socioeconomic status

71
Ichiro Kawachi, MD, PhD Monica Wang, ScD, MS Harvard School of Public Health PH201x: Health and Society Socioeconomic Status (SES)

Upload: janmali

Post on 21-Oct-2015

52 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Socioeconomic Status

Ichiro Kawachi, MD, PhD

Monica Wang, ScD, MS

Harvard School of Public Health

PH201x: Health and Society

Socioeconomic Status (SES)

Page 2: Socioeconomic Status

Magazine Die Gartenlaube, Titanic Sinking, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:St%C3%B6wer_Titanic.jpg, PD

Page 3: Socioeconomic Status

Titanic casualties

Total on Board: 2,201 Lifeboat Capacity: 1,178 Total Deaths: 1,490 Dead

68%

Saved 32%

Chart created by Dr. Kawachi

Page 4: Socioeconomic Status

Titanic survivors

Class Saved Total

First 203 325

Second 118 285

Third 178 706

Crew 212 885

Page 5: Socioeconomic Status

Titanic survivors

62.5

41.4

25.2 24

0

20

40

60

80

First Second Third Crew

Class

Perc

ent S

aved

Chart created by Dr. Kawachi

Page 6: Socioeconomic Status

Why was there a social class gradient in mortality on the Titanic?

Lord Mersey

John Bigham, 1st Viscount Mersey (1840-1929), Barclay Bros. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:1stViscountMersey.jpg, PD

Page 7: Socioeconomic Status

Why was there a social class gradient in mortality on the Titanic?

Confounding by age and sex of passengers in different sections of the boat.

Age, sex

Class Survival

Page 8: Socioeconomic Status

Mortality on board Titanic, by gender and class

Class Men Women/Children

First 67.4% 2.7%

Second 91.7% 11.2%

Third 83.8% 57.8%

Page 9: Socioeconomic Status

Why was there a social class gradient in mortality on the Titanic?

1.  Confounding by age and sex of passengers in different sections of the boat.

2.  Upper class people were physically more fit, or quicker to respond to instructions of the crew.

3.  Discrimination against 3rd class passengers / Preferential treatment of 1st class passengers.

4.  Structural differences in access to life-saving resources (lifeboats).

Page 10: Socioeconomic Status

Cutaway diagram of RMS Titanic, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Titanic_cutaway_diagram.png, PD

Page 11: Socioeconomic Status

Evidence of SES gradients in health in contemporary society

§  Income §  Educational attainment §  Occupation

Page 12: Socioeconomic Status

Relative risks of all-cause mortality by household income level: U.S. panel study of income dynamics

3.03

2.49

2

1.45 1.36 1

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

3.5

<15,000 -20,000 -30,000 -50,000 -70,000 >70,000

Rel

ativ

e ris

k

Household income (1993 $)

Source: McDonough et al. 1997

Page 13: Socioeconomic Status

Chronic disease mortality among US adults aged 25-64 by education level

0

100

200

300

400

500

600

Men Women

Dea

ths

per

100,

000

< 12 yrs12 yrs13+ yrs

Education

Source: Health, United States, SES and Health Chartbook 1998

Page 14: Socioeconomic Status

Chronic disease mortality among US adults aged 25-64 by education level

0

100

200

300

400

500

600

Men Women

Dea

ths

per

100,

000

< 12 yrs12 yrs13+ yrs

Education

Source: Health, United States, SES and Health Chartbook 1998

Is  this  cause  and  effect?  

Page 15: Socioeconomic Status

Mortality gradient by occupational class in the British Whitehall Study

00.5

11.5

22.5

33.5

44.5

Admin Prof/Exec Clerical Other

RR

of m

orta

lity

Marmot, MG et al. (1978). "Employment grade and coronary heart disease in British civil servants". Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 32 (4): 244–249. doi:10.1136/jech.32.4.244

Page 16: Socioeconomic Status

Two types of threat to causal inference

•  Reverse Causation •  Confounding (omitted variable bias)

Page 17: Socioeconomic Status

Reverse Causation: Bad health compromises educational attainment, not the other way round

Education Health

Page 18: Socioeconomic Status

Reverse Causation: Bad health compromises educational attainment, not the other way round

Education Health

Page 19: Socioeconomic Status

It is often asserted that education is less susceptible to reverse causation (compared to income & occupation)

•  Most people have completed their schooling by the time they develop chronic disease

•  If you get sick, you can’t lose education (in the way that you can lose income or your job)

•  But is that strictly accurate?

Page 20: Socioeconomic Status

Evidence from 1958 British Birth Cohort (National Child Development Study, NCDS)

•  Chronic health conditions during childhood do appear to have an adverse impact on educational attainment (Case, Fertig & Pason, 2005)

•  Even after taking into account household and parental characteristics,

each chronic condition reported at age 7 leads to on average 0.3 fewer subjects passed on General Certificate of Education O-level examinations at age sixteen

•  In short, chronic conditions during childhood – e.g. diabetes, ADHD, or mental health problems – result in children missing school

Page 21: Socioeconomic Status

In summary …

•  Reverse causation is real! •  Though not every instance of SES

gradients reflects this bias

Page 22: Socioeconomic Status

The relationship between SES and health is reciprocal and dynamic across the life course

Adler et al. Reaching for a Healthier Life, The John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Socioeconomic Status and Health. http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/downloads/reaching_for_a_healthier_life.pdf

Page 23: Socioeconomic Status

Two types of threat to causal inference

•  Reverse Causation •  Confounding (omitted variable bias)

Page 24: Socioeconomic Status

Confounding

Association between SES & health is spurious, and reflects the influence of omitted “third” variables.

SES Health

?

Page 25: Socioeconomic Status

Sidebar (A bit of Epi review)

Carrying matches in your pocket

Lung cancer Relative risk = 2.0

Should we advise people to stop carrying matches around?

Page 26: Socioeconomic Status

Sidebar (A bit of Epi review)

Carrying matches in your pocket

Lung cancer Relative risk = 2.0

smoking

How would you demonstrate that the association is confounded by cigarette smoking?

Page 27: Socioeconomic Status

Association between carrying matches & lung cancer stratified by smoking status

Smokers   Non-­‐smokers  

Relative risk of lung cancer among match-carriers vs. non-carriers

Page 28: Socioeconomic Status

Association between carrying matches & lung cancer stratified by smoking status

Smokers   Non-­‐smokers  

Relative risk of lung cancer among match-carriers vs. non-carriers

1.00

Page 29: Socioeconomic Status

Association between carrying matches & lung cancer stratified by smoking status

Smokers   Non-­‐smokers  

Relative risk of lung cancer among match-carriers vs. non-carriers

1.00

1.00

Page 30: Socioeconomic Status

Is it necessary to adjust for “carrying matches” when studying the association of smoking with lung cancer?

Lung cancer

Carrying matches

Relative risk = 20 Smoking

Page 31: Socioeconomic Status

Is it necessary to adjust for “carrying matches” when studying the association of smoking with lung cancer?

Lung cancer

Carrying matches

Relative risk = 20 Smoking

Page 32: Socioeconomic Status

Moving beyond observational data

Identification Strategies to Assess Causality §  Randomized controlled trials §  Quasi-experiments

Page 33: Socioeconomic Status

Randomized controlled trials of schooling

•  Extremely sparse due to low feasibility and ethical concerns

•  Two trials studied intensive preschool interventions

a.  High/Scope Perry Pre-school Program b.  Abecedarian Program

Page 34: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7)

Page 35: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7) •  Enrolled N=123 disadvantaged

black children, age 3-4 years

Page 36: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7) •  Enrolled N=123 disadvantaged

black children, age 3-4 years

•  Treatment: Daily dose of 2.5 hr instruction by teachers with Masters degree on weekday mornings, plus weekly 90-min home visit by teacher

Page 37: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7) •  Enrolled N=123 disadvantaged

black children, age 3-4 years

•  Treatment: Daily dose of 2.5 hr instruction by teachers with Masters degree on weekday mornings, plus weekly 90-min home visit by teacher

•  Duration: 30 wk/yr for 2 years •  Follow-up through age 40

Page 38: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7) •  Enrolled N=123 disadvantaged

black children, age 3-4 years

•  Treatment: Daily dose of 2.5 hr instruction by teachers with Masters degree on weekday mornings, plus weekly 90-min home visit by teacher

•  Duration: 30 wk/yr for 2 years •  Follow-up through age 40

Abecedarian Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Chapel Hill, NC (1972-7)

Page 39: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7) •  Enrolled N=123 disadvantaged

black children, age 3-4 years

•  Treatment: Daily dose of 2.5 hr instruction by teachers with Masters degree on weekday mornings, plus weekly 90-min home visit by teacher

•  Duration: 30 wk/yr for 2 years •  Follow-up through age 40

Abecedarian Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Chapel Hill, NC (1972-7) •  Enrolled N=111 disadvantaged

black children, age 4 months

Page 40: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7) •  Enrolled N=123 disadvantaged

black children, age 3-4 years

•  Treatment: Daily dose of 2.5 hr instruction by teachers with Masters degree on weekday mornings, plus weekly 90-min home visit by teacher

•  Duration: 30 wk/yr for 2 years •  Follow-up through age 40

Abecedarian Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Chapel Hill, NC (1972-7) •  Enrolled N=111 disadvantaged

black children, age 4 months •  Treatment: 8 hrs/day, 5 days/wk

academic enrichment program for first 5 years. Additional activities from grade 1-3

Page 41: Socioeconomic Status

Perry Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Ypsilanti, MI (1962-7) •  Enrolled N=123 disadvantaged

black children, age 3-4 years

•  Treatment: Daily dose of 2.5 hr instruction by teachers with Masters degree on weekday mornings, plus weekly 90-min home visit by teacher

•  Duration: 30 wk/yr for 2 years •  Follow-up through age 40

Abecedarian Program •  Intensive pre-school program in

Chapel Hill, NC (1972-7) •  Enrolled N=111 disadvantaged

black children, age 4 months

•  Treatment: 8 hrs/day, 5 days/wk academic enrichment program for first 5 years. Additional activities from grade 1-3

•  Duration: 50 wk/yr for 8 years •  Follow-up through age 21

Page 42: Socioeconomic Status

High/Scope Perry trial outcomes at age 27

Data from: http://www.highscope.org/content.asp?ContentId=219

Intervention

Control

Page 43: Socioeconomic Status

Abecedarian program outcomes at age 21

Intervention

Control

Data from: http://abc.fpg.unc.edu/

Page 44: Socioeconomic Status

WHY IS EARLY EDUCATION IMPORTANT FOR HEALTH?

Page 45: Socioeconomic Status

What did Mischel find?

•  Experimenter left room for up to 15 mins

•  Almost no child lasted 15 mins •  More than a quarter caved within 2 mins •  Average child lasted 6 mins •  Another 25 percent lasted more than 10 mins

Page 46: Socioeconomic Status

Does marshmallow test predict anything of consequence?

Maybe the kids who skipped breakfast on the morning of the experiment had low blood glucose and were more likely to ring the bell…

Page 47: Socioeconomic Status

Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: identifying diagnostic conditions

•  Re-contacted subsample of original kids (N=185) when they were 16-18 years old

•  Parental questions inquiring about adolescent’s coping styles and SAT scores

Shoda et al. (1990). Developmental Psychology, 26(6): 978-986.

Page 48: Socioeconomic Status

Correlations between delay time on marshmallow test & adolescent outcomes

How  likely  is  your  child  to  yield  to  tempta4on?   -­‐.50***  

How  capable  is  your  child  of  exhibi4ng  self-­‐control  when  frustrated?   .40**  

SAT  Verbal   .42*  

SAT  Quan4ta4ve   .57**  

Shoda et al. (1990). Developmental Psychology, 26(6): 978-986.

Page 49: Socioeconomic Status

Correlations between delay time on marshmallow test & adolescent outcomes

How  likely  is  your  child  to  yield  to  tempta4on?   -­‐.50***  

How  capable  is  your  child  of  exhibi4ng  self-­‐control  when  frustrated?   .40**  

SAT  Verbal   .42*  

SAT  Quan4ta4ve   .57**  

*4-­‐year  old  kids  who  waited  5  mins.  longer  scored  on  average  300  points  higher  on  SATs  in  high  school  (171  points  higher  on  quan4ta4ve  &  126  points  higher  on  verbal).    

Shoda et al. (1990). Developmental Psychology, 26(6): 978-986.

Page 50: Socioeconomic Status

Broader implication

•  Early education teaches children to be more patient (executive function)

•  Self regulation promotes healthier lifestyles in adulthood

Early  Educa4on   Preven4ve  behavior  

Willpower  

Page 51: Socioeconomic Status

Quasi-Experiments

Life is short; most of us will not get around to doing experiments on social determinants.

If we can’t directly manipulate the exposure, we

can resort to- •  Instrumental variables •  Natural experiments

Page 52: Socioeconomic Status

Instrumental variables

•  Find a variable (Z) that causes variation in x, but has no direct effect on outcome y

•  i.e. find exogenous source of variation (like a coin toss)

x y

u

Z

Page 53: Socioeconomic Status

Can we find an instrument for education?

Education Risk of mortality

IQ, genes, personality,

etc.

Z

Page 54: Socioeconomic Status

Can we find an instrument for education?

Educational attainment Risk of mortality

IQ, genes, personality,

etc.

Compulsory schooling laws in the state of one’s birth

Page 55: Socioeconomic Status

Compulsory schooling laws (CSLs) as instruments for education

•  In the U.S., the number of years a child must spend in school is determined by state law

•  Requirements have historically varied between states, and states changed their CSLs repeatedly during the first half of the 20th century

•  States extended mandatory schooling by: a) lowering the age at which children had to begin school, or b) raising the age at which they could drop out or get a work permit

•  Hence, we can treat CSLs as “natural policy experiments”

Page 56: Socioeconomic Status

Source: Lleras-Muney A (2005). “The Relationship between Education and Adult Mortality in the U.S.” Rev Econ Stud, vol. 72.

Page 57: Socioeconomic Status

2 Stage least squares (2SLS)

edictorsPrOther X̂ k α+ Ζα+α= 10

ε + β+ β+β= 10 edictorsPrOther X̂Y k

Page 58: Socioeconomic Status

Findings

•  IV estimates of education on mortality suggest strong protective effect

•  Each additional year of schooling lowers 5-year mortality rates by between 3.6 to 5 percent1

•  Recent analyses using similar approach also suggest causal effect of education on improved cognitive functioning at older ages2

1.Lleras-Muney (2005). 2.Glymour, Kawachi, Robins & Berkman (2008) JECH, 62(2):532-437

Page 59: Socioeconomic Status

Explanations for the association between schooling and health

1.  Non causal: –  Reverse causation –  Confounding by IQ or other measure of ability –  Less time spent in hazardous jobs (“warehousing”)

0

100

200

300

400

500

600

Men Women

Death

s per

100,0

00

< 12 yrs12 yrs13+ yrs

Source: Health, United States, SES and Health Chartbook 1998

Page 60: Socioeconomic Status

Explanations for the association between schooling and health

2. Causal – cognitive mechanisms: –  Acquisition of knowledge –  Health literacy

Page 61: Socioeconomic Status

Explanations for the association between schooling and health

2. Causal – cognitive mechanisms: –  Acquisition of knowledge –  Health literacy

3. Causal – non-cognitive mechanisms: –  Self-regulation: patience, ability to plan for the future –  Executive functioning

Page 62: Socioeconomic Status

Explanations for the association between schooling and health

4. Indirect mechanisms: –  Schooling and credentials as gateways to safer jobs, higher

income 5.  Other:

–  Higher prestige/status in community –  More social connections (“social capital”) –  Improved future prospects leads to greater incentive to invest

in health

Page 63: Socioeconomic Status

If we accept that schooling matters, where should we invest? •  Knudsen et al. (2006). Economic, neurobiological,

and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. PNAS, 103(27): 10155-10162.

“There is a striking convergence of four core concepts that have emerged from decades of mutually independent research in economics, neuroscience, and developmental psychology…” [ ]

Page 64: Socioeconomic Status

Core concept #1 [From neuroscience]

•  Skill development and brain maturation are

hierarchical processes in which higher-level functions depend on (and build on) lower level functions.

•  Skills beget skills.

Page 65: Socioeconomic Status

Core concept #2 [From developmental psychology]

•  Early experiences have a uniquely powerful

influence on the development of cognitive and social skills and on brain architecture.

•  There are developmentally sensitive periods for optimal learning, e.g. language acquisition.

Page 66: Socioeconomic Status

Knudsen et al. (2006). Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. PNAS, 103(27): 10155-10162.

Page 67: Socioeconomic Status

•   Bowl  or  rice  •   Bowl  of  lice  

Knudsen et al. (2006). Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. PNAS, 103(27): 10155-10162.

Page 68: Socioeconomic Status

Core concept #3 [from economics]

•  Early intervention lowers the cost of later

investment.

•  Later remediation efforts are less effective – e.g. adult literacy services, prisoner rehabilitation programs & education programs for disadvantaged adults yield lower economic returns than early intervention.

Page 69: Socioeconomic Status

Knudsen et al. (2006). Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. PNAS, 103(27): 10155-10162.

Page 70: Socioeconomic Status

Core concept #4 •  Everything is not all over by age 4!

•  Early interventions are sustained best when they are followed by continued high quality learning experiences.

•  Early investments must be followed by later investments to recoup maximum value.

Page 71: Socioeconomic Status

Conclusion •  The most cost effective strategy for education is

investing in the social and cognitive environments of children who are disadvantaged, beginning as early in life as possible.

•  [The estimated rate of return per dollar of cost in the Perry Program is > 17%, i.e. far higher than the standard rate of return on stock market equity].