9':;&

& & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & &

<6:& =& >1& >& ?& >1& @& A& B& ?& =& B& 1& ?& A& B& & @A&

& &&&&=C1D& >1CEF& &&>CFA& &&?C1E& >1CEF& &&&&@CBB& &&&&AC>=& &&&&BC>B& &&&&?C1E& &&=C1D& &&&&BC>B& &&&&1CF@& &&&&?C1E& &&&&AC>=& &&&&BC>B& & @=CEF&

& >FFGFF& ??C?=& EFCFF& @EC=>& D1CB>& >FFCFF& >FFCFF& >FFCFF& >FFCFF& =FCFF& >FFCFF& >FFCFF& >FFCFF& >FFCFF& >FFCFF& & &

& &&&&@CBB& >AC1D& &&>C>D& &&=C>A& >AC1D& &&&&DCE1& &&&&AC=?& &&&&BCE=& &&&&=C>A& &&@CBB& &&&&BCEF& &&&&1CB@& &&&&=C>A& &&&&AC=?& &&&&BCE=& & &

& FC>1EF& FC@D1D& FCB1>A& FCFF1?& FCFBAB& FC>A1D& FCF=>A& FCFEB?& FC>F=>& FCBEFF& FCFEB?& FCFBE=& FC>F=>& FCF=>A& FCFEB?& & &

& & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & &

H&1CF& F& ?& >& >& >& F& F& F& F& B& F& F& F& F& F& & >1&

& FCFF& &&?C1E& &&>CFA& &&>CFA& >CFA& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& &&BC>B& &&FCBD& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& & >1CEF&

& FCFF& BBCBB& EFCFF& >AC1D& =C?D& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& BFCFF& >=C?E& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& & &

& FCFF& EFCFF& &&@CBB& &&@CBB& @CBB& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& 1ECFF& &&1C@?& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& FCFF& & &

& @C@E=F& !"#$%%& 1C1EFF& FCF>=D& FC1AFA& >CFFFF& FCEFFF& FCB=EF& FC=EFF& 1CAEFF& FCB=EF& FC1EFF& FC=EFF& & BCB=EF& & &

& & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & & &

& =& >@& 1& =& >B& @& A& B& ?& >F& B& 1& ?& A& B& & D?&

& =C1D& >@C=E& 1CF@& =C1D& >BCE& BCAA& AC>=& BC>B& ?C1E& >FCA1& BC>B& 1CF@& ?C1E& AC>=& BC>B& & &

&Sig. at .025

141

142

contingency analysis for the 2005 cohort did not display significance. Cell chi-square for the

six students with ENFP preference unable to achieve a 2.0 grade point was found to be 6.25,

significant at 0.025.

Null hypothesis 5 was rejected because there was significant difference in Myers-

Briggs preference for students in the study by grade point in the first college semester in

comparison for the 2004 cohort year and for the full research population. In the full research

population, Tukey post-hoc test found ENTJ, INFJ, ENFJ, and ISTJ to have a significant

mean difference for grade point among students with ENFP preference. Tukey analysis for

the 2004 cohort found ENFJ with a significant mean difference from ENFP.

Hypothesis 6 was rejected as there was a significant difference in Myers-Briggs

preference for students who were able to achieve a 2.00 grade point in the first college

semester was identified for the 2005 cohort year and for the research population. Of students

unable to achieve a 2.0 grade point in the 2005 cohort, 50% shared ENFP preferences, as did

30.48% of students not achieving a 2.0 grade point in the full research population. These

results were found significant with cell chi-square.

The findings indicate that, although many students identified with the ENFP

preference, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, and were capable of adapting

within personal type preferences and learning style to find academic success; others with the

ENFP preference faced difficulty in the transition to the first college semester. As students

who are unable to achieve a 2.0 grade point at the university are placed on academic warning

or probation and in jeopardy of continuing their enrollment, there is need for understanding

of type and learning style to improve college transition and development of academic

143

strengths. These data are important for college administrators, specifically those working

with students during orientation and courses in the first college year.

Relationship of ACT, high school percentile rank, and Myers-Briggs preference to first-

semester grade point by cohort year and for research sample

Research Question 7: Are there statistical relationships for ACT, high school percentilerank and Myers-Briggs preference for student grade point in the first college semester by each cohort year and across groups?

H07: There is no correlation of ACT, class rank, or Myers-Briggs preference

by grade point in the first college semester in comparison by each

cohort year and for the research population.

The model in this analysis was constructed with three types of variables following

Astin’s (1984) conceptual I-E-O framework (input-environment-outcome). The background

characteristics of the student through the Myers-Briggs preference (input); the two high

school academic aptitude characteristics: high school percentile and ACT score

(environment); and the cumulative GPA at the end of the first college semester (outcome).

Multiple logistic regression was implemented for the variables of class rank, ACT

composite, and Myers-Briggs preference for the full research sample and 2004 to 2011

cohorts. Regression analysis uses the relationship between variables to make a prediction

based on observation. As ACT and percentile class rank are considered similar variables of

academic aptitude, a test for multicollinearity and singularity was utilized.

Multicollinearity and singularity

Multicollinearity is a condition in which the independent variables are highly

correlated (.90 or greater) and singularity is when the independent variables are perfectly

144

correlated and one independent variable is a combination of one or more of the other

independent variables. If multicollinearity or singularity exists, then the independent

variables are redundant and do not hold predictive value over another independent variable

(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Prior to applying statistical modeling, the fit between the

academic aptitude variables (ACT and percentile class rank), was checked for

multicollinearity and singularity through Pearson correlation. Tabachnick and Fidell

suggested that independent variables that correlate with one another at .70 or greater should

not be included. As the correlation for ACT and percentile rank was 0.4475, it was

determined the ACT and percentile rank variables could be used in Research Question seven

logistic regression modeling.

To test for normality of the regression model, residuals were collected and a

distribution was calculated finding a normal distribution. To check for autocorrelation of the

residuals, the Durbin-Watson statistic was calculated at 1.782 with a p value of 0.0004. As

the Durbin-Watson is near 2.0, there is no autocorrelation among the residuals. A review of

the Residual by Predicted Plot also found no nonlinear effects.

A validation of the research population models was tested with the Predicted Error

Sum of Squares (PRESS) statistic. The PRESS calculates the residual for each observation

from a new regression that excludes the observation from the estimation. For a model to be

valid the PRESS should be close to but not less than the Sum of Squared Errors (SSE) found

in the ANOVA. As the PRESS for the regression model was found to be 379.11566 as

compared to the SSE of 362.12401, the model was found valid. Subsequent PRESS statistics

for the individual cohort models also demonstrated validity.

145

Models examining MBTI, ACT and percentile rank as variables to GPA

To understand the relationship of academic success in the first college semester to

Myers-Briggs preference and academic aptitude, this analysis examined whether any

relationships existed between first semester grade point and Myers-Briggs preference, ACT

composite, and%ile class rank. A logistic regression analysis was utilized to answer this

question for the students in the sample to describe the average effect, if any, of predictor

variables on the criterion variable of first semester grade point. There were sixteen exclusive

type categories examined for the full research sample and each cohort year.

The model was computed with the sixteen Myers-Briggs type categories, ACT

Composite, and high school class percentile rank seeking relationship to first semester grade

point. As the MBTI variable sorts to the 16 type categories upon regression analysis,

coefficients were not illustrated for MBTI in Table 4.41. The primary variables are listed in

R-Square (R2) ascending order showing increasing proportion of the variation for fit of the

model. The highest possible R2 is 1.0 while the lowest us 0.0. The R2 as a model comparison

can indicate that variables are poorly measured, that variables have been excluded, or that the

model has been incorrectly specified. However, the R2 is only the indicator of completeness

of the regression model, as the p-value of the model is a better factor to determine the

goodness of a regression. The p-value for each of the variables and combination of variables

was significant at <.0001.

To determine the accuracy of model, it was necessary to understand the model fit in

comparison to test results with the variables (Table 4.42). The model coefficient had an R2

of 0.2740, indicating that 27% of factor influence on first-semester grade point could be

146

Table 4.41. Multiple regression of MBTI, ACT composite, and percentile rank for first-semester grade point for research sample, ascending R2

Variable B R2 F p

MBTI - 0.0414 3.2296 <.0001*

ACT 0.729 0.1088 94.3642 <.0001*

MBTI & ACT# 0.073# 0.1629 9.2217 <.0001*

Percentile Rank 0.028 0.2236 222.672 <.0001*

ACT# 0.033#

0.2411 122.6427 <.0001*

0.2548 16.195 <.0001*

MBTI, ACT# 0.035#

0.2740 16.805 <.0001*

* p < .05

Table 4.42. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper MBTI

ENFP -0.212 0.062 0.0007* -0.335 -0.089

ENTJ 0.390 0.175 0.0263* 0.046 0.735

INFJ 0.364 0.133 0.0062* 0.104 0.625

Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.023 0.002 <.0001* 0.019 0.027

ACT Composite 0.035 0.008 <.0001* 0.020 0.051

N = 775; *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

explained with this model. The p value was significant at < .0001, indicating an adequate fit

of the data to the model.

Three Myers-Briggs preferences were found to have relationship with first semester

grade point for the full research population. ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition

with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to negatively relate to first-semester grade point

147

upon regression analysis for the full research population and was significant at 0.0007 where

the p value is <0.05. ENFP was the preference for 17.9% of the research population, which

is 2.21 times more than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base national sample. ENTJ,

the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, demonstrated a positive

relationship for first semester grade point and was significant at 0.0263. The ENTJ

preference, at 1.8% of the research population, was equal to the national sample percentage.

INFJ, the preference for Introverted Intuition with Feeling and Judging, demonstrated a

positive relationship for first semester grade point and was significant at 0.0062. The INFJ

preference, at 3.3% of the research population, was 2.2 times greater than the national sample

percentage of 1.5%.

Both measures of academic aptitude, ACT composite and high school percentile rank,

were found to positively affect first semester grade point for the full research population.

Percentile rank and ACT were each significant at <.0001, where the p value is < 0.05 for the

model.

The 2004 Cohort model coefficient (Table 4.43) demonstrated R-square of 0.3989,

indicating that nearly 40% of factor influence on first-semester grade point could be

explained with this model. The p value was significant at 0.0004, indicating an adequate fit

of the data to the model.

Two Myers-Briggs preferences were found to have relationship with first semester

grade point for the 2004 Cohort. ENFJ, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling

and Judging, demonstrated a positive relationship for first semester grade point and was

significant at 0.0437. The ENFJ preference, at 10.3% of the 2004 cohort, was 2.5 times

greater than the national sample percentage of 2.5%. ENFP, the preference for Extraverted

148

Table 4.43. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance pre N = 97)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper MBTI

ENFJ 0.386 0.188 0.0437* 0.011 0.761

ENFP -0.354 0.168 0.0380* -0.688 -0.020

Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.017 0.005 0.0005* 0.008 0.027 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to negatively relate to first-semester grade

point upon regression analysis and was significant at 0.0380. ENFP was the preference for

16.5% of the cohort, which is 2.04 times more than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the

base national sample.

For measures of academic aptitude, high school percentile rank was found to

positively affect first semester grade point for the 2004 cohort. Percentile rank was

significant at 0.0005, where the p value is <0.05 for the model.

Regression analysis for the 2005 Cohort model (Table 4.44) demonstrated R-square

of 0.3954, indicating that nearly 40% of factor influence on first-semester grade point could

be explained with this model. The p value was significant at 0.0003, indicating a fit of the

data to the model.

ENFP, the preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was

found to negatively relate to first-semester grade point upon regression analysis and was

significant at 0.0022. ENFP was the preference for 18.8% of the cohort, which is 2.32 times

greater than the expected occurrence of 8.1% in the base national sample.

149

Table 4.44. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance p N = 96)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper MBTI

ENFP -0.528 0.167 0.0022* -0.860 -0.195

Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.028 0.007 0.0001* 0.014 0.042 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

For measures of academic aptitude, high school percentile rank was found to

positively affect first semester grade point for the 2005 cohort. Percentile rank was

significant at 0.0001, where the p value is <0.05 for the model.

A review of the regression for the 2006 Cohort model (Table 4.45) demonstrated R-

square of 0.3914, indicating a 39% of factor influence on first-semester grade point could be

explained with this model. The p value was significant at 0.0007, indicating a fit of the data

to the model.

INTP, the preference for Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, was

found to negatively relate to first-semester grade point upon regression analysis and was

significant at 0.0076. INTP was the preference for 3.2% of the 2006 cohort, which with a

Table 4.45. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance N = 95)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper MBTI

INTP -0.942 0.344 0.0076* -1.626 -0.258

Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.021 0.006 0.0011* 0.009 0.033 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

150

self-selection of 0.97, is slightly less than the expected occurrence of 3.3% in the base

national sample.

As a measure of academic aptitude, high school percentile rank was found to

positively affect first semester grade point for the 2006 cohort. Percentile rank was

significant at 0.0011, where the p value is <0.05 for the model.

A review of the regression for the 2007 Cohort model (Table 4.46) demonstrated R-

square of 0.3655, indicating nearly 37% of factor influence on first-semester grade point

could be explained with this model. The p value was significant at 0.0020, indicating a fit of

the data to the model.

No Myers-Briggs preferences were found to have relationship to first semester grade

point for the cohort. For measures of academic aptitude, high school percentile rank was

found to positively affect first semester grade point with significance at 0.0120. ACT also

held a positive relationship with first semester GPA and demonstrated a 0.0236 significance.

Table 4.46. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance N = 96)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.021 0.008 0.0120* 0.005 0.037

ACT Composite 0.072 0.031 0.0236* 0.010 0.134 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

Regression analysis for the 2008 Cohort model (Table 4.47) demonstrated R-square

of 0.5162, indicating that 51% of factor influence on first-semester grade point could be

explained with this model. The p value was significant at <.0001, indicating a satisfactory fit

of the data to the model.

151

Table 4.47. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance N = 99)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper

Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.034 0.005 <.0001* 0.023 0.045 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

Only a measure of academic aptitude, high school percentile rank, was found with a

relationship to first semester grade point. Percentile rank was a positive influence to first

semester grade point for the 2008 cohort and was significant at <.0001, where the p value is

<0.05 for the model.

The regression for the 2009 Cohort model (Table 4.48) posted an R-square of 0.3214,

indicating that 32% of factor influence on first-semester grade point could be explained with

this model. The p value was significant at .0043, indicating a fit of the data to the model.

The measures of academic aptitude, ACT composite and percentile rank, were found

to have a relationship to first semester grade point. ACT was positively related to first

semester grade point for the 2008 cohort and was significant at .0095, where the p value is

<0.05 for the model. Percentile rank was also positively related with significance at 0.0030.

Table 4.48. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance ade point, 2009 cohort (N = 100)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.018 0.021 0.0095* 0.006 0.030

ACT Composite 0.056 0.006 0.0030* 0.014 0.098 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

152

Regression analysis for the 2010 Cohort model (Table 4.49) demonstrated R-square

of 0.3005, indicating that 30% of factor influence on first-semester grade point could be

explained with this model. The p value was significant at .0212, indicating fit of the data to

the model.

Only a measure of academic aptitude, high school percentile rank, was found with a

relationship to first semester grade point for this cohort. Percentile rank had a positive

relationship to first semester grade point for the 2010 cohort and was significant at .0192,

where the p value is <0.05 for the model.

A regression analysis for the 2011 Cohort model (Table 4.50) resulted in an R-square

of 0.3805, indicating that a 38% factor influence on first-semester grade point could be

explained with this model. The p value was significant at 0.0012, indicating a fit of the data

to the model.

Table 4.49. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance N = 97)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper

Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.018 0.008 0.0192* 0.003 0.033 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

Table 4.50. Summary of regression analysis model for variables with significance t (N = 95)

95% C.I. Variable B S.E. Sig. Lower Upper MBTI

ESTJ -0.483 0.236 0.0444* -0.954 -1.013

Academic Aptitude percentile rank 0.268 0.007 0.0001* 0.013 0.040 *p < 0.05 (two-tailed)

153

ESTJ, the preference for Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, was found

to negatively relate to first-semester grade point and was significant at 0.0444. ESTJ was the

preference for 5.3% of the cohort, which is significantly less than the expected occurrence of

11.6% in the base national sample.

For measures of academic aptitude, high school percentile rank was found to

positively affect first semester grade point for the 2011 cohort. Percentile rank was

significant at 0.0001, where the p value is <0.05 for the model.

Null hypothesis 7 was rejected because there was evidence of correlation of ACT,

class rank, or Myers-Briggs preference by grade point in the first college semester in

comparison by each for the research population and multiple cohort years. ENFP, the

preference for Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to negatively

relate to first-semester grade point for the research population and 2004 and 2005 cohorts.

As previously illustrated in Table 4.38, students with the ENFP preference have tendency to

struggle for academic success in the first semester and may benefit from additional guidance

and support. High school percentile class rank was consistently significant for the positive

relationship to first semester grade point being found as a variable for the research population

and each cohort year. Myers-Briggs preference and academic aptitude are not the full

contributing factors to first semester grade for the students in the population, but there is

substantial results from the regression analysis to suggest that type preference offers an

additional variable for assisting students.

154

Summary

This chapter presented the quantitative results of the study through various

descriptive and inferential statistical analyses. The descriptive statistics provided the

background characteristics of the population in this study by gender, STEM major, and

academic aptitude measurements of ACT Composite and high school graduating class

percentile rank. Findings for the research population included the nearly even distribution of

female students (50.1%) and male students (49.8%). The mean composite ACT score was

24.31, and the mean percentile rank was 81.35. STEM majors were selected for 55% of

students in the study.

Sections RQ 2–7 provided the inferential statistical results for the research questions

and hypotheses stated in each section. Statistically significant differences in the distribution

of Myers-Briggs type preferences in comparison to the national base population sample were

reported in Section RQ 2. In the full research sample of 775 students, seven type preferences

were found to be a lower percentage than the national base sample, eight preferences were

found to be greater than the base sample, and one, ENTJ was found to have an equal

percentage in the research sample as in the national base sample. For the eight cohort years

in the study (8 cohorts, 16 type preferences), only three type preference comparisons were

found to be equal to the base national sample for the preference, two for INTJ and one ENTJ.

Section RQ 3 found statistically significant differences in the distribution of type

preferences among males and females in each cohort group of students and for the full

research sample. ENFP or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving is the most

frequent type preference of males for the research population at 17.4% (N = 67) and females

at 18.5% (N = 71). The least frequent preference for male students is ENTJ, or Extraverted

155

Intuition with Thinking and Judging at 1.3% of the population (N = 5). The least frequent

preference for female students is INTP, or Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving

at 1.5% (N = 6).

Section RQ 4 examined the presence of a statistically significant difference in the

type preference of students by whether they had selected a STEM or non-STEM major.

Analysis found statistically significant differences in the distribution of type preferences

among STEM majors in each cohort and among seven of eight cohorts for non-STEM majors

and for the full research sample. Only the 2010 non-STEM cohort had no type preferences

found to be significantly different from the base sample. ENFP or Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Perceiving was found as the most frequent type preference for the research

population with STEM majors at 17.0% (N = 73) and non-STEM majors at 19.1% (N = 66).

The least frequent preference for STEM students is ENTJ, or Extraverted Intuition with

Thinking and Judging at 1.86% of the population (N = 8). The least frequent preference for

non-STEM students is INTJ, or Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging at 1.16% (N

= 4).

To answer Research Question 5, cross-tabulations, one-way analysis of variance

(ANOVA), and Tukey-Kramer HSD post hoc tests were utilized to determine any difference

in ACT and high school percentile rank and Myers-Briggs preference by cohort year and for

the research population. Significant difference in ACT Composite score and percentile class

rank were identified in this study’s research population but not by individual cohort year.

Mean ACT composite for the research population was 24.31 while nine Myers-Briggs

preference groups in the population had a mean ACT above that score. Additionally, mean

percentile rank for the research sample was 81.35 with a standard deviation of 12.82.

156

Section RQ 6 compared differences in Myers-Briggs preference for student grade

point in the first college semester by each cohort year and across groups. A significant

difference in Myers-Briggs preference for students who are able to achieve a 2.00 grade point

in the first college semester was identified for the 2005 cohort year and for the research

population. Of students unable to achieve a 2.0 grade point in the 2005 cohort, 50% shared

ENFP preferences, as did 30.48% of students not achieving a 2.0 grade point in the full

research population.

Section RQ 7 detailed logistic regression results finding evidence of correlation of

ACT, class rank, and Myers-Briggs preference by grade point in the first college semester in

for the research population and multiple cohort years. ENFP, the preference for Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to negatively relate to first-semester grade

point for the research population and 2004 and 2005 cohorts. High school percentile class

rank was consistently significant for positive relationship to first semester grade point as a

variable for the research population and each cohort year.

This study produced indicators to suggest that Myers-Briggs preference, particularly

preferences for ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, may impact student

academic progress for this population. Identification and understanding of these preferences

may assist in compensating for student learning differences and academic direction. As type

theory tells us that preferences are not related to ability or motivation, identifying a trend

toward specific type preferences related to academic achievement may provide support for

the student population in this research. These data are important for college administrators,

specifically those working with students during orientation, advisement, and courses in the

first college year.

157

CHAPTER 5. DISCUSSION

Institutions are increasingly scrutinized for their ability to retain and graduate

students. Higher education administrators are aware of these expectations and consistently

measure predictors, productivity, programs, and funding to enhance retention. The evolving

student demographic provides ever-changing opportunities for examining student behaviors,

academic, and personal goals.

Usage of a learning style assignment in a first-year seminar course has revealed that a

student cohort with anomalous Myers-Briggs preferences for Extraverted Intuition with

Feeling and Perceiving could provide analysis for the field of typological student

development and student retention research. Delving further into a comparison of type

preferences for students who are not able to achieve a 2.0 grade point average and who may

be withdrawing from an institution prior to completing a degree may offer insight to

improving student success. These circumstances define the hypothesis that psychological

type preferences may be a variable for success for this student cohort within the university.

As human learning has been described as individual as human fingerprints (Dryden &

Vos, 2005), an understanding of psychological type as made available by the MBTI

instrument can be a mechanism for assisting students and college administrators in

understanding learning and student success in the post-secondary institutions. Seidman

(2012) described early identification, or the assessment of student skill levels, as an essential

component to retention. Establishing student learning style through the Myers-Briggs

assessment or other measures contributes to this formula. As university cultures are wide

158

and varying, establishing student fit and learning style within the culture is one method for

enhancing student satisfaction and progress to graduation.

There is evidence that statistical analysis of type distributions for a specific

population may help identify whether MBTI preferences correlate to student academic

difficulty in the first college year. The review of the literature supports that type preferences,

particularly preferences for Perceiving, may have an effect on student academic progress and

that identification may assist in compensating for student learning differences. Type

dichotomies are similar to arguments for lateralization of brain functions, science shows that

individuals use both sides of their brain for all activities, but one hemisphere may have

dominance, as it is with type and learning. Type theory tells us that preferences are not

related to ability or motivation and that all preferences are equal in their validity and strength

to the individual and no type is of more advantage than another. This research has sought to

confirm these questions to plan for future assessment and so that adaptive programming may

be aimed at increasing student success.

The purpose of this study was to examine the perceived relationship between a

personality and learning style assessment and student success in the first college semester.

This study had three goals: (a) understand the results of the Myers-Briggs assessment for the

research population in comparison to a national base sample; (b) explore the relationship of

the Myers-Briggs assessment to gender and STEM major for the research population; and (c)

investigate the correlation of academic aptitude and Myers-Briggs assessment results to

grade point in the first college semester. This chapter discusses the quantitative results and

overall findings of this study. First, a summary of the study is provided followed by the

findings of the quantitative research. Next, this chapter will discuss implications for practice,

159

application of the study, recommendations for future research and final thoughts regarding

the significance of the study.

The literature review in Chapter 2 presented an overview of type theory, and the

benefit of measuring type and academic success. It outlined the framework, importance, and

critical need for this research. The review also provided a basis for addressing the research

questions and hypotheses, and for determining which variables (such as ACT and Class rank)

to consider for use in the logistic regression models. The chapter also provided a synopsis of

the positive aspects of type and learning style identification as an asset to college academic

success.

Chapter 3 presented the quantitative methodology for using a secondary database to

examine the research questions in this study. Myers-Briggs preferences gathered from

students in a first semester learning community seminar course, supplemented by academic

aptitude, major, and grade point information were sorted by the university registrar and

investigated for relationship to academic success. Descriptive and inferential statistics were

used to analyze the demographic pre-college academic aptitude characteristics and,

specifically, the Myers-Briggs assessment results collected for students entering the

university 2004 to 2011. The research questions, hypotheses, research design, setting,

population, data collection, variables, data management, and analyses were stated. The

chapter also discussed ethical considerations, limitations, and delimitations of the study.

Findings

Through descriptive and inferential statistical analyses, the results of seven research

questions were presented in Chapter 4. Research Question 1 identified descriptive statistics

160

found for the research population of 775 students. Each cohort year enrollment averaged 97

students with the smallest class being 95 students and the largest class enrolling 100 students.

Representation of male and female percentage for the research sample of students was

roughly equal (female: 50.1%). The percentage of female students by cohort year ranged

from 39% to 58%. A majority of students in the research sample had STEM majors (55%)

with the smallest percentage of STEM being the 2004 cohort year with 45% and the largest

proportion being the 2005 cohort year with 66%. For measures of academic aptitude, the

research population had a mean composite ACT score of 24.31 and mean high school class

percentile rank of 81.35. The mean first semester cumulative GPA was 2.87 for the research

samples with the 2004 cohort posting the highest mean grade point at 3.01 and the 2008

cohort registering the lowest first semester grade point at 2.75.

Majority of students in the study have ENFP preferences

Research Question 2 utilized SRTT analysis for comparison of the research sample

Myers-Briggs type preferences to a national base sample. Significant differences were

identified in the distribution of type preferences in each cohort group of students and for the

full research sample compared to the national sample. The preference of Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, ENFP, was the most frequent type and was found in

17.9% of the population, a self-selection index of 2.21. Described by Myers (1980) the

ENFP individual is strong in initiative and creativity, but is not always successful at

completing projects. They will frequently demonstrate impulsive energy but not always the

willpower to get things done. The ENFP hates routine, thrives on variety, may lack planned

purpose, but is full of ideas.

161

Findings for the research sample illustrated seven type preferences were found to be

of a lower percentage than the national base sample, eight preferences were found to be

greater than the base sample, and one, ENTJ, Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and

Judging, was found to have an equal percentage in the research sample as in the national base

sample. For the eight cohort years in the study (eight cohort years, 16 type preferences), only

three type preference comparisons were found to be equal to the base national sample for the

preference, two INTJ, Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, and one ENTJ,

Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging.

Differing frequency of type preferences for male and female students

Significant differences were identified in the distribution of type preferences among

males and females in each cohort group of students and for the full research sample in

research Question 3. Cohort years 2009 and 2010 demonstrated two significantly different

female type preferences in comparison to the base sample although the overall sample for

female students in those cohorts did not appear significant. ENFP or Extraverted Intuition

with Feeling and Perceiving was found to be the most frequent type preference for the

research population, males and females. The least frequent preference for male students was

ENTJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, while the least frequent

preference for female students is INTP, or Introverted Intuition with Thinking and

Perceiving. In the full research sample and eight cohort years, only one type preference for

male and for female students was found to be equal to the base national sample for the

preference. ISTP, Introverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, for male students and

162

ESTJ, Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Judging, for female students, were found equal

to the national sample in the 2010 cohort year measurement.

ENFP most frequent type for STEM and non-STEM majors

Research Question 4 found significant differences in Myers-Briggs preferences for

STEM and non-STEM students in the study by cohort year and for the research population in

comparison to the distribution of a national population. Statistically significant differences in

the distribution of type preferences were found among STEM majors in each cohort, for

seven of eight cohorts for non-STEM majors, and for the full research sample. Only the

2010 non-STEM cohort had no type preferences found to be significantly different from the

base sample.

ENFP or Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving was the most frequent

type preference for the research population with STEM and non-STEM majors, mirroring the

finding for the research population. The least frequent preference for STEM students is

ENTJ, or Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, while the least frequent

preference for non-STEM students is INTJ, or Introverted Intuition with Thinking and

Judging. Analyzing the full research sample and eight cohort years, only two type

preferences for STEM majors were found to be equal to the base national sample for the

preference, ESTP, Extraverted Sensing with Thinking and Perceiving, in the 2006 cohort

year, and ENTJ, Extraverted Intuition with Thinking and Judging, in the 2011 cohort.

163

Variances exist between ACT and class rank and Myers-Briggs preference

Upon analysis of the pre-college academic aptitude variables, ACT composite and

percentile class rank, significance was found for each variable in comparison to the full

research population in Research Question 5. There was also significant difference in ACT

Composite score and percentile class rank earned by this study’s research population in

comparison between type preferences. No significance for ACT or percentile rank and type

preference was found by individual cohort year. Nine instances of significant difference

were found between the means of ACT and Myers-Briggs preference for students in the full

research population. INTP, or Introverted Intuition with Thinking and Perceiving, had

significantly different ACT Composite from ESFP, ESFJ, ISFJ, and ENFP. Type preferences

for ENTP, ESTJ, ISTP, ENFJ, and INFP were found to have significant difference for ACT

Composite from ESFP, or Extraverted Sensing with Feeling and Perceiving.

Variances exist between first semester GPA and Myers-Briggs preference

In a review of first-semester grade point comparisons in Research Question 6,

significant difference was found in Myers-Briggs preference for students in the study for the

2004 cohort year and for the full research population. Students with ENTJ, INFJ, ENFJ, and

ISTJ preferences were found to have a significantly higher grade point than students with

ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, preference in the full research

population. Students in the 2004 cohort with ENFJ, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and

Judging, preference were found with significantly higher grade point than those with ENFP,

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, preference in the cohort. A significant

difference in Myers-Briggs preference for students able to achieve a 2.00 grade point in the

164

first college semester was identified for 2005 cohort year and for the research population. Of

students unable to achieve a 2.0 grade point in the 2005 cohort, 50% shared ENFP,

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, preferences, as did 30.48% of students not

achieving a 2.0 grade point in the full research population.

ENFP preference has negative impact for some students

Finally, evidence of correlation for ACT, class rank, and Myers-Briggs preference by

grade point in the first college semester was found among each variable measured for the

research population and multiple cohort years. ENFP, the preference for Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, was found to negatively relate to first-semester grade

point for the research population, 2004, and 2005 cohorts. High school percentile class rank

was consistently significant for the positive relationship to first semester grade point as a

variable for the research population and each cohort year. Myers-Briggs preference and

academic aptitude did not demonstrate as full contributing factors to first semester grade for

the students in the population, but there were substantial results from the regression analysis

to suggest that type preference offers an additional variable for assisting and evaluating

student strengths and learning styles.

Conclusions

Questions central to this study inquire whether the overrepresentation of certain

Myers-Briggs type preferences are distinct to this participant population and whether it leads

to an overrepresentation of specific type preferences among participants who are not

successful in the first college year. The collection of quantitative archival data related to

participant academic persistence is key to the type preference analysis. If one compares

165

these results with Astin’s (1993) Input-Environment-Outcome model it can be noted that

precollege academic aptitude characteristics, student first semester learning experiences and

grade point each intertwine to influence first semester grade point outcomes.

The findings indicate that although many students identify with the ENFP preference,

Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, and are capable of adapting within

personal type preferences and learning style to find academic success; many other students

with the ENFP preference face difficulty in the transition to the first college semester. As

illustrated in the grade point comparison in Chapter 4, students with the ENFP preference

have a tendency to struggle for academic success in the first semester and may benefit from

additional guidance and support. As students who are unable to achieve a 2.0 grade point at

the university are placed on academic warning or probation and in jeopardy of continuing

their enrollment, there is need for understanding of type and learning style to improve college

transition and development of academic strengths.

This study produced indicators that suggest Myers-Briggs preference, particularly

preferences for ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, may impact student

academic progress for this population. Identification and understanding of these preferences

may assist in compensating for student learning differences and academic direction. As type

theory tells us that preferences are not related to ability or motivation, identifying a trend

toward specific type preferences related to academic achievement may lead to better support

for the student population in this research. These data are important for college

administrators, specifically those working with students during orientation, advisement, and

transition courses in the first college year.

166

Implications for Practice

When examining a conceptual framework of this research, the dichotomies of

psychological type, learning styles, and influences of academic aptitude and academic

success in the first semester are important to the analysis. There are four MBTI type

dichotomies or opposite preferences: (a) personal energy, or Extraversion and Introversion;

(b) how an individual takes in information or Sensing and Intuiting; (c) the logical or

harmonious decision-making process of Thinking or Feeling; and (d) the organization and

orientation to the outer world or Judging and Perceiving. When assessing psychological

type, an individual is always allowed to determine their best-fit type, while environment,

academics, and self-knowledge may influence this best fit. Additional factors of college-

going success including preparation, goals, beliefs, obstacles and motivation solidify

structure. Although these findings emphasize that psychological type, specifically, is not the

only indicator of student difficulty in the first college year, it may complement other early-

identification factors to enhance intrusive first-year advising and retention efforts.

Multiple Regression implemented for the variables of percentile class rank, ACT, first

semester grade point and type preference for the full cohort and each cohort group found

high school percentile class rank with the strongest relationship to grade point. This is in

contrast to Kalsbeek’s (1986) Myers-Briggs assessment research finding the SAT as the best

predictor of first semester grade point. The study also highlighted that Myers-Briggs

preference, particularly preferences for ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and

Perceiving, may impact student academic progress when the individual student has a difficult

transition in learning style.

167

Identification and understanding of these preferences may assist in compensating for

student learning differences. Identifying a trend toward specific type preferences related to

academic achievement may provide support for the student population. Future research will

seek to confirm these questions with a goal to develop adaptive programming aimed at

increasing student success.

The results of this study are similar to those found by DiRienzo, Das, Synn, Kitts, and

McGrath (2010), who identified Judging types with generally higher average GPAs and

Barrineau (2005) who discovered that found students preferring P (Perceiving), NP (Intuition

Perceiving), and ENFP, Extraverted Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, were moderately

but significantly more likely to be at risk of retention. As failure to find academic success is

a major factor in student persistence, Kalsbeek (2003) emphasized that the MBTI instrument

is useful for academic success programs and can help identify special challenges for students,

as a method for responding to students in need of academic support. Likewise, Tinto (2012)

encouraged the incorporation of new student assessments that attend less to knowledge and

skills and pay more attention to what defines student success in college and can also consider

the interaction of student personality and environment.

Beckham’s (2012) analysis of academically successful Perceiving preference college

students presents a variety of methods for engaging students with preferences incongruent to

the university academic culture. Understanding how some students, particularly those with

ENFP preferences, view time in a continuous, more fluid process and how they find strength

in last minute or deadline pressure is a step toward enhancing the academic success of

different learners. Beckham recommends that institutions provide students with the freedom

to construct their own learning experiences. Findings by Reason, Terenzini, and Domingo

168

(2006) support the individual student experience as a strong predictor for success and also

that perceived level of student support has great influence on the development of academic

competence. They additionally recommend that faculty use a variety of teaching methods to

appeal to student learning. As discussed by Erickson and Strommer (2005), institutions and

instructors offering a variety of educational activities and assignments to engage learning can

assist students of all preferences to find academic success.

An understanding of psychological type as made available by the MBTI instrument

can be a mechanism for assisting students and college administrators to understand learning

and student success in various post-secondary institutions. As university cultures are wide

and varying, establishing student fit within that culture through use of type assessments is an

efficient method for enhancing student satisfaction and progress to graduation.

Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research

This study was designed to capture data for an identified research population at this

university and should be carefully interpreted before comparison to other student groups or

institutions. The population sample was deemed of sufficient sample size to address the data,

but may not be representative of the student population based upon demographic and

socioeconomic status. Specifically, as this research population was comprised predominantly

of students with high financial need and as students with higher budgeted financial need are

significantly less likely to persist or graduate (Whalen, Saunders and Shelley, 2010), the

population may be predisposed to academic difficulty.

As the Myers-Briggs is a self-reporting instrument, the assumption must be made that

the respondents are of normal mental health and objectively report their preferences when

169

completing the assessment. Additionally, as respondents were asked to complete the Myers-

Briggs assessment as part of a first-year seminar assignment on learning styles as opposed to

self-selecting to complete the instrument, an assumption is made that the respondents were

objectively reporting their preferences and were not influenced by the assignment directive.

Questions central to this study sought to define if the overrepresentation of certain

type preferences is distinct to this participant population and whether it leads to an

overrepresentation of specific type preferences among participants who are not successful in

the first college year. Without this information, the study would not have sufficient

foundation to build adequate research questions. Additionally, participant verification of

type understanding is needed to determine if type preferences were viewed as a contributing

factor in academic success and successfully linked to the study.

Effective application of type knowledge allows recognition that individuals do not

interact, process information and produce decisions in similar manners. This same

acceptance and appreciation of type principles may not transpire with an introduction to type

for the first year college student, dependent upon where they are in their own personal

identity development. This breach in understanding could be a disadvantage for future

research in distinguishing participants who find value in the instrument.

Incongruence for this research includes relating existing theory for MBTI preferences

and academic success with a trend toward specific type preferences in participants who are

not successful in the first college year. As the Myers-Briggs assessment is not intended as a

predictor of academic success, but rather as a knowledge base for measuring personality style

differences, future research must be cautious in definition of results. Additionally, the

findings here may be distinct to the research population and in need of a more expansive

170

population to enhance validity of the research. Completing a longitudinal study following

the academic success, persistence, and graduation completion of students will aid in

determining if students with specific type preferences face more difficulty in their path to a

degree. Ideally, this information would provide early identification of students who may

benefit from enhanced programming to meet academic needs.

To extend upon this research, a combination of data collection and student interviews

is recommended to gather information as a case study seeking additional validation of the

correlation of MBTI preferences to academic success in college. With the questions involved

in this research, further analysis through a case study methodology would provide a valuable

a mixed methods review of quantitative and qualitative data collected and interpreted for the

research study. As in this study, MBTI assessment information for the student cohort in the

study will be gathered to establish a framework of modal type and type culture for the

research population. This information will be merged with college entry data including class

rank, ACT and grade point averages of the students in the study to examine academic

preparation of student participants and those not achieving a 2.0 in the first semester. A

comparison of type preferences and grade point will occur to assess type preferences of

students with academic difficulty and trend data of type preferences for the student cohort.

Case study would enable analysis of the potential links of psychological type and

academic success in a general context that allows new questions to unfold for future research.

Assessing the student satisfaction and understanding of type comprehension would enrich the

opportunities for creating adaptive learning style programming. Formal and informal

interviews could follow the seminar course type assessment to establish student

understanding of type preferences and if this understanding connects to potential for

171

academic success. The students may be selected for interview based upon their assessed

MBTI preferences to provide a cross-section of type. Those students with over-represented

type preferences in comparison to the type population may provide researcher understanding

of MBTI type in addition to questions regarding their student satisfaction and motivation.

Student opinion, personal data, and information related to the context of the study may allow

type understanding to emerge.

In a conceptual framework of this evaluation, the dichotomies of psychological type,

learning styles, and influences of college-going and academic success must be considered.

Collecting archival data for clarity strength score of type preference and type pair samples

measured against a national sample would provide valuable measurement of the type outliers,

a source for this research. Additional areas for expansion of the research include dissection

of type preferences by STEM major, including a breakout by male and female students;

examination of type and ethnic diversity of the research population or additional student

populations; and further type comparision to socioeconomic and financial need status of

participants.

Assessments for future research comparison with these research results include the

Cooperative Institutional Research Program Freshman Survey (CIRP) collection of pre-

college information; College Student Inventory (CSI), a pre-college early alert survey; and

the Making Achievement Possible Works (MAP-Works) early alert mechanism. The

students in the research population each participated in one or more of these surveys and

additional opportunities for measurement may offer validity to the results of this study. An

MBTI comparison to the Kolb Learning Style Inventory (LSI) as described by Salter, Evans,

and Forney (2004) may also be a direction for additional research.

172

The measurement of the learning style and retention study completed by Kalsbeek

(2003) assessed several variables not included in this study; learning style related to program

of study or college major, strength score of MBTI preference dichotomies, and grade point at

upper level classification. Each of these components could add additional validity to the

inferences of the study and become areas for further research. Additionally, increased

sample size, particularly for students with low academic achievement in the first semester,

will aid in understanding the influence of the MBTI instrument on this group.

The St. Louis University TRAILS Project (Tracking Retention and Academic

Integration by Learning Style), (Kalsbeek, 2003) was an impetus for this research effort to

collect Myers-Briggs assessment data for intuitional persistence studies. The study is

important as it highlights the value of incorporating type knowledge for the larger population

of students entering the university. Although Kalsbeek did not describe a type facilitation

module in the TRAILS Project, implementation of the Myers-Briggs assessment across the

campus should include an interpretation function for all student participants. As personal

understanding of preferences and adaptation within type incongruence is the responsibility of

the individual, it is essential that this instruction be included in the retention equation.

Finally, the development of type learning modules that include type-alike peer mentoring to

address the needs of ENFP learners and others facing academic difficulty could be a

financially productive and efficient retention bridge for gaps in student persistence on our

university campuses.

173

Validity challenges

Several challenges were faced in this study to find a correlation of MBTI preferences

as a gauge of academic success in college. Collection of the data and involvement with the

participants may not have been available for a significant period of time to justify the study.

As the type preferences of each student cohort are collected once annually in the fall

semester, only eight cohort populations have completed the MBTI assessment for review.

While that allowed analysis for 775 students, only 13.5% (n = 105) in those eight years have

achieved less than a 2.00 grade point in the first semester.

Student satisfaction measurements of how the type assessment meets learning

outcomes of the first-year seminar course are collected each fall semester. However, these

surveys do not adequately measure if learning and understanding of psychological type has

occurred. Questions remain as to whether the study participants have sufficient

understanding of psychological type preferences or validation of the topic to determine a link

to academic success. Not all students will have the same acceptance and appreciation of type

principles based upon their first type assessment. Much of this will be dependent upon where

the students are in their personal identity development. Additionally, there are considerably

more factors beyond psychological type that may be indicators of student difficulty in the

first college year. Astin and Oseguera (2012) assert that degree completion is complex and

affected by a multitude of pre-college, environmental and institutional characteristics.

However, determining if type is a factor for early-identification of academic difficulty may

be a method to enhance intrusive first-year advising and retention. Determining if the data

collected on type preferences are true anomalies is key to the validity of this research.

Additional review of the type preference relationships will be required along with the

174

identification of additional variables leading to academic difficulty in the participants. These

associations can be helpful to campus retention efforts by explaining college entry data and

academic success related to MBTI type and learning style.

As preferences of psychological type are equal in their validity and strength to the

individual student, this understanding can be a key for easing the transition to university

learning. Future research will seek to confirm these questions with a goal to develop

adaptive programming aimed at increasing student success and persistence to graduation.

Summary

This study was conducted to examine if student Myers-Briggs preferences correlate to

academic success in the first college semester. Descriptive and inferential statistics, and

standard multiple regression were used to analyze the relationship between first semester

grade point and variables of gender, STEM major, and academic aptitude. Results indicated

that there are significant differences of type preferences for the research population in

relationship to the national base sample. Additionally, while ACT and percentile class rank

were found to positively predict grade point, Myers-Briggs preferences were found to be

positive and negative influences on first semester grade point. Ideally, this information

validates that type correlation merits further research for the early identification of students

requiring academic intervention.

Tinto (2012) postulated that, while institutions have focused funding on retention,

little has been done to change the college classroom and the student experience in the

classroom. He asserted that any gains in retention and graduation rates must begin with

student success. Identification and understanding of type preferences may be one approach

175

to support student learning differences and academic success. This study produced indicators

suggesting that Myers-Briggs preferences, particularly preferences for ENFP, Extraverted

Intuition with Feeling and Perceiving, may impact student academic progress for this

population. Identifying a trend toward specific type preferences related to academic

achievement may provide support for the student population in this research. These data

have important implications for college administrators who wish to make an impact on

student persistence to graduation.

176

APPENDIX A. HUMAN SUBJECTS APPROVAL

IOWA STATE UNIVERSITY OF SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY

Date: 7/13/2012 To: Debra Sanborn CC: Dr. Daniel Robinson 1080 Hixson-Lied Student Success Center N247 Lagomarcino Perry, GA 31069 Dr. Larry Ebbers

N256 Lagomarcino From: Office for Responsible Research

Title: MBTI Preferences and Academic Success in the First College Semester

IRB ID: 12-339

Approval Date: 7/12/2012 Date for Continuing Review: 7/11/2014

Submission Type: New Review Type: Expedited

The project referenced above has received approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB} at Iowa State University according to the dates shown above. Please refer to the IRB ID number shown above in all correspondence regarding this study.

To ensure compliance with federal regulations (45 CFR 46 & 21 CFR 56), please be sure to:

• Use only the approved study materials in your research, including the recruitment materials and informed consent documents that have the IRB approval stamp.

• Retain signed informed consent documents for 3 years after the close of the study, when documented consent is required.

• Obtain IRB approval prior to implementing any changes to the study by submitting a Modification Form for Non-Exempt Research or Amendment for Personnel Changes form, as necessary.

• Immediately inform the IRB of (1) all serious and/or unexpected adverse experiences involving risks to subjects or others; and (2) any other unanticipated problems involving risks to subjects or others.

• Stop all research activity if IRB approval lapses, unless continuation is necessary to prevent harm to research participants. Research activity can resume once IRB approval is reestablished.

• Complete a new continuing review form at least three to four weeks prior to the date for continuing review as noted above to provide sufficient time for the IRB to review and approve continuation of the study. We will send a courtesy reminder as this date approaches.

Please be aware that IRB approval means that you have met the requirements of federal regulations and ISU policies

governing human subjects research. Approval from other entities may also be needed. For example, access to data from private records (e.g. student, medical, or employment records, etc.) that are protected by FERPA, HIPAA, or other confidentiality policies requires permission from the holders of those records. Similarly, for research conducted in institutions other than ISU (e.g., schools, other colleges or universities, medical facilities, companies, etc.), investigators must obtain permission from the institution(s) as required by their policies. IRB approval in no way implies or guarantees that permission from these other entities will be granted.

Upon completion of the project, please submit a Project Closure Form to the Office for Responsible Research, 1138 Pearson

Hall, to officially close the project. Please don’t hesitate to contact us if you have questions or concerns at 515-294-4566 or [email protected].

Institutional Review Board Office of Responsible Research Vice President for Research 1138 Pearson Hall Ames, Iowa 50011-2207

515 294-4566 FAX 515 294-4267

177

APPENDIX B. MBTI SAMPLE ITEMS

Your answers will help show you how you like to look at things and how you like to go about deciding things. There are no “right” and “wrong” answers to these questions. Knowing your own preferences and learning about other people’s can help you understand what your strengths are, what kinds of work you might enjoy, and how people with different preferences can relate to one another and contribute to society.

Part I: Which answer comes closest to telling how you usually feel or act? 16. Are you inclined to

A. value sentiment more than logic, or B. value logic more than sentiment?

20. Do you prefer to A. arrange dates, parties, etc., well in advance, or B. be free to do whatever looks like fun when the time comes?

Part II: Which word in each pair appeals to you more? Think about what the words mean, not about how they look or sound.

36. A. systematic B. casual

58. A. sensible B. fascinating

Part III: Which answer comes closest to describing how you usually feel or act? 59. When you start a big project that is due in a week, do you !

A. take time to list the separate things to be done and the order of doing them, or ! B. plunge right in?

67. At parties do you A. do much of the talking, or B. let others do most of the talking?

Part IV: Which word in each pair appeals to you more? Think about what words mean, not about how they look or how they sound.

79. A. imaginative B. realistic

91. A. devoted ! B. determined The sample items listed above were taken from the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator® Form M

Item Booklet, by Katharine C. Briggs and Isabel Briggs Myers, copyright 1998 by Peter B Myers and Katharine D. Myers. All rights are reserved. Further reproduction is prohibited without written consent of the publisher, CPP, Inc.

MBTI, Myers-Briggs, and Myers-Briggs Type Indicator are trademarks or registered

trademarks of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Trust in the United States and other countries.

178

REFERENCES

Allen, E. (2007). Integrating learning types and cognitive taxonomies: Wedding two classic models. Journal of Psychological Type, 67(7), 59-70.

Alaoutinen, S., Heikkinen, K., & Porras, J. (2012). Experiences of learning styles in an intensive collaborative course. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 22(1), 25-49.

Astin, A. (1984). Student involvement: A developmental theory for higher education. Journal of College Student Personnel, 25(3), 297–308.

Astin, A.W., & Oseguera, L. (2012). Pre-college and institutional influences on degree attainment. In A. Seidman (Ed.), College student retention (2nd ed), pp. 119-145. Lanham, MD: Bowman and Littlefield.

Barrineau, P. (2005). Personality types among undergraduates who withdraw from liberal arts colleges. Journal of Psychological Type, 65(4), 27-32.

Beckham, M. H. (2012). Building momentum: The unconventional strengths of perceiving college students. Journal of Psychological Type, 72(2), 27-40.

Campbell, D.P, Borgen, F. H, Eastes, S. H., Johansson, C. B., & Peterson, R.A. (1968). A set of basic interest scales for the Strong Vocational Interest Blank for Men. Journal of Applied Psychology Monograph, 52(6), 1-54.

Campbell, D. P, & Holland, J. L. (1972). A merger in vocational interest research: Applying Holland’s theory to Strong’s data. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 2, 353-376.

Campbell, F. B., & White, J. Z. (2005). Psychological type and minority student success in undergraduate social work programs. Journal of Psychological Type, 69(4), 50-62.

Capraro, R. M., & Capraro, M. M. (2002). Myers-Briggs type Indicator score reliability across studies: A meta-analytic reliability generalization study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62(4), 590-602.

Chesborough, S. (2005). Do athletes learn differently? Implications for improving the learning environment for athletes. Journal of Psychological Type, 64(4), 31-40.

Chesborough, S. (2009). Do social work students learn differently? MBTI implications for teaching strategies that address social work students’ current learning styles. Journal of Psychological Type, 69(2), 23-41.

Creswell, J. W. (2009). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (3rd ed). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

179

De Veaux, R. D., Velleman, P. F., & Bock, D. E. (2012). Stats: Data and models with statistical methods for the social sciences (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

DiRienzo, C., Das, J., Synn, W., Kitts, J., & McGrath, K. (2010). The relationship between MBTI and academic performance: A study across academic disciplines. Journal of Psychological Type, 70(5), 53-67.

DiTiberio, J. K., & Jammer, A. L. (1993). Introduction to type in college. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Dryden, G., & Vos, J. (2005). The new learning revolution: How Britain can lead the world in learning, education, and schooling. Stafford, U.K.: Network Education Press.

Dunning, D. (2008). Introduction to type and learning. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Erickson, B. L., & Strommer, D. W. (2005). Inside the first-year classroom. In Upcraft, M. L. Gardner, J. N. & Barefoot, B. O. (Eds.), Challenging and supporting the first-year student. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Evans, N. J., Forney, D. S., Guido, F. M., Patton, L. D., & Renn, K. A. (2010). Student development in college: Theory, research and practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

Haller, C. S., & Courvoisier, D. S. (2010). Personality and thinking style in different creative domains. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(3), 149-160.

Hammer, A. L. (Ed.). (1996). MBTI Applications: A decade of research on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Hill, A. B. (2006). Developmental student achievement: The personality factor. Journal of Psychological Type, 66(9), 79-87.

Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Research.

Hunter, M. S. (2006). Lessons learned: Achieving institutional change in support of students in transition. New Directions for Student Services, 2006(114), 7-15.

Jensen, G. H. (2003). Learning styles. In J. A. Provost & S. Anchors (Eds.), Using the MBTI instrument in colleges and universities. Gainesville, FL: Center for Applications of Psychological Type.

Jung, C. G. (1971). Psychological Types. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

180

Kalsbeek, D. H. (1986, June). Linking learning style theory with retention research: The TRAILS project. Paper presented at the Association for Institutional Research forum, Orlando FL.

Kalsbeek, D. H. (2003). Campus retention: The MBTI instrument in institutional self-studies. In J. A. Provost & S. Anchors (Eds.), Using the MBTI instrument in colleges and universities (pp. 87-122). Gainesville, FL: Center for Applications of Psychological Type.

Keup, J. R. (2006). Promoting new-student success: Assessing academic development and achievement among first-year students. New Directions for Student Services, 2006(114), 27-46.

Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Lawrence, G. D. (2009). Type, motivation and learning style. In People types and tiger stripes (4th ed), pp. 33-46. Gainesville, FL: Center for Applications of Psychological Type.

Macdaid, G. P. (2003). Research approaches using the MBTI instrument. In J. A. Provost & S. Anchors (Eds.), Using the MBTI instrument in colleges and universities (pp. 405-438). Gainesville, FL: Center for Applications of Psychological Type.

Maxwell, J. A. (2005). Qualitative research design: An interpretive approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

McCaulley, M. H. (1985). The selection ratio type table: A research strategy for comparing type distributions. Journal of Psychological Type, 10, 46-56.

Myers, I. B. (1980). Gifts differing. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Myers, I. B., McCaulley, M. H. Quenk, N. L., & Hammer, A. L. (1998). MBTI manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Nardi, D. (2001). Multiple intelligences and personality type: Tools and strategies for developing human potential. Huntington Beach, CA: Telos.

Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (2005). How college affects students: A third decade of research (2nd ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Pittenger, D. J. (2005). Cautionary comments regarding the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 57(3), 210-221.

181

Reason, R. D., Terenzini, P. T., & Domingo, R.J. (2006). First things first: Developing academic competence in the first year of college. Research in Higher Education, 47(2), 149-175.

Reynierse, J. H., & Harker, J. B. (2005). Type interactions: MBTI relationships and self-report questionnaire scale scores. Journal of Psychological Type, 64(6a), 57-75.

Robinson, D. C., & Taylor, K. A. (2003). Diverse learners in the 21st century . In J. A. Provost & S. Anchors (Eds.), Using the MBTI instrument in colleges and universities (pp. 157-202). Gainesville, FL: Center for Applications of Psychological Type.

Salter, D. W., Evans, N. J., & Forney, D. S. (2004). A longitudinal study of learning style preferences on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and Learning Style Inventory. Journal of College Student Development, 47(2), 173-184.

Schaubhut, N. A., & Thompson, R. C. (2011). MBTI type tables for college majors. Mountain View, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Seidman, A. (2012). Taking action: A retention formula and model for student success. In A. Seidman (Ed.), College student retention (2nd ed), pp. 267-284. Lanham, MD: Bowman and Littlefield.

Stricker, L. J., Schiffman, H., & Ross, J. (1965). Prediction of college perfromance with the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 25(4), 1081-1095.

Svinicki, M. D. (2004). Learning and motivation in the postsecondary classroom. Bolton, MA: Anker.

Tabachnick, B. G., Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

Tinto, V. (2012). Completing college: Rethinking institutional action. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Tinto, V. (1975). Dropout from higher education: A theoretical synthesis of recent research. Review of Educational Research, 43, 89-125.

VanSant, S. S. (2003). Wired for conflict: The role of personality in resolving differences. Gainesville, FL: Center for Applications of Psychological Type.

Whalen, D., & Saunders, K., & Shelley, M. (2010). Leveraging what we know to enhance short-term and long-term retention of university students. Journal of College Student Retention ,11(3), 407-430.

182

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Throughout my doctoral program, I have enjoyed encouragement, assistance, and

support from numerous individuals making this a memorable journey. Each person kept me

focused, inspired, and confident that I could pursue and complete my Ph.D.

Most notable is Professor Daniel Robinson, who pointed me forward and kept me

challenged. Dan instilled in me a love for type and inspired this research. Professors Larry

Ebbers, Mack Shelley, Sharon Drake, and Loren Zachary provided encouragement and

motivation, and never waivered in their belief that I could complete this process.

To my parents, Cheryl and Jack Kimberley—Thank you for your love, support, and

for teaching me the value of education. You always encouraged me to pursue my dreams,

and celebrated my successes.

To my sister, Kricket, and her family—Thank you for your love and support, for

thinking differently, and keeping me grounded. We are unique in our talents, but share the

same goals for life and family.

Special thanks to Allison Severson-Haban—Grateful for your ISTJ; Dr. Penny

Rosenthal—You made it look easy; Japannah Kellogg—For talking me out of the trees; and

Dr. Mary Jo Gonzales—For reminding me to breathe. To my colleagues in the Dean of

Students Office—Thank you for your many words of reinforcement and helpful insight.

Special appreciation is offered to my multiple cohorts of fellow graduate students—

Your support, good humor, and knowledge have been immeasurable. I look forward to

tracking your future successes. Thank you to the Iowa State Office of the Registrar for their

help in database constuction. Thank you to my editor, Pat Hahn. Many thanks to the

183

students of the Hixson Program for always reaching for the stars. And for the many

opportunitunities they have provided, Liz Beck, David Bousquet, Marc Harding, and Kent

McElvania, I offer my gratitude.

Most importantly, thank you to my family. This effort would not have been possible

without the love, support, and patience of my husband and partner, Chad, and our children,

Delaney and Deckard. I am inspired by each of you and fortunate to have you as my family.

This dissertation and degree are as much yours as they are mine, and I hope they serve as a

reminder to always treasure your education adventures. To the Batcave!