participants thirty-five children completed both waves of data collection (18 female, 17 male) 1...

1
E licited Im itation Perform ance and C M S Scores 2 4 6 8 0 1 2 3 4 5 Individual ActionsR ecalled at20 m onths 9-Step Picture Sequencing Im m ediate R ecall of O rdered Pairsat6 years Participants Thirty-five children completed both waves of data collection (18 female, 17 male) 1 male was diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder between waves 1and 2. His data were not part of the current report. Mean age at Wave 1 = 20 months (SD = 27 days) Mean age at Wave 2 = 6 years, 7 months (SD = 2 months, 3 weeks) Results Relations Between Concurrent Tasks at 6 years: Performance on the Children’s Memory Scale was significantly correlated with our laboratory measure of memory (i.e., 9-step picture sequencing). However, performance on the Woodcock Johnson III speed of processing tasks were not related to performance on the 9-step picture sequencing task. Bolded correlations are illustrated in scatterplots below; + = p <. 10, * = p < .05. Results Wave 1: Performance on 4-Step Sequences: At 20 months of age, children’s performance on 3-exposure was related to performance on 1-exposure (ps <.05) Wave 2: Performance on 6-Year Tasks: All scores of children’s performance on the Children’s Memory Scale and Woodcock Johnson were within the normal range. Beyond the Gong: Relations Between Elicited Imitation Performance at 20-40 Months of Age and Memory at 6 Years Tracy DeBoer, Carol L. Cheatham, Emily Stark, & Patricia J. Bauer Institute of Child Development University of Minnesota Wave 2 @ 6 years: Follow-up Standardized Memory Assessment and Memory Battery Standardized memory assessment (the Children’s Memory Scale®; Cohen, 1997) Total Scaled Score Visual Memory: Memory for Dot Locations, Recognition Memory for Faces Verbal Memory: Story Recall, Memory for Word Pairs Attention/Concentration: Numbers (digit span forward and backwards), Sequences (speed & accuracy of familiar sequences: e.g., alphabet) Learning: Learning Dot Locations, Learning for Word Pairs Speed of processing (Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities III®; Woodcock et al., 2001) Visual Matching, Pair Cancellation, Rapid Picture Naming 9step picture sequencing task (Phill, 2001) Immediate, 10-minute Deferred Im itation ofIndividualTargetActions 0 1 2 3 4 3-E xposure 1-E xposure N um berofE xposuresBefore Im itation T argetActions B aseline Im m ediate Im itation Background Elicited/deferred imitation is a commonly used measure of preverbal infants’ explicit memory abilities. In this paradigm, infants observe a sequence of actions demonstrated by an experimenter and are subsequently given the opportunity to reproduce the modeled actions either immediately (elicited imitation), after a delay (deferred imitation), or both. This technique is accepted by many as a nonverbal analogue to explicit memory report (Bauer, 2002) Performance is at least partially dependent on regions of the medial temporal lobe (Carver & Bauer, 1999, 2001; McDonough et al., 1995). Additionally, this paradigm has been used to assess memory in infants at-risk for memory impairment (e.g., infants of diabetic mothers, DeBoer et al., in press, and infants born preterm, Cheatham, 2004). However, to date, relations between performance on the elicited imitation paradigm and memory performance later in life have not been tested. In this investigation we examined the long-term predictability of the elicited imitation paradigm from 20 months to 6 years of age. Discussion Results indicate that performance on the elicited imitation task during toddlerhood is related to some aspects of memory performance at 6 years of age. Specifically, both measures of standardized memory performance and measures of speed of processing correlated with imitation performance. 4-step elicited imitation performance was also correlated with 9-step picture sequencing imitation performance suggesting that these may be analogous measures of memory in older children. Moreover, concurrent measures of 9-step picture sequencing were related to performance on the Children’s Memory Scale. These findings add support to the argument that recall abilities are reliable and robust by 20 months of age (Bauer, 2002) and that elicited imitation is an appropriate task to index these abilities in preverbal children. Procedure Wave 1 @ 20 months: Immediate Imitation of 8 4-Step Sequences 4 sequences were demonstrated 3 times, 4 sequences were demonstrated 1 time (exposure sessions were 1 week apart) Each sequence consisted of 1)a baseline measure 2)demonstration (i.e., modeling) by the experimenter 3)immediate imitation Consistent with previous research, two measures were indicative of recall: 1) individual target actions and 2) pairs of target actions in the correct temporal order 9-Step Ordered Pairs Immediate 9-Step Ordered Pairs Delay CMS Score .39* Visual Immediate Score Visual Delay Score .31+ Verbal Immediate Score .35* Verbal Delay Score .34* Attention/Concentration Score .39* Learning Score .37* Pair Cancellation Visual Matching 9-Step Picture Sequening and C M S Scores 10 15 20 25 30 3 6 9 Im m ediate R ecall ofO rdered Pairsat6 years CM S Attention/C oncentration Score at6 years E licited Im itation Perform ance and C M S Scores 50 75 100 125 150 0.5 1.5 2.5 3.5 4.5 Individual Actionsof3-E xposure SequencesR ecalled at20 months CM S G eneral M em ory Score at 6 years E licited Im itation Perform ance and C M S Scores 10 20 30 40 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 Individual ActionsRecalled at20 m onths CM S Verbal Im m ediate M em oryScoreat6years E licited Im itation Perform ance and C M S Scores 10 15 20 25 30 0.5 1.5 2.5 3.5 4.5 Individual ActionsR ecalled at20 m onths CM S Attention/C oncentration Score at6 years E licited Im itation Perform ance and C M S Scores 15 25 35 45 55 0.5 1.5 2.5 3.5 4.5 Individual ActionsR ecalled at20 m onths W JIIIPairC ancellation Score at6 years E licited Im itation Perform ance and C M S Scores 15 20 25 30 35 0 0.25 0.5 0.75 1 1.25 1.5 1.75 2 2.25 2.5 2.75 3 PairsofActionsR ecalled at20 m onths(1-E xposure) W JIIIVisual M atching at6 years Task at 6 Years 20-Month Elicited Imitation Performance 3-Exposure Sequences 1-Exposure Sequences Individual Target Actions Pairs of Actions Individual Target Actions Pairs of Actions Childre n’s Memory Scale CMS - General Memory .29+ .38* .38* Visual Immediate Memory .32+ .28+ .43* .49** Verbal Immediate Memory .34* .32+ .38* .37* Attention/Concentration .43* .36* .40* .41* Learning .46** .49** Woodcoc k Johnson III Pair Cancellation .37* .30+ .42* .50** Visual Matching .34* .54** .60** 9-Step Picture Seq. 9-Step Immediate Ordered Pairs .41* .35* .32+ 9-Step Delayed Ordered Pairs References Bauer, P. J. (2002). Early memory development. In Goswami, U. (Ed). Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Blackwell handbooks of developmental psychology. (pp. 127-146). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. Carver, L. J., & Bauer, P. J. (1999). When the event is more than the sum of its parts: Nine- month-olds’ long term ordered recall. Memory, 7, 147-174. Carver, L. J., & Bauer, P. J. (2001). The dawning of a past: The emergence of long-term explicit memory in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 726-745. Cheatham, C.L. (2004). Recall deficits in infants born preterm: Sources of individual differences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities. Cohen, M. J. (1997). Children’s Memory Scale. San Antonio, TX : Psychological Corp. DeBoer, T., Wewerka, S., Bauer, P. J., Georgieff, M. K., & Nelson, C. A. (in press). Declarative memory performance in infants of diabetic mothers at 1 year of age. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. McDonough, L., Mandler, J. M., McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. R. (1995). The deferred imtiation task as a nonverbal measure of declarative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 7580-7584. Phill, C. (2001). What’s in a picture? Enabling relations, event recall, and stimulus type. Unpublished manuscript. University of Minnesota. Woodcock R. W., McGrew, K. S., Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III®). Riverside Publishing. Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank the Members of the Cognition in Transition Laboratory for their continued support of this project and the families who donated their time for Note: The one data point illustrating performance of 1 individual target action on the immediate imitation task was not 2 SD below the mean. Significant results remained the same even when this data point was removed. Im itation ofPairsofActionsin the C orrectO rder 0 1 2 3 3-E xposure 1-E xposure N um berofE xposuresBefore Im itation Pairsof Actions Baseline Im m ediate Im itation 1 3 2 4 7 5 9 6 8 Results Relations Between Imitation Performance at Wave 1 and Performance on Tasks at Wave 2: Bolded correlations are illustrated in scatterplots below; + = p <. 10, * = p < .05, ** = p < .01 9-Step Picture Sequencing and C M S Scores 50 75 100 125 150 3 6 9 D elayed R ecallofO rdered Pairsat6 years CM S G eneralM em ory Score at6 years

Post on 20-Jan-2016

217 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Participants  Thirty-five children completed both waves of data collection (18 female, 17 male)  1 male was diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder between

Elicited Imitation Performance and CMS Scores

2

4

6

8

0 1 2 3 4 5

Individual Actions Recalled at 20 months

9-St

ep P

ictu

re S

eque

ncin

g Im

med

iate

Rec

all o

f O

rder

ed P

airs

at 6

yea

rs

Participants

Thirty-five children completed both waves of data collection (18 female, 17 male) 1 male was diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder between waves 1and 2. His data were not part of the current report.

Mean age at Wave 1 = 20 months (SD = 27 days) Mean age at Wave 2 = 6 years, 7 months (SD = 2 months, 3 weeks)

ResultsRelations Between Concurrent Tasks at 6 years:Performance on the Children’s Memory Scale was significantly correlated with our laboratory measure of memory (i.e., 9-step picture sequencing). However, performance on the Woodcock Johnson III speed of processing tasks were not related to performance on the 9-step picture sequencing task. Bolded correlations are illustrated in scatterplots below; + = p <. 10, * = p < .05.

Results

Wave 1: Performance on 4-Step Sequences:

At 20 months of age, children’s performance on 3-exposure was related to performance on 1-exposure (ps <.05)

Wave 2: Performance on 6-Year Tasks: All scores of children’s performance on the Children’s Memory Scale and Woodcock Johnson were within the normal range.

Beyond the Gong: Relations Between Elicited Imitation Performance at 20-40 Months of Age and Memory at 6 YearsTracy DeBoer, Carol L. Cheatham, Emily Stark, & Patricia J. Bauer

Institute of Child Development University of Minnesota

Wave 2 @ 6 years: Follow-up Standardized Memory Assessment and Memory Battery Standardized memory assessment (the Children’s Memory Scale®; Cohen, 1997)Total Scaled ScoreVisual Memory: Memory for Dot Locations, Recognition Memory for FacesVerbal Memory: Story Recall, Memory for Word PairsAttention/Concentration: Numbers (digit span forward and backwards), Sequences (speed & accuracy of familiar sequences: e.g., alphabet)Learning: Learning Dot Locations, Learning for Word Pairs

Speed of processing (Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities III®; Woodcock et al., 2001)Visual Matching, Pair Cancellation, Rapid Picture Naming

9step picture sequencing task (Phill, 2001)Immediate, 10-minute Deferred

Imitation of Individual Target Actions

01234

3-Exposure 1-Exposure

Number of Exposures Before Imitation

Tar

get A

ctio

ns

Baseline Immediate Imitation

Background

Elicited/deferred imitation is a commonly used measure of preverbal infants’ explicit memory abilities.

In this paradigm, infants observe a sequence of actions demonstrated by an experimenter and are subsequently given the opportunity to reproduce the modeled actions either immediately (elicited imitation), after a delay (deferred imitation), or both.

This technique is accepted by many as a nonverbal analogue to explicit memory report (Bauer, 2002) Performance is at least partially dependent on regions of the medial temporal lobe (Carver & Bauer, 1999, 2001; McDonough et al., 1995).

Additionally, this paradigm has been used to assess memory in infants at-risk for memory impairment (e.g., infants of diabetic mothers, DeBoer et al., in press, and infants born preterm, Cheatham, 2004).

However, to date, relations between performance on the elicited imitation paradigm and memory performance later in life have not been tested.

In this investigation we examined the long-term predictability of the elicited imitation paradigm from 20 months to 6 years of age.

Discussion Results indicate that performance on the elicited imitation task during toddlerhood is related to some aspects of memory performance at 6 years of age.

Specifically, both measures of standardized memory performance and measures of speed of processing correlated with imitation performance.

4-step elicited imitation performance was also correlated with 9-step picture sequencing imitation performance suggesting that these may be analogous measures of memory in older children.

Moreover, concurrent measures of 9-step picture sequencing were related to performance on the Children’s Memory Scale.

These findings add support to the argument that recall abilities are reliable and robust by 20 months of age (Bauer, 2002) and that elicited imitation is an appropriate task to index these abilities in preverbal children.

Procedure

Wave 1 @ 20 months: Immediate Imitation of 8 4-Step Sequences

4 sequences were demonstrated 3 times, 4 sequences were demonstrated 1 time (exposure sessions were 1 week apart)

Each sequence consisted of1) a baseline measure 2) demonstration (i.e., modeling) by the experimenter3) immediate imitation

Consistent with previous research, two measures were indicative of recall: 1) individual target actions and 2) pairs of target actions in the correct temporal order

  9-Step Ordered

Pairs Immediate

9-Step Ordered Pairs

Delay

CMS Score   .39*

Visual Immediate Score    

Visual Delay Score   .31+

Verbal Immediate Score   .35*

Verbal Delay Score   .34*

Attention/Concentration Score .39*  

Learning Score   .37*

Pair Cancellation

Visual Matching9-Step Picture Sequening and CMS Scores

10

15

20

25

30

3 6 9

Immediate Recall of Ordered Pairs at 6 years

CM

S Atten

tion

/Con

cent

ration

Sc

ore

at 6

yea

rs

Elicited Imitation Performance and CMS Scores

50

75

100

125

150

0.5 1.5 2.5 3.5 4.5

Individual Actions of 3-Exposure Sequences Recalled at 20 months

CM

S G

ener

al M

emor

y Sc

ore

at

6 ye

ars

Elicited Imitation Performance and CMS Scores

10

20

30

40

0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5

Individual Actions Recalled at 20 months

CM

S Ver

bal I

mm

edia

te

Mem

ory

Scor

e at

6 y

ears

Elicited Imitation Performance and CMS Scores

10

15

20

25

30

0.5 1.5 2.5 3.5 4.5

Individual Actions Recalled at 20 months

CM

S Atten

tion

/Con

cent

ration

Sc

ore

at 6

yea

rs

Elicited Imitation Performance and CMS Scores

15

25

35

45

55

0.5 1.5 2.5 3.5 4.5

Individual Actions Recalled at 20 months

WJ II

I Pai

r Can

cella

tion

Sco

re

at 6

yea

rs

Elicited Imitation Performance and CMS Scores

15

20

25

30

35

0 0.25 0.5 0.75 1 1.25 1.5 1.75 2 2.25 2.5 2.75 3

Pairs of Actions Recalled at 20 months (1-Exposure)

WJ II

I Visua

l Mat

chin

g at

6

year

s

Task at 6 Years

20-Month Elicited Imitation Performance

3-Exposure Sequences 1-Exposure Sequences

Individual Target Actions

Pairs of Actions

Individual Target Actions

Pairs of Actions

Children’s

Memory Scale

CMS - General Memory .29+   .38* .38*

Visual Immediate Memory .32+ .28+ .43* .49**

Verbal Immediate Memory .34* .32+ .38* .37*

Attention/Concentration .43* .36* .40* .41*

Learning     .46** .49**

Woodcock

Johnson III

Pair Cancellation .37* .30+ .42* .50**

Visual Matching .34*  .54** .60**

9-Step Picture

Seq.

9-Step Immediate Ordered Pairs .41* .35*   .32+

9-Step Delayed Ordered Pairs         References Bauer, P. J. (2002). Early memory development. In Goswami, U. (Ed). Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Blackwell handbooks of developmental psychology. (pp. 127-146). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. Carver, L. J., & Bauer, P. J. (1999). When the event is more than the sum of its parts: Nine-month-olds’ long term ordered recall. Memory, 7, 147-174. Carver, L. J., & Bauer, P. J. (2001). The dawning of a past: The emergence of long-term explicit memory in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 726-745. Cheatham, C.L. (2004). Recall deficits in infants born preterm: Sources of individual differences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities. Cohen, M. J. (1997). Children’s Memory Scale. San Antonio, TX : Psychological Corp. DeBoer, T., Wewerka, S., Bauer, P. J., Georgieff, M. K., & Nelson, C. A. (in press). Declarative memory performance in infants of diabetic mothers at 1 year of age. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. McDonough, L., Mandler, J. M., McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. R. (1995). The deferred imtiation task as a nonverbal measure of declarative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 7580-7584. Phill, C. (2001). What’s in a picture? Enabling relations, event recall, and stimulus type. Unpublished manuscript. University of Minnesota. Woodcock R. W., McGrew, K. S., Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III®). Riverside Publishing.

Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank the Members of the Cognition in Transition Laboratory for their

continued support of this project and the families who donated their time for this research endeavor. This research was supported by a grant from the NICHD to Patricia J. Bauer (HD28425), and Small

grants from both the Institute of Child Development and Center for Neurobehavioral Development at the University of Minnesota to Tracy DeBoer and Carol Cheatham.

Please send comments or questions to the first author at [email protected]

Note: The one data point illustrating performance of 1 individual target action on the immediate imitation task was not 2 SD below the mean. Significant results remained the same even when this data point was

removed.

Imitation of Pairs of Actions in the Correct Order

0123

3-Exposure 1-Exposure

Number of Exposures Before Imitation

Pairs

of A

ctio

ns

Baseline Immediate Imitation

1 32 4 75 96 8

ResultsRelations Between Imitation Performance at Wave 1 and Performance on Tasks at Wave 2: Bolded correlations are illustrated in scatterplots below; + = p <. 10, * = p < .05, ** = p < .01

9-Step Picture Sequencing and CMS Scores

50

75

100

125

150

3 6 9

Delayed Recall of Ordered Pairs at 6 years

CM

S G

ener

al M

emor

y Sc

ore

at 6

yea

rs