proquestdocuments-2014-12-17 (5)

34
_______________________________________________________________ _______________________________________________________________ Report Information from ProQuest December 17 2014 15:41 _______________________________________________________________ 17 December 2014 ProQuest

Upload: onomatopeeportocalie

Post on 25-Sep-2015

222 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

DESCRIPTION

res

TRANSCRIPT

  • _______________________________________________________________

    _______________________________________________________________ Report Information from ProQuestDecember 17 2014 15:41_______________________________________________________________

  • Document 1 of 1 Autism Author: Lai, Meng-Chuan; Lombardo, Michael V; Baron-Cohen, Simon ProQuest document link Abstract: Autism is a set of heterogeneous neurodevelopmental conditions, characterised by early-onsetdifficulties in social communication and unusually restricted, repetitive behaviour and interests. The worldwidepopulation prevalence is about 1%. Autism affects more male than female individuals, and comorbidity iscommon (>70% have concurrent conditions). Individuals with autism have atypical cognitive profiles, such asimpaired social cognition and social perception, executive dysfunction, and atypical perceptual and informationprocessing. These profiles are underpinned by atypical neural development at the systems level. Genetics hasa key role in the aetiology of autism, in conjunction with developmentally early environmental factors. Large-effect rare mutations and small-effect common variants contribute to risk. Assessment needs to bemultidisciplinary and developmental, and early detection is essential for early intervention. Early comprehensiveand targeted behavioural interventions can improve social communication and reduce anxiety and aggression.Drugs can reduce comorbid symptoms, but do not directly improve social communication. Creation of asupportive environment that accepts and respects that the individual is different is crucial. Full text: Definition In 1943, child psychiatrist Leo Kanner described eight boys and three girls,1 including 5-year-old Donald whowas "happiest when left alone, almost never cried to go with his mother, did not seem to notice his father'shome-comings, and was indifferent to visiting relatives...wandered about smiling, making stereotypedmovements with his fingers...spun with great pleasure anything he could seize upon to spin.... Words to him hada specifically literal, inflexible meaning.... When taken into a room, he completely disregarded the people andinstantly went for objects". In 1944, paediatrician Hans Asperger described four boys, 2 including 6-year-old Fritzwho "learnt to talk very early...quickly learnt to express himself in sentences and soon talked 'like anadult'...never able to become integrated into a group of playing children...did not know the meaning of respectand was utterly indifferent to the authority of adults...lacked distance and talked without shyness even tostrangers...it was impossible to teach him the polite form of address.... Another strange phenomenon...was theoccurrence of certain stereotypic movements and habits". These seminal reports1,2 vividly portray what is now called autism or the autism spectrum. The spectrum is wide,encompassing classic Kanner's syndrome (originally entitled autistic disturbances of affective contact) andAsperger's syndrome (originally called autistic psychopathy in childhood). Understanding of autism has evolvedsubstantially in the past 70 years, with an exponential growth in research since the mid-1990s ( figure ). Autism isnow thought of as a set of neurodevelopmental conditions, some of which can be attributed to distinctaetiological factors, such as Mendelian single-gene mutations. However, most are probably the result ofcomplex interactions between genetic and non-genetic risk factors. The many types are collectively defined byspecific behaviours, centring on atypical development in social communication and unusually restricted orrepetitive behaviour and interests. The mid-20th century view of autism as a form of childhood psychosis is no longer held. The first operationaldefinition appeared in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III),and was strongly influenced by Michael Rutter's conceptualisation of impaired social development andcommunicative development, insistence on sameness, and onset before 30 months of age. 3 The subsequentrevisions in the fourth edition (DSM-IV) and the 10th revision of the International Classification of Diseases(ICD-10), in which autism was referred to as pervasive developmental disorder, emphasised the early onset of atriad of features: impairments in social interaction; impairments in communication; and restricted, repetitive, and

    http://search.proquest.com/docview/1508931440?accountid=15533
  • stereotyped behaviour, interests, and activities. The latest revision of DSM--DSM-5, published in May, 20134 --adopted the umbrella term autism spectrumdisorder without a definition of subtypes, and reorganised the triad into a dyad: difficulties in socialcommunication and social interaction; and restricted and repetitive behaviour, interests, or activities (table 1 ).Atypical language development (historically linked to an autism diagnosis) was removed from the criteria, and isnow classified as a co-occurring condition, even though large variation in language is characteristic of autism.5

    The new criteria give improved descriptions and organisation of key features, emphasise the dimensional natureof autism, provide one diagnostic label with individualised specifiers, and allow for an assessment of theindividual's need for support (helping provision of clinical services). 6 How prevalence estimates will be affectedby the new criteria and how autism spectrum disorder will relate to the newly created social (pragmatic)communication disorder (defined by substantial difficulties with social uses of both verbal and non-verbalcommunication, but otherwise not meeting criteria for autism spectrum disorder) remain to be assessed. Autism could potentially be subgrouped at clinical (eg, by developmental pattern or trajectory and comorbidity),cognitive, and aetiological levels (eg, by genetic and environmental correlates).6 Although the term autismspectrum disorder is frequently used, the term autism spectrum condition also signals a biomedical diagnosisfor which individuals need support and recognises areas in which affected individuals are different from thosewithout autism, but without the negative overtones of the disorder label. Epidemiology Prevalence The prevalence of autism has been steadily increasing since the first epidemiological study,7 which showed that41 of every 10 000 individuals in the UK had autism. The increase is probably partly a result of changes indiagnostic concepts and criteria.8 However, the prevalence has continued to rise in the past two decades,particularly in individuals without intellectual disability, despite consistent use of DSM-IV criteria.9 An increase inrisk factors cannot be ruled out. However, the rise is probably also due to improved awareness and recognition,changes in diagnosis, and younger age of diagnosis.10,11 Nowadays, the median worldwide prevalence of autism is 062-070%,10,11 although estimates of 1-2% havebeen made in the latest large-scale surveys.12-19 A similar prevalence has been reported for adults alone.20

    About 45% of individuals with autism have intellectual disability,11 and 32% have regression (ie, loss ofpreviously acquired skills; mean age of onset 178 years).21 Early studies showed that autism affects 4-5 times more males than females, although the difference decreasedin individuals with intellectual disability.11 However, large-scale population-based studies12,13,16,19 have shownthat 2-3 times more males are affected, probably irrespective of intellectual disability. Females with autism mighthave been under-recognised.22 Empirical data suggest high-functioning females are diagnosed later than malesare,23,24 and indicate a diagnostic bias towards males.25 Females need more concurrent behavioural or cognitiveproblems than males do to be clinically diagnosed.26 This diagnostic bias might be a result of behaviouralcriteria for autism or gender stereotypes, and might reflect better compensation or so-called camouflage infemales.6,26-29 Nevertheless, a male predominance is a consistent epidemiological finding that has aetiological implications. Itcould imply female-specific protective effects, such that females would have to have a greater aetiological(genetic or environmental) load than would males to reach the diagnostic threshold. These protective effectswould mean that relatives of female probands would have an increased risk of autism or more autisticcharacteristics than would relatives of male probands. 30 Alternatively, male-specific risks could heightensusceptibility.22,31 The existence of sex-linked aetiological load and susceptibility emphasises the importance ofstratification by sex, and of comparisons between males and females to disentangle the aetiological role of sex-linked factors at genetic, endocrine, epigenetic, and environmental levels. Risk and protective factors

  • Epidemiological studies have identified various risk factors,32 but none has proven to be necessary or sufficientalone for autism to develop. Understanding of gene-environment interplay in autism is still at an early stage.33

    Advanced paternal or maternal reproductive age, or both, is a consistent risk;34-36 the underlying biology isunclear, but could be related to germline mutation, particularly when paternal in origin.37-41 Alternatively,individuals who have children late in life might do so because they have the broader autism phenotype--ie, mildtraits characteristic of autism--which is known to be associated with having a child with autism,42 although thisidea needs further research. Additionally, prevalence of autism has been reported to be two times higher incities where many jobs are in the information-technology sector than elsewhere; parents of children with autismmight be more likely to be technically talented than are other parents. 43 Gestational factors that could affect neurodevelopment, such as complications during pregnancy44,45 andexposure to chemicals,32,46-49 have been suggested to increase risk of autism. A broad, non-specific class ofconditions reflecting general compromises to perinatal and neonatal health is also associated with increasedrisk.50 Conversely, folic acid supplements before conception and during early pregnancy seem to be protective.51 There is no evidence that the MMR (measles, mumps, and rubella) vaccine,52 thiomersal-containing vaccines,53 or repeated vaccination54 cause autism. Co-occurring conditions More than 70% of individuals with autism have concurrent medical, developmental, or psychiatric conditions (table 2 )55-59 --a higher proportion than that for psychiatric outpatients60 and patients in tertiary hospitals.61

    Childhood co-occurring conditions tend to persist into adolescence.62 Some co-occurring conditions, such asepilepsy and depression (table 2 ), can first develop in adolescence or adulthood. Generally, the more co-occurring conditions, the greater the individual's disability.58 The high frequency of comorbidity could be a resultof shared pathophysiology, secondary effects of growing up with autism, shared symptom domains andassociated mechanisms, or overlapping diagnostic criteria. Prognosis and outcome A meta-analysis63 showed that individuals with autism have a mortality risk that is 28 times higher (95% CI 18-42) than that of unaffected people of the same age and sex. This difference is mostly related to co-occurringmedical conditions.64 Studies done before the widespread application of early intervention programmes65-67

    showed that 58-78% of adults with autism have poor or very poor outcomes in terms of independent living,educational attainment, employment, and peer relationships. Higher childhood intelligence, communicativephrase speech before age 6 years, and fewer childhood social impairments predict a better outcome. 65-67 Yet,even for individuals without intellectual disability, adult social outcome is often unsatisfactory in terms of qualityof life and achievement of occupational potential,67 although it is associated with cognitive gain and improvedadaptive functioning during development.68 Childhood follow-up studies have shown varying developmentaltrajectories in children with autism69,70 and in their siblings.71 The best possible outcome--ie, reversal ofdiagnosis, negligible autistic symptoms, and normal social communication--has also been reported.72 Transition to adulthood, which often involves loss of school support and child and adolescent mental healthservices, is a challenge. The end of secondary education is often accompanied by slowed improvement,probably due to reduced occupational stimulation 73 and insufficient adult services.74 More than half of youngpeople in the USA who have left secondary education in the past 2 years are not participating in any paid workor education.75 The mean proportion of adults with autism in employment (regular, supported, or sheltered) orfull-time education is 46%.76 Furthermore, little is known about how ageing affects people with autism.76,77 Early signs and screening Early identification allows early intervention. Previously, children with autism were often identified when olderthan 3-4 years, but toddlers are now frequently diagnosed because atypical development is recognised early.Early indicators are deficits or delays in the emergence of joint attention (ie, shared focus on an object) andpretend play, atypical implicit perspective taking, deficits in reciprocal affective behaviour, decreased response

  • to own name, decreased imitation, delayed verbal and non-verbal communication, motor delay, unusuallyrepetitive behaviours, atypical visuomotor exploration, inflexibility in disengaging visual attention, and extremevariation in temperament. 78,79 These indicators contribute to screening and diagnostic instruments for toddlers.79

    However, identification of high-functioning individuals is still often later than it should be,80 particularly forfemales.23,24 Variability in age, cognitive ability, and sex leads to differential presentation and the need for appropriatescreening instruments (table 3 ). Care should be taken during selection of screening instruments (and the cutoff forfurther action), because the target sample and purpose of screening vary.81 Routine early screening at ages 18and 24 months has been recommended.82 The advantages and disadvantages of action after a positive resultshould be carefully considered,83 as should the identification and management of individuals who have false-positive results. Studies of siblings of probands from an early age could potentially identify early behavioural and neuralpredictors of emerging autism.78 Signs of autism are not reliably present at birth, but emerge through a processof diminishing, delayed, or atypical development of social-communication behaviours, starting between the agesof 6 and 12 months.84 Examples of potential predictors of a subsequent autism diagnosis are poor attention tosocial scenes or human faces at age 6 months,85 little infant-parent interaction (reduced dyadic mutuality,including shared attention, infant acceptance of parental involvement, playing together, interactive flow, andshared body orientation; infant positive affect; and attentiveness to parent) at age 12 months, 86 and reducedflexibility in control of visual attention or orientation (disengagement) at ages 7 months87 and 14 months.88 Brainresponse when infants view faces with dynamic eye gaze at age 6-10 months (measured by event-relatedpotential) predicts an autism diagnosis at 36 months.89 Developmental trajectory of white-matter-tractorganisation from age 6 to 24 months predicts diagnosis at 24 months.90 Even some high-risk siblings who donot qualify for an autism diagnosis by age 3 years still have residual signs of delayed development and moreautistic signs than do low-risk siblings, suggesting that developmental surveillance and early intervention is alsoimportant for these individuals. 91 Clinical assessment Diagnostic assessment should be multidisciplinary and use a developmental framework of an interview with theparent or caregiver, interaction with the individual, collection of information about behaviour in communitysettings (eg, school reports and job performance), cognitive assessments, and a medical examination. 92 Co-occurring conditions should be carefully screened. The interview of the parent or caregiver should cover the gestational, birth, developmental, and health history,and family medical and psychiatric history. It should have specific foci: the development of social, emotional,language and communication, cognitive, motor, and self-help skills; the sensory profile; and unusual behavioursand interests. Behavioural presentation across different contexts should be investigated. Ideally, a standardised,structured interview should be incorporated into the assessment process ( table 3 ). Adaptive skills should bechecked with standardised instruments (eg, Vineland adaptive behaviour scales). In children, parent-childinteraction and parent coping strategies should be specifically investigated, because they are relevant for theplanning of interventions. Interviews with the individual should be interactive and engaging to enable assessment of social-communicationcharacteristics in both structured and unstructured contexts. Again, information should ideally be gathered withstandardised instruments (table 3 ). For adolescents and adults capable of reporting their inner state, self-reportquestionnaires are helpful (table 3 ), but their validity should be weighed against the individual's level of insight.How individuals cope in a peer environment should also be assessed. School reports and job performance records are valuable data indicating an individual's strengths and difficultiesin real-life settings. They also help with individualisation of educational and occupational planning. Cognitiveassessments of intelligence and language are essential; standardised, age-appropriate, and development-

  • appropriate instruments should be used to measure both verbal and non-verbal ability. 92 Neuropsychologicalassessments are helpful for individualised diagnosis and service planning. A medical examination is important in view of the high frequency of comorbidity. Physical and neurologicalexaminations (eg, head circumference, minor physical anomalies and skin lesions, and motor function)93 andgenetic analyses (eg, G-banded karyotype analysis, FMR1 testing, and particularly chromosomal microarrayanalysis)94,95 should be done. Other laboratory tests--eg, electroencephalography when awake and asleep ifseizures are suspected, neuroimaging when intracranial lesions are suspected, and metabolic profiling whenneurometabolic disorders are suspected--can be done as necessary. Cognition and neuroscience In the mid-20th century, autism was thought to originate from the emotional coldness of the child's mother, eventhough this hypothesis had no empirical support. By contrast, concurrent neurobiological hypotheses96 andKanner's proposal of an "innate inability to form the usual, biologically provided affective contact with people"1

    have received scientific support. Cognition and neurobiology are related, and their development is characterisedby a complex interplay between innate and environmental factors. Cognition provides a guide to simplify thevarious behavioural manifestations of autism, and can help investigation of underpinning neurobiology. 97

    Cognitive perspectives of autism can be grouped according to domains of concern (table 4 ), although they are bynature interlinked. Since impaired theory of mind was specifically reported in children with autism in 1985,98 difficulties withmentalising--ie, understanding of mental states in both self and others--are believed to be core to social-communication deficits (table 4 ). Studies99,100 have confirmed that development is atypical not only for thebehavioural expressions of mentalising, but also for their developmental precursors in triadic social interaction(eg, joint attention and pretend play) and dyadic social perception (eg, eye contact, emotion perception, action-perception mirroring, social orienting, biological motion processing, and face processing). Although many (high-functioning) individuals with autism achieve some degree of explicit or controlledmentalising,101 the implicit, automatic, and intuitive components are still impaired, even in adulthood.102 Early-onset mentalising difficulties seem to be specific to autism, but late-onset deficits are reported in disorders suchas schizophrenia.103 Mentalising is closely entwined with executive control and language,104 so that thedichotomous view of social versus non-social cognition is potentially misleading in autism. Historically, the domain of mentalising has been largely centred on others, but self-referential cognition and itsneural substrates are also atypical in autism.105,106 Therefore, deficits in the social domain are not only aboutdifficulties in the processing of information about other people, but also about processing of self-referentialinformation, the relationship that self has in a social context, and the potential for using self as a proxy tounderstand the social world. A consistent network of brain regions--including the medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus,temporoparietal junction, amygdala, and fusiform gyrus--are hypoactive in autism across tasks in which socialperception and cognition are used.100,107,108 Dysfunction in the so-called mirror system (ie, brain regions that areactive both when an individual performs an action and observes another person performing the same action)has been inconsistently implicated in imitation or observation of action or emotion in autism. 109 However, brainstructures do not act separately. Although studies of autism showing atypical development of the so-calledsocial brain are promising,100 equal attention should be paid to how these brain structures interact with the restof the neural system. Executive dysfunction could underlie both the unusually repetitive stereotyped behaviours and social-communication deficits in autism (table 4 ). However, the consistency of reports has been challenged,110 andimpaired performance could be underpinned by difficulties with mentalising.111 Imaging studies have shown thatfrontal, parietal, and striatal circuitry are the main systems implicated in executive dysfunction in autism.107,108

    Executive dysfunction is not specific to autism; it is commonly reported in other neuropsychiatric conditions

  • (although with different patterns). One view is that strong executive function early in life could protect at-riskindividuals from autism or other neurodevelopmental conditions by compensating for deficits in other brainsystems. 112 Individuals with autism often have a preference for, and superiority in, processing of local rather than globalsensory-perceptual features (table 4 ). Individuals without autism often show the opposite profile. This differencecould explain the excellent attention to detail, enhanced sensory-perceptual processing and discrimination, andidiosyncratic sensory responsivity (ie, hyper-reactivity or hyporeactivity to sensory input or unusual interest insensory features of the environment) in autism. It could also contribute to the exceptional abilitiesdisproportionately recorded in individuals with autism. 113,114 Additionally, top-down information processing inindividuals with autism is often characterised by reduced recognition of the global context,115 and a strongpreference to derive rule-based systems.113 The neural bases are spatially distributed and task dependent, butconverge on enhanced recruitment of primary sensory cortices, reduced recruitment of association and frontalcortices involved in top-down control,116 and enhanced synchronisation of parietal-occipital circuits.117 Neurobiology Neurobiological investigation has identified patterns of brain perfusion and neural biochemical characteristics,which are described elsewhere.118,119 Additionally, systems-level connectivity features and plausibleneuroanatomical, cellular, and molecular underpinnings of autism have been identified. Evidence fromelectrophysiology and functional neuroimaging (resting-state and task-based connectivity), 120 structuralneuroimaging (white-matter volume and microstructural properties),121-123 molecular genetics (cell adhesionmolecules and synaptic proteins, and excitatory-inhibitory imbalance),124 and information processing have givenrise to the idea that autism is characterised by atypical neural connectivity, rather than by a discrete set ofatypical brain regions. Ideas about the precise way in which connectivity is atypical vary, from decreased fronto-posterior and enhanced parietal-occipital connectivity, 117,125 reduced long-range and increased short-rangeconnectivity,126 to temporal binding deficits.127 Although none fully explains all the data (findings depend on thedefinition of connectivity, the developmental stage of the individual, the spatial and temporal scales, task vs no-task conditions, how motion artifacts are handled, and specific neural systems of concern), they support theheuristic value of the tenet that neural networks in autism are atypical in various ways. One frequently reported neuroanatomical feature of autism is a trajectory of generalised early brain overgrowthwhen aged 6-24 months.128 Other than increases in total brain volume, the amygdala is enlarged in youngchildren with autism,129 although this enlargement is no longer present by adolescence.130 Early brainovergrowth tends to be reported more in boys who have developmental regression than in other subgroups,131

    and might be a result of generalised physical overgrowth132 or biased norms of head circumference in paststudies.133 Additionally, meta-analyses suggest some consistent neuroanatomical differences across thelifespan in both grey-matter (eg, amygdala, hippocampus, and precuneus)134 and white-matter structures (eg,arcuate and uncinate fasciculi).123 A reduction in the volume of the corpus callosum is also a fairly consistentfinding.122 Many findings are age dependent,135 indicating the importance of developmental change. Post-mortem studies have shown a reduction in neuron number in the amygdala, fusiform gyrus, andcerebellum, and signs of persistent neuroinflammation.136 However, most donated brain tissue is from olderchildren, adolescents, and adults, so might not show early atypical development. One exception is a study ofyoung children137 that showed significant increases (rather than decreases) in neuron number in the prefrontalcortex. Genes typically differentially expressed across frontal and temporal cortices are less differentially expressed inautism; gene networks implicated in neuronal mechanisms are underexpressed in autism and enriched withautism susceptibility genes, whereas gene networks involved in immune processes are overexpressed. 138

    Neocortical dysgenesis marked by atypical patterning of cortical minicolumns (reduction in size, increasedneuronal density, and increase in cell dispersion)139 is also of interest and is potentially associated with atypical

  • synaptogenesis and an imbalanced excitatory-to-inhibitory ratio,140 both of which are important for neuralconnectivity. Interaction between the immune and the nervous systems is substantial throughout life, challenging the dogmaof the so-called immune privilege of the CNS.141 Frequency of immunological anomalies is increased inindividuals with autism and their families.142 In autism, altered immune processes affect a wide array ofneurodevelopmental processes (eg, neurogenesis, proliferation, apoptosis, synaptogenesis, and synapticpruning), with persistent active neuroinflammation, increased concentrations of pro-inflammatory cytokines inserum and cerebrospinal fluid, and altered cellular immune functions. 143 Maternal IgG antibodies that target thefetal brain or other gestational immune dysregulation could be pathogenic in some cases.144 Neuroimmunemechanisms could have key roles in some aspects of the pathophysiology of autism, but the exact biologyawaits clarification. In autism, alterations in both serotonin and -aminobutyric-acid (GABA) systems have been reported quiteconsistently,145 such as hyperserotonaemia and an altered developmental trajectory of brain serotonin synthesiscapacity, and reduction in the expression of GABA synthetic enzymes and receptors. Because of their relationwith affiliative and social behaviours, the oxytocin and vasopressin systems' roles in social impairments inautism are an active focus of investigation, including treatment trials. 146 The role of androgens (and oestrogens)in modulation of risks and protections, particularly prenatally, in the emergence of autism is also being tested22,147 in view of the accumulating evidence of a link between fetal testosterone and autistic traits.22 Prenatalhormones could be associated with the extreme-male-brain cognitive profile of reduced mentalising andenhanced systemising in autism development.22 Genetics Twin studies have suggested that autism has high heritability (more than 80%).148 This heritability occurs in thecontext of environmental risks and gene-environment interplay,33 because the monozygotic concordance ratesare never 100%. Epigenetic mechanisms and specific gene-environment interplay are important butunderstudied. From an evolutionary viewpoint, autistic traits could have been subject to positive selectionpressure, 149 because of the potential benefits of a solitary single-minded obsessive focus on innovativeunderstanding of a system.113 Such individuals might have successfully traded products or their building andfixing skills, thus acquiring resources and increasing their reproductive fitness, which could have contributed tothe maintenance of autism alleles in the gene pool. The genetic architecture of autism has proved to be complex and heterogeneous, as shown by studies ofcytogenetics, linkage, association, whole-genome linkage or association, and whole-genome or exomesequencing.124,150 Many genetic variants linked to autism have a high degree of pleiotropy (ie, one gene affectsmore than one phenotype). A high degree of locus heterogeneity has also been reported, with speculations thatup to 1000 genes are implicated.124,150 Both rare mutations with large effect sizes and common variations withsmaller effect sizes have a role.124,151 Rare mutations (ie, minor allele frequency

  • routine clinical examination.94,95 In terms of common variants (eg, single nucleotide polymorphisms with allele frequency >5% in the generalpopulation), genome-wide association studies have identified some important single nucleotide polymorphisms,but none has a large enough effect to be deemed causal. 124 However, up to 40% of simplex families and 60%of multiplex families (in which more than one individual has autism) could have several single nucleotidepolymorphisms that, when combined, have an additive effect on risk.154 Thus, common variability within singlenucleotide polymorphisms could contribute to the emergence of autism, the associated features in families (thebroader autism phenotype),42 the increased incidence of autism in offspring of parents with increased autistictraits,149 and autistic traits in the general population.155 Contributions from rare and common genetic variants arenot mutually exclusive.124 As the genetics of autism unfolds, information is continually updated. The rapid progress of genetics, along withanimal model systems and systems biology methods will enable the identification of diverse aetiologies andcommon molecular and cellular pathways crucial for neurodevelopment in autism. Such clarification could affecthow the autisms are classified, diagnosed, and treated in the future. Intervention Overview Intervention and support should be individualised and, if appropriate, multidimensional and multidisciplinary.The goals are to maximise an individual's functional independence and quality of life through development andlearning, improvements in social skills and communication, reductions in disability and comorbidity, promotion ofindependence, and provision of support to families. Additionally, individuals should be helped to fulfil theirpotential in areas of strength. Although autism is rooted in biology, most effective interventions so far arebehavioural and educational; drugs have had only a minor role so far. Behavioural approaches Various behavioural approaches exist,156-158 and are classified here into five complementary categories (table 5 ).Comprehensive approaches target a broad range of skills (cognitive, language, sensorimotor, and adaptivebehaviours) via long-term intensive programmes, and are grouped into applied behaviour analysis andstructured teaching (table 5 ).159 The models based on applied behaviour analysis originate from the Lovaasmethod160 and are collectively referred to as early intensive behavioural intervention. The Early Start DenverModel is a further development, in which a developmental framework and relationship aspects are emphasised (table 5 ). Early intensive behavioural intervention seems to enable the development of intelligence,communication, and adaptive function, and, to a lesser extent, language, daily living skills, and socialisation.161

    A shift from atypical to typical neurophysiology has been reported after 2 years of intervention with the EarlyStart Denver Model.162 However, too few randomised controlled trials have been done.158,161 The secondcomprehensive approach, structured teaching, originates from the TEACCH (Treatment and Education ofAutistic and related Communication-handicapped Children) model (table 5 ). It is widely used across a broad agerange, but little evidence is available from randomised controlled trials.158 Targeted approaches focus on specific cognitive behavioural domains. For non-verbal individuals, the PictureExchange Communication System (table 5 ) could be helpful, at least in the short term.158 Some evidence ofeffectiveness is available for models promoting emotion recognition, theory of mind, imitation, and functionalcommunication (table 5 ), but their generalisability to other domains of development is unclear.163 Joint attention orengagement training seems to be effective,163 and could be generalisable to natural contexts164 and languagedevelopment.165 A curriculum targeting socially synchronous engagement for toddlers also seems to beeffective.166 Social skill training for older children, adolescents, and adults is also promising (table 5 ). Programmesestablishing independence are often used but still need systematic assessment (table 5 ). Vocational interventionis important, especially for transition into adulthood, but more randomised controlled trials are needed to assesstheir effectiveness (table 5 ). Targeted behavioural intervention can also be beneficial by reducing anxiety and

  • aggression (table 5 ). Parent-mediated intervention has the advantage of bringing treatment into home and community settings toenable transfer of skills to real-life settings, and increasing parents' and caregivers' self-confidence (table 5 ).156

    Programmes can be comprehensive (eg, parent delivery of the Early Start Denver Model) or targeted (eg, atjoint attention or communication; table 5 ). The benefit of parent-mediated intervention alone is unclear, and resultsare inconsistent (table 5 ). Nevertheless, parental and family involvement is important in therapist-mediatedprogrammes.79,158 Sensory integration therapy--frequently used in occupational therapy--is sometimes offered as one componentof a comprehensive programme to address sensory-based problems. However, its effectiveness is inconclusive167 and it should not be considered as a routine intervention for autism.168 The US Health Resources and Services Administration158 and the UK National Institute for Health and CareExcellence74 have provided clinical guidelines for behavioural interventions. They stress that comprehensiveintervention should immediately follow diagnosis, and should be individualised (on the basis of developmentallevel, needs, and assets) and engage the family. Additionally, they emphasise that social-communicationtraining (with a focus on social skills) should be offered, and non-verbal individuals should have opportunities touse the Picture Exchange Communication System (or alternative communication interventions if that isunsuccessful). The guidelines stress that functional analysis should be integrated into design of interventions forchallenging behaviours. Supported employment should be offered for adults who have difficulty obtaining ormaintaining jobs. Support for families is crucial. Importantly, more randomised controlled trials are needed for allintervention models to improve evidence for choosing an intervention for each individual and family. Finally,creation of autism-friendly environments is essential. Future research needs to focus on monitoring ofoutcomes, understanding of specific needs for preverbal and non-verbal individuals as well as adolescents andadults, and identification of key components in effective strategies. 158 Generalisation of skills is still a majorchallenge. Drugs No biomedical agent has been shown to reliably improve social communication; experimental trials of drugstargeting various systems (eg, oxytocin, and cholinergic and glutamatergic agents) are in progress.169

    Antipsychotic drugs have been shown to effectively reduce challenging and repetitive behaviours in childrenwith autism, and insufficient evidence of usefulness in adolescents and adults is available (table 5 ). The risk ofadverse effects is grounds for concern.170 Serotonin reuptake inhibitors might reduce repetitive behaviours,although findings are inconsistent (table 5 ). The effect of stimulants on co-occurring symptoms of attention-deficithyperactivity disorder requires more study170 but is promising and has been recommended (table 5 ).171 Initialevidence suggests that atomoxetine also reduces co-occurring symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder.172 Some complementary and alternative medicines might be tolerated (eg, melatonin, vitamins, a gluten-casein-free diet, omega-3 fatty acids), but their effectiveness is not established.173,174 No treatment benefit of secretinhas been recorded.175 Chelation therapies, hyperbaric oxygen therapy, intravenous immunoglobulin, andantifungal agents all have serious safety concerns without evidenced benefits, and should not be used.173,174 Conclusions Understanding of autism has changed substantially in the 70 years since it was first described. With the recentexponential increase in research and the inclusion of scientists from a wide range of disciplines, understandingwill continue to evolve at an accelerated rate. The specialty has achieved much: it has reached a consensusabout behavioural definition; accepted the increased prevalence; improved understanding about earlypresentation; established systematic clinical assessments and evidence-based interventions; clarified specificcognitive processes; and used a multidomain, systems-level approach to understand neurobiology. It isdiscovering rare and common, mutated and transmitted genetic variants, and potential epigenetic and

  • environmental factors. Nevertheless, future work is needed in many areas. First, to understand aetiologies and development,clarification of the substantial heterogeneity by subgrouping is essential.6 Second, progress needs to be madein understanding of early developmental mechanisms on which early recognition and interventions rely. Third,effective individualised educational and biomedical interventions for the whole lifespan need to be established.Fourth, key environmental factors that interact with the complex genetic architecture of autism need to beidentified. Fifth, how autism affects individuals in different cultural contexts needs to be understood. Finally,environments should be made more autism friendly. Search strategy and selection criteria We searched PubMed, PsycINFO, the Cochrane Library, and Google Scholar for reports published betweenJan 1, 2000, and June 20, 2013. We used the search terms "autism", "autism spectrum disorder", "pervasivedevelopmental disorder", and "Asperger syndrome". We searched for other relevant earlier reports in thereference lists of reports identified through the database search. We mainly report summary findings fromsystematic reviews, meta-analyses, authoritative book chapters, and research articles published since 2008.We cite major updated reviews to provide further reading. Contributors M-CL did the initial literature search, summarised findings, and prepared the first draft of the report. MVLprepared figures. All authors contributed to the writing of the report. Conflicts of interest We declare that we have no conflicts of interest. Acknowledgments All authors are supported by the European Autism Interventions--A Multicentre Study for Developing NewMedications (which receives support from the Innovative Medicines Initiative Joint Undertaking [grantagreement 115300], resources of which are composed of financial contribution from the European Union'sSeventh Framework Programme [FP7/2007-2013], European Federation of Pharmaceutical Industries andAssociations companies, and Autism Speaks). M-CL is supported by Wolfson College (University of Cambridge,UK). MVL is supported by the British Academy and Jesus College (University of Cambridge, UK). SB-C issupported by the Wellcome Trust, the UK Medical Research Council, the National Institute for Health ResearchCollaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care for Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHSFoundation Trust, the Autism Research Trust, the European Union ASC-Inclusion Project, and Target AutismGenome. We thank Wei-Tsuen Soong and Digby Tantam for valuable discussions. Supplementary Material

    Features

    Core features in DSM-5 criteria*Persistent deficits in social communication and socialinteraction across multiple contexts

    Deficits in social-emotional reciprocityDeficits in non-verbal communicative behaviours used for socialinteractionDeficits in developing, maintaining, andunderstanding relationships

    Restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests,or activities

  • Stereotyped or repetitive motor movements, use ofobjects, or speechInsistence on sameness, inflexibleadherence to routines, or ritualised patterns of verbalor non-verbal behaviourHighly restricted, fixatedinterests that are abnormal in intensity or focusHyper-reactivity or hyporeactivity to sensory input or unusualinterest in sensory aspects of the environment

    Associated features not in DSM-5 criteria

    Atypical language development and abilities

    Age =6 years:deviant pragmatics, semantics, and morphology, withrelatively intact articulation and syntax (ie, earlydifficulties are resolved)

    Motor abnormalitiesMotor delay; hypotonia; catatonia; deficits incoordination, movement preparation and planning,praxis, gait, and balance

    Excellent attention to detail ..

    Proportion of individuals withautism affected

    Comments

    Developmental Intellectual disability ~45%

    Prevalence estimate isaffected by the diagnosticboundary and thedefinition of intelligence(eg, whether verbal abilityis used as a criterion)Inindividuals, discrepantperformance betweensubtests is common

    Language disorders Variable

    In DSM-IV, languagedelay was a definingfeature of autism (autisticdisorder), but is no longerincluded in DSM-5Anautism-specific languageprofile (separate fromlanguage disorders)exists, but with substantialinter-individual variability

    Attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder

    28-44%

  • In DSM-IV, not diagnosedwhen occurring inindividuals with autism, butno longer so in DSM-5Clinical guidanceavailable

    Tic disorders 14-38%

    ~65% have Tourette'ssyndrome

    Motor abnormality

  • ~5%

    Collectively called syndromicautismExamples includefragile x syndrome (21-50%of individuals affected haveautism), Rett syndrome (mosthave autistic features but withprofiles different fromidiopathic autism), tuberoussclerosis complex (24-60%),Down's syndrome (5-39%),phenylketonuria (5-20%),CHARGE syndrome(coloboma of the eye; heartdefects; atresia of thechoanae; retardation ofgrowth and development, orboth; genital and urinaryabnormalities, or both; andear abnormalities anddeafness; 15-50%),Angelman syndrome (50-81%), Timothy syndrome (60-70%), and Joubert syndrome(~40%)

    Sleep disorders

    50-80%Insomnia is the mostcommonClinical guidanceavailable

    Psychiatric

    Anxiety 42-56%

    Common across all age groupsMost common aresocial anxiety disorder (13-29% of individuals withautism; clinical guidance available) andgeneralised anxiety disorder (13-22%)High-functioning individuals are more susceptible (orsymptoms are more detectable)

    Depression 12-70%

    Common in adults, less common in childrenHigh-functioning adults who are less socially impairedare more susceptible (or symptoms are moredetectable)

    Obsessive-compulsivedisorder

    7-24%

    Shares the repetitive behaviour domain withautism that could cut across nosologicalcategoriesImportant to distinguish betweenrepetitive behaviours that do not involve intrusive,anxiety-causing thoughts or obsessions (part ofautism) and those that do (and are part ofobsessive-compulsive disorder)

  • Psychotic disorders 12-17%

    Mainly in adultsMost commonly recurrenthallucinosisHigh frequency of autism-like features(even a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder orpervasive developmental disorder) precedingadult-onset (52%) and childhood-onsetschizophrenia (30-50%)

    Substance use disorders

  • Could be secondary torepeated failure in socialexperiences

    Behavioural Aggressive behaviours

  • 24-item questionnaire: completed byparent or caregiver; takes 5-10 min

    Quantitative checklist forautism in toddlers (Q-CHAT)

    18-24 months

    25-item questionnaire: completed byparent or caregiver; takes 5-10 min;ten-item short version available

    Screening tool for autism inchildren aged 2 years(STAT)

    24-36 months

    12 items and activities: assessed byclinician or researcher after interactingwith the child; takes 20 min; intensivetraining necessary; level-two screeningmeasure

    Screening: older childrenand adolescents

    Social communication questionnaire(SCQ)

    >4 years (and mental age >2 years)40-item questionnaire:completed by parent orcaregiver; takes 10-15 min

    Social responsiveness scale, first orsecond edition (SRS, SRS-2)

    >25 years

    65-item questionnaire:completed by parent,caregiver, teacher, relative,or friends (self-report formavailable for adult in SRS-2); takes 15-20 min

    Childhood autism screening test(CAST)

    4-11 years37-item questionnaire:completed by parent orcaregiver; takes 10-15 min

    Autism spectrum screeningquestionnaire (ASSQ)*

    7-16 years

    27-item questionnaire:completed by parent,caregiver, or teacher; takes10 min

    Autism spectrum quotient (AQ), childand adolescent versions*

    Child: 4-11 years; adolescent: 10-16years

    50-item questionnaire:completed by parent orcaregiver; takes 10-15 min;ten-item short versionsavailable

    Screening: adults

    Autism spectrum quotient (AQ), adultversion*

    >16 years (with average orabove-average intelligence)

    50-item questionnaire: self-report;takes 10-15 min; ten-item short versionavailable

    The Ritvo autism Asperger diagnosticscale-revised (RAADS-R)

    >18 years (with average orabove-average intelligence)

    80-item questionnaire: self-report; donewith a clinician; takes 60 min

    Diagnosis: structured interviewThe autism diagnosticinterview-revised (ADI-R)

    Mental age >2 years

    93-item interview of parent orcaregiver; takes 15-3 h; intensivetraining necessary

    The diagnostic interview forsocial and communicationdisorders (DISCO)

    All chronological and mental ages

  • 362-item interview of parent orcaregiver; takes 2-4 h; intensivetraining necessary

    The developmental,dimensional, anddiagnostic interview (3Di)

    >2 years

    266-item computer-assisted interviewof parent or caregiver; takes 2 h; 53-item short form available, which takes45 min; intensive training necessary

    Diagnosis: observationalmeasure

    The autism diagnostic observationschedule, first or second edition(ADOS, ADOS-2)

    >12 months

    Clinical observation viainteraction: select one fromfive available modulesaccording to expressivelanguage level andchronological age; takes40-60 min; intensivetraining necessary

    Childhood autism rating scale, first orsecond edition (CARS, CARS-2)

    Main behavioural featuresMain cognitive (psychological)constructs

    Social cognition and socialperception

    Atypical social interaction and socialcommunication

    Gaze and eye contact; emotionperception; face processing;biological motion perception; socialattention and orienting; socialmotivation; social reward processing;non-verbal communication; imitation;affective empathy and sympathy; jointattention; pretend play; theory ofmind or mental perspective taking;self-referential cognition; alexithymia(difficulty understanding anddescribing own emotions);metacognitive awareness

    Executive functionRepetitive and stereotypedbehaviour; atypical social interactionand social communication

    Cognitive flexibility; planning;inhibitory control; attention shifting;monitoring; generativity; workingmemory

    Bottom-up and top-down (localvs global) informationprocessing*

    Idiosyncratic sensory-perceptualprocessing; excellent attention todetail; restricted interests andrepetitive behaviour; atypical socialinteraction and social communication

    Global vs local perceptual functioning(superior low-level sensory-perceptual processing); centralcoherence (global vs localpreference); systemising (drive toconstruct rule-based systems, abilityto understand rule-based systems,knowledge of factual systems)

    Target groupEvidence foreffectiveness*

    Intervention frameworkand goals

  • Behaviouralapproaches

    Comprehensive: ABA-based

    Early intensivebehaviouralintervention

    Young children (usuallyaged

  • Moderate orinsufficient forESDM; notestablished forfloortime

    ESDM: aims toaccelerate children'sdevelopment in alldomains; interventiontargets derived fromassessment ofdevelopmental skills;stresses social-communicativedevelopment,interpersonalengagement, imitation-based interpersonaldevelopment, andsocial attention andmotivation; integrationof ABA principles andpivotal responsetraining (ie, anaturalistic approachtargeting so-calledpivotal areas of achild's development,including motivation,response to multiplecues, self-management, andinitiation of socialinteractions)Floortime:emphasises functionalemotionaldevelopment, individualdifferences in sensorymodulation, processingand motor planning,relationships, andinteractions

    Comprehensive: structuredteaching

    Treatment andEducation of Autisticand relatedCommunication-handicapped Children(TEACCH)

  • Children,adolescents, andadults

    Low

    Providesstructures oftheenvironmentand activitiesthat can beunderstood bythe individual;usesindividuals'relativestrengths invisual skillsand intereststo supplementweaker skills;usesindividuals'specialinterests toengage forlearning;supports self-initiated useof meaningfulcommunication

    Targeted skill-basedintervention

    Picture ExchangeCommunicationSystem

    Non-verbal individuals Moderate

    Teaches spontaneoussocial-communicationskills through use ofsymbols or pictures

    Training in jointattention, pretendplay, sociallysynchronousbehaviour,imitation, emotionrecognition,theory of mind,and functionalcommunication

    Children

    Notestablished,but potentiallyeffective

    Fairly short-term(weeks to months)training sessionstargeting establishmentof particular socialcognitive abilitiesfundamental to typicalsocial-communicationdevelopment

  • Teaching socialskills (eg, emotionrecognition, turn-taking) with areasof interests (eg, inmachines andsystems)

    Children, adolescents,and adults

    Notestablished,but potentiallyeffective

    Short-term (weeks tomonths) interventionswith DVDs (eg,Mindreading or TheTransporters) or Legotherapy

    Social skilltraining

    Children aged >=6years, adolescents,and adults

    Low ormoderate

    Fairly short-term(weeks to months)training sessions tobuild social skills,usually through a groupformat

    Training in livingskills andautonomy

    Children, adolescents,and adults

    Notestablished

    Targets establishmentof living skills and self-management to buildautonomy; positivebehaviour support

    Vocationalintervention

    Adolescents and adults InsufficientEg, interview trainingand on-the-job support

    Targeted behaviouralintervention for anxietyand aggression

  • Cognitive behaviouraltherapy; ABA

    Children,adolescents,and adults

    Not established

    Cognitive behaviouraltherapy to reduceanxiety: modifiesdysfunctional thoughts;compared with ordinarycognitive behaviouraltherapy, therapymodified for autismrelies less onintrospection and moreon teaching of practicaladaptive skills withconcrete instructions;often combined withsocial skill training;systematicdesensitisation isuseful particularly forindividuals withintellectualdisabilityABA to reduceaggression: appliesfunctional behaviourassessment andteaches alternativebehaviours; skillsinclude antecedentmanipulations,changes in instructionalcontext, reinforcement-based strategies, andbehaviour reductionstrategies

    Parent-mediatedearly intervention

    Training forjoint attention,parent-childinteraction,andcommunication; or modelslike pivotalresponsetraining,parentdelivery of theESDM, andMore ThanWords

    Young children Insufficient or low

  • Teaches parentor caregiverinterventionstrategies thatcan be applied inhome andcommunitysettings,potentiallyincreasingparental efficacyand enablingchild'sgeneralisation ofskills to real-lifesettings

    DrugsAntipsychoticdrugs

    Risperidone;aripiprazole

    Children,adolescents, andadults

    Children: moderate(risperidone) or high(aripiprazole) for effect,and high for adverseeffect; adolescents andadults: insufficient, butmight have effects asin children

    To reducechallengingbehavioursand repetitivebehaviours;potentialadverseeffects includeweight gain,sedation,extrapyramidal symptoms,andhyperprolactinaemia(risperidone)

    Selective serotoninreuptake inhibitors

    Citalopram;escitalopram;fluoxetine; andothers

    Children, adolescents,and adults

    Insufficient foreffect andadverse effect

    To reduce repetitivebehaviours; potentialadverse effects includeactivation symptoms(agitation) andgastrointestinaldiscomfort

    Stimulant

  • References 1 L Kanner, Autistic disturbances of affective contact, Nerv Child, Vol. 2, 1943, 217-250 2 H Asperger, U Frith, Autism and Asperger syndrome, 1991, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 37-92 3 M Rutter, Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism, J Autism Child Schizophr, Vol. 8, 1978, 139-161 4 American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn., 2013,American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, VA 5 J Boucher, J Child Psychol Psychiatry, Vol. 53, 2012, 219-233 6 MC Lai, MV Lombardo, B Chakrabarti, S Baron-Cohen, PLoS Biol, Vol. 11, 2013, e1001544 7 V Lotter, Epidemiology of autistic conditions in young children, Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, Vol. 1,1966, 124-135 8 GS Fisch, Nosology and epidemiology in autism: classification counts, Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet,Vol. 160C, 2012, 91-103 9 KM Keyes, E Susser, K Cheslack-Postava, C Fountain, K Liu, PS Bearman, Cohort effects explain theincrease in autism diagnosis among children born from 1992 to 2003 in California, Int J Epidemiol, Vol. 41,2012, 495-503 10 M Elsabbagh, G Divan, YJ Koh, Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders,Autism Res, Vol. 5, 2012, 160-179 11 E Fombonne, S Quirke, A Hagen, Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders, DG Amaral, GDawson, DH Geschwind, Autism spectrum disorders, 2011, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, 90-111 12 ML Mattila, M Kielinen, SL Linna, Autism spectrum disorders according to DSM-IV-TR and comparison withDSM-5 draft criteria: an epidemiological study, J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 50, 2011, 583-592 13 YS Kim, BL Leventhal, YJ Koh, Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in a total population sample, Am JPsychiatry, Vol. 168, 2011, 904-912 14 S Baron-Cohen, FJ Scott, C Allison, Prevalence of autism-spectrum conditions: UK school-based populationstudy, Br J Psychiatry, Vol. 194, 2009, 500-509 15 S-W Hsu, P-H Chiang, L-P Lin, J-D Lin, Disparity in autism spectrum disorder prevalence among TaiwanNational Health Insurance enrollees: age, gender and urbanization effects, Res Autism Spectr Disord, Vol. 6,2012, 836-841 16 S Idring, D Rai, H Dal, Autism spectrum disorders in the stockholm youth cohort: design, prevalence andvalidity, PLoS One, Vol. 7, 2012, e41280 17 SJ Blumberg, MD Bramlett, MD Kogan, LA Schieve, JR Jones, MC Lu, 2013, National Center for HealthStatistics, Hyattsville, MD 18 G Russell, LR Rodgers, OC Ukoumunne, T Ford, Prevalence of parent-reported ASD and ADHD in the UK:findings from the Millennium Cohort Study, J Autism Dev Disord, 2013, published online May 30. 19 E Saemundsen, P Magnusson, I Georgsdottir, E Egilsson, V Rafnsson, Prevalence of autism spectrumdisorders in an Icelandic birth cohort, BMJ Open, Vol. 3, 2013, e002748

    MethylphenidateChildren,adolescents,and adults

    Insufficient for effectand adverse effect;might be helpful;clinical guidelineestablished

    To reduce attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder symptoms;potential adverseeffects includeinsomnia, decreasedappetite, weight loss,headache, andirritability

  • 20 TS Brugha, S McManus, J Bankart, Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders in adults in the community inEngland, Arch Gen Psychiatry, Vol. 68, 2011, 459-465 21 BD Barger, JM Campbell, JD McDonough, Prevalence and onset of regression within autism spectrumdisorders: a meta-analytic review, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 43, 2013, 817-828 22 S Baron-Cohen, MV Lombardo, B Auyeung, E Ashwin, B Chakrabarti, R Knickmeyer, PLoS Biol, Vol. 9,2011, e1001081 23 S Begeer, D Mandell, B Wijnker-Holmes, Sex differences in the timing of identification among children andadults with autism spectrum disorders, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 43, 2013, 1151-1156 24 E Giarelli, LD Wiggins, CE Rice, Sex differences in the evaluation and diagnosis of autism spectrumdisorders among children, Disabil Health J, Vol. 3, 2010, 107-116 25 G Russell, C Steer, J Golding, Social and demographic factors that influence the diagnosis of autisticspectrum disorders, Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, Vol. 46, 2011, 1283-1293 26 K Dworzynski, A Ronald, P Bolton, F Happe, J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 51, 2012, 788-797 27 S Kopp, C Gillberg, Res Dev Disabil, Vol. 32, 2011, 2875-2888 28 K Cheslack-Postava, RM Jordan-Young, Autism spectrum disorders: toward a gendered embodiment model,Soc Sci Med, Vol. 74, 2012, 1667-1674 29 MC Lai, MV Lombardo, G Pasco, A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioningautism spectrum conditions, PLoS One, Vol. 6, 2011, e20835 30 EB Robinson, P Lichtenstein, H Anckarsater, F Happe, A Ronald, Examining and interpreting the femaleprotective effect against autistic behavior, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, Vol. 110, 2013, 5258-5262 31 DM Werling, DH Geschwind, Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders, Curr Opin Neurol, Vol. 26, 2013,146-153 32 PM Rodier, Environmental exposures that increase the risk of autism spectrum disorders, DG Amaral, GDawson, DH Geschwind, Autism spectrum disorders, 2011, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, 863-874 33 MA Corrales, M Herbert, Autism and environmental genomics: synergistic systems approaches to autismcomplexity, DG Amaral, G Dawson, DH Geschwind, Autism spectrum disorders, 2011, Oxford University Press,New York, NY, 875-892 34 S Sandin, CM Hultman, A Kolevzon, R Gross, JH MacCabe, A Reichenberg, Advancing maternal age isassociated with increasing risk for autism: a review and meta-analysis, J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry,Vol. 51, 2012, 477-486 35 CM Hultman, S Sandin, SZ Levine, P Lichtenstein, A Reichenberg, Advancing paternal age and risk ofautism: new evidence from a population-based study and a meta-analysis of epidemiological studies, MolPsychiatry, Vol. 16, 2011, 1203-1212 36 KM Lampi, S Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki, V Lehti, Parental age and risk of autism spectrum disorders in a Finnishnational birth cohort, J Autism Dev Disord, 2013, published online March 12. 37 JJ Michaelson, Y Shi, M Gujral, Whole-genome sequencing in autism identifies hot spots for de novogermline mutation, Cell, Vol. 151, 2012, 1431-1442 38 A Kong, ML Frigge, G Masson, Rate of de novo mutations and the importance of father's age to disease risk,Nature, Vol. 488, 2012, 471-475 39 BM Neale, Y Kou, L Liu, Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders,Nature, Vol. 485, 2012, 242-245 40 BJ O'Roak, L Vives, S Girirajan, Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network ofde novo mutations, Nature, Vol. 485, 2012, 246-250 41 SJ Sanders, MT Murtha, AR Gupta, De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are stronglyassociated with autism, Nature, Vol. 485, 2012, 237-241 42 E Sucksmith, I Roth, RA Hoekstra, Autistic traits below the clinical threshold: re-examining the broader

  • autism phenotype in the 21st century, Neuropsychol Rev, Vol. 21, 2011, 360-389 43 MT Roelfsema, RA Hoekstra, C Allison, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 42, 2012, 734-739 44 AS Brown, A Sourander, S Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki, IW McKeague, J Sundvall, HM Surcel, Elevated maternalC-reactive protein and autism in a national birth cohort, Mol Psychiatry, 2013, published online Jan 22. 45 H Gardener, D Spiegelman, SL Buka, Prenatal risk factors for autism: comprehensive meta-analysis, Br JPsychiatry, Vol. 195, 2009, 7-14 46 HE Volk, F Lurmann, B Penfold, I Hertz-Picciotto, R McConnell, Traffic-related air pollution, particulatematter, and autism, JAMA Psychiatry, Vol. 70, 2013, 71-77 47 EM Roberts, PB English, JK Grether, GC Windham, L Somberg, C Wolff, Maternal residence nearagricultural pesticide applications and autism spectrum disorders among children in the California CentralValley, Environ Health Perspect, Vol. 115, 2007, 1482-1489 48 J Christensen, TK Gronborg, MJ Sorensen, Prenatal valproate exposure and risk of autism spectrumdisorders and childhood autism, JAMA, Vol. 309, 2013, 1696-1703 49 D Rai, BK Lee, C Dalman, J Golding, G Lewis, C Magnusson, Parental depression, maternal antidepressantuse during pregnancy, and risk of autism spectrum disorders: population based case-control study, BMJ, Vol.346, 2013, f2059 50 H Gardener, D Spiegelman, SL Buka, Perinatal and neonatal risk factors for autism: a comprehensive meta-analysis, Pediatrics, Vol. 128, 2011, 344-355 51 P Suren, C Roth, M Bresnahan, Association between maternal use of folic acid supplements and risk ofautism spectrum disorders in children, JAMA, Vol. 309, 2013, 570-577 52 KM Madsen, A Hviid, M Vestergaard, A population-based study of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccinationand autism, N Engl J Med, Vol. 347, 2002, 1477-1482 53 SK Parker, B Schwartz, J Todd, LK Pickering, Thimerosal-containing vaccines and autistic spectrumdisorder: a critical review of published original data, Pediatrics, Vol. 114, 2004, 793-804 54 F DeStefano, CS Price, ES Weintraub, Increasing exposure to antibody-stimulating proteins andpolysaccharides in vaccines is not associated with risk of autism, J Pediatr, 2013, published online March 29. 55 E Simonoff, A Pickles, T Charman, S Chandler, T Loucas, G Baird, Psychiatric disorders in children withautism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample, JAm Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 47, 2008, 921-929 56 B Hofvander, R Delorme, P Chaste, Psychiatric and psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligenceautism spectrum disorders, BMC Psychiatry, Vol. 9, 2009, 35 57 T Lugnegard, MU Hallerback, C Gillberg, Psychiatric comorbidity in young adults with a clinical diagnosis ofAsperger syndrome, Res Dev Disabil, Vol. 32, 2011, 1910-1917 58 ML Mattila, T Hurtig, H Haapsamo, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 40, 2010, 1080-1093 59 T Lugnegard, MU Hallerback, C Gillberg, Compr Psychiatry, Vol. 53, 2012, 333-340 60 G Joshi, J Wozniak, C Petty, Psychiatric comorbidity and functioning in a clinically referred population ofadults with autism spectrum disorders: a comparative study, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 43, 2013, 1314-1325 61 IS Kohane, A McMurry, G Weber, The co-morbidity burden of children and young adults with autismspectrum disorders, PLoS One, Vol. 7, 2012, e33224 62 E Simonoff, CR Jones, G Baird, A Pickles, F Happe, T Charman, The persistence and stability of psychiatricproblems in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders, J Child Psychol Psychiatry, Vol. 54, 2013, 186-194 63 S Woolfenden, V Sarkozy, G Ridley, M Coory, K Williams, Dev Med Child Neurol, Vol. 54, 2012, 306-312 64 D Bilder, EL Botts, KR Smith, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 43, 2013, 1196-1204 65 P Howlin, S Goode, J Hutton, M Rutter, Adult outcome for children with autism, J Child Psychol Psychiatry,Vol. 45, 2004, 212-229 66 E Billstedt, C Gillberg, C Gillberg, Autism after adolescence: population-based 13- to 22-year follow-up study

  • of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 35, 2005, 351-360 67 P Howlin, P Moss, S Savage, M Rutter, Social outcomes in mid- to later adulthood among individualsdiagnosed with autism and average nonverbal IQ as children, J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 52,2013, 572-581 68 MA Farley, WM McMahon, E Fombonne, Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average ornear-average cognitive abilities, Autism Res, Vol. 2, 2009, 109-118 69 C Fountain, AS Winter, PS Bearman, Six developmental trajectories characterize children with autism,Pediatrics, Vol. 129, 2012, e1112-e1120 70 K Gotham, A Pickles, C Lord, Trajectories of autism severity in children using standardized ADOS scores,Pediatrics, Vol. 130, 2012, e1278-e1284 71 RJ Landa, AL Gross, EA Stuart, M Bauman, Latent class analysis of early developmental trajectory in babysiblings of children with autism, J Child Psychol Psychiatry, Vol. 53, 2012, 986-996 72 D Fein, M Barton, IM Eigsti, Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism, J Child PsycholPsychiatry, Vol. 54, 2013, 195-205 73 JL Taylor, MM Seltzer, Changes in the autism behavioral phenotype during the transition to adulthood, JAutism Dev Disord, Vol. 40, 2010, 1431-1446 74 S Pilling, S Baron-Cohen, O Megnin-Viggars, R Lee, C Taylor, Recognition, referral, diagnosis, andmanagement of adults with autism: summary of NICE guidance, BMJ, Vol. 344, 2012, e4082 75 PT Shattuck, SC Narendorf, B Cooper, PR Sterzing, M Wagner, JL Taylor, Postsecondary education andemployment among youth with an autism spectrum disorder, Pediatrics, Vol. 129, 2012, 1042-1049 76 P Howlin, P Moss, Adults with autism spectrum disorders, Can J Psychiatry, Vol. 57, 2012, 275-283 77 F Happe, RA Charlton, Aging in autism spectrum disorders: a mini-review, Gerontology, Vol. 58, 2012, 70-78 78 M Elsabbagh, MH Johnson, Getting answers from babies about autism, Trends Cogn Sci, Vol. 14, 2010, 81-87 79 L Zwaigenbaum, S Bryson, C Lord, Clinical assessment and management of toddlers with suspected autismspectrum disorder: insights from studies of high-risk infants, Pediatrics, Vol. 123, 2009, 1383-1391 80 DS Mandell, MM Novak, CD Zubritsky, Factors associated with age of diagnosis among children with autismspectrum disorders, Pediatrics, Vol. 116, 2005, 1480-1486 81 T Charman, K Gotham, Child Adolesc Ment Health, Vol. 18, 2013, 52-63 82 CP Johnson, SM Myers, Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders, Pediatrics,Vol. 120, 2007, 1183-1215 83 M Al-Qabandi, JW Gorter, P Rosenbaum, Pediatrics, Vol. 128, 2011, e211-e217 84 S Ozonoff, AM Iosif, F Baguio, A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism, JAm Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 49, 2010, 256-266 85 K Chawarska, S Macari, F Shic, Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infantslater diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders, Biol Psychiatry, Vol. 74, 2013, 195-203 86 MW Wan, J Green, M Elsabbagh, M Johnson, T Charman, F Plummer, J Child Psychol Psychiatry, Vol. 54,2013, 763-771 87 JT Elison, SJ Paterson, JJ Wolff, White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds atrisk for autism, Am J Psychiatry, 2013, published online March 20. 88 M Elsabbagh, J Fernandes, S Jane Webb, G Dawson, T Charman, MH Johnson, Disengagement of visualattention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood, Biol Psychiatry, Vol. 74, 2013, 189-194 89 M Elsabbagh, E Mercure, K Hudry, Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with lateremerging autism, Curr Biol, Vol. 22, 2012, 338-342 90 JJ Wolff, H Gu, G Gerig, Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in

  • infants with autism, Am J Psychiatry, Vol. 169, 2012, 589-600 91 D Messinger, GS Young, S Ozonoff, Beyond autism: a baby siblings research consortium study of high-riskchildren at three years of age, J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 52, 2013, 300-308 92 S Ozonoff, BL Goodlin-Jones, M Solomon, Evidence-based assessment of autism spectrum disorders inchildren and adolescents, J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol, Vol. 34, 2005, 523-540 93 M Coleman, C Gillberg, The autisms, 4th edn., 2012, Oxford University Press, New York, NY 94 KM Heil, CP Schaaf, Curr Psychiatry Rep, Vol. 15, 2013, 334 95 DT Miller, MP Adam, S Aradhya, Consensus statement: chromosomal microarray is a first-tier clinicaldiagnostic test for individuals with developmental disabilities or congenital anomalies, Am J Hum Genet, Vol. 86,2010, 749-764 96 B Rimland, Infantile autism: the syndrome and its implications for a neural theory of behavior, 1964,Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, NY 97 U Frith, Why we need cognitive explanations of autism, Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), Vol. 65, 2012, 2073-2092 98 S Baron-Cohen, AM Leslie, U Frith, Cognition, Vol. 21, 1985, 37-46 99 J Boucher, Putting theory of mind in its place: psychological explanations of the socio-emotional-communicative impairments in autistic spectrum disorder, Autism, Vol. 16, 2012, 226-246 100 KA Pelphrey, S Shultz, CM Hudac, BC Vander Wyk, Research review: constraining heterogeneity: thesocial brain and its development in autism spectrum disorder, J Child Psychol Psychiatry, Vol. 52, 2011, 631-644 101 FG Happe, The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects withautism, Child Dev, Vol. 66, 1995, 843-855 102 A Senju, Spontaneous theory of mind and its absence in autism spectrum disorders, Neuroscientist, Vol.18, 2011, 108-113 103 YS Chung, D Barch, M Strube, A meta-analysis of mentalizing impairments in adults with schizophreniaand autism spectrum disorder, Schizophr Bull, 2013, published online May 17. 104 IA Apperly, Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), Vol. 65, 2012, 825-839 105 MV Lombardo, S Baron-Cohen, Unraveling the paradox of the autistic self, Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci,Vol. 1, 2010, 393-403 106 MV Lombardo, B Chakrabarti, ET Bullmore, Atypical neural self-representation in autism, Brain, Vol. 133,2010, 611-624 107 GS Dichter, Functional magnetic resonance imaging of autism spectrum disorders, Dialogues ClinNeurosci, Vol. 14, 2012, 319-351 108 RC Philip, MR Dauvermann, HC Whalley, K Baynham, SM Lawrie, AC Stanfield, A systematic review andmeta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders, Neurosci Biobehav Rev, Vol. 36, 2012,901-942 109 AF Hamilton, Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: a systematic review of current theories, DevCogn Neurosci, Vol. 3, 2013, 91-105 110 HM Geurts, B Corbett, M Solomon, The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism, Trends Cogn Sci, Vol. 13,2009, 74-82 111 SJ White, The triple I hypothesis: taking another('s) perspective on executive dysfunction in autism, JAutism Dev Disord, Vol. 43, 2013, 114-121 112 MH Johnson, Executive function and developmental disorders: the flip side of the coin, Trends Cogn Sci,Vol. 16, 2012, 454-457 113 S Baron-Cohen, E Ashwin, C Ashwin, T Tavassoli, B Chakrabarti, Talent in autism: hyper-systemizing,hyper-attention to detail and sensory hypersensitivity, Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, Vol. 364, 2009,1377-1383

  • 114 L Mottron, L Bouvet, A Bonnel, Veridical mapping in the development of exceptional autistic abilities,Neurosci Biobehav Rev, Vol. 37, 2013, 209-228 115 F Happe, U Frith, The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrumdisorders, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 36, 2006, 5-25 116 F Samson, L Mottron, I Soulieres, TA Zeffiro, Enhanced visual functioning in autism: an ALE meta-analysis,Hum Brain Mapp, Vol. 33, 2012, 1553-1581 117 NJ Minshew, TA Keller, The nature of brain dysfunction in autism: functional brain imaging studies, CurrOpin Neurol, Vol. 23, 2010, 124-130 118 T Ohnishi, H Matsuda, T Hashimoto, Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow in childhood autism, Brain, Vol.123, 2000, 1838-1844 119 JM Baruth, CA Wall, MC Patterson, JD Port, Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy as a probe into thepathophysiology of autism spectrum disorders (ASD): a review, Autism Res, 2013, published online Feb 21. 120 ME Vissers, MX Cohen, HM Geurts, Brain connectivity and high functioning autism: a promising path ofresearch that needs refined models, methodological convergence, and stronger behavioral links, NeurosciBiobehav Rev, Vol. 36, 2012, 604-625 121 BG Travers, N Adluru, C Ennis, Diffusion tensor imaging in autism spectrum disorder: a review, AutismRes, Vol. 5, 2012, 289-313 122 TW Frazier, AY Hardan, A meta-analysis of the corpus callosum in autism, Biol Psychiatry, Vol. 66, 2009,935-941 123 J Radua, E Via, M Catani, D Mataix-Cols, Voxel-based meta-analysis of regional white-matter volumedifferences in autism spectrum disorder versus healthy controls, Psychol Med, Vol. 41, 2011, 1539-1550 124 DH Geschwind, Genetics of autism spectrum disorders, Trends Cogn Sci, Vol. 15, 2011, 409-416 125 MA Just, TA Keller, VL Malave, RK Kana, S Varma, Autism as a neural systems disorder: a theory offrontal-posterior underconnectivity, Neurosci Biobehav Rev, Vol. 36, 2012, 1292-1313 126 MK Belmonte, G Allen, A Beckel-Mitchener, LM Boulanger, RA Carper, SJ Webb, Autism and abnormaldevelopment of brain connectivity, J Neurosci, Vol. 24, 2004, 9228-9231 127 J Brock, CC Brown, J Boucher, G Rippon, The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism, DevPsychopathol, Vol. 14, 2002, 209-224 128 E Courchesne, K Campbell, S Solso, Brain growth across the life span in autism: age-specific changes inanatomical pathology, Brain Res, Vol. 1380, 2011, 138-145 129 CW Nordahl, R Scholz, X Yang, Increased rate of amygdala growth in children aged 2 to 4 years withautism spectrum disorders: a longitudinal study, Arch Gen Psychiatry, Vol. 69, 2012, 53-61 130 CM Schumann, J Hamstra, BL Goodlin-Jones, The amygdala is enlarged in children but not adolescentswith autism; the hippocampus is enlarged at all ages, J Neurosci, Vol. 24, 2004, 6392-6401 131 CW Nordahl, N Lange, DD Li, Brain enlargement is associated with regression in preschool-age boys withautism spectrum disorders, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, Vol. 108, 2011, 20195-20200 132 K Chawarska, D Campbell, L Chen, F Shic, A Klin, J Chang, Early generalized overgrowth in boys withautism, Arch Gen Psychiatry, Vol. 68, 2011, 1021-1031 133 A Raznahan, GL Wallace, L Antezana, Biol Psychiatry, 2013, published online May 23. 134 E Via, J Radua, N Cardoner, F Happe, D Mataix-Cols, Arch Gen Psychiatry, Vol. 68, 2011, 409-418 135 EG Duerden, KM Mak-Fan, MJ Taylor, SW Roberts, Regional differences in grey and white matter inchildren and adults with autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimate (ALE) meta-analysis,Autism Res, Vol. 5, 2012, 49-66 136 CM Schumann, SC Noctor, DG Amaral, Neuropathology of autism spectrum disorders: postmortem studies,DG Amaral, G Dawson, DH Geschwind, Autism spectrum disorders, 2011, Oxford University Press, New York,NY, 539-565

  • 137 E Courchesne, PR Mouton, ME Calhoun, Neuron number and size in prefrontal cortex of children withautism, JAMA, Vol. 306, 2011, 2001-2010 138 I Voineagu, X Wang, P Johnston, Transcriptomic analysis of autistic brain reveals convergent molecularpathology, Nature, Vol. 474, 2011, 380-384 139 MF Casanova, Neuropathological and genetic findings in autism: the significance of a putativeminicolumnopathy, Neuroscientist, Vol. 12, 2006, 435-441 140 JL Rubenstein, Three hypotheses for developmental defects that may underlie some forms of autismspectrum disorder, Curr Opin Neurol, Vol. 23, 2010, 118-123 141 AK McAllister, J van de Water, Breaking boundaries in neural-immune interactions, Neuron, Vol. 64, 2009,9-12 142 P Goines, A Zimmerman, P Ashwood, J Van de Water, The immune system, autoimmunity, allergy, andautism spectrum disorders, DG Amaral, G Dawson, DH Geschwind, Autism spectrum disorders, 2011, OxfordUniversity Press, New York, NY, 395-419 143 C Onore, M Careaga, P Ashwood, The role of immune dysfunction in the pathophysiology of autism, BrainBehav Immun, Vol. 26, 2012, 383-392 144 D Braunschweig, J Van de Water, Maternal autoantibodies in autism, Arch Neurol, Vol. 69, 2012, 693-699 145 DC Chugani, Neurotransmitters, DG Amaral, G Dawson, DH Geschwind, Autism spectrum disorders, 2011,Oxford University Press, New York, NY, 566-575 146 H Yamasue, JR Yee, R Hurlemann, Integrative approaches utilizing oxytocin to enhance prosocialbehavior: from animal and human social behavior to autistic social dysfunction, J Neurosci, Vol. 32, 2012,14109-14117 147 DW Pfaff, I Rapin, S Goldman, Male predominance in autism: neuroendocrine influences on arousal andsocial anxiety, Autism Res, Vol. 4, 2011, 163-176 148 A Ronald, RA Hoekstra, Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies, Am JMed Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, Vol. 156B, 2011, 255-274 149 S Baron-Cohen, Autism and the technical mind, Sci Am, Vol. 307, 2012, 72-75 150 JD Murdoch, MW State, Recent developments in the genetics of autism spectrum disorders, Curr OpinGenet Dev, Vol. 23, 2013, 310-315 151 MW State, P Levitt, The conundrums of understanding genetic risks for autism spectrum disorders, NatNeurosci, Vol. 14, 2011, 1499-1506 152 B Devlin, SW Scherer, Genetic architecture in autism spectrum disorder, Curr Opin Genet Dev, Vol. 22,2012, 229-237 153 JL Stein, NN Parikshak, DH Geschwind, Rare inherited variation in autism: beginning to see the forest anda few trees, Neuron, Vol. 77, 2013, 209-211 154 L Klei, SJ Sanders, MT Murtha, Common genetic variants, acting additively, are a major source of risk forautism, Mol Autism, Vol. 3, 2012, 9 155 B Chakrabarti, F Dudbridge, L Kent, Genes related to sex steroids, neural growth, and social-emotionalbehavior are associated with autistic traits, empathy, and Asperger syndrome, Autism Res, Vol. 2, 2009, 157-177 156 G Dawson, K Burner, Behavioral interventions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: areview of recent findings, Curr Opin Pediatr, Vol. 23, 2011, 616-620 157 LA Vismara, SJ Rogers, Annu Rev Clin Psychol, Vol. 6, 2010, 447-468 158 MA Maglione, D Gans, L Das, J Timbie, C Kasari, Nonmedical interventions for children with ASD:recommended guidelines and further research needs, Pediatrics, Vol. 130, Iss. suppl 2, 2012, S169-S178 159 K Callahan, S Shukla-Mehta, S Magee, M Wie, ABA versus TEACCH: the case for defining and validatingcomprehensive treatment models in autism, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 40, 2010, 74-88

  • 160 T Smith, S Eikeseth, O Ivar Lovaas: pioneer of applied behavior analysis and intervention for children withautism, J Autism Dev Disord, Vol. 41, 2011, 375-378 161 B Reichow, EE Barton, BA Boyd, K Hume, Early intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) for young childrenwith autism spectrum disorders (ASD), Cochrane Database Syst Rev, Vol. 10, 2012, CD009260. 162 G Dawson, EJ Jones, K Merkle, Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain activity inyoung children with autism, J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 51, 2012, 1150-1159 163 C Kasari, S Patterson, Interventions addressing social impairment in autism, Curr Psychiatry Rep, Vol. 14,2012, 713-725 164 A Kaale, L Smith, E Sponheim, A randomized controlled trial of preschool-based joint attention interventionfor children with autism, J Child Psychol Psychiatry, Vol. 53, 2012, 97-105 165 C Kasari, T Paparella, S Freeman, LB Jahromi, Language outcome in autism: randomized comparison ofjoint attention and play interventions, J Consult Clin Psychol, Vol. 76, 2008, 125-137 166 RJ Landa, KC Holman, AH O'Neill, EA Stuart, Intervention targeting development of socially synchronousengagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial, J Child PsycholPsychiatry, Vol. 52, 2011, 13-21 167 M Zimmer, L Desch, Sensory integration therapies for children with developmental and behavioraldisorders, Pediatrics, Vol. 129, 2012, 1186-1189 168 R Lang, M O'Reilly, O Healy, Sensory integration therapy for autism spectrum disorders: a systematicreview, Res Autism Spectr Disord, Vol. 6, 2012, 1004-1018 169 C Farmer, A Thurm, P Grant, Pharmacotherapy for the core symptoms in autistic disorder: current status ofthe research, Drugs, Vol. 73, 2013, 303-314 170 ML McPheeters, Z Warren, N Sathe, A systematic review of medical treatments for children with autismspectrum disorders, Pediatrics, Vol. 127, 2011, e1312-e1321 171 R Mahajan, MP Bernal, R Panzer, Pediatrics, Vol. 130, Iss. suppl 2, 2012, S125-S138 172 M Harfterkamp, G van de Loo-Neus, RB Minderaa, J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Vol. 51, 2012,733-741 173 E Anagnostou, R Hansen, Medical treatment overview: traditional and novel psycho-pharmacological andcomplementary and alternative medications, Curr Opin Pediatr, Vol. 23, 2011, 621-627 174 RS Akins, K Angkustsiri, RL Hansen, Complementary and alternative medicine in autism: an evidence-based approach to negotiating safe and efficacious interventions with families, Neurotherapeutics, Vol. 7, 2010,307-319 175 S Krishnaswami, ML McPheeters, J Veenstra-Vanderweele, A systematic review of secretin for childrenwith autism spectrum disorders, Pediatrics, Vol. 127, 2011, e1322-e1325 Subject: Autism; Children & youth; Behavior; Epidemiology; Males; Females; MeSH: Autistic Disorder -- epidemiology, Autistic Disorder -- genetics, Early Diagnosis, Genetic Predispositionto Disease, Humans, Mass Screening -- methods, Prevalence, Prognosis, Risk Factors, Autistic Disorder --diagnosis (major), Autistic Disorder -- therapy (major) Publication title: The Lancet Volume: 383 Issue: 9920 Pages: 896-910 Publication year: 2014

  • Publication date: Mar 8, 2014 Year: 2014 Publisher: Elsevier Limited Place of publication: London Country of publication: United States Publication subject: Medical Sciences ISSN: 01406736 CODEN: LANCAO Source type: Scholarly Journals Language of publication: English Document type: Journal Article DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1 Accession number: 24074734 ProQuest document ID: 1508931440 Document URL: http://search.proquest.com/docview/1508931440?accountid=15533 Copyright: Copyright Elsevier Limited Mar 8, 2014 Last updated: 2014-11-25 Database: ProQuest Central

    _______________________________________________________________ Contact ProQuest Copyright 2014 ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. - Terms and Conditions

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1http://search.proquest.com/docview/1508931440?accountid=15533http://www.proquest.com/go/contactsupporthttp://search.proquest.com/info/termsAndConditionsAutism