processing of proper names in mandarin chinese

1
Processing of proper names in Mandarin Chinese 1, 2 Huei-Ling Yen ([email protected]) 1 Horst M. Müller ([email protected]) 1 Experimental Neurolinguistics Group, Faculty of Linguistics University of Bielefeld, P.O. Box 100 131, 33501 Bielefeld, GERMANY 2 Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica, Nankang, Taipei 115, TAIWAN Introduction The distinction between concrete and abstract nouns has been postulated since several thousand years in ancient Greek and in Chinese language philosophy, (e.g., Kripke, 1972; Jiang, 1993). Furthermore, within the category of concrete nouns the proper names are considered as a distinct subgroup. Such linguistic categories may reflect cognitive structures since they are common in all languages - or they may be completely artificial (Müller & Kutas, 1996). The theoretical assumptions that proper names have a cognitive reality are supported by recent case studies (e.g., Fukatsu et al., 1999) and experimental studies in English and German (Schuth, Werner & Müller, 2002; Weiss & Müller, 2003). To investigate processing of proper names and common nouns in a non Indo-European language, we conducted an experiment with Mandarin Chinese (lexical decision task). Subjects and Methods Participants were 40 Mandarin Chinese native speakers (20 f, 20 m), between 21 and 35 years (27.6 y), right-handed according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory and with normal hearing threshold according to their own statements. Auditory condition: All target words were spoken by a young woman and edited with a computer. 40 single common nouns (CN) and 100 proper names (40 persons’ first names (FN), 40 geographic names (GN) and 20 brand names (BN)) were presented by loud speakers. The mean acoustic length of the stimuli was 441 ms (± 94 ms). Visual condition: Different words were used (40 CN, 40 FN, 40 GN, 20 BN) and displayed on a computer screen in standard Chinese logographic characters. While the subjects had to make a lexical decision whether the target word was a proper name or not by pressing two buttons (CN vs. PN), the reaction time was measured. Results In both the auditory and the visual condition, a significant difference in lexical decision time for PNs and CNs could be observed. t-Tests showed that subjects responded signifi- cantly faster to PNs than CNs (t = -5.127, p ≤ 0.001, see figure). A similar significant difference was found between the decision times of the auditory condition: PNs 959 ms, CNs 1058 ms, GNs 942 ms and BNs 1071 ms (t -3.361, p 0.001). No significant differences were found between the sexes when processing male or female first names. Conclusions Mandarin Chi- nese speakers are able to process proper names fa- ster than com- mon nouns in a lexical decision task. This is true for persons’ first names and geo- graphic names, but not for brand names. This finding is modality independ- ent. In terms of reaction time, brand names are more similar to common nouns. Despite of major differences between Chinese and Indo-European languages (e.g., tone language and logographic system), the same dissociation between proper names and common nouns has been found as in German and English. This can be seen as an additional hint for the existence of cognitive universals. Acknowledgments We thank Dr. Chu-Ren Huang, Dr. Kathleen Ahrens and Dr. Lorenz Sichelschmidt for support and discussions. Sup- ported by the German Science Foundation (SFB 360). References Fukatsu, R., Fujii, T. Tsukiura, T., Yamadori, A. & Otsuki, T. (1999). Proper name anomia after left temporal lobec- tomy: A patient study. Neurology, 52, 1096-1099. Jiang, G. (1993). The History of Chinese Thoughts (in Chi- nese). Taipei: Wen-Jin. Kripke, S. (1972). Naming and Necessity. Oxford: Black- well. Müller, H.M. & Kutas, M. (1996). What’s a in a name? Electrophysiological differences between spoken nouns, proper names and one’s own name. NeuroReport, 8, 221- 225. Schuth, A., Werner, R. & Müller, H.M. (2002). Die Sonder- stellung der Eigennamen: Reaktionszeit-, Gating- und EDA-Befunde. In M. Baumann, A. Keinath & J.F. Krems (Eds.),. Experimentelle Psychologie. Abstracts der 44. Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (pp. 205). Chemnitz: Roderer Verlag. Weiss, S. & Müller, H.M. (2003). The contribution of EEG coherence to the investigation of language. Brain & Lan- guage, 85, 325-343. 1418

Upload: others

Post on 03-Feb-2022

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Processing of proper names in Mandarin Chinese1, 2 Huei-Ling Yen ([email protected])

1 Horst M. Müller ([email protected])

1 Experimental Neurolinguistics Group, Faculty of LinguisticsUniversity of Bielefeld, P.O. Box 100 131, 33501 Bielefeld, GERMANY

2 Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica, Nankang, Taipei 115, TAIWAN

IntroductionThe distinction between concrete and abstract nouns hasbeen postulated since several thousand years in ancientGreek and in Chinese language philosophy, (e.g., Kripke,1972; Jiang, 1993). Furthermore, within the category ofconcrete nouns the proper names are considered as a distinctsubgroup. Such linguistic categories may reflect cognitivestructures since they are common in all languages - or theymay be completely artificial (Müller & Kutas, 1996). Thetheoretical assumptions that proper names have a cognitivereality are supported by recent case studies (e.g., Fukatsu etal., 1999) and experimental studies in English and German(Schuth, Werner & Müller, 2002; Weiss & Müller, 2003).

To investigate processing of proper names and commonnouns in a non Indo-European language, we conducted anexperiment with Mandarin Chinese (lexical decision task).

Subjects and MethodsParticipants were 40 Mandarin Chinese native speakers (20f, 20 m), between 21 and 35 years (27.6 y), right-handedaccording to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory and withnormal hearing threshold according to their own statements.

Auditory condition: All target words were spoken by ayoung woman and edited with a computer. 40 singlecommon nouns (CN) and 100 proper names (40 persons’first names (FN), 40 geographic names (GN) and 20 brandnames (BN)) were presented by loud speakers. The meanacoustic length of the stimuli was 441 ms (± 94 ms).

Visual condition: Different words were used (40 CN, 40FN, 40 GN, 20 BN) and displayed on a computer screen instandard Chinese logographic characters.

While the subjects had to make a lexical decision whetherthe target word was a proper name or not by pressing twobuttons (CN vs. PN), the reaction time was measured.

ResultsIn both the auditory and the visual condition, a significantdifference in lexical decision time for PNs and CNs couldbe observed. t-Tests showed that subjects responded signifi-cantly faster to PNs than CNs (t = -5.127, p ≤ 0.001, seefigure). A similar significant difference was found betweenthe decision times of the auditory condition: PNs 959 ms,CNs 1058 ms, GNs 942 ms and BNs 1071 ms (t -3.361, p ≤0.001). No significant differences were found between thesexes when processing male or female first names.

ConclusionsMandarin Chi-nese speakers areable to processproper names fa-ster than com-mon nouns in alexical decisiontask. This is truefor persons’ firstnames and geo-graphic names,but not for brand names. This finding is modality independ-ent. In terms of reaction time, brand names are more similarto common nouns. Despite of major differences betweenChinese and Indo-European languages (e.g., tone languageand logographic system), the same dissociation betweenproper names and common nouns has been found as inGerman and English. This can be seen as an additional hintfor the existence of cognitive universals.

AcknowledgmentsWe thank Dr. Chu-Ren Huang, Dr. Kathleen Ahrens and Dr.Lorenz Sichelschmidt for support and discussions. Sup-ported by the German Science Foundation (SFB 360).

ReferencesFukatsu, R., Fujii, T. Tsukiura, T., Yamadori, A. & Otsuki,

T. (1999). Proper name anomia after left temporal lobec-tomy: A patient study. Neurology, 52, 1096-1099.

Jiang, G. (1993). The History of Chinese Thoughts (in Chi-nese). Taipei: Wen-Jin.

Kripke, S. (1972). Naming and Necessity. Oxford: Black-well.

Müller, H.M. & Kutas, M. (1996). What’s a in a name?Electrophysiological differences between spoken nouns,proper names and one’s own name. NeuroReport, 8, 221-225.

Schuth, A., Werner, R. & Müller, H.M. (2002). Die Sonder-stellung der Eigennamen: Reaktionszeit-, Gating- undEDA-Befunde. In M. Baumann, A. Keinath & J.F. Krems(Eds.),. Experimentelle Psychologie. Abstracts der 44.Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (pp. 205).Chemnitz: Roderer Verlag.

Weiss, S. & Müller, H.M. (2003). The contribution of EEGcoherence to the investigation of language. Brain & Lan-guage, 85, 325-343.

1418