neurobiological foundations of an attribute model of memory€¦ · neurobiological foundations 29...

31
Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University of Utah Memory is a complex phenomenon due to a large number of potential interactions that are associated with the organiza- tion of memory at the psychological and neural system level. In this review article a tripartite, multiple attribute, multiple process memory model with different forms of memory and its neurobiological underpinnings is represented in terms of the nature, structure or content of information representation as a set of different attributes including language, time, place, response, reward value (affect) and visual object as an example of sensory-perception. For each attribute, information is processed in the event-based, knowledge-based, and rule-based memory systems through multiple operations that involve multiple neural underpinnings. Of the many processes associated with the event-based memory system, the emphasis will be placed on short-term or working memory and pattern separation. Of the many processes associated with the knowledge- based memory system, the emphasis will be placed on perceptual processes. Of the many processes associated with the rule-based memory system the emphasis will be on short-term or working memory and paired associate learning. For all three systems data will be presented to demonstrate differential neuroanatomical mediation and where available parallel results will be presented in rodents, monkeys and humans. Keywords: event-based memory, knowledge-based memory, rule-based memory, attribute memory model, hippocampus, amygdala, caudate nucleus, perirhinal cortex, prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex and TE2 cortex Introduction The structure and utilization of memory is central to one’s knowledge of the past, interpretation of the present, and prediction of the future. Therefore, the understanding of the structural and process components of memory systems at the psychological and neurobiological level is of paramount importance. There have been a number of attempts to di- vide learning and memory into multiple memory systems. Schacter & Tulving (1994) have suggested that one needs to define memory systems in terms of the kind of informa- tion to be represented, the processes associated with the op- eration of each system, and the neurobiological substrates including neural structures and mechanisms that subserve each system. Furthermore, it is likely that within each sys- tem, there are multiple forms or subsystems associated with each memory system and there are likely to be multiple pro- cesses that define the operation of each system. Finally, there are probably multiple neural structures that form the overall substrate of a memory system. Currently, the most established models of memory can Contact information: Raymond Kesner, Department of Psychol- ogy, University of Utah, Salt Lake City , Utah. Phone: (801) 5817430. Volume 8, pp 29 - 59 2013 ISSN: 1911-4745 doi: 10.3819/ccbr.2013.80003 © Raymond Kesner 2013 be characterized as dual memory system models with an emphasis on the hippocampus or medial temporal lobe for one component of the model and a composite of other brain structures as the other component. For example, Squire (1994) and Squire et al.(2004) have proposed that memory can be divided into a medial temporal-lobe-dependent de- clarative memory which provides for conscious recollection of facts and events and a non-hippocampal dependent non- declarative memory which provides for memory without conscious access for skills, habits, priming, simple classi- cal conditioning and non-associative learning. Others have used different terms to reflect the same type of distinction, including a hippocampal dependent explicit memory versus a non-hippocampal dependent implicit memory (Schacter, 1987), and a hippocampal dependent declarative memory based on the representation of relationships among stimuli versus a non-hippocampal dependent procedural memory based on the representation of a single stimulus or configu- ration of stimuli (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993; Eichenbaum, 2004). Olton (1983) has suggested a different dual memory system in which memory can be divided into a hippocam- pal dependent working memory defined as memory for the specific, personal, and temporal context of a situation and a

Upload: others

Post on 13-Aug-2020

21 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 29

Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of MemoryRaymond Kesner

University of Utah

Memory is a complex phenomenon due to a large number of potential interactions that are associated with the organiza-tion of memory at the psychological and neural system level. In this review article a tripartite, multiple attribute, multiple process memory model with different forms of memory and its neurobiological underpinnings is represented in terms of the nature, structure or content of information representation as a set of different attributes including language, time, place, response, reward value (affect) and visual object as an example of sensory-perception. For each attribute, information is processed in the event-based, knowledge-based, and rule-based memory systems through multiple operations that involve multiple neural underpinnings. Of the many processes associated with the event-based memory system, the emphasis will be placed on short-term or working memory and pattern separation. Of the many processes associated with the knowledge-based memory system, the emphasis will be placed on perceptual processes. Of the many processes associated with the rule-based memory system the emphasis will be on short-term or working memory and paired associate learning. For all three systems data will be presented to demonstrate differential neuroanatomical mediation and where available parallel results will be presented in rodents, monkeys and humans.

Keywords: event-based memory, knowledge-based memory, rule-based memory, attribute memory model, hippocampus, amygdala, caudate nucleus, perirhinal cortex, prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex and TE2 cortex

Introduction The structure and utilization of memory is central to one’s knowledge of the past, interpretation of the present, and prediction of the future. Therefore, the understanding of the structural and process components of memory systems at the psychological and neurobiological level is of paramount importance. There have been a number of attempts to di-vide learning and memory into multiple memory systems. Schacter & Tulving (1994) have suggested that one needs to define memory systems in terms of the kind of informa-tion to be represented, the processes associated with the op-eration of each system, and the neurobiological substrates including neural structures and mechanisms that subserve each system. Furthermore, it is likely that within each sys-tem, there are multiple forms or subsystems associated with each memory system and there are likely to be multiple pro-cesses that define the operation of each system. Finally, there are probably multiple neural structures that form the overall substrate of a memory system.

Currently, the most established models of memory can Contact information: Raymond Kesner, Department of Psychol-ogy, University of Utah, Salt Lake City , Utah. Phone: (801) 5817430.

Volume 8, pp 29 - 592013

ISSN: 1911-4745 doi: 10.3819/ccbr.2013.80003 © Raymond Kesner 2013

be characterized as dual memory system models with an emphasis on the hippocampus or medial temporal lobe for one component of the model and a composite of other brain structures as the other component. For example, Squire (1994) and Squire et al.(2004) have proposed that memory can be divided into a medial temporal-lobe-dependent de-clarative memory which provides for conscious recollection of facts and events and a non-hippocampal dependent non-declarative memory which provides for memory without conscious access for skills, habits, priming, simple classi-cal conditioning and non-associative learning. Others have used different terms to reflect the same type of distinction, including a hippocampal dependent explicit memory versus a non-hippocampal dependent implicit memory (Schacter, 1987), and a hippocampal dependent declarative memory based on the representation of relationships among stimuli versus a non-hippocampal dependent procedural memory based on the representation of a single stimulus or configu-ration of stimuli (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993; Eichenbaum, 2004). Olton (1983) has suggested a different dual memory system in which memory can be divided into a hippocam-pal dependent working memory defined as memory for the specific, personal, and temporal context of a situation and a

Page 2: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 30

non-hippocampal dependent reference memory, defined as memory for rules and procedures (general knowledge) of specific situations. Different terms have been used to re-flect the same distinction including episodic versus semantic memory (Tulving, 1983).

However, memory is more complex and involves many neural systems in addition to the hippocampus. To remedy this situation, Kesner (2002) has proposed a tripartite attri-bute based theoretical model of memory which is organized into event-based, knowledge-based, and rule-based memory systems. Each system is composed of the same set of mul-tiple attributes or forms of memory, characterized by a set of process oriented operating characteristics and mapped onto multiple neural regions and interconnected neural circuits (for more detail see Kesner 1998, 2002).

On a psychological level (see Tables 1, 2, 3), the event-based memory system provides for temporary represen-tations of incoming data concerning the present, with an emphasis upon data and events that are usually personal or egocentric and that occur within specific external and inter-nal contexts. The emphasis is upon the processing of new and current information. During initial learning, great em-phasis is placed on the event-based memory system, which will continue to be of importance even after initial learning in situations where unique or novel trial information needs to be remembered. This system is akin to episodic memory (Tulving, 1983) and some aspects of declarative memory (Squire, 1994).

Table 1. Event-Based Memory

Table 2. Knowledge-Based Memory

Attribute

Language

Time

Place

Response

Reward Value(Affect)

Sensory-Perceptione.g. Visual Object

(M=Monkey, R=Rat, H=Human)

Process Charecteristics:

1) Selective filtering or attenuation of interference of temporary memory2) Encoding new information3) Short-term and intermediate-term memory for new information4) Consolidation or elaborative rehearsal of new information5) Retrieval of familiar information based on flexibility, action, and p attern completion

Neural Substrates

Hippocampus (H)

Hippocampus (M,H,R)

Hippocampus (M,H,R)

Caudate (M,H,R)

Amygdala (M,H,R)

Perirhinal Cortex (M,H,R)

Features

PhonologicalLexicalMorphological

DurationTemporal OrderTime Perspective (Past)

Spatial DistanceSpatial DirectionSpatial Location

Egocentric (feedback from motor responses)Response Selection

Reward Value (Positive-negative)

Height, Color, Shape,

Orientation, Motion, Contrast

The knowledge-based memory system provides for more permanent representations of previously stored information in long-term memory and can be thought of as one’s gen-eral knowledge of the world. The knowledge-based mem-ory system would tend to be of greater importance after a task has been learned, given that the situation is invariant and familiar. The organization of these attributes within the knowledge-based memory system can take many forms and are organized as a set of attribute-dependent cognitive maps and their interactions, that are unique for each memory. This system is akin to semantic memory (Tulving, 1983).

Attribute

Language

Time

Place

Response

Reward Value(Affect)

Sensory-Perceptione.g. Visual Object

(M=Monkey, R=Rat, H=Human)

Process Charecteristics:

1) Encolding of new information2) Selective attention and selective filtering associated with permanent memory representations of familiar information3) Perceptual memory4) Consolidation and long-term memory storage partly based on arbitrary and/or pattern associations5) Retrieval of familiar information based on flexibility and action

Neural Substrates

Posterior Parietal Cortex,Wernicke’s AreaBrocha’s Area (H)

Dorsal Prefrontal Cortex (M,H)Anterior Cingulate (R)

Parietal Cortex (M,H,R)

Pre-Motor Cortex, Supplementary Motor Cortex (M,H)Precentral Cortex (R)Cerebellum (M,H,R)

Orbital-Frontal Cortex (M,H,R)

Inferotemporal Cortex (M,H)TF 2 Cortex (R)

Features

SyntaxSemanticsLexicon

DurationTemporal OrderTime Perspective (Future)

Spatial DistanceSpatial DirectionSpatial Location

Egocentric (feedback from motor responses)Response Selection

Reward Value (Positive-negative)

Height, Color, Shape,

Orientation, Motion, Contrast

The rule-based memory system receives information from the event-based and knowledge-based systems and inte-grates the information by applying rules and strategies for subsequent action. In most situations, however, one would expect a contribution of all three systems with a varying pro-portion of involvement of one relative to the other.

The three memory systems are composed of the same forms, domains, or attributes of memory. Even though there could be many attributes, the most important attributes in-clude space, time, response, sensory-perception, and reward value (affect). In humans a language attribute is also added. A spatial (space) attribute within this framework involves memory representations of places or relationships between places. It is exemplified by the ability to encode and remem-ber spatial maps and to localize stimuli in external space. Memory representations of the spatial attribute can be fur-ther subdivided into specific spatial features including al-

Page 3: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 31

locentric spatial distance, egocentric spatial distance, allo-centric direction, egocentric direction, and spatial location. A temporal (time) attribute within this framework involves memory representations of the duration of a stimulus and the succession or temporal order of temporally separated events or stimuli, and from a time perspective, the memory repre-sentation of the past. A response attribute within this frame-work involves memory representations based on feedback from motor responses (often based on proprioceptive and vestibular cues) that occur in specific situations as well as memory representations of stimulus-response associations. A reward value (affect) attribute within this framework in-volves memory representations of reward value, positive or negative emotional experiences, and the associations be-tween stimuli and rewards. A sensory-perceptual attribute within this framework involves memory representations of a set of sensory stimuli that are organized in the form of cues as part of a specific experience. Each sensory modality (olfaction, auditory, vision, somatosensory, and taste) can be considered part of the sensory-perceptual attribute compo-nent of memory. A language attribute within this framework involves memory representations of phonological, lexical, morphological, syntactical, and semantic information. The attributes within each memory system can be organized in many different ways and are likely to interact extensively with each other, even though it can be demonstrated that these attributes do in many cases operate independent of each other. The organization of these attributes within the event-based memory system can take many forms and are probably organized hierarchically and in parallel. The or-ganization of these attributes within the knowledge-based

memory system can take many forms, are assumed to be or-ganized as a set of cognitive maps or neural nets, and their interactions are unique for each memory. It is assumed that long-term representations within cognitive maps are more abstract and less dependent upon specific features. The or-ganization of these attributes within the rule-based memory system can also take many forms; these are assumed to be organized to provide flexibility in executive function in de-veloping rules, development of goals, and affecting decision processes.

Within each system attribute, information is processed in different ways based on different operational characteristics. For the event-based memory system, specific processes in-volve (a) selective filtering or attenuation of interference of temporary memory representations of new information and is labeled pattern separation, (b) encoding of new informa-tion, (c) short-term and intermediate-term memory for new information, (d) the establishment of arbitrary associations, (e) consolidation or elaborative rehearsal of new informa-tion, and (f) retrieval of new information based on flexibility, action, and pattern completion.

For the knowledge-based memory system, specific pro-cesses include (a) encoding of new information, (b) selective attention and selective filtering associated with permanent memory representations of familiar information, (c) percep-tual memory, (d) consolidation and long-term memory stor-age partly based on arbitrary and/or pattern associations, and (e) retrieval of familiar information based on flexibility and action.

For the rule-based memory system, it is assumed that in-formation is processed through the integration of informa-tion from the event-based and knowledge-based memory systems for the use of major processes that include the selec-tion of strategies and rules for maintaining or manipulating information for subsequent decision making and action, as well as short-term or working memory for new and familiar information, development of goals, and affecting decision processes.

On a neurobiological level (see Tables 1, 2, 3) each at-tribute maps onto a set of neural regions and their intercon-nected neural circuits. For example, within the event-based memory system, it has been demonstrated that in animals and humans (a) the hippocampus supports memory for spa-tial, temporal and language attribute information, (b) the caudate mediates memory for response attribute informa-tion, (c) the amygdala subserves memory for reward value (affect) attribute information, and (d) the perirhinal and ex-trastriate visual cortex support memory for visual object at-tribute information as an example of a sensory-perceptual attribute and the ventral hippocampus supports memory for

Attribute

Language

Time

Place

Response

Reward Value(Affect)

Sensory-Perceptione.g. Visual Object

(M=Monkey, R=Rat, H=Human)

Process Charecteristics:

1) The selection of strategies and rules for maintaining or manipulating informaiton for decision making and subsequent action2) Short-term or working memory for new and familiar information

Neural Substrates

Lateral Prefrontal Cortex

Dorsal Prefrontal Cortex (M,H)Anterior Cingulate (R)

Lateral Prefrontal Cortex (M,H)Infra and Prelimbic Cortex (R)

Pre-Motor Cortex, Supplementary Motor Cortex (M,H)Precentral Cortex (R)Cerebellum (M,H,R)

Orbital-Frontal Cortex (M,H,R)Agranular Insular Cortex (R)

Lateral Prefrontal Cortex (M,H)Infra and Prelimbic Cortex (R)

Features

SyntaxSemanticsPhonologicalLexicalMorphological

DurationTemporal OrderTime Perspective (Future)

Spatial DistanceSpatial DirectionSpatial Location

Egocentric (feedback from motor responses)Response Selection

Reward Value (Positive-negative)

Height, Color, Shape,

Orientation, Motion, Contrast

Table 3. Rule-Based Memory

Page 4: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 32

odor information as another example of a sensory –percep-tual attribute (for more detail see Kesner, 1998, 2002).

Within the knowledge-based memory system, it has been demonstrated that in animals and humans (a) the posterior parietal cortex supports memory for spatial attributes, (b) the dorsal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and/or anterior cingulate support memory for temporal attributes, (c) the premotor, supplementary motor, and cerebellum in mon-keys and humans and precentral cortex and cerebellum in rats support memory for response attributes, (d) the orbital prefrontal cortex supports memory for reward value (affect) attributes, (e) the inferotemporal cortex in monkeys and hu-mans and TE2 cortex in rats subserves memory for sensory-perceptual attributes (e.g. visual objects), and (f) the parietal cortex, Broca and Wernicke’s areas subserve memory for the language attribute (for more detail see Kesner, 1998, 2002). Within the rule-based memory system it can be shown that different subdivisions of the prefrontal cortex support differ-ent attributes. For example,( a) the dorso-lateral and ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex in humans support spatial, object, and language attributes and the infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in rats supports spatial and visual object attributes, (b) the pre-motor and supplementary motor cortex in monkeys and humans and precentral cortex in rats support response attributes, (c) the dorsal, dorso-lateral, and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys and humans and anterior cin-gulate in rats mediate primarily temporal attributes, and (d) the orbital prefrontal cortex in monkeys and humans and agranular insular cortex in rats support affect attributes (for more detail see Kesner, 2000, 2002).

Given the complexity of the nature of memory representa-tions and the multitude of processes associated with learning and memory associated with any specific task, it is clear that prior to analyzing the neural circuits that support mnemonic processing, one must determine which attributes and which systems and associated underlying processes are essential for memory analysis of the proposed task. One example will suffice. If one assumes that the hippocampus supports the processing of the spatial attribute within the event-based memory system, then any task that minimizes the impor-tance of the spatial attribute and emphasizes the importance of reward value, response, and sensory-perceptual attributes are not likely to involve the hippocampus. I will concen-trate in this article primarily on specific processes for which there are sufficient data to determine the role of the neuro-biological attribute-based model of memory for the event-base, knowledge-based, and rule-based components of the attribute model.

Event-Based Memory

For the event-based memory system I will concentrate on

specific processes that mediate short-term memory for new information and a selective filtering or attenuation of inter-ference of temporary memory representations of new infor-mation which is labeled pattern separation. For the other processes including the establishment of arbitrary associa-tions, consolidation or elaborative rehearsal of new informa-tion, and retrieval of new information based on flexibility, action, and pattern completion there is not a sufficient data set to differentiate the contribution of the different attributes associated with mnemonic processing of information.

Short-term or Working Memory -- Spatial Attribute

The most extensive data set is based on the use of para-digms that measure the short-term or working memory pro-cess such as matching or non-matching-to-sample, delayed conditional discrimination, continuous recognition memory of single or lists of items, and recognition memory based on exploratory information and detection of novelty. Figure 1 depicts the location of the hippocampus in the rat. Figure 2 depicts the location of the different subregions of the hippo-campus [dentate gyrus (DG), CA3, and CA1] as well as the medial and lateral perforant path inputs from the entorhinal cortex inputs into the different subregions of the hippocam-pus in the rat.

With respect to spatial attribute information, there is ex-tensive data that show with the use of the above mentioned paradigms to measure short-term or working memory for spatial information that there are severe impairments for rats, monkeys, and humans with right hippocampal damage or bilateral hippocampal damage (Chiba, Kesner, Matsuo, & Heilbrun, 1990; Hopkins, Kesner, & Goldstein, 1995a; Kes-ner, 1990; Olton, 1983, 1986; Parkinson, Murray, & Mish-kin,1988; Pigott & Milner, 1993; Smith & Milner, 1981). To

Figure 1. Pictorial representation of the hippocampus (HF), entorhinal cortex (EC), perirhinal cortex (PER), postrhinal cortex (POR), and amygdala (AMY) in the rat.

POR

HFAMY

PEREC

Page 5: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 33

examine the temporal dynamic of hippocampal involvement in short-term and intermediate-term memory in the context of processing spatial information in humans, Holdstock, Shaw, and Aggleton (1995) tested patients with hippocampal damage with a delayed matching-to-sample paradigm analo-gous to tasks used for rats. In this task a single stimulus was presented in a specific location and following delays of 3-40 s, the patients had to remember that location compared with a location not previously seen. The results indicated that there were no memory deficits for delays up to 20 s followed by a deficit at the 40 s delay. However, Cave and Squire (1992) found no deficits for short-term memory for a dot on a line or memory for an angle. In a different experiment, hypoxic subjects with bilateral hippocampal damage were tested on a short-term memory test to determine whether the hippocam-pus supports short-term or intermediate-term memory for a spatial relationship based on distance information. Control subjects and hypoxic subjects with bilateral hippocampal damage were tested for memory for spatial distance infor-mation for delays of 1, 4, 8, 12, or 16 s. The hypoxic subjects had impaired memory for distance information at the long, but not short, delays compared to normal controls (Kesner & Hopkins, 2001).

With respect to specific spatial features, such as allocen-

tric spatial distance, egocentric spatial distance, and spatial location, it has been shown in both rats and humans with bilateral hippocampal damage that there are severe deficits in short-term memory for these spatial features (Long & Kesner, 1996). These data are consistent with the recording of place cells (cells that increase their firing rate when an animal is located in a specific place) within the hippocam-pus of rats (Kubie & Ranck, 1983; McNaughton, Barnes, & O’Keefe, 1983, O’Keefe, 1983; O’Keefe & Speakman, 1987). One specific example is provided by the use of a con-tinuous spatial recognition memory task where it has been shown that hippocampal lesions produced a profound defi-cit (Jackson-Smith, Kesner, & Chiba, 1993). However, it should be noted that lesions of the dorsal lateral thalamus, pre- and para-subiculum, medial entorhinal cortex and pre- and infra-limbic cortex produce profound deficits similar to what has been described for hippocampal lesions, suggest-ing that other neural regions contribute to the spatial attri-bute within the event-based memory system (Kesner, et al., 2001). The exact contribution of each of these areas needs to be investigated, especially because grid cells have been recorded from medial entorhinal cortex (Moser et al. 2008) and place cells have been recorded in the parasubiculum (Muller et al., 1996).

Figure 2. The Hippocampal Network: The hippocampus forms a principally uni-directional network, with input from the Entorhinal Cortex (EC) that forms connections with the Dentate Gyrus (DG) and CA3 pyramidal neurons via the Perforant Path (PP – split into lateral and medial). CA3 neurons also receive input from the DG via the mossy fibres (MF). They send axons to CA1 pyramidal cells via the Schaffer Collateral Pathway (SC), as well as to CA1 cells in the contralateral hippocampus via the Associational Commissural pathway (AC). CA1 neurons also receive input directly from the Perforant Path and send axons to the Subiculum (Sb). These neurons in turn send the main hippocampal output back to the EC, forming a loop.

AC CA3

SC/AC

MF

CA1

DG

Sb

LPP

PPII/IV

III/V

MPP

LEC

MEC

Page 6: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 34

Short-term memory for the spatial direction feature has also been investigated. Based on a delayed matching-to-sample task for assessing memory for direction in rats, it was shown that hippocampal lesions disrupt memory for di-rection (DeCoteau, Hoang, Huff, Stone, & Kesner, 2004). It should be noted that medial caudate nucleus lesions also produced an impairment in memory for direction (DeCoteau et al., 2004). Furthermore, it is likely that spatial short-term memory representations within the hippocampus might be important to amplify a subsequent consolidation process when necessary and spatial short-term memory representa-tions within the pre-and infra-limbic prefrontal cortex might be important to engage a retrieval, action or strategy selec-tion process. Thus, in general, the hippocampus represents some of the spatial features associated with the spatial at-tribute, within short-term memory.

Based on a subregional analysis of hippocampal function, it appears that different subregions subserve differential roles in spatial processing of short-term memory. For exam-ple, using a paradigm developed by Poucet (1989), rats with CA3 or CA1 lesions were tested for the detection of a novel spatial configuration of familiar objects. The results indi-cated that CA3, but not CA1, lesions disrupted novelty de-tection of a spatial location (Lee, Jerman, & Kesner, 2005b). Based on the idea that the medial perforant path input into the CA3 or CA1 mediates spatial information via activation of NMDA receptors, rats received direct infusions of AP5 into the CA3 or CA1 and were tested for the detection of a novel spatial configuration of familiar objects and the detec-tion of a novel visual object change using the same paradigm mentioned above. The results indicated that AP5 infusions into the CA3 disrupted both novelty detection of a spatial location and a visual object, whereas AP5 infusions into the CA1 disrupted novelty detection of a spatial location, but not the detection of a novel object (Hunsaker, Mooy, Swift, & Kesner, 2007). In this case, it appears the medial perforant path and the recurrent collateral system in CA3 were either actively maintaining the spatial and non-spatial information as a single behavioral episode in the network over the 3 min intersession interval or else the rich spatial context avail-able to the rats on the test session was sufficient to guide re-trieval of the previous experience to guide test performance, reflective of event-based memory processing. CA1, in the absence of recurrent circuitry, appeared to be acting directly upon the spatially rich medial perforant path inputs to re-trieve the spatial information needed to perform the test. It is of interest that CA1, as opposed to CA3, did not appear to retrieve the overall behavioral episode in this case to guide retrieval, only the spatial aspects of the experience.

In other research, Lee, Rao, and Knierim (2004) showed physiologically that plasticity mechanisms in CA3 were ac-tivated only when animals encountered novel spatial con-

figurations of familiar cues for the first time. Specifically, rats were trained to circle clockwise on a ring track whose surface was composed of four different textural cues (lo-cal cues). The ring track was positioned in the center of a curtained area in which various visual landmarks were also available along the curtained walls. To produce a novel cue configuration in the environment, distal landmarks and local cues on the track were rotated in opposite directions (distal landmarks were rotated clockwise and local cues were ro-tated counterclockwise by equal amounts). It is well known that principal cells in the hippocampus fire when the animal occupies a certain location of space, known as the place field of the cell. Mehta and colleagues (Mehta, Barnes, & Mc-Naughton, 1997; Mehta, Quirk, & Wilson, 2000) originally showed that the location of the CA1 place field (measured by the center of mass of the place field) changed over time (shifting backward opposite to the direction of rat’s mo-tion) in a familiar environment as the animal experienced the environment repeatedly. When the rats encountered the changed cue configurations for the first time in the Lee et al. (2004) experiment, the CA3 place fields shifted their loca-tions backwards prominently compared to the place fields in CA1. However, such a prominent shift was not observed in CA3 from Day 2 onwards (CA1 place fields started to ex-hibit a similar property from Day 2). This double dissocia-tion in the time course of plasticity between CA1 and CA3 place fields suggests that CA3 reacts rapidly to any changed components in the environment, presumably to incorporate the novel components into an existing event-based short-term memory system or contribute to a new representation of the environment mediated by an event-based short-term memory system if changes are significant. CA1 appears to be performing a similar function, but within an intermediate-term event-based memory system as demonstrated by the different time course than CA3, suggesting that the repre-sentation of the behavioral episode in CA1 is processed on a more lengthy timescale than in CA3. These data suggest that in some cases CA3 processes information and commu-nicates that information to CA1 via the Schaffer collateral projections. This is similar to a finding by Hampson, Hey-ser, and Deadwyler (1993) who recorded ensembles of CA3 and CA1 neurons during a spatial DNMS task. They found cells responsive to spatial location, nonspatial attributes of the task, as well as cells responsive to conjunctions of spatial and nonspatial information (called conjunctive cells in their report). What is of interest is that quite often they found activity in CA1 to be highly correlated to CA3 activity, but later in time, suggesting information transfer from CA3 to CA1.

Short-term or Working Memory -- Temporal Attribute

Memory for duration. In this section I will concentrate on memory for duration rather than the processing of time

Page 7: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 35

and time perception as measured by time estimation and time-scale invariance. Previous research has indicated that fimbria-fornix lesioned rats are impaired in remembering the duration of a stimulus across a short delay interval, even though there is only a small change in estimating the passage of time (Meck, Church, & Olton, 1984; Olton, 1986; Olton, Wenk, Church, & Meck, 1988). In an attempt to replicate these results using a different paradigm, rats were trained on a short-term memory for duration task using a delayed sym-bolic conditional discrimination procedure (Jackson-Smith et al., 1998). It had previously been shown that rats acquire high proficiency in short-term memory for duration informa-tion (Santi & Weise, 1995). In the Jackson-Smith et al. (1998) experiment, the rats had to learn that a black rectangle stimu-lus that was visible for 2 s would result in a positive (go) reinforcement for one object (a ball) and no reinforcement (no go) for a different object (a bottle). However, when the black rectangle stimulus was visible for 8 s then there would be no reinforcement for the ball (no go), but a reinforcement for the bottle (go). After rats learned to respond differential-ly in terms of latency to approach the object, they received large (dorsal and ventral) lesions of the hippocampus, medi-al prefrontal cortex (anterior cingulate) lesions, or lesions of the cortex dorsal to the dorsal hippocampus. Following re-covery from surgery they were retested. The results indicate that in contrast to cortical control lesions, there were major impairments following hippocampal lesions, as indicated by smaller and statistically non-significant latency differences between positive and negative trials on post-surgery tests. In order to ensure that the deficits observed with hippocampal lesions were not due to a discrimination problem, new rats were trained in an object (black rectangle) duration discrimi-nation task. In this situation the rats were reinforced for ei-ther a 2 or 10 s exposure (duration) of the black rectangle. The stimulus was presented and remained visible for either 2 or 10 s, following which the door was raised and latency to move the stimulus was measured. Half of the animals in each group received a piece of Froot Loop on trials with a short stimulus duration and the other half were reinforced on those trials with a long stimulus duration. After rats learned to respond differentially in terms of latency to approach the object, they received large (dorsal and ventral) lesions of the hippocampus, as well as medial prefrontal cortex lesions for comparison purposes, or lesions of cortex dorsal to the dor-sal hippocampus. Following recovery from surgery the rats were retested. The results indicate that after hippocampal le-sions there was an initial deficit followed by complete recov-ery.

Thus, the hippocampus mediates memory for duration, but does not mediate duration discrimination. The data are consistent with previous research that indicates that fimbria-fornix rats are impaired in remembering the duration of a

stimulus across a short delay interval, even though there is only a small change in estimating the passage of time (Meck, Church, & Olton, 1984; Olton, 1986; Olton et al., 1988). Furthermore, it has been suggested that trace conditioning requires memory for the duration of the conditioned stimu-lus. Thus, it is of importance to note that rabbits with hippo-campal lesions are impaired in acquisition (consolidation) of trace but not delayed eye-blink conditioning (Moyer, Deyo, & Disterhoft, 1990). Based on a subregional analysis of hip-pocampal function, it can be shown that the dorsal CA1 sup-ports object-trace odor paired associate learning (Kesner et al., 2005), but both the dorsal CA1 and CA3 support object trace-place paired associate learning (Hunsaker et al., 2006) and the ventral CA1 supports trace fear conditioning (Rog-ers et al., 2006). It should be noted that the hippocampus is not directly involved in representing memory information concerning specific objects (Bussey, Saksida, & Murray, 2002; Kesner, Bolland, & Dakis, 1993; Mumby & Pinel, 1994; Norman & Eacott, 2004).

To what extent can one generalize from hippocampal function in rats to humans with respect to memory represen-tation of duration as one feature of temporal attribute infor-mation? To answer this question, a number of experiments were conducted using humans that were exposed to hypoxia due to a variety of causes, but primarily carbon monoxide poisoning (Hopkins & Kesner, 1994; Hopkins et al., 1995a). These subjects have anterograde amnesia and, based on MRI data, have bilateral damage to the hippocampus, but no de-tectable damage to the entorhinal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, or temporal cortex. They also show no signs of pre-frontal cortex dysfunction based on normal performance on tests of fluency and Wisconsin Card sorting. The hypoxic subjects with hippocampus damage and age matched con-trols were tested for short-term memory for duration of a visual object. Subjects were presented with a single object (square, circle, etc) on a computer screen for a duration of 1 or 3 s. They were instructed to remember the duration of presentation of the object. After a delay of 1, 4, 8, 12, 16, or 20 s, the same object appeared for the same or different duration. The subjects were asked to indicate whether the duration was the same or different from the duration shown in the study phase. The results indicate that the hypoxic sub-jects were impaired relative to control damaged subjects in short-term memory for duration for all but the shortest delay (Kesner & Hopkins, 2001). In order to determine whether the deficits may have been due to impaired memory for the objects per se, a control task was administered to the same subjects. They were presented with a single object for 1 or 3 s and were asked to remember the object. After a delay of 1, 4, 8, 12, 16, or 20 s either the identical or a different object appeared on the screen. The subjects were asked if it was the same or a different object. The results indicate that there

Page 8: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 36

were minimal differences between the hypoxic and control subjects (Kesner & Hopkins, 2001). The impairment could not be due to an inability to estimate time accurately, be-cause in an additional experiment with objects the subjects were asked to estimate the time elapsed before each of the 1, 4, 8, 12, 16, 20 s delay intervals. The results indicate that for hypoxic subjects, time estimates were accurate up to 8 s followed by some underestimation with longer delays, so that short-term memory for the duration of 1 or 3 s stimulus exposure could not be due to difficulty in estimating time (Kesner & Hopkins, 2001). The process of estimating time may not require active participation of short-term memory and may, therefore, appear to be independent of short-term memory for the duration of exposure of a stimulus.

Rat data are also consistent with previous research which indicated that humans with hypoxia resulting in bilateral hippocampal damage are impaired in acquisition (consolida-tion) of trace but not delayed eye-blink conditioning (Dister-hoft, Carrillo, Hopkins, Gabrieli, & Kesner, 1996). Thus, the results suggest that like rodents, humans with hippocampal damage have difficulty in representing short-term memory for duration of an object, but not short-term memory for a single object.

Memory for sequential spatial information. In order to examine sequential learning of spatial information, a task was developed in which rats were required to remember multiple places. During the study phase, rats were presented with four different places within sections that were sequen-tially visited by opening of one door to a section at a time on a newly devised maze (i.e., Tulum maze). Each place was cued by a unique object that was specifically associated with each location within the section during the study phase. Fol-lowing a 15 s delay and during the test phase, one door to one section would be opened and in the absence of the cued object in that section, rats were required to recall and revisit the place within that section of the maze that had been pre-viously visited. Once animals were able to reliably perform this short-term episodic memory task, they received lesions to either CA3 or CA1 subregions of the hippocampus. Both CA1 and CA3 lesions disrupted accurate relocation of a pre-viously visited place (Lee et al., 2005a).

In a different task, rats learned trial-unique sequences of spatial locations along a runway box. Each trial consisted of a study phase made up of the presentation of a linear se-quence of four spatial locations marked by neutral blocks. After a 30 s interval, the animal was given the test phase. The test phase consisted of the same sequence presented during the study phase, but although one of the spatial loca-tions was not marked by a block, it still contained a reward. The unmarked spatial location was pseudo-randomly dis-tributed equally between the first, second, third, and fourth

item in the sequence. To receive a reward, the rat had to visit the correct, unmarked spatial location. Once animals were able to reliably perform this short-term event-based memory task, they received lesions to either CA3 or CA1. Animals with lesions to either CA3 or CA1 had difficulty with short-term event-based memory processing, although CA1 lesioned animals had a much greater deficit. However, when animals were trained on a fixed version of the same task, hippocampal lesions had no effect. These results sug-gest that CA3 and CA1 both contribute to short-term event-based memory processing, since lesions to CA3 or CA1 re-sult in an inability to process spatial information within the event based-memory system, whereas they have no effect on non-event-based memory information processing (Hunsaker et al., 2008).

In order to determine temporal order memory for visual objects, we used a paradigm described by Hannesson, How-land, and Phillips (2004). This paradigm involves long dura-tion study phases (on the order of minutes) and long duration tests (also on the order of minutes) for temporal preference and thus it is likely to involve more directly the intermedi-ate-term event-based memory processes we have proposed for CA1. In this experiment, rats with CA3 or CA1 lesions were placed inside a box to explore each set of three objects (referred to as A-A, B-B, and C-C) for 5 min with a 3 min inter-session interval. After the third set of objects, the rats were given a 3 min time-out after which one of the two A objects and one of the two C objects were placed in opposite ends of the box. The rats were then returned to the box to measure preference for A versus C for 5 min. On a subse-quent day with new objects, the same animals were tested for detection of a novel object as a control using the same procedure previously described with the exception that one of the two A objects and one new object D were placed in opposite ends of the box to measure preference for A versus D for 5 min. All rats were tested once in the A-C prefer-ence test (temporal order) and on the A-D preference test (detection of object novelty) for a total of 2 days of testing. The results indicated that CA1 lesions impaired choice (they preferred C over A), but CA3 lesioned rats showed the same preference as controls (they preferred A over C). All groups preferred D in the novelty test (Hoge & Kesner, 2007; Hun-saker et al., 2008). The data indicate that controls prefer A rather than C. In order to explain this preference for A, it is assumed that rats prefer A because the rat has had more time for consolidation within an intermediate-term event-based memory operation for object A in comparison with object C and thus has greater memory strength for object A. Fur-thermore, CA1, but not CA3, lesioned rats prefer C suggest-ing an impairment for CA1, but not CA3, in temporal order memory for visual objects. A possible explanation for the observation that CA1 lesioned rats prefer C rather than A

Page 9: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 37

is based on the assumption that the trace of A has not been consolidated properly and thus may be difficult to retrieve, but C may still be processed by the short-term event-based memory system mediated by CA3. Thus, the rat prefers C because of a short-term recency effect. This would also ex-plain the lack of deficit observed following a lesion of CA3.

Using the same paradigm as described above but with shorter delays to examine the effects of dorsal and ventral CA1 lesions on temporal and novelty processing of visual objects, odor, and spatial location information revealed that memory for temporal order information for visual objects is impaired following dorsal and ventral CA1 lesions, for odors following ventral CA1, but not dorsal CA1 lesions, and for spatial locations for dorsal CA1, but not ventral CA1 lesions (Hunsaker et al., 2008). Thus, CA1 appears to be involved in separating events in time for spatial and non-spatial information, so that one event can be remembered distinctly from another event, but ventral CA1 might play a more important role than dorsal CA1 for odor information. There were no disruptive effects for dorsal or ventral CA1 lesions on novelty detection for odors, spatial locations, and objects (Hunsaker et al., 2008). It has been shown, however, that lesions to CA3 eliminate any preference for one spatial location over another, suggesting CA3 is also involved in temporal ordering for spatial locations, but only insofar as the information to be temporally processed is spatial in na-ture.

Short-term or Working Memory -- Response Attribute

With respect to response attribute information, it can be shown that with the use of the above mentioned paradigms to measure short-term memory, that for rats with caudate-putamen lesions and humans with caudate-putamen damage due to Huntington’s disease (HD), there are profound deficits for a right or left turn response or a list of hand motor move-ment responses (Cook & Kesner, 1988; Davis, Filoteo, Kes-ner, & Roberts, 2003; Kesner et al., 1993; Figure 3 depicts the location of the caudate nucleus in the rat). For example, it has been shown that electrolytic induced caudate lesions in rats impair short-term or working memory for a specific motor response (right-left turn) without any impairments in memory for a visual object or for a spatial location (Kesner et al., 1993). Similarly, a lack of effects has been reported following medial caudate lesions in working memory per-formance for spatial locations on an 8 arm maze (Colombo, Davis, & Volpe, 1989; Cook & Kesner, 1988). A similar pattern of results has been reported following dysfunction of the caudate nucleus in patients with HD. For example, Davis et al. (2003) administered tests of spatial and motor working memory to a small group of HD patients. During the study phase of the spatial memory task, subjects were shown a subset of six stimulus locations (X's) randomly se-

Figure 3. Pictorial representation of the caudate nucleus in the rat.

Neocortex

Caudate

PallidumAC

OC

lected from a set of 16 and presented in a sequential man-ner. Immediately following the study phase, the test phase was presented. During the test phase, two stimulus locations (X's) were presented simultaneously. The subject was asked to indicate which one they had seen during the study phase. During the study phase of the hand position memory task, subjects were shown sequential presentations of six hand po-sitions randomly selected from a set of 16 and were asked to imitate the hand position in the display. On the test phase, subjects were shown two pictures of different hand positions and were asked to determine which one they had seen in the study phase. The results of this study indicate that, relative to normal controls, the HD patients are differentially im-paired in the motor memory task as compared to the spatial memory task. Interestingly, in two studies, Pasquier, et al. (1994) and Davis, Filoteo and Kesner (2007) demonstrated that HD patients were impaired on a task requiring them to recall the spatial distance of the displacement of a handle on the apparatus. The results of the above mentioned studies, suggest that patients with HD and rats with caudate lesions are impaired on working-memory tasks, particularly when the task places a heavy demand on motor information. Ad-ditional data based on a patient with a caudate nucleus lesion showed a decrease in accuracy of memory-guided saccades implying that the caudate nucleus mediates spatial short term memory for eye movements (Vermersch et al., 1999).

Furthermore, rats, monkeys, and humans with caudate le-sions have deficits in tasks like delayed response, delayed al-ternation, and delayed matching to position (Divac, Rosvold,

Page 10: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 38

& Szwarcbart, 1967; Dunnett, 1990; Oberg & Divac, 1979; Partiot et al., 1996; Sanberg, Lehmann, & Fibiger, 1978). One salient feature of delayed response, delayed alternation, and delayed matching to position tasks is the maintenance of spatial orientation to the baited food, relative to the po-sition of the subject's body, often based on proprioceptive and vestibular feedback. These data suggest that the caudate-putamen plays an important role in short-term memory rep-resentation for the feedback from a motor response feature of response attribute information. The memory impairments following caudate-putamen lesions are specific to the re-sponse attribute, because these same lesions in rats do not impair short-term memory performance for spatial location, visual object, or affect attribute information (Kesner et al., 1993; Kesner & Williams, 1995).

Short-term or Working Memory -- Affect Attribute

With respect to affect attribute information, it can be shown that with the use of the above mentioned paradigms to measure short-term memory, that for rats with amygdala lesions and humans with amygdala damage there are ma-jor deficits for reward value associated with magnitude of reinforcement or for a liking response based on the mere exposure of a novel stimulus (Kesner & Williams, 1995; Chiba, Kesner, Matsuo, & Heilbrun, 1993), suggesting that the amygdala plays an important role in short-term memory representation for reward value as a critical feature of the af-fect attribute. Figure 1 depicts the location of the amygdala in rats. Since very few studies have measured the role of the amygdala in mediating short-term memory for affect, it was necessary to develop a new task (Kesner & Williams, 1995). In the study phase of the task, rats were given one of two ce-reals – one cereal contained 25% sugar, the other 50% sugar. One of the two cereals was always designated as the positive stimulus and the other as the negative stimulus. This study phase was followed by the test phase, in which the rat was shown an object which covered a food well. If the rat was given the negative food stimulus during the study phase, no food was placed beneath the object. If the rat was given the positive food stimulus during the study phase, another food reward was placed beneath the object. Latency to approach the object was used as the dependent measure. Rats learn to approach the objects quickly when they expect a reward and they are slow to approach the object when they expect no re-ward. After they reached criterion of at least a 5 s difference between the positive and negative trials, the rats were giv-en amygdala or control lesions. The results indicate that in contrast to controls, the amygdala lesioned rats displayed a deficit in performance as indicated by smaller latency differ-ences between positive and negative trials on post-surgery tests. This deficit persisted at both short and long delays. In additional experiments, it was shown that the amygdala le-sioned rats, like controls, had similar taste preferences and

transferred readily to different cereals containing 25% or 50% sugar. A similar result was reported by Kesner, Walser, and Winzenried (1989), who showed that amygdala lesioned rats were impaired in short-term memory performance for 1 versus 7 pieces of food associated with different spatial locations on an 8 arm maze. Thus, the amygdala appears to mediate short-term affect-laden information based on the re-ward value (magnitude) of reinforcement.

To what extent can one generalize from amygdala func-tion in rats to humans with respect to affect attribute in-formation? Previous research has shown that bilateral damage to the amygdala in humans impairs recognition of affect embedded within facial expressions (Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, 1994). In order to elaborate further on the role of the amygdala in humans, Chiba et al. (1993) developed a liking test based on the mere exposure effect described by Zajonc (1968). Based on this principle, a com-puterized liking task was designed to test the presence of the mere exposure effect. The liking task consisted of eight abstract pictures and eight unknown words that were se-quentially presented on the computer screen. Following the individual presentation of each of these 16 study stimuli, 16 liking trials were presented. In each liking trial, two stimuli – one study stimulus and a matched lure – were simultane-ously presented on the computer screen. Subjects were then asked which of the two stimuli they liked better. Four groups of subjects were tested on this task -- college students as control subjects, subjects with partial complex epilepsy of temporal lobe origin, subjects who had undergone unilateral temporal lobe resections, including the temporal cortex and the hippocampus, and subjects who had undergone unilat-eral temporal lobe resections including the temporal cor-tex, hippocampus, and amygdala. Results indicated that for mean percent preference for abstract pictures and words, all subject groups showed a stable liking or mere exposure ef-fect for both sets of stimuli, with the exception of those who sustained amygdala damage. It appears that the integrity of the amygdala is critical to the existence of the liking effect.

Thus, it is likely that the amygdala of animals and humans is involved in a short-term memory representation of the af-fective quality and quantity (reward value) of stimuli. This idea is an extension of earlier theoretical notions that the amygdala is involved in the interpretation and integration of reinforcement (Weiskrantz, 1956), serves as a reinforcement register (Douglas & Pribram, 1966), mediates stimulus-rein-forcement associations (Jones & Mishkin, 1972) and serves to associate stimuli with reward value (Gaffan, 1992).

Short-term or Working Memory -- Sensory-Perceptual Attribute

With respect to sensory-perceptual attribute information,

Page 11: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 39

I will concentrate on visual object information as an exem-plar of memory representation of the sensory-perceptual at-tribute. Figure 4 depicts the location of the perirhinal cortex in the rat. It can be shown that with the use of the above mentioned paradigms to measure short-term memory, that there are severe impairments in visual object information for rats and monkeys with extra-striate or perirhinal cortex le-sions (Bussey et al., 2002; Gaffan & Murray, 1992; Horel, Pytko-Joiner, Boytko, & Salsbury, 1987; Kesner et al., 1993; Mumby & Pinel, 1994; Norman & Eacott, 2004; Suzuki, Zola-Morgan, Squire, & Amaral, 1993), suggesting that the extra-striate and perirhinal cortex play an important role in short-term memory representation for visual object infor-mation as an exemplar of the sensory-perceptual attribute. Further support derives from single unit studies in rats and monkeys which indicate that activity of neurons in the rhinal cortex reflect stimulus repetition which is an integral part of the delayed non-matching to sample tasks used to measure short-term recognition memory for objects (Zhu, Brown, & Aggleton, 1995).

Using a paradigm developed by Poucet (1989), rats with CA3 lesions that were tested for the detection of a novel vi-sual object change showed no disruption (Lee et al., 2005b). Based on the idea that the lateral perforant path inputs into CA3 mediate visual object information (i.e. “what” informa-tion) via activation of opioid receptors, rats received direct infusions of naloxone (a µ opiate antagonist) into CA3 and CA1 and were tested for the detection of a novel spatial con-figuration of familiar objects and the detection of a novel visual object. The results indicate that naloxone infusions into the CA3 disrupted novelty detection of a spatial loca-tion and a visual object, but naloxone injections into CA1 disrupted novelty detection for a visual object, but not for a spatial location (Hunsaker et al., 2007). The primary impli-cation of these data is that CA3 is capable of simultaneous processing of both spatial (“where”) and nonspatial (“what”) elements of event-based memory. Based on the idea that the medial perforant path inputs into CA3 mediate spatial loca-tion information (i.e. “where” information) via activation of NMDA receptors, rats received direct infusions of AP5 (an NMDA antagonist) into CA3 and were tested for the detec-tion of a novel spatial configuration of familiar objects and the detection of a novel visual object. The results indicate that NMDA infusions into the CA3 disrupted novelty de-tection of a spatial location and a visual object, but NMDA injections into CA1 disrupted novelty detection for a spatial location, but not for a visual object (Hunsaker et al, 2007). Disruption of either medial perforant path (NMDA-ergic) or lateral perforant path (µ opioid-ergic) plasticity resulted in spatial and novel object detection deficits. In CA1, it ap-pears that the spatial and nonspatial elements are processed separately. Disrupting the lateral perforant path by infusing

naloxone was sufficient to disrupt novel object detection, but not sufficient to disrupt detection of a spatial change. These data suggest that CA3, but not CA1, is critically important for spatial/nonspatial associative binding critical for event-based memory. Similar to the argument provided earlier, it appears that CA3 is involved in rapid spatial and nonspatial information binding into coherent behavioral episodes in the time-scale of this task (each episode is of approximately 6 min duration). When CA3 is disrupted, the rat fails to retrieve any elements of the event. This is in contrast to CA1, where it appears that CA1 is involved in temporally tagging information into events, and that this is carried out upon each type of information separately (e.g., spatial and nonspatial information). Thus a disruption to nonspatial in-formation disrupts only nonspatial processing in CA1.

Short term or Working Memory -- Language Attribute

With respect to language attribute information, it can be shown that with the use of the above mentioned paradigms to measure short-term memory that there are severe impair-ments for lists of words for humans with left hippocampal or bilateral hippocampal damage (Hopkins, Kesner, & Gold-stein, 1995b), suggesting that the hippocampus plays an im-portant role in short-term memory representation of word information as an important feature of language attribute information. There is a good deal of evidence supporting the idea of important lateralization for hippocampal function in humans with the right hippocampus representing spatial information and the left hippocampus representing linguis-tic information (Milner, 1971; Smith & Milner, 1981). For example, Milner tested patients who had left or right tem-poral lobectomies on a task of recall for a visual location. In this task subjects made a mark on an 8 in line in order to reproduce as close as possible the exact position of the previously shown circle. Subjects with right temporal lobe lesions were impaired on this task, whereas subjects with left temporal lobe lesions were not significantly different from control subjects. Smith and Milner (1981) tested patients with right and left temporal lobectomies and control subjects on a memory task involving incidental recall of the locations of the objects. Subjects were asked to estimate the prices of several objects which were placed in a spatial array on a test board. After a short or 24 h delay, subjects were asked to place the objects in their appropriate locations. Left tem-poral lobe and control subjects performed well on this task at both the immediate and delayed recall of the object loca-tions. Right temporal lobe subjects were impaired for both the immediate and delayed recall of the object locations. Even though hypoxic subjects or left temporal resected pa-tients are impaired for new linguistic information, they are not impaired when they can use semantic or syntactic infor-mation to remember the order of presentation of syntacti-cally and semantically meaningful sentences (Hopkins et al.,

Page 12: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 40

1995b).

Event-Based Memory -- Pattern Separation

Pattern separation is defined as a process to remove re-dundancy from similar inputs so that events can be separated from each other and interference can be reduced and in addi-tion can produce a more orthogonal, sparse, and categorized set of outputs.

Pattern Separation -- Spatial Attribute

The determination of a spatial pattern separation process has been developed extensively by computational models of the subregions of the hippocampus with a special emphasis on the dentate gyrus (DG). Based on the empirical findings that all sensory inputs are processed by the DG subregion of the hippocampus ((Aggleton, Hunt, & Rawlins, 1986; Jackson-Smith et al., 1993; Kesner et al., 1993; Mumby, Wood, & Pinel, 1992; Otto & Eichenbaum, 1992), it has been suggested that a possible role for the hippocampus might be to provide for sensory markers to demarcate a spa-tial location, so that the hippocampus can more efficiently mediate spatial information. It is thus possible that one of the main process functions of the hippocampus is to encode and separate spatial events from each other. This would ensure that new highly processed sensory information is organized within the hippocampus and enhances the possibility of re-membering and temporarily storing one place as separate from another place. It is assumed that this is accomplished via pattern separation of event information, so that spatial events can be separated from each other and spatial interfer-ence reduced. This process is akin to the idea that the hip-pocampus is involved in orthogonalization of sensory input information (Rolls, 1989), in representational differentiation (Myers, Gluck, & Granger, 1995), and indirectly in the utili-zation of relationships (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993).

Rolls’ (1996) model proposes that pattern separation is facilitated by sparse connections in the mossy-fiber sys-tem, which connects DG granular cells to CA3 pyramidal neurons. Separation of patterns is accomplished based on the low probability that any two CA3 neurons will receive mossy fiber input synapses from a similar subset of DG cells. Mossy fiber inputs to CA3 from DG are suggested to be essential during learning and may influence which CA3 neurons fire based on the distributed activity within the DG. Cells of the DG are suggested to act as a competitive learn-ing network with Hebb-like modifiability to reduce redun-dancy and produce sparse, orthogonal outputs. O’Reilly & McClelland (1996) and Shapiro & Olton (1994) also sug-gested that the mossy fiber connections between the DG and CA3 may support pattern separation.

To examine the contribution of the DG to spatial pattern

separation, Gilbert, Kesner, and Lee (2001) tested rats with DG lesions using a paradigm which measured short-term memory for spatial location information as a function of spatial similarity between spatial locations. Specifically, the study was designed to examine the role of the DG subregion in discriminating spatial locations when rats were required to remember a spatial location based on distal environmen-tal cues and to differentiate between the to-be-remembered location and a distractor location with different degrees of similarity or overlap among the distal cues.

Animals were tested using a cheeseboard maze apparatus (the cheese board is similar to a dry land water maze with 177 circular, recessed holes on a 119 cm diameter board) on a delayed-match-to-sample for a spatial location task. Ani-mals were trained to displace an object which was randomly positioned to cover a baited food well in 1 of 15 locations along a row of food wells. Following a short delay, the ani-mals were required to choose between objects which were identical to the sample phase object: one object was in the same location as the sample phase object and the second ob-ject was in a different location along the row of food wells. Rats were rewarded for displacing the object in the same spatial location as the sample phase object (correct choice), but they received no reward for displacing the foil object (incorrect choice). Five spatial separations, from 15 cm to 105 cm, were used to separate the correct object and the foil object during the choice phase. Rats with DG lesions were significantly impaired at short spatial separations; however, during the choice phase, performance of DG-lesioned ani-mals increased as a function of greater spatial separation be-tween the correct and foil objects. The performance of rats with DG lesions matched control rats at the largest spatial separation. The graded nature of the impairment and the significant linear improvement in performance as a function of increased separation illustrate a deficit in pattern separa-tion. Based on these results, it was concluded that lesions of the DG decrease the efficiency of spatial pattern separa-tion, which results in impairments on trials with increased spatial proximity and increased spatial similarity among working memory representations. Holden, Hoebel, Loftis, and Gilbert (2012) used an analogous task to that used for rats (Gilbert et al., 2001) to test young participants compared to aged participants who are likely to have DG dysfunction (see Small, et al., 2011). They report that aged participants that do not perform well on standard memory tests are im-paired in displaying a pattern separation function. One limi-tation of the dot task is that it does not assess the ability to separate spatial patterns in the real world. In order to assess real world spatial pattern separation, hypoxic subjects with hippocampal damage and matched normal controls were ad-ministered a geographical spatial distance task (cities on a map; Hopkins & Kesner, 1993). The subjects were shown

Page 13: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 41

8 cities on a map of New Brunswick, one at a time, for 5 s each. Subjects were instructed to remember the city and its spatial location on the map. In the test phase, the subjects were presented with the names of two cities that occurred in the study phase and were asked which of the cities was locat-ed further to the east (on separate trials, subjects were asked which city occurred further north, south, or west). There were two trials for each compass direction. Spatial distances of 0, 2, 4, and 6 as measured by the number of cities in the study phase that were geographically situated between the two test cities were measured. There were 8 trials for each distance. The hypoxic subjects were impaired for all spatial distances for spatial geographical information compared to control subjects (Hopkins & Kesner, 1993).

Thus, the DG may function to encode and to separate lo-cations in space to produce spatial pattern separation. Such spatial pattern separation ensures that new highly processed sensory information is organized within the hippocampus, which in turn enhances the possibility of encoding and tem-porarily remembering one spatial location as separate from another.

Based on the observations that cells in CA3 and CA1 re-gions respond to changes in metric and topological aspects of the environment (Jeffery & Anderson, 2003; O’Keefe & Burgess, 1996), one can ask whether these different features of the spatial environment are processed via the DG and then are subsequently transferred to the CA3 subregion or if these features are communicated via the direct perforant path pro-jection to the CA3 subregion. In both cases, information may then be transferred to the CA1 subregion.

To answer this question, Goodrich-Hunsaker, Hunsaker, and Kesner (2005) examined the contributions of the DG to memory for metric spatial relationships. Using a modified version of an exploratory paradigm developed by Poucet (1989), rats with DG, CA3, and CA1 lesions as well as con-trols, were tested on tasks involving a metric spatial manipu-lation. In this task, a rat was allowed to explore two different visual objects separated by a specific distance on a cheese-board maze. On the initial presentation of the objects, the rat explored each object. However, across subsequent presenta-tions of the objects in the same spatial locations, the rat ha-bituated and eventually spent less time exploring the objects. Once the rat had habituated to the objects in their locations, the metric spatial distance between the objects was manipu-lated so that the two objects were either closer together or farther apart. The time the rat spent exploring each moved object was recorded. The results showed that rats with DG lesions spent significantly less time exploring the two ob-jects that were displaced relative to controls, indicating that DG lesions impair the detection of metric distance changes. Rats with CA3 or CA1 lesions displayed mild impairments

relative to controls, providing empirical validation for the role of DG in spatial pattern separation and support the pre-dictions of computational models (Rolls, 1996; Rolls & Kes-ner, 2006). Stark, Yassa, and Stark (2010) used an analogous task to that used for rats (Goodrich-Hunsaker et al., 2005) to measure spatial pattern separation based on distance, and in this case angle as well, to test young and healthy aging humans. Even though there were some individual differ-ences, they reporedt an impairment in spatial pattern separa-tion. Also, Baumann, Chan, and Mattingley (2012) reported activation of the posterior hippocampus in spatial pattern separation using the task used by Goodrich-Hunsaker et al. (2005).

Based on the observation that neurogenesis occurs in the DG and that new DG granule cells can be formed over time, it has been proposed that the DG mediates a spatial pattern-separation mechanism as well as generates patterns of epi-sodic memories within remote memory (Aimone, Wiles, & Gage, 2006). Thus far, it has been shown in mice that disruption of neurogenesis using low-dose x-irradiation was sufficient to produce a loss of newly born DG cells. Further testing indicated impairments in spatial learning in a delayed non-matching-to-place task in the radial arm maze. Specifi-cally, impairment occurred for arms which were presented with little separation, but no deficit was observed when the arms were presented farther apart, suggesting a spatial pat-tern separation deficit. Also, the disruption of neurogenesis using lentivirus expression of a dominant Wnt protein pro-duced a loss of newly born DG cells, as well, and was ob-served in an associative object-in-place task with different spatial separations as a function of the degree of separation, again suggesting a spatial pattern separation deficit (Clelland et al., 2009). These data suggest that neurogenesis in the DG may contribute to the operation of spatial pattern separation. Thus, spatial pattern separation may play an important role in the acquisition of new spatial information and there is a good possibility that the DG may be the subregion responsi-ble for the impairments in the various tasks described above.

Pattern Separation -- Temporal Attribute

There are data to support the existence of memory for or-der information, but it is not always clearly demonstrated whether memory for a particular sequence has been learned and can be accurately recalled. Estes (1986) summarized data demonstrating that, in human memory, there are fewer errors for distinguishing items (by specifying the order in which they occurred) that are far apart in a sequence than those that are temporally adjacent. Other studies have also shown that order judgments improve as the number of items in a sequence between the test items increases (Banks, 1978; Chiba, Kesner, & Reynolds, 1994; Madsen & Kesner, 1995). This phenomenon is referred to as a temporal distance effect

Page 14: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 42

[sometimes referred to as a temporal pattern separation ef-fect (Kesner, Lee, & Gilbert, 2004)]. The temporal distance effect is assumed to occur because there is more interference for temporally proximal events than for temporally distant events.

Based on these findings, Gilbert et al. (2001) tested ro-dents memory for the temporal order of items in a one-trial sequence learning paradigm. In the task, each rat was given one daily trial consisting of a sample phase followed by a choice phase. During the sample phase, the animal visited each arm of an 8-arm radial maze once in a randomly pre-determined order and was given a reward at the end of each arm. The choice phase began immediately following the presentation of the final arm in the sequence. In the choice phase, two arms were opened simultaneously and the ani-mal was allowed to choose between the arms. To obtain a food reward, the animal had to enter the arm that occurred earlier in the sequence that it had just followed. Temporal separations of 0, 2, 4, and 6 were randomly selected for each choice phase. These values represented the number of arms in the sample phase that intervened between the arms that were to be used in the test phase. After reaching criterion, rats received CA1 lesions. Following surgery, control rats matched their preoperative performance over all temporal separations. In contrast, rats with CA1 lesions performed at chance across 0, 2, or 4 temporal separations and a little bet-ter than chance in the case of a separation of 6 items. The re-sults suggest that the CA1 subregion is involved in memory for spatial location as a function of temporal separation of spatial locations; lesions of the CA1 decrease efficiency in temporal pattern separation. CA1 lesioned rats cannot sepa-rate events over time, perhaps due to an inability to inhibit interference that may be associated with sequentially occur-ring events. The increase in temporal interference impairs the rat’s ability to remember the order of specific events. To-lentino et al. (2012) used an analogous task to that used for rats (Gilbert et al., 2001) to test young compared to non-de-mented older participants in a spatial temporal pattern sepa-ration task and report temporal pattern separation problems for the older participants. In another spatial location task, patients with a hypoxic condition and hippocampal damage were impaired in displaying a temporal pattern separation function (Hopkins et al., 1995a).

In a more recent experiment using a paradigm described by Hannesson et al. (2004), it was shown that temporal order information for spatial location was impaired only for CA1 (Hunsaker et al., 2008). Thus, it can be suggested that the CA1 hippocampal subregion serves as a critical substrate for sequence learning and temporal pattern separation for the spatial attribute.

It has been suggested that the perirhinal cortex and CA1

subregion of the hippocampus plays an important role in supporting temporal processing of visual object information (Hoge & Kesner, 2007; Hunsaker et al., 2008). In humans it can be shown that a temporal pattern separation process can be observed in hypoxic patients in a temporal order test memory test for abstract figures (Hopkins et al., 1995a), sug-gesting that the hippocampus may also play a role in tempo-ral pattern separation for visual stimuli, at least in humans.

Does the hippocampus support temporal pattern separa-tion processes for sensory-perceptual information other than space and visual objects? To answer this question, memory for the temporal order for a sequence of odors was assessed in rats based on a varied sequence of five odors, using a sim-ilar paradigm described for sequences of spatial locations. Kesner, Gilbert, and Barua (2002) found that rats with hip-pocampal lesions were impaired relative to control animals for memory for all temporal distances between the odors, de-spite an intact ability to discriminate between the odors. For-tin, Agster, and Eichenbaum (2002) reported similar results with fimbria fornix lesions. In a further subregional analy-sis, rats with dorsal CA1 lesions showed a mild impairment in memory for the temporal distance for odors, but rats with ventral CA1 lesions showed a severe impairment (Kesner, Hunsaker, & Ziegler, 2010). Thus, the CA1 appears to be involved in separating events in time for spatial and nonspa-tial information, so one event can be remembered distinctly from another event; however, the dorsal CA1 might play a more important role than the ventral CA1 for spatial infor-mation (Chiba, Johnson, & Kesner, 1992), and conversely the ventral CA1 might play a more important role than the dorsal CA1 for odor information. The mechanism that could subserve the above mentioned findings is based on the mem-ory question that asks which of two items occurred earlier in the list. To implement this type of memory, some tempo-rally decaying memory trace or temporally increasing mem-ory trace via a consolidation process might provide a model (Marshuetz, 2005); in such a model, temporally adjacent items would have memory traces of more similar strength and would be harder to discriminate than the strengths of the memory traces of more temporally distant items.

Pattern Separation -- Response Attribute

A delayed-match-to-sample task was used to assess mem-ory for motor responses in rats with control, hippocampus, or medial caudate nucleus (MCN) lesions. All testing was conducted on a cheeseboard maze in complete darkness us-ing an infrared camera. A start box was positioned in the center of the maze facing a randomly determined direction on each trial. In the sample phase, a phosphorescent object was randomly positioned to cover a baited food well in 1 of 5 equally spaced positions around the circumference of the maze forming a 180-degree arc 60 cm from the box. On each

Page 15: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 43

trial, the door to the start box was opened, the rat exited, displaced the object to receive food, and returned to the box. The box was then rotated to face a different direction. The food well in the same position relative to the box was baited and an identical phosphorescent object was positioned to cover the well. A second identical object was positioned to cover a different unbaited well. On the choice phase, the rat was allowed to choose between the 2 objects. The object in the same position relative to the start box as the object in the sample phase was the correct choice and the foil object was the incorrect choice. The rat must remember the motor response made on the sample phase and make the same mo-tor response on the choice phase to receive a reward. Four separations of 45, 90, 135, and 180 degrees were randomly used to separate the correct object from the foil in the choice phase. Hippocampus-lesioned and control rats improved as a function of increased angle separation and matched the per-formance of controls. However, rats with MCN lesions were impaired across all separations (Kesner & Gilbert, 2006). Results suggest that the MCN, but not the hippocampus, may support working memory and/or a process aimed at re-ducing interference for motor response selection based on vector angle information.

Pattern Separation -- Affect Attribute

Male Long-Evans rats were tested on a modified version of Flaherty, Turovsky, & Krauss’ (1994) anticipatory con-trast paradigm, to assess pattern separation for reward value. Prior to testing, each rat received either a control, hippocam-pal, or amygdala lesion. In the home cage, each rat was allowed to drink a water solution containing 2% sucrose for 3 min followed by a water solution containing 32% sucrose for 3 min. Over 10 days of testing, the rats in each lesion group showed significantly increased anticipatory discrim-inability as a function of days. In order to assess the opera-tion of a pattern separation mechanism, each rat was then tested using the same procedure, except the 2% solution was followed by a 16% solution for 10 days and then by an 8% solution for 10 days. Control and hippocampal-lesioned rats continued to show high discriminability when the 2% solu-tion was followed by a 16% solution, however, the amyg-dala-lesioned rats showed low anticipatory discriminability. On trials where the 2% sucrose solution was followed by an 8% sucrose solution, all groups showed low discriminabil-ity scores, suggesting that when two reward values are very similar, even control animals are not able to separate the re-ward values in memory. However, the results of a prefer-ence task revealed that all groups can perceptually discrimi-nate between a 2% and an 8% sucrose solution (Gilbert & Kesner, 2002). The data suggest that the amygdala, but not the hippocampus, is involved in the separation of patterns based on reward value.

Pattern Separation -- Sensory-Perceptual Attribute (Objects)

In order to determine whether the perirhinal cortex plays a role in object-based pattern separation, rats with perirhi-nal cortex, hippocampal, or sham lesions were trained on a successive discrimination go/no-go task to examine recog-nition memory based on pattern separation for an array of visual objects with varying interference among the objects in the array. Rats were trained to recognize a target array consisting of four particular objects that could be presented in any one of four possible configurations to cover baited food wells. If the four target objects were presented, the rat should displace each object to receive food. However, if a novel object replaced any one or more of the target objects, then the rat should withhold its response. The number of novel objects presented on non-rewarded trials varied from one to four. The fewer the number of novel objects in the array, the more interference the array shared with the target array, therefore increasing task difficulty, requiring an object pattern separation mechanism to solve the task. The results indicated that an increased number of novel objects resulted in a pattern separation effect with less interference for the target array as indicated by decreased task difficulty. Al-though accuracy was slightly lower in rats with hippocam-pal lesions, compared to controls, the learning of the groups was not statistically different. In contrast, rats with peri-rhinal cortex lesions were significantly impaired in utilizing a pattern separation function compared to both control and hippocampal-lesioned rats (Gilbert & Kesner, 2003). The results suggest that temporal pattern separation for objects is affected by stimulus interference in rodents and is medi-ated by the perirhinal cortex. Other research supports these results for the perirhinal mediation of object-based pattern separation (Bussey et al., 2002; Norman & Eacott, 2004).

In studies with humans, a modified continuous recogni-tion task was used. In one study with young participants us-ing high resolution fMRI with this task, it was found that the hippocampus distinguished among correctly identified true stimulus repetitions, correctly rejected presentations of similar lure stimuli, and false alarm lures (Kirwan & Stark, 2007). In a subsequent study it was shown that in aged com-pared to young participants that the DG/CA3 subregions of the hippocampus played an important role in deficits found in aged participants (Yassa et al., 2010). For a review of the human pattern separation data see (Yassa & Stark, 2011).

Pattern Separation -- Sensory-Perceptual Attribute (Odors)

Working memory and pattern separation for odor infor-mation was assessed in rats using a matching-to-sample for odors paradigm. The odor set consisted of a five aliphatic

Page 16: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 44

acids with unbranched carbon chains that varied from two-six carbons in length. Each trial consisted of a sample phase followed by a choice phase. During the sample phase, rats would receive one of five different odors. During the choice phase 15 s later, one of the previous odors was pre-sented simultaneously side by side with a different odor that was based on the number of aliphatic acids that varied in the carbon chains from two-six carbons in length and rats were allowed to choose between the two odors. The rule to be learned in order to receive a food reward was to al-ways choose the odor that occurred during the study phase. Odor separations of 1, 2, 3 or 4 were selected for each choice phase which represented the carbon chain difference be-tween the study phase odor and the test phase odor. Once an animal reached a criterion of 80-90% correct across all temporal separations based on the last 16 trials, rats received a control or ventral dentate gyrus lesion and were retested on the task. On postoperative trials, there were no deficits at the 15 s delay for either the controls or the ventral dentate gyrus lesioned rats. However, when the delay was increased to 60 s, rats with ventral DG lesions were significantly im-paired at short spatial separations and performance of DG-lesioned animals increased as a function of greater spatial separation between the correct and foil objects. The per-formance of rats with ventral DG lesions matched control rats at the largest odor based separation. The graded nature of the impairment and the significant linear improvement in performance as a function of increased separation illustrate a deficit in odor pattern separation. Based on these results, it was concluded that lesions of the ventral DG decrease the efficiency of odor based pattern separation, which results in impairments on trials with increased spatial proximity and increased odor similarity among working memory represen-tations (Weeden, Hu, Ho, & Kesner, 2012). The data suggest that the ventral hippocampus, but not dorsal hippocampus, supports pattern separation for odor information.

In summary, within the event-based memory system, dif-ferent brain regions process different attributes in support of short-term or working memory and pattern separation processes. Data are presented to support this assertion by demonstrating that the dorsal hippocampus mediates spatial and temporal attribute information, the caudate mediates re-sponse attribute information, the amygdala mediates affect attribute information, the perirhinal cortex mediates senso-ry-perceptual attribute information for visual objects, the ventral hippocampus mediates sensory-perceptual attribute information for odors, and the hippocampus mediates lan-guage attribute information. Where data are available, there are parallel results found in rodents, monkeys and humans.

Knowledge-Based Memory

The organization of the attributes within the knowledge-

based memory system can take many forms and they are as-sumed to be organized as a set of cognitive maps or neural nets, the interactions of which are unique for each memory. It is assumed that long-term representations within cogni-tive maps are more abstract and less dependent upon specific features. Some interactions between attributes are important and can aid in identifying specific neural regions that might subserve a critical interaction. For example, the interaction between sensory-perceptual attributes and the spatial attri-bute can provide for the long-term memory representation of a spatial cognitive map or spatial schemas, the interaction between temporal and spatial attributes can provide for the long-term memory representation of scripts, the interaction between temporal and affect attributes can provide for the long-term memory representation of moods, and the inter-action between sensory-perceptual and response attributes can provide for the long-term memory of skills. Based on a series of experiments, it can be shown that within the knowl-edge-based memory system, different neural structures and circuits mediate different forms or attributes of memory. The most extensive data set is based on the use of paradigms that measure repetition priming, the acquisition of new in-formation, discrimination performance, executive functions, strategies and rules to perform in a variety of tasks including skills and the operation of a variety of long-term memory programs.

For the knowledge-based memory system, I will concen-trate on specific processes that mediate perceptual memory within long-term memory. For the other processes, includ-ing selective attention and selective filtering associated with permanent memory representations of familiar information, selection of strategies and rules (“executive functions”), and retrieval of familiar information based on flexibility and action, the establishment of arbitrary associations, consoli-dation or elaborative rehearsal of new information, and re-trieval of new information based on flexibility, action, and pattern completion, there is not a sufficient data set to differ-entiate the contribution of the different attributes associated with mnemonic processing of information.

Spatial Attribute

The emphasis will be on the role of the parietal cortex (PPC) in perceptual and long-term memory processing of complex spatial information within the knowledge-based memory system, see Figure 4 for the location of the PPC in rats. Rats with PPC lesions display deficits in both the acqui-sition and retention of spatial navigation tasks that are pre-sumed to measure the operation of a spatial cognitive map within a complex environment (DiMattia & Kesner, 1988b; Kesner, Farnsworth, & Kametani, 1992). They also display deficits in the acquisition and retention of spatial recognition memory for a list of five spatial locations (DiMattia & Kes-

Page 17: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 45

a role in spatial perceptual memory within the knowledge-based memory system, but does not play a role in spatial memory within the event-based memory system.

In humans there is a general loss of topographic sense, which may involve loss of long-term geographical knowl-edge as well as an inability to form cognitive maps of new environments. Using PET scan and functional MRI data, it can be shown that complex spatial information results in ac-tivation of the parietal cortex (Ungerleider, 1995). Thus, memory for complex spatial information appears to be im-paired (Benton, 1969; De Renzi, 1982). Furthermore, in patients with parietal lesions and spatial neglect, there is a deficit in spatial repetition priming without a loss in short-term or working memory for spatial information (Ellis, Sala, & Logie, 1996). Keane, et al. (1995) reported that a patient with occipital-lobe damage (extending into PC) showed a deficit in perceptual priming but had no effect on recognition memory, whereas a patient with bilateral medial temporal lobe damage (including hippocampus) had a loss of recogni-tion memory, but no loss of perceptual memory.

Sensory-Perceptual Attribute

The emphasis will be on visual perceptual processing within the knowledge-based system, see Figure 4 for the lo-cation of the TE2 cortex in the rat. I will concentrate on tem-poral cortex (TE2) and make comparisons with the (PPC). In rats using a visual object-place recognition task, TE2 le-sioned rats fail to detect a visual object change, whereas PPC lesioned rats fail to detect a spatial location change (Tees, 1999) suggesting that the two cortical areas play a distinc-tive role in perceptual processing of visual versus spatial lo-cation information. Similar results were reported by Ho et al. (2011) who showed that rats with TE2 lesions had object recognition problems at 20 min, but not at 5 min delays. In rats there is a deficit in processing positive priming for fea-tures of visual objects (a component of perceptual memory system), but the rats performed well in positive priming for spatial location (Kesner, unpublished observations). In mon-keys deficits for visual objects and in a working memory task and visual paired comparison task were observed fol-lowing TE2 deficits suggesting that TE2 cortex may play an important role in visual perceptual processing (Buffalo et al., 1999). It can also be shown that lesions of the inferotem-poral cortex in monkeys and humans and temporal cortex (TE2) in rats result in visual object discrimination problems (Dean, 1990; Fuster,1995; Gross, 1973; McCarthy & War-rington, 1990; Weiskrantz & Saunders, 1984), suggesting that the inferotemporal or TE2 may play an important role in mediating long-term representations of visual object in-formation. Additional support comes from PET scan and functional MRI data in humans, where it can be shown that

Figure 4. Pictorial representation of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and TE2 cortex in the rat.

VLO

AGm(Fr2)

Te3Te2

Fr1 Par 1

Par 2Te1

Oc1

Oc2L

PPC

ner, 1988a). In a complex discrimination task in which a rat has to detect the change in location of an object in a scene, rats with PPC lesions are profoundly impaired (DeCoteau & Kesner, 1996), yet on less complex tasks involving the dis-crimination or short-term memory for single spatial features including spatial location, allocentric and egocentric spatial distance (Long & Kesner, 1996) there are no impairments. When the task is more complex, involving the association of objects and places (components of a spatial cognitive map), then PPC plays an important role. Support for this conclu-sion comes from the finding that rats with PPC lesions are impaired in the acquisition and retention of a spatial loca-tion plus object discrimination (paired associate task), but show no deficits for only spatial or object discriminations (Long et al., 1998). Comparable deficits are found within an egocentric-allocentric distance paired associate task (Long & Kesner, 1996), but there is no deficit for an object-object paired associate task, suggesting that spatial features are es-sential in activating and involving the PPC (unpublished ob-servations). Finally, it should be noted that in rats, neurons have been found within the PPC that encode spatial location and head direction information and that many of these cells are sensitive to multiple cues including visual, propriocep-tive, sensorimotor and vestibular cue information (Chen, Lin, Barnes, & McNaughton, 1994; McNaughton, Chen, & Marcus, 1991). Additional support comes from studies with parietal lesioned monkeys. These animals demonstrate defi-cits in place reversal, landmark reversal, distance discrimi-nation, bent wire route-finding, pattern string-finding, and maze-learning tasks (Milner, Ockleford, & DeWar, 1977; Petrides & Iversen, 1979; Pohl, 1973).

In a somewhat different study, rats with PPC lesions are impaired in an implicit spatial repetition priming experiment but perform without difficulty in processing positive priming for features of visual objects and a short-term or working-memory for a spatial location experiment (Chiba, Kesner, & Jackson, 2002), suggesting that the parietal cortex plays

Page 18: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 46

visual object information results in activation of inferotem-poral cortex (Ungerleider, 1995). In a somewhat different study, Sakai and Miyashita (1991) have shown that neurons within the inferotemporal cortex responded more readily af-ter training to a complex visual stimulus that had been paired with another complex visual stimulus across a delay, sug-gesting the formation of long-term representations of object-object pairs.

In summary, within the knowledge-based memory system, different brain regions process different attributes in support of perceptual processes. Data are presented to support this assertion by demonstrating that the PPC mediates the spa-tial attribute for spatial perceptual information and the TE2 cortex mediates the sensory-perceptual attribute for visual object information. Where data are available, there are paral-lel results found in rodents, monkeys and humans.

Rule-Based Memory

For the rule-based memory system, it is assumed that there is integration of information from the event-based and knowledge-based memory systems for the use of major pro-cesses that include the selection of strategies and rules for maintaining or manipulating information for subsequent de-cision making and action as well as short-term or working memory for new and familiar information.

I will concentrate on two processes, namely working (short-term memory) and paired associate learning and I will emphasize the importance of prefrontal cortex (PFC) subregions in the mediation of different attributes. Figure 5 depicts the organization of the PFC in the rat.

Working Memory (Short-term Memory)

Working or short-term memory is a process for short-term

active maintenance of information as well as for process-ing maintained information. The most extensive data set aimed at addressing the role of the different PFC subregions in supporting different forms of working memory is based on experiments using paradigms that measure short-term or working memory in tasks such as matching or non-match-ing-to-sample for single or lists of items, continuous or n-back recognition memory, and novelty detection based on recognition memory.

Evidence supporting a role for the rat PFC in working memory is based on the findings that lesions of the anterior cingulate and precentral (AC/PC) cortex that spare the pre-limbic-infralimbic (PL/IL) cortex produce a deficit in work-ing memory for motor response information such as working memory for a motor (right-left turn) response (Kesner, Hunt, Williams, & Long, 1996; Ragozzino & Kesner, 2001), but not working memory for visual object (Ennaceur, Neave, & Aggleton, 1997; Kesner et al., 1996; Shaw & Aggleton, 1993), or affect (food reward value) information, (DeCote-au, Kesner, & Williams, 1997; Ragozzino & Kesner, 1999). There are also no deficits, with a few exceptions, in working memory for spatial information using delayed non-matching to position, delayed spatial alternation, or non-matching-to sample in a T- maze, 8 arm maze, and continuous spatial recognition memory procedures (Ennaceur et al., 1997; Har-rison & Mair, 1996; Kesner et al., 1996; Kolb, Sutherland, & Whishaw, 1983; Passingham, Myers, Rawlins, Lightfoot, & Fearn, 1988; Ragozzino, Adams, & Kesner, 1998; San-chez-Santed, deBruin, Heinsbroek, & Verwer, 1997; Shaw & Aggleton, 1993). Thus, the data suggest that the AC cortex and PC cortex process working memory for motor response information, but do not process working memory for visual object, spatial, or affect (food value) information. In mon-keys, enhanced single unit activity was recorded in the pre-motor cortex in relation to the go and no-go component of a delayed conditional go/no go task, suggesting an involve-ment of the premotor cortex in a working memory task com-ponent associated with motor movement (Watanabe, 1986). Recent work with humans using fMRI techniques showed that activation was observed in the premotor cortex in a de-layed response task (Turner & Levine, 2006).

The PL-IL cortex appears to play an important role in working memory for visual object and spatial location in-formation. Supporting evidence is based on the findings that lesions of the PL-IL cortex produce deficits in work-ing memory for spatial information (Brito & Brito, 1990; Delatour & Gisquet-Verrier, 1996; Granon, Vidal, Thinus-Blanc, Changeux, & Poucet, 1994; Horst & Laubach, 2009; Ragozzino et al., 1998; Seamans, Floresco, & Phillips, 1995), and working memory for visual object information (Di Pietro, Black, Green-Jordan, Eichenbaum, & Kantak, 2004; Kesner et al., 1996; Ragozzino, Detrick, & Kesner,

Figure 5. Frontal areas of the rat: A. Medial View. B. Ventral view. Abbreviations: PrCm (PC)-precentral cortex; AC-dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate; PL-IL-prelimbic andinfralimbic cortex; MO-medial orbital cortex; AI-dorsal and ventral agranular insular cortex; LO-lateral orbital cortex; VO-ventral orbital cortex; VLO-ventrolateral orbital cortex.

MOLO

VO

VLO

B. Ventral View

ALV

rs

Ald

PrCm

ACd

ACv

TT IL

cc

MO

PL

MOLO

VO

VLO

A. Medial View

ALV

rs

Ald

PrCm

ACd

ACv

TT IL

cc

MO

PL

Page 19: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 47

2001). However, PL-IL lesions do not produce a deficit in working memory for a food reward (DeCoteau et al., 1997; Ragozzino & Kesner, 1999). Further support of this conclu-sion was reported by Chang, Chen, Luo, Shi, and Woodward (2002), who found sustained neural firing in the PL-IL cor-tex during the delay within a delayed matching-to-position task, and Baeg et al. (2003) who recorded from the PL-IL cortex in a spatial delayed alternation task reported an in-crease in neural firing during the delay period.

In early research, Goldman-Rakic (1987, 1996) proposed that one could fractionate functions of the PFC on the basis of differential subregional contributions. She suggested that the main function of the PFC is to support working memory defined as a specialized process by which a remembered stimulus is held on line to guide behavior in the absence of external cues. Furthermore, she postulated a modular or-ganization of working memory based on the use of differ-ent domains or attributes of information processing. This is called the domain-specificity model, which would be con-sistent with the attribute model. In monkeys, the cortex sur-rounding the principal sulcus (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) is specialized for on line processing of spatial information, whereas the inferior convexity (ventrolateral prefrontal cor-tex) is specialized for on-line processing of visual object information. In addition, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex could also support other sensory domains. Support for this model is based on the observation of delay-specific cells in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for only spatial tasks, such as delayed response, delayed alternation, and delayed oculo-motor tasks, and the observation that lesions of the dorsolat-eral prefrontal cortex disrupt performance on a delayed re-sponse, delayed alternation, delayed oculomotor, and spatial search tasks (Butters & Pandya, 1969; Funahashi, Bruce, & Goldman-Rakic, 1993; Goldman & Rosvold, 1970; Mish-kin, 1957; Passingham, 1985). In contrast, delay-specific cells for a visual object delay task are found in the ventrolat-eral prefrontal cortex and lesions in this area disrupt visual object recognition (Mishkin & Manning, 1978; Wilson, Sc-alaidhe, & Goldman-Rakic, 1993).

Challenges to Goldman-Rakic’s domain specificity model for working memory as the main organizing principle for the prefrontal cortex have emerged based on research with mon-keys and humans. First, Rao, Rainer, and Miller (1997) have shown that one can record both spatial location and visual object information from the same cell within the dorsolat-eral prefrontal cortex and these cells can change very readily based on the demands of the task. Second, Fuster, Bauer, and Jervey (1982) showed that delay cells can be found in both the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in a visual-visual stimulus or spatial location-spatial loca-tion matching-to-sample tasks. In humans, D’Esposito et al. (1998) reported a meta-analysis of neuroimaging results

based on visual object and spatial location working memory tasks which provided a strong case for processing of both visual object and spatial location information in working memory in both the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Similar findings based on a meta-analysis of sixty PET and fMRI studies using working memory paradigms were reported by Wager and Smith (2003), showing that working memory for spatial and object location information resulted in activation of the ventral and dorsolateral prefron-tal cortex. Using another meta-analytic review, Owen (2000) reported results that support the findings mentioned in the above studies. Thus, it is clear that there is a large body of evidence based on recording and lesion studies supporting a working memory or short-term memory role for the PL-IL cortex in rats and the ventral and dorsolateral frontal cortex in monkeys and humans in working memory for spatial loca-tions and objects.

Based on anatomical and behavioral data, the agranular insular and lateral orbital (AI/LO) cortex appears to play an important role in working memory for affect attribute in-formation based on odor and taste. Supporting evidence is based on the findings that lesions of the AI/LO cortex pro-duce deficits in working memory for affect based on taste or odor information (DeCoteau et al., 1997; Di Pietro et al., 2004; Otto & Eichenbaum, 1992; Ragozzino & Kesner, 1999). Further support can be found in a study where sus-tained neuronal firing was observed in the AI/LO cortex dur-ing the delay period in a non-matching-to-sample for odors task (Ramus & Eichenbaum, 2000). It should be noted that AI/LO cortical lesions do not produce an effect on visual object or spatial working memory (DeCoteau et al., 1997; Di Pietro et al., 2004; Horst & Laubach, 2009; Ragozzino & Kesner, 1999), although in a recent study there were deficits using a delayed alternation task for odors with lesions of the PL-IL cortex, but there was extra damage to the AI/LO cor-tex (Kinoshita et al., 2008). Deficits in short-term memory for odors have been reported following damage to the orbital frontal cortex in humans (Jones-Gotman & Zatorre, 1993). Furthermore, Dade et al. (2001) reported increased activity based on PET scans in the orbital frontal cortex as well as dorsal and ventral lateral prefrontal cortex in an n-back task based on odor information.

In summary, within the rule-based memory system, dif-ferent brain regions process different attributes in support of short-term or working memory. Data are presented to sup-port this assertion by demonstrating that the AC/PC cortex mediates response attribute information, the PL/IL mediates spatial location and visual object attribute information, and AI/OL cortex mediates odor and taste information within the sensory-perceptual attribute. It appears that the different re-gions can be dissociated from each other based on specific attributes. Parallel results are found in monkeys and humans

Page 20: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 48

in that response information is mediated by the premotor cortex, spatial location and visual object information are mediated by the dorsal and ventral lateral prefrontal cortex, and odor and taste information are mediated by the orbital frontal cortex.

Paired Associate Learning

It is assumed that in addition to processing of temporal in-formation, the prefrontal cortex is also involved in mediating higher-order processes, such as rule learning based on the use of biconditional discrimination or paired associate para-digms. Passingham et al. (1988) showed that lesions of the AC and PC cortex in rats resulted in deficits in a visual con-ditional motor associative task. Similar results with the same type of lesion and the same visual conditional motor associa-tive task were reported by (St-Laurent, Petrides, & Sziklas, 2009). Based on behavioral and anatomical data, the PC cortex in the rat is assumed to be homologous to the premo-tor and supplementary motor area in monkeys and humans (periarcuate or posterior dorsal lateral area; Brodmann areas 6 and 8) in that the deficits observed in the visual-response conditional task and working memory for a motor response task in rats are rather similar to what has been described for monkeys and humans. For example, in monkeys, Halsband and Passingham (1985) showed that the premotor cortex is directly involved in mediating a visual-conditional motor task, but not a visual conditional non-motor task, suggesting that the response component is critical. Similar deficits in a visual conditional response task following premotor cor-tex lesions have been found in monkeys and humans (Hals-band & Freund, 1990; Petrides, 1982, 1985a, 1997). Based on fMRI analysis of learning arbitrary visual-response as-sociations, increased activity in the dorsal premotor cortex (Toni, Ramnani, Josephs, Ashburner, & Passingham, 2001) has been observed in addition to supplementary motor cor-tex interacting with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Boettiger & D’Esposito, 2005).

In a different set of studies, it has been shown that rats with lesions of the PL/IL, but not of the AC and PC cortex, fail to acquire an object-place association (Kesner & Ragozzino, 2003). In a subsequent study Lee and Solivan (2008) showed that temporary inactivation of the PL/IL cortex with musci-mol led to profound impairments in an object-place paired association task. Furthermore, impairment in a novelty de-tection paradigm using an object-in-place learning task has been observed in in rats with PL/IL lesions (Barker, Bird, Alexander, & Warburton, 2007).

Based on a different set of arbitrary associations, it has been shown in rats that lesions of AI/LO impair the learning of a cross-modal association involving odor and tactile stim-uli (Whishaw, Tomie, & Kolb, 1992). Furthermore, based

on single unit recording within the OL cortex of rats, it was found that many neurons were active during odor-location learning. In monkeys, in the orbital frontal cortex, many neurons were activated by both taste and odor stimuli (Rolls & Baylis, 1994), suggesting that flavor may be processed by orbital frontal cortex leading to pleasant experiences of-ten associated with reward. In humans, Small et al. (1999) showed that based on fMRI data there was activation of the orbital frontal cortex during the processing of taste and odor information.

In the context of other types of paired associate learning, Petrides (1985b) has shown that humans with PFC cortex lesions have difficulty in learning a paired associate task and Pigott and Milner (1993) reported that frontal lobe damaged patients are impaired for objects and places in a complex vi-sual scene task. Also, Klingberg and Roland (1998) showed that based on PET scans, the dorsal lateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex are activated during new learning of visual-auditory paired associates.

There are many interactions among the three systems. I will present just one example, namely an interaction between the prefrontal cortex (rule-based system) and hippocampus (event-based memory system) in the context of temporal processing of short-term memory information. In this study Lee and Kesner (2003) examined the dynamic interactions between the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus by training and testing rats on a delayed non-matching-to-place task on a radial 8-arm maze requiring memory for a single spatial location following short-term (i.e., 10 s or 5 min) delays. The results showed that inactivating both regions at the same time resulted in a severe impairment of short-term and inter-mediate memory for spatial information, suggesting that one of the structures needs to function properly for intact pro-cessing of short-term or intermediate-term spatial memory. Thus, the two regions interact with each other to ensure the processing of spatial information oveer a dynamic tempo-ral range including both short-term and intermediate term memory. The current results provide compelling evidence indicating that a mnemonic time window is a critical factor in dissociating the function of the hippocampal system from that of the medial prefrontal cortex in a delayed choice task. That is, the dorsal hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex appear to process spatial memory in parallel within a short-term range, whereas the dorsal hippocampal function be-comes more essential once the critical time window requires spatial memory for a time period exceeding that range.

In summary, within the rule-based memory system differ-ent brain regions process different attributes in support of paired associate learning. Data are presented to support this assertion by demonstrating that the PC cortex mediates as-sociative processes based on a visual conditional response

Page 21: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 49

learning, the PL/IL cortex mediates associative processes based on visual-spatial learning, and AI/LO cortex mediates associative processes based primarily on odor-taste learning. PPC mediates the spatial attribute for spatial perceptual in-formation and the TE2 cortex mediates the sensory-percep-tual attribute for visual object information. Parallel results are found in monkeys and humans in that for monkeys and humans the premotor mediates associative processes based on visual conditional response learning, the dorsal and ven-tral lateral prefrontal cortex mediate object-place learning, and the orbital prefrontal cortex mediates odor-taste associa-tions.

Other Theories of Memory

I will compare the tripartite attribute model with the O'Keefe and Nadel (1978) and Moscovitch et al. (2005); the Squire (1994) and Squire et al. (2004); and the Cohen and Eichenbaum, (1993), Eichenbaum (2004) and Eichenbaum et al., (2007) models. I will apply Schacter and Tulving (1994) suggestion that one needs to define memory systems in terms of the kind of information to be represented, the processes associated with the operation of each system, and the neurobiological substrates including neural structures and mechanisms that subserve each system.

O'Keefe and Nadel (1978) have concentrated on the role of the hippocampus as the neurobiological substrate in pro-cessing spatial and contextual information and more re-cently Nadel and Moscovitch (1998) have suggested that the hippocampus stores episodic memory. They do suggest that transfer of episodic information to the neocortex can occur to store semantic memories. The tripartite attribute model does not accept that all episodic (event-based) memories are stored in the hippocampus and emphasizes that the hip-pocampus supports temporal, odor, and language informa-tion in addition to space and context. The tripartite attribute model also states that new information is stored in a seman-tic or knowledge-based memory system via a consolidation process, but the model emphasizes that different neocortical areas store different attributes of memory which utilizes dif-ferent processes from the event-based memory system, such as perception, selective attention, and implicit memory. The Nadel model does not incorporate the prefrontal cortex in their memory model.

Squire proposes a declarative versus non-declarative sys-tem long-term memory model (Squire 1994; Squire et al., (2004). It is assumed that the declarative memory system is based on explicit information that is easily accessible and is concerned with specific facts or data. It includes episodic and semantic representations of propositions and images. On the other hand, the non-declarative memory system is based on implicit information that is not easily accessible

and includes unaware representations of motor, perceptual, and cognitive skills, as well as priming, simple classical conditioning, and non-associative learning. In this model the hippocampus and interconnected neural regions, such a perirhinal cortex, postrhinal/ parahippocampal gyrus, and entorhinal cortex encompasses the medial temporal lobe and is assumed to be the critical neural substrate in mediating all forms of memory within the declarative memory system. The non-declarative memory system include the mediation of skills and habits by the striatum, priming by the neocor-tex, simple classical conditioning of emotional responses by the amygdala, simple classical conditioning of skeletal mus-culature by the cerebellum, and non-associative learning by reflex pathways. It is assumed that the two memory systems are independent of each other. The Squire model assumes that the declarative memory system is based on conscious awareness and is involved in consolidation, and that all the areas of the medial temporal lobe are critical for recollection of all types of sensory information. In the tripartite attribute model, I do not differentiate the attributes on the basis of conscious awareness. Furthermore, I assume that for each attribute, the same processes operate in the event-based memory system and thus, non-declarative memory does not operate in the tripartite attribute model. In my model, the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus subserve different at-tribute information, such as object information and spatial information, respectively. Also, the prefrontal cortex is not usually incorporated in a specific memory system and the emphasis has not been on the different attributes of memory.

Cohen and Eichenbaum, (1993) and Eichenbaum (2004) propose that the declarative memory system is dependent upon the hippocampus, which provides a substrate for re-lational representation of all forms of memory including conjunctive, configural and arbitrary associations, as well as representational flexibility allowing for the retrieval of memories in novel situations. In contrast, the procedural sys-tem is independent of the hippocampus and is characterized by individual representations and inflexibility in retrieving memories in novel situations. In contrast to this declarative/non-declarative perspective, relational memory theory pro-poses that the medial temporal lobe function is independent of conscious awareness.

Recently, a model for episodic recognition memory was proposed and extended by Eichenbaum et al, (2007). This model, called the Binding Items and Context (BIC) model, proposed that information pertaining to item identity (i.e., “what”) resides primarily in the perirhinal cortex and infor-mation pertaining to the context wherein an item was ex-perienced (i.e., “where”) resides primarily in the postrhinal/parahippocampal cortex. The item and context information are transmitted through the lateral and medial entorhinal cor-tices, respectively, and they enter the hippocampus, at which

Page 22: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 50

point the item and the context are bound together into coher-ent episodes. I assume that for each attribute the same pro-cesses operate in the event-based memory system and thus, the different components of the medial temporal lobe utilize similar functions. Even though Eichenbaum is clearly aware of the importance of the prefrontal cortex in its interactions with the hippocampus, the prefrontal cortex is not included in the overall memory model.

Summary

Memory is a complex phenomenon due to a large number of potential interactions that are associated with the organi-zation of memory at the psychological and neural system level. Thus, it is not surprising that there many different models of memory (see other theories of memory section).

In the Kesner tripartite, multiple attribute, multiple pro-cess memory model, different forms of memory and its neu-robiological underpinnings are represented in terms of the nature, structure, or content of information representation as a set of different attributes including language, time, place, response, reward value (affect), and visual object as an ex-ample of sensory-perception. For each attribute, information is processed in the event-based memory system through a variety of operations but especially for short-term and in-termediate-term memory and pattern separation based on orthogonalization of specific attribute information. In ad-dition, for each attribute, information is processed in the knowledge-based system through a variety of operations, but especially for long-term storage and perceptual memory. Finally, for each attribute, it is assumed that information is processed in the rule-based memory system through the inte-gration of information from the event-based and knowledge-based memory systems for the use for major processes that include especially short-term or working memory and paired associate learning. The neural systems that subserve specific attributes within a system can operate independent of each other, even though there are also many possibilities for inter-actions among the attributes. Although the event-based and knowledge-based memory systems are supported by neural substrates and different operating characteristics, suggesting that the two systems can operate independent of each other, there are also important interactions between the two sys-tems, especially during the consolidation of new information and retrieval of previously stored information. Finally, be-cause it is assumed that the rule-based system is influenced by the integration of event-based and knowledge-based memory information, there should be important interactions between the event-based and knowledge-based memory sys-tems and the rule-based memory system. Thus, for each at-tribute, there is a neural circuit that encompasses all three memory systems in representing specific attribute informa-tion. In general, the tripartite attribute memory model repre-

sents the most comprehensive memory model capable of in-tegrating the extant knowledge concerning the neural system representation of memory.

It is important to note the new information that has been obtained during the last decade. For the event-based memo-ry system there has been (a) an extensive elaboration of the temporal attribute in terms of memory for duration, memory for sequential spatial processing, and memory for the senso-ry-perceptual attribute based on the operation of short-term memory or working memory, and (b) an emphasis on the characterization of an attribute-based pattern separation pro-cess. For the knowledge-based memory system, only a few studies have been added. For the rule-based memory system there has been a more elaborate detailed delineation of the different subregions of the prefrontal cortex in mediating working memory and paired associate learning, as well as a discussion of potential interactions between the event-based and rule-based memory systems.

References

Aimone, J. B., Wiles, J. & Gage, F. H. (2006). Potential role for adult neurogenesis in the encoding of time in new memories. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 723–727.

doi.org/10.1038/nn1707 PMid:16732202Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1994).

Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions fol-lowing bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature, 372, 669-672. doi.org/10.1038/372669a0

PMid:7990957Aggleton, J. P., Hunt, P. R., & Rawlins, J. N. P. (1986). The

effects of hippocampal lesions upon spatial and non-spa-tial tests of working memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 19, 133-146. doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(86)90011-2

Baeg, E. H., Kim, Y. B., Huh, K., Mook-Jung, I., Kim, H. T., & Jung, M. W. (2003). Dynamics of population code for working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 40, 177-188. doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00597-X

Banks, W. P. (1978). Encoding and processing of symbolic information in comparative judgements. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Ad-vances in theory and research (pp. 101–159). New York: Academic.

Barker, G. R. I., Bird, F., Alexander, V., & Warburton, E. C. (2007). Recognition memory for objects, place, and temporal order: A disconnection analysis of the role of the medial prefrontal cortex and perirhinal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 2948-2957.

doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5289-06.2007 PMid:17360918Baumann, O., Chan E., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Distinct

neural networks underlie encoding of categorical versus

Page 23: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 51

coordinate spatial relations during active navigation. Neu-roimage, 60, 1630-1637.

doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.089 PMid:22300811Benton, A. L. (1969). Disorder of spatial orientation. In P. J.

Vinken & G. W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neu-rology (Vol. 3). Amsterdam: North Holland.

Boettiger, C. A., & D'Esposito, M. (2005). Frontal networks for learning and executing arbitrary stimulus-response as-sociations. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2723-2732.

doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3697-04.2005 PMid:15758182Brito, G. N. O., & Brito, L. S. O. (1990). Septohippocampal

system and the prelimbic sector of frontal cortex: A neu-ropsychological battery analysis in the rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 36, 52-58.

doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(90)90167-DBuffalo, E. A., Ramus, S. J., Clark, R. E., Teng, E., Squire,

L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1999). Dissociation between the ef-fects of damage to perirhinal cortex and area TE. Learning and Memory, 6, 572-599. doi.org/10.1101/lm.6.6.572

PMid:10641763 PMCid:311316Bussey, T. J., Saksida, L. M., & Murray, E. A. (2002). Peri-

rhinal cortex resolves feature ambiguity in complex dis-criminations. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 365–374. doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01851.x

PMid:11849302Butters, N., & Pandya, D. (1969). Retention of delayed-al-

ternation: Effect of selective lesions of sulcus principalis. Science, 165, 1271-1273.

doi.org/10.1126/science.165.3899.1271 PMid:4979528Cave, C. B., & Squire, L. R. (1992). Intact verbal and non-

verbal short-term memory following damage to the hu-man hippocampus. Hippocampus, 2, 151-163.

doi.org/10.1002/hipo.450020207 PMid:1308180Chang, J. Y., Chen, L., Luo, F., Shi, L. H., & Woodward, D.

J. (2002). Neuronal responses in the frontal cortico-basal ganglia system during delayed matching-to-sample task: Ensemble recording in freely moving rats. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 67-80.

doi.org/10.1007/s00221-001-0918-3 PMid:11797085Chen, L. L., Lin, L., Barnes, C. A., & McNaughton, B. L.

(1994). Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex II: Contributions of visual and ideothetic information to the directional firing. Experimental Brain Research, 101, 24-34. doi.org/10.1007/BF00243213 PMid:7843299

Chiba, A. A., Johnson, D. L., & Kesner, R. P. (1992). The effects of lesions of the dorsal hippocampus or the ventral hippocampus on performance of a spatial location order recognition task. Society for Neuroscience Abstract, 18, 1422.

Chiba, A. A., Kesner, R. P., & Jackson, P. (2002). Two forms of spatial memory: A double dissociation between the pa-

rietal cortex and the hippocampus in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 874-883.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.116.5.874 PMid:12369807Chiba, A. A., Kesner, R. P., Matsuo, F., & Heilbrun, M. P.

(1990). A dissociation between verbal and spatial memory following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Society for Neu-roscience Abstracts, 16, 286.

Chiba, A. A., Kesner, R. P., Matsuo, F., & Heilbrun, M. P. (1993). A dissociation between affect and recognition fol-lowing unilateral temporal lobectomy including the amyg-dala. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 19, 792.

Chiba, A. A., Kesner, R. P., & Reynolds, A. M. (1994). Memory for spatial location as a function of temporal lag in rats: Role of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 61, 123–131.

doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(05)80065-2Clelland, C. D., et al. (2009). A functional role for adult hip-

pocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Sci-ence, 325, 210–213. doi.org/10.1126/science.1173215

PMid:19590004 PMCid:2997634Cohen, N. J., & Eichenbaum, H. B. (1993). Memory, am-

nesia, and hippocampal function. Cambridge: MIT Press.Colombo, P. J., Davis, H. P., & Volpe, B. T. (1989). Allocen-

tric spatial and tactile memory impairments in rats with dorsal caudate lesions are affected by preoperative behav-ioral training. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 1242-1250.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.103.6.1242 PMid:2610917Cook, D., & Kesner, R. P. (1988). Caudate nucleus and

memory for egocentric localization. Behavioral Neural Biology, 49, 332-343.

doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(88)90338-XDade, L. A., Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., & Jones-Gotman,

M. (2001). Working memory in another dimension: Func-tional imaging of human olfactory working memory. Neu-roImage, 14, 650-660. doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0868

PMid:11506538Davis, J. D., Filoteo, J. V., Kesner, R. P., & Roberts, J. W.

(2003). Recognition memory for hand positions and spa-tial locations in patients with Huntington's disease: Dif-ferential visuospatial memory impairment? Cortex, 39, 239-253. doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70107-2

Davis, J. D., Filoteo, J. V., & Kesner, R. P. (2007). Is short-term memory for discrete arm movements impaired in Huntington's disease? Cortex, 43, 255-263.

doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70480-5Dean, P. (1990). Sensory cortex: Visual perceptual func-

tions. In B. Kolb & R. C. Tees (Eds.), Cerebral cortex of the rat (pp. 275-308). Cambridge: MIT Press.

PMid:1975555DeCoteau, W. E., Hoang, L., Huff, L., Stone, A., & Kesner,

R. P. (2004). Effects of hippocampus and medial caudate nucleus lesions on memory for direction information in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 540-545.

Page 24: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 52

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.118.3.540 PMid:15174931DeCoteau, W. E., & Kesner, R. P. (1998). Effects of hip-

pocampal and parietal cortex lesions on the processing of multiple object scenes. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 68-82. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.112.1.68

PMid:9517816DeCoteau, W. E., Kesner, R. P., & Williams, J. M. (1997).

Short-term memory for food reward magnitude: The role of the prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 88, 239-249. doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00044-2

Delatour, B., & Gisquet-Verrier, P. (2001). Involvement of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in temporal behavioral sequencing: Subregional analysis of the medial prefrontal cortex in rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 126, 105-114.

doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00251-0D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E., Ballard, D., Shin,

R. K., & Lease, J. (1998). Functional MRI studies of spa-tial and nonspatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Re-search, 7, 1-13. doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00004-4

De Renzi, E. (1982). Disorders of space exploration and cognition. New York: Wiley.

DiMattia, B. V., & Kesner, R. P. (1988a). The role of the posterior parietal association cortex in the processing of spatial event information. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 397-403. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.102.3.397

PMid:3395449DiMattia, B. V., & Kesner, R. P. (1988b). Spatial cognitive

maps: Differential role of parietal cortex and hippocampal formation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 471-480.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.102.4.471 PMid:3166721Di Pietro, N. C., Black, Y. D., Green-Jordan, K., Eichen-

baum, H. B., & Kantak, K. M. (2004). Complementary tasks to measure working memory in distinct prefrontal cortex subregions in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 1042-1051. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.1042

PMid:15506886Disterhoft, J. F., Carrillo, M. C., Hopkins, R. O., Gabrieli,

J. D. E., & Kesner, R. P. (1996). Impaired trace eyeblink conditioning in severe medial temporal lobe amnesics. So-ciety for Neuroscience Abstracts, 22, 1866.

Divac, I., Rosvold, H. E., & Szwarcbart, M. K. (1967). Be-havioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucle-us. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol-ogy, 63, 184-190. doi.org/10.1037/h0024348

PMid:4963561Douglas, R. J., & Pribram, K. H. (1966). Learning and lim-

bic lesions. Neuropsychology, 4, 197-220. doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(66)90028-5Dunnett, S. B. (1990). Role of prefrontal cortex and striatal

output systems in short-term memory deficits associated with ageing, basal forebrain lesions, and cholinergic-rich grafts. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 44, 210-232.

doi.org/10.1037/h0084240 PMid:2383812Eichenbaum, H. (2004). Hippocampus: Cognitive processes

and neural representations that underlie declarative mem-ory. Neuron, 44, 109-120.

doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.028 PMid:15450164Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007).

The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. An-nual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 123-152.

doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328 PMid:17417939 PMCid:2064941Ellis, A. X., Sala, S. D., & Logie, R. H. (1996). The Bai-

liwick of visuo-spatial working memory: evidence from unilateral spatial neglect. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 71-78. doi.org/10.1016/0926-6410(95)00031-3

Ennaceur, A., Neave, N., & Aggleton, J. P. (1997). Sponta-neous object recognition and object location memory in rats: The effects of lesions in the cingulate cortices, the medial prefrontal cortex, the cingulum bundle and the for-nix. Experimental Brain Research, 113, 509-519.

doi.org/10.1007/PL00005603Estes, W. K. (1986). Memory for temporal information. In J.

A. Michon & J. L. Jackson (Eds.), Time, mind and behav-ior (pp. 151–168). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Flaherty, C. F., Turovsky, J., Krauss, K. L. (1994). Relative hedonic value modulates anticipatory contrast. P h y s i o l -ogy and Behavior, 55, 1047–1054.

doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)90386-7Fortin, N. J., Agster, K. L., & Eichenbaum, H. B. (2002).

Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 458-462.

PMid:11976705Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993).

Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed performance: Evidence for mnemonic "scotomas." Neuro-science, 13, 1479-1497. PMid:8463830

Fuster, J. M. (1995). Memory in the cerebral cortex: An em-pirical approach to neural networks in the human and nonhuman primate. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Fuster, J. M., Bauer, R. H., & Jervey, J. P. (1982). Cellular discharge in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the mon-key in cognitive tasks. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 443-450.

Gaffan, D. (1992). Amygdala and the memory of reward. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss.

Gaffan, D., & Murray E. A. (1992). Monkeys (Macaca fas-cicularis) with rhinal cortex ablations succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hr intertrial intervals and fail at matching to sample despite double sample pre-sentations. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 30-38.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.106.1.30 PMid:1554436Gilbert, P. E., & Kesner, R. P. (2002). The amygdala but not

the hippocampus is involved in pattern separation based

Page 25: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 53

on reward value. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 77, 338–353. doi.org/10.1006/nlme.2001.4033

PMid:11991762Gilbert, P. E., & Kesner, R. P. (2003). Recognition memory

for complex visual discrimination is influenced by stimu-lus interference in rodents with perirhinal cortex damage. Learning and Memory, 10, 525–530.

doi.org/10.1101/lm.64503 PMid:14657264 PMCid:305468Gilbert, P. E., Kesner, R. P., & Lee, I. (2001). Dissociating

hippocampal subregions: A double dissociation between the dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus, 11, 626–636.

doi.org/10.1002/hipo.1077 PMid:11811656Goldman, P. S., & Rosvold, H. E. (1970). Localization of

function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. Experimental Neurology, 27, 291-304.

doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(70)90222-0Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Circuitry of primate prefron-

tal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational memory. In F. Plum & V. Mountcastle (Eds.), Handbook of physiology: The nervous system (pp. 373-417). Bethes-da: American Physiological Society.

Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996). The prefrontal landscape: Implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philosophi-cal Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Biology, 351, 1445-1453. doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1996.0129

PMid:8941956Goodrich-Hunsaker, N. J., Hunsaker, M. R., & Kesner, R.

P. (2005). Dissociating the role of the parietal cortex and dorsal hippocampus for spatial information processing. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 1307-1315.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.5.1307 PMid:16300437 Granon, S., Vidal, C., Thinus-Blanc, C., Changeux, J.-P., &

Poucet, B. (1994). Working memory, response selection, and effortful processing in rats with medial prefrontal le-sions. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 883-891.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.108.5.883 PMid:7826511Gross, C. G. (1973). Inferotemporal cortex and vision. In E.

Stellar & J. M. Sprague (Eds.), Progress in physiological psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 77-123). New York: Academic.

Halsband, U., & Freund, H. J. (1990). Premotor cortex and conditional motor learning in man. Brain, 113, 207-222.

doi.org/10.1093/brain/113.1.207 PMid:2302533Halsband, U., & Passingham, R. E. (1985). Premotor cor-

tex and the conditions for movement in monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioural Brain Research, 18, 269-276.

doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(85)90035-XHampson, R. E., Heyser, C. J., & Deadwyler, S. A. (1993).

Hippocampal cell firing correlates of delayed-match-to-sample performance in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 715-739. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.107.5.715

PMid:8280383

Hannesson, D. K., Howland, J. G., & Phillips, A. G. (2004). Interaction between perirhinal and medial prefrontal cor-tex is required for temporal order but not recognition memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 4596–4603.

doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5517-03.2004 PMid:15140931Harrison, L. D., & Mair, R. G. (1996). A comparison of the

effects of frontal cortical and thalamic lesions on mea-sures of spatial learning and memory in the rat. Behav-ioural Brain Research, 75, 195-206.

doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(96)00173-8Ho, J. W.-T., Narduzzo, K. E., Outram, A., Tinsley, C .J.,

Henley, J. M., Warburton, E. C., & Brown, M. W. (2011). Contributions of area Te2 to rat recognition memory. Learning and Memory, 18, 493-501.

doi.org/10.1101/lm.2167511 PMid:21700715 PMCid:3125610Hoge, J., & Kesner R. P. (2007). Role of CA3 and CA1 sub-

regions of the dorsal hippocampus on the temporal pro-cessing of objects. Neurobiology of Learning and Memo-ry, 88, 225–231. doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2007.04.013

PMid:17560815 PMCid:2095779Holden, H. M., Hoebel, C., Loftis, K., & Gilbert, P. E. (2012).

Spatial pattern separation in cognitively normal young and older adults. Hippocampus. Advanced online publication. doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22017 PMid:22467270

Hopkins, R. O. & Kesner, R. P. (1993). Memory for tempo-ral and spatial distances for new and previously learned geographical information in hypoxic subjects. Paper pre-sented at the Society for Neuroscience Meeting, Washing-ton, DC.

Hopkins, R. O., & Kesner, R. P. (1994). Short-term memory for duration in hypoxic subjects. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 20, 1075.

Hopkins, R. O., Kesner, R. P., & Goldstein, M. (1995a). Item and order recognition memory for words, pictures, abstract pictures, spatial locations, and motor responses in subjects with hypoxic brain injury. Brain and Cognition, 27, 180-201. doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1995.1016

PMid:7772332Hopkins, R. O., Kesner, R. P., & Goldstein, M. (1995b).

Memory for novel and familiar spatial and linguistic tem-poral distance information in hypoxic subjects. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 454-468.

doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700000552 PMid:9375231Horel, J. A., Pytko-Joiner, D. E., Boytko, M. L., & Salsbury,

K. (1987). The performance of visual tasks while seg-ments of the inferotemporal cortex are suppressed by cold. Behavioural Brain Research, 23, 29-42.

doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(87)90240-3Horst, N. K., & Laubach, M. (2009). The role of rat dorso-

medial prefrontal cortex in spatial working memory. Neu-

Page 26: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 54

roscience, 164, 444-456. doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.004 PMid:19665526 PMCid:2761984Hunsaker, M. R., Thorup, J. A., Welch, T., & Kesner, R. P.

(2006). The role of CA3 and CA1 in the acquisition of an object-trace-place paired associate task. Behavioral Neu-roscience, 120, 1252-1256.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1252 PMid:17201469Hunsaker, M. R., Fieldsted, P. M., Rosenberg, J. S., & Kes-

ner, R. P. (2008). Dissociating the roles of dorsal and ven-tral CA1 for the temporal processing of spatial locations, visual objects, and odors. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 643–650. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.122.3.643

PMid:18513134Hunsaker, M. R., Mooy, G. G., Swift, J. S., & Kesner, R. P.

(2007). Dissociations of the medial and lateral perforant path projections into dorsal DG, CA3, and CA1 for spatial and nonspatial (visual object) information processing. Be-havioral Neuroscience, 121, 742-750.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.121.4.742 PMid:17663599Jackson-Smith, P., Kesner, R. P., & Chiba, A. A. (1993).

Continuous recognition of spatial and nonspatial stimuli in hippocampal lesioned rats. Behavioral and Neural Biol-ogy, 59, 107-119.

doi.org/10.1016/0163-1047(93)90821-X Jackson, P. A., Kesner, R. P., & Amann, K. (1998). Memory

for duration: Role of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 70, 328-348. doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1998.3859 PMid:9774525

Jeffery, K. J., & Anderson, M. I. (2003). Dissociation of the geometric and contextual influences on place cells. Hip-pocampus, 13, 868–872. doi.org/10.1002/hipo.10162

PMid:14620882Jones, B., & Mishkin, M. (1972). Limbic lesions and the

problem of stimulus-reinforcement associations. Experi-mental Neurology, 36, 362-377.

doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(72)90030-1Jones-Gotman, M., & Zatorre, R. J. (1993). Odor recogni-

tion memory in humans: Role of right temporal and orbi-tofrontal regions. Brain and Cognition, 22, 182-198.

doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1993.1033 PMid:8373572Keane, M. M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Mapstone, H. C., John-

son, K. A., & Corkin, S. (1995). Double dissociation of memory capacities after bilateral occipital-lobe or medial temporal-lobe lesions. Brain, 118, 1129-1148.

doi.org/10.1093/brain/118.5.1129 PMid:7496775Kesner, R. P. (1998). Neurobiological views of memory. In J.

L. Martinez & R. P. Kesner (Eds.), Neurobiology of learn-ing and memory (pp. 361-416). San Diego: Academic.

doi.org/10.1016/B978-012475655-7/50011-3Kesner, R. P. (2000). Subregional analysis of mnemonic

functions of the prefrontal cortex in the rat. Psychobiol-ogy, 28, 219-228.

Kesner, R. P. (2002). Memory neurobiology. In V. S. Ram-achadran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the human brain (Vol. 2, pp. 783-796). San Diego: Academic.

doi.org/10.1016/B0-12-227210-2/00200-4Kesner, R. P., Bolland, B. L., & Dakis, M. (1993). Memory

for spatial locations, motor responses, and objects: Triple dissociation among the hippocampus, caudate nucleus, and extrastriate visual cortex. Experimental Brain Re-search, 93, 462-470. doi.org/10.1007/BF00229361

PMid:8519335Kesner, R. P., Farnsworth, G., & Kametani, H. (1992). Role

of parietal cortex and hippocampus in representing spatial information. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 367-373.

doi.org/10.1093/cercor/1.5.367Kesner, R. P., Hunsaker, M. R. & Gilbert P. E. (2005). The

role of CA1 in the acquisition of an object-trace-odor paired associate task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 781-786. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.781

PMid:15998199Kesner, R. P., & Gilbert, P. E. (2006). The role of the medial

caudate nucleus, but not the hippocampus, in a matching-to sample task for a motor response. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1888–1894.

doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04709.x PMid:16623845Kesner, R. P., Gilbert, P. E., & Barua, L. A. (2002). The role

of the hippocampus in memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odors. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 286–290. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.116.2.286

PMid:11996313Kesner, R., & Hopkins, R. (2001). Short-term memory for

duration and distance in humans: Role of the hippocam-pus. Neuropsychology 15, 58-68.

doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.15.1.58 PMid:11216890Kesner, R. P, Hunsaker M. R., & Ziegler, W. (2010). The

role of the dorsal CA1 and ventral CA1 in memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odors. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 93, 111-116.

doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.08.010 PMid:19733676Kesner, R. P., Hunt, M. E., Williams, J. M., & Long, J. M.

(1996). Prefrontal cortex and working memory for spatial response, spatial location, and visual object information in the rat. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 311-318.

doi.org/10.1093/cercor/6.2.311 PMid:8670659Kesner, R. P., Lee, I., & Gilbert, P. (2004). A behavioral as-

sessment of hippocampal function based on a subregional analysis. Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 333–351.

doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2004.15.5.333 PMid:15575490Kesner, R. P., & Ragozzino, M. E. (2003). The role of the

prefrontal cortex in object-place learning: A test of the attribute specificity model. Behavioural Brain Research, 146, 159-165. doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.024

Page 27: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 55

PMid:14643468Kesner, R. P., Ravindranathan, A., Jackson, P., Giles, R., &

Chiba, A. A. (2001). A neural circuit analysis of visual ob-ject recognition memory: Role of perirhinal, medial and lateral entorhinal cortex. Learning & Memory, 8, 87-95.

doi.org/10.1101/lm.29401 PMid:11274254 PMCid:311369Kesner, R. P., Walser, R. D., & Winzenried, G. (1989). Cen-

tral but not basolateral amygdala mediates memory for positive affective experiences. Behavioural Brain Re-search, 33, 189-195.

doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(89)80050-6Kesner, R. P., & Williams, J. M. (1995). Memory for magni-

tude of reinforcement: Dissociation between the amygdala and hippocampus. Neurobiology of Learning and Memo-ry, 64, 237-244. doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1995.0006

PMid:8564377Kinoshita, S., Yokohama, C., Masaki, D., Yamashita, T.,

Tsuchida, H., Nakatomi, Y., & Fukui, K. (2008). Effects of rat medial prefrontal cortex lesions on olfactory serial reversal and delayed alternation tasks. Neuroscience Re-search, 60, 213-218.

doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2007.10.012 PMid:18077035Kirwan, C. B., & Stark, E. L. (2007). Overcoming interfer-

ence: An fMRI investigation of pattern separation in the medial temporal lobe. Learning and Memory, 14, 625–633. doi.org/10.1101/lm.663507 PMid:17848502

PMCid:1994079Klingberg, T., & Roland, P. E. (1998). Right prefrontal acti-

vation during encoding, but not during retrieval, in a non-verbal paired-associates task. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 73-79.

doi.org/10.1093/cercor/8.1.73 PMid:9510387Kolb, B., Sutherland, R. J., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1983). A com-

parison of the contributions of the frontal and parietal as-sociation cortex to spatial localization in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 97, 13-27.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.97.1.13 PMid:6838719Kubie, J. L., & Ranck, J. B. (1983). Sensory-behavioral cor-

relates in individual hippocampal neurons in three situa-tions: Space and context. In W. Seifert (Ed.), Neurobiology of the hippocampus (pp. 433-447). New York: Academic.

Lee, I., Hunsaker, M. R., & Kesner, R. P. (2005b). The role of hippocampal subregions in detecting spatial novelty. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 145-153.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.145 PMid:15727520Lee, I., Jerman, T. S., & Kesner, R. P. (2005a). Disruption of

delayed memory for a sequence of spatial locations fol-lowing CA1- or CA3-lesions of the dorsal hippocampus. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 84, 138-147.

doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2005.06.002 PMid:16054848 Lee, I., & Kesner, R. P. (2003). Time-dependent relationship

between the dorsal hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex in spatial memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1517-

1523. PMid:12598640Lee I., Rao G., & Knierim, J. J. (2004). A double dissociation

between hippocampal subfields: differential time course of CA3 and CA1 place cells for processing changed envi-ronments. Neuron, 42, 803-815.

doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.05.010 PMid:15182719Lee, I., & Solivan, F. (2008). The roles of the medial prefron-

tal cortex and hippocampus in a spatial paired-association task. Learning and Memory, 15, 357-367.

doi.org/10.1101/lm.902708 PMid:18463175 PMCid:2364607Long, J. M., & Kesner, R. P. (1996). The effects of dorsal

vs. ventral hippocampal, total hippocampal, and parietal cortex lesions on memory for allocentric distance in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, 922-932.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.110.5.922 PMid:8918996Long, J. M., Mellem, J. E., & Kesner, R. P. (1998). The ef-

fects of parietal cortex lesions on an object/spatial location paired-associate task in rats. Psychobiology, 26, 128-133.

Madsen, J., & Kesner, R. P. (1995). The temporal-distance effect in subjects with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 9, 94–100.

doi.org/10.1097/00002093-199509020-00006 PMid:7662329Marshuetz, C. (2005). Order information in working mem-

ory: An integrative review of evidence from brain and be-havior. Psychology Bulletin, 131, 323–339.

doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.323 PMid:15869331McCarthy, R. A. & Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive psy-

chology: A clinical introduction. London: Academic.McNaughton, B. L., Barnes, C. A., & O'Keefe, J. (1983).

The contributions of position, direction and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Experimental Brain Research, 52, 41-49.

doi.org/10.1007/BF00237147 PMid:6628596McNaughton, B. L., Chen, L. L., & Marcus, E. J. (1991).

"Dead reckoning", landmark learning, and the sense of di-rection: A neurophysiological and computational hypoth-esis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 190-202.

doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1991.3.2.190Mehta, M. R., Barnes, C. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1997).

Experience-dependent, asymmetric expansion of hippo-campal place fields. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences US, 94, 8918-8921.

doi.org/10.1073/pnas.94.16.8918Mehta, M. R., Quirk, M. C., & Wilson, M. A. (2000). Ex-

perience-dependent asymmetric shape of hippocampal re-ceptive fields. Neuron, 25, 707-715.

doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81072-7Meck, W. H., Church, R. M., & Olton, D. S. (1984). Hippo-

campus, time and memory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 3-22. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.98.1.3 PMid:6696797

Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the local-

Page 28: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 56

ization of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272-277. PMid:4937273

Milner, A. D., Ockleford, E. M., & DeWar, W. (1977). Vi-suo-spatial performance following posterior parietal and lateral frontal lesions in stumptail macaques. Cortex, 13, 170-183.

Mishkin, M. (1957). Effects of small prefrontal lesions on delayed alternation in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiol-ogy, 220, 615-622.

Mishkin, M., & Manning, F. J. (1978). Non-spatial memory after selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys. Brain Re-search, 143, 313-323.

doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(78)90571-1Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, Addis DR, West-

macott R, Grady C, McAndrews MP, Levine B, Black S, Winocur G, and Nadel L.(2005). Functional neuroanato-my of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. Journal of Anatomy, 207, 35-66.

doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x PMid:16011544 PMCid:1571502Moser, E. I., Kropff. E. & Moser, M. B. (2008). Place cells,

grid cells, and the brain 's spatial representation system. Annual Review Neuroscience, 31, 69-89.

doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723 PMid:18284371Moyer, J. R. Jr., Deyo, R. A., & Disterhoft, J. F. (1990). Hip-

pocampectomy disrupts trace eye-blink conditioning in rabbits. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 243-252.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.104.2.243 PMid:2346619Muller, R. U., Ranck, J. B. Jr., & Taube, J. S. (1996). Head

direction cells: Properties and functional significance. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 6, 196-206.

doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80073-0Mumby, D. G., & Pinel, J. P. J. (1994). Rhinal cortex lesions

and object recognition in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 11-18. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.108.1.11

PMid:8192836Mumby, D. G., Wood, E. R., & Pinel, J. P. J. (1992). Object

recognition memory is only mildly impaired in rats with lesions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Psychobiology, 20, 18-27.

Myers, C. E., Gluck, M. A., & Granger, R. (1995). Dissocia-tion of hippocampal and entorhinal function in associative learning: A computational approach. Psychobiology, 23, 116-138.

Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. (1998) Hippocampal contri-butions to cortical plasticity. Neuropharmacology, 37, 431–439.

Norman, G., & Eacott, M. J. (2004). Impaired recognition with increasing levels of feature ambiguity in rats with perirhinal cortex lesions. Behaviour Brain Research, 148, 79-91. doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00176-1

Oberg, R. G. E., & Divac, I. (1979). "Cognitive" functions of the neostriatum. In I. Divac & R. G. E. Oberg (Eds.), The neostriatum (pp. 291-313). Oxford: Pergamon.

O"Keefe J. & Nadel L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cogni-tive map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

O'Keefe, J. (1983). Spatial memory within and without the hippocampal system. In W. Seifert (Ed.), Neurobiology of the hippocampus (pp. 375-403). London: Academic.

O'Keefe, J., & Burgess, N. (1996). Geometric determinants of the place field of hippocampal neurons. Nature, 381, 425–428. doi.org/10.1038/381425a0

PMid:8632799O'Keefe, J., & Speakman, A. (1987). Single unit activity in

the rat hippocampus during a spatial memory task. Experi-mental Brain Research, 68, 1-27.

doi.org/10.1007/BF00255230Olton, D. S. (1983). Memory functions and the hippocam-

pus. In W. Seifert (Ed.), Neurobiology of the hippocam-pus. New York: Academic.

Olton, D. S. (1986). Hippocampal function and memory for temporal context. In R. L. Isaacson & K. H. Pribram (Eds.), The hippocampus (Vol. 3). New York: Plenum.

doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-8024-9_9Olton, D. S., Wenk, G. L., Church, R. M., & Weck, W. H.

(1988). Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by simultaneous temporal processing. Neuropsychologia, 26, 307-318. doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(88)90083-8

O'Reilly, R. C., & McClelland, J. L. (1994). Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus, 4, 661–682.

doi.org/10.1002/hipo.450040605 PMid:7704110Otto, T., & Eichenbaum, H. (1992). Complementary roles of

the orbital prefrontal cortex and the perirhinal-entorhinal cortices in an odor-guided delayed-nonmatching-to-sam-ple task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 762-775.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.106.5.762 PMid:1445656Owen, A. M. (2000). The role of the lateral frontal cortex

in mnemonic processing: The contribution of functional neuroimaging. Experimental Brain Research, 133, 33-43.

doi.org/10.1007/s002210000398Parkinson, J. K., Murray, E. A., & Mishkin, M. (1988). A

selective mnemonic role for the hippocampus in monkeys: Memory for the location of objects. Journal of Neurosci-ence, 8, 4159-4167. PMid:3183716

Partiot, A., Verin, M., Pillon, B., Teixeira-Ferreira, C., Agid, Y., & Dubois, B. (1996). Delayed response tasks in basal ganglia lesions in man: Further evidence for a striato-fron-tal cooperation in behavioural adaptation. Neuropsycholo-gia, 34, 709-721.

doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00143-3Pasquier, F., Van Der Linden, M., Lefebvre, C., Bruyer, R.,

& Petit, H. (1994). Motor memory and the preselection effect in Huntington's and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsy-

Page 29: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 57

chologia, 32, 951-968.Passingham, R. E. (1985). Memory of monkeys (Macaca

mulatta) with lesions in prefrontal cortex. Behavioral Neu-roscience, 99, 3-21. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.99.1.3

PMid:4041231Passingham, R. E., Myers, C., Rawlins, N., Lightfoot, V., &

Fearn, S. (1988). Premotor cortex in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 101-109.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.102.1.101 PMid:3355650Petrides, M. (1982). Motor conditional associative learning

after selective prefrontal lesions in the monkey. Behav-ioural Brain Research, 5, 407-413.

doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(82)90044-4Petrides, M. (1985a). Deficits in nonspatial conditional as-

sociative learning after periarcuate lesions in the monkey. Behavioural Brain Research, 16, 95-101.

doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(85)90085-3Petrides, M. (1985b). Deficits on conditional associate learn-

ing tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 23, 601-614.

doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(85)90062-4Petrides, M. (1997). Visuo-motor conditional associative

learning after frontal and temporal lesions in the human brain. Neuropsychologia, 35, 989-997.

doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00026-2Petrides, M., & Iversen, S. D. (1979). Restricted posterior

parietal lesions in the rhesus monkey and performance on visuo-spatial tasks. Brain Research, 161, 63-77.

doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(79)90196-3Pigott, S. & Milner, B. (1993). Memory for different aspects

of complex visual scenes after unilateral temporal- or frontal-lobe resection. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1-15.

doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(93)90076-CPoucet, B. (1989). Object exploration, habituation, and re-

sponse to a spatial change in rats following septal or me-dial frontal cortical damage. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 1009–1016. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.103.5.1009 PMid:2803548

Pohl, W. (1973). Dissociation of spatial discrimination defi-cits following frontal and parietal lesions in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 227-239. doi.org/10.1037/h0033922

PMid:4632974Quirk, G. J., Muller, R. U., Kubie, J. L.,& Ranck, J. B. Jr.

(1992). The positional firing properties of medial entorhi-nal neurons: Description and comparison with hippocam-pal place cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 1945-1963.

Ragozzino, M. E., Adams, S., & Kesner, R. P. (1998). Dif-ferential involvement of the dorsal anterior cingulate and prelimbic-infralimbic areas of the rodent prefrontal cor-tex in spatial working memory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 293-303. doi.org/10.1037/h0090326

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.112.2.293 PMid:9588479Ragozzino, M. E., & Kesner, R.P. (1999). The role of the

agranular insular cortex in working memory for food reward value and allocentric space in rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 98, 103-112.

doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00058-8Ragozzino, M. E., Detrick, S., & Kesner, R. P. (2001). The

effects of prelimbic and infralimbic lesions on working memory for visual objects in rats. Neurobiology of Learn-ing and Memory, 77, 29-43.

doi.org/10.1006/nlme.2001.4003 PMid:11749084Ragozzino, M. E., & Kesner, R. P. (2001). The role of rat

dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in working memory for egocentric responses. Neuroscience Letters, 308, 145-148.

doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02020-1Ramus, S. J., & Eichenbaum, H. (2000). Neural corre-

lates of olfactory recognition memory in the rat orbito-frontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8199-8208. PMid:11050143

Rao, S. R., Rainer, G., & Miller, E. K. (1997). Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 276, 821-823. doi.org/10.1126/science.276.5313.821

PMid:9115211Rogers, J. L., Hunsaker, M. R., & Kesner, R. P. (2006). Ef-

fects of ventral and dorsal CA1 subregional lesions on trace fear conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86, 72-81. doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.002

PMid:16504548Rolls, E. T. (1989). Functions of neuronal networks in the

hippocampus and neocortex in memory. In J. H. Bryne & W.O. Berry (Eds.), Neural models of plasticity: Experi-mental and theoretical approaches (pp. 240–265). San Diego: Academic.

Rolls, E. T. (1996). A theory of hippocampal function in memory. Hippocampus, 6, 601–620.

doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:6<601::AID-HIPO5>3.0.CO;2-J

Rolls, E. T., & Baylis, L.L. (1994). Gustatory, olfactory, and visual convergence within the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 5437-5452. PMid:8083747

Rolls, E. T., & Kesner, R. P. (2006). A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory. Progress in Neurobiology, 79, 1–48.

doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.04.005 PMid:16781044Sakai K., & Miyashita,Y. (1991). Neural organization for the

long-term memory of paired associates. Nature, 354, 152-155. doi.org/10.1038/354152a0 PMid:1944594

Sanberg, P. R., Lehmann, J., & Fibiger, H. C. (1978). Im-paired learning and memory after kainic acid lesions of the striatum: A behavioral model of Huntington's disease. Brain Research, 149, 546-551.

doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(78)90502-4

Page 30: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 58

Sanchez-Santed, F., de Bruin, J. P. C., Heinsbroek, R. P. W., & Verwer, R. W. H. (1997). Spatial delayed alternation of rat in a T-maze: Effects of neurotoxic lesions of the medi-al prefrontal cortex and of T-maze rotations. Behavioural Brain Research, 84, 73-79.

doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(97)83327-XSanti, A., & Weise, L. (1995). The effects of scopolamine

on memory for time in rats and pigeons. Pharmacology, Bochemistry, and Behavior, 51, 271-277.

doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(94)00376-TSchacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and cur-

rent status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn-ing, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501-518.

doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.501Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994). Memory sys-

tems 1994. Cambridge: MIT Press.Seamans, J. K., Floresco, S. B., & Phillips, A. G. (1995).

Functional differences between the prelimbic and anterior cingulate regions of the rat prefrontal cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 1063-1073.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.109.6.1063 PMid:8748957Shapiro, M. L., & Olton, D. S. (1994). Hippocampal func-

tion and interference. In: D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 141–146). Cambridge: MIT Press. PMid:8185958

Shaw, C., & Aggleton, J. P. (1993). The effects of fornix and medial prefrontal lesions on delayed non-matching-to-sample by rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 54, 91-102.

doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(93)90051-QSmall, S. A., Schobel, S. A., Buxton, R . B., Witter, M. P.,

& Barnes, C. A. (2011). A pathophysiological framework of hippocampal dysfunction in aging and disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 585-601.

doi.org/10.1038/nrn3085 PMid:21897434 PMCid:3312472Small, D. M., Zald, D. H., Jones-Gotman, M., Zatorre, R. J.,

Pardo, J. V., Frey, S., & Petrides, M. (1999). Human corti-cal gustatory areas: A review of functional neuroimaging data. Neuroreport, 10, 7-14.

doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199901180-00002 PMid:10094124Smith, M. L., & Milner, B. (1981). The role of the right hip-

pocampus in the recall of spatial location. Neuropsycholo-gia, 19, 781-793. doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(81)90090-7

Squire, L. R. (1994). Declarative and nondeclarative memo-ry: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memo-ry. In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 203-231). Cambridge: MIT Press.

Squire, L.R., Stark, C.E. & Clark R.E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annual review Neuroscience, 27, 279-306.

doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130 PMid:15217334Suzuki, W. A., Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., & Amaral, D.

G. (1993). Lesions of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in the monkey produce long-lasting memory im-pairment in the visual and tactual modalities. The Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 2430-2451. PMid:8501516

Stark, S. M., Yassa, M. A., & Stark, C. E. (2010). Individual differences in spatial pattrn separation performance asso-ciasted with healthy aging humans. Learning and Memo-ry, 17, 284-288. doi.org/10.1101/lm.1768110

PMid:20495062 PMCid:2884287St-Laurent, M., Petrides, M., & Sziklas, V. (2009). Does the

cingulate cortex contribute to spatial conditional associa-tive learning in the rat? Hippocampus, 19, 612-622.

St-Laurent, M., Petrides, M., & Sziklas, V. (2009). Does the cingulate cortex contribute to spatial conditional associa-tive learning in the rat? Hippocampus, 19, 612-622.

doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20539 PMid:19123251Tees, R. C. (1999). The effects of posterior parietal and pos-

terior temporal cortical lesions on multimodal spatial and nonspatial competences in rats. Behavioural Brain Re-search, 106, 55-73.

doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00092-3Tolentino, J. C., Pirogovsky, E., Luu, T., Toner, C. K., & Gil-

bert, P. E. (2012). The effect of interference on temporal order memory for random and fixed sequences in nonde-mented older adults. Learning and Memory, 19, 1-6.

doi.org/10.1101/lm.026062.112 PMid:22615480Toni, I., Ramnani, N., Josephs, O., Ashburner, J., & Passing-

ham, R. E. (2001). Learning arbitrary visuomotor associa-tions: Temporal dynamic of brain activity. NeuroImage, 14, 1048-1057. doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0894

PMid:11697936Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford:

Clarendon.Turner, G. R., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuro-

anatomy of classic delayed response tasks in humans and the limitations of cross-method convergence in prefrontal function. Neuroscience, 139, 327-337.

doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.067 PMid:16324791Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional brain imaging stud-

ies of cortical mechanisms of memory. Science, 270, 769-775. doi.org/10.1126/science.270.5237.769

PMid:7481764Vermersch, A.-I., Gaymard, B. M., Rivaud-Pechoux, S.,

Ploner, C. J., Agid, Y., Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1999). Memory guided saccade deficit after caudate nucleus le-sions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychia-try, 66, 524-527. doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.66.4.524

Wager, T. D., & Smith, E. E. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affec-tive, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 255-274.

doi.org/10.3758/CABN.3.4.255Watanabe, M. (1986). Prefrontal unit activity during delayed

Page 31: Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory€¦ · Neurobiological Foundations 29 Neurobiological Foundations of an Attribute Model of Memory Raymond Kesner University

Neurobiological Foundations 59

conditional Go/No-Go discrimination in the monkey. II. Relation to Go and No-Go responses. Brain Research, 10, 15-27. doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(86)90105-8

Weeden, C. S. S. , Hu, N. J., Ho, L. Y. N. & Kesner, R. P. (2012). The role of the ventral dentate gyrus in olfactory learning and memory. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts.

Weiskrantz, L. (1956). Behavioral changes with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. Journal of Compar-ative Physiological Psychology, 49, 381-391.

doi.org/10.1037/h0088009Weiskrantz, L., & Saunders, C. (1984). Impairments of visu-

al object transforms in monkeys. Brain, 107, 1033-1072. doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.4.1033 PMid:6509307Whishaw, I. A., Tomie, J., & Kolb, B. (1992). Ventrolateral

prefrontal cortex lesions in rats impair the acquisition and retention of a tactile-olfactory configural task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 597-603.

doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.106.4.597 PMid:1503655Wilson, F. A. W., Scalaidhe, S. P. O., & Goldman-Rakic, P.

S. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 260, 1955-1957. doi.org/10.1126/science.8316836

PMid:8316836Yassa, M. A., Lacy, J. W., Stark, S. M., Albert, M. S., Galla-

gher, M., & Stark, C. E. L. (2010). Pattern deficits associ-ated with increased hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus in nondemented older adults. Hippocampus, 21, 968-979. PMid:20865732 PMCid:3010452

Yassa, M. A., & Stark, C. E. L. (2011). Pattern separation in the hippocampus. Trends in Neurosciences, 34, 515-525.

doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.006 PMid:21788086 PMCid:3183227Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1-27. doi.org/10.1037/h0025848Zhu, X. O., Brown, M. W., & Aggleton, J. P. (1995). Neuro-

nal signaling of information important to visual recogni-tion memory in rat rhinal and neighbouring cortices. Eu-ropean Journal of Neurosciences, 7, 753-765.

doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb00679.x