rethinking giftedness and talent in sport

18
265 The authors are with the School of Sport and Education, Brunel University, Uxbridge, UB8 3PH UK. Email: [email protected]. Quest 2006, 58, 265-282 © 2006 National Association for Kinesiology and Physical Education in Higher Education Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport Peter Tranckle and Christopher J. Cushion The purpose of this paper is to understand how gifts are discovered and talents developed within sport. The current literature is critically discussed, highlighting contributions and gaps in current knowledge. Due to issues concerning terminology and the nature versus nurture debate, research on talent faces challenges relating to continuity and meaningful progression. Williams and Reillyʼ s (2000) denitions, along with Gagnéʼ s Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent are introduced and used to organize and add conceptual clarity to the existing knowledge base. Using Gagnéʼ s model, areas for further research begin to evolve. A case is made for the qualitative exploration of how gifts are discovered and why people commit to talent development, noting the advantages of phenomenological methods. Talent is a rare and valuable resource for society and a great deal of potentially valuable human talent is wasted in every generation. This waste stems, essentially, from a lack of understanding and knowledge about the nature of talent and a failure to grasp what makes people motivated enough to develop talent (Csikszentmihalyi, Rathunde, & Whalen, 1993). Yet, as Gardner contends, “for a society concerned about survival, no issue is more important than cultivation of its talented young, no outcome more devastating than the loss of talented individuals” (cited in Csikszent- mihalyi et al., 1993, p. 310). Within the sporting context, the Australian Institute for Sport (AIS, 2001), for example, noted that there is a need to actively unearth talent in order for nations to remain internationally competitive. However, unearthing and developing talent is more than merely “turning esh into gold” (Lloyd, 1995). For the individual concerned, neglecting or ignoring gifts could lead to regret that could linger for the rest of their life. The value and importance of talent should not be underestimated, and it is perhaps unsurprising that talent, its detection and development, is a central concern to coaches, teachers, and researchers in sport sciences and education. This interest is based rmly on the premise that predicting future performance based on present knowledge underlies the competitive process (Régnier, Salmela, & Russell, 1993). However, as Régnier at al. (1993) suggest, “it is somewhat surprising that such an engaging enterprise has received so little attention from a broad conceptual stand- point” (p. 309). This is due, in part, to talent being an incredibly complex concept. 05Trankle(265).indd 265 05Trankle(265).indd 265 4/12/06 4:22:19 PM 4/12/06 4:22:19 PM

Upload: carlos-garcia

Post on 24-Oct-2014

134 views

Category:

Documents


19 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

265

The authors are with the School of Sport and Education, Brunel University, Uxbridge, UB8 3PH UK. Email: [email protected].

Quest 2006, 58, 265-282© 2006 National Association for Kinesiology and Physical Education in Higher Education

Rethinking Giftednessand Talent in Sport

Peter Tranckle and Christopher J. Cushion

The purpose of this paper is to understand how gifts are discovered and talents developed within sport. The current literature is critically discussed, highlighting contributions and gaps in current knowledge. Due to issues concerning terminology and the nature versus nurture debate, research on talent faces challenges relating to continuity and meaningful progression. Williams and Reillyʼs (2000) defi nitions, along with Gagnéʼs Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent are introduced and used to organize and add conceptual clarity to the existing knowledge base. Using Gagnéʼs model, areas for further research begin to evolve. A case is made for the qualitative exploration of how gifts are discovered and why people commit to talent development, noting the advantages of phenomenological methods.

Talent is a rare and valuable resource for society and a great deal of potentially valuable human talent is wasted in every generation. This waste stems, essentially, from a lack of understanding and knowledge about the nature of talent and a failure to grasp what makes people motivated enough to develop talent (Csikszentmihalyi, Rathunde, & Whalen, 1993). Yet, as Gardner contends, “for a society concerned about survival, no issue is more important than cultivation of its talented young, no outcome more devastating than the loss of talented individuals” (cited in Csikszent-mihalyi et al., 1993, p. 310). Within the sporting context, the Australian Institute for Sport (AIS, 2001), for example, noted that there is a need to actively unearth talent in order for nations to remain internationally competitive. However, unearthing and developing talent is more than merely “turning fl esh into gold” (Lloyd, 1995). For the individual concerned, neglecting or ignoring gifts could lead to regret that could linger for the rest of their life.

The value and importance of talent should not be underestimated, and it is perhaps unsurprising that talent, its detection and development, is a central concern to coaches, teachers, and researchers in sport sciences and education. This interest is based fi rmly on the premise that predicting future performance based on present knowledge underlies the competitive process (Régnier, Salmela, & Russell, 1993). However, as Régnier at al. (1993) suggest, “it is somewhat surprising that such an engaging enterprise has received so little attention from a broad conceptual stand-point” (p. 309). This is due, in part, to talent being an incredibly complex concept.

05Trankle(265).indd 26505Trankle(265).indd 265 4/12/06 4:22:19 PM4/12/06 4:22:19 PM

Page 2: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

266 Tranckle and Cushion

Yet when asked, most would recognize the word talent, and have a grasp of what it means. Diffi culty comes when terms such as talent, talent identifi cation, and talent development are frequently cited and often used interchangeably. Arguably, an absence of a clear defi nition has perpetuated this casual and frequently mislead-ing use of the word. This is well illustrated by Gagné (1996, 2000), who pointed out that talent has been used to describe two distinct things: raw material as well as the end product of a developmental process. This inconsistent use of terms has resulted in a lack of both defi nitional and conceptual clarity, leaving the fi eld, at times, imprecise and speculative.

A good starting point for developing a clearer defi nition of talent comes from Csikszentmihalyi et al. (1993), who argue that talent is socially constructed: “It is a label of approval we place on traits that have a positive value in the particular context in which we live” (p. 23). The social dimension to Csikzentmihalyi et al.ʼs (1993) defi nition is perhaps particularly relevant for sporting contexts. Of course, talents do not exist in a vacuum but in reality; these are relative to a given context and labeled only as talent when valued and approved by the subculture and wider society in which these exist. It would seem, then, that the complex nature of talent derives, in part, from an essentially relative quality. Indeed, it would seem that an acknowledgement of the social construction of talent is crucial for its defi nition. Put simply, therefore, talent can only be talent and recognized as such where it is valued.

Recently, Durand-Bush and Salmela (2002) drew upon Howe, Davidson, and Slobodaʼs (1998) list of properties associated with talent as a working defi nition:

(1) It originates in genetically transmitted structures and hence is at least partly innate. (2) Its full effects may not be evident at an early stage, but there will be some advance indications, allowing trained people to identify the presence of talent before exceptional levels of mature performance have been demon-strated. (3) These early indications of talent provide a basis for predicting who is likely to excel. (4) Only a minority are talented, for if all children were, there would be no way to predict or explain differential success. Finally, (5) talents are relatively domain-specifi c. (p. 399-400)

Although insightful, and indeed useful, Howeʼs list of properties describes the characteristics of talent but does not defi ne it. In a more recent contribution, Brown (2002) turned to dictionary defi nitions as a starting point, describing the word talent as “a special, natural ability” and “a capacity for achievement or success.” In his work, Brown recognized the absence of a defi nition that suited the sporting domain and acknowledged that dictionary defi nitions were too vague.

Arguably, within the sporting domain, there remains a need to develop a work-ing defi nition of talent, a common language that can contribute meaningfully to both research and practice. However, in some quarters there is instead a preoccupation with arguing the origins of talent. The nature versus nurture debate remains intrinsi-cally valuable; however, for some the resulting polarization of the area is a blind alley for the fi eld (Durand-Bush & Salmela, 2002; Howe et al., 1998). Arguably, it is more important to formulate clear defi nitions and a greater understanding of talent. This would allow attention to focus on how talent can be maximized in the lives of performers. To this end, it is somewhat surprising that the contribution of

05Trankle(265).indd 26605Trankle(265).indd 266 4/12/06 4:22:22 PM4/12/06 4:22:22 PM

Page 3: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 267

Gagnéʼs well-developed work from the educational fi eld has not been appreciated within the sporting domain. Indeed, Gagnéʼs work does not feature in any reviews of the talent literature pertaining to sport (e.g., Durand-Bush & Salmela, 2002; Régnier et al., 1993; Williams & Reilly, 2000).

The purpose of this paper is to understand and classify the knowledge base of talent research in an attempt to contribute to the development of a clearer concept of talent. This will be conducted through a critical evaluation of fi ndings, while considering these fi ndings in light of Gagnéʼs Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent (DMGT; 1993, 2000). The signifi cance of the paper is that such analyti-cal mapping provides us with a systematic and progressive way to evaluate and process both past and future research (Hart, 1998), with the ultimate aim to bring clarity and renewed direction to the fi eld. The paper is organized into four sections. The fi rst introduces Gagneʼs work and outlines terms. The second considers cur-rent talent research related to sport, specifi cally that relating to talent identifi cation and talent detection. The third seeks to organize the existing knowledge base in an attempt to add conceptual clarity, thus creating a picture of how we currently know the discovery of gifts and the development of talent. Throughout and where appropriate, the existing research is considered in light of Gagneʼs research. Using Gagnéʼs model as a conceptual framework, areas for further research begin to evolve, which are discussed in the fourth section as avenues for advancing current knowledge and understanding.

Gagné’s WorkGagné reifi ed talent essentially because of a dissatisfaction with the frequent,

broad, and all encompassing use of the word. Indeed, the terms talent, gifted, and aptitude can, and frequently are, used interchangeably and can be found in most dictionary defi nitions where any one of these words tends to be used to describe the other (e.g., Brown, 2002; Chambers, 1993; Webster, 1970). Furthermore, examples of how the words gifted and talented are used without distinction can be found in numerous Government reports both in the UK and overseas (e.g., Marland, 1972; OFSTED 2001; UK Youth Sports Trust, 2003). Even within the scientifi c literature, there is an assumption of common understanding and shared concepts, as defi nitions are rarely offered (e.g., Abbott & Collins, 2004; Côté, 1999).

Gagné suggested the one term fi ts all use of talent was unacceptable and proposed a clarifi cation by developing a defi nition based on meaning rather than description or properties. This resulted in Gagné making what is a useful distinction between the raw material at one end of a continuum, which he described as “. . . the possession and use of untrained and spontaneously expressed natural abilities (called aptitudes or gifts), in at least one ability domain, to a degree that places a child at least among the top 10% of his or her age peers” (Gagné, 2000, p. 67). Such aptitudes or gifts might result from maturation or informal learning. While at the other end of the continuum is the end product of development, which he described as “ . . . the superior mastery of systematically developed abilities (called competencies or talent) and knowledge in at least one fi eld of human activity to a degree that places a childʼs achievement within at least the upper 10% of age peers who are actively in that fi eld or fi elds” (Gagné, 2000, p. 67). This relatively

05Trankle(265).indd 26705Trankle(265).indd 267 4/12/06 4:22:22 PM4/12/06 4:22:22 PM

Page 4: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

268 Tranckle and Cushion

simple approach results in more conceptual clarity with two distinct but interrelated areas, namely giftedness and talent, instead of the current and common one term fi ts all approach.

Gagneʼs work is useful here as it offers a continuum, from aptitudes or gifts at one end, through a developmental process, to talent at the other. While it would be impossible to capture the moment a gift becomes a talent, it seems inappropriate to describe raw material and an end product as the same thing. Gagné demonstrates this and consolidates his defi nitions of giftedness and talent with the differentiated model of giftedness and talent (DMGT).

The (1993, 2000) model (Figure 1), outlined four broad domains that Gagné terms natural abilities. These are intellectual, creative, socio-affective, and senso-rimotor. Gagné suggests that these natural abilities or aptitudes can be described as gifts if they are in the top 10% of peers, for physical performance elements of a number of these domains can contribute toward talent development, such as the sensorimotor and creative gifts of a dancer. However, Gagné outlines these natural abilities in their raw form, where they have yet to undergo formal training. In terms of systematically developed skills, Gagné broadly categorizes academic, arts, busi-ness, leisure, social action, sports, and technology as fi elds of human endeavor; however, this was not intended as an exhaustive list. Gagné suggests that these systematically developed skills or expertise (as described by Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Römer, 1993) are the end product of the developmental process and may be described as talents if they reach the top 10% in that fi eld.

Impacting on the developmental process are three forms of catalyst that can have positive or negative effects on the process. Gagné called these intrapersonal catalysts, environmental catalysts, and chance. Intrapersonal catalysts include physical, motivation, volition, self-management, and personality. Environmental catalysts include milieu, other people, provisions, and events. When Gagné revised his model in 2000, he gave far more emphasis to chance, linking it clearly to natu-ral abilities, intrapersonal and environmental catalysts. The link between chance and natural abilities relates to Gagnéʼs belief that gifts are genetically determined. Similarly, the link between intrapersonal catalysts, chance, and the developmental process relates to the suitability of the individual to the fi eld of expertise in which they are trying to develop skills. The fi nal link with chance is to environmental catalysts; this relates to the chance of an individual being born within a certain culture, surrounded by certain people, having access to provisions and the effects of positive or negative events in their lives. Although Gagné has often suggested the relevance of the model for sports as well as other fi elds (Gagné, 1993), the model has yet to receive any recognition within sports research or indeed even be considered in the sports talent literature.

What We Know So Far: Talent Identifi cationAptitudes or gifts can be evident in sport at various levels of performance and

at different levels of participation, from non-participant to elite performer. As we have already discussed, different terms have arisen to refer to the interest in these aptitudes or gifts at various stages. Bearing in mind that the sport domain rarely

05Trankle(265).indd 26805Trankle(265).indd 268 4/12/06 4:22:22 PM4/12/06 4:22:22 PM

Page 5: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 269

Fig

ure

1—

Gag

neʼs

(20

00)

Dif

fere

ntia

ted

Mod

el o

f G

ifte

dnes

s an

d Ta

lent

. Rep

rint

ed w

ith p

erm

issi

on.

05Trankle(265).indd 26905Trankle(265).indd 269 4/12/06 4:22:23 PM4/12/06 4:22:23 PM

Page 6: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

270 Tranckle and Cushion

differentiates between gifts and talents, two of the most commonly used terms in the literature are talent identifi cation and talent development.

Through a combination of maturation and informal learning, differences can be noticed in the initial aptitudes of participants to a given sport. Talent identifi cation, and sometimes talent detection, are terms used to describe when the early indica-tions of aptitudes or giftedness are assessed. These terms have largely been used interchangeably to describe the recruitment of gifted people into sport, as well as the identifi cation of gifted and talented people already within a sport. For example, talent identifi cation was used to describe the recognition of gifts in participants in a number of studies (e.g., Kozel, 1996; Malina, 1997; Prescott 1996; Williams & Reilly, 2000). However, further muddying the waters, the recognition of giftedness in current participants has also been described as talent detection (Bompa,1985; Régnier et al., 1993). While talent detection was used by Harre (1982) to describe the recruitment of gifted people into sport, the term talent identifi cation has also been used by the Australian Institute of Sport, in reference to recruitment of school-aged children (AIS, 2001). Although the AIS used talent identifi cation as an all-encompassing umbrella term, it also used the term talent search to describe the title of its recruitment program. In these simple examples it is possible to see how confusion is possible when terms such as talent detection and talent identifi cation are used inconsistently and interchangeably.

The talent literature in sport has begun to recognize this lack of clarity surround-ing the term talent, and in an article about talent identifi cation and development in soccer, Williams and Reilly (2000) attempted to defi ne and make a clear distinction between talent detection, talent identifi cation, and talent development, among other terms. They described talent detection as “the discovery of potential performers who are currently not involved in the sport in question” (p. 658). Similarly, Hohmann and Seidel (2003) also described talent detection as the detection of talented children based on their initial performances before sport specifi c training. Linking these defi nitions to Gagnéʼs work we can see that talent detection is clearly concerned with how gifts are discovered, leading to recruitment based on such gifts.

Williams and Reilly further clarifi ed their terms, narrowing their use of the term talent identifi cation to mean “the process of recognising current participants with the potential to become elite players” (2000, p. 658). This work is particularly useful as it clearly supports the notion that talent detection and identifi cation are in fact two separate processes. Thus describing two different ways that talent can be discovered, giving more focused meaning and clearer distinction to the terms talent detection and identifi cation. Consequently, the talent identifi cation process is often the start of talent development.

A feature of all three defi nitions is the particular use of the word potential. This may have been a conscious effort on the part of Williams and Reilly to ensure that their readers would not confuse initial attributes that have not yet been developed, with the refl ection of talents in expert performance. Considering the common aim running through all three stages, it would appear that Williams and Reillyʼs (2000) terms are also consistent with Gagnéʼs distinction between gifts and talents. There-fore, talent detection, identifi cation, and development as defi ned by Williams and Reilly (2000) aim to detect, identify, and develop “future” talent.

05Trankle(265).indd 27005Trankle(265).indd 270 4/12/06 4:22:23 PM4/12/06 4:22:23 PM

Page 7: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 271

Talent DetectionFor those who value talent detection (AIS, 2001; Bompa, 1985), how individu-

als fi nd a domain to express their aptitudes or gifts is of central importance. Gagné s̓ (2000) model suggested that people hold aptitudes at various levels in intellectual, creative, socio-affective, and sensorimotor domains. Gagne also proposes that these aptitudes or gifts, if extraordinary, usually show up as accelerated learning in different tasks (Gagné, 1993).

The sport specifi c talent detection literature, however, has been driven by the assumption that the underlying factors prerequisite for sporting excellence exist. As such, researchers have tried to uncover the structures, abilities, and traits that might explain performance (Régnier et al., 1993). This line of research has suf-fered through issues of validity and generalizability. A succinct summary of prob-lematic nature of this is provided by Régnier and Salmela (1987), who reminded the academic community of Heilbrunʼs (1966) thoughts of human potentiality: “In defi ning human potentiality, we refer to that which exists in possibility, not actuality, whereas in testing procedures we evaluate that which exists in actuality, not in possibility” (cited in, Régnier et al., 1993, p. 290). This supports Geronʼs (1978) fi ndings where she realized that what it took to become a champion was different from what it was to be a champion. Moreover, defi ciencies in one area of performance could be compensated for by high levels in another, bringing in a “compensation phenomenon,” adding to the diffi culty of identifying reliable variables (Bartmus, Neumann, & de Marées, 1987).

Indeed, research suggests that aptitude or giftedness is an unreliable predictor of future levels of performance (Bartmus et al., 1987; Bloom, 1985). The coachʼs judgment remains the best solution for detecting or identifying potential talent (Bartmus et al., 1987). It is also worth noting that some evidence has been advanced to substantiate the ability of expert observers to detect giftedness (Thomas & Thomas, 1999). In addition to such criticism, there have also been concerns raised over the ethics of such talent selection. For example, Régnier et al. (1993) raised the concern over what they described as “talent elimination,” suggesting that talent selection efforts could eliminate, or discourage, individuals from participating. This concern was also raised by Malina (1997) who indicated that talent selec-tion programs have problems related to decision making, exclusion, economic discrimination, and discrimination along maturation lines. In addition, Howe et al. (1998) suggests that young people who were not identifi ed as talented were likely to be denied the help and encouragement they would need to attain high levels of competence.

Despite these concerns, scientifi c selection programs like Talent Search (AIS, 2001) remain an idealistic convention of interested and well-resourced societies. However, this is not to underestimate the power of individuals. For example, informed recommendations have been made by experienced teachers that can help individuals to fi nd a fi eld of expertise. Indeed, there remain examples of where teachers intervene in choices or inspire students to try something new because they think they may fi nd something meaningful (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1993). In

05Trankle(265).indd 27105Trankle(265).indd 271 4/12/06 4:22:24 PM4/12/06 4:22:24 PM

Page 8: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

272 Tranckle and Cushion

addition, Bloom (1985) also stressed the importance of teachers in the early years of talent development. Although teacher recommendations are not as structured as a national talent detection program (e.g., Talent Search), they can, arguably, be far more informed than natural selection.

Despite this, natural selection, such as an individual taking part in a sport as a result of local infl uence like school tradition, parents ̓wishes, or peers, remains a signifi cant method for talent detection (Bompa, 1985; Côté, 1999). Whether children fi nd a fi eld of expertise themselves or through their parents, they would probably have been infl uenced by presocialization (Donnelly & Young, 1988). This is a process where individuals form preconceptions, often stereotypically about a sport subculture, usually through peers, family, or media. If these preconceptions were appealing, they would serve to prepare the individual for the subculture in perhaps a rather naive way. Due to the public image of various sports in the media, presocialization plays an important role in natural selection in sport. This form of talent detection is very similar to Côtéʼs (1999) notion of sampling various activi-ties until children fi nd one that they identify with.

Gagnéʼs work is particularly useful when considering the detection and identifi cation of talent and has the potential, through his clear defi nitions of gifts and talent, to demystify some of the current confusion. Indeed, these defi nitions arguably present a clearer viewpoint from which to make sense of talent literature by distinguishing between raw material (gifts) and the end product of formal learn-ing (talent). Furthermore, Gagnéʼs work suggests a positive solution to the fi eldʼs dilemma concerning Heilbrunʼs notion of potentiality. That is, that aptitude, or giftedness if extraordinary, can be recognized by the rate of learning, rather than a level of ability (Gagné, 2003). This is a useful contribution as it encourages selectors engaged in identifi cation and detection to take a more careful look at an individualʼs aptitude for learning within a domain rather than what they have already been fortunate enough to have been taught.

Committing to a Talent FieldAlthough talent detection programs and presocialization offer interesting

insight, they do not directly offer an explanation as to why individuals commit to a talent fi eld. Csikszentmihalyi et al. (1993) proposed that gifted individuals were more prone to “fl ow” and that such autotelic experiences could motivate an individual to replicate the experience again and again. In addition, Côté (1999) noted that “the main reason that adolescents choose to specialise in one activity over others resides in the positive value associated with the experience” (p. 404). Moreover, Côté (1999) also noted the importance of critical incidents in the deci-sion to pursue an activity, such incidents being positive experiences with a coach, encouragement from an older sibling or friends, or simply enjoying the activity.

While Côté (1999) describes the stages of development that gifted individu-als progress through, commitment to a talent fi eld, and the impetus for entering a development process is less understood, particularly from the performers ̓perspec-tive. In a similar vein to critical incidents, but more related to the individual, are Walters and Gardnerʼs (1986) crystallizing experiences. Crystallizing experiences have yet to be identifi ed or researched in sport; however, it has been theorized that autotelic experiences in sport, such as fl ow, can have personal meaning for

05Trankle(265).indd 27205Trankle(265).indd 272 4/12/06 4:22:24 PM4/12/06 4:22:24 PM

Page 9: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 273

individuals that are almost as profound. Walters and Gardner (1986) note that unlike fl ow, crystallizing experiences are not always fully appreciated at the time, their impact only being assessed retrospectively. There are two types of crystallizing experience, initial and refi ning (Walters & Gardner, 1986). Initial crystallizing experiences are those that might occur early in life, connecting an individual to a general domain of giftedness, such as sensorimotor. These are the experiences that set an individual off on the exploration of the domain in which they feel an affi nity. Later, in the development process, a refi ning crystallizing experience might draw an individual to a particular talent fi eld. An example of a refi ning experience might be an individual, developing their sensorimotor gifts, who fi nds a particular talent fi eld to express fully their gifts and develop talent. This would be where an individual becomes connected to the talent fi eld, through their developing identity as a talented performer within that fi eld.

In light of Gagnéʼs (2000) Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent, crystallizing experiences could facilitate a clearer understanding of how individuals become committed to a talent fi eld and by what mechanisms, which is relevant to the detection and identifi cation of talent. Talent detection and subsequent identi-fi cation is impotent unless the selectors have the means to inspire the individuals to commit to the developmental process.

The Development Process: Gagne’s DMGTand Factors Impacting Talent Development

Despite the discovery of giftedness, talent will not necessarily evolve (Gagné, 2003). Just because individuals possess favorable attributes does not guarantee their future as talented performers. Even with giftedness, a myriad of favorable intrapersonal qualities and environmental factors must manifest at the right time to facilitate the development of talent. It is for this reason that the journey neces-sary to transform gifts into talent has recently been described as multidimensional (Abbott & Collins, 2004). Indeed, with its consideration of intrapersonal and environmental catalysts, natural abilities, the developmental process, and chance, Gagnéʼs DMGT can be described as multidimensional and refl ects the complexity of the talent development process.

Consequently, it would be useful to position current research within this frame-work. This is a useful exercise because existing knowledge needs to be organized in the context of a multidimensional process of talent development thus enabling an assessment of what we know and what is yet to be explored. What will follow then is an examination of the current research regarding the talent development process as it relates to Gagnéʼs DMGT, specifi cally the learning process, intraper-sonal catalysts, environmental catalysts, and the infl uence of chance.

What Gagne Offers:The Development Process

The development process is how we recognize the transformation of gifts into talent. Gagné (2003) suggests that this process can take four different forms that can be classifi ed as maturation, informal learning, formal noninstitutional learning,

05Trankle(265).indd 27305Trankle(265).indd 273 4/12/06 4:22:24 PM4/12/06 4:22:24 PM

Page 10: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

274 Tranckle and Cushion

and formal institutional learning. Maturation refers to the growth and transforma-tion of biological structures such as bones and internal organs, including the brain. Gagné (2003) notes that the maturation process is completely controlled by the genome. Contrary to this physical view of maturation, forms of learning are more infl uenced by environmental factors. Informal learning refers to the knowledge and skills that can be acquired almost unintentionally during daily activities. However, formal noninstitutional and institutional learning refer to intentional self-teaching and being taught respectively. Although Gagné briefl y describes the developmental process, he has purposely avoided being too specifi c so that his model can be easily applied to different contexts, thus facilitating its use as a framework for viewing empirical research. Therefore, the learning process can be further understood by considering and integrating the work of Bloom (1985), Côté (1999), and Ericsson et al. (1993).

Within our understanding of the developmental process, Côté (1999) and Bloomʼs (1985) work serves to paint a fuller picture of how seemingly simple demonstrations of aptitude can be nurtured into remarkable talents. Furthermore, Ericsson et al. (1993) adds a temporal dimension to the development of talent with their 10 year or 10,000 hour rule for the development of expertise, which sits well conceptually with Gagnéʼs (2003) work.

As Bloomʼs phases of learning were based on the development of musical talent, Côté (1999) refi ned them, making them more sport specifi c to create his own “stages of learning,” consisting of the sampling years, specializing years, and investment years. These stages show the shifting emphasis on play and practice in the development of expertise. A particular feature of Côtéʼs (1999) stages was the recognition that the development of expertise or talent in sports usually occurs before the age of 18. This has lead to Côtéʼs (1999) stages all but superseding Bloomʼs phases within the understanding of talent development in sport. The popu-larity of Côtéʼs stages of learning is evident from their prevalence in the literature and more recent conceptual models. For example, Abbott and Collins (2004) use Côtéʼs (1999) stages of learning to illustrate how, as they describe it, potential is transformed into performance.

The advantage of viewing Côtéʼs stages of learning within Gagnéʼs (2000) DMGT is that gaps within our knowledge become apparent. Although Côté s̓ stages augment our understanding of how informal and formal learning develop talent, the role of maturation in this process lacks the same careful attention. For example how do genetics and biological development contribute to initial differences within indi-viduals? Arguably, a more encompassing view of the development process makes differences between performers with similar training more understandable.

Intrapersonal and Environmental Catalysts

Within the development process, intrapersonal and environmental catalysts constantly interact with and impact talent development. As Gagné (2003) notes, these catalysts can be examined with regard to their direction (i.e., positive or negative infl uence) and strength of causal impact. Other models tend to take a more deterministic stance by only showing factors that should positively and strongly infl uence the development of talent (e.g., Abbott & Collins, 2004). By suggesting that these catalysts are contributive rather than determining, the DMGT offers a

05Trankle(265).indd 27405Trankle(265).indd 274 4/12/06 4:22:25 PM4/12/06 4:22:25 PM

Page 11: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 275

more considered framework for viewing research that adds to our knowledge of the talent development process.

Gagné (2003) subdivides intrapersonal catalysts into physical and psychologi-cal factors, all under the partial infl uence of genetics. The physical factors relate to characteristics, handicaps, and health. Indeed, Gagné (2003) notes that dance schools and sports with defi ned physical templates often base selection on physi-cal parameters. Certainly, physical parameters form a crucial fi rst part of the AIS (2001) Talent Search Program.

The psychological factors of motivation, volition, self-management, and personality interrelate considerably, particularly where the fi rst three factors are concerned. The broader use of the term motivation would probably encompass these three factors, but Gagné pulls on the work of Kuhl and Beckmann (1985) to form a distinction between motivation and volition to show how motivation can activate, direct, and maintain behavior, while volition relates to the intensity of such behavior. Abbott and Collins (2004) also note that motivation is a (if not the) crucial determining factor in developing and maintaining talent. Furthermore, self-management and psychological characteristics such as goal setting, realistic performance evaluation, imagery, and commitment have been found to differentiate between medal and non-medal winners (Gagné, 1999; Gagné, Neveu, Simard & St.Pere, 1996; Gould, Eklund, & Jackson 1992; Orlick, Hansen, Reed, & OʼHara, 1979).

The relevance of both physical and psychological intrapersonal catalysts are akin to Ericsson et al. s̓ (1993) theory that motivational and effort constraints must be overcome to achieve expertise. Motivational constraints were based on the assump-tion that deliberate practice would neither be pleasant or immediately rewarding but necessary to eventually develop expertise. Effort constraints referred to dealing with the demands of deliberate practice while avoiding injury and burnout. These constraints correspond well with Galtonʼs (1869/1979) work that noted a need for individuals to have adequate power to do a great deal of very laborious work.

Although the notion of personality as a predictor of talent was a popular topic for research between the 1950s and 1970s, this body of research was inconclusive (Abbott & Collins, 2004). Even though personality did not make its way into Abbott and Collins ̓more deterministic model of talent development (2004), Gagné still maintained it within his more contributive DMGT (Gagné, 2000, 2003). He consid-ered that personality might still affect the developmental process, either positively or negatively, however subtly. Bearing in mind that intrapersonal catalysts can also interact directly with the environment (Gagné, 2003), the possibility exists that the relationship between the personalities of coach and gifted individual might be signifi cant (Lyle, 2002).

Although Abbott and Collins ̓(2004) model presents the positive infl uence of physical and psychological factors, they do not emphasize the potential for these factors to also work negatively. Furthermore, Abbott and Collins (2004) do not consider the role of environmental factors within their model. Given the contributions of Csikszentmihalyi et al. (1993), it should be recognized that the developmental process does not take place within a vacuum and environmental factors cannot be ignored.

Within the environmental catalysts, Gagné (1993) notes that mileau, persons, provisions, and events can act upon the developmental process. Mileau refers to

05Trankle(265).indd 27505Trankle(265).indd 275 4/12/06 4:22:25 PM4/12/06 4:22:25 PM

Page 12: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

276 Tranckle and Cushion

the macroscopic factors of geography and sociology and microscopic factors of family size and socioeconomic status. This overlaps to some extent with provi-sions, which relate to the various programs or services that might be available to develop talent in various fi elds. Certainly these factors will infl uence what talent fi elds are available for gifted individuals to pursue; however, there remains a dearth of research within this area of talent development. Although both Côté (1999) and Bloom (1985) focus considerable attention upon the role of the family, theirs is a more psychological rather than sociological analysis.

Arguably, one of the most crucial environmental catalysts is the role played by the teacher or coach (Bloom, 1985; Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1993; Walters & Gardner, 1986). In Bloomʼs (1985) phases of learning, the role of the teacher was shown to change through the phases. Initially, teachers were very motivating, almost inspirational (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1993), but throughout the phases of learning, they became more business like, only working with outstanding individuals and demanding great perfection (Bloom, 1985). Bloom went on to suggest that it would be very rare for the same teacher to progress an individual through all three phases because of the different requirements at each stage of talent development.

In addition to the infl uence of teachers and coaches during the developmental process, Côté (1999) noted that a greater infl uence during childhood came from the family. Although Bloom (1985) provided a parental perspective in his study of talent development, more recent work (e.g., Côté, 1999) has looked at the infl uence of the family as a whole, including siblings.

Furthermore, a number of important intrapersonal catalysts (Gagné, 2000) that affect the developmental process are cultivated within the family environment. For example, parents of committed individuals across a range of domains have been found to promote values related to the importance of achievement, hard work, suc-cess, persistence, and being active (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1993; Monsaas, 1985; Sloboda & Howe, 1991; Sloan, 1985; Sosniak, 1985). Furthermore, Côté (1999) also found that older siblings could have an important part to play in the develop-ment of the work ethic through their own behavior as role models.

The appropriate level of parental involvement in sport was theorized by Hellstedt (1987) as a continuum from under involvement to over involvement. However, Côté (1999) gave a more dynamic view of parental involvement by describing the shifting roles through the sampling, specializing, and investment years of talent development. This presented the fi eld with a detailed framework that recognized the complexity of the development process, providing a means for analysis, research, and understanding.

Although considerable attention has been paid to the roles of the coach (Bloom, 1985; Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1993) and the roles of the parents (Côté, 1999; Hell-stedt, 1987), these roles are rarely viewed together. By comparing Bloomʼs (1985) phases of learning with Côtéʼs (1999) stages of learning, a more comprehensive view of their contrasting roles emerge. This highlights the potential for taking a more focused look at the contrasting roles of these individuals.

The fi nal environmental catalyst and perhaps the one most infl uenced by chance are called “events.” By this Gagné (1993) refers to winning competitions; suffering bereavements, illness, or accidents; or getting married, certainly any events that might facilitate or hinder, even permanently derail, the pursuit of talent. Although signifi cant events have been examined within the discovery of giftedness (Walters

05Trankle(265).indd 27605Trankle(265).indd 276 4/12/06 4:22:25 PM4/12/06 4:22:25 PM

Page 13: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 277

& Garner, 1986), the signifi cance of such events upon the development of talent is perhaps more a matter of common sense than empirical research. Nonetheless, the importance of environmental factors in the development of talent cannot be ignored.

Chance

As environmental catalysts are akin to the notion of opportunity, the various factors can be viewed with a sense of luck. Indeed, Gagné (1985) originally placed chance within the environmental catalysts but later separated chance because of its infl uence throughout the components of the DMGT. From the endowment of aptitudes to the development of expertise, the infl uence of chance is ever present. In the fi rst instance, Atkinson (1978) claims that all human accomplishment can be ascribed to two crucial rolls of the dice that determine an individual s̓ circumstances of birth and background. Gagné (2003) also notes that chance is the most prominent component in his DMGT. Not only does chance determine genetic endowment and therefore infl uencing aptitudes and intrapersonal catalysts, chance is also evident within the environmental catalysts of mileau, persons, provisions, and events.

Although chance has been recognized and examined by some theorists (Bloom, 1985; Gagné 2000; Tannenbaum, 1983), it is often overlooked when conducting research into giftedness and talent (e.g., Abbott & Collins, 2004; Durand-Bush & Salmela, 2001; Régnier et al. 1993). For researchers who strive to predict talent from giftedness, the notion of chance in talent development might seem an unpalatable consideration simply because it is an uncontrollable variable. Prediction rests upon the assumption that giftedness can predict talent. However, the presence of chance disrupts the predictive value of giftedness and clearly makes the understanding of the talent development process hugely problematic. This is particularly the case with a nomothetic approach to understanding and predicting talent. However, accepting that all components of talent development are subject to chance logically suggests the need for more fl exible approaches to the developmental process that in turn encompasses and refi nes its multidimensional nature.

Advancing Knowledge and Understanding:A Research Agenda

Considering the numerous perspectives on talent development (Abbott & Col-lins, 2004; Bloom, 1985; Côté, 1999; Gagné, 2000; Howe et al., 1998; Régnier et al., 1993), the summary of the state of knowledge and understanding provided by Kozel (1996) remains applicable:

Talent is an extremely complex attribute; genetically determined, complicated in structure and subject to environmental conditions. It is for this reason that there is no consensus of opinion, nationally or internationally, regarding the theory and methodology of talent identifi cation, selection and training in sport, although sport science research continues to identify the required character-istics for elite performance. Generally, it is still the coachʼs eye and expertʼs judgment which is decisive in the talent screening and selection process. (Kozel, 1996, p. 5)

05Trankle(265).indd 27705Trankle(265).indd 277 4/12/06 4:22:25 PM4/12/06 4:22:25 PM

Page 14: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

278 Tranckle and Cushion

In this paper we have attempted to address issues surrounding terminology and the current lack of conceptual clarity within the talent fi eld by using Gagnéʼs DMGT theory as a frame of reference. Arguably, any future research efforts will be hampered until some consensus is reached over these issues, as it may remain unclear if researchers are investigating gifts, or talents, or both, or indeed using one to describe or assess the other.

While the research dedicated to identifying and developing talent in sport is considerable (in excess of 80 articles since 1975), there remains a dearth of literature based on in-depth analysis of experiences or insight of those at the very heart of the talent process, i.e., coaches, performers, and their signifi cant others. Notable exceptions include the work of Bloom (1985), Csikszentmihalyi et al. (1993), and Côté, (1999). Several authors (Bartmus et al., 1987; Kozel, 1996, inter-alia) have reported that the assessments of expert coaches remain more reliable than any talent detection system devised from sports sciences. A worthy addition to the literature, therefore, would be an exploration of what pertinent information these coaches utilize as indicators of talent (Régnier et al., 1993; William & Reilly, 2000). Clearly a step in the right direction to explore this topic would be to consider the coaches ̓mechanisms for talent detection and relate their views as practitioners to some of the theoretical work that currently exists on talent, including Gagné.

How individuals come to discover their gifts and develop their talent is as equally subjective as the judgment of coaches. While commitment to a talent fi eld has been touched on earlier, their remains a number of research issues that require further elucidation to better connect individuals with a given talent fi eld in sport. These include, for example, Csikszentmihalyi et al.ʼs (1993) fl ow experience and the connection between fl ow and talent and fl ow and commitment to talent fi elds, Walter and Gardnerʼs (1986) crystallizing experiences and the life altering impact they appear to have, and fi nally a deeper understanding of the perspective of parents involved in discovery and development of gifts and talent. Clearly, sport achieve-ment does not just emerge, and investigating the perspectives and experiences of those involved remains an important step toward understanding and improving the processes of identifi cation and development. To further develop talent research and to explore the perspectives of coaches, performers, and their families, an approach is required that can grasp subjectivity and facilitate understanding of how giftedness and talent is perceived. To this end, consideration must be given to both careful research design and paradigm to assist in bringing progression and continuity to the fi eld of study.

Arguably, the most valuable contributions (Bloom, 1985; Côté, 1999; Csik-szentmihalyi et al., 1993) seemed to take particular care in creating appropriate research design. In particular, Bloom (1985) studied talent retrospectively by inter-viewing individuals who had already developed talent, while Csikszentmihalyi et al. (1993) studied the experiences of gifted teenagers developing talent by selecting participants who had been identifi ed as gifted by their teachers.

There can be no denying that the label of giftedness and talent can be subjective and complicated and as such it is important to appreciate how that subjectivity is viewed. This is illustrated well when considering the nature/nurture debate, with mixed responses refl ecting how values and beliefs can create different perspec-tives (Howe et al., 1998). Indeed, discussions and beliefs concerning the origins of giftedness can angle the perspectives of not only coaches, performers and their

05Trankle(265).indd 27805Trankle(265).indd 278 4/12/06 4:22:26 PM4/12/06 4:22:26 PM

Page 15: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 279

families, but also researchers. We believe the debate to be moot; however, a coach who believes in environmental determinism may see giftedness very differently from a coach who believes in genetic determinism. This is important because evidence suggests that coaches may recruit performers who share similar values, resulting in congruence between the value framework of coach and performer (Lyle, 2002). Therefore, it is crucial that further research also explores the values that are constructing a participantʼs subjective view.

Certainly the exploration of values and perspectives falls outside of the focus of, and is arguably incongruous, with a postivistic approach to research. The pow-erful knowledge and perspectives of coaches, performers, and their families are both personal and individualistic, which mitigates against any form of generalized positivistic inquiry (Jones, Armour, & Potrac, 2003). Therefore, there is a need for a more interpretive approach that can embrace and capture the subjectivity of participants. Phenomenology is one such approach that can explore the essence of human experience by preserving the perspectives of the participants. It was almost thirty years ago when a case was fi rst made to use phenomenology for sport-related inquiry (Whitson, 1976), which is still called for today (Kerry & Armour, 2000). Indeed, Bain (1995) has indicated a need to strive for a greater balance between scientifi c and subjective knowledge in sport related inquiry.

In order to facilitate the reconstruction of subjective views, phenomenology would seem to be an appropriate method for exploring this area, as it attempts to preserve the essence of the participants ̓perspectives. Indeed, although a number of qualitative strategies could be employed, interviews have already proven to be revealing in this area (Bloom, 1985; Côté, 1999). Perhaps a more phenomenological exploration of the perspectives of coaches, gifted individuals, and their parents will offer new insight into how gifts are discovered and talents developed.

ConclusionThis paper has presented a picture of the current state of knowledge regarding

the discovery of gifts and the development of talent in sport. As well as presenting research from both the sporting and educational fi elds, Gagnéʼs DMGT (2000), together with defi nitions from Williams and Reilly (2000), have been applied to clarify terminology. In particular, it has been demonstrated that utilizing the DMGT is one way that can facilitate a more structured view of current research in sport, therefore facilitating more continuity and a more solid foundation upon which to advance knowledge.

By using the DMGT to organize research from sport, it can be seen that promising contributions have been made in the areas of intrapersonal (Durand-Bush & Salmela, 1993) and environmental catalysts (Côté, 1999), the develop-mental process (Côté, 1999; Ericsson et al., 1993), and systematically developed skills (Brown, 2002). This demonstrates somewhat of an imbalance in the sports talent literature, with the area of talent development receiving considerably more research attention, while the area of discovering gifts has been largely neglected. However, the education fi eld has recognized the importance of this area and has already made some valuable contributions (Bloom, 1985; Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1993; Walters & Gardner, 1986) that may usefully be considered and developed with sport in mind.

05Trankle(265).indd 27905Trankle(265).indd 279 4/12/06 4:22:26 PM4/12/06 4:22:26 PM

Page 16: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

280 Tranckle and Cushion

In order to advance knowledge, it has been proposed that a greater understand-ing of how individuals discover their gifts may be uncovered by exploring the perspectives of coaches, gifted individuals, and their families. Gaps in the literature need to be addressed concerning signifi cant events, values, beliefs, and meanings associated with the discovery of gifts. Whether giftedness and talent are a reality or myth (Howe et al., 1998), they are perceived, and the infl uence of that perception remains real and powerful. Research in the fi eld of giftedness and talent in sport will not be easy because of its complex and subjective nature (Kozel, 1996). How-ever, a greater understanding will have value not only for sport, but for societies concerned about their own survival (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1993).

ReferencesAbbott, A., & Collins, D. (2004). Eliminating the dichotomy between theory and practice

in talent identifi cation and development: Considering the role of psychology. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22, 395-408.

Atkinson, J.W. (1978). Motivational determinants of intellective performance and cumula-tive achievement. In J.W. Atkinson & J.O. Raynor (Eds.), Personality, motivation, and achievement (pp. 221-242). New York: Wiley.

Australian Institute of Sport (2001). Talent Search. Australian Institute of Sport. Retrieved October 10, 2001, from http://www.ais.org.au/talent.htm.

Bain, L.L. (1995). Mindfulness and subjective knowledge. Quest, 47, 238-253.Bartmus, U., Neumann,. E., & de Marées, H. (1987). The talent problem in sports. Interna-

tional Journal of Sports Medicine, 8, 415-416Bloom, B.S. (1985). Developing talent in the young. New York: Ballantine Books.Bompa, T.O. (1985). Talent Identifi cation. Science Periodical On Research and Technology

in Sport (February). Ottawa: Coaching Association of Canada.Brown, J. (2002). Sports Talent. How to identify and develop outstanding athletes. Cham-

paign, IL: Human Kinetics.Chambers (1993). The Chambers dictionary. Edinburgh: W & R Chambers.Côté, J.(1999). The infl uence of the family in development of talent in sport. The Sport

Psychologist, 13, 395-417.Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented teenagers: The roots

of success and failure. New York: Cambridge University Press.Donnelly, P., & Young, K. (1988). The construction and confi rmation of identity in sports

subcultures. Sociology of Sport Journal, 5, 223-240.Durand-Bush, N., & Salmela, J.H., (2002). The development of talent in sport. In R.N.

Singer, H.A. Hausenblas, & C.M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology ( pp. 269-289). New York: Wiley.

Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., & Tesch- Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363-406.

Gagné, F. (1985). Giftedness and talent: Re-examining a re-examination of the defi nitions. Gifted Childs Quarterly, 29, 103-112.

Gagné, F. (1993). Constructs and models pertaining to exceptional human abilities. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mönks, & A.H. Passow (Eds.), International handbook of research and development of giftedness and talent (pp. 63-85). Oxford: Pergamon press.

Gagné, F. (1996). A thoughtful look at talent development. [On-Line] Available: www.coeunt.edu/gifted/resources/Gagné.htm.

Gagné, F. (1999). The multigifts of multitalented individuals. In S. Cline & K.T. Hegeman (Eds.), Gifted education in the twenty-fi rst century: Issues and concerns (pp. 17-45). Delray Beach, FL: Winslow Press.

05Trankle(265).indd 28005Trankle(265).indd 280 4/12/06 4:22:26 PM4/12/06 4:22:26 PM

Page 17: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

Giftedness and Talent in Sport 281

Gagné, F. (2000). Understanding the complete choreography of talent development through DMGT-based analysis. In K.A. Heller (Ed.), International handbook of giftedness and talent (2nd ed., pp. 67-79). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science Ltd.

Gagné, F. (2003). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT as a developmental theory. In N. Colangelo & G.A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education, (3rd ed.; pp. 60-74). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Gagné, F., Neveu, F., Simard, L., & St. Peré, F. (1996). How a search for multitalented individuals challenged the concept itself. Gifted and talented international, 11, 4-10.

Galton, F. (1979). Heredity genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Julian Friedman Publishers. (Original work published 1869).

Geron, E. (1978). Psychological assessment of sport giftedness. In U. Simri (Ed.), Proceed-ings of the International Symposium on Psychological Assessment in Sport (pp. 216-231). Netyana. Israel: Wingate Institute for Physical Education and Sport.

Gould, D., Eklund, R.C., & Jackson, S.A. (1992). 1988 U.S. Olympic wrestling excellence: I. Mental preparation, precompetitive cognition, and affect. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 358-382.

Harre, D. (1982) Trainingslebre. Berlin, Germany: SportverlagHart, C. (1998). Doing a literature review: Releasing the social science research imagina-

tion. London: Sage.Heilbrun, A.B. (1966) Testing for potentialities. In H.A. Otto (Ed.), Explorations in poten-

tialities (pp. 286-292). Springfi eld, IL: Thomas.Hellstedt, J.C. (1987). The coach / parent / athlete relationship. The Sport Psychologist, 1,

151-160.Hohmann, A., & Seidel, I. (2003) Scientifi c aspects of talent development. International

Journal of Sport Psychology, 40(1), 9-20.Howe, M.J.A., Davidson, J.W., & Sloboda, J.A., (1998), Innate talents: Reality or myth?

Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 21, 399-442.Jones, R.L., Armour, K.M., & Potrac, P. (2003). Constructing expert knowledge: A case

study of a top-level professional soccer coach. Sport, Education and Society, 8(2), 213-229.

Kerry D.S., & Armour, K.M. (2000). Sport sciences and the promise of phenomenology: Philosophy, method, and insight. Quest, 52, 1-17.

Kozel, J. (1996). Talent identifi cation and development in Germany. Coaching Focus, 31, 5-6 .

Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1985). Action control: From cognition to behaviour. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Lloyd, B. (1995). Blinded by science. Inside Sport, March.Lyle, J. (2002). Sports coaching concepts. A framework for coaches ̓behaviour. London &

New York: Routledge.Malina, R.M., (1997). Talent identifi cation and selection in sport. Spotlight on youth sports,

20(1), 1-3.Marland, S.P. (1972). Education of the gifted and talented: Report to the congress of the

United States by the U.S. commissioner of education. Washington, DC: U.S. Govern-ment Printing Offi ce.

Monsaas, J.A. (1985). Learning to be a world class tennis player. In B.S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 211-269). New York: Ballantine Books.

OFSTED (2001). Providing for gifted and talented pupils: An evaluation of excellence in cities and other grant-funded programmes.[On-Line] Available: www.ofsted.gov.uk

Orlick, T.D., Hansen, H., Reed, A., & OʼHara, T. (1979). Psychological attributes and on-ice indicators of high calibre hockey players. In J. Terauds & H.J. Gros (Eds.), Science in skiing, skating and hockey: Proceedings of the international congress of sports sciences (pp. 151-157). Del Mar, CA: Academic publishers.

05Trankle(265).indd 28105Trankle(265).indd 281 4/12/06 4:22:27 PM4/12/06 4:22:27 PM

Page 18: Rethinking Giftedness and Talent in Sport

282 Tranckle and Cushion

Prescott, J. (1996). Talent identifi cation and development in female gymnastics. Coaching Focus, 31, 12-13.

Régnier, G., & Salmela, J.H. (1987). Predictors of success in Canadian male gymnasts. In B. Petiot, J.H. Salmela, & T.B. Hoshizki (Eds.), World identifi cation systems for gymnastic talent (pp. 143-150). Montreal, Canada: Sport Psyche Editions.

Régnier, G., Salmela, J.H., & Russell, S.J. (1993). Talent detection and development in sport. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphey, & I.K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research in sport psychology (pp. 290-313). New York: Macmillan.

Sloboda, J.A., & Howe, M.J.A. (1991). Biographical precursors of musical excellence: An interview study. Psychology of Music, 19, 3-21.

Sloan, L.A. (1985). Phases of learning. In B.S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young people. New York: Ballantine.

Sosniak, L.A. (1985). Learning to be a concert pianist. In B.S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young people. (pp. 19-67). New York: Ballantine Books.

Tannenbaum, A.J. (1983). Gifted children: Psychological and educational perspectives. New York: Macmillan.

Thomas, K.T., & Thomas, J.R. (1999). What squirrels in the trees predict about expert athletes. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 30, 221-234.

UKʼs Youth Sports Trust. (2003). NTFfS gifted and talented pupils policy. [On-Line] Avail-able: www.youthsporttrust.org/talentladder/talentladder_2003/gifted_policy.html

Walters, J. & Gardner, H. (1986). The crystallising experience: Discovering an intellectual gift. In R. Sternberg & J. Davidson (Ed.), Concepts of giftedness (pp. 306-331). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Webster, D. (1970). Webster s̓ new world dictionary (College ed.). Toronto: Nelson, Foster & Scott.

Whitson, D. (1976). Method in sport sociology: The potential of a phenomenological con-tribution. International Review of Sport Sociology, 11, 53-68.

Williams, A.M., & Reilly, T. (2000). Talent identifi cation and development in soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 657-667.

05Trankle(265).indd 28205Trankle(265).indd 282 4/12/06 4:22:27 PM4/12/06 4:22:27 PM