quantifying the performance of meg source reconstruction ... › content › biorxiv › early ›...

22
1 Quantifying the performance of MEG source reconstruction using resting 1 state data 2 3 Simon Little , James Bonaiuto 2 , Sofie S Meyer 3,4 , Jose Lopez 5 , Sven 4 Bestmann 1,3 * & Gareth Barnes 3 * 5 6 7 1. Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience & Movement Disorders, UCL Institute of 8 Neurology, Queen Square, London. UK 9 2. Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive, CNRS UMR 5229-Université Claude Bernard Lyon I, 69675 10 Bron Cedex, France 11 3. Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Institute of Neurology, 12 Queen Square, 12 London, UK 13 4. Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, WC1N 3AR, UK 14 5. Electronic Engineering Department, Universidad de Antioquia, UdeA, Calle 70 No. 52-21, 15 Medellín, Colombia 16 17 ɸ Corresponding Author 18 Simon Little - [email protected] 19 * Joint last author 20 21 22 was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (which this version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252 doi: bioRxiv preprint

Upload: others

Post on 02-Feb-2021

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 1

    Quantifying the performance of MEG source reconstruction using resting 1

    state data 2

    3

    Simon Little1ɸ, James Bonaiuto2, Sofie S Meyer3,4, Jose Lopez5, Sven 4

    Bestmann1,3* & Gareth Barnes3* 5

    6

    7

    1. Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience & Movement Disorders, UCL Institute of 8

    Neurology, Queen Square, London. UK 9

    2. Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive, CNRS UMR 5229-Université Claude Bernard Lyon I, 69675 10

    Bron Cedex, France 11

    3. Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL Institute of Neurology, 12 Queen Square, 12

    London, UK 13

    4. Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, WC1N 3AR, UK 14

    5. Electronic Engineering Department, Universidad de Antioquia, UdeA, Calle 70 No. 52-21, 15

    Medellín, Colombia 16

    17

    ɸ Corresponding Author 18

    Simon Little - [email protected] 19

    * Joint last author 20

    21

    22

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 2

    Abstract 23

    Resting state networks measured with magnetoencephalography (MEG) form transiently stable 24

    spatio-temporal patterns in the subsecond range, and therefore fluctuate more rapidly than 25

    previously appreciated. These states populate and interact across the whole brain, are simple to 26

    record, and possess the same dynamic structure of task related changes. They therefore provide a 27

    generic, heterogeneous, and plentiful functional substrate against which to test different MEG 28

    recording and reconstruction approaches. Here we validate a non-invasive method for quantifying the 29

    resolution of different inversion assumptions under different recording regimes (with and without 30

    head-casts) based on resting state MEG. Spatio-temporally partitioning of data into self-similar periods 31

    confirmed a rich and rapidly dynamic temporal structure with a small number of regularly reoccurring 32

    states. To test the anatomical precision that could be resolved through these transient states we then 33

    inverted these data onto libraries of systematically distorted, subject specific, cortical meshes and 34

    compared the quality of the fit using Cross Validation and a Free Energy metric. This revealed which 35

    inversion scheme was able to best support the least distorted (most accurate) anatomical models. 36

    Both datasets showed an increase in model fit as anatomical models moved towards the true cortical 37

    surface. In the head-cast MEG data, the Empirical Bayesian Beamformer (EBB) algorithm showed the 38

    best mean anatomical discrimination (3.7 mm) compared with Minimum Norm / LORETA (6.0 mm) 39

    and Multiple Sparse priors (9.4 mm). This pattern was replicated in the second (conventional dataset) 40

    although with a marginally poorer prediction of the missing (cross-validated) data. Our findings 41

    suggest that the abundant resting state data now commonly available could be used to refine and 42

    validate MEG source reconstruction methods or recording paradigms. 43

    44

    45

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 3

    Introduction 46

    Resting state network analyses have emerged as a powerful tool for deconstructing and mapping 47

    large scale networks in the human brain. These have been shown to be linked to a strikingly wide 48

    range of cognitive functions in health and neurological and psychiatric disorders (Barttfeld et al., 49

    2015; Kaiser et al., 2015; Lewis et al., 2009; Li et al., 2012; Peterson et al., 2014; Philippi et al., 2015; 50

    Reineberg et al., 2015; Sheline and Raichle, 2013; Tessitore et al., 2012; Venkataraman et al., 2012; 51

    Wu et al., 2014; Wurina et al., 2012). Emerging evidence from magnetoencephalography (MEG) 52

    suggests that resting state networks may have a significantly finer temporal scale than previously 53

    recognised, with network state fluctuations occurring on the scale of 100-200 milliseconds (Baker et 54

    al., 2014; Koenig et al., 2002; Wackermann et al., 1993; Woolrich et al., 2013). These states are 55

    thought to be relevant because they effectively rehearse the transient dynamic patterns observed 56

    during task performance (O’Neill et al., 2017). 57

    MEG detects electromagnetic fields at sensors outside the head with the resultant challenge of 58

    inferring the neuronal sources responsible for these measured external electromagnetic changes. 59

    One solution is to restrict the multiple potential solutions to this challenge through a priori 60

    assumptions relating to the model of neural activity, including the anatomical structure of the brain 61

    (the cortical mesh) and temporal relationship between sources (i.e. source co-variance matrix). Both 62

    these priors have uncertainty attached to them: for the anatomy, the uncertainty regarding head 63

    position, due to co-registration error and within-session head movement (Bonaiuto et al., 2018; 64

    Hillebrand and Barnes, 2011; Meyer et al., 2017b; Troebinger et al., 2014a, 2014b); whereas 65

    assumptions about the temporal relationship between sources are continually being refined and 66

    debated (Baillet, 2015; Baillet et al., 2001; Wipf and Nagarajan, 2009). 67

    Here we leverage new analytic techniques to quantify the sensitivity of MEG source inversion 68

    schemes by progressively deforming the anatomical models (López et al., 2017, 2012; Stevenson et 69

    al., 2014). Specifically, here we quantify how distortions in the MRI-extracted cortical manifold affect 70

    our ability to predict or model the underlying current distribution (through cross validation error and 71

    Free Energy respectively). The rationale is that the best MEG inversion scheme will be the most 72

    sensitive to subtle distortions of the anatomy (as we know that MEG data derives from grey matter 73

    structure). This spatial distortion metric then provides a principled choice between different a priori 74

    inversion assumptions (i.e. different algorithms) and recording techniques. 75

    In addition to distinguishing between algorithms, here we also tested whether we could use the 76

    same methods to distinguish between datasets collected with a head-cast (Meyer et al., 2017a; 77

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 4

    Troebinger et al., 2014a, 2014b), where the forward model is more precisely known, and those 78

    collected without. 79

    The paper proceeds as follows. We first parcellated our two resting state datasets into brief epochs, 80

    based on the dominant spatio-temporal network during short temporal epochs, using a hidden 81

    Markov model. The epochs for the four most dominant networks were then amalgamated into four 82

    enriched datasets and taken forwards for inversion. These datasets were then inverted onto a library 83

    of subject specific distorted meshes, for which we had control over the spatial detail available in the 84

    forward model. For each of these meshes, and inversion scheme we quantified model fit using Cross 85

    validation and Free Energy metrics. We found an approximately monotonic improvement in model 86

    fit with decreasing amounts of distortion towards the real mesh. We then used this spatial 87

    quantification to compare different inversion co-variance prior assumptions as implemented in a 88

    number of different, commonly utilised schemes. For these data we found that the beamformer 89

    based priors (EBB) were the most sensitive to small deviations from the true anatomy. Finally we 90

    directly compared data recorded with head-casts to data recorded conventionally and found 91

    marginal (but not significant) differences between the two recording techniques. 92

    93

    Methods 94

    MRI 95

    Subjects underwent two MRI scans using a Siemens Tim Trio 3 T system (Erlangen, Germany). For 96

    the head-cast scan, the acquisition time was 3 min 42 s, in addition to 45 s for the localizer 97

    sequence. The sequence implemented was a radiofrequency (RF) and gradient spoiled T1 weighted 98

    3D fast low angle shot (FLASH) sequence with image resolution 1 mm3 (1 mm slice thickness), field-of 99

    view set to 256, 256, and 192 mm along the phase (A–P), read (H–F), and partition (R–L; second 3D 100

    phase encoding direction) directions respectively. A single shot, high readout bandwidth (425 101

    Hz/pixel) and minimum echo time (2.25ms) was used. This sequence was optimised to preserve head 102

    and scalp structure (as opposed to brain structure). Repetition time was set to 7.96 ms and 103

    excitation flip angle set to 12° to ensure sufficient SNR. A partial Fourier (factor 6/8) acquisition was 104

    used in each phase-encoded direction to accelerate acquisition. For the anatomical scan later used 105

    to construct the cortical model, multiple parameter maps (MPM) were acquired to optimise spatial 106

    resolution of the brain image (to 0.8 mm). The sequence comprised three multi-echo 3D FLASH (fast 107

    low angle shot) scans, one RF transmit field map and one static magnetic (B0) field map scan 108

    (Weiskopf et al., 2013). 109

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 5

    110

    Head-cast construction 111

    Scalp surfaces from the head-cast MRI data were extracted using SPM12 112

    (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) by registering MRI images to a tissue probability map which 113

    classified voxels according to tissue makeup (e.g. skull, skin, grey matter etc.). The skin tissue 114

    probability map was transformed into a surface using the ‘isosurface’ function in MATLAB® and then 115

    into standard template library format with the outlines of three fiducial coils digitally placed at 116

    conventional sites (left/right pre-auricular & nasion). Next, a positive head model was printed using 117

    a Zcorp 3D printer (600 x 540 dots per inch resolution) and this model placed inside a replica dewar-118

    helmet with liquid resin poured between the two, resulting in a flexible, subject specific, foam head-119

    cast with fiducial indentations in MRI-defined locations (Meyer et al., 2017a). 120

    121

    MEG recording 122

    Resting state data was acquired from 12 healthy subjects using head-casts (age: 26.6 ± 1.0 yrs) and 123

    12 other healthy subjects without head-casts (age: 25.2 ± 1.9 yrs). All subjects were right handed, 124

    had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and had no history of neurological or psychiatric disease. 125

    Informed written consent was given by all subjects and recordings were carried out after obtaining 126

    ethical approval from the University College London ethics committee (ref. number 3090/001). 127

    All subjects underwent a 10 minutes resting state scan with eyes open, using a CTF 275 Omega MEG 128

    system. The head was localised using the three head-cast-embedded fiducials (head-cast subjects) or 129

    fiducials placed on the nasion and left/right pre-auricular points (non-head-cast subjects). Average 130

    absolute range of head movement within the 10 minute resting state recording was 0.26 ± 0.06, 0.24 131

    ± 0.05, 1.1 ± 0.54 mm (X,Y,Z directions; ± SEM) for head-cast and 3.2 ± 0.5, 3.0 ± 0.5, 3.3 ± 0.2 (X,Y,Z 132

    directions; ± SEM) for non-head-cast data. The data were sampled at a rate of 1200 Hz, imported 133

    into SPM12 and filtered (4th order butterworth bandpass filter: 1-90 Hz, 4th order butterworth 134

    bandstop filter 48-52 Hz) and downsampled to 250 Hz. 135

    In order to parcellate the data into self-similar periods that capture the resting state network 136

    transitions (100-200 ms), a Hidden Markov Model (HMM) was used that could identify the rapid 137

    formation and dissolution of recurring resting state networks. With this, a ‘statepath’ was estimated 138

    for each 10 minute resting state block, which tracks the fine spatiotemporal dynamics and allocates 139

    each point in time to one of eight dominant network states (Baker et al., 2014). For this statepath 140

    determination, a copy of each subject data was dimensionally reduced using principle component 141

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 6

    analysis (PCA) to derive 40 components of unit variance and mean (Woolrich et al., 2013). With 142

    these data, an 8 state Hidden Markov model (HMM; www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/∼woolrich/HMMtoolbox) 143

    was then applied to derive the most probable state at all points in time (the statepath) (Fig. 1A) 144

    (Baker et al., 2014). The continuous 600s of data were then epoched into 200ms blocks/epochs (the 145

    average scale of individual state transitions (Baker et al., 2014) and each 200ms data epoch allocated 146

    to a new dataset according to the state that was most dominant during that time period. This 147

    resulted in 8 new datasets each with a collection of epochs that corresponded to a distinct network 148

    state as identified by the HMM. The four most dominant states for each individual subject (as 149

    measured by most time spent in that state) was taken forward for further analysis and spatial 150

    estimates averaged across those four inversions for each subject. As the HMM was performed on 151

    individual, rather than group concatenated data, state numbers did not directly correspond across 152

    subjects. This however permits superior partitioning within subjects since it allows the model to 153

    optimally fit states to the individual subjects, rather than fitting individual data to group states that 154

    are common across all subjects. This resulted in 815 ± 56.9 data segments per partitioned dataset - 155

    equivalent to 163s of continuous data. Since the HMM selected periods of self-similarity within the 156

    resting state, partial correlation maps were examined for all subjects to check that segregation into 157

    separate networks had occurred and this segregation did not relate to eye blinks, muscle artefacts or 158

    cardiac interference (Figure 1B). 159

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 7

    160

    161

    Figure 1. MEG data and Hidden Markov Model network allocation. A. Top panels show a selection of MEG 162 sensor time series for head-cast subject 1 (representative and chosen at random). Middle panel demonstrates 163 the statepath as determined by the HMM, showing rapid transitions between different states. Timecourses 164 recorded from a random subset of sensors is shown. Bottom panel shows a short (2s) period of data, expanded 165 with the corresponding section of the statepath (black line). Overlying the statepath trace (black) is the modal 166 statepath for each 200ms period (dash red line) and this modal statepath trace was used to sort each 200ms 167 data epoch into to 1 of 8 states according to which state was most frequent during that period. These epochs 168 of data for each of these states were aggregated together to form 8 new datasets. B. The statepath traces for 169 the three most dominant states here (1, 7, 5) are shown correlated against the time series amplitudes in all 170 sensors to derive a map of activity (red, greater positive correlation, blue, less positive correlation) associated 171 with each particular state. Data are shown in sensor space in order to check that differential network 172 parcellation had indeed occurred according to the statepath detection method and was not artefactual (e.g. 173

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 8

    that the topography resembles that expected for an eye blink). In the example shown here – state 5 is the 174 most common state – corresponding to the posterior alpha rhythm. 175

    176

    Subject specific cortical mesh libraries 177

    To extract the cortical pial mesh surface, Freesurfer software was used, optimised for MPM scans 178

    (anisotropic Freesurfer filter and a hard white matter threshold with no normalisation) (Lutti et al., 179

    2014). Then, for each individual subject, the pial cortical mesh was taken and deformed using a 3D 180

    weighted Fourier analysis that effectively decomposes the original 3D mesh structure into spatial 181

    harmonic components (Chung et al., 2007). These are then sequentially combined to form a set of 182

    meshes of progressively increasing spatial detail (Figure 2B) (Stevenson et al., 2014). These meshes 183

    are called the Weighted Fourier Series (WFS) and result in a library of meshes for each subjects with 184

    different levels of spatial detail, from a completely smooth pair of ovoid surfaces (mesh 1) up to a 185

    mesh similar to that of the real brain (mesh 50) (see Figure 2B). The WFS can be expressed as 186

    follows: 187

    188

    where σ is the bandwidth of the smoothing kernel (set at 0.0001), L is the harmonic order of the 189

    surface, Slm is the spherical harmonic of degree l and order m, and the Fourier coefficients are given 190

    by flm=〈f, Slm〉, where f is determined by solving a system of linear equations (Chung et al., 2007). All 191

    meshes, including the true mesh, were downsampled by a factor of 10 in Freesurfer to ~ 33,000 192

    vertices to aid computational efficiency. 193

    194

    Source reconstruction 195

    HMM parcellated datasets were projected from 272 spatial (3 channels damaged) and 16 temporal 196

    modes and inverted sequentially using our library of individualised and distorted cortical meshes 197

    (WFS) for each subject. Notably, using a cortical mesh further constrains solutions to those which are 198

    located at the cortical surface. We firstly implemented an Empirical Bayesian Beamformer (EBB) 199

    inversion. Beamforming makes a direct estimate of source covariance based on the assumption that 200

    there are no zero-lag correlated sources, a form of spatial filtering that ensures that no two parts of 201

    the brain has exactly the same neuroelectric activity at any given time. This was compared with 202

    alternative inversion schemes that are based on different prior assumptions regarding source co-203

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 9

    variance. Here we used standard (and non-optimized) versions of Minimum norm (MMN) (Uutela et 204

    al., 1999), LORETA (LOR) (Pascual-Marqui et al., 2002), Empirical Bayes Beamformer (EBB) and 205

    Multiple Sparse priors (MSP) (Friston et al., 2008) inversion schemes as implemented in SPM12. Each 206

    of these methods can be described by a different selection of source covariance matrices 207

    (Belardinelli et al., 2012; Friston et al., 2008; López et al., 2014). 208

    In order to quantify the quality of the fit we used two complementary metrics: cross validation and 209

    Free energy. For cross validation error, a subset of sensors (10%; 27 sensors) are turned off. An 210

    estimate of current flow is made based on the remaining 90% of the MEG channels (and a specific 211

    inversion scheme). This current flow is then projected back outside of the head to the 10% of 212

    disabled sensors and the error between the predicted and measured data calculated. This was 213

    repeated 10 times for each inversion with different randomly selected subsets of removed sensors 214

    for each iteration. The cross validation error was then converted to a percentage of source data 215

    explained and averaged across all iterations and across the 4 network datasets inverted per subject. 216

    The Free Energy metric approximates the log model evidence for the final generative model based 217

    on all of the sensor data by rewarding the accuracy of fit whilst also penalizing model complexity 218

    (Friston et al., 2007). 219

    Both cross validation error explained and Free Energy provide metrics which can be used to directly 220

    compare different models of the same data with increasing values of each suggesting an improved 221

    model fit (Henson et al., 2009). Whilst the Free Energy provides a useful relative model fit metric for 222

    any given dataset the absolute value is data dependent and therefore cannot be used to compare 223

    between datasets. In contrast, the cross validation error explained gives a meaningful quantification 224

    of the total amount of data explained and can be used to compare across the different groups (head-225

    cast versus non head-cast). 226

    Here we performed these inversions using our subject specific libraries of anatomically degraded 227

    meshes from the WFS with different levels of distortion and compared them to an inversion 228

    performed using the real mesh for each subject. In order to facilitate comparisons of how the 229

    anatomical distortion affected the inversions for different subjects with different baseline measures 230

    of model fit to their real brain meshes, we normalised the cross validation percentage error 231

    explained and free energy metrics by subtracting the values for the real mesh to derive a relative 232

    measure (ΔCV & ΔF). As such, a worse fit gives a negative value of ΔCV / ΔF and we would predict 233

    that as the anatomical complexity of the mesh increases (mesh is less distorted) and approaches that 234

    of the real mesh – the quality of the model should improve and the ΔCV / ΔF should approach zero. 235

    Across our group of subjects we determined the level of distortion at which this first becomes 236

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 10

    statistically distinguishable from the real mesh using a t-test of ΔCV values for each harmonic 237

    compared to zero and similarly for ΔF using Bayesian model comparison. This point, labelled the 238

    highest distinguishable harmonic (HDH), identifies the minimum amount of mesh distortion that can 239

    be reliably distinguished from the real mesh by inversion and can be converted into a conventional 240

    spatial metric (mms) by comparing vertex distances. The three dimensional (Euclidean) distance (in 241

    mm) between every vertex from the HDH to the corresponding vertex on the true mesh was 242

    therefore calculated and the upper, 95th percentile, distance averaged over all vertices in the mesh. 243

    This method is more conservative than that previously employed (Stevenson et al., 2014), as this 244

    directly matches corresponding vertices and therefore reduces the underestimation that could result 245

    if, following harmonic distortion, a vertex now lies closer to a non-corresponding other vertex. This 246

    distance therefore represents an upper bound on the spatial discriminability of both head-cast and 247

    non-head-cast resting state data. 248

    249

    Control analyses 250

    In order to verify our findings, we performed a number of control analyses with distorted 251

    data/sensors positions for the head-cast / EBB inversion. Firstly, we used our same data but 252

    destroyed its correspondence with the MEG sensor locations by randomly shuffling the MEG 253

    channels labels and repeated the analysis above 10 times for each subject and averaged over the 254

    ΔCV and ΔF for these different shuffled dataset inversions. Following this, we used the correct 255

    sensor labels but next degraded our data by introducing different amounts of scaled white noise to 256

    change the signal-to-noise ratio of the sensor level data (5 dB TO - 20 dB) (Troebinger et al., 2014a). 257

    In both cases (sensor shuffling and noise addition) one would expect the ability to discriminate the 258

    true generative model from distorted ones to decrease. 259

    260

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 11

    RESULTS 261

    Anatomical cortical model 262

    In order to determine the sensitivity of the inversion to the level of detail in the underlying 263

    anatomical mesh model we calculated the ΔCV and ΔF for each subject in the head-cast dataset 264

    across their subject specific library of distorted meshes (Fig. 2). This showed increasing cross 265

    validation sensor data explained (ΔCV; reduced error) and increasing Free energy (ΔF) for all 266

    subjects. Statistical group level testing revealed that meshes lower (more deformed) than the 35 267

    harmonic could be distinguished from the real mesh by ΔCV (HDH 35; t11= -2.49, p=0.03) and lower 268

    than 31 by ΔF (BMC, exceedance p = 0.046; Fig. 2) 269

    270

    271

    Figure 2. Relative Cross Validation and Free Energy results for a library of different meshes for head-cast 272 resting data using an EBB inversion. A. Increasing cross validation data explained (ΔCV) and relative Free 273 Energy ΔF with improving spatial resolution of harmonic meshes (from left right). Top plots show individual 274 subject ΔCV (left) and ΔF (right) values with superimposed mean value (dashed black line). Lower panels show 275 the group level statistical significance using t test (ΔCV) and Bayesian model comparison (ΔF). B. Example 276

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 12

    selection of meshes from 1 subject showing different levels of distortion, from smooth ovoid surfaces (WFS 277 mesh 1) up to real mesh (shown inset). 278

    279

    Inversion algorithms / source covariance priors 280

    The effect of source co-variance prior assumptions was then assessed by repeating the process for 281

    three other commonly implemented inversion algorithms (MNM, LOR, MSP) on our head-cast 282

    dataset. These showed lower anatomical mesh discriminability for all alternative algorithms with an 283

    HDH for MNM of 25 (t11=-1.80 -, p=0.036 ), an HDH of 25 (t11=-1.79; p=0.039) for LOR and an HDH of 284

    17 for MSP (t11=-2.55; p=0.027) (Fig 3A). 285

    The mean distance between vertices on these meshes and the real mesh was then calculated for 286

    each subject. These were then averaged to give a spatial measure of anatomical discriminability (Fig 287

    3A), under the different prior covariance assumptions as implemented in the different inversion 288

    algorithms. This ranged from 3.7 mm for EBB to 6.0 mm for MNN/LOR and 9.4 mm for MSP (Fig 3B). 289

    Directly comparing the absolute cross validation error explained (CVp) across inversion conditions 290

    (using the real mesh) demonstrated a significant difference between EBB and the other 3 algorithms 291

    (EBB/MMN; paired t-test – t11=7.6 ,p

  • 13

    MMN and LOR have very similar values and therefore lines are closely overlapping). B. Mean 300 distance of vertices from highest significant mesh identified in the left hand panel and the real mesh 301 for each subject. Error bars show SEM of this distance across subjects using their individualised 302 meshes and group level HDH. 303

    Notably – a 2 factor within subject ANOVA of cross validation with factors – inversion type (EBB, 304

    MMN, MSP) and mesh smootheness (harmonic 1 & harmonic 50) showed a strongly significant 305

    interaction between inversion type and mesh harmonic level (F2=29.9, p

  • 14

    Figure 4. Comparison of head-cast versus conventional MEG Cross validation results. A. 321 Comparison of ΔCV for different recording methodologies with head-cast data (blue) showing higher 322 discrimination (35) than non-head-cast data (red; 29). B. Absolute cross validation error explained is 323 also higher in the head-cast versus the conventional MEG. Note that this holds for all inversion types 324 and for all levels of mesh distortion, but was not significantly different on statistical testing. 325

    326

    Control analyses 327

    Finally, we checked our analyses by repeating our inversions (Head-cast dataset, EBB algorithm) but 328

    only after degrading the consistent relationship between our sensor positions and sources by 329

    shuffling the sensor labels. As expected, this resulted in a breakdown of the previously shown 330

    relationship between Cross validation and Free Energy with mesh distortion. Notably we found that 331

    lower harmonics (smoother) meshes now showed higher ΔCV (Fig 5A) (smoother surfaces superior 332

    when sensors shuffled). Thereafter we degraded our data (without sensor shuffling) by the addition 333

    of varying levels of Gaussian white noise (5 -50 dB) and again repeated our analysis (Figure 4B). 334

    This demonstrated that with increasing levels of noise (decreasing SNR), the curve describing the 335

    relationship between Cross validation and harmonic mesh function flattened and the crossing point 336

    (HDH) reduced, indicating that the inversion was no longer able to statistically distinguish the more 337

    complex meshes from the real mesh, as would be expected if the data are primarily noise. 338

    339

    340

    341

    342

    343

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 15

    Figure 5. Effects of shuffling sensors (A) and replacing data with varying levels of Gaussian White noise (B). 344 A. The relationship between the Cross validation % error explained (ΔCV) of inversion and increasing harmonic 345 mesh after random shuffling of sensor labels. Note the decreasing Cross validation fit with increasing mesh 346 harmonic (increasing mesh detail) with the dashed line showing the average across all subjects. B. The 347 relationship between (mean of all 12 subjects) ΔCV of inversion and increasing harmonic mesh after addition 348 of Gaussian white noise across a range of noise additions (from SNR 5 to SNR -20). 349

    350

    351

    Discussion 352

    Resting state data is rapidly dynamic, emulates task induced network changes and is simple to 353

    acquire (Baker et al., 2014; O’Neill et al., 2017). Here we show that it can provide a ready substrate 354

    for principled testing of MEG recording methods and inversion assumptions including anatomical 355

    forward modelling and functional (co-variance) priors. 356

    We showed that in moving the cortical surface from heavily distorted to the true anatomy, all of the 357

    models, based on commonly used imaging assumptions, showed a significant and monotonic 358

    improvement. This improvement saturated for some imaging assumptions before others, with the 359

    beamformer based algorithms (closely followed by Minimum norm) continuing to improve up until 360

    the cortical surface deviated by on average 4mm from the ground-truth. We also found that a 361

    marginal, although non-significant, improvement in our models when using MEG data based on 362

    head-cast recordings compared to conventional recordings. Critically rather than compare methods 363

    through simulation or through a limited task set (with ground truth from another modality) we have 364

    presented a method to optimize MEG recording methods, forward and inverse models without 365

    introducing selection bias and based on a plentiful supply of non-invasive human data. 366

    We compared between algorithms in two ways: by the model fit (or amount of data predicted), and 367

    by comparing the sensitivity of each algorithm to the true anatomy. These two tests need not 368

    necessarily have been in accord. For example- had we used a bunny-shaped blancmange mould 369

    instead of a cortical surface we would still have been able to rank the algorithms based the amount 370

    of data predicted; but we would not have expected any monotonic improvement as features were 371

    added to the bunny. A related control analysis (figure 5) is that when used the same data but with 372

    shuffled lead-fields (destroying the link between the sensors and the anatomy) the amount of data 373

    we are able to predict actually decreases as the cortical model approaches the truth. It is therefore 374

    striking that the models that benefitted most from the true cortical manifold were also those that 375

    predicted the most data. This not only adds anatomical validity (confirming that the data being 376

    described is indeed generated by pyramidal cell populations normal to the cortical surface) but also 377

    allows us to quantify algorithm performance in millimetres (Stevenson et al., 2014). 378

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 16

    Across anatomy (Fig. 2) and inversion assumptions (Fig. 3), the parametric (Free energy from 379

    empirical Bayes) and non-parametric (cross-validation) metrics of model fit were in accord. This 380

    helps build confidence in the parametric free-energy metric which is considerably faster, makes use 381

    of all the available data, and has a direct probabilistic interpretation. The Bayesian formalism is 382

    however predicated on comparing how different models explain the same data; the use of cross-383

    validation, which provides an absolute quantitative measure of data predicted, also allowed us to 384

    compare between different datasets (head-cast and non-head-cast). 385

    Resting state MEG analysis has been impeded by the spectral/dimensional complexity of the 386

    datasets (Vidaurre et al., 2016) as well as reduced spatial resolution that limits the ability to 387

    discriminate different sources (Colclough et al., 2016; Liuzzi et al., 2016). Recent improvements in 388

    methods have permitted enhanced temporal discrimination (Baker et al., 2014; Woolrich et al., 389

    2013). These and other development have resulted in the emergence of a number of potential 390

    clinical applications for resting state MEG, although these have yet to transition to clinical utilisation, 391

    in part due to reduced spatial resolution (Bosboom et al., 2006; Bosma et al., 2009; Hinkley et al., 392

    2011). It is notable that most of the empirical MEG literature on the resting state is dominated by 393

    what we found here to be the most likely functional priors (beamformer and Minimum norm 394

    assumptions). 395

    We confirm here earlier reports that within-session head movements are greatly reduced for head-396

    cast versus non-head-cast MEG (Bonaiuto et al., 2017b; Meyer et al., 2017a, 2017b) however we 397

    were surprised that the head-cast did not offer a greater modelling improvement over the non-398

    head-cast data. Empirically this could be due to recording problems- for example in some subjects it 399

    is possible that their heads were not within the head-casts in their expected position. i.e. although 400

    the head-casts remained still the subject’s anatomy was not where we expected it to be. Another 401

    limitation could be that the models we are using do not capture the physics or physiology of the 402

    generators of the measured magnetic fields; and that the resolution is constrained by the models 403

    and not the recording. This could include for example, unmodelled noise sources such as the 404

    heartbeat, eye-blinks and other sources of noise. Although the HMM states we used were visually 405

    inspected to avoid common artefacts such as eye-blinks, it is possible that some of the modelling 406

    deficiencies come from failure to explain data that does not arise from the cortex (eg heart-beats, 407

    passing cars etc). Finally, the head-cast and non-head-cast cohorts did not overlap and a more 408

    sensitive analysis would have been to perform a within subject comparison. 409

    We found the Multiple Sparse Priors algorithm had the least dependence on the true anatomy and 410

    also explained the least data. We should note however that the MSP-based analyses implemented 411

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 17

    here were generic and constructed from a limited set of 512 patches (or priors) placed at evenly 412

    spaced vertices. The MSP algorithm, although perhaps the most elegant and comprehensive method 413

    we tested, is also computationally disadvantaged by the need to search over a large space of 414

    possible patch/prior combinations and the inherent pitfalls of local extrema in this optimization. A 415

    more robust way to implement this algorithm would have been to select the best model from many 416

    random patch choices (Troebinger et al., 2014b). 417

    This study was analysed using broadband (1-90 Hz) resting state data. Therefore, whether a similar 418

    level of spatial discriminability at the mm scale can be demonstrated when more selective data is 419

    used (e.g. frequency filtered or spatially restricted) remains to be shown. For example, future work 420

    might test different frequency bands (eg 30Hz) against different anatomy for example 421

    infra/supra granular cortical surfaces (Arnal and Giraud, 2012; Bastos et al., 2012; Bonaiuto et al., 422

    2017a, 2017b). 423

    Conclusion 424

    This study uses resting state data to compare different forward models, inversion assumptions and 425

    recording methods to provide a principled (non – biased) method for optimising and quantifying 426

    source localisation. All source localisation techniques were able to benefit from increasing 427

    anatomical precision in the underlying model but this was most pronounced for EBB in head-cast 428

    recorded data. Using this method, we demonstrate a notably high sensitivity (

  • 18

    REFERENCES 432

    Arnal, L.H., Giraud, A.-L., 2012. Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 433

    390–8. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.003 434

    Baillet, S., 2015. Forward and Inverse Problems of MEG/EEG, in: Encyclopedia of Computational 435

    Neuroscience. Springer, New York. 436

    Baillet, S., Mosher, J.C., Leahy, R.M., 2001. Electromagnetic brain mapping. IEEE Signal Process. Mag. 437

    18, 14–30. doi:10.1109/79.962275 438

    Baker, A.P., Brookes, M.J., Rezek, I. a., Smith, S.M., Behrens, T., Smith, P.J.P., Woolrich, M., 2014. 439

    Fast transient networks in spontaneous human brain activity. Elife 2014, 1–18. 440

    doi:10.7554/eLife.01867 441

    Barttfeld, P., Uhrig, L., Sitt, J.D., Sigman, M., Jarraya, B., Dehaene, S., 2015. Signature of 442

    consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, 443

    887–92. doi:10.1073/pnas.1418031112 444

    Bastos, A.M., Usrey, W.M., Adams, R.A., Mangun, G.R., Fries, P., Friston, K.J., 2012. Canonical 445

    Microcircuits for Predictive Coding. Neuron. 446

    Belardinelli, P., Ortiz, E., Barnes, G.R., Noppeney, U., Preissl, H., 2012. Source reconstruction 447

    accuracy of MEG and EEG Bayesian inversion approaches. PLoS One 7, e51985. 448

    doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051985 449

    Bonaiuto, J.J., Meyer, S.S., Little, S., Rossiter, H., Callaghan, M.F., Dick, F., 2017a. Laminar-specific 450

    cortical dynamics in human visual and sensorimotor cortices 1–25. doi:10.1101/226274 451

    Bonaiuto, J.J., Rossiter, H.E., Meyer, S.S., Adams, N., Little, S., Callaghan, M.F., Dick, F., Bestmann, S., 452

    Barnes, G.R., 2018. Non-invasive laminar inference with MEG: Comparison of methods and 453

    source inversion algorithms. Neuroimage 167. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.068 454

    Bonaiuto, J.J., Rossiter, H.E., Meyer, S.S., Adams, N., Little, S., Callaghan, M.F., Dick, F., Bestmann, S., 455

    Barnes, G.R., 2017b. Non-invasive laminar inference with MEG: Comparison of methods and 456

    source inversion algorithms. bioRxiv. 457

    Bosboom, J.L.W., Stoffers, D., Stam, C.J., van Dijk, B.W., Verbunt, J., Berendse, H.W., Wolters, E.C., 458

    2006. Resting state oscillatory brain dynamics in Parkinson’s disease: an MEG study. Clin. 459

    Neurophysiol. 117, 2521–31. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2006.06.720 460

    Bosma, I., Reijneveld, J.C., Klein, M., Douw, L., van Dijk, B.W., Heimans, J.J., Stam, C.J., 2009. 461

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 19

    Disturbed functional brain networks and neurocognitive function in low-grade glioma patients: 462

    a graph theoretical analysis of resting-state MEG. Nonlinear Biomed. Phys. 3, 9. 463

    doi:10.1186/1753-4631-3-9 464

    Chung, M.K., Dalton, K.M., Shen, L., Evans, A.C., Davidson, R.J., 2007. Weighted Fourier series 465

    representation and its application to quantifying the amount of gray matter. IEEE Trans. Med. 466

    Imaging 26, 566–581. doi:10.1109/TMI.2007.892519 467

    Colclough, G.L.L., Woolrich, M., Tewarie, P.K.K., Brookes, M.J.J., Quinn, A.J.J., Smith, S.M.M., 2016. 468

    How reliable are MEG resting-state connectivity metrics? Neuroimage 138, 284–293. 469

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.070 470

    Friston, K., Harrison, L., Daunizeau, J., Kiebel, S., Phillips, C., Trujillo-Barreto, N., Henson, R., Flandin, 471

    G., Mattout, J., 2008. Multiple sparse priors for the M/EEG inverse problem. Neuroimage 39, 472

    1104–1120. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.048 473

    Friston, K., Mattout, J., Trujillo-Barreto, N., Ashburner, J., Penny, W., 2007. Variational free energy 474

    and the Laplace approximation. Neuroimage 34, 220–234. 475

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.035 476

    Henson, R.N., Mattout, J., Phillips, C., Friston, K.J., 2009. Selecting forward models for MEG source-477

    reconstruction using model-evidence. Neuroimage 46, 168–176. 478

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.062 479

    Hillebrand, A., Barnes, G.R., 2011. Practical constraints on estimation of source extent with MEG 480

    beamformers. Neuroimage 54, 2732–2740. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.036 481

    Hinkley, L.B.N., Vinogradov, S., Guggisberg, A.G., Fisher, M., Findlay, A.M., Nagarajan, S.S., 2011. 482

    Clinical symptoms and alpha band resting-state functional connectivity imaging in patients with 483

    schizophrenia: Implications for novel approaches to treatment. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 1134–1142. 484

    doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.029 485

    Kaiser, R.H., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Wager, T.D., Pizzagalli, D.A., RJ, D., J, J., 2015. Large-Scale Network 486

    Dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 603. 487

    doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0071 488

    Koenig, T., Prichep, L., Lehmann, D., Sosa, P.V., Braeker, E., Kleinlogel, H., Isenhart, R., John, E.R., 489

    2002. Millisecond by Millisecond, Year by Year: Normative EEG Microstates and Developmental 490

    Stages. Neuroimage 16, 41–48. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1070 491

    Lewis, C.M., Baldassarre, A., Committeri, G., Romani, G.L., Corbetta, M., 2009. Learning sculpts the 492

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 20

    spontaneous activity of the resting human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 106, 17558–63. 493

    doi:10.1073/pnas.0902455106 494

    Li, P., Li, S., Dong, Z., Luo, J., Han, H., Xiong, H., Guo, Z., Li, Z., 2012. Altered resting state functional 495

    connectivity patterns of the anterior prefrontal cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. 496

    Neuroreport 23, 681–686. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328355a5fe 497

    Liuzzi, L., Gascoyne, L.E., Tewarie, P.K., Barratt, E.L., Boto, E., Brookes, M.J., 2016. Optimising 498

    experimental design for MEG resting state functional connectivity measurement. Neuroimage 499

    In Press. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.064 500

    López, J.D., Litvak, V., Espinosa, J.J., Friston, K., Barnes, G.R., 2014. Algorithmic procedures for 501

    Bayesian MEG/EEG source reconstruction in SPM. Neuroimage 84, 476–487. 502

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.002 503

    López, J.D., Penny, W.D., Espinosa, J.J., Barnes, G.R., 2012. A general Bayesian treatment for MEG 504

    source reconstruction incorporating lead field uncertainty. Neuroimage 60, 1194–1204. 505

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.077 506

    López, J.D., Valencia, F., Flandin, G., Penny, W., Barnes, G.R., 2017. Reconstructing anatomy from 507

    electro-physiological data. Neuroimage 163, 480–486. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.049 508

    Lutti, A., Dick, F., Sereno, M.I., Weiskopf, N., 2014. Using high-resolution quantitative mapping of R1 509

    as an index of cortical myelination. Neuroimage 93, 176–188. 510

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.005 511

    Meyer, S.S., Bonaiuto, J.J., Lim, M., Rossiter, H., Waters, S., Bradbury, D., Bestmann, S., Brookes, M., 512

    Callaghan, M.F., Weiskopf, N., Barnes, G.R., 2017a. Flexible head-casts for high spatial precision 513

    MEG. J. Neurosci. Methods 276, 38–45. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.11.009 514

    Meyer, S.S., Rossiter, H., Brookes, M., Woolrich, M., Bestmann, S., Barnes, G.R., 2017b. Using 515

    generative models to make probabilistic statements about hippocampal engagement in MEG. 516

    Neuroimage 149, 468–482. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.029 517

    O’Neill, G.C., Tewarie, P.K., Colclough, G.L., Gascoyne, L.E., Hunt, B.A.E., Morris, P.G., Woolrich, 518

    M.W., Brookes, M.J., 2017. Measurement of dynamic task related functional networks using 519

    MEG. Neuroimage 146, 667–678. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.061 520

    Pascual-Marqui, R.D., Esslen, M., Kochi, K., Lehmann, D., 2002. Functional imaging with low-521

    resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): a review. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. 522

    Pharmacol. 24 Suppl C, 91–5. 523

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 21

    Peterson, A., Thome, J., Frewen, P., Lanius, R.A., 2014. Resting-State Neuroimaging Studies: A New 524

    Way of Identifying Differences and Similarities among the Anxiety Disorders? Can. J. Psychiatry 525

    59, 294–300. doi:10.1177/070674371405900602 526

    Philippi, C.L., Pujara, M.S., Motzkin, J.C., Newman, J., Kiehl, K.A., Koenigs, M., 2015. Altered Resting-527

    State Functional Connectivity in Cortical Networks in Psychopathy. J. Neurosci. 35. 528

    Reineberg, A.E., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Depue, B.E., Friedman, N.P., Banich, M.T., 2015. Resting-state 529

    networks predict individual differences in common and specific aspects of executive function. 530

    Neuroimage 104, 69–78. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.045 531

    Sheline, Y.I., Raichle, M.E., 2013. Resting State Functional Connectivity in Preclinical Alzheimer’s 532

    Disease. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 340–347. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.028 533

    Stevenson, C., Brookes, M., López, J.D., Troebinger, L., Mattout, J., Penny, W., Morris, P., Hillebrand, 534

    A., Henson, R., Barnes, G.R., 2014. Does function fit structure? A ground truth for non-invasive 535

    neuroimaging. Neuroimage 94, 89–95. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.02.033 536

    Tessitore, A., Amboni, M., Esposito, F., Russo, A., Picillo, M., Marcuccio, L., Pellecchia, M.T., Vitale, C., 537

    Cirillo, M., Tedeschi, G., Barone, P., 2012. Resting-state brain connectivity in patients with 538

    Parkinson’s disease and freezing of gait. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 18, 781–787. 539

    doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.03.018 540

    Troebinger, L., Lopez, J., Lutti, A., Bestmann, S., Barnes, G.R., 2014a. Discrimination of cortical 541

    laminae using MEG. Neuroimage (in press). 542

    Troebinger, L., López, J.D., Lutti, A., Bradbury, D., Bestmann, S., Barnes, G.R., 2014b. High precision 543

    anatomy for MEG. Neuroimage 86, 583–91. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.065 544

    Uutela, K., Hämäläinen, M., Somersalo, E., 1999. Visualization of Magnetoencephalographic Data 545

    Using Minimum Current Estimates. Neuroimage 10, 173–180. doi:10.1006/nimg.1999.0454 546

    Venkataraman, A., Whitford, T.J., Westin, C.-F., Golland, P., Kubicki, M., 2012. Whole brain resting 547

    state functional connectivity abnormalities in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 139, 7–12. 548

    doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.04.021 549

    Vidaurre, D., Quinn, A.J., Baker, A.P., Dupret, D., Tejero-Cantero, A., Woolrich, M., 2016. Spectrally 550

    resolved fast transient brain states in electrophysiological data. Neuroimage 126, 81–95. 551

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.047 552

    Wackermann, J., Lehmann, D., Michel, C.M., Strik, W.K., 1993. Adaptive segmentation of 553

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252

  • 22

    spontaneous EEG map series into spatially defined microstates. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 14, 269–554

    283. doi:10.1016/0167-8760(93)90041-M 555

    Weiskopf, N., Suckling, J., Williams, G., Correia M., M.M., Inkster, B., Tait, R., Ooi, C., Bullmore T., 556

    E.T., Lutti, A., 2013. Quantitative multi-parameter mapping of R1, PD*, MT, and R2* at 3T: A 557

    multi-center validation. Front. Neurosci. 7, 1–11. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00095 558

    Wipf, D., Nagarajan, S., 2009. A unified Bayesian framework for MEG/EEG source imaging. 559

    Neuroimage 44, 947–966. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.059 560

    Woolrich, M., Baker, A.P., Luckhoo, H., Mohseni, H., Barnes, G.R., Brookes, M., Rezek, L., 2013. 561

    Dynamic state allocation for MEG source reconstruction. Neuroimage 77, 77–92. 562

    doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.036 563

    Wu, J., Srinivasan, R., Kaur, A., Cramer, S.C., 2014. Resting-state cortical connectivity predicts motor 564

    skill acquisition. Neuroimage 91, 84–90. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.026 565

    Wurina, Zang, Y.-F., Zhao, S.-G., 2012. Resting-state fMRI studies in epilepsy. Neurosci. Bull. 28, 449–566

    455. doi:10.1007/s12264-012-1255-1 567

    568

    was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted January 15, 2018. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/248252doi: bioRxiv preprint

    https://doi.org/10.1101/248252