racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in...

17
Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in adolescence LILLYBELLE K. DEER, a GRANT S. SHIELDS, a SUSANNAH L. IVORY, b CAMELIA E. HOSTINAR, a AND EVA H. TELZER b a University of California, Davis; and b University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill Abstract Racial/ethnic minorities are more vulnerable to mental and physical health problems, but we know little about the psychobiological underpinnings of these disparities. In this study, we examined racial/ethnic differences in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect as initial stepstoward elucidating long-term health disparities. A racially/ethnically diverse (39.5% White, 60.5% minority) sample of 370 adolescents (57.3% female) between the ages of 11.9 and 18 years (M ¼ 14.65 years, SD ¼ 1.39) participated in this study. These adolescents provided 16 cortisol samples (4 samples per dayacross 4 days), allowing the computation of diurnal cortisol slopes, the cortisol awakening response, and diurnal cortisol output (area under the curve), as well as daily diary ratings of high- arousal and low-arousal positive and negative affect. Consistent with prior research, we found that racial/ethnic minorities (particularly African American and Latino youth) exhibited flatter diurnal cortisol slopes compared to White youth, F (1, 344.7) ¼ 5.26, p ¼ .02, effect size g ¼ 0.25. Furthermore, African American and Asian American youth reported lower levels of positive affect (both high arousal and low arousal) compared to White youth. Racial/ethnic differences in affect did not explain differences in cortisol patterns, suggesting a need to refine our models of relations between affect and hypothalamic– pituitary–adrenocortical activity. We conclude by proposing that a deeper understanding of cultural development may help elucidate the complex associations between affect and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical functioning and how theyexplain racial/ethnic differences in both affect and stress biology. American youth belonging to racial/ethnic minority groups (e.g., African Americans, Latinos, and Asian Americans; henceforth, minorities) are more vulnerable to mental and physical health problems (Alegria, Vallas, & Pumariega, 2010; Braveman, Cubbin, Egerter, Williams, & Pamuk, 2010; Merikangas et al., 2010), including mood and anxiety disorders (Merikangas et al., 2010), chronic fatigue (Dinos et al., 2009), and obesity (Ogden et al., 2006). These health disparities are potentially the result of differential exposure to stressful life experiences, which in turn result in differences in the daily patterning of affective, cognitive, and biological processes that accumulate over time to foster psychopathol- ogy or physical illness (DeSantis et al., 2007; Levy, Heissel, Richeson, & Adam, 2016; Mays, Cochran, & Barnes, 2007; Shields, Moons, & Slavich, 2017; Susman, 2007). These physical and mental health disparities can be thought of as the products of developmental cascades, defined as the cumu- lative outcomes of interactions within and among developing systems that can spread effects across domains of functioning and across generations (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). The field of cultural development and psychopathology has begun ex- amining how cultural processes within the individual and within society can act within developmental cascades and set the stage for normal or abnormal behavior, risk, or resili- ence (for review, see Causadias, 2013). However, there is a scarcity of research testing the links between culturally bound daily psychological experiences and biological processes that may help explain racial inequalities in long-term health out- comes. The current study aimed to address this gap by exam- ining associations between racial/ethnic 1 a differences in daily affective states and diurnal variation in the functioning of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis, one of the body’s primary stress-response systems. We focus on the HPA axis given evidence linking HPA dysregulation with concurrent and future symptoms of numerous mental and physical health conditions across the life span (Adam Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Eva H. Telzer, Davie Hall Psychology Building, 235 E. Cameron Avenue, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3270; E-mail: [email protected]; or Camelia E. Hostinar, University of California-Davis, 202 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA 95618; E-mail: cehosti- [email protected]. This manuscript was prepared with support from the Center for Poverty Re- search at University of California, Davis and National Science Foundation Grant 1327768 (to L.K.D. and C.E.H.), the National Institutes of Health Grant R01DA039923 and National Science Foundation Grant SES 1459719 (to E.H.T.), the Department of Psychology at the University of Illi- nois, and the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. We greatly appreciate Lynda Lin and Mi- chelle Miernicki for assistance with collecting and preparing the data. 1. We consider race and ethnicity to be sociocultural categories based on physical appearance or cultural affiliation that people self-identify with (e.g., see definitions by Garcı ´a Coll et al., 1996). As both categories reflect socially constructed groups, we do not differentiate one from the other in this manuscript and instead use race/ethnicity to refer to self-identified be- longing to a social group. Development and Psychopathology, 2018, page 1 of 17 # Cambridge University Press 2018 doi:10.1017/S0954579418001098 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 1

Upload: others

Post on 08-Aug-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affectin adolescence

LILLYBELLE K. DEER,a GRANT S. SHIELDS,a SUSANNAH L. IVORY,b CAMELIA E. HOSTINAR,a AND

EVA H. TELZERb

aUniversity of California, Davis; and bUniversity of North Carolina, Chapel Hill

Abstract

Racial/ethnic minorities are more vulnerable to mental and physical health problems, but we know little about the psychobiological underpinnings of thesedisparities. In this study, we examined racial/ethnic differences in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect as initial steps toward elucidating long-term healthdisparities. A racially/ethnically diverse (39.5% White, 60.5% minority) sample of 370 adolescents (57.3% female) between the ages of 11.9 and 18 years(M ¼ 14.65 years, SD ¼ 1.39) participated in this study. These adolescents provided 16 cortisol samples (4 samples per day across 4 days), allowing thecomputation of diurnal cortisol slopes, the cortisol awakening response, and diurnal cortisol output (area under the curve), as well as daily diary ratings of high-arousal and low-arousal positive and negative affect. Consistent with prior research, we found that racial/ethnic minorities (particularly African American andLatino youth) exhibited flatter diurnal cortisol slopes compared to White youth, F (1, 344.7) ¼ 5.26, p ¼ .02, effect size g ¼ 0.25. Furthermore, AfricanAmerican and Asian American youth reported lower levels of positive affect (both high arousal and low arousal) compared to White youth. Racial/ethnicdifferences in affect did not explain differences in cortisol patterns, suggesting a need to refine our models of relations between affect and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical activity. We conclude by proposing that a deeper understanding of cultural development may help elucidate the complex associationsbetween affect and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical functioning and how they explain racial/ethnic differences in both affect and stress biology.

American youth belonging to racial/ethnic minority groups(e.g., African Americans, Latinos, and Asian Americans;henceforth, minorities) are more vulnerable to mental andphysical health problems (Alegria, Vallas, & Pumariega,2010; Braveman, Cubbin, Egerter, Williams, & Pamuk,2010; Merikangas et al., 2010), including mood and anxietydisorders (Merikangas et al., 2010), chronic fatigue (Dinoset al., 2009), and obesity (Ogden et al., 2006). These healthdisparities are potentially the result of differential exposureto stressful life experiences, which in turn result in differencesin the daily patterning of affective, cognitive, and biologicalprocesses that accumulate over time to foster psychopathol-ogy or physical illness (DeSantis et al., 2007; Levy, Heissel,Richeson, & Adam, 2016; Mays, Cochran, & Barnes, 2007;Shields, Moons, & Slavich, 2017; Susman, 2007). These

physical and mental health disparities can be thought of asthe products of developmental cascades, defined as the cumu-lative outcomes of interactions within and among developingsystems that can spread effects across domains of functioningand across generations (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). The fieldof cultural development and psychopathology has begun ex-amining how cultural processes within the individual andwithin society can act within developmental cascades andset the stage for normal or abnormal behavior, risk, or resili-ence (for review, see Causadias, 2013). However, there is ascarcity of research testing the links between culturally bounddaily psychological experiences and biological processes thatmay help explain racial inequalities in long-term health out-comes. The current study aimed to address this gap by exam-ining associations between racial/ethnic1a differences in dailyaffective states and diurnal variation in the functioning of thehypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis, one ofthe body’s primary stress-response systems. We focus onthe HPA axis given evidence linking HPA dysregulationwith concurrent and future symptoms of numerous mentaland physical health conditions across the life span (Adam

Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Eva H. Telzer, DavieHall Psychology Building, 235 E. Cameron Avenue, Chapel Hill, NC27599-3270; E-mail: [email protected]; or Camelia E. Hostinar, Universityof California-Davis, 202 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA 95618; E-mail: [email protected].

This manuscript was prepared with support from the Center for Poverty Re-search at University of California, Davis and National Science FoundationGrant 1327768 (to L.K.D. and C.E.H.), the National Institutes of HealthGrant R01DA039923 and National Science Foundation Grant SES1459719 (to E.H.T.), the Department of Psychology at the University of Illi-nois, and the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience at the Universityof North Carolina at Chapel Hill. We greatly appreciate Lynda Lin and Mi-chelle Miernicki for assistance with collecting and preparing the data. 1. We consider race and ethnicity to be sociocultural categories based on

physical appearance or cultural affiliation that people self-identify with(e.g., see definitions by Garcıa Coll et al., 1996). As both categories reflectsocially constructed groups, we do not differentiate one from the other inthis manuscript and instead use race/ethnicity to refer to self-identified be-longing to a social group.

Development and Psychopathology, 2018, page 1 of 17# Cambridge University Press 2018doi:10.1017/S0954579418001098

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

1

Page 2: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

et al., 2017; Shields & Slavich, 2017). In adolescence, dysre-gulated cortisol patterns have been associated with greaterrisk of current and future psychopathology such as depressionand anxiety (Adam, 2006; Adam et al., 2010; Carnegie et al.,2014; Doane et al., 2013; Van den Bergh & Van Calster,2009), as well as higher odds of common physical health con-ditions such as obesity (Ruttle et al., 2013). We focused onaffective states and their daily association with cortisol be-cause there is increasing evidence that approximately 50%of variability in diurnal HPA functioning is state dependent,as opposed to showing traitlike continuity (Ross, Murphy,Adam, Chen, & Miller, 2013). Given the high responsivityof this system to daily fluctuations based on experience, itis important to understand its associations with daily subjec-tive perceptions of affective states to better understand howthe system may be calibrated by experience during develop-ment. Even though we do not examine health disparities di-rectly in this study, our goal is to begin elucidating ethnic/ra-cial differences in affective and biological processes that mayset the stage for long-term health disparities.

Brief Overview of the HPA Axis

When confronted with physical or psychological challengesthat tax or overwhelm the organism’s capacity to cope, thebody initiates a stress response consisting of physiologicaland behavioral responses mediated by the nervous, endo-crine, and immune systems (Smith & Vale, 2006). TheHPA axis plays an integral role in these processes by mobiliz-ing energy for coping with stressors and modifying the indi-vidual’s responses to similar stressors in the future (Gunnar,Doom, & Esposito, 2015; Sapolsky, Romero, & Munck,2000). The activity of the HPA axis can be characterizedalong two basic dimensions, basal functioning and reactivityto stressors (Spencer & Deak, 2016). Basal HPA functioningfollows a diurnal rhythm whereby cortisol, one of the mainproducts of the HPA axis, is secreted in a pulsatile fashionacross the day, reaching peak levels in the morning approxi-mately 30 min after awakening, and declining graduallyacross the day to reach minimum levels at night (Gunnaret al., 2015). Superimposed on this basal rhythm is the reac-tivity of the HPA axis to physical or psychological threats towell-being (i.e., stressors). The HPA axis is powerfully acti-vated by social threats (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004; Gunnar& Adam, 2012), as well as by physical threats more typicallystudied in nonhuman animals, such as immobilization (Smith& Vale, 2006).

There are three indices of basal HPA functioning that aremost commonly examined and that we focused on in thisstudy. The diurnal slope is a negative slope from morningto evening cortisol levels, and deviations from this typicalpattern such as flatter (i.e., less negative) slopes have beenlinked to deleterious emotional and physical health problems,particularly immune-related and metabolic outcomes (Adamet al., 2017). The cortisol awakening response is the rise incortisol production from wake-up to approximately 30 min

later and is thought to reflect distinct processes related to an-ticipated demands for energy mobilization for the day (Fries,Dettenborn, & Kirschbaum, 2009). General life stress and de-pression have been associated with an elevated cortisol awa-kening response, whereas posttraumatic stress, fatigue, burn-out, and exhaustion have been linked to an atypically lowcortisol awakening response (Boggero, Hostinar, Haak, Mur-phy, & Segerstrom, 2017; Chida & Steptoe, 2009). Finally,the area under the curve represents an integrated measureof total daily cortisol output, which is calculated based on re-peated measurements throughout the day and the spacing be-tween them (Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid, & Hell-hammer, 2003). Chronic stress has been associated withgreater total daily cortisol output, combined with a flatterslope; that is, this pattern results from lower than expectedmorning levels but higher afternoon and evening production(Miller, Chen, & Zhou, 2007).

Racial/Ethnic Differences in HPA Activity

The prior literature on racial/ethnic differences in HPA activ-ity is somewhat limited, but these differences in physiologyare important to explore as they may help us explain racial/ethnic health disparities. Some intriguing patterns have begunto emerge in studies with children (Bush, Obradovic, Adler,& Boyce, 2011; Martin, Bruce, & Fisher, 2012), adolescents(DeSantis et al., 2007; Hostinar, McQuillan, Mirous, Grant,& Adam, 2014; Zeiders, Causadias, & White, 2017), andadults (Bennett, Merritt, & Wolin, 2004; Cohen et al.,2006; Hajat et al., 2010; McCallum, Sorocco, & Fritsch,2006; Suglia et al., 2010). In particular, flatter cortisol slopeshave been observed in African American compared to Whiteyouth (DeSantis et al., 2007; Martin et al., 2012). Results aremore inconsistent when comparing other minorities (e.g., La-tino or multiracial youth) with White youth, with one studyfinding that Latino adolescents exhibited flatter slopes thannon-Latino White adolescents (DeSantis et al., 2007),whereas another did not detect a statistically significant dif-ference (Martin et al., 2012). In adults, studies have consis-tently found flatter slopes in both African American and La-tino compared to White adults (Cohen et al., 2006; Hajatet al., 2010; McCallum et al., 2006). In terms of daily cortisoloutput (area under the curve), ethnic minority kindergartenchildren tend to exhibit higher area under the curve comparedto White children (Bush et al., 2011), whereas Latino andAfrican American adults show lower area under the curvecompared to White adults (Hajat et al., 2010). Finally, forthe cortisol awakening response, African American adults ex-hibit a lower cortisol awakening response compared to Whiteadults, an effect that is even more pronounced in low- socio-economic status African Americans (Bennett et al., 2004).Overall, these patterns suggest that ethnic minorities tend toshow flatter diurnal cortisol slopes and lower cortisol awaken-ing response levels, with differences most consistently ob-served when comparing African Americans with Whitesand results being more mixed when comparing Latino with

L. K. Deer et al.2

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

121

122

123

124

125

126

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

136

137

138

139

140

141

142

143

144

145

146

147

148

149

150

151

152

153

154

155

156

157

158

159

160

161

162

163

164

165

166

167

168

169

170

171

172

173

174

175

176

177

178

179

180

181

182

183

184

185

186

187

188

189

190

191

192

193

194

195

196

197

198

199

200

201

202

203

204

205

206

207

208

209

210

211

212

213

214

215

216

217

218

219

220

221

222

223

224

Page 3: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

non-Latino Whites. Racial/ethnic differences in total cortisoloutput (area under the curve) have been more rarely investi-gated, as only two studies to date, one examining children(Bush et al., 2011) and one adults (Hajat et al., 2010), haveexplored these differences.

Current theory suggests that these ethnic/racial differencesare related to experiences of chronic stress in general andexperiences of discrimination more specifically, which aremore prevalent among African American and Latino groups(Lewis, Cogburn, & Williams, 2015). For instance, studieshave found that both recent and long-term exposure to per-ceived discrimination is associated with flatter cortisol slopes(Adam et al., 2015; Zeiders, Hoyt, & Adam, 2014) or greaterdaily cortisol output (Zeiders, Doane, & Roosa, 2012). How-ever, some studies have found that discrimination does notexplain the associations between race/ethnicity and diurnalcortisol (Cohen et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2012). A recentmeta-analysis of associations between racial discriminationand cortisol parameters sheds some light on these discrepantfindings (Korous, Causadias, & Casper, 2017). This meta-analysis reported that the average effect size for this associa-tion is small (r ¼ .04, though effects are slightly larger whenexperimental protocols are used, such as exposing partici-pants to instances of discrimination in the laboratory). Thesmall effect size suggests that more research is needed to ex-plore other factors that might further explain racial/ethnic dis-parities in cortisol output (Korous et al., 2017). Furthermore,researchers propose that specific experiences such as discri-mination may not fully or consistently account for racial/eth-nic differences in diurnal cortisol rhythms because they donot take into account individual differences in processingand coping with such experiences. Instead, considering par-ticipants’ affective states as aggregate measures of their re-sponses to multiple streams of experiences might be a moreproximal predictor of HPA patterns (Cohen et al., 2006).For this reason, the current study aimed to examine the extentto which racial/ethnic differences in affect may explain racial/ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol patterns.

Another possible explanation for racial/ethnic differencesin HPA activity is that they might be due to differences in so-cioeconomic status, which is closely intertwined with race/ethnicity in the United States. Previous studies have reportedassociations between socioeconomic status and cortisol pa-rameters, with minorities and low-socioeconomic status par-ticipants exhibiting similar profiles. However, this researchhas indicated that racial/ethnic differences in cortisol param-eters persist after accounting for socioeconomic status and in-dependently predict cortisol outcomes (Bush et al., 2011; Co-hen et al., 2006; DeSantis et al., 2007; Hajat et al., 2010;Martin et al., 2012). Moreover, ethnic minorities appear tobenefit less from high socioeconomic status compared toWhites (Bennett et al., 2004). Overall, these patterns suggestthat race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status may have uniqueimpacts on cortisol output. As such, the current study will ex-amine socioeconomic status as a covariate in the relationshipbetween race/ethnicity and cortisol parameters.

Associations Between HPA Functioning and Affect

Because the HPA axis reacts powerfully to social and physi-cal threats to well-being, most prior literature has focused onassociations between HPA activity and negative affect such asfear, sadness, or worry. For instance, one naturalistic study ofdiurnal cortisol rhythms in adolescents found that momentaryincreases in state negative affect (worry, stress, anger, or frus-tration) were significantly associated with higher basal corti-sol levels (Adam, 2006). In another study of a diverse sampleof adolescents, higher levels of negative affect were associ-ated with flatter diurnal cortisol slopes (DeSantis et al., 2007).

Research on associations between positive affect andHPA functioning has begun to emerge. For instance, excitingevents can elevate cortisol, such as competitive events amongadults (Bateup, Booth, Shirtcliff, & Granger, 2002; Carre,Muir, Belanger, & Putnam, 2006) and Christmas Eve forchildren (Flinn, 2006). Furthermore, the “cortisol boosthypothesis” suggests that cortisol elevations provide a boostof energy and may promote positive, alert states as well asbe protective against subsequent elevations in negativemood (Hoyt, Zeiders, Ehrlich, & Adam, 2016). Thus, the ex-tent to which cortisol levels are differentially associated withnegative versus positive affective states remains to be fullycharacterized. Furthermore, we know little about the extentto which differences in experiences of both positive andnegative affect may explain racial/ethnic disparities in HPAactivity. Nevertheless, we predicted that racial/ethnic minori-ties would report higher levels of negative affect and lowerlevels of positive affect compared to White youth based onprior evidence that they are at higher risk of developingmood and anxiety disorders (Merikangas et al., 2010), aswell as their greater exposure to life stressors, perceived dis-crimination, stereotype threat, and sleep difficulties (Levyet al., 2016), all of which can predispose toward negativeaffect and reduce positive affect.

The Present Study

Biological processes have historically been assumed to be hard-wired and universal, but recent research has challenged thisview and begun exploring the interplay between biology andculture, including racial/ethnic differences in life experiencesand psychopathology (Causadias, 2013; Causadias, Telzer, &Gonzales, 2018; Causadias, Telzer, & Lee, 2017). Many ques-tions remain unexplored in this area of inquiry, particularly re-garding the psychological interface between cultural and bio-logical variation. The current study aims to address this gapby examining the role of affective processes as possible media-tors of racial/ethnic differences in biological processes. Specif-ically, we aim to address the following questions:

Question 1. What is the magnitude of racial/ethnic differencesin diurnal cortisol patterns in adolescence?Hypothesis 1.1. We hypothesize that minority participantswill exhibit flatter cortisol slopes than White participants.

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 3

225

226

227

228

229

230

231

232

233

234

235

236

237

238

239

240

241

242

243

244

245

246

247

248

249

250

251

252

253

254

255

256

257

258

259

260

261

262

263

264

265

266

267

268

269

270

271

272

273

274

275

276

277

278

279

280

281

282

283

284

285

286

287

288

289

290

291

292

293

294

295

296

297

298

299

300

301

302

303

304

305

306

307

308

309

310

311

312

313

314

315

316

317

318

319

320

321

322

323

324

325

326

327

328

329

330

331

332

333

334

335

336

Page 4: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

Hypothesis 1.2. We hypothesize that minority participantswill exhibit a lower cortisol awakening response than Whiteparticipants.Hypothesis 1.3. We hypothesize that minority participantswill exhibit higher total cortisol output (area under the curve)than White participants.Question 2. What is the magnitude of racial/ethnic differencesin low-arousal and high-arousal positive and negative affectstates in adolescence?Hypothesis 2. We hypothesize that racial/ethnic minoritieswill report on average greater levels of negative affect andlower levels of positive affect.Question 3. Do differences in affective patterns explain (i.e.,statistically mediate) racial/ethnic differences observed indiurnal cortisol?Hypothesis 3. We hypothesize that affective patterns willstatistically mediate racial/ethnic differences observed indiurnal cortisol.

We focused on adolescence because this is a period of mas-sive changes in both the activity of the HPA axis and affectiveprocesses, which may explain the onset of psychopathologyduring this developmental window for many youth (Dahl &Gunnar, 2009). We used linear mixed modeling to capturedaily within-person variations in cortisol slopes, cortisol awa-kening response, and area under the curve. We focused onmultiple HPA indices given that they are influenced by differ-ent factors (Fries et al., 2009) and may point to different pa-thophysiological mechanisms toward later dysfunction. Westudied a diverse group of adolescents, with sizable subsam-ples for multiple racial/ethnic backgrounds including AfricanAmerican, Latino, Asian American, White, and multiracialyouth. This allowed us to capture racial/ethnic differencesacross multiple groups. In addition, we utilized a daily diaryapproach for assessing both positive and negative mood.Daily diary reports are the measurement method of choice

for assessing affect because they are more accurate thanmethods that can be subject to recall biases (Bolger, Davis,& Rafaeli, 2003).

Method

Participants

Participants included 370 adolescents (57.3% female) be-tween the ages of 11.9 and 18 years (M age ¼ 14.65 years,SD¼ 1.39 years; see Table 1 for detailed demographic infor-mation). Participants were recruited from the communityusing convenience sampling, including posting flyers atschools, posting on listservs serving ethnic minority families,recruiting participants from other studies who agreed to becontacted for other research studies, and word of mouth.Due to this method of recruitment, we do not have informa-tion on the percentage of the sample approached who partic-ipated. The sample was diverse in terms of race/ethnicity:39.46% non-Latino White (from here on referred to as White,N ¼ 146), 25.4% Asian American (N ¼ 94), 17.8% Latino(N ¼ 66), 10.8% African American (N ¼ 40), and 6.5%mixed race or other race (N¼ 24). The sample covered a fairlybroad range of the socioeconomic spectrum. When consider-ing maternal education as an index of socioeconomic status,9.73% of mothers had less than an eighth-grade education,2.43% completed junior high school, 11.35% attendedsome high school, 24.05% completed high school, 4.59% at-tended trade or vocational school, 21.89% completed college,and 22.97% completed graduate school (2.97% declined toanswer). Adolescents and parents completed written assentand consent in accordance with the institutional review board.

Procedure

Participants received diary checklists for 14 days and a salivacollection kit to complete on days 2 through 5. They had the

Table 1. Sample demographic information and descriptive statistics for primary constructs

Variable Mean SD Range N

Area under the curve 160.73 68.3 2185.58–492.92 358Cortisol awakening response 4.45 9.56 227.48–41.60 369Cortisol slope 21.27 0.74 24.52–1.18 360High-arousal positive affect 3.08 0.91 1.00–5.00 368Low-arousal positive affect 3.40 0.79 1.08–5.00 368High-arousal negative affect 1.66 0.69 1.00–4.06 368Low-arousal negative affect 1.77 0.64 1.00–4.61 368Age 14.65 1.39 11.91–18.02 370Sex

Male 158Female 212

RaceAfrican American 40Asian American 94Latino 66Mixed/other 24White 146

L. K. Deer et al.4

337

338

339

340

341

342

343

344

345

346

347

348

349

350

351

352

353

354

355

356

357

358

359

360

361

362

363

364

365

366

367

368

369

370

371

372

373

374

375

376

377

378

379

380

381

382

383

384

385

386

387

388

389

390

391

392

393

394

395

396

397

398

399

400

401

402

403

404

405

406

407

408

409

410

411

412

413

414

415

416

417

418

419

420

421

422

423

424

425

426

427

428

429

430

431

432

433

434

435

436

437

438

439

440

441

442

443

444

445

446

447

448

Page 5: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

option to complete the diaries with paper and pencil or via asecure website. For those completing with paper/pencil, wemonitored completion of the checklists by providing partici-pants with 14 manila envelopes and an electronic time stam-per. The time stamper is a small, handheld device that im-prints the current date and time and is programmed with asecurity code so that the correct date and time cannot be al-tered. Participants were instructed to place their completedchecklists into a sealed envelope each night and to stampthe seal of the envelope with the time stamper. For those com-pleting the surveys on the secure website, an e-mail with thelink to each daily survey was sent, and the time and date ofcompletion were recorded via the website.

Measures

Diurnal cortisol. Adolescents provided saliva samples acrossdays 2 through 5 of the daily diaries at four time points eachday (i.e., 16 total samples): (a) wake-up time, (b) 30 min afterwaking up, (c) 5 p.m. (or before dinner), and (d) 8 p.m. (orbefore bed). Participants were instructed to take their samplesbefore or at least 30 min after brushing their teeth, drinking,eating, or using tobacco. Participants were provided with acard to log the times of each sample using an electronictime stamper (Dymo Corporation, Stamford, CT), which im-printed the current date and time and was programmed with asecurity code such that adolescents could not alter the correcttime and date. Adolescents were instructed to stamp the cardbeside the appropriate heading for each sample and to placethe sample in their freezer. At the end of the saliva collectionperiod, research staff returned to the home or participantsbrought the samples to the lab, which were immediatelystored at 280 8C until shipment to the Laboratory ofBiological Psychology at the Technical University ofDresden, Germany, where they were assayed using high-sensitivity chemiluminescence-immunoassays (IBL Interna-tional, Hamburg, Germany). The interassay coefficient ofvariation was below 8%. A subsample of 251 participantsprovided information regarding medication they were taking,and among them, n¼ 6 reported using hormonal medications(corticosteroids, estradiol, etc.). Results were largely identical(within rounding error) when excluding these 6 participants,and thus we report our results on the full sample on which weconducted our analyses.

Participants received $10 for completing the diaries and$10 for completing the saliva samples. In addition, adoles-cents were told that they would receive a $20 bonus if inspec-tion of the data indicated that they had completed all the dia-ries and saliva samples correctly and on time.

We computed cortisol slopes, the cortisol awakening re-sponse, and cortisol area under the curve using standard for-mulas. Cortisol slopes were computed as the difference be-tween the fourth (bedtime) cortisol sample and the firstmorning sample, divided by the time elapsed between thesetwo samples. We computed the cortisol awakening responseas the increase in cortisol from wake to 30 min postwake. The

total cortisol area under the curve was computed from thefirst, third, and fourth cortisol concentrations (i.e., excludingthe second sample, which reflects the cortisol awakening re-sponse) using the standard trapezoid method (Pruessner et al.,2003).

Affect. Daily mood diary data from the 4 days when salivarycortisol was collected were used for our analyses in order tobest capture potential associations between daily affect andcortisol. Daily mood was assessed with items taken fromthe Profile of Mood States (McNair, Lorr, & Droppelman,1971). Adolescents used a 5-point scale ranging from 1(not at all) to 5 (extremely) to indicate the extent to whichthey felt a number of affective states each day: enthusiastic,excited, interested, and joyful (these were the high-arousalpositive valence states); calm, cheerful, and happy (the low-arousal positive valence states); angry, nervous, on edge,mad, uneasy, worried, embarrassed, and stressed (the high-arousal negative valence states); and discouraged, exhausted,fatigued, hopeless, sad, lonely, and bored (the low-arousalnegative valence states). The scores for the affective stateswithin each of these four superordinate categories were aver-aged together to create one score for the category. Confirmingour grouping of these emotions in the four superordinate ca-tegories, the internal consistencies for these subscales werehigh for high-arousal positive affect (a ¼ 0.88), low-arousalpositive affect (a ¼ 0.79), high-arousal negative affect (a ¼0.89), and low-arousal negative affect (a ¼ 0.82) states.

Data analysis plan

All analyses performed were linear mixed models that nesteddays (Level 1) within participants (Level 2). Fixed effectswere tested at the level of participants (i.e., Level 2). This sta-tistical approach accounts for dependency within participantsand introduces less bias related to missing data compared totraditional statistical analyses, such as repeated-measuresanalysis of variance (Finch, Bolin, & Kelley, 2014; Rauden-bush & Bryk, 2002). All analyses were conducted using the Rstatistical programming language, version 3.4.0 (R CoreTeam, 2017). Linear mixed models were estimated usingthe lmerTest package in R, version 2.0-33 (Kuznetsova,Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2017); estimated marginal meansand standard errors were derived using the lsmeans packagein R, version 2.26-3 (Lenth, 2016). All degrees of freedomwere estimated using the Satterthwaite approximation, whichmakes the analyses more robust to outliers and violations ofnormality but entails that the degrees of freedom containnumbers that are not integers (Keselman, Algina, Kowalchuk,& Wolfinger, 1999; Kuznetsova et al., 2017).

To address our first question, we conducted linear mixedmodels that included race/ethnicity as a Level 2 predictor ofcortisol parameters (slope, cortisol awakening response, andarea under the curve) examined independently in separateanalyses. We first treated race/ethnicity as a binary variable(0 ¼ “White,” 1 ¼ “racial/ethnic minority”), which was fur-

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 5

449

450

451

452

453

454

455

456

457

458

459

460

461

462

463

464

465

466

467

468

469

470

471

472

473

474

475

476

477

478

479

480

481

482

483

484

485

486

487

488

489

490

491

492

493

494

495

496

497

498

499

500

501

502

503

504

505

506

507

508

509

510

511

512

513

514

515

516

517

518

519

520

521

522

523

524

525

526

527

528

529

530

531

532

533

534

535

536

537

538

539

540

541

542

543

544

545

546

547

548

549

550

551

552

553

554

555

556

557

558

559

560

Page 6: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

ther probed in follow-up analyses examining specific con-trasts between five racial/ethnic groups: White, Asian Amer-ican, Latino, African American, and mixed/other. For thesefollow-up contrasts, we report which findings remain signif-icant after applying a Bonferroni correction (p , .005) tominimize risk of Type I error given the number of pairwisecomparisons conducted. We report estimated marginal meansand standard errors derived from our models for each racial/ethnic group, as these best represent our results. Age andsex were included as covariates in all analyses, though the re-sults were essentially identical without these covariates in-cluded. For our second research question, we conducted lin-ear mixed models that included race/ethnicity as a Level 2predictor of four affect variables (low-arousal and high-arousal positive affect, and low-arousal and high-arousalnegative affect). Similar to cortisol analyses, we first treatedrace/ethnicity as a binary variable, and then examined spe-cific contrasts between all five racial/ethnic groups. For ourthird question, we examined whether racial/ethnic differencesin affect explain cortisol differences. We entered the affectvariables into linear mixed models as predictors of cortisolparameters to test cortisol–affect associations and to explorewhether racial/ethnic differences in cortisol patterns persistafter accounting for differences in affect. Finally, we con-ducted sensitivity analyses to test the extent to which the re-sults of these analyses change when accounting for time ofwake-up and maternal education (a proxy for socioeconomicstatus), both of which have been linked to cortisol functioningin past studies (e.g., Kudielka, Hellhammer, & Wust, 2009).We report confidence intervals and effect sizes (Hedge’s g)for all primary results.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 displays sample characteristics on major constructs.Table 2 displays bivariate correlations among the major con-structs of interest in the entire sample, whereas Table 3 showsthe same correlations separately for the minority group andthe White group. As can be seen in Table 2, cortisol indices

showed expected correlations with each other, and affect vari-ables also showed significant correlations with each other.The only cortisol measure showing significant associationswith affect was the cortisol awakening response, which waspositively correlated with high-arousal negative affect and in-versely correlated with low-arousal positive affect. The slopeand area under the curve did not show any strong or signifi-cant bivariate associations with affect variables.

Question 1. What is the magnitude of racial/ethnicdifferences in diurnal cortisol patterns in adolescence?

Effects of minority status. A comparison of White andminority participants’ cortisol slopes controlling for ageand sex indicated that there was a significant main effect ofrace/ethnicity, F (1, 344.7) ¼ 5.26, p ¼ .02, v2

partial ¼ .012,such that minority participants had flatter (i.e., less negative)slopes (M ¼ –1.19, SE ¼ 0.05) compared to White partici-pants (M ¼ –1.36, SE ¼ 0.06), 95% confidence intervaldifference (CIdiff ) [0.02, 0.31], g ¼ 0.25. This effect can beobserved in Figure 1, which displays average cortisol diurnalslopes for minority and White youth. There was also a signif-icant main effect of age, F (1, 350.0) ¼ 28.67, p , .001,v2

partial ¼ .073, indicating that slopes were flatter for older par-ticipants than younger (B ¼ 0.14, SE ¼ 0.03, b ¼ 0.19). Theeffect of sex was not significant (p¼ .13; g¼ –0.16). Similaranalyses of participants’ cortisol awakening responseindicated that there was not a significant main effect of race(p ¼ .055, v2

partial ¼ .008), age (p ¼ .06, v2partial ¼ .008), or

sex (p ¼ .23, g ¼ 0.13).Analyses of participants’ area under the curve cortisol out-

put indicated that there was not a significant main effect ofrace (p ¼ .21, v2

partial ¼ .002). There were, however, signifi-cant effects for both covariates. The significant main effect ofage, F (1, 351.2)¼ 33.64, p , .001, v2

partial ¼ .085, indicatedthat cortisol area under the curve was lower for olderparticipants compared to younger ones (B ¼ –14.06, SE ¼2.42, b ¼ –0.20). The significant main effect of sex, F(1, 352.7) ¼ 6.09, p ¼ .01, v2

partial ¼ .014, indicated thatfemale participants exhibited larger cortisol areas under thecurve (M ¼ 168.02, SE ¼ 5.23) than male participants (M ¼151.30, SE ¼ 4.40), 95% CIdiff [3.39, 30.04], g ¼ 0.26.

Table 2. Bivariate correlations among major constructs in the entire sample

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.

1. Area under the curve 12. Cortisol awakening response –.06 13. Cortisol slope –.62** .26** 14. High-arousal positive affect –.10 –.08 .10 15. Low-arousal positive affect –.05 –.10* .05 .79** 16. High-arousal negative affect .00 .13* .06 –.11* –.32** 17. Low-arousal negative affect .01 .10 .06 –.22** –.38** .84** 18. Age –.31** –.08 .30** .02 –.05 .08 .09 1

*p , .05. **p , .001.

L. K. Deer et al.6

561

562

563

564

565

566

567

568

569

570

571

572

573

574

575

576

577

578

579

580

581

582

583

584

585

586

587

588

589

590

591

592

593

594

595

596

597

598

599

600

601

602

603

604

605

606

607

608

609

610

611

612

613

614

615

616

617

618

619

620

621

622

623

624

625

626

627

628

629

630

631

632

633

634

635

636

637

638

639

640

641

642

643

644

645

646

647

648

649

650

651

652

653

654

655

656

657

658

659

660

661

662

663

664

665

666

667

668

669

670

671

672

Page 7: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

Follow-up by specific racial/ethnic categories. To furtherprobe the effects of minority status, we next consideredrace/ethnicity as a five-level category and examined pairwisecontrasts between specific groups. For cortisol slopes, therewas again a significant main effect of race, F (4, 348.5) ¼2.91, p ¼ .02, v2

partial ¼ .021, and age, F (1, 344.8) ¼16.65, p , .001, v2

partial ¼ .043, with the effect of sex beingnonsignificant (p ¼ .12, v2

partial ¼ .004). As illustrated inFigure 2, contrasts indicated that the cortisol slopes of African

American participants (M ¼ –1.02, SE ¼ 0.11) were signifi-cantly flatter than the slopes of the White group (M ¼ –1.36,SE ¼ 0.06), t (339.8) ¼ –2.76, p ¼ .006, 95% CIdiff [0.10,0.57], g ¼ 0.30, and the Asian American group (M ¼

–1.31, SE ¼ 0.07), t (344.8) ¼ 2.21, p ¼ .03, 95% CIdiff

[0.03, 0.54], g ¼ 0.24. Furthermore, Latino youth alsoshowed flatter slopes than the White group (M ¼ –1.08, SE¼ 0.10), t (361.8) ¼ –2.38, p ¼ .02, 95% CIdiff [0.05,0.51], g ¼ 0.25. However, when the Bonferroni correction

Table 3. Bivariate correlations among major constructs within minority group (top panel) and White participants (bottompanel)

Minority participants 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.

1. Area under the curve 12. Cortisol awakening response –.07 13. Cortisol slope –.61*** .17* 14. High-arousal positive affect –.20** –.11 .19** 15. Low-arousal positive affect –.13 –.10 .14* .76*** 16. High-arousal negative affect .03 .09 .03 –.06 –.26*** 17. Low-arousal negative affect .08 .09 .03 –.19** –.33*** .84*** 18. Age –.31*** –.07 .26*** .04 –.08 .08 .06 1

White participants 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.1. Area under the curve 12. Cortisol awakening response –.08 13. Cortisol slope –.63*** .41*** 14. High-arousal positive affect .02 –.06 .04 15. Low-arousal positive affect .04 –.14 –.02 .83*** 16. High-arousal negative affect –.04 .17* .10 –.19* –.43*** 17. Low-arousal negative affect –.09 .10 .11 –.27** –.47*** .85*** 18. Age –.27*** –.08 .33*** .02 .04 .10 .15 1

*p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001.

Fig

.1

-B

/Won

line,

B/W

inpr

int

Figure 1. Average diurnal cortisol patterns by race/ethnicity coded in a binary fashion as minority versus White.

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 7

673

674

675

676

677

678

679

680

681

682

683

684

685

686

687

688

689

690

691

692

693

694

695

696

697

698

699

700

701

702

703

704

705

706

707

708

709

710

711

712

713

714

715

716

717

718

719

720

721

722

723

724

725

726

727

728

729

730

731

732

733

734

735

736

737

738

739

740

741

742

743

744

745

746

747

748

749

750

751

752

753

754

755

756

757

758

759

760

761

762

763

764

765

766

767

768

769

770

771

772

773

774

775

776

777

778

779

780

781

782

783

784

Page 8: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

was applied using a cutoff of p , .005, these pairwise differ-ences were no longer statistically significant. As before, thesignificant main effect of age indicated that older participantshad flatter slopes than younger participants (B ¼ 0.12, SE ¼0.03, b ¼ 0.16).

For the cortisol awakening response, analyses indicatedthat there was not a significant effect of race (p ¼ .08,v2

partial ¼ .013), age (p ¼ .33, v2partial , .001), or sex (p ¼

.32, v2partial , .001). Contrasts indicated that there was a sig-

nificant difference between White and Latino participants,t (332.3) ¼ 2.35, p ¼ .02, 95% CIdiff [–6.34, –0.56], g ¼–0.26, such that Latino participants exhibited a lower cortisolawakening response (M ¼ 2.11, SE ¼ 1.23) compared toWhite participants (M¼ 5.56, SE¼ 0.74; see Figure 3). How-ever, this was not a significant difference when applying theBonferroni correction (p , .005). There were no other signif-icant pairwise differences, ps . .05, jgjs , .17.

For cortisol area under the curve, analyses revealed a sig-nificant main effect of race, F (4, 354.5) ¼ 5.52, p , .001,v2

partial ¼ .048, age, F (1, 351.9) ¼ 11.41, p , .001, v2partial

¼ .029, and sex F (1, 353.6) ¼ 5.38, p ¼ .02, v2partial ¼

.012. The significant main effect of race was driven by Latinoparticipants exhibiting significantly lower area under thecurve values than each of the other groups and mixed/otherparticipants showing significantly higher values than eachof the other groups (see Figure 4). Latino participants (M ¼125.20, SE ¼ 9.13) showed lower area under the curve corti-sol than White participants (M ¼ 165.19, SE ¼ 5.10),t (357.4) ¼ 3.74, p , .001, 95% CIdiff [–61.03, –18.96], g¼ –0.39, African American participants (M ¼ 152.83, SE¼ 10.13), t (362.4) ¼ 1.99, p ¼ .05, 95% CIdiff [0.35,

54.91], g ¼ 0.21, Asian American participants (M ¼

164.55, SE ¼ 6.58), t (364.0) ¼ 3.36, p , .001, 95% CIdiff

[16.29, 62.40], g ¼ 0.35, and mixed/other participants (M¼ 192.73, SE ¼ 12.74), t (347.7) ¼ –4.26, p , .001, 95%CIdiff [–98.69, –36.38], g ¼ –0.46. Mixed/other participantsexhibited higher area under the curves than White partici-pants, t (340.2) ¼ –2.02, p ¼ .04, 95% CIdiff [0.68, 54.40],g ¼ 0.22, African American participants, t (349.5) ¼–2.47, p ¼ .01, 95% CIdiff [–71.73, –8.08], g¼ –0.26, AsianAmerican participants, t (344.8)¼ –1.98, p¼ .05, 95% CIdiff

[–56.21, –0.16], g ¼ –0.21, and, as noted above, Latinoparticipants. When employing the Bonferroni correction(p , .005), the remaining significant contrasts were betweenLatino and White participants, Latino and Asian Americanparticipants, and Latino and mixed/other participants. As be-fore, the significant main effect of age indicated that olderparticipants had lower cortisol area under the curve thanyounger ones (B ¼ –9.14, SE ¼ 2.71, b ¼ –0.13), and thatfemale participants (M ¼ 167.77, SE ¼ 4.79) had larger cor-tisol area under the curve than males (M ¼ 152.43, SE ¼5.59), 95% CIdiff [2.33, 28.35], g ¼ 0.25.

Question 2. What is the magnitude of racial/ethnicdifferences in low-arousal and high-arousal positive andnegative affect states in adolescence?

Effects of minority status. In analyses controlling for age andsex, minority participants reported lower levels of positive af-fect (both low-arousal and high-arousal positive affect) com-pared to White participants, with no significant differences innegative affect (see Figure 5). Specifically, a comparison of

Fig

.2

-B

/Won

line,

B/W

inpr

int

Figure 2. Average cortisol slopes by race/ethnicity (all five groups shown).

L. K. Deer et al.8

785

786

787

788

789

790

791

792

793

794

795

796

797

798

799

800

801

802

803

804

805

806

807

808

809

810

811

812

813

814

815

816

817

818

819

820

821

822

823

824

825

826

827

828

829

830

831

832

833

834

835

836

837

838

839

840

841

842

843

844

845

846

847

848

849

850

851

852

853

854

855

856

857

858

859

860

861

862

863

864

865

866

867

868

869

870

871

872

873

874

875

876

877

878

879

880

881

882

883

884

885

886

887

888

889

890

891

892

893

894

895

896

Page 9: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

White and minority participants’ levels of high-arousal posi-tive affect indicated a significant main effect of race, F(1, 387.9)¼ 6.43, p¼ .01, v2

partial ¼ .014, such that minorityyouth reported lower levels of high-arousal positive affect(M ¼ 2.94, SE ¼ 0.06) compared to White participants(M ¼ 3.17, SE ¼ 0.07), 95% CIdiff [–0.40, –0.05], g ¼–0.26. Similarly, there was a significant main effect of race

for low-arousal positive affect, F (1, 387.2) ¼ 6.59, p ¼.01, v2

partial ¼ .014, such that minority participants reportedsignificantly lower levels of low-arousal positive affect(M ¼ 3.26, SE ¼ 0.05) compared to White participants(M ¼ 3.45, SE ¼ 0.06), 95% CIdiff [–0.35, –0.05], g ¼–0.26. The main effect of race was much weaker and didnot reach statistical significance for either high-arousal or

Fig

.3

-B

/Won

line,

B/W

inpr

int

Figure 3. Average cortisol awakening response by specific racial/ethnic group. *p , .05.

Fig

.4

-B

/Won

line,

B/W

inpr

int

Figure 4. Average area under the curve (total daily cortisol output) by specific racial/ethnic group. Significant pairwise contrasts shown.*p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001.

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 9

897

898

899

900

901

902

903

904

905

906

907

908

909

910

911

912

913

914

915

916

917

918

919

920

921

922

923

924

925

926

927

928

929

930

931

932

933

934

935

936

937

938

939

940

941

942

943

944

945

946

947

948

949

950

951

952

953

954

955

956

957

958

959

960

961

962

963

964

965

966

967

968

969

970

971

972

973

974

975

976

977

978

979

980

981

982

983

984

985

986

987

988

989

990

991

992

993

994

995

996

997

998

999

1000

1001

1002

1003

1004

1005

1006

1007

1008

Page 10: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

low-arousal negative affect, p¼ .50, 95% CIdiff [–0.17, 0.09],g ¼ –0.07, and p ¼ .31, 95% CIdiff [–0.19, 0.06], g ¼ 0.10,respectively.

In terms of sex and age differences, for high-arousalnegative affect there was a significant main effect of sex,F (1, 394.9) ¼ 5.48, p ¼ .02, such that female participants(M¼ 1.72, SE¼ 0.04) reported higher levels of high-arousalnegative affect than male participants (M¼ 1.57, SE¼ 0.05),95% CIdiff [0.02, 0.28], g¼ 0.24. In addition, there was a sig-nificant main effect of both age and sex for low-arousalnegative affect. The significant main effect of age, F(1, 402.2)¼ 4.14, p¼ .04, indicated that older participants ex-perienced higher levels of low-arousal negative affect thanyounger ones (B¼ 0.05, SE¼ 0.02,b¼ 0.08). The significantmain effect of sex, F (1, 396.8)¼ 6.37, p¼ .01, indicated thatfemale participants (M ¼ 1.84, SE ¼ 0.04) reported higherlevels of low-arousal negative affect than male participants(M ¼ 1.68, SE ¼ 0.05), 95% CIdiff [0.03, 0.28], g ¼ 0.25.

Follow-up by specific racial/ethnic categories. Next,we probed racial/ethnic differences in affect using the five-category race/ethnicity variable and each of the four affect

variables as outcomes: high-arousal positive affect, low-arousal positive affect, high-arousal negative affect, andlow-arousal negative affect. All models controlled for ageand sex. Analyses predicting high-arousal positive affect re-vealed a significant main effect of race, F (4, 399.8) ¼5.81, p , .001, v2

partial ¼ .045. Specifically, White youth(M¼ 3.16, SE¼ 0.07) reported higher levels of high-arousalpositive affect than African American youth (M¼ 2.71, SE¼0.13), t (398.3) ¼ 3.07, p ¼ .002, 95% CIdiff [–0.73, –0.16],g¼ –0.31, and Asian American youth (M¼ 2.81 SE¼ 0.08),t (381.8) ¼ 3.25, p ¼ .001, 95% CIdiff [–0.56, –0.41], g ¼–0.33. Furthermore, Latino youth (M ¼ 3.37, SE ¼ 0.12)also reported higher levels of high-arousal positive affectthan both African American, t (415.8) ¼ –3.63, p , .001,95% CIdiff [–1.01, –0.30], g ¼ –0.36, and Asian Americanyouth, t (417.8) ¼ –3.63, p , .001, 95% CIdiff [–0.86,–0.25], g ¼ –0.35. All of these differences remained signifi-cant after Bonferroni correction (p , .005). White and Latinoyouth did not differ from each other, p ¼ .15, 95% CIdiff

[–0.49, 0.07], g ¼ –0.14 (see Figure 6).Similarly, analyses of racial differences in low-arousal

positive affect revealed a significant main effect of race, F

Fig

.5

-B

/Won

line,

B/W

inpr

int

Figure 5. Self-reported affect by race/ethnicity (binary-coded as minority vs. White). *p , .05.

L. K. Deer et al.10

1009

1010

1011

1012

1013

1014

1015

1016

1017

1018

1019

1020

1021

1022

1023

1024

1025

1026

1027

1028

1029

1030

1031

1032

1033

1034

1035

1036

1037

1038

1039

1040

1041

1042

1043

1044

1045

1046

1047

1048

1049

1050

1051

1052

1053

1054

1055

1056

1057

1058

1059

1060

1061

1062

1063

1064

1065

1066

1067

1068

1069

1070

1071

1072

1073

1074

1075

1076

1077

1078

1079

1080

1081

1082

1083

1084

1085

1086

1087

1088

1089

1090

1091

1092

1093

1094

1095

1096

1097

1098

1099

1100

1101

1102

1103

1104

1105

1106

1107

1108

1109

1110

1111

1112

1113

1114

1115

1116

1117

1118

1119

1120

Page 11: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

(4, 398.7)¼ 2.99, p¼ .02, v2partial ¼ .019. Contrasts indicated

that White participants (M¼ 3.45, SE¼ 0.06) reported higherlevels of low-arousal positive affect than both African Amer-ican (M ¼ 3.12, SE ¼ 0.11), t (396.5) ¼ 2.59, p ¼ .01, 95%CIdiff [0.08, 0.57], g¼ 0.26, and Asian American participants(M¼ 3.19, SE¼ 0.07), t (378.0)¼ 2.76, p¼ .006, 95% CIdiff

[0.07, 0.44], g ¼ 0.28, while Latino participants (M ¼ 3.44,SE¼ 0.11) reported higher levels than African American par-ticipants, t (417.2)¼ –2.00, p¼ .05, 95% CIdiff [–0.00, 0.62],g¼ 0.20. However, none of these comparisons held up to theBonferroni correction (p , .005).

There was not a significant main effect of race forhigh-arousal negative affect, p ¼ .66, v2

partial , .001, butthere were significant main effects of age, F (1, 390.2) ¼3.95, p ¼ .05, v2

partial ¼ .007, and sex, F (1, 395.0) ¼ 5.14,p ¼ .02, v2

partial ¼ .010. Older participants experienced morehigh-arousal negative affect than younger participants (B ¼0.053, SE ¼ 0.026, b ¼ 0.09). Females (M ¼ 1.70, SE ¼0.05) experienced more high-arousal negative affect than males(M ¼ 1.56, SE¼ 0.05), 95% CIdiff [0.02, 0.28], g ¼ 0.23.

Finally, analyses of racial differences for low-arousalnegative affect revealed significant main effects of race,F (4, 399.9) ¼ 2.70, p ¼ .03, v2

partial ¼ .017, age,F (1, 392.1) ¼ 7.33, p ¼ .01, v2

partial ¼ .016, and sex, F

(1, 396.7) ¼ 5.06, p ¼ .02, v2partial ¼ .010. The main effect

of race appeared to be driven by African American partici-pants reporting higher levels of low-arousal negative affect(M ¼ 1.94, SE ¼ 0.09) than both Asian American (M ¼1.70, SE ¼ 0.06), t (395.8) ¼ 2.24, p ¼ .03, 95% CIdiff

[0.03, 0.45], g ¼ 0.22, and Latino participants (M ¼ 1.57,SE ¼ 0.09), t (414.7) ¼ 2.94, p ¼ .003, 95% CIdiff [0.12,0.62], g ¼ 0.29. However, only the difference betweenAfrican American and Latino participants held up to theBonferroni correction (p , .005). Older participants alsoexperienced more low-arousal negative affect than youngerparticipants (B ¼ 0.067, SE¼ 0.024, b ¼ 0.12), and females(M ¼ 1.83, SE ¼ 0.04) experienced more low-arousalnegative affect than males (M ¼ 1.70, SE ¼ 0.05), 95%CIdiff [0.02, 0.26], g ¼ 0.23.

Question 3. Do differences in affective patterns explainracial/ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol?

We next attempted to determine if racial/ethnic differences inaffect explained differences in diurnal rhythms of cortisol.However, none of the affect variables we considered were sig-nificantly related to cortisol area under the curve or the corti-sol awakening response (ps . .05) in models including race/

Fig

.6

-B

/Won

line,

B/W

inpr

int

Figure 6. Self-reported affect by specific racial/ethnic categories. *p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001.

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 11

1121

1122

1123

1124

1125

1126

1127

1128

1129

1130

1131

1132

1133

1134

1135

1136

1137

1138

1139

1140

1141

1142

1143

1144

1145

1146

1147

1148

1149

1150

1151

1152

1153

1154

1155

1156

1157

1158

1159

1160

1161

1162

1163

1164

1165

1166

1167

1168

1169

1170

1171

1172

1173

1174

1175

1176

1177

1178

1179

1180

1181

1182

1183

1184

1185

1186

1187

1188

1189

1190

1191

1192

1193

1194

1195

1196

1197

1198

1199

1200

1201

1202

1203

1204

1205

1206

1207

1208

1209

1210

1211

1212

1213

1214

1215

1216

1217

1218

1219

1220

1221

1222

1223

1224

1225

1226

1227

1228

1229

1230

1231

1232

Page 12: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

ethnicity, age, and sex, entailing that racial/ethnic differencesin affect did not mediate differences in diurnal rhythms ofcortisol as measured by cortisol area under the curve or thecortisol awakening response. As for cortisol slopes, high-arousal positive affect was related to more positive (i.e.,steeper) cortisol slopes, B ¼ 0.09, t (1174.3) ¼ 2.21, p ¼.03, as was low-arousal negative affect, B ¼ 0.16,t (1103.4)¼ 2.28, p¼ .02, in models including race/ethnicity,age, and sex. However, these affect variables did not mediateracial differences in cortisol slopes (ps . .05), which re-mained largely identical after controlling for affect variables.

Sensitivity analyses

We conducted follow-up analyses to examine whether resultswere robust when controlling for time of wake-up, an impor-tant variable when considering diurnal variation in cortisolpatterns, and when taking into account socioeconomic status.Results were unchanged when controlling for time of wake-up. When considering the binary race/ethnicity variable, con-trolling for time of wake-up did not change any of the results:in these analyses, race/ethnicity still did not significantlymoderate the cortisol awakening response, F (1, 331.8) ¼3.06, p ¼ .081, v2

partial ¼ .006, or area under the curve, F(1, 328.8) ¼ 2.42, p ¼ .121, v2

partial ¼ .004, while still mod-erating cortisol slopes, F (1, 332.9) ¼ 5.97, p ¼ .015, v2

partial¼ .015. When the race/ethnicity variable including multiplecategories was used, race/ethnicity was still not a significantmoderator of the cortisol awakening response, F (4, 342.3)¼1.71, p ¼ .146, v2

partial ¼ .008, but consistent with prior anal-yses remained a significant moderator of cortisol slopes, F(4, 341.0) ¼ 2.55, p ¼ .039, v2

partial ¼ .018, and area underthe curve, F (4, 341.8) ¼ 5.74, p , .001, v2

partial ¼ .052.We then added maternal education (a proxy for socioeco-

nomic status) to models including race/ethnicity, age, andsex. After controlling for maternal education, the previous re-sults regarding cortisol patterns, affect patterns, and media-tion of cortisol differences by affect were unchanged, andmany of the significant results identified above were strength-ened (data available upon request). This is despite a signifi-cant role of maternal education in some of these models,which indicated that higher maternal education was associ-ated with steeper diurnal cortisol slopes, F (1, 325.7) ¼9.77, B¼ 0.072, SE¼ 0.023, b¼ 0.129, p¼ .002, and lowercortisol area under the curve, F (1, 333.5)¼ 4.63, B¼ –4.63,SE¼ 2.15, b¼ –0.089, p¼ .032. Maternal education was notsignificantly associated with the cortisol awakening response,F (1, 337.0)¼ 0.14, B¼ 0.11, SE¼ 0.31, b¼ 0.014, p¼ .71.

Discussion

The present study had three primary goals: (a) to examine ra-cial/ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol patterns in adoles-cence, (b) to investigate how adolescents’ low-arousal andhigh-arousal positive and negative affect states differ byrace/ethnicity, and (c) to assess whether differences in daily

affective patterns explain racial/ethnic differences in diurnalcortisol.

Racial/ethnic differences in HPA activity

We first examined racial/ethnic differences in diurnal cortisolproduction. These analyses indicated that minority adoles-cents exhibited flatter cortisol slopes than White adolescents,with a small overall effect size for race/ethnicity (g ¼ 0.25).Follow-up analyses indicated that these overall patterns weredue to both African American and Latino youth exhibitingflatter slopes than White youth. The pairwise contrasts didnot survive the Bonferroni correction, indicating that the ef-fect sizes were small. Nevertheless, these results are consis-tent with prior research reporting flatter cortisol slopes inAfrican American children and adolescents (DeSantis et al.,2007; Martin et al., 2012), adults (Cohen et al., 2006; Hajatet al., 2010), and older adults (McCallum et al., 2006) thanin Whites. Replicating results from another study with adoles-cents, we also found flatter cortisol slopes among Latinoyouth compared to White youth (DeSantis et al., 2007).The finding that Asian American youth had steeper slopesthan African American youth and slopes that were similarto White youth is a novel finding. The cortisol slope patternsare noteworthy because flatter slopes have been linked tomultiple emotional and physical health problems, includingimmune-related and metabolic conditions (Adam et al.,2017). Of note, these conditions (e.g., diabetes) are moreprevalent among African American and Latino individualscompared to White and Asian American (Centers for DiseaseControl and Prevention, 2017). Together, these results sug-gest that flattened diurnal cortisol slopes may play a role inbiological processes that contribute to racial/ethnic health dis-parities.

In contrast, the cortisol awakening response appeared to bea weaker candidate for explaining racial/ethnic health dispari-ties, given only marginal differences in the cortisol awaken-ing response by race/ethnicity irrespective of whether we con-sidered it as a binary outcome (minority vs. White) or as afive-level categorical variable. The only significant pairwisedifference we identified was that of a lower cortisol awaken-ing response in Latino compared to White youth, which mir-rors findings from another study with adults (Hajat et al.,2010). However, the effect size was small and this findingdid not survive the Bonferroni correction. It is possible thatthis pattern may signal a specific but small vulnerability ofLatino groups to conditions linked in prior meta-analyses toa low cortisol awakening response, such as posttraumaticstress, fatigue, burnout, and exhaustion (Boggero et al.,2017; Chida & Steptoe, 2009).

Racial/ethnic comparisons regarding the cortisol area un-der the curve revealed that Latino youth exhibited the lowestarea under the curve, whereas youth belonging to mixed/othergroups showed the highest area under the curve of all groups.Even with the Bonferroni correction, Latino youth continuedto show significantly lower area under the curve compared to

L. K. Deer et al.12

1233

1234

1235

1236

1237

1238

1239

1240

1241

1242

1243

1244

1245

1246

1247

1248

1249

1250

1251

1252

1253

1254

1255

1256

1257

1258

1259

1260

1261

1262

1263

1264

1265

1266

1267

1268

1269

1270

1271

1272

1273

1274

1275

1276

1277

1278

1279

1280

1281

1282

1283

1284

1285

1286

1287

1288

1289

1290

1291

1292

1293

1294

1295

1296

1297

1298

1299

1300

1301

1302

1303

1304

1305

1306

1307

1308

1309

1310

1311

1312

1313

1314

1315

1316

1317

1318

1319

1320

1321

1322

1323

1324

1325

1326

1327

1328

1329

1330

1331

1332

1333

1334

1335

1336

1337

1338

1339

1340

1341

1342

1343

1344

Page 13: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

White, Asian American, and mixed/other youth. However,the pattern showing higher area under the curve for themixed/other group was less robust, did not survive Bonferronicorrection, and should be replicated given lack of prior litera-ture regarding this heterogeneous group. Again, we foundthat Asian American youth had area under the curve levelsthat were similar to White youth, which is a novel findingand suggests that similar biological processes may be inplay for these two groups. Prior studies have revealed a poten-tial developmental shift, such that racial/ethnic minority chil-dren display lower area under the curve compared to Whitechildren (Bush et al., 2011), whereas in adults, Latino andAfrican American groups show lower area under the curvecompared to Whites (Hajat et al., 2010). It is possible thatearly in development chronic stress associated with racial/eth-nic minority status leads to increased daily cortisol output(Bush et al., 2011), but over time this results in downregula-tion of the HPA axis and lower area under the curve, cortisolawakening response, and flatter slopes, the effects noted inLatino and African American participants in our sample.These complex patterns of hypocortisolism may be explainedby a meta-analysis of studies on chronic stress and HPA activ-ity (Miller et al., 2007), which revealed that HPA activity in-creases acutely after stressor onset but reduces over time asstressors become more chronic.

The psychological mechanisms that explain the HPA ra-cial/ethnic differences we observed are not well understood.Experiences of discrimination in particular and life stress ingeneral may influence patterns of cortisol production, as theHPA axis responds strongly to social stressors (Dickerson& Kemeny, 2004; Gunnar & Adam, 2012). However, somerecent research has indicated that cortisol rhythms are not as-sociated with or persist after accounting for exposure to dis-crimination (Cohen et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2012). This in-dicates that other factors need to be explored in futureresearch, as these may help to explain the racial/ethnic differ-ences observed. We considered affect differences as a possi-ble explanation.

Racial/ethnic differences in affect

The second goal of the study was to explore racial/ethnic dif-ferences in affect. These analyses indicated that racial/ethnicminority participants endorsed lower levels of positive affect(both high and low arousal) compared to White youth,whereas the differences between the two groups on negativeaffect (either high- or low-arousal negative affect) were small,close to zero. The most pronounced differences were thatAfrican American and Asian American youth reported lowerlevels of positive affect (both high arousal and low arousal)compared to White youth. Effect sizes were larger for high-arousal positive affect, where all the contrasts remained sig-nificant even after the Bonferroni correction. These resultsare consistent with the greater risk of mood disorders in ethnicminorities compared to White youth (Merikangas et al.,2010), and findings that indicate lower positive affect among

minority samples are also consistent with some previous re-search that has noted differences in emotion expression andvalence between Asian American and White children (Lewis,Takai-Kawakami, Kawakami, & Sullivan, 2009) and adults(Gross, Richards, & John, 2006). Research on cultural differ-ences in ideal affect, or affect that people would like to feel,has also indicated that Asian Americans are less likely to en-dorse high arousal positive states and more likely to endorselow arousal positive states than European Americans (for re-view, see Tsai, 2007).

Positive affect and greater neural responsivity to rewardsprotect against the development of adolescent depression(Forbes & Dahl, 2005, 2012). Furthermore, positive affectis associated with better health, lower morbidity, and greaterlongevity (Pressman & Cohen, 2005). Thus, our findingssuggest that African American and Asian American youthmay be at increased risk of developing mental and physicalhealth problems across their life span.

The group differences in measures of negative valence af-fect were small, suggesting either that the true difference inthe population is zero or that the effect is too small to have suf-ficient power to detect it with a sample of 370. Previousresearch with adults has noted that affect may not be felt andexpressed in the same manner across races (Jang, Kwag, &Chiriboga, 2012), or that there may be differences in the idealaffect to be expressed among cultures (Tsai, 2007), and differ-ent associations with risk of psychopathology across races(Moazen-Zadeh & Assari, 2016). Thus, it could be that ra-cial/ethnic minority youth were underreporting their negativeaffect, as some studies have found that African Americanstend to underreport negative affect (Bardwell & Dimsdale,2001). However, taking the findings at face value could alsosuggest that this was a high-functioning, low-risk minoritysample that experienced low levels of negative affect. While re-search is often biased toward finding deficits or signs of dep-rivation among minorities (for review, see Garcıa Coll et al.,1996), we must remember that there is considerable diversitywithin and between minorities and that many minority youthare in good mental health despite facing race-based stressors.

Differences in affect did not explain cortisol differences

The third goal of the study was to assess whether affectivepatterns explain racial/ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol.We did not find any evidence of statistical mediation thatwould indicate this. This is consistent with another study ofadolescents, which found that higher levels of negative affectwere associated with flatter slopes, but negative affect did notexplain racial/ethnic differences in cortisol slopes (DeSantiset al., 2007). There are several possible explanations for thesefindings. We may need to consider refining our models of re-lations between affect and HPA activity for a number of rea-sons. For instance, transactions between affect and HPAphysiology occur on multiple time scales, from moments todays and years (Adam, 2012), thus incorporating informationon either affect or HPA physiology from other time points

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 13

1345

1346

1347

1348

1349

1350

1351

1352

1353

1354

1355

1356

1357

1358

1359

1360

1361

1362

1363

1364

1365

1366

1367

1368

1369

1370

1371

1372

1373

1374

1375

1376

1377

1378

1379

1380

1381

1382

1383

1384

1385

1386

1387

1388

1389

1390

1391

1392

1393

1394

1395

1396

1397

1398

1399

1400

1401

1402

1403

1404

1405

1406

1407

1408

1409

1410

1411

1412

1413

1414

1415

1416

1417

1418

1419

1420

1421

1422

1423

1424

1425

1426

1427

1428

1429

1430

1431

1432

1433

1434

1435

1436

1437

1438

1439

1440

1441

1442

1443

1444

1445

1446

1447

1448

1449

1450

1451

1452

1453

1454

1455

1456

Page 14: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

may allow us to better parse out momentary correlations be-tween affect and physiology. Another possibility is that af-fect–physiology associations are moderated by various pro-tective factors (e.g., coping strategies) that may differ byrace/ethnicity. This would explain why some ethnic minori-ties differ from the majority group in levels of affect (e.g.,Asian American youth compared to Whites), but this didnot translate into physiological differences in HPA function-ing, perhaps due to successful coping efforts. Furthermore,research indicates that the HPA system serves multiple bio-logical functions (e.g., metabolic and immune) beyond its in-volvement in emotional processes (Gunnar & Adam, 2012).Thus, it may be that affect–cortisol associations are difficultto capture without a large panel of covariates relevant todiet, physical activity, immune function, and so on. Further-more, the HPA axis is only one of the body’s stress-mediatingsystems and likely cannot fully explain racial/ethnic healthdisparities on its own. Considering multisystem indicatorsof allostatic load may capture stronger differences by race/ethnicity (Doan & Evans, 2017).

A second possibility is that racial/ethnic differences inphysiology and affect may be small, making the detectionof relations between them challenging. Deficit models ofminority development often assume that racial/ethnic minori-ties should consistently show worse outcomes on any givenmeasure (Causadias, Vitriol, & Atkin, 2018), but our findingsindicate that differences between minority and White youthare not always found, and even when differences exist, effectsizes tend to be small. As such, the current study adds to theimportant evidence base documenting both similarities anddifferences between minority and White youth. These find-ings suggest that, even in a society where there is structuralinequality, we find evidence of equifinality (i.e., different de-velopmental pathways can lead to similar outcomes on corti-sol and affect) and multifinality (i.e., those with similar devel-opmental pathways can have differing outcomes; Cicchetti &Rogosch, 1996). The heterogeneity within and between ra-cial/ethnic groups should be further explored to understandthe developmental processes underlying equifinality andmultifinality.

Finally, another possible explanation is that the racial/eth-nic groups we considered here may be heterogeneous andcontain multiple meaningful subgroups once we considerother cultural aspects (e.g., immigration status, acculturationstress, family norms and values, and identity processes).Thus, a single aspect of racial/ethnic identity may not fullyexplain biological differences observed (for a more in-depthdiscussion of the complex nature of culture–biology inter-play, see Causadias, Telzer, & Gonzalez, 2018). For example,an individual’s identification with his/her culture could act inconcert with experiences they have in everyday life to influ-ence their biology and affect (Causadias, Telzer, & Gonzalez,2018; Zeiders, Causadias, & White, 2017). We propose that,until we account for multiple aspects of cultural identity, theobserved racial/ethnic differences in affect and physiologymay remain difficult to explain.

The roles of age, sex, and maternal education

Our results indicate some noteworthy effects for age and sexin predicting both diurnal cortisol and daily affect. Age was asignificant predictor of diurnal cortisol slopes and cortisolarea under the curve, such that older participants exhibitedflatter slopes and lower area under the curve than youngerparticipants. These findings are somewhat consistent withprevious research in adolescents, which has indicated thatolder adolescents exhibit flatter cortisol slopes (due to lowermorning and higher evening cortisol output) and higher areaunder the curve cortisol than younger adolescents (Gunnar,Wewerka, Frenn, Long, & Griggs, 2009; Shirtcliff et al.,2012). In previous work, however, the age of the participantsranged from age 9 to 15, while the participants in the currentstudy ranged from age 11.9 to 18. Previous research has pro-posed that there is a possible “U-shaped” curve in the cortisoloutput of older children and adolescents, such that levels de-crease in the preteen years, increase in early adolescence, andthen decrease across adolescence (Shirtcliff et al., 2012). Al-though this study did not examine children in the preteenstage, participants in the current study ranged from earlythrough later adolescence so the results may have capturedadolescents at the peak of the curve and on the way down.One hypothesis is that this curve signifies a time of increasedenvironmental and neurobiological sensitivity early in ado-lescence (Shirtcliff et al., 2012). Future research should ex-amine the full extent of this developmental pattern from latechildhood through early adulthood.

Sex was a significant predictor of cortisol area under thecurve, such that girls had higher area under the curve valuesthan boys. This is consistent with previous evidence indicat-ing that girls tend to exhibit higher afternoon and evening cor-tisol levels than boys (Klimes-Dougan, Hastings, Granger,Usher, & Zahn-Waxler, 2001), although more research isneeded to corroborate these patterns. When examining affect,there were no significant differences by age or sex for the pos-itive affect indices. However, there were significant differ-ences for both of the negative affect indices, such that olderparticipants and girls reported higher levels of negative affect(both low arousal and high arousal). This result is consistentwith previous research, which has indicated that there may beincreases in negative affect as adolescents get older (Larson,Moneta, Richards, & Wilson, 2002) and that girls tend to ex-hibit higher levels of negative affect than boys (Silk, Stein-berg, & Morris, 2003). Furthermore, girls have a higher inci-dence of depression during adolescence (Avenevoli,Swendsen, He, Burstein, & Merikangas, 2015). These find-ings point to a need to consider the cumulative roles ofrace/ethnicity, age, and sex in predicting and mitigating futurepsychopathology and health problems.

When examining the role of socioeconomic status, asindexed by maternal education, we found that racial/ethnicdifferences persisted after accounting for maternal educationstatistically, consistent with prior studies with children, ado-lescents, and adults (Bush et al., 2011; Cohen et al., 2006; De-

L. K. Deer et al.14

1457

1458

1459

1460

1461

1462

1463

1464

1465

1466

1467

1468

1469

1470

1471

1472

1473

1474

1475

1476

1477

1478

1479

1480

1481

1482

1483

1484

1485

1486

1487

1488

1489

1490

1491

1492

1493

1494

1495

1496

1497

1498

1499

1500

1501

1502

1503

1504

1505

1506

1507

1508

1509

1510

1511

1512

1513

1514

1515

1516

1517

1518

1519

1520

1521

1522

1523

1524

1525

1526

1527

1528

1529

1530

1531

1532

1533

1534

1535

1536

1537

1538

1539

1540

1541

1542

1543

1544

1545

1546

1547

1548

1549

1550

1551

1552

1553

1554

1555

1556

1557

1558

1559

1560

1561

1562

1563

1564

1565

1566

1567

1568

Page 15: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

Santis et al., 2007; Hajat et al., 2010; Martin et al., 2012).This suggests that maternal education does not explain theseracial/ethnic differences, even though differences by maternaleducation mirrored the patterns observed in ethnic minorityyouth, for example, flatter cortisol slopes were noted forboth minority youth and youth whose mothers had lower edu-cational attainment. These patterns may be explained by sim-ilar processes, for example, a higher stress burden in both eth-nic minority youth and low-socioeconomic status youth,which encompasses different types of stressors for eachgroup, though this possibility will need to be confirmed em-pirically in future research.

Conclusions

This study has some noteworthy strengths. This is a large anddiverse sample for adolescent research, which allows morefine-grained comparisons between the various racial/ethnicgroups rather than a simple contrast between minorities ver-sus Whites, which is common in prior literature. In addition,the frequent and rigorous sampling of cortisol (4 samples perday on 4 separate days) provides greater reliability of mea-surement than sampling participants on 1 or 2 days. However,this study is not without its limitations. As this was a cross-sectional design, we do not know how trait or long-term pat-terns of affect might relate to cortisol. Similarly, the measuresof diurnal cortisol included here only capture momentary out-put, while a more chronic measure of output, such as hair cor-tisol (Meyer & Novak, 2012), may uncover a different set ofassociations. Moreover, analyses in this study modeled corti-sol diurnal slopes by the difference between morning andevening levels. Therefore, we may have lost variabilityamong individuals using this approach. Relatedly, a higherfrequency sampling schedule would have added precisionto our area under the curve estimates (Hoyt, Ehrlich, Cham,& Adam, 2016). The number of cortisol samples was chosento minimize participant burden, and a recent study suggests

that while adding more samples might have improved theaccuracy of area under the curve estimates, it likely wouldnot have increased accuracy of the cortisol awakeningresponse and diurnal slope estimates though it would haveimposed a much higher burden on participants (Hoyt,Ehrlich, et al., 2016). Finally, another limitation is that theAfrican American and mixed/other groups had fewer than50 participants in each group, potentially limiting our abilityto detect significant differences between groups if thesedifferences exist. Future studies should replicate our analyseswith larger samples.

In sum, this study replicates a number of prior findings re-garding racial/ethnic differences in cortisol and affect, butalso raises novel questions regarding these patterns giventhat affective differences did not explain differences in hor-monal output. Race/ethnicity is frequently treated as a simpledemographic variable that is associated with specific experi-ences such as discrimination, but prior studies have shownthat this factor does not fully account for the observed ra-cial/ethnic differences in cortisol patterns. We hypothesizethat conceptualizing race/ethnicity in a broader culturalframework that includes numerous cultural aspects such asnorms, attitudes, media exposure, family and social networks,connections with a home country, and so on, may shed morelight on racial/ethnic differences in affect and biology thanour study and previous empirical investigations. In particular,understanding the complex influences on the socioemotionaldevelopment of minority youth will require a comprehensivemeasurement of developmental competencies and challengesat multiple levels of analysis (Garcıa Coll et al., 1996). Fur-thermore, more research is needed that will assess cultural as-pects of development, affect, biology, and health outcomeswithin the same participants, in order to test key assumptionsabout the pathways to racial/ethnic health disparities. Whenwe can better understand these pathways, we will be betterequipped to design culturally sensitive interventions thatcan effectively combat existing health disparities.

References

Adam, E. K. (2006). Transactions among adolescent trait and state emotion anddiurnal and momentary cortisol activity in naturalistic settings. Psychoneu-roendocrinology, 31, 664–679. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.01.010

Adam, E. K. (2012). Emotion-cortisol transactions occur over multiple timescales in development: Implications for research on emotion and the de-velopment of emotional disorders. Monographs of the Society for Re-search in Child Development, 77, 17–27. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2012.00657.x

Adam, E. K., Doane, L. D., Zinbarg, R. E., Mineka, S., Craske, M. G., &Griffith, J. W. (2010). Prospective prediction of major depressive disor-der from cortisol awakening responses in adolescence. Psychoneuroen-docrinology, 35, 921–931. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.007

Adam, E. K., Heissel, J. A., Zeiders, K. H., Richeson, J. A., Ross, E. C.,Ehrlich, K. B., . . . Eccles, J. S. (2015). Developmental histories of per-ceived racial discrimination and diurnal cortisol profiles in adulthood:A 20-year prospective study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 279–291.doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.018

Adam, E. K., Quinn, M. E., Tavernier, R., McQuillan, M. T., Dahlke, K. A.,& Gilbert, K. E. (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physicalhealth outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuro-endocrinology, 83, 25–41. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018

Alegria, M., Vallas, M., & Pumariega, A. J. (2010). Racial and ethnicdisparities in pediatric mental health. Child and Adolescent Psychiat-ric Clinics of North America, 19, 759–774. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2010.07.001

Avenevoli, S., Swendsen, J., He, J. P., Burstein, M., & Merikangas, K. R.(2015). Major depression in the National Comorbidity Survey—Adoles-cent Supplement: Prevalence, correlates, and treatment. Journal of theAmerican Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 37–44.doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.010

Bardwell, W. A., & Dimsdale, J. E. (2001). The impact of ethnicity and re-sponse bias on the self-report of negative affect. Journal of Applied Bio-behavioral Research, 6, 27–38.

Bateup, H. S., Booth, A., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Granger, D. A. (2002). Testos-terone, cortisol, and women’s competition. Evolution and Human Behav-ior, 23, 181–192. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00100-3

Bennett, G. G., Merritt, M. M., & Wolin, K. Y. (2004). Ethnicity, education,and the cortisol response to awakening: A preliminary investigation. Eth-nicity & Health, 9, 337–347. doi:10.1080/1355785042000285366

Boggero, I. A., Hostinar, C. E., Haak, E. A., Murphy, M. L. M., & Seger-strom, S. C. (2017). Psychosocial functioning and the cortisol awakeningresponse: Meta-analysis, P-curve analysis, and evaluation of the eviden-

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 15

1569

1570

1571

1572

1573

1574

1575

1576

1577

1578

1579

1580

1581

1582

1583

1584

1585

1586

1587

1588

1589

1590

1591

1592

1593

1594

1595

1596

1597

1598

1599

1600

1601

1602

1603

1604

1605

1606

1607

1608

1609

1610

1611

1612

1613

1614

1615

1616

1617

1618

1619

1620

1621

1622

1623

1624

1625

1626

1627

1628

1629

1630

1631

1632

1633

1634

1635

1636

1637

1638

1639

1640

1641

1642

1643

1644

1645

1646

1647

1648

1649

1650

1651

1652

1653

1654

1655

1656

1657

1658

1659

1660

1661

1662

1663

1664

1665

1666

1667

1668

1669

1670

1671

1672

1673

1674

1675

1676

1677

1678

1679

1680

Page 16: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

tial value in existing studies. Biological Psychology, 129, 207–230.doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.058

Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life asit is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579–616. doi:10.1146/an-nurev.psych.54.101601.145030

Braveman, P. A., Cubbin, C., Egerter, S., Williams, D. R., & Pamuk, E.(2010). Socioeconomic disparities in health in the United States: Whatthe patterns tell us. American Journal of Public Health, 100(Suppl. 1),S186–S196. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2009.166082

Bush, N. R., Obradovic, J., Adler, N., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Kindergartenstressors and cumulative adrenocortical activation: The “first straws” ofallostatic load? Development and Psychopathology, 23, 1089–1106.doi:10.1017/S0954579411000514

Carnegie, R., Araya, R., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Glover, V., O’Connor, T. G.,O’Donnell, K. J., . . . Lewis, G. (2014). Cortisol awakening responseand subsequent depression: Prospective longitudinal study. British Jour-nal of Psychiatry, 204, 137–143. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.113.126250

Carre, , J., Muir, C., Belanger, J., & Putnam, S. K. (2006). Pre-competitionhormonal and psychological levels of elite hockey players: Relationshipto the “home advantage.” Physiology & Behavior, 89, 392–398.doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.07.011

Causadias, J. M. (2013). A roadmap for the integration of culture into devel-opmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4,Pt. 2), 1375–1398.

Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (2018). Introduction toculture and biology interplay. In J. M. Causadias, E. H. Telzer, & N.A. Gonzales (Eds.), Handbook of culture and biology. New York: Wiley.

Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Lee, R. M. (2017). Culture and biology in-terplay: An introduction. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychol-ogy, 23, 1–4. doi:10.1037/cdp0000121

Causadias, J. M., Vitriol, J. A., & Atkin, A. L. (2018). Do we overemphasizethe role of culture in the behavior of racial/ethinc minorities? Evidence ofa cultural (mis)attribution bias in American Psychology. American Psy-chologist. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/amp0000099

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). National Diabetes Statis-tics Report, 2017. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Preven-tion, U.S. Dept of Health and Human Services.

Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2009). Cortisol awakening response and psychoso-cial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychol-ogy, 80, 265–278. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.004

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in de-velopmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8,597–600.

Cohen, S., Schwartz, J. E., Epel, E., Kirschbaum, C., Sidney, S., & Seeman,T. (2006). Socioeconomic status, race, and diurnal cortisol decline in theCoronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study.Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 41–50. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000195967.51768.ea

Dahl, R. E., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Heightened stress responsiveness andemotional reactivity during pubertal maturation: Implications for psycho-pathology. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 1–6. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000017

DeSantis, A. S., Adam, E. K., Doane, L. D., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., &Craske, M. G. (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in cortisol diurnalrhythms in a community sample of adolescents. Journal of AdolescentHealth, 41, 3–13. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.03.006

Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol re-sponses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psy-chological Bulletin, 130, 355–391. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355

Dinos, S., Khoshaba, B., Ashby, D., White, P. D., Nazroo, J., Wessely, S., &Bhui, K. S. (2009). A systematic review of chronic fatigue, its syndromesand ethnicity: Prevalence, severity, co-morbidity and coping. Interna-tional Journal of Epidemiology, 38, 1554–1570. doi:10.1093/ije/dyp147

Doan, S. N., & Evans, G. W. (2017). Relations among culture, poverty, stress,and allostatic load. In J. M. Causadias, E. H. Telzer, & N. A. Gonzalez(Eds.), The handbook of culture and biology. New York: Wiley.

Doane, L. D., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., Craske, M., Griffith, J. W., &Adam, E. K. (2013). Are flatter diurnal cortisol rhythms associatedwith major depression and anxiety disorders in late adolescence? Therole of life stress and daily negative emotion. Development and Psycho-pathology, 25, 629–642. doi:10.1017/S0954579413000060

Finch, W. H., Bolin, J. E., & Kelley, K. (2014). Multilevel modeling using R.Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Flinn, M. V. (2006). Evolution and ontogeny of stress response to social chal-lenges in the human child. Developmental Review, 26, 138–174.doi:10.1016/j.dr.2006.02.003

Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2005). Neural systems of positive affect: Rele-vance to understanding child and adolescent depression? Developmentand Psychopathology, 17, 827–850. doi:10.1017/S095457940505039x

Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Research Review: Altered rewardfunction in adolescent depression: What, when and how? Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 3–15. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02477.x

Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., & Kirschbaum, C. (2009). The cortisol awakeningresponse (CAR): Facts and future directions. International Journal ofPsychophysiology, 72, 67–73. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014

Garcıa Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, H. P., Crnic, K., Wasik,B. H., & Garcia, H. V. (1996). An integrative model for the study of de-velopmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67,1891–1914. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01834.x

Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in ev-eryday life. In D. K. Snyder, J. Simpson, & J. N. Huges (Eds.), Emotionregulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction and health(pp. 13–35). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.doi:10.1037/11468-001

Gunnar, M. R., & Adam, E. K. (2012). The hypothalamic-pituitary-adreno-cortical system and emotion: Current wisdom and future directions.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77,109–119. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00669.x

Gunnar, M. R., Doom, J. R., & Esposito, E. A. (2015). Psychoneuroendo-crinology of stress: Normative development and individual differences.In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmentalscience (7th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 106–151). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Gunnar, M. R., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J. D., & Griggs, C. (2009).Developmental changes in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity overthe transition to adolescence: Normative changes and associations withpuberty. Developmental Psychopathology, 21, 69–85. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000054

Hajat, A., Diez-Roux, A., Franklin, T. G., Seeman, T., Shrager, S., Ranjit, N.,. . . Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Socioeconomic and race/ethnic differences indaily salivary cortisol profiles: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclero-sis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 932–943. doi:10.1016/j.psy-neuen.2009.12.009

Hostinar, C. E., McQuillan, M. T., Mirous, H. J., Grant, K. E., & Adam, E. K.(2014). Cortisol responses to a group public speaking task for adoles-cents: Variations by age, gender, and race. Psychoneuroendocrinology,50, 155–166. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.015

Hoyt, L. T., Ehrlich, K. B., Cham, H., & Adam, E. K. (2016). Balancing sci-entific accuracy and participant burden: Testing the impact of samplingintensity on diurnal cortisol indices. Stress, 19, 476–485. doi:10.1080/10253890.2016.1206884

Hoyt, L. T., Zeiders, K. H., Ehrlich, K. B., & Adam, E. K. (2016). Positiveupshots of cortisol in everyday life. Emotion, 16, 431–435.doi:10.1037/emo0000174

Jang, Y., Kwag, K. H., & Chiriboga, D. A. (2012). Not saying I am happydoes not mean I am not: Cultural influences on responses to positive af-fect items in the CES-D. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psycholog-ical Sciences and Social Sciences, 65, 684–690. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq052

Keselman, H. J., Algina, J., Kowalchuk, R. K., & Wolfinger, R. D. (1999).The analysis of repeated measurements: A comparison of mixed-modelSatterthwaite F tests and a nonpooled adjusted degrees of freedom multi-variate test. Communications in Statistics—Theory and Methods, 28,2967–2999. doi:10.1080/03610929908832460

Klimes-Dougan, B., Hastings, P. D., Granger, D. A., Usher, B. A., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2001). Adrenocortical activity in at-risk and normally devel-oping adolescents: Individual differences in salivary cortisol basal levels,diurnal variation, and responses to social challenges. Development andPsychopathology, 13, 695–719.

Korous, K. M., Causadias, J. M., & Casper, D. M. (2017). Racial discrimina-tion and cortisol output: A meta-analysis. Social Science & Medicine,193, 90–100. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.09.042

Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., & Wust, S. (2009). Why do we respondso differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol re-sponses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 2–18.doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.004

L. K. Deer et al.16

1681

1682

1683

1684

1685

1686

1687

1688

1689

1690

1691

1692

1693

1694

1695

1696

1697

1698

1699

1700

1701

1702

1703

1704

1705

1706

1707

1708

1709

1710

1711

1712

1713

1714

1715

1716

1717

1718

1719

1720

1721

1722

1723

1724

1725

1726

1727

1728

1729

1730

1731

1732

1733

1734

1735

1736

1737

1738

1739

1740

1741

1742

1743

1744

1745

1746

1747

1748

1749

1750

1751

1752

1753

1754

1755

1756

1757

1758

1759

1760

1761

1762

1763

1764

1765

1766

1767

1768

1769

1770

1771

1772

1773

1774

1775

1776

1777

1778

1779

1780

1781

1782

1783

1784

1785

1786

1787

1788

1789

1790

1791

1792

Page 17: Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in …dsnlab.web.unc.edu/files/2018/10/Deer-Shields-Ivory... · 2018-10-08 · Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest:Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-33 [Compu-ter software]. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/

Larson, R. W., Moneta, G., Richards, M. H., & Wilson, S. (2002). Continu-ity, stability, and change in daily emotional experience across adoles-cence. Child Development, 73, 1151–1165.

Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. Journalof Statistical Software, 69, 1–33. doi:10.18637/jss.v069.i01

Levy, D. J., Heissel, J. A., Richeson, J. A., & Adam, E. K. (2016). Psycho-logical and biological responses to race-based social stress as pathwaysto disparities in educational outcomes. American Psychologist, 71,455–473. doi:10.1037/a0040322

Lewis, M., Takai-Kawakami, K., Kawakami, K., & Sullivan, M. W. (2009).Cultural differences in emotional responses to success and failure. Inter-national Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 53–61. doi:10.1177/0165025409348559

Lewis, T. T., Cogburn, C. D., & Williams, D. R. (2015). Self-reported experi-ences of discrimination and health: Scientific advances, ongoing contro-versies, and emerging issues. Annual Review Clinical Psychology, 11,407–440. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112728

Martin, C. G., Bruce, J., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Racial and ethnic differencesin diurnal cortisol rhythms in preadolescents: The role of parental psycho-social risk and monitoring. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 661–668.doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.025

Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Developmentand Psychopathology, 22, 491–495. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000222

Mays, V. M., Cochran, S. D., & Barnes, N. W. (2007). Race, race-based dis-crimination, and health outcomes among African Americans. Annual Re-view of Psychology, 58, 201–225. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190212

McCallum, T. J., Sorocco, K. H., & Fritsch, T. (2006). Mental health anddiurnal salivary cortisol patterns among African American and EuropeanAmerican female dementia family caregivers. American Journal of Ger-iatric Psychiatry, 14, 684–693. doi:10.1097/01.JGP.0000225109.85406.89

McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppelman, L. F. (1971). Manual for the profileof mood states. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Ser-vice.

Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S.,Cui, L. H., . . . Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mentaldisorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the National ComorbiditySurvey Replication—Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of theAmerican Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 980–989.doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017

Meyer, J. S., & Novak, M. A. (2012). Minireview: Hair cortisol: A novel bio-marker of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical activity. Endocrinol-ogy, 153, 4120–4127. doi:10.1210/en.2012-1226

Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it goes up, must it comedown? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocorticalaxis in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 25–45. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25

Moazen-Zadeh, E., & Assari, S. (2016). Depressive symptoms predict majordepressive disorder after 15 years among Whites but not Blacks. Fron-tiers in Public Health, 4. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2016.00013

Ogden, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Curtin, L. R., McDowell, M. A., Tabak, C. J., &Flegal, K. M. (2006). Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the UnitedStates, 1999–2004. Journal of the American Medical Association, 295,1549–1555. doi:10.1001/jama.295.13.1549

Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health?Psychological Bulletin, 131, 925–971. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925

Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., & Hellhammer, D. H.(2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve repre-sent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependentchange. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 916–931. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00108-7

Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models:Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical comput-ing [Computer software]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for StatisticalComputing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/

Ross, K. M., Murphy, M. L. M., Adam, E. K., Chen, E., & Miller, G. E.(2014). How stable are diurnal cortiosl activity indices in healthy indi-viduals? Evidence from three multi-wave studies. Psychoneuroendocrin-ology, 39, 184–193. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.016

Ruttle, P. L., Javaras, K. N., Klein, M. H., Armstrong, J. M., Burk, L. R., &Essex, M. J. (2013). Concurrent and longitudinal associationsbetween diurnal cortisol and body mass index across adolescence.Journal of Adolescent Health, 52, 731–737. doi:10.1016/j.jado-health.2012.11.013

Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocor-ticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive,stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21, 55–89.doi:10.1210/edrv.21.1.0389

Shields, G. S., Moons, W. G., & Slavich, G. M. (2017). Inflammation, self-regulation, and health: An immunologic model of self-regulatory failure.Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 588–612. doi:10.1177/1745691616689091

Shields, G. S., & Slavich, G. M. (2017). Lifetime stress exposure and health:A review of contemporary assessment methods and biological mecha-nisms. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11, e12335.doi:10.1111/spc3.12335

Shirtcliff, E. A., Allison, A. L., Armstrong, J. M., Slattery, M. J., Kalin, N. H.,& Essex, M. J. (2012). Longitudinal stability and developmental proper-ties of salivary cortisol levels and circadian rhythms from childhood toadolesence. Developmental Psychobiology, 54, 493–502. doi:10/1002/dev.20607

Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion reg-ulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior.Child Development, 74, 1869–1880.

Smith, S. M., & Vale, W. W. (2006). The role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in neuroendocrine responses to stress. Dialogues ClinicalNeuroscience, 8, 383–395.

Spencer, R. L., & Deak, T. (2016). A users guide to HPA axis research. Phys-iology & Behavior, 178, 43–65. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.11.014

Suglia, S. F., Staudenmayer, J., Cohen, S., Enlow, M. B., Rich-Edwards,J. W., & Wright, R. J. (2010). Cumulative stress and cortisol disruptionamong black and Hispanic pregnant women in an urban cohort. Psycho-logical Trauma—Theory Research Practice and Policy, 2, 326–334.doi:10.1037/a0018953

Susman, E. J. (2007). Toward a psychobiologic understanding of youthhealth disparities. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 1–2. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.04.002

Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences.Perspectives in Psychological Science, 2, 242–259. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x

Van den Bergh, B. R. H., & Van Calster, B. (2009). Diurnal cortisol profilesand evening cortisol in post-pubertal adolescents scoring high on theChildren’s Depression Inventory. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 791–794. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.12.008

Zeiders, K. H., Causadias, J. M., & White, R. M. B. (2017). The healthcorrelates of culture: Examining the association between ethnic-racialidentity and diurnal cortisol slopes. Journal of Adolescent Health, 62,349–351. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.09.020

Zeiders, K. H., Doane, L. D., & Roosa, M. W. (2012). Perceived discrimina-tion and diurnal cortisol: Examining relations among Mexican Americanadolescents. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 541–548. doi:10.1016/j.yh-beh.2012.01.018

Zeiders, K. H., Hoyt, L. T., & Adam, E. K. (2014). Associations betweenself-reported discrimination and diurnal cortisol rhythms among youngadults: The moderating role of racial-ethnic minority status. Psychoneu-roendocrinology, 50, 280–288. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.023

Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol and affect 17

1793

1794

1795

1796

1797

1798

1799

1800

1801

1802

1803

1804

1805

1806

1807

1808

1809

1810

1811

1812

1813

1814

1815

1816

1817

1818

1819

1820

1821

1822

1823

1824

1825

1826

1827

1828

1829

1830

1831

1832

1833

1834

1835

1836

1837

1838

1839

1840

1841

1842

1843

1844

1845

1846

1847

1848

1849

1850

1851

1852

1853

1854

1855

1856

1857

1858

1859

1860

1861

1862

1863

1864

1865

1866

1867

1868

1869

1870

1871

1872

1873

1874

1875

1876

1877

1878

1879

1880

1881

1882

1883

1884

1885

1886

1887

1888

1889

1890

1891

1892

1893

1894

1895

1896

1897

1898

1899

1900

1901

1902

1903

1904