slowness of movement in parkinson’s diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/marsden89.pdfslowness of...

12
Movement Disorders Vol. 4, Suppl. 1, 1989, pp. S26-S37 0 1989 Movement Disorder Society Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical Neurology, Institute of Neurology, Nation6 for Nervous Diseases, London, England Hospital Loss of the ability to move is the most characteristic and fundamental motor deficit in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonian patients exhibit akinesia (inability to initiate movement), hypokinesia (reduced movement), and bradykinesia (slow- ness of the movement itself). (All these phenomena will be subsumed under the title akinesia.) Rigid muscles may contribute to akinesia, but they are not the cause. This is evident from the effects of both stereotactic surgery and treatment with levodopa. Stereotactic thalamotomy is an effective means of abolishing contralateral rigidity (and tremor), but does not relieve akinesia, which progresses to cause increasing disability. Levodopa therapy, on the other hand, dramatically relieves akinesia, even in those who have undergone previous stereotactic surgery. The akinesia of Parkinson’s disease represents a major negative symptom and, as such, probably gives the best clue as to what goes wrong with basal ganglia function in that condition (1,2). However, analysis of the pathophysiology of akinesia has proved difficult. One is dealing with higher-order motor function in all its complexity. Normal Movement Two approaches have been employed to investigate events in the brain before the initiation of movement in Parkinson’s disease; (a) the study of reaction times, and (b) the investigation of the Bereitschaftspotential. REACTION TIMES Patients with Parkinson’s disease, on average, take longer than normal to move to an external visual, auditory, or peripheral kinesthetic stimulus (3-5). Unlike normal subjects, patients with Parkinson’s disease who know what is required seem less able to use advance information to initiate the response to a simple cue. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. C. D. Marsden at University Department of Clinical Neurology, Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Nervous Diseases, Queen Square, London, WCIN 3BG, England. S26

Upload: others

Post on 03-Jun-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

Movement Disorders Vol. 4, Suppl. 1, 1989, pp. S26-S37 0 1989 Movement Disorder Society

Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease

C . D. Marsden

University Department of Clinical Neurology, Institute of Neurology, Nation6 for Nervous Diseases, London, England

Hospital

Loss of the ability to move is the most characteristic and fundamental motor deficit in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonian patients exhibit akinesia (inability to initiate movement), hypokinesia (reduced movement), and bradykinesia (slow- ness of the movement itself). (All these phenomena will be subsumed under the title akinesia.)

Rigid muscles may contribute to akinesia, but they are not the cause. This is evident from the effects of both stereotactic surgery and treatment with levodopa. Stereotactic thalamotomy is an effective means of abolishing contralateral rigidity (and tremor), but does not relieve akinesia, which progresses to cause increasing disability. Levodopa therapy, on the other hand, dramatically relieves akinesia, even in those who have undergone previous stereotactic surgery.

The akinesia of Parkinson’s disease represents a major negative symptom and, as such, probably gives the best clue as to what goes wrong with basal ganglia function in that condition (1,2). However, analysis of the pathophysiology of akinesia has proved difficult. One is dealing with higher-order motor function in all its complexity.

Normal Movement

Two approaches have been employed to investigate events in the brain before the initiation of movement in Parkinson’s disease; (a) the study of reaction times, and (b) the investigation of the Bereitschaftspotential.

REACTION TIMES

Patients with Parkinson’s disease, on average, take longer than normal to move to an external visual, auditory, or peripheral kinesthetic stimulus (3-5). Unlike normal subjects, patients with Parkinson’s disease who know what is required seem less able to use advance information to initiate the response to a simple cue.

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. C. D. Marsden at University Department of Clinical Neurology, Institute of Neurology, National Hospital for Nervous Diseases, Queen Square, London, WCIN 3BG, England.

S26

Page 2: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

SLOW MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE S2 7

Normals knowing what is required can select and prepare the movement to deliver it more quickly than parkinsonian patients. However, the extent of impairment of simple reaction times does not correlate closely with clinical akinesia.

The difference between simple and choice reaction times (central delay) gives some indication of the ability to formulate the correct response to a stimulus to move. The central delay is thus a measure of the time taken to select the correct motor programme to respond to the choice signal. The overall conclusions from such experiments (5,6) is that the difference between simple and choice reaction times in patients with Parkinson’s disease is no greater than normal. Furthermore, parkinsonian patients respond to the choice correctly. Bloxham et al. (6) point out that the major deficit in Parkinson’s disease is the prolonged simple reaction time, whereas choice reaction times are similar to normal. This can be interpreted to indicate a failure to prepare a movement known to be required in advance, a failure to hold that movement in store, or a failure to deliver the prepared stored motor programme.

Patients with Parkinson’s disease, under some circumstances, can make use of a warning before the imperative cue to improve reaction time in the same way as normal subjects (4,7). This indicates that they can select the correct motor pro- gramme in response to the warning for subsequent delivery when the instruction to move occurs. They can also correct wrong moves when the cue is false (8,9), albeit more slowly than normal. This also suggests that parkinsonian patients do prepare motor programmes in advance of a stimulus to move. The sum of evi- dence suggests that the delayed response to a simple trigger t o move is due to failure to hold or deliver the stored motor programme with normal speed.

Patients with Parkinson’s disease can learn motor tasks and can formulate an “internal plan” of a motor sequence (10). Despite earlier claims to the contrary, they can also take predictive motor action in a learned motor task (6,lO). Thus, having learned a new motor sequence to track a target, they can move in advance of their perception of the target to anticipate its track, albeit less successfully than normal subjects (1 1,12). In visual tracking of known targets, parkinsonian patients exhibit greater error than normal, and are more dependent on visual feedback.

The ability to deliver motor commands in advance of perception of a target means that parkinsonian patients must be able to select a sequence of motor programmes in preparation to move. However, they are still more heavily depen- dent on visual information than normals; if the target is made to disappear, they often fail to execute the known required movement (13,14).

These data suggest that patients with Parkinson’s disease can: (a) correctly perceive the stimulus to move; (b) take appropriate action by selecting the correct motor programme to prepare for movement; (c) learn new motor tasks and try to deliver appropriate motor programmes in advance of any feedback; (d) but either cannot hold, or cannot deliver, these prepared motor programmes in response to their established internal plan of action; (e) as a result, they are less successful in employing predictive motor action, and are more dependent on visual feedback to achieve movement.

A crucial question is whether parkinsonian individuals cannot store and hold selected motor programmes, or whether they cannot deliver those held in store.

Movement Disorders, Vol. 4. Supplement I , 1989

Page 3: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

S28 C . D. MARSDEN

Unfortunately, too little is known about motor “memory,” or motor programme storage and retrieval to comment further.

BEREITSCHAFI’SPOTENTIAL

Several lines of evidence emphasise the role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in the breakdown of motor control in Parkinson’s disease. Anatomical studies of primate basal ganglia systems show that a major portion of pallidal output is directed to the nonprimary motor areas of frontal cortex, in particular to the supplementary motor area (15,16). In humans, cerebral blood flow studies have suggested that the SMA is crucial to the organisation of sequential hand movements (17,18), and an abnormality of sequencing hand and elbow move- ments has been demonstrated in a patient with unilateral SMA damage (19). A similar disturbance of sequenced hand and elbow movements has been demon- strated in patients with Parkinson’s disease (20) (see Sequential Motor Actions in Parkinson’s Disease, below).

Kornhuber and Deecke (21) suggested that the activity of the SMA may be studied by averaging surface electroencephalographic (EEG) activity during the organisation and performance of a self-paced voluntary movement. Such a move- ment is preceded by an EEG potential called the Bereitschaftspotential (BP) after Kornhuber and Deecke (21). Kornhuber and Deecke (22) and Deecke (23) argued that the BP may reflect activity in the SMA, because its maximum amplitude is a t the vertex (overlying the SMA) for several different types of voluntary movement (eye, hand, arm, and foot). In a recent review, Tamas and Shibasaki (24) divided the BP into an early and a late component. The late component is lateralized to the hemisphere contralateral to the limb movement and is thought to represent activ- ity in the motor cortex (25), perhaps due to input from the dentatothalamic tract (26,27). The early component is not lateralized and reaches its maximum ampli- tude over the midline. Boschert et al. (28) concluded that it is the early BP, rather than the late component, which reflects SMA activity.

If the main functional target area for pallidal output is the SMA, one would anticipate an abnormality of the BP in Parkinson’s disease and the abnormality might selectively affect the early BP.

Initial BP studies from different laboratories suggested that there was an ab- normally small BP in Parkinson’s disease (23,29). However, recently certain methodological difficulties in this earlier work have been reassessed (30,31); it has been suggested that the apparently smaller BP observed in Parkinson’s disease could have arisen as a result of problems triggering the averager due to the gradual onset of electromyogram (EMG) activity in akinetic patients (32). In addition, it has become clear that the amplitude of the BP is critically dependent on age. Barrett et al. (33) and Deecke (23) found that older normal subjects have a smaller BP than do younger control subjects. Consequent on improved triggering and the use of older normal subjects for comparison, Barrett et al. (32) have suggested that no difference exists between the BP of normal old subjects and patients with Parkinson’s disease.

Treatment of patients with Parkinson’s disease may also confound BP studies.

Movement Disorders, Vol. 4 , Supplement 1, 1989

Page 4: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

SLOW MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE S29

Dick et al. (34) pointed out that the oral administration of levodopa alters the amplitude of the BP, particularly of the early BP, in both patients with Parkinson’s disease and normal subjects. Some of the patients studied by Barrett et al. (32) may have been receiving drug therapy. This would have caused an increase in the early BP component, which could have hidden any decrease in its size. When drug administration was withheld, Dick et al. (34) found, like Barrett et al. (32), that the peak BP negativity of patients with Parkinson’s disease was normal for simple finger extension movements; however, unlike Barrett et al. (32), Dick et al. (35) and Simpson and Khuraibet (36) found a smaller early component of the BP in patients with Parkinson’s disease.

The decreased amplitude of the early BP from midline leads in patients with Parkinson’s disease may be due to impaired SMA function. However, there is as yet no definitive proof that the early component of the BP arises in SMA.

With regard to the later lateralized components of the BP, the results in Par- kinson’s disease show a more prominent negativity than normal in this late phase of the BP.

What then does the defective early BP and the prominent later negativity reflect in terms of the breakdown of movement control in Parkinson’s disease? Goldberg (37) has suggested that there are two distinct systems for the control of voluntary movements. The first is a predictive, feedforward mode whereby the subject decides “internally” the format of a desired movement. The second is a respon- sive mode whereby the subject responds to environmental cues, which them- selves determine how a motor response is organised. On the basis of embryolog- ical, anatomical, and modem neurophysiological evidence (primate lesion studies, cortical single unit studies and blood flow studies), Goldberg (37) suggests that these two systems are anatomically distinct; the former involves the medially situated SMA; the latter involves the dorsolateral premotor area (PMA).

Connectivity studies in primates suggest that the major input to SMA is ulti- mately derived from the basal ganglia (15,16), whereas the dorsolateral PMA has a significantly greater input from areas of sensory cortex, particularly the visual cortex (38). It is tempting to suggest that patients with Parkinson’s disease rely on the dorsolateral system of motor control because their medial system is defective. This would be consistent with the inordinate dependence of parkinsonian patients on visual information for the successful initiation and performance of a variety of movements.

This model of SMA and premotor area function permits an interpretation of the BP findings. The impaired early BP may reflect poor cortical activation of the medial predictive system, involving SMA and basal ganglia, used to generate internally generated movement. The augmented negativity in the late phase of the BP may reflect compensatory overactivity of the lateral externally responsive system, involving the dorsolateral PMA and visual (and other) sensory systems.

SIMPLE LIMB MOVEMENTS

When a patient with Parkinson’s disease attempts a self-paced, simple fast movement of one joint of the upper limb, the movement is executed slowly (3). In

Movement Disorders, Voi. 4, Supplement I , 1989

Page 5: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

S30 C . D. MARSDEN

reaction paradigms it is the movement time, rather than response time, that is most compromised in Parkinson’s disease (5).

A normal person who executes a simple fast or ballistic movement shows a stereotyped pattern of EMG activity in agonist and antagonist muscles (accom- panied as necessary by similarly stereotyped activity in synergists and postural fixators). The agonist fires first to generate the impulsive force required to pro- duce the fast movement; the antagonist then fires to provide any required braking force; the agonist may then fire again to provide any required final correction. Normal subjects make larger movements with faster velocities. Movement of increasing amplitude and velocity are achieved by increasing the size and, to a lesser extent, the duration of the first agonist burst of EMG activity. The parkin- sonian patient activates agonist and antagonist muscles in the correct sequence and brings in appropriate anticipatory activity in postural muscles (39). In other words, the selection of muscles and the relative timing of their activation is cor- rect, so the basic form of the motor programme is preserved.

The reason why a fast ballistic movement in Parkinson’s disease is slow is that the force of the initial impulsive activity in the agonist is inadequate. This is because the size of the initial EMG burst in the agonist is reduced (40). This is true even for distal limb movements involving no postural support (41).

The size of the first agonist burst can be graded to make larger movements in parkinsonian patients (42), but it is consistently underscaled for the amplitude of movement intended. As a result, the velocity of the resulting movement is too slow and the displacement achieved undershoots the point of aim. The latter is finally reached by a subsequent series of additional small bursts of EMG activity, which adds to the overall delay in achieving the required movement. Parkinsonian patients, unlike normals, tend to make movements of different size at the same velocity (3,40).

As a consequence of their inability to produce large agonist bursts to initiate movement, parkinsonian patients are particularly impaired in making high- velocity movements (1 1). Normal subjects make an initial fast movement to within 220% of the target position, and then make a small number of corrections to hit the point of aim. In contrast, parkinsonians creep by small steps towards the required position, and appear inordinately dependent on visual information to achieve their objective (1 1) . It is as if parkinsonian individuals cannot employ accurate preprogrammed or “open-loop” ballistic movements without the need for feedback, but are constrained to operate in a “closed-loop” mode requiring visual information (11,13,14).

Sanes (43) has extended these observations on movement accuracy by assess- ing how well patients with Parkinson’s disease adapt to Fitts’ law (44). In normal subjects executing rapidly alternating movements, Fitts showed that movement time vaned systematically with changes in movement amplitude and target width, providing accuracy was held constant. Movements that are easier, because they are smaller or directed to larger targets, are performed more rapidly. Sanes (43) found that parkinsonian patients had exaggerated difficulty in coping with larger movements (target size held constant), or smaller target sizes (target separation

Movement Disorders. Vol. 4 , Supplement I , 1989

Page 6: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

SLOW MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE S3l

held constant). These results are consistent with a deficit in executing high- velocity movements, but also point to the conclusion that the greater the difficulty in the task, requiring more movement accuracy, the greater the problem for the parkinsonian patient.

Failure to scale the initial agonist activity appropriate to the intended amplitude and velocity of movement is undoubtedly a fundamental abnormality of limb motor control in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonian individuals appear unable to decide in advance, or to deliver, the size of agonist muscle activity required to generate fast large movements to the target they know they wish to achieve.

Is this, however, sufficient to explain all of the akinesia so characteristic of that illness? There are good reasons to think not. For example, there is no close correlation between the extent of slowness of such simple ballistic movements of a single joint and the degree of clinical disability. Patients who are remarkably mobile when taking dopaminergic therapy show only modest improvement in their capacity to execute such simple movements, compared with their performance when drugs are withdrawn and they are dramatically immobile (42).

COMPLEX LIMB MOVEMENTS

Motor behaviour in daily life demands much more complex motor control than that required for the regulation of simple single movements of one joint. Many goal-directed motor acts require the performance of a series of movements, se- quentially and/or simultaneously, at different joints and of different limbs. For instance, to drink we need to advance one or both arms to the cup, grasp it, then lift it to the lips, before engaging the sucking and swallowing mechanisms. Each of these actions involves an individual motor programme, but the whole act re- quires appropriate sequencing and superimposition of the individual motor pro- grammes into a coherent motor plan, so as to achieve a smooth and accurate overall performance.

Patients with Parkinson’s disease exhibit many problems in executing complex motor actions such as repetitive, simultaneous, or sequential motor acts (45). Commonplace clinical observation reveals that parkinsonian patients have par- ticular difficulty in repeatedly tapping the finger on the thumb or the ball of the foot on the floor. The initial movement is slow and undershoots, but subsequent repetitions progressively decrease in amplitude and velocity until the movement may cease. There is a progressive decrease in the size of letters in micrographia. More complex motor acts, involving switching from one to another motor pro- gramme in a sequence, or the execution of two different motor acts with opposite limbs, are obviously compromised (46).

These clinical observations suggest that there is something more to the motor deficits of Parkinson’s disease than just the underscaling of the initial agonist burst. For this reason, analysis has turned to the examination of simukaneous and sequential movements. The BP preceding such simultaneous and sequential movements is larger and longer than that before similar simple movements (47), suggesting greater activation of premotor cortical areas with complex movements.

Movement Disorders, Vol. 4 , Supplement I, 1989

Page 7: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

S32 C . D. MARSDEN

SIMULTANEOUS MOTOR ACTIONS IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE

The basic paradigm that has been used for testing the execution of simultaneous voluntary movements is as follows: Patients and controls are asked to perform a fast isotonic elbow flexion (flex) and an isometric opposition of the thumb to the fingers against a strain gauge to exert a force (squeeze), separately or simulta- neously. Duration of the elbow flex movement time is measured from the velocity signal; duration of the fingerkhumb squeeze is measured from the force signal. Starting and target positions, as well as the elbow position and hand force, are displayed to the individual on an oscilloscope screen. The individuals are asked to undertake (a) elbow flex by itself, (b) hand squeeze by itself, or (c) both move- ments simultaneously (squeeze and flex).

The results may be summarized as follows (48). 1. The movement times of flex and squeeze vary from trial to trial in each

individual, but there is no correlation between them in the simultaneous flex- and-squeeze task, in either normal people or parkinsonian patients. This indicates that the complex task is executed by superimposition of two separate motor programmes, rather than by the use of a new single generalized motor programme (49,50).

2. The separate individual movements of flex or squeeze with the same arm are performed more slowly by the parkinsonian patients than by the normal individ- uals.

3. When the two movements are performed simultaneously with the same arm (flex and squeeze), there is no change in the duration or speed of either movement in normal individuals compared with their performance of each movement by itself.

4. However, in the parkinsonian patients there is an additional slowness of both movements when performed together with the same arm, compared with that seen for the separate movements alone.

5. Similar results have been obtained when patients and controls squeeze with one arm but flex with the opposite limb. However, the extra slowness of the movements when done together by the parkinsonian patients is less marked than when the same arm is used for both tasks.

The conclusion is that patients with Parkinson’s disease have added difficulty (expressed as extra slowness) when they try to execute two motor programmes simultaneously, especially with the same arm. In other words, they have added difficulty superimposing two motor programmes simultaneously for separate joint movements, over and above the difficulty that is evident when they execute either movement by itself.

SEQUENTIAL MOTOR ACTIONS IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE

The same paradigm has been used to test sequential motor action (20). Patients and controls are instructed to (a) squeeze with the right hand, then flex the right elbow, and (b) squeeze with the left hand then flex with the right elbow. They are

Movement Disorders, Vol. 4. Supplement 1 , 1989

Page 8: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

SLOW MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE s33

given two instructions: (a) move as rapidly as possible, and (b) start the second movement immediately after the end of the first. Their performance on these ipsilateral and contralateral sequential tasks has been compared with their per- formance when they undertake the individual movements by themselves. In ad- dition, the interval between the onset of the first movement and that of the second (the interonset latency, IOL), and the interval between the termination of the first movement and the onset of the second (the pause) also has been measured, as has the total time taken to complete both movements.

The results may be summarized as follows (20). 1. As with simultaneous movements, there is no correlation between the times

for squeeze and flex in sequential movements, or between movement times and the pause between movements, in either normal individuals or parkinsonian pa- tients. Again this indicates that the complete movements of squeeze-then-flex are executed by two separate motor programmes.

2. The separate single movements are slower in the parkinsonian patients than in the control individuals, and there is a further decrease in speed in the parkin- sonian patients when the two movements are executed sequentially. Similar re- sults have been observed for the bilateral squeeze with the left hand, then flex with the right.

3 . The IOL between the beginning of the first squeeze and the second flex movements is automatically chosen at -244 ms by normal individuals (51). The first movement is completed in -150 ms, so there is a pause of some 70-100 ms before the second movement is started. This pause occurs despite the instruction to execute the second movement as soon as possible after the first. When normal subjects are asked to vary the interval between the two movements, by making it shorter or longer at will, it transpires that at IOLs of <-200 ms the speed of the second elbow flex decreases; the shorter the IOL <200 ms, the slower the second movement. This is interpreted to indicate that normal individuals automatically switch from one motor programme to another with an optimal minimal delay of a little longer than -200 ms, to execute the second movement with maximum speed (51). This optimal interval between movements corresponds to the known maxi- mum rate of tapping movements of the hand at -5 Hz.

4. Patients with Parkinson’s disease find it difficult to learn the sequential task of squeeze-then-flex. When they have done so, the IOL is longer than normal (-425 ms), as is the pause between the two movements (-171 ms). If they try to execute the two movements with IOLs of <400 ms they find it extremely difficult; when they succeed the speed of the second movement declines progressively (20,52). In other words, the optimum interval between movements, so as to main- tain the speed of the second movement, is considerably longer in the parkinsonian patients than in the controls.

Patients with Parkinson’s disease thus have added difficulty when attempting to execute two motor programmes sequentially with the same or opposite arms. They perform the individual movements in the sequence more slowly than when each is carried out alone, and the interval between the two movements is pro- longed. It turns out that the extra slowness in executing such a complex motor

Movement Disorders. Vol. 4 . Supplement 1 , 1989

Page 9: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

s34 C . D. MARSDEN

sequence is more closely related to the degree of clinical bradykinesia than to the slowness of single movements.

Berardelli et al. (53) have obtained similar results in a study of the ability of patients with Parkinson’s disease to execute the fast, complex arm movements required to draw triangles and squares on a writing tablet. The movements were accurate, but the time taken to trace the geometric figures, and the pauses at the angles, were prolonged. They concluded that the parkinsonian’s difficulties in generating two joint ballistic movements were due to problems in running the motor programmes required to generate complex arm trajectories.

CONCLUSIONS

The studies reviewed here have shown that damage to the basal ganglia in Parkinson’s disease causes (a) slowness of single simple arm movements with one joint, and (b) extra difficulties in the execution of simultaneous or sequential complex arm movements with two joints.

Can the first abnormality explain the second? Is the difficulty that patients with Parkinson’s disease have in single movements sufficient to explain their problems with movement sequences?

The more recent findings presented here suggest that the slowness of simple single movements (due to an underscaling of the size of the first agonist EMG burst) is not in itself sufficient to explain the added problems encountered in complex simultaneous and sequential motor acts. Not only does the performance of two movements expose an extra slowness of each component, but in the se- quential task there is also extra delay between the two movements in parkinsonian patients compared with normal individuals.

It might be predicted that the extra problems encountered with two motor programmes would become even more evident if even longer sequences involving three or more motor programmes were examined. Indeed, the extra slowness and delay of the next movement in a long sequence might become more evident as the sequence progresses, to produce the progressive fade and collapse of complex repetitive or sequential motor actions so characteristic of Parkinson’s disease. As suggested earlier (2,46), “each motor programme in itself is imprecise, so it is not altogether surprising that, when repeated, the whole sequence is in error. How- ever, the details of the impairment of repetitive action suggests that there is more than this. The critical feature is that, as the sequence is continued, the individual movement or motor programme progressively degrades. The time to initiation gets slower and the size of the movement progressively fades. Thus, the final movement in a sequence is far worse than when that individual movement is performed by itself. Repetition of the movement unearths a greater deficit than is apparent in single movements. Likewise, when two movements are made to- gether, they are each performed much worse than if they are undertaken alone. This suggests that there is a fundamental breakdown in the capacity to run the sequence of movements that comprises a motor plan. In particular, there appears to be a major problem in switching from one motor programme to another. In

Movement Disorders, Vol. 4 , Supplement I , 1989

Page 10: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

SLOW MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE s35

other words, the sequence of the motor plan does not run smoothly in Parkinson’s disease. ’ ’

Such problems might explain many of the characteristics of parkinsonian aki- nesia. For example, the fatigue of repetitive tapping of fingers or feet, or the progressive nature of micrographia or hypophonia, might all represent the com- pound effect of difficulty in generating long sequences of movement.

The deficit in sequencing arm movements seen in patients with Parkinson’s disease leads to the following proposition. Perhaps the motor basal ganglia are more concerned with directing what happens to the next movement in a sequence than with the initial movement.

It has been difficult to correlate the pattern of single-unit discharges in the basal ganglia of subhuman primates with a single motor act (54). Single units in the output z w e s of globus pallidus interna and substantia nigra pars reticulata show somatotopic organization, with correlation between cell discharge in localized regions and active movements of specific body parts. However, such neuronal discharges do not clearly or consistently precede the first EMG changes in mus- cles; overall, changes in neuronal discharge seem to occur later in the basal ganglia than in the motor cortex.

This is not surprising, because the basal ganglia receive a readout of the motor cortex output that drives the movement itself, via corticostriatal pathways from sensorimotor cortex to putamen. Putamen units (and probably those of the globus pallidus) fire in relation to the direction and amplitude of the movement (rather than to the activity in the individual muscles involved), suggesting that these basal ganglia areas monitor parameters of the movement. The suggestion is that this information is used to set up the premotor areas to select the correct parameters for the next movement. To test this hypothesis, those who record the activity of basal ganglia units in awake performing primates would need to correlate such neuronal discharge with the next movement the animal performs, not with the immediate obvious motor action as has been done so far.

How would such a theory explain the slowness of a simple single one-joint movement? Why do parkinsonian patients underscale the size of the first agonist EMG burst in relation to the amplitude and velocity required to execute the desired movement? One suggestion is that such simple movements are defective because they must be based on what has gone before. In other words, the correct setting of the parameters of the motor programme required to execute even a single movement must depend on information about the motor state before its execution. If the readout of existing motor activity from sensorimotor cortex delivered to the basal ganglia is mishandled, the output of the basal ganglia to premotor areas might misdirect the selection of parameters for even a simple subsequent single movement.

Such a hypothesis has the attraction of encompassing all the defects of move- ment demonstrated in Parkinson’s disease within one basic abnormality, namely a failure of basal ganglia direction of premotor areas to select the correct param- eters for subsequent motor programmes. -As a result, the individual motor pro- grammes necessary for complex actions are specified incorrectly, and the linkage of motor programmes into a unified motor plan collapses.

Movement Disorders. Vol. 4 . Supplement I . 1989

Page 11: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

S36 C. D. MARSDEN

REFERENCES

1. Denny-Brown D. The basal ganglia and their relation to disorders of movement. London: Oxford

2. Marsden CD. The mysterious motor function of the basal ganglia. Neurology 1982;32:514-39. 3. Draper IT, Johns RJ. The disordered movement in parkinsonism and the effect of drug treatment.

Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp 1964;115:465-80. 4. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Watson RT, Greer M. Reaction times in Parkinson’s disease. Arch

Neurol 1976;33: 139-40. 5. Evarts EV, Teravainen H, Calne DB. Reaction time in Parkinson’s disease. Bruin 1981;104:167-

86. 6. Bloxham CA, Mindell TA, Frith CD. Initiation and execution of predictable and unpredictable

movements in Parkinson’s disease. Bruin 1984;107:371-84. 7. Bloxham CA, Dick DJ, Moore M. Reaction times and attention in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol

Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987;50: 1178-83. 8. Angel RW, Alston W, Higgins JR. Control of movement in Parkinson’s disease. Bruin 1970;93: 1-

14. 9. Raphal RD, Posner M, Walker J , Stebbins K. Is slowness in initiating movement in Parkinson’s

disease or frontal lobe lesions due to slowness in mentally preparing to move? Neurology 1983;33(suppl 2): 103.

10. Day BL, Dick JPR, Marsden CD. Patients with Parkinson’s disease can employ a predictive motor strategy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1984;47: 1299-1306.

11. Flowers KA. Visual “closed-loop” and “open-loop’’ characteristics of voluntary movement in patients with parkinsonism and intention tremor. Bruin 1976;99269-3 10.

12. Flowers KA. Lack of prediction in the motor behaviour of parkinsonism. Bruin 1978;101:35-52. 13. Cooke JD, Brown JD, Brooks VB. increased dependence on visual information for movement

control in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Can J Neurol Sci 1978;5:413-5. 14. Stern Y, Mayeux R, Rosen J , Ikon J. Perceptual motor function in Parkinson’s disease: a deficit

in sequential and predictive voluntary movement. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1983;46: 145-5 1. 15. Jurgens U. The efferent and afferent connections of the supplementary motor area. Bruin Res

1984;300:63-87. 16. Schell GR, Strick PL. The origin of thalamic inputs to the arcuate premotor and supplementary

motor areas. J Neurosci 1984;4:539-60. 17. Roland PE, Larsen B, Lassen NA, Skinhoj E. Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas

in organisation of voluntary movements in man. J Neurophysiol 1980;43: 118-36. 18. Roland PE, Meyer E, Shibasaki T, Yamamoto YL, Thompson CJ. Regional cerebral blood flow

changes in cortex and basal ganglia during voluntary movements in normal human volunteers. J Neurophysiol 1982;48:467-80.

19. Dick JPR, Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Marsden CD. Simple and complex movements in a patient with infarction of the right supplementary motor area. Movement Disorders 1986;1:255-66.

20. Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Dick JPR, Day BL, Marsden CD. Disturbances of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Bruin 1987;110:361-79.

21. Kornhuber HH, Deecke L. Hirnpotentialanderungen bei Willkurbewegungen reafferente Poten- tiale. Pflugers Arch 1965;284: 1-17.

22. Kornhuber HH, Deecke L. An electrical sign of participation of the mesial supplementary motor cortex in human voluntary finger movement. Bruin Res 1978;159:473-6.

23. Deecke L. Cerebral potentials related to voluntary actions: parkinsonian and normal subjects. In: Delwaide PJ, Agnoli A, eds. Clinical neurophysiology in parkinsonism. Amsterdam: Elsevier,

24. Tamas LB, Shibasaki H. Cortical potentials associated with movement: a review. J Clin Neuro- physiol 1985 ;2: 157-7 1.

25. Shibasaki H, Shima R, Kuriowa Y. Clinical studies of the movement-related cortical potential (MP) and the relationship between the dentatorubrothalamic pathway and readiness potential (RP). J Neurol 1978;219: 15-25.

26. Sasaki K, Gemba H, Hashimoto S. Influences of cerebellar hemispherectomy upon cortical po- tentials preceding visually initiated hand movements in the monkey. Bruin Res 1981 ;210:425-30.

27. Sasaki K, Gemba H. Field potentials in different cortical areas prior to conditioned hand move- ments. Bruin Res 1985;372:315-27.

28. Boschert J , Hink RF, Deecke L. Finger movement versus toe movement-related potentials: fur- ther evidence for supplementary motor area (SMA) participation prior to voluntary action. Exp Bruin Res 1983;55:73-80.

University Press, 1962.

1985 :91-105.

Movement Disorders, Vol. 4 , Supplement I , 1989

Page 12: Slowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Diseasee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/Marsden89.pdfSlowness of Movement in Parkinson’s Disease C. D. Marsden University Department of Clinical

SLOW MOVEMENT IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE s3 7

29. Deecke L, Englitz HG, Kornhuber HH, Schmidt G. Cerebral potentials preceding voluntary movements in patients with bilateral or unilateral Parkinsonian akinesia. In: Desmedt JE, ed. Progress in clinical neurophysiology. vol 1. Basel: Karger, 1977: 151-63.

30. Barrett G, Shibasaki H, Neshige R. Technical aspects of recording movement associated pre- movement potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1985;60:276-81.

31. Barrett G, Shibasaki H, Neshige R. Cortical potentials preceding voluntary movement: evidence for three periods of preparation in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1986;63:327-39.

32. Barrett G, Shibasaki H, Neshige R. Cortical potential shifts preceding voluntary movement are normal in parkinsonism. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1986;63:34&8.

33. Barrett G, Neshige R, Shibasaki H. Cortical potentials preceding voluntary self-paced movement in young, old and parkinsonian subjects. J Physiol (Lon4 1985;372:48P.

34. Dick JPR, Cantello R, Buruma 0, et al. The Bereitschaftspotential, 1-DOPA and Parkinson’s disease. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987;66:263-74.

35. Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, Day BL, et al. The Bereitschaftspotential is abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain (in press).

36. Simpson JA, Khuraibet AJ. Readiness potential of cortical area 6 preceeding self paced movement in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 198730: 1184-91.

37. Goldberg G. Supplementary motor area: structure and function: review and hypothesis. Brain Behav Sci 1985;8:567-616.

38. Pandya DN, Kuypers HGJM. Cortico-cortical connections in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res

39. Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, Berardelli A, et al. Associated postural adjustments in Parkinson’s dis-

40. Hallett M, Khoshbin S. A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. Brain 1980;103:3014. 41. Berardelli A, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Marsden CD. Movements not involved in posture are ab-

normal in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett 1984;47:47-50. 42. Berardelli A, Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Marsden CD. Scaling of the size of the first agonist

EMG burst during rapid wrist movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neuro- surg Psychiatry 1986;49: 1273-9.

43. Sanes JN. Information processing deficits in Parkinson’s disease during movement. Neuropsy- chologia 1985;23:38 1-92.

44. Fitts TM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 1954;47:381-91.

45. Schwab RS, Chafetz ME, Walker S. Control of two simultaneous voluntary motor acts in normals and in parkinsonism. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1954;72:591-8.

46. Marsden CD. Which motor disorder in Parkinson’s disease indicates the true motor function of the basal ganglia? In: Functions of the basal ganRlia. London: Pitman (Ciba Found Symp., 107),

1969; 13: 13-36.

ease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1986;49: 1378-85.

- - . .

1984:25-37. 47. Benecke R. Dick JPR. Rothwell JC. Dav BL. Marsden CD. Increase of the BereitschaftsDotential .~

in simultaneous and sequential movements. Neurosci Lett 1985;62:347-52.

ments in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1986;109:739-57. 48. Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Dick JPR, Day BL, Marsden CD. Performance of simultaneous move-

49. Schmidt FL4. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychol Rev 1975;82:225-60. SO. Carter MC, Shapiro DC. Control of sequential movements: evidence for generalised motor pro-

grammes. J Neurophysiol 1984;52:787-96. 51. Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Dick JPR, Marsden CD. Motor strategies involved in the

performance of sequential movements. Exp Brain Res 1987;63:585-95. 52. Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Dick JPR, Day BL, Marsden CD. Simple and complex movements off

and on treatment in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry - . 1987;50:296-303.

53. Berardelli A, Accornero N, Argenta M, Meco G, Manfredi M. Fast complex arm movements in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1986;49: 1146-9.

54. Delong MR, Georgopoulos AP, Crutcher MD, Mitchell SJ, Richardson RT, Alexander GE. Func- tional organization of the basal ganglia: contributions of single-cell recording studies. In: Func- tions o f the basal ganglia. London: Pitman (Ciba Found Symp., 107), 198454-78.

Movement Disorders, Vol. 4 , Supplement I , 1989